Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

A brief introduction to environmental psychology

Peter M Forster
Charles Darwin University

Figure 1: Forest of Dean

Abstract and theories in the field, and to highlight one or


Environmental psychology is a relatively two trends.
new branch of psychology that studies the
interactions between humans and their
environments from a psychological Perceiving and navigating through
perspective. This brief overview of some environments
main themes and theories is intended to
introduce newcomers to this exciting and
One of the longest-studied areas of
important field. Ideas introduced here
include the effects of environments on environmental psychology is that of how we
perception, on health and well-being and on perceive and navigate through environments.
crime and aggression. Most psychologists who have studied visual
perception have studied object perception.
Environmental psychology is the study of the Environmental perception is different from
interconnections between people and object perception:
environments, including natural landscapes and
built environments. It is a diverse and • Environments are larger and more
multidisciplinary area of study that includes complex
topics such as wayfinding, restorative • We experience environments from within
environments and the management of shared • Environments require navigation skills
spaces. With more recent attention being paid to
global changes, the promotion of conservation Seeing environments
behaviour and the need for a ‘conservation We locate ourselves in environments by an
psychology’ is being promoted (Clayton & active process of constructing internal
Myers, in press). The aim of this brief representations or cognitive maps. These
introduction to environmental psychology is to structures bring together past experience with
provide an overview of some of the main topics current perceptions. They include

-1-
representations of cognitions, affects and Perspectives on what we see
behaviour. They enable us to plan our
movements as well as navigate through the Evolutionary perspective
present environment. According to Appleton (1975, p73), our
environmental preferences are derived from our
This combination of past and present means that evolutionary origins. His Prospect-Refuge
we never see the world as it is. We see the theory proposes that our modern preferences are
world we expect, the world coloured by our based on the needs of our hunter-gatherer
feelings, the world coloured by our memories of ancestors to see prey and predators from a long
the present scene and those it reminds us of. way off, combined with the need to hide nearby
Even in new places we see the past not the very quickly. His theory predicts that we will
present. tend to prefer environments like the savannah in
which humans are said to have lived for much
Siegel and White (1975) propose that we begin of our evolutionary history.
the process of constructing cognitive maps by
identifying and remembering landmarks. We
Needs perspective
then put landmarks together into routes, which
Kaplan and Kaplan (1989) developed these
include landmarks and other decision points, for
ideas further and proposed that our
the purpose of wayfinding. Finally they propose
environmental preferences evolved from the
that routes are combined into networks to
most salient environments for seeking food and
provide a more holistic representation of an
fulfilling other fundamental needs. In other
environment.
words, humans have positive aesthetic reactions
to environments in which we have functioned
Others, such as Kuipers (1982) for example, say
more effectively over the greater part of our
that we represent space more like an atlas than a
evolutionary history.
map; that we have representations at different
A study consistent with these ideas was carried
levels of detail. One level might be a relatively
out by Orians & Heerwagen (1992). They
detailed representation of the area around one’s
invited participants from three countries to rate
own house; another might be a less detailed
their preferences for different types of trees. The
representation of the forest in which one’s
most popular were those resembling savannah
house is located; while yet another might be a
trees, while the least popular were those that
course representation of the region of which the
departed most from that type of tree in terms of
forest is a part.
height and density of foliage.
We prefer some places to others. We prefer and
From their study of people’s preferences for
approach places where we expect to experience
photographs of different scenes, the Kaplans
positive feelings and avoid those where we
identified four components of environmental
expect negative feelings (Veitch & Arkkelin,
preferences:
1995, p97). A place that one person perceives
as beautiful and restorative may be seen by
• Coherence: the level of organisation of a
another as sinister and dangerous because of
scene
past experience.
• Legibility: the ease of processing the
Salience produces distortions in our elements of a scene
environmental representations. Ladd (1970) • Complexity: the diversity of the elements in
carried out a classic study of black children who the scene
lived in a district of Boston, USA. Ladd asked • Mystery: the potential of a scene to provide
the children to draw maps of the area and then new information
interviewed them about their drawings.
Distortions included children drawing the areas Urban and natural environments
where white people lived as larger and more Although the natural environment presents city
central than their own area. Some also drew the dwellers with a number of challenges from
street that marked the boundary between the minor problems with the weather and pests, to
black area and the white area as larger than major problems with earthquakes and floods,
other streets. for example, studies that compare people’s
preference for images of urban versus rural
environments usually find a preference for rural
(see Kaplan, Kaplan & Wendt, 1972; and Evans
& Wood, 1980, for example). Viewing a video
of a natural environment also induces more

-2-
relaxation, as measured by heart rate, compared benefits of participating in gardening. The latter
to viewing an urban environment (Laumann, study also reported benefits of increased
Gärling and Stormark, 2003). sociability, reduced vandalism and
Kaplan, Ivancich and De Young (2007), in neighbourhood revitilisation. At an individual
describing the many roles played by natural level, this study also reported increased self-
elements within urban settings, emphasise the esteem. Medical checks on the participants
importance of nature within urban found lowered blood-pressure, reduced need for
environments. As the authors put it, “Urban medication, and feeling more relaxed and
nature is not just an amenity; it is essential.” needed. Both active gardening and the passive
contemplation of plants appear to have
Health and wellbeing therapeutic effects.

The origins of environmental psychology, and According to Mitchell and Popham (2008),
its connections to health and wellbeing can be parks, playing fields and forests provide both a
traced to the work of Proshansky and his stress-reducing restorative effect and also allow
colleagues who, in studying which hospital more physical activity, both of which improve
designs would provide health benefits to health by lowering the risk of heart disease. The
patients, can be said to have founded a new authors observed that having access to such
discipline (Proshansky, Ittelson, & Rivlin, environments substantially narrowed the health
1970). gap between rich and poor in terms of death
rates.
Natural environments and health
According to Ulrich and Addoms (1981), even
In his classic and oft-cited study, Ulrich (1984) people who do not visit parks feel better for
examined hospital patients after they had major knowing that they are there; an argument in
surgery. The patients were in two groups, one of favour of creating parks and reserves in remote
which had a view of trees, plants and grass areas with restricted or perhaps only webcam
while the other had a view of a brick wall. It access.
was found that the patients with a natural view
spent less time in hospital, required fewer pain- A study in Sweden of elderly (average age 86!)
relieving drugs, had fewer complications and residents in a care home, showed that they
received fewer negative comments in nurse's obtained higher scores on tests of concentration
notes compared to patients with the brick view. when they had a rest period in a natural setting
A potential criticism of the study was that the compared to an indoor setting (Ottosson &
differences may have been due to differences Grahn, 2005).
other than the naturalness of the scene, such as
visual complexity. However, subsequent Natural environments and mental health
research suggested that the naturalness of the
view was a significant factor (see Ulrich and Several studies have shown that children
Lunden, 1990, for example). diagnosed with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD) experience an improvement
On a smaller scale, it has also been shown that in their symptoms following outdoor green play,
the presence of indoor plants in a hospital room compared to outdoor urban play or indoor play
can have positive effects on health, including (Faber, Kuo, & Sullivan, 2001; Kuo, & Faber,
recovery from surgery (Park, 2006). This 2004).
suggests that hospitals, hospices and clinics
without access to a natural environment can Natural environments have also been shown to
benefit patients’ health by making indoor plants reduce the impact of stressful events on
available. children, particularly those who had
experienced a greater number of stressful events
(Wells & Evans, 2003).
Natural environments and wellbeing
A longitudinal study of older people living in
Two studies of gardeners found benefits of France demonstrated a link between gardening
active engagement with garden environments. and a reduced risk of developing dementia
Kaplan (1973) studied largely middle-class (Fabrigoule, Letenneur, Commenges &
members of the American Horticulture Society Barberger-Gateau, 1995).
while Lewis (1996) studied working-class urban
gardeners in New York, Philadelphia, Chicago
and Vancouver, Canada. Both reported
peacefulness, tranquillity and wellbeing as

-3-
Theories on the restorative effects of activities, there is evidence that green areas with
environments trees are more likely to attract local residents of
all ages to use the space and thus deter crime
(Coley, Sullivan, & Kuo, 1997).
Biophilia – Wilson (1984) defined biophilia as In a study of felt safety, one hundred residents
“the innately emotional affiliation of human of a single area in Chicago reported that their
beings to other living organisms. Innate means sense of safety was influenced by the density of
hereditary and hence part of ultimate human trees and the maintenance of the grass in an area
nature”. adjacent to their homes (Kuo, Bacaicoa, &
Sullivan, 1998).
Wilson proposes that humans have evolved to
respond positively to natural environments to A further study using police crime reports in a
help us survive. The theory proposes that we are city neighbourhood found that more vegetation
attracted to environments where we feel more around a building was associated with fewer
positive emotions and can live more effectively. violent crimes and fewer property crimes (Kuo
The theory accounts for some aspects of & Sullivan, 2001a).
behaviour, but needs extending to account for
the biophobia experienced by some. It is unclear whether such effects are due to
reduced mental fatigue leading to reduced
Attention Restoration Theory – This model impulsiveness or to the perception that an area
assumes that there are two types of attention. is better cared for and protected, and is thus a
One type of attention requires focussed less suitable target for crime. What is clear is
concentration and is tiring. Indirect attention, that the presence of trees, grass and other
however, is said to attract our attention with vegetation in an otherwise built environment
little effort and is therefore restorative. The has an important role in influencing the social
model further proposes that natural interactions that take place there (Sullivan, Kuo,
environments are more likely to facilitate & Depooter, 2004).
indirect attention (Chang, Hammitt, Chen,
Machnik, & Su, 2008; Kaplan, 1995). Directed
attention, such as that required to read this
article, takes effort and is tiring. The result,
Effects on aggression
according to Kaplan (2001), is distractibility, A further study by Kuo & Sullivan (2001b)
planning impairment, impulsive behaviour and supports the view that natural environments can
irritability. This model has been evoked to contribute to reduced aggression and violence in
account for the reduction in crime observed in inner cities by reducing mental fatigue and thus
areas with more natural features such as trees irritability. In this study, levels of aggression
and grassy areas. and violence by single mothers was reduced in
those who had a view of trees and a green area,
Psycho-physiological Stress Recovery Theory – compared to those with a built (labelled
This model is based on the observation that ‘barren’) view. The study involved random
viewing natural environments leads to a allocation of families to a housing project. Bell,
reduction in heart rate and blood pressure in Greene, Fisher & Baum (2005) discuss this
people who report feelings of stress (Ulrich, effect in terms of attention restoration theory.
1983; Velarde, Fry, & Tveit, 2007). This model
proposes that natural settings are restorative Conclusion
because they had survival-related advantages
for early humans, thus helping us cope with Although a very slim introduction to
stress. Research within this model includes the environmental psychology, the areas outlined
observation that participants who view even a here are clearly relevant and important to
video of natural surroundings recover more human health and well-being. Yet, from even
rapidly from stress (Ulrich, Simons, Losito, the early days of the field (see Proshansky,
Fiorito, Miles & Zeison, 1991). 1987, for example) doubts have been cast about
the viability of the area. Stokols (1995) offers a
These models are not mutually exclusive. different perspective and suggests that the field
of environmental psychology is flourishing, but
Crime and aggression that it is losing its distinct identity as its
concepts and methods become diffused through
Effects on crime many other branches of psychology and other
Within cities, although it is possible that dense disciplines.
vegetation could provide cover for criminal

-4-
For a contemporary account of the field and a Journal of Environmental Psychology,
more in-depth review of the trends that have 29, 375-386.
contributed to the current state of environmental Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of
psychology, see Giuliani and Scopelliti (2009). nature: toward an integrative
framework. Journal of Environmental
For an exciting synthesis of environmental Psychology, 15, 241-248.
psychology with another relatively new field,
positive psychology, see Kaplan and Kaplan, Kaplan, S. (2001). Meditation, restoration and
(2008). the management of mental fatigue.
Environment & Behavior, 33(4), 480-
It has been an aim of this paper to illustrate the 506.
relevance of environmental psychology to a Kaplan, R., Ivancich, J.E. & De Young, R.
number of important and pressing issues. (2007). Nearby nature in the city:
Indeed, many issues related to human health, Enhancing and preserving livability.
well-being, and both pro-social and anti-social Retrieved 23rd December 2010 from:
behaviour, cannot be understood without http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/48784
including the perspectives and findings of Kaplan, R. & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience
environmental psychology. of Nature: A psychological perspective.
New York: Cambridge University
References Press.
Appleton, J. (1975). The Experience of Kaplan, R. & Kaplan, S. (2008). Bringing out
Landscape. London: John Wiley. the best in people: A psychological
Bell, P. A., Greene, T. C., Fisher, J. D., & perspective. Conservation Biology,
Baum, A. S. (2005). Environmental 22(4), 826-829.
psychology (Fifth ed.). New York: Kaplan, S., Kaplan, R., & Wendt, J. S. (1972).
Harcourt College. Rated preference and complexity for
Chang, C.-Y., Hammitt, W. E., Chen, P.-K., natural and urban visual material.
Machnik, L., & Su, W.-C. (2008). Perception and Psychophysics, 12,
Psychophysiological responses and 354-356.
restorative values of natural Kuipers, B., 1982, The `Map in the head'
environments in Taiwan. Landscape metaphor. Environment and Behavior,
and Urban Planning, 85(2), 79-84. 14, 202-220.
Clayton, S., & Myers, O. E. J. (in press). Kuo, F. E., Bacaicoa, M., & Sullivan, W. C.
Conservation Psychology: (1998). Transforming inner-city
Understanding and promoting human landscapes: trees, sense of safety and
care for nature. New York: Wiley- preference. Environment and Behavior,
Blackwell. 30(1), 28-59.
Coley, R. L., Sullivan, W. C., & Kuo, F. E. Kuo, F.E. & Faber, T.A. (2004). A potential
(1997). Where Does Community natural nreatment for Attention-Deficit
Grow? The Social Context Created by Hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from
Nature in Urban Public Housing a national study. American Journal of
Environment and Behavior, 29(4), 468- Public Health, 94(9), 1580-1586.
494. Kuo, F. E., & Sullivan, W. C. (2001a).
Evans, G.W., & K.W. Wood. 1980. Assessment Environment and Crime in the Inner
of environmental aesthetics in scenic City: Does Vegetation Reduce Crime?
highway corridors. Environment & Environment and Behavior, 33(3), 343-
Behavior, 12, 255-273. 367.
Faber, T.A., Kuo, F. & Sullivan, W. (2001). Kuo, F. E., & Sullivan, W. C. (2001b).
Coping with ADD: the surprising Aggression and violence in the inner
connection to green play setting. city: Effects of environment via mental
Environment and Behavior. 33, 54-77. fatigue. Environment and Behavior,
Fabrigoule, C., Letenneur, L., Commenges, D. 33(4), 543-571.
& Barberger-Gateau, P.J. (1995) Social Kuo, F. E., & Sullivan, W. C. (2001a).
and leisure activities and risk of Environment and Crime in the Inner
dementia: a prospective longitudinal City: Does Vegetation Reduce Crime?
study. Journal of the American Environment and Behavior, 33(3), 343-
Geriatric Society, 43(5), 485-490. 367.
Giuliani, M.V. & Scopelliti, M. (2009).
Empirical research in environmental
psychology: past, present and future.

-5-
Ladd, F. C. 1970. Black youths view their Ulrich, R.S. and Lunden, O. (1990). Effects of
environment. Environment and nature and abstract pictures on patients
Behavior 2, 74-99. recovering from open heart surgery.
Laumann, K., Gärling, T. & Stormark, K.M. Paper presented at the International
(2003). Selective attention and heart Congress of Behavioral Medicine,
rate responses to natural and urban Uppsala, Sweden, June 27-30.
environments. Journal of Ulrich, R., Simons, R.F., Losito, E., Fiorito, E.,
Environmental Psychology, 23(2), 125- Miles, M.A. & Zelson, M. (1991).
134. Stress recovery during exposure to
Lewis, C.A. (1996). Green Nature, Human natural and urban environments.
Nature: The Meaning of Plants in Our Journal of Environmental Psychology,
Lives. Chicago: University of Illinois 11, 201-230.
Press. Veitch, R. and Arkkelin, D. (1995).
Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Environmental psychology: An
Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. interdisciplinary perspective. Prentice-
H. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby Hall, Inc.
(Eds.), The adapted mind: Velarde, M. D., Fry, G., & Tveit, M. (2007).
Evolutionary psychology and the Health effects of viewing landscapes –
generation of culture (pp. 555-579). landscape types in environmental
New York: Oxford University Press. psychology. Urban Forestry and
Ottosson, J. & Grahn, P. (2005). A comparison Urban Greening, 6(4), 199-212.
of leisure time spent in a garden with Wells, N.M. & Evans, G.W. (2003). Nearby
leisure time spent indoors: On nature: a buffer of life stress among
measures of restoration in residents in rural children. Environment and
geriatric care. Landscape Research, Behavior, 35(3), 311-330.
30(1) 23-55. Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia: The Human
Park, S.-H. (2006). Randomized clinical trials Bond with Other Species. Cambridge:
evaluating therapeutic influences of Harvard University Press.
ornamental indoor plants in hospital
rooms on health outcomes of patients
recovering from surgery. Unpublished
Dissertation, Kansas State University.
Proshansky, H. M. (1987). The field of
environmental psychology: securing its
future. In D. Stokols, & I. Altman
(eds.), Handbook of Environmental
Psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 1467–1488).
New York: Wiley.
Proshansky, H. M., Ittelson, W. H., & Rivlin, L.
G. (1970). Environmental psychology.
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Siegel, A.W. & White, S.H. (1975). The
development of spatial representations
of large-scale environments, In H.W.
Reese (ed) Advances in Child
Development & Behavior by H.W.
Reese 10, 9-55. New York: Academic
Press.
Stokols, D. (1995). The paradox of
environmental psychology. American
Psychologist, 50, 821–837.
Sullivan, W. C., Kuo, F. E., & Depooter, S. F.
(2004). The Fruit of Urban Nature:
Vital Neighborhood Spaces.
Environment and Behavior, 36(5), 678-
700.
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective
response to natural environment. In I.
Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.),
Behavior and the Natural Environment
(pp. 85-125). New York: Plenum Press. Figure 2: Todd River, Alice Springs

-6-

S-ar putea să vă placă și