Sunteți pe pagina 1din 45

Narcissism, Identity, and Legitimacy Author(s): Andrew D. Brown Source: The Academy of Management Review, Vol. 22, No.

3 (Jul., 1997), pp. 643-686 Published by: Academy of Management Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/259409 . Accessed: 08/01/2011 15:12
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at . http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=aom. . Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Academy of Management Review.

http://www.jstor.org

Academy of Management Review 1997, Vol. 22, No. 3, 643-686.

AND LEGITIMACY NARCISSISM,IDENTITY,


ANDREW D. BROWN University of Cambridge
The theory of narcissism can be employed usefully to analyze the dynamics of group and organizational behavior. Just as individuals seek to regulate their self-esteem through such ego-defense mechanisms as denial, rationalization, attributional egotism, sense of entitlement, and ego aggrandizement, which ameliorate anxiety, so too do groups and organizations. An understanding of the behaviors by which groups and organizations regulate self-esteem is important, because it sheds light on the dynamics underlying the legitimacy attributions made by organizational participants. In this article I enlarge and illustrate these arguments.

Ideas ... gain their value from what they allow us to see in organizations.Evocative ideas need to be cultivated by theorists fromthe beginning because belief, not skepticism, precedes observation.... If believing affects seeing, and if theories are significant beliefs that affect what we see, then theories should be adopted more to maximise what we see than to summarise what we have already seen. (Weick, 1987:122) "Narcissism" is an important label that permits us to group cognitions and behaviors, which have, as their common factor, a role to play in the regulation of self-esteem. Although the theory of narcissism most usually is applied at the level of the individual, it also can be usefully employed to inform our understanding of group and organizational behavior. Individuals, groups, and organizations possess identities that are preserved through individual and social processes of self-esteem regulation. Selfesteem is narcissistically regulated through ego-defense mechanisms, such as denial, rationalization, attributional egotism, sense of entitlement, and ego aggrandizement. In organization studies the concept and theory of narcissism can be used to deepen our understanding of both group and organization identity and the dynamics underlying the legitimacy attributions made by organizational participants and external institutions. The theory of narcissism provides us with a lens for understanding organized activity-the research utility of which is at least twofold. First,

The insightful comments of Nick Bacon, Susan Bartholomew, John Child, Graeme Currie, Geoff Eagleson, John Hendry, Nick Oliver, John Roberts, Mick Rowlinson, Susan Jackson, and five anonymous reviewers are gratefully acknowledged. 643

644

Academy of Management

Review

July

narcissism is a potentially valuable organizing label that permits us to make a connection between what we previously have considered disparate aspects of organized activity (e.g., denial, rationalization, attributional egotism, self-aggrandizement, and sense of entitlement). In turn, this suggests that research on, for example, corporate concealment (Abrahamson & Park, 1994), escalation of commitment (Staw, 1980), causal reasoning (Bettman & Weitz, 1983; Salancik & Meindl, 1984; Staw, McKechnie, & Puffer, 1983), and uniqueness claims (Martin, Feldman, Hatch, & Sitkin, 1983; Selznick, 1957) are linked more intimately than we have recognized to date. At a time when our understanding of organization is becoming increasingly fragmented (Martin, 1992; Reed & Hughes, 1992), the integrative power of the narcissism concept may in itself be deemed valuable. Second, viewing organizations as narcissistic entities provides us with a further perspective, focusing on the importance of self-esteem regulation as it is achieved through various ego-defense mechanisms. Various authors have described organizations as systems (Burns & Stalker, 1961; Trist & Bamforth, 1951), theaters (Mangham & Overington, 1983), socially constructed cultures (Pettigrew, 1979), and political arenas (Pfeffer, 1981; Pfeffer & Salancik, 1978). The already substantial literature on self-esteem (Brockner, 1988; Cohen, 1959; Erez & Earley, 1993; Festinger, 1954; Maslow, 1954; Ring & Van de Ven, 1989; Staw, 1980) suggests that the concept of organizations as social entities for the regulation of self-esteem deserves to be considered alongside those more conventional perspectives focusing on information, power, efficiency, and meaning. In this article I pursue these arguments in five major sections. First, I construct a concept of narcissism that can be operationalized in an organizational context. In the second section I employ Tajfel and Turner's (1986) theories of social identity to demonstrate the applicability of the identity concept to groups and organizations. Third, I exemplify the narcissistic tendencies of individuals, groups, and organizations. In the fourth section I outline a theory of group and organizational legitimacy in which the notions of self-esteem and narcissism are crucially implicated. Finally, I draw some conclusions regarding the potential of the issues discussed for further research. NARCISSISM Derived from the Greek myth of Narcissus, the concepts of "self" and "narcissism" in modern psychoanalysis have their origins in Freud's (1914) On Narcissism: An Introduction (Fine, 1986: 294). For Freud, narcissism referred to a state of being the center of a loving world in which the individual could act spontaneously and purely out of desire. Since we once experienced this state as infants, as adults we project the possibility of our returning to narcissism by means of the "ego ideal" (i.e., our model of the person that we must become in order for the world to love us as it did when we were young). Given that mortality, vulnerability, and limitation are existential facts of the human situation of each individual

1997

Brown

645

(Becker, 1973; Lichstenstein, 1977), no individual can ever in reality attain the ego ideal. In our attempts to realize our ego ideal, and thus to preserve our sense of self, we are assisted by ego-defense mechanisms. The suggestion here is that we can employ these predispositions, together with the notion of anxiety, to define the narcissistic personality for use in organization studies. Central to the theory of narcissism outlined here is the idea that individuals have a need to maintain a positive sense of self, and they engage in ego-defensive behavior in order to preserve self-esteem (Banaji & Prentice, 1994; Erez & Earley, 1993; Greenwald, 1980; Ring & Van de Ven, 1989). A "need" may be defined as "an internal state of disequilibrium or deficiency which has the capacity to trigger a behavioral response" (Steers & Porter, 1991: 97). "Self-esteem" is defined here "as the degree of correspondence between an individual's ideal and actual concepts of him[her]self" (Cohen, 1959: 103). Self-esteem, thus, is tied intimately to an individual's self-representation-to one's opinion of and respect for oneself. A separate motivation for self-knowledge also has been identified (Banaji & Prentice, 1994). Although the need for self-esteem largely is uncontested, it is important to note that there are considerable differences in the degree to which individuals exhibit self-esteem and that these can have important implications for behavior (Baumeister, Tice, & Hutton, 1989; Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1993; Baumgardner, Kaufman, & Cranford, 1990; Brockner, 1988; Brown, Collins, & Schmidt, 1988; Campbell, 1990; Rhodewalt, Morf, Hazlett, & Fairfield, 1991; Schlenker, Weigold, & Hallam, 1990). For example, high esteem is associated with a preference for ego defenses that repress, deny, or ignore challenges, whereas lowesteem individuals are more open to social influence (Brockner, 1988; Cohen, 1959; Hovland & Janis, 1959). Since 1914, the concept of narcissism has undergone "many changes in meaning" (Cooper, 1986: 112) and has "suffered terminologic slippage and overinclusiveness" to the extent that the term now lacks specificity (Goren, 1995: 339). Part of the problem is that Freud's original ideas on narcissism contained "contradictions, inconsistencies and gaps" (Teicholz, 1978: 833), allowing later authors to exploit the concept in pursuit of their own theorizing. As Pulver asserts, "In the voluminous literature on narcissism, there are probably only two facts on which everyone agrees: first that the concept of narcissism is one of the most important contributions of psychoanalysis; second, that it is one of the most confusing" (1970: 319). How then is the narcissistic personality to be defined? This is, as Lasch argues, a key issue not just because the idea is "readily susceptible to moralistic inflation" but because of the tendency to describe as narcissistic "everything selfish and disagreeable" (1978: 32). Based on Akhtar and Thompson (1982), the American Psychiatric Association (1980, 1986), Bromberg (1986), Emmons (1987), Fine (1986), Freud (1914, 1955), Frosh (1991), Gabriel, Critelli, and Ee (1994), Kernberg (1966), Kohut (1971), Lax (1975), Reich (1960), Rhodewalt and Morf (1995), Rothstein (1980), Shengold (1995),

646

Academy of Management

Review

July

Tobacyk and Mitchell (1987), and Westen (1990), among others, one can summarize the characteristics of the narcissistic personality in terms of six broad behavioral/psychological predispositions: (1) denial, (2) rationalization, (3) self-aggrandizement, (4) attributional egotism, (5) sense of entitlement, and (6) anxiety. Individuals engage in these behaviors unselfconsciously, rather than for the benefit of an intended audience (internal or external), in response to a deeply felt need to preserve selfesteem. This is, like all characterizations of the narcissistic personality, a contestable construct. In its defense it can be argued that it reflects significant areas of consensus in the literature and it lends itself to being operationalized in an organizational context. Denial has been described as "a primitive and desperate unconscious method of coping with otherwise intolerable conflict, anxiety, and emotional distress or pain," which can lead to increased confidence and feelings of invulnerability (Laughlin, 1970: 57). The narcissistic personality often is characterized by the denial of a difference between the ideal and actual self. Through denial, narcissistic individuals seek to disavow or to disclaim awareness, knowledge, or responsibility for faults that might otherwise attach to them (Gabriel et al., 1994; Lax, 1975; Rothstein, 1980; Shengold, 1995). Rationalization is an individual's attempt to justify or find reasons for unacceptable behavior or feelings and thus present them in a form consciously tolerable and acceptable. This mechanism involves a measure of self-deception, which is required in order to make what is consciously repugnant appear more creditable (Laughlin, 1970: 251). For the narcissistic personality, the resort to rationalization may involve the (unconscious) alteration of meanings of people, things, and events when self-esteem is threatened (Akhtar & Thompson, 1982; Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995). Self-aggrandizement refers to a general tendency of an individual to overestimate his or her abilities and accomplishments (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1986; Shengold, 1995; Tobacyk & Mitchell, 1987; Westen, 1990). Such overestimates often are described as fantasies, which are emotionally significant unconscious wishes for fulfillment or gratification (Laughlin, 1970: 110). In the narcissistic personality these fantasies may be accompanied by extreme self-absorption, a tendency toward exhibitionism, claims to uniqueness, and a sense of invulnerability. A further manifestation of this need for self-enhancement is an individual's tendency to distort reality through selective perception. For example, an individual might judge others on personally relevant criteria only, selectively seek out positive information about themselves, and selectively remember events that support their self-concept (Gecas, 1982; Markus & Wurf, 1987; Wood, 1989). Attributional egotism refers to the tendency of an individual to offer explanations for events that are "self-serving" or "hedonic" and that typically involve the attribution of favorable outcomes to causes internal to the self and unfavorable outcomes to external causes (Bettman & Weitz,

1997

Brown

647

1983; Bradley, 1978; Dunning & Cohen, 1992; Dunning, Meyerowitz, & Holzberg, 1989; Dunning, Perie, & Story, 1991; Greenwald, 1980; Hill, Smith, & Lewicki, 1989; Miller & Ross, 1975; Staw, 1980; Zuckerman, 1979). The idea that narcissists likely will display attributional egotism and thus make self-serving attributions to protect vulnerable self-esteem generally is acknowledged (Brown & Rogers, 1991; Emmons, 1987; Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995; Tennen & Herzberger, 1987; Westen, 1990). Kunda (1987) has suggested an alternative explanation of attributional egotism: it is the result of cognitive bias in the search for and mental processing of information, rather than ego defensiveness. Although it seems likely that both accounts of the causes of attributional egotism are valid, my emphasis in this article is on attributional egotism as it relates to the preservation of self-esteem. The narcissist's sense of entitlement often is associated with both a strong belief in his/her right to exploit others and an inability to empathize with the feelings of others (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1986; Lasch, 1978). Somewhat curiously, the narcissist's lack of interest and empathy for others is accompanied by an insatiable eagerness to obtain their admiration and approval (Reich, 1960). The narcissist, thus, is faced with the dilemma that she/he "holds in contempt and perhaps feels threatened by the very individuals upon whom he or she is dependent for positive regard and affirmation" (Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995: 18). Most of the other aspects of the narcissistic personality, such as denial, rationalization, attributional egotism, and especially self-aggrandizement, bolster this predisposition. Anxiety is not an ego-defense mechanism but what the ego-defense mechanisms are designed to ameliorate. The idea that narcissists suffer from feelings of dejection, worthlessness, and hypochondria (Reich, 1960); are despairing, empty, and fragile (Bromberg, 1986; Miller, 1986); and are hypersensitive and fraught with feelings of worthlessness (Akhtar & Thompson, 1982) is well documented (Rothstein, 1980). Lasch admirably captures this aspect of the narcissistic personality when writing that the narcissist "cannot live without an admiring audience. His apparent freedom from family ties and institutional constraints does not free him to stand alone or to glory in his individuality. On the contrary it contributes to his insecurity" (1978: 10). These traits define the narcissistic personality in general terms, but a fundamental distinction must be made between "normal" or "healthy" narcissism on the one hand and "pathological" narcissism on the other. It is important to note that "narcissism per se is a normal phenomenon" (Reich, 1960: 45) and "a universal and healthy attribute of personality" (Cooper, 1986: 115), which represents a "healthy concern with the self and with self-esteem regulation" (Frosh, 1991: 75). In short, "we all have some degree and variety of narcissistic delusion" (Shengold, 1995: 29; see also, Freud, 1914; Kohut, 1971; Rothstein, 1980). Normal or healthy narcissism implies, for example, that the ego is relatively stable, that the

