Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to perceive and comprehend language,

as well as to produce and use words and sentences to communicate. Language acquisition usually refers to first-language acquisition, which studies infants' acquisition of their native language. This is distinguished from second-language acquisition, which deals with the acquisition (in both children and adults) of additional languages. The capacity to successfully use language requires one to acquire a range of tools including phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and an extensive vocabulary.

Language Acquisition
Betty Birner
Download this document as a pdf.

How do children acquire language? Do parents teach their children to talk?


No. Children acquire language quickly, easily, and without effort or formal teaching. It happens automatically, whether their parents try to teach them or not. Although parents or other caretakers don't teach their children to speak, they do perform an important role by talking to their children. Children who are never spoken to will not acquire language. And the language must be used for interaction with the child; for example, a child who regularly hears language on the TV or radio but nowhere else will not learn to talk. Children acquire language through interaction - not only with their parents and other adults, but also with other children. All normal children who grow up in normal households, surrounded by conversation, will acquire the language that is being used around them. And it is just as easy for a child to acquire two or more languages at the same time, as long as they are regularly interacting with speakers of those languages. The special way in which many adults speak to small children also helps them to acquire language. Studies show that the 'baby talk' that adults naturally use with infants and toddlers tends to always be just a bit ahead of the level of the child's own language development, as though pulling the child along. This 'baby talk' has simpler vocabulary and sentence structure than adult language, exaggerated intonation and sounds, and lots of repetition and questions. All of these features help the child to sort out the meanings, sounds, and sentence patterns of his or her language.

When do children learn to talk?


There is no one point at which a child learns to talk. By the time the child first utters a single meaningful word, he or she has already spent many months playing around with the sounds and intonations of language and connecting words with meanings. Children acquire language in stages, and different children reach the various stages at different times. The order in which these stages are reached, however, is virtually always the same. The first sounds a baby makes are the sounds of crying. Then, around six weeks of age, the baby will begin making vowel sounds, starting with aah, ee, and ooh. At about six months, the baby starts to produce strings of consonant-vowel pairs like boo andda. In this stage, the child is playing around with the sounds of speech and sorting out the sounds that are important for making words in his or her language from the sounds that aren't. Many parents hear a child in this stage produce a combination like "mama" or "dada" and excitedly declare that the child has uttered his or her first word, even though the child probably didn't attach any meaning to the 'word'. Somewhere around age one or one and a half, the child will actually begin to utter single words with meaning. These are always 'content' words like cookie, doggie, run,and see - never 'function' words like and, the, and of. Around the age of two, the child will begin putting two words together to make 'sentences' like doggie run. A little later on, the child may produce longer sentences that lack function words, such as big doggie run fast. At this point all that's left to add are the function words, some different sentence forms (like the passive), and the more complex sound combinations (like str). By the time the child enters kindergarten, he INTRODUCTION Childrens acquisition of language has long been considered one of the uniquely defining characteristics ofhuman behaviour. Still today, it is the commonly held belief that children acquire their mother tongue through imitation of the parents, caregivers or the people in their environment. Linguists too had the same conviction until 1957, when a then relatively unknown man, A. Noam Chomsky, propounded his theory that the capacity to acquire language is in fact innate. This revolutionized the study of language acquisition, and after a brief period of controversy upon the publication of his book, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, in 1964, his theories are now generally accepted as largely true. As a consequence, he was responsible for the emergence of a new field during the 1960s, Developmental Psycholinguistics, which deals with childrens first language acquisition. He was not the first to question our hitherto mute acceptance of a debatable concept long before, Plato wondered how children could possibly acquire so complex a skill as language with so little experience of life. Experiments have clearly identified an ability to discern syntactical nuances in very young infants, although they are still at the pre-linguistic stage. Children of three, however, are able to manipulate very complicated syntactical sentences, although they are unable to tie their own shoelaces, for example. Indeed, language is not a skill such as many others, like learning to drive or perform mathematical operations it cannot be taught as such in these early stages. Rather, it is the acquisition of language which fascinates linguists today, and how it is possible. Noam Chomsky turned the worlds eyes to this enigmatic question at a time when it was assumed to have a deceptively simple explanation. Further in this term-paper I am going to describe the stages in child language acquistion starting

from the very birth of an infant till the onset of puberty.

LANGUAGE ACQUISTION There are many facts that are intriguing about the language. The fact that all humans have it, and all non-humans do not. The fact that children are able to learn it very quickly. The fact that there exist so many languages and children of all countries are able to acquire them at the same speed. This list can go on and on, and I am sure that the reader can think of many other fascinating features of language. As stated by cognitive researches Language is paramount among the capacities that characterize humans, setting us off from even the most perfectly formed and functioning of the other beasts on earth so. As a matter of species pride if nothing else we would hold up language as a marker of our humanity and thus a focus of our scientific interests. By understanding language, we understand something important about ourselves. (5,29) The way in which children acquire language is at the centre of a debate. Learning theorists such as Skinner (1957) maintained that language is acquired through reinforcement. Chomsky (1959) argued that language was far too complex to be learned so competently in such a short space of time, by cognitively immature toddlers, merely by reinforcement. He argued that the neonate arrives equipped with a LAD (Language Acquisition Device). This contains a set of rules common to all languages and allows children to learn any language which they are exposed to. Slobin (1985) suggested a similar innate device the LMC (Language-Making Capacity). The interactionist perspective suggests that a combination of biological and cognitive factors plus the linguistic environment are all necessary for the acquisition of language. (11, 17-28) There are many distinctions between the processes of learning and acquisition. For instance, the terms are generally used to separate first language acquisition from second language learning, and implied within this distinction is the gap between children of 0-5 years learning their mother tongue, and those beyond puberty who may begin at this stage to learn a second language, or more. The process is a conscious one in learning whereas it is subconscious in acquisition and in language acquisition the focus is on communication or reception of a message as opposed to syntax and grammar as is the case in language learning. Moreover, the context is usually crucial and meaningful in language acquisition, but need not be important to the same extent in language learning. Motivation, too, is a factor that may broaden the gulf between learning and acquisition, but need not be factor that may broaden the gulf between learning and acquisition, as for the latter the language is a matter of urgent necessity. Most importantly, however, the usual outcome of language acquisition is fluency, which is by no means guaranteed in language learning. We often ask questions such as, do you remember when you learned to tie your shoes, ride a bike, and eat with a fork. Sometimes we can remember because a parent helped us learn how to do these things. So, since we always speak the language of our parents, they must have helped us learn to speak our first language. But do you remember when your mother taught you the past tense? When your father laid down the rules for passive sentences? We do not remember these important moments of our childhood because they never occurred. Our parents did not teach us how to walk and they did not teach us how to talk. Yet, we learned from them. How can this be? Certainly there must have been a subtle, perhaps intuitive teaching process that neither our parents nor we were aware of. We begin by imitating what we hear our parents say as best as we can, repeating random phrases. Our parents in subtle ways punish us for

the childish speech errors we make and reward correct phrases. As our speech improves, our parents respond more positively and less negatively. The evidence then indicates that children do, in fact, absorb a massive number of sentences and phrases but rather than parrot them back, they abstract rules from them and create their own grammar which they then apply to create new utterances they have never heard before. Despite the fact that children dont know when their parents are speaking grammatically and when they are making errors, all children grow up knowing the language perfectly.

THE STAGES OF LANGUAGE ACQUISTION Children with normal hearing develop speech and language in predictable stages. While there may be variation in the times of onset, and length of time of each of these stages, they are always present. Should this pattern not develop normally, the child may face a lifelong communication handicap. Normal hearing is crucial for this development and thus deviation from this pattern of development is often a sign that the child has a hearing loss. There exist a crucial time period in which a child must be exposed to the linguistic environment or any form of communication. The time of language acquisition can be described by the following timeline: Shortly before their first birthday, babies begin to understand words, and around that birthday, they start to produce them. This one-word can last from two months to a year. Around 18 month of age, language changes in two ways. Vocabulary growth increases; the child begins to learn words at a rate of one every two waking hours and will keep learning at that rate of faster throughout adolescence. Primitive syntax emerges, including two-word strings like papa away or see pretty. By the time they are two many children speak in complex sentences. It is safe t o way that except for constructions that are rare, predominantly used in written language, or mentally taxing even to an adult, all parts of all languages are acquired before the child turns 4. It is worth mentioning that continuous tracking of language acquisition is a difficult task: Between the late 2s and mid-3s, childrens language blooms into fluent grammatical conversation so rapidly that it overwhelms the researchers who study it; no one has worked out the exact sequence. Sentence length increases steadily and, because grammar is a combinatorial system, reaching thousands before the third birthday. An interesting task would be to find out if this growth depends on culture or environment. Amazingly, children do not seem to favour any language. They swiftly acquire free word order, rich systems of case and agreement, strings of agglutinated suffixes, ergative case marking, all languages are acquired before the child turns 4. Babies start acquiring communication skills as soon as they are born. Luckily they are equipped with one of the most effective tools for communicating - their cry. And they make good use of this tool, as any sleep deprived parent can attest! Pretty quickly, babies are able to produce different cries; one for hunger; one for fatigue; one for fright. Since babies learn to differentiate their cries without any adult intervention, I think many parents assume that their baby will learn language just by hearing other people talk. While, it is true that babies will pick up some language, inflection and other conversational skills from what they hear, and the most effective method to insure that a child will acquire language is to talk directly to them, early and often. That is because babies are born imitators. (7, 35-39) The language learning process goes through several stages: Prelinguistic stage (from birthday till around 6 months of age) crying, cooing, vocal play

Babbling (starts around 6 months of age) consists in the seemingly random production of sounds One-word utterances (12-15 months) the child starts to say the words of the parents language Two-word utterances (beginning at approximately 1-2 years old) the child begins to form twoword utterances Telegraphic speech (2-3 years) - Full sentences with syntactic structure appear

THE PRELINGUISTIC STAGE 0-1 month (new-born stage): Reflexive behaviour, suck-swallow patterns, nondifferentiated crying, vegetative sounds. New-borns prefer the sound of speech to other rhythmic sounds. DeCasper & Fifer (1980) find out that by 3 days of age new-born babies can recognize their mothers' voices. They can also discriminate consonant sounds such as ba and pa. 2-3 months (cooing stage): Definite stop and start to oral movement. By 2 months they can discriminate the vowel sounds a and i. Three months gurgles and makes more vowel sounds. 4-6 months (babbling): Greater independent control of tongue, prolonged strings of sounds, experiments with sounds, sound play (da, ma, di, du ...). Five months imitates sounds. Six months babbles, using first single and then double syllables, combines vowels and consonants and talks to himself in a singsong. Deaf infants will babble manually at around the same age. Gestures and other non-verbal responses start at around 8-10 months. (9,44)

BABBLING 6-10 months (reduplication babbling): Repetitive syllable production, increased lip control, incompletely formed plosives (p,b,t,d) and nasals. Seven to nine months babbling rises and falls and sounds more like real conversation; baby imitates adult noises like a shout or cough, shouts to get attention. The child produces the full range of possible speech sounds even those which do not occur in speech heard in immediate environment, and which she or he may later find impossible to reproduce when learning a foreign language. Thus, this does not occur by imitating the sounds in the parents language, since phonemes from other languages occur, and the frequency of phoneme production matches the frequency in the worlds languages rather than in the parents language. Also, infants at this age can tell sounds apart that are not distinguished by older members of the childs linguistic community. Around the age of 9 months, children usually begin to experiment with the early developing sounds such as p, m and b. These may consist of consonant-vowel (CV) combinations, such as ma or ba, and can increase to CVC mom or CVCV mama strings. Because these sounds are relatively visible, parent modelling can play an important role in a childs acquisition of consonants. Oviatt (1980) found that infants of 12-17 months can understand the meaning of many words before they can use them in their own speech. This is called 'receptive language' (9, 49-52) If you introduce language and communication to a young baby in a responsive and caring manner,

the baby will reciprocate with sounds, sound combinations and later with word approximations and eventually words. Here are three simple ideas to help babies of varying ages learn to communicate: 1) A simple method with very young infants is to hold them close, make eye contact, exaggerate your smile and talk with a lilting voice. Within minutes, you have an infant talking back to you trying to imitate your facial and mouth movements. Since most babies are sensitive to sound and can pick up tension or upset in a persons voice, it is important to remember to modulate your voice when speaking to them. 2) By about four months, a baby will squeal with delight, coo, laugh and make sounds when looking at toys or familiar people. One of the best methods to elicit those responses is to read to a baby. People may kid you when you begin reading to your child when he/she is only three months old. It is for sure that exposing them to language at such an early age is one of the reasons, those teenagers devours books now, loves writing and will talk endlessly on the phone. Not only do books encourage language and communication but they also help to increase babys attention span . 3) By about eight months, babies usually are making two letter sound productions such as ga, da, ka or ba. When a little one makes these sounds, it is a good idea to copy him or her and make the exact sounds back. Most times, a baby will figure out the game. (6, 57-63)

ONE-WORD UTTERANCES Shortly before their first birthday, babies begin to understand words, and around that time, they start to produce them. Altogether, it is between ten and twenty months the child finally begins to produce recognizable single words, and we say at this point that it is beginning to speak. The child starts to say the words of the parents language. This one-word stage continues for concrete time, as the child slowly adds new words to its vocabulary. That vocabulary does not grow rapidly. It includes several different kinds of words, but does not include any grammatical words, nor does the child use any grammatical endings such as past tenses or plurals. The words are often pronounced differently (probably because the childs hearing hasnt developed fully yet); for example, children tend to delete parts of hard-to-pronounce consonant clusters (such as the str in string), sometimes producing words with sounds that arent part of the parents language (such as sring sr is not a permitted consonant cluster in English); finally, the errors the child makes often result from under or overgeneralization (the proverbial case of calling every four-legged creature doggie). The words produced in one-word stage (or holophrastic stage) are not just any words. For example you get: cookie, drink, bad, fast, go, yes/no. But never: in, the and a. It looks as though the distinction between Open class and Closed class words (open-class words are "context" words like nouns, verbs, and adjectives, while closed-class words are "function" words such as articles, conjunctions, and auxiliaries) come into play. This then is further evidence to the Psychological reality of that division. These single words may even function as illocutionary acts: may assert, command or question. (9, 55) Single words represent an entire sentence, i.e. Milk spoken by a toddler at this stage of language development, may be used to represent the sentence, 'Please may I have some milk'. Scientists have discovered that infants first words, and the lists are almost identical all over the world. Children differ only in how much they name objects or engage in social interaction using memorized routines. Presumably children record some words parents use in isolation, or in stressed final positions. Then they look for matches to these words in longer stretches of speech, and find other words by extracting these residues in between the matched portions.(9, 56)

TWO-WORD UTTERANCES This stage begins at approximately at the age of one year to one and half. The child begins to form two-word utterances, of a sort that look like edited versions of grammatically correct, complete sentences. Vocabulary growth humps to a new word every two hours minimum rate that the child will maintain through adolescence. Syntax begins, with strings of the minimum length that allows it, namely, two. These two-word sentences already reflect the language being acquired in ninety five percent of them, the words are properly ordered. Importantly, a child at this stage hardly ever says anything with non-adult word order. Still, however, there are no grammatical words or endings. There is more going on in childrens minds than in what comes out of their mouths. Even bef ore they put two words together, babies can comprehend a sentence using its syntax. (9, 58) At this stage some pronouns, especially me/you appear. Also appears the intonation and a structure of a sentence (often N V). Children at this stage might say doggie gone an edited or telescoped version of The doggie is gone This shows grasp of phrase structure rules. Combinations of words into short sentences, which tend to leave out articles and prepositions, start at around 18-24 months. These sentences demonstrate that children use syntactically correct word orders. The short sentences which children utter at this stage usually omit words such as a, on, and the, Gerken et al (1990) suggest that this may be because they see these function words as 'spacers' between the more heavily stressed content words. At this stage the great development of language can be observed, and it is very interesting to record babys each word, at what age and how did it produce. As language learning proceeds, the pace quickens and by 18-24 months infants may add up to 40 new words a week to their vocabulary, Dromi (1987). In total at age of 18 months: Vocabulary of a baby may be of 20-100 words including nouns and verbs and other parts of speech and single word sentences. For example, Debbys mother recorded the words she had pronounced during the 8 months after the appearance of her first word at 9 months (this was [adi], used both for her "daddy" and her sister "Annie") - she had acquired about 35 words. In the two left hand columns is a list of these words, organized very roughly by category ("Names 1", etc.). During the two weeks from 17 months - 17 months and a half, she more than doubled her vocabulary (the words in the two right hand columns, "Names 2", etc.). (9, 69) TIPS TO HELP DEVELOP SPEECH COMMUNICATION IN A CHILD: 1. Ensure that the child is wearing appropriate amplification or other sensory aids during all waking hours of the day. 2. Talk to the child clearly, at a normal conversational level, and at close range. 3. Be aware of the child's interests, and talk to him/her about the features in the objects and events that are taking place while observing them. 4. Follow the child's focus of attention. Don't try to constantly lead or control topics of discussion. 5. Talk to the child about the routine situations in his/her life. 6. Talk about shared experiences and events and objects that are common to both of you.

7. Use different voice patterns when indicating various aspects of speech to the child. 8. Use intonation to mark important elements in utterances. 9. Allow the child plenty of time to respond during conversations. 10. Shift gradually the responsibility for being understood from you to the child as he acquires more spoken language. 11. Use mainly complete sentences when speaking. 12. Provide the child with the opportunity to acquire language by participating in activities which are common in life. 13. Maintain a close relationship with professionals in order to provide the child with the optimal opportunities to develop spoken language communication. 14. Be prepared to be the primary person responsible for the initial development of the child's spoken language. 15. Work in as many one-on-one situations as possible until the child has acquired reasonable communication skills. 16. Remember children who have learned in primarily one-on-one situations have to learn additional communication skills to interact appropriately in a group setting. 17. Remember to set aside time to just play with the child without concentrating on language development. 18. Do not begin formal teaching of vowels and consonants until ample time has been spent on the informal learning of spoken language. 19. Remember that speech production and reception are complex issues and give the child adequate time to learn them before you conclude that he/she cannot handle them. 20. Use meaningful communication as a base for developing spoken language. 21. Optimize the child's contact with his/her hearing peers in order to enhance natural development of his/her spoken language skills. 22. Motivate the child to talk by ensuring that his/her experiences with the acquisition of spoken language are successful. 23. Adopt an alternative communication system only as a last resort. CONCLUSION Language acquisition refers to the process where a normally developed child is able to become a proficient language user without having to make seemingly difficult efforts. Acquisition, therefore, is in contrast with learning which refers to the processes where deliberate efforts have to be made in order to become proficient with the use of language, for instance, the ability to write and read. Children seem to be able to acquire a language at a speed no adults can match. By the age of three, a child is able to communicate with adults as well as with peers for his/her needs. From the analysis of the childs acquisition process of a language, it seems that the child is a linguist him/herself in that the child is able to construct his/her grammar of the language, hence the little linguist theory. Each child goes through several stages before he/she starts talking. The acquisition of language starts as soon as a child is born. From birthday till around six months of age is the prelinguistic stage, when a child is crying and cooing. Then comes the stage of babbling that lasts from about seven to nine months, the child then produces the full range of possible speech sounds - even those which do not occur in speech heard in immediate environment. One-word utterances start from twelve until fifteen month of age. The child's vocabulary includes some several different kinds of words, but does not include any grammatical words, nor grammatical endings. The child's vocabulary in this stage does not grow rapidly. But the rapidity of growth of vocabulary starts at the stage of two-word utterances. Here the intonation and a structure of a sentence appear. However, there are no grammatical words or endings. At the age of three the child starts to pronouns the full sentences with syntactic structure and the vocabulary has grown to almost one thousand of words

including nouns, verbs, pronouns and adjectives. However, what a child does when she or he learns language: what exactly does a child do? Whatever it is, it is universal it is acquired regardless of culture, language, class, etc. it is effortless.

What materials and tools do I need to study a language?


Possibly the most useful tools there are for learning a language are writing implements, such as pens and pencils, and something to write on, such as paper or a notebook. You can use them not only to make notes and lists of vocabulary, phrases, etc, but also to practise writing in your L2. The range of other materials and tools available to help you with your language studies is huge. From language courses and dictionaries, to mp3 players and portable translators. All of them can help you in different ways, and your language learning goals will determine, to some extent, which ones are most useful to you. Below is a guide to the types of some of the language learning materials and tools currently available. Language courses | Dictionaries | Grammars | Phrasebooks | Flash cards | Audio books |Recording devices | Sound editors | Electronic translators

Language courses
Language courses come in a variety of guises, including traditional ones, all audio ones, CDROMs and online ones.

Traditional courses
The traditional ones tend to consist of textbooks containing dialogues, exercises, notes on grammar, and maybe cultural information. Accompanying cassettes, CDs and/or mp3s are often available. Most such courses are intended for complete beginners or people with only minimal knowledge of the language. Intermediate and advanced-level courses are available for some of the more popular languages, such as French, German, Italian and Spanish. Examples of this type of course include:

Linguaphone - available for 100 languages and include all audio courses, explorer courses for those who just want to learn the basics, and in-depth courses for more serious students.

Teach Yourself and Colloquial courses - these are similar and are available for a wide range of languages. They focus on teaching you everyday language, and introduce you to new alphabets or other writing systems where appropriate.

All audio courses


This type of course usually consists solely of cassettes or CDs, though some may also include a limited amount of printed material. All dialogues, exercises, instructions and explanations are recorded and the focus of these courses is teaching you to understand and speak the language. Examples of this type of course include:

Pimsleur language courses - the best-known audio courses. They are very good at teaching you authentic pronunciation and a manageable amount of vocabulary. They also give you a good feel for the language you're learning. The Level 1 courses are suitable for complete beginners. More advanced courses are available for some languages.

Computer-aided learning
Various language courses are available on CD-ROMs and DVDs. Each course is different but they generally include dialogues, audio, exercises and tests. Some also include phrasebooks, dictionaries, videos, games and provide online and/or telephone support. Examples of these courses include:

Teach Me! - include CD-Rom with vocabularly and grammar exercises, a series of graded stories and a talking dictionary. An accompanying booklet contains the text of all the stories, and an audio CD includes the vocabulary and the stories. Available for over 40 languages, including Bavarian, Breton, Cornish, Manx, Occitan, Quechua and Romansh.

Transparent Language - a range of language courses for many different languages. Their site also includes useful phrases, games, cultural information, articles, quizzes and proficiency tests.

A selection of multi-language courses

The Bilingualizer
The Bilingualize helps language students read and enjoy texts written in their original language. It ads a discrete word-by-word translation under the original text for the comfort of the student: a great help to build up your vocabulary while reading what you like!

Pronunciation and listening comprehension

Some tips on learning how to pronounce foreign languages and on improving your listening comprehsion.

Why good pronunciation is important


Learning how to pronounce a foreign language like a native speaker is difficult but not impossible. The better your pronunciation, the better people will understand you and the easier you will find it to understand them.

Tuning your ears and practising listening comprehension


Before you start trying to speak a foreign language, spend some time tuning your ears to its sounds and rhythms. You can do this by listening to the language as much as possible via the radio, TV, movies and native speakers in your neighbourhood. There are online radio stationsin a wide variety of languages. When you first try listening to your chosen language, you'll understand only a little or nothing at all, other than perhaps the names of people and places and the odd word, unless you're learning a language closely related to your L1. You may even find it difficult to believe that what you're listening to is a real language that people are able to converse in. However if you continue to listen to your L2 as much as possible, you will gradually become familiar with the the sounds and rhythms of the language and start to be able to pick out words and phrases. Eventually you'll be able to understand most if not all of what you hear in your L2. There are a number of things you try to help you understand radio and TV broadcasts:

Rather than just listening or watching at random, find out what programmes are on and choose one that sounds interesting. In this way you will have an idea of what the programme is about.

Set yourself goals, for example when listening to the news try to work out the main points, names, dates and times.

Brush up relevant vocabulary before tuning in. For example if you're planning to watch a cooking programme, revise food words first

Even if you can't understand very much at all, try to identify word and sentence boundaries

If possible listen to the news in your L1 first, then when you listen to it in your L2 you'll already be aware of the mains stories.

Don't try to listen to or watch too much in one go: if you're finding it a struggle, take a break and/or do something else.

Listening to songs in your L2 is another way to improve your listening comprehension, especially if you have the lyrics written out so that you can follow them as you listen.

Learning pronunciation
With your ears tuned to the sounds and rythmns of your L2, you'll find learning how to pronounce the language less difficult. If your language course includes a recorded pronunciation guide and exercises, listen to them until you have internalized all the points, but don't try to learn everything in one go. Beware of pronunciation guides that compare the sounds of your L2 with your L1. This can be misleading as the sounds, particularly the vowels, are often different.

Better pronunciation through song


If you enjoy singing, try learning some songs in the language you're learning. This is a fun way to improve your pronunciation and vocabulary. You could also try learning to recite poems and stories. Listening to songs in your L2 is an enjoyable way to improve your listening comprehension, especially if you have the lyrics written out so that you can follow them as you listen.

Phonetics and phonology


Learning a bit about phonetics and phonology will enable you to pronounce your L2 better. A good way to start is to learn the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), which can be used to represent all the phonemes of human speech and is therefore a reliable guide to pronunciation.

Accents
Human speech uses a total of over 800 different phonemes. A phoneme is the smallest contrastive unit in the sound system of a language. Babies can hear all possible phonemes but quickly develop a preference for the phonemes of their native language(s). Children tend to be very good mimics, an ability which enables them to learn foreign languages with a native accent. When learning foreign languages in later life, most people find it difficult to hear and pronounce phonemes not present in their native language. This is why most adult language learners have a foreign accent.

Learning tones
If you're learning a tonal language such as Chinese or Thai, you need to find a way to associate each syllable with the appropriate tone. One possible way to do this is to imagine a series of shelves, each one representing a tone. To assign tones to shelves you could number the shelves and/or imagine them having the shape of the tone contours. Each time you learn a new word, picture the word together with the thing, action, quality or whatever it represents on the appropriate shelf.

Learning vocabulary
Building up your vocabulary in a foreign language can take many years. Learning words in context from written and spoken material is probably the most effective way to do this. You could also try learning words in a more systematic way - perhaps a certain number of words every day.

Learning grammar
Familiarity with the grammar of a language enables you to understand it, and also to construct your own phrases and sentences. It's not essential to know all the grammatical terminology or to understand why words change, as long as you're able to apply to relevant changes when necessary.

Learning alphabets and other writing systems


If the language you're learning is written with a different alphabet or other type of writing system, learning it is well worth the effort. Some alphabets, such as Cyrillic and Greek, can be learnt without too difficulty. Others, such as Devanagari and Thai, are a more challenging.

S-ar putea să vă placă și