648

Academy of Management Review

July

satisfactions gained from fantasies are not too great, and that the fantasies' content is not too divorced from reality (Fine, 1986). Indeed, some theories of narcissism suggest that narcissism is required for creativity, empathy with others, and the acceptance of death (Frosh, 1991: 96). Taken to one of two extremes, however, narcissism can constitute a pathological disorder that interferes with individuals' abilities to function adequately or to form meaningful relationships with others. Excessive self-esteem, for instance, implies ego instability and engagement in grandiose and impossible fantasies serving as substitutes for reality. Inadequate self-esteem, the other extreme, is likely to be associated with hypersensitivity, hypochondria, insecurity, and fear of death. Thus, narcissism ranges along a continuum, with the two extremes associated with pathological conditions. The dividing point between what constitutes healthy and what is pathological at either end of the spectrum requires a subjective evaluation. In this article I focus on narcissism in organizations as defined by the six general categories listed above, largely without attempting to arbitrarily sort the healthy from the pathological. However, I take up the healthy versus pathological debate again in the Discussion. For organization studies, the concept of narcissism opens up interesting avenues for research at the level of the individual, the group, the organization, and society. Perhaps least contentiously, narcissism can be employed as a psychoanalytic concept to explain the behavior of individuals in organizations. Although scholars of organization rarely have implicated the concept directly (exceptions include Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985, and Sankowsy, 1995), some scholars have paid considerable attention to attributes of the narcissistic personality and, more typically, the need for self-esteem (Bass, 1981; Bass & Coates, 1953; Feinberg, 1953; Gilchrist, 1952; Heynes, 1950; Lippit, Thelen, & Leff, undated; Sherif & Sherif, 1953; Sherif, White, & Harvey, 1955; Van Zelst, 1951; Whyte, 1943). My primary objective in this article is the application of the narcissism concept to groups and organizations and, specifically, the descriptive and explanatory advantages of extending the use of the concept in this way. Finally, narcissism can be employed at the level of macroculture to describe a persistent theme in human history symptomized by such phenomena as slavery and nationalism (Fine, 1986) or to describe the egocentrism, individualism, and superficiality of western capitalist society (Lasch, 1978, 1984; see also, Cooper, 1986; Gendlin, 1987; Richards, 1989). These applications of the narcissism concept at the societal level, though often unsystematic, are provocative, and they provide much of the inspiration for this article. IDENTITYAND LEVELSOF ANALYSIS Application of the narcissism concept to groups and organizations rarely has been attempted (Kets de Vries, 1996; Schwartz, 1987a,b,c,

1997

Brown

649

1990a,b). Part of the reason for this may be scholars' uncertainty about how to apply a psychoanalytic concept at the organization or group level without reification. The essential problem is to make meaningful notions of collective needs for self-esteem and collective efforts at self-esteem regulation. At an individual level the motivation to preserve and increase self-esteem is tied to the self-concept, but it is not immediately clear what might be meant by an organizational need for self-esteem. Scholars can use at least two strategies for dealing with this issue: (1) they can consider narcissism as a metaphor for understanding collective behavior, and (2) they can define organizations cognitively as existing in the social identities of their participants. The first possibility involves understanding the narcissistic personality described above as an image that can be employed as a metaphor for understanding organized behavior. A number of scholars have suggested that metaphors are a "basic structural form of experience through which human beings engage, organize and understand their world," creating meaning by helping us understand one phenomenon through another (Morgan, 1983: 601; see also Jakobson, 1962; Jakobson & Halle, 1956; Lacan, 1966; Morgan, 1980, 1986; Ortony, 1975; Palmer & Dunford, 1996). Indeed, the argument made here has, to an extent, been prefigured by Staw, who argues that "micro [psychological] theory can serve as a useful metaphor for organization-level theory" (1991: 811). Viewed from this perspective, groups and organizations are not literally narcissistic entities, but they behave in ways that are, in certain important respects, analogous to the behavior exhibited by narcissistic individuals. Understanding narcissism as a metaphor effectively absolves us from having to locate an actual organizational need for self-esteem. Just as we can argue that organizations have machine-like qualities without suggesting that they have all the properties of machines, or have organismic properties without implying that they are actually alive, so we can borrow some of the language associated with the theory of narcissism and selectively apply those concepts that aid us in gaining an understanding of our target domain (Tsoukas, 1991, 1993). Although the concept and theory of narcissism reasonably can be applied metaphorically, I do not, as a matter of fact, pursue this line of reasoning in this article. Instead, I argue that collective entities, in the form of groups and organizations, literally have needs for self-esteem that are regulated narcissistically. Based on the work of Tajfel and Turner and their colleagues (see Tajfel, 1972;Tajfel & Turner, 1986;Turner, 1985;Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987; Turner & Oakes, 1989), I argue here that organizations consist in the common social identification of participating individuals, that the phrase "organizational behavior" refers to the behavior of individuals acting as their organization, and that organizational self-esteem consists of the collective self-esteem of individuals acting as the organization. At the level of the individual, the self-concept may be conceived of as a cognitive structure consisting of a set of concepts subjectively

650

Academy of Management

Review

July

available to a person in attempting to define him/herself (Gecas, 1982; Gergen, 1971; Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Knorr-Cetina, 1981; Markus & Wurf, 1987; Schlenker, 1985; Weick, 1995). This structure consists of two subcomponents: (1) personal identity, which refers to person-specific attributes, and (2) social identity, which refers to social group memberships. Organizations and their component subgroups play an important role in helping to confer social identity by offering images (concepts) with which their participants may identify (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Douglas, 1987; Dutton, Dukerich, & Harquail, 1994; Kramer, 1991). Identification is a key concept here, for it refers to a perception of oneness with or belongingness to a social category. The "strength of a member's organizational identification reflects the degree to which the content of the member's self-concept is tied to his or her organizational membership" (Dutton et al., 1994: 242; see also Gonzalez, 1996, and Katz & Kahn, 1966: 345-346). Furthermore, different elements of the self-concept (called self-images) dominate cognition and behavior at different times, with the saliency of a given self-image being a function of relative accessibility and fit (Turner, 1987). The important implications of this theory are twofold. First, organizations and their subgroups are social categories and, in psychological terms, exist in the participants' common awareness of their membership. In an important sense, therefore, organizations exist in the minds of their members, organizational identities are parts of their individual members' identities, and organizational needs and behaviors are the collective needs and behaviors of their members acting under the influence of their organizational self-images. This argument may be regarded as a clarification of Chatman, Bell, and Staw's contention that the identity of organizations is manifested in the actions of individuals who act "as the organization" when they embody the values, beliefs, and goals of the collectivity (1986: 211; see also Wegner, 1987, and Weick & Roberts, 1993). Second, we can now clearly distinguish between individual and collective levels of self-esteem. An individual who is motivated to preserve and enhance his/her self-image based entirely or primarily on personal identity is operating as a unique person. In this instance the need for selfesteem is unambiguously located at the level of the individual. An individual who is motivated to preserve and enhance his/her self-image based entirely or primarily on social identity is operating as a representative of a social category. Here, the individual is, in a sense, behaving as that social category, and his/her individual need for self-esteem is also identifiable as the social category's need for self-esteem. When all or most members of a social category are motivated to preserve and enhance their self-images based entirely or primarily on that social category, then the sum of their individual needs for self-esteem represent the collective need for self-esteem of that social category. Thus, there is a "continuous reciprocal interaction and functional interdependence between the psychological processes of individuals and their activity, relations and products" (Turner, 1987: 205-206) as groups and organizations. Individuals and

1997

Brown

651

the social categories they participate in are "mutual preconditions," and "simultaneously emergent properties" of each other (Turner, 1987: 205-206).

IN ORGANIZATIONS NARCISSISM
If it is true that groups and organizations have collective needs for self-esteem that are regulated narcissistically, it should be possible to discern the ego-defensive behaviors noted above in the extant organization studies literature. The importance of self-esteem in an organizational context was popularized initially by Maslow (1954), who tackled the concept explicitly, and by Festinger (1954), in whose theory of social comparison processes it is implicit (see also Heynes, 1950, and Van Zelst, 1951). For very many authors, self-esteem is an important theme or organizing principle structuring organizational life. Blau (1955), for example, describes how individuals who are socially integrated, secure, and derive satisfaction from their work tend to exert greater effort. In the work of Zaleznik (1966), participants struggle to maintain a sense of self in a context where their esteem is constantly under threat from status and competition anxiety, as well as fear of authority and uncertainty. For Katz and Kahn, the reward for participation in organizations is the establishment of self-identity and the expression of "values appropriate to this self-concept" (1966: 346). Terkel describes his book about work as "a search ... for daily meaning" in which people attempt to "maintain a sense of self" (1972: xiii, xviii). For Berger, "the socially established nomos" is "a shield against terror" (1973: 31-32). In Barley's (1986) study of CAT scanners, the importance of self-esteem maintenance is suggested by his description of the actions taken by individuals to manage their anxieties. More recently, Watson (1994) has argued that managers are perennially seeking to maintain their concept of self and to maintain their often fragile self-esteem (see also Baumeister, 1986; Brockner, 1988; Douglas, 1987; Harvey, Weber, & Orbuch, 1990; Lasch, 1978). However, recognition of the importance of self-esteem in organization studies has not led to a focus on collective ego-defensive behaviors. These behaviors are, though, detectable as implicit themes in the literature (see Table 1 for a summary).

Denial
Denial is an unconsciously deployed means of coping with what would otherwise be intolerable conflict, anxiety, and emotional distress (Laughlin, 1970: 57). The denial theme figures prominently in accounts of self-presentation (Abrahamson & Park, 1994; Elsbach, 1994; Niskala & Pretes, 1995; Wiseman, 1982) and in myth making (Alvesson & Berg, 1992; Boje, Fedor, & Rowland, 1982; Filby & Willmott, 1988; Gabriel, 1991; Morgan, 1986; Pettigrew, 1979; Schwartz, 1987b). These pieces are dominated by agency theories, assuming intentionality on the part of individuals, who are depicted as engaging in denial in their attempts to maximize

652

Academy of Management

Review

July

4.1
:s

06 I

4.1
<

0
U
0

a) 0 co a) $: M tl4 0.6 O
-4 00 6

Cq
4

:s

06I 0 CZ 0 0 :s rn rn
co

>
-

tlCO a)

U M lot
#-.4

0
U

O W

U
> 0

co

4.1 0 0 rM -0
0

4.1 U
016
0 U M ., co

4.1
0
-I....4

U 0

co
co

U)
X

U
0

--4

Uj 0
>4 co

--4 x
O

0
U -4 1-1

a)
)..4 0

0 :s I.= 0
N C-

CO U -g
0

0 Cn E-4
U

W W a)
Cj

U") (n

O
0 :IZ >

-I--,
--4

0
4.1

-1- -4 U
--4

a)
(n

a)
0 0

U)
O 4.1 N

O'd a) Z
U)

01
0 0

C,

co CT)

0
0

O ."
0

O
0

4.1 00
C-,)

rn

0)

0,

O
-O-,

>
(>I,) 0

>,

CD

CT)

0)

UO

_-z
Ui

CT) 00
0 --4 --4 CT) 0

(n
-I..,

4.1

CT

>4

-4 Cn
_4

--4

(n
U

Uj
--4 0 --4 -

-4
0

En
En 0
.P.4

4.1 4.1 0
U -4 0)

>
4.1 -4

>
U

00 a)
00 4.1

(n OC) Cr) (1)


00

-4

93
-#4

N
:s 0 CT)

'a)
4.1

(n
4.1 0

(n

(n
0 U 0 -4

Mx U O 0
0

00

__Z Cq CT)

>
tm

(1) U ja, Cn 0,
Cq

0
(n 0
-4-

1-4

O O

C,)

$: .- .- 5 O

>

C --4 r. 4
>4

(1) (n

---4

N 0Z --4 Q 4.1
0

U Cr)

LO

U CT)
14-1 0 -"a) --4 :s

LC) Cr)

(n
4 O

C:6

--4
0 --4 --4

(n
U

LO

(n
(1) Q

.(n
:s V

O
-Q 00

(n
0 CT)
--4

--4

>4 4.1

0
U

0 CT) U LO

4.1

8)) 4.1 4.1 O 0


-4 U --4

>
Cr)

U 00

--4 00) (n

CT)

U
0 U

U 00
C" CT)

CT)

Cq U
CT) --4

0
U
U)

-.4
U)

U
U 0 -4 --4 O

00

-4-1
U 0 0

0 0

0)
0 0)

(n
--4 0 0 U 0

(n
(1)

CD 00
Cr) CT)

CD

(n

(n (n

(n CT) CT)
-.1 -4

LO

(n

Lr)
CT)

CT) (n
Ci U
0 >

00 CT)

U (1)

0
--4

U 0
(n

00
--4 CT)
4.1

00 CT)

C-,

06
0

0
0

--4

..4 > ...4

--4 > --4

U)

CT)

CT)

0 O (n

Cr)

0
4 U

En En En Cn

-4

0)
O --4 0 --4

0)
U)

1997

Brown

653

a)

O _

:,a)
0

U-

>

-: _ r~~~~~~~.10

o" =~~~~~~~~~'

r, o>

- u U o.o@X=o

0 -'A

LO

0
u
Cn

> a)

a)

Q On

a)3 00
>
U: --

::S
E

E , c.
_oO
COCT

a) |~
.
.

u V~~~~~~~~~~::

U ;

d4

4-

-I.a., o

- 6 , .>

C- v'
CD

; X

O O) 0-_

CT)

U)C

OC

4cr

<

<0 U~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~c) o

654

Academy of Management

Review

July

economic gain (Dutton & Dukerich, 1991; Elsbach, 1994). Although some of this activity almost certainly is conceived deliberately, the suggestion here is that participants also conceal/deny disagreeable truths from themselves as well as others in an unconscious and self-deceptive attempt to preserve self-esteem (Staw et al., 1983). At an individual level, the tendency of organizational leaders to deny the reality of market demands and resource constraints has received some attention (Conger, 1990; Sankowsky, 1995; Schwartz, 1987b). Goffman (1959) has identified organization members who self-deceptively hide or deny facts about themselves from themselves. The idea that individuals, acting as representatives of their organizations, deny fault and responsibility for problems also has been embraced explicitly by impression management researchers. The literature on self-presentation suggests that participants' have a sense of their organization's identity that they attempt to maintain, therefore influencing their choice of action (Dutton & Dukerich, 1991; Weick, 1988: 306); this literature also contends that participants are acutely sensitive to their organizations' external reputations, which they seek to preserve and enhance by means of the information they disclose (Caves & Porter, 1977; Dutton & Jackson, 1987; Fombrun & Shanley, 1990; Fombrun & Zajac, 1987). In performing these activities, these people, as shown in abundant evidence, provide prejudiced explanations for occurrences (denying, concealing, and omitting) that attenuate personal and organizational responsibility for controversial events (Leary & Kowalski, 1990; Pfeffer, 1981; Schlenker, 1980; Schonbach, 1980). Consider, for example, Elsbach's (1994) study of the California cattle industry, which uncovered many instances of denial (in the form "we weren't involved" and "it didn't happen") engaged in by spokespersons in their efforts to separate their organizations from controversy. In their attempts to deny fault, corporate officers sometimes also will conceal negative organizational outcomes (Abrahamson & Park, 1994). A variety of case study material suggests that people suppress information (Ashforth & Gibbs, 1990: 180), hide negative financial data (Whetton, 1980: 162), conceal failure (Sutton & Callahan, 1987), and even "launch propaganda campaigns that deny the existence of crises" (Starbuck, Greve, & Hedberg, 1978: 118). This phenomenon has been documented at GM, where, despite considerable evidence of failings, executives refused to acknowledge "the truth about their corporate parent. They didn't want to believe" (Keller, 1989: 65, 66; see also Schwartz, 1990a,b). At the group level, denial has been elaborated by Janis (1972) under the label of "groupthink." The literature on myth making casts further light on the denial behavior of individuals, as well as groups and organizations (Alvesson & Berg, 1992; Boje et al., 1982; Brown, 1994; Filby & Willmott, 1988; Pettigrew, 1979; Schwartz, 1987a,b). Participants engage in myth making while attempting to make sense of things and events, and this "sensemaking occurs in the service of maintaining a consistent, positive self-conception" (Weick, 1995: 23; see also Steele, 1988). Myths help

1997

Brown

655

define "truth" and "rationality," while "glossing over excessive complexity, turbulence, or ambiguity" (Boje et al., 1982); they function as "patterning devices" that cohere and give order to participants' understandings (Alvesson & Berg, 1992); and they establish what ideas and behaviors are unacceptable and what is legitimate (Pettigrew, 1979: 576; see also Abravanel, 1983; Barthes, 1972; Burrell & Morgan, 1979; Cohen, 1969; Trice & Beyer, 1985). In doing so, myths sometimes overtly deny that something is the case; often conceal conflicting or contradictory information; and, because of their exclusivity, always omit other equally valid interpretations and evaluations. Myths, then, are a vehicle for denying errors and responsibilities when self-esteem is threatened (Gabriel, 1991; Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985: 138). When shared, myths serve as collective wish fulfillments that cannot be negated by contrary evidence (Zaleznik, 1989), for they function as self-deceptions that assist in individuals' and groups' avoidance of reality (Gabriel, 1991; Schwartz, 1985). An example of this is Schwartz's (1987b) analysis of the Space Shuttle Challenger disaster, which portrays NASA as an organization whose participants denied there was a difference between their ideal organization and the actual NASA organization. This, in turn, led them to fantasize that, as members of NASA, they were incapable of failure-with catastrophic consequences. Perhaps the most thoroughly researched medium through which an organization denies its problems and responsibilities is the organization's annual report and its associated letters to shareholders (Bell, 1984; Graves, Flesher, & Jordan, 1996; Hager & Scheiber, 1990; Ingram & Frazier, 1983; Judd & Tims, 1991; Preston, Wright, & Young, 1996). These "kaleidoscopic, glamorous, and entertaining" documents (Graves et al., 1996: 59) seek to present memorable "facts," emphasizing the image the organization wishes to portray by means of sophisticated visual and textual strategies, while at the same time denying, omitting, and concealing information incompatible with such rhetoric (Bolton, 1989; Ewen, 1988;Lewis, 1984). The most blatant examples of denial by omission occur in the form of environmental disclosures, which are often incomplete and not related to the firm's actual environmental performance (Niskala & Pretes, 1995; Wiseman, 1982). Rationalization A rationalization is an attempt to develop plausible and acceptable justifications for motives or actions (which participants may feel are not credit worthy) in an attempt to maintain sense of self (Laughlin, 1970: 251). At the level of the individual in organizations, Zaleznik (1966: 31) comments on how executives immobilized by difficult problems tend to rationalize their inaction, using arguments ranging from their having inadequate authority to their subordinates providing too little, or confused, information. The idea that organization participants offer rationalizations for their past actions is implicit in Weick's (1979: 1995) concept of

656

Academy of Management

Review

July

retrospective sense making, which suggests that individuals offer explanations for their activities that preserve their self-esteem. Rationalization is also central to our understanding of how individuals redefine their past actions using arguments leading to their escalation of commitment (Staw, 1980). Pfeffer (1981) makes explicit use of the notion to refer to the tendency of organizational leaders to offer justifications for policies that fit the social context in which they are offered. For Pfeffer, rationalizations are linguistic devices that secure legitimacy for actions and that obfuscate inequitable resource distributions in ways that are both deceptive and self-deceptive. The concept has been dealt with most adequately by Janis (1989), who argues that policy makers are often more inclined to satisfy their own personal motives and emotional needs than the requirements of their organizations. The result is that decisions are made for egocentric reasons, which then must be justified/rationalized (often un-self-consciously) by means of "impressive-sounding reasons," which present "the new policy in terms of benefits to the organization" (Janis, 1989: 65). The significance of rationalization is suggested, for example, in Auletta's (1985a,b, 1986) accounts of the fall of the Lehman brothers and power and greed on Wall Street. These include a detailed description of Lewis L. Gluckman's ousting of his co chief executive officer, Peter G. Peterson, for self-serving motives that needed to be rationalized in order to be found acceptable both by Gluckman and the business community. Some scholars have recognized that rationalization can also be a feature of group behavior (Filby & Willmott, 1988; Janis, 1972; Laughlin, 1970; Pfeffer, 1981; Salancik & Pfeffer, 1978). As Laughlin (1970: 627, 628) has made clear, social or collective rationalization involving the large scale, and shared development of rationalizations for motives and behavior, are features of national and geographical groups, classes of people, political parties, and large organizations. Pfeffer (1981) writes of "management" as a collective group offering rationalizations for structures and behaviors in order to maintain and enhance organizational legitimacy. For Janis (1972), rationalization is a psychological defense mechanism associated with the phenomenon of groupthink. In Filby and Willmott's (1988) analysis of a public relations department, the public relations specialists are depicted as rationalizing their low status through myth and humor. This case study is particularly interesting, for it illustrates that although rationalization can help a group manage its sense of self-esteem, it can do so in ways that are self-defeating. At the organization level, the pervasiveness of rationalization is hinted at by Douglas in her description of how institutions "create shadowed places in which nothing can be seen and no questions asked," whereas "other areas show finely discriminated detail" (1987: 69). Her argument is that the social order of institutions establishes "selective principles" (rationalizations) that highlight and obscure events, control memory, provide categories for thought, set the terms for self-knowledge, and fix identities. This phenomenon has been commented on directly by

1997

Brown

657

Starbuck (1983), who notes that organizations act unreflectively and nonadaptively most of the time. However, because of strong societal demands for rationality, organizations retrospectively seek out and label events as problems, threats, and opportunities, and they characterize their actions as logical responses to these needs. To support their accounts, organizations develop strong ideologies for rational problem-solving behavior, and these ideologies often are supported by ceremonies and selection devices. Starbuck's conclusion is that a large portion of what seems to be rational behavior actually consists of actions that are post hoc justified (rationalized) to the organization's employees and to managers themselves. Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter's (1956) case study of a sect that had predicted the end of the world at a specific time is an excellent example of organizational rationalization. As the time specified approached, the sect announced that the world had been spared as a result of its activities and that the sect had been charged with the responsibility of preaching this good news. Rather than confront the possibility of contradictory evidence, the sect offered a rationalization for its beliefs and behavior that preserved intact its self-concept and self-esteem. More recently, Dutton and Dukerich have shown how the New York Port Authority rationalized its initial decision not to respond to the issue of homelessness on the grounds that it "lacked the social service skills necessary" (1991: 545). Interestingly, the decision to treat homelessness as a moral rather than a business issue later was reversed; drop-in centers, which became "a source of pride" for employees, were opened; and the earlier line of reasoning was overtly exposed as a defensive rationalization. In Ross and Staw's (1993) account of the construction of the Shoreham Nuclear Power Plant, the authors illustrate how the Long Island Lighting Company employed defensive rationalizations for its activities that were so inaccurate the company was indicted for fraud. Perhaps the best known example of organizational rationalization is GM's steadfast defense of the Corvair-despite the evidence of deaths, injuries, legal cases, and the company's own engineering records (Nader & Taylor, 1986; Wright, 1979). Self-Aggrandizement Self-aggrandizement refers to the tendency of an individual to overestimate his or her merits and accomplishments, which in the fantasy life of the narcissist is often accompanied by extreme self-absorption, exhibitionism, claims to uniqueness, and a sense of invulnerability (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1986). As explanations for the behavior of individuals in organizations, these traits have been commented on widely (Janis, 1989; Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985; Lasch, 1978; Watson, 1994; Zaleznik, 1966). Indeed, the ideas that leaders constantly engage in fantasies of omnipotence and control (Gabriel, 1991; Lasch, 1978; Zaleznik, 1966), often fall prey to grandiosity and exhibitionism (Kets de Vries &

658

Academy of Management

Review

July

Miller, 1985), and sometimes even seek "to create a culture cast in their own image, to perpetuate their own personal values and achieve an organizational form of immortality" (Martin, 1992: 8) are now well established. In its mildest form the enhancement of self-esteem through fantasies may be beneficial to the organization, as shown, for example, in Watson's description of those managers who spoke of "loving the people here" and enjoying the "buzz" of being in a position to direct events (1994: 70). Relatively mild self-aggrandizing behavior also is manifested by individuals in their narration of stories and myths-activities that are, per se, exhibitionistic and ego enhancing (Harvey et al., 1990: 13). In addition, the content of the stories and myths narrated provides additional opportunities for individuals to reaffirm their social identity, to illustrate their knowledge and prowess, to exert influence, and to offer explanations for events that flatter them and enhance self-esteem (Boje et al., 1982; Gabriel, 1991). More acute forms of self-aggrandizement are described in Janis's (1989) analysis of egocentric executives and in Lamb's (1987) account of "nonsensical acquisitions" in the 1980s, which he explains in terms of chief executives' needs for ego enhancement-especially their desire for excitement, power, and prestige (see also Conger, 1990, and Miller & Droge, 1986). Rituals also provide opportunities for individuals to build positive self-esteem. Consider, for example, the practice at General Motors Corporation of meeting visiting senior staff at the airport with a retinue proportional to their status-a ritual directly bolstering those individuals' self-esteem by fulfilling their needs for attention and allowing them to experience attitudes of superiority (Wright, 1979). The importance of self-aggrandizement as a motive for individual behavior is suggested as well in biographies and autobiographies of successful leaders. Sculley's (1987) immodest account of his exploits at Pepsi and Apple, and Royko's (1971) characterization of Mayor Daley of Chicago, provide interesting insights in this respect. Consider, for example, Royko's comment about the Mayor's retirement: "He'll settle for something simple, like maybe another jet airport built on a man-made island in the lake, and named after him, and maybe a statue outside the Civic Center, with a simple inscription, 'The greatest mayor in the history of the world.' And they might seal his office as a shrine" (1971: 17). Self-aggrandizement as a feature of group behavior also has been commented upon widely (Blau, 1955; Filby & Willmott, 1988; Janis, 1972; Schwartz, 1987c; Sculley, 1987). The tendency of groups to use myth and humor to exaggerate their sense of self-worth (Filby & Willmott, 1988), to fantasize of unlimited ability during times of stress (Janis, 1972), to engage in social cohesion ceremonies that are overtly exhibitionistic (Trice & Beyer, 1985, 1993), and to develop special languages and symbols that emphasize uniqueness (Blau, 1955) all have been researched thoroughly. These same themes are yet more evident in the literature on organizations. When Kluckhon suggests that culture is a "giant effort to mask life's

1997

Brown

659

fundamental insecurities" (1942: 66), and when Ferguson writes of conventions and institutions remaining "the objects of passionate adoration long after they have outlived their usefulness" (1936: 29), both are touching on aspects of self-aggrandizement in the social process of organizing. Perhaps the clearest statement of this in the organization literature is Douglas's contention: Institutions systematically direct individual memory and channel our perceptions into forms compatible with the relations they authorize. They fix processes that are essentially dynamic, they hide their influence, and they rouse their emotions to a standardized pitch on standardized issues. Add to all this that they endow themselves with rightness and send their mutual corroborationcascading through all the levels of our information system. No wonder they easily recruit us into joining their narcissistic self-contemplation. (1987:92) The literature on organizational uniqueness and organizational architecture provides particularly graphic examples of organizational aggrandizement. Organizations make claims to uniqueness through, among other things, stories (Martin et al., 1983; Selznick, 1957: 151), ceremonies (Ash, 1981; Rosen, 1985), rituals (Evered, 1983), myths (Schwartz, 1987a), the speeches of senior executives (Piccardo, Varchetta, & Zanarini (1990), annual reports (Preston et al., 1996; Salancik & Meindl, 1984), corporate logos (Hampden-Turner, 1990), and corporate histories (Rowlinson, 1987; Rowlinson & Hassard, 1994). These claims to uniqueness often emphasize the prowess and accomplishments of the organization in ways that are palpably exhibitionistic and exaggerated. There are many examples. For instance, at AT&T employees like to narrate the story of how they dealt with a major fire, which exaggerates their ability to cope with disasters (Kleinfield, 1981: 307). The visit of an admiral to a U.S. Navy ship is associated with an elaborate ritual, suggestive of exhibitionism and intense self-absorption (Evered, 1983: 134). In the United Kingdom, companies such as Cadbury and Fry deliberately have sought to carve out unique identities based on the religious ideals of their founders by commissioning corporate histories emphasizing that they are "special" and "imbued with a morality lacking in other firms" (Rowlinson, 1987: 86). The annual reports of many organizations tend to stress their individuality and achievements, often using color photographs and re-presenting information to send the "right message" (Bolton, 1989; Ewen, 1988; Howard, 1991; Lewis, 1984; Pettit, 1990; Preston et al., 1996; Salancik & Meindl, 1984). Self-aggrandizement is apparent as well in the architecture and office layouts that organizations deploy (Berg & Kreiner, 1990). The importance of corporate architecture and office design as an influence on behavior, an expression of corporate identity, and a symbol of status has been commented upon widely (Davis, 1984; Grafton-Small, 1985; Holm-Lofgren, 1980; Schwantzer, 1986; Seiler, 1984). It is apparent that organizations

660

Academy of Management

Review

July

spend vast amounts of resources on stylish buildings, office layouts, and landscaped gardens in an attempt to express their uniqueness and, inadvertently, their vanity. These intentional manipulations of physical space are palpable claims to superiority, often designed to communicate a sense of invulnerability and to excite admiration. One of the most vivid accounts of the narcissistic displays engaged in by organizations and their impact on self-esteem has been provided by Sculley's description of the PepsiCo boardroom: Like nearly everything else at PepsiCo Inc., the room's stately power and elegance made you stand a little straighter. A large abstract painting by Jackson Pollock's widow, Lee Krasner, dominated the far wall. Custom-designed carpeting in earthtone colors cushioned the floor. The bronze-plated ceiling, perhaps more appropriate for a church, reflected burnished mahogany-paneled walls. High-backed beige leather chairs, so imposing they could have carried corporate titles of their own, surrounded the boardroom table. (1987:1) Attributional Egotism Attributional egotism refers to the phenomenon wherein agents offer self-serving explanations for events, attributing favorable outcomes to their own efforts and unfavorable outcomes to external factors (Bradley, 1978; Miller & Ross, 1975; Schlenker, 1980; Snyder, Stephan, & Rosenfield, 1978; Staw, 1980; Zuckerman, 1979). As an explanation for the behavior of individuals in organizations, the concept of attributional egotism has attracted considerable attention (Abrahamson & Park, 1994; Erez & Earley, 1993; Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985; Martin et al., 1983; Ross and Staw, 1993; Schwartz, 1987a; Weiner et al., 1971). Zaleznik suggests that there is a tendency for executives "to blame external authority for personal plight and deprivation" (1966: 143). Abrahamson and Park (1994) suggest it as one possible explanation for corporate concealment. Schwartz (1987a) suggests a role for this form of self-enhancement bias in his discussion of the psychodynamics of totalitarianism. For Martin et al. (1983: 449), organizational stories provide a vehicle for self-enhancing explanations, which preserve individual reputations and self-esteem. One particularly detailed account of the importance of attributional egotism in organizations is contained in Ross and Staw's (1993) analysis of the Long Island Lighting Company's decision to build and operate the Shoreham Nuclear Power Plant. Their case study suggests "that decision makers saw the failing decision not so much a product of their own faulty calculus or lack of management as the result of intervention by external regulators and 'antisocial elements'" (Ross & Staw, 1993: 717). The attributional egotism concept also has been used widely to explain the behavior of collectivities, most usually through analyses of annual reports (Bettman & Weitz, 1983; Bowman, 1976; Ingram & Frazier, 1983; Salancik & Meindl, 1984; Staw et al., 1983). As long ago as 1976, Bowman found that the annual reports of food-processing companies for underper-

1997

Brown

661

forming organizations tended to blame unfavorable conditions and government regulations for their problems. Ingram and Frazier's research on the annual reports of organizations in the metals and chemicals industries concluded that "management attributes good performance to itself and poor performance to external factors" (1983: 59). The three largest scale surveys of this type (Bettman & Weitz, 1983; Salancik & Meindl, 1984; Staw et al., 1983) also reach similar conclusions. Although scholars agree that this phenomenon at the collective level exists, some contest the egodefensive explanation. For example, Miller and Ross (1975) argue that self-serving attributions are due to informational processing effects (Erez & Earley, 1993; Kunda, 1987). In this article I follow recent research, which suggests that individuals operating in groups and organizations are and selfprone to egotistic attribution for both information-processing concept-preservation reasons (Bettman & Weitz, 1983; Riess, Rosenfield, Melburg, & Tedeschi, 1981; Staw et al., 1983). I note, however, that central to the argument pursued here is the opportunity that attributional egotism offers for defending and enhancing self-esteem. Sense of Entitlement The narcissist's sense of entitlement (to success, power, and acclaim, for instance) often is accompanied by a tendency toward exploitiveness and a lack of empathy with the feelings of others, which manifests itself in interpersonal relationships that overtly lack depth (Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995; 1, 2). Various studies of the behavior of individuals in organizations suggest that these traits can be commonly observed (Jennings, 1971; Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985; Lasch, 1978; Macoby, 1976; Sankowsky, 1995). Indeed, there is some research to suggest that a sense of entitlement expressed in the form of self-confidence makes people more persuasive and thus better able to fulfill a leadership role (Kipnis & Lane, 1962; Mowday, 1979). According to Lasch, the prevalence of these behaviors is stimulated by "bureaucratic institutions, which put a premium on the manipulation of interpersonal relations, [and] discourage the formation of deep personal attachments" (1978: 44). In a similar vein, Macoby (1976: 107) suggests that the corporate "gamesman" has little capacity for personal intimacy and commitment and minimal loyalty, even to the company for which he/she works. In Kets de Vries and Miller's analysis of different types of narcissistic leader, a category of individuals is identified who "live under the illusion that they are entitled to be served" and who care "little about hurting and exploiting others in the pursuit of [their] own advancement" (1985: 588, 596). One example of this phenomenon is Abrahamson and Park's (1994) finding that, when circumstances permit, corporate officers express their sense of entitlement (in this case to financial rewards) by favoring their own interests over those of shareholders. Perhaps the best exemplifications of individuals' sense of entitlement and lack of empathy in an organization setting are contained in the biographies and autobiographies of those organizational leaders who have

662

Academy of Management

Review

July

been deemed or consider themselves successful (Harvey-Jones, 1988; Royko, 1971; Sculley, 1987). These themes are addressed explicitly more rarely at either the group or organizational level, although evidence for their pervasiveness is to be found across a wide range of literature. For example, in his study of a federal enforcement agency, Blau (1955: 167) describes a social cohesion ceremony in which one group, "Agents," sang a song that expressed a sense of entitlement to promotion. In Blau's (1955: 110, 111) other case study of a state employment agency, he illustrates how a group used humor, mostly involving the ridiculing of clients' accents, names, and clothes, in ways that evidenced an inability to empathize with their feelings. Rosen's (1985) analysis of an annual breakfast at the Spiro advertising agency provides further insight into exploitiveness at the level of the collectivity. Here, Rosen describes senior executives' speeches that portrayed the agency as providing generous rewards for its employees at a time when base salaries were lower than those paid by comparable agencies in the area and when salary increases were being delayed deliberately. The ideas that modern bureaucratic organizations are structured according to a principle of entitlement to exploit and that many shareholders and their professional managers adopt exploitative attitudes toward their workforce and the external environment are well explored (Callahan, 1962; Lasch, 1978; Marglin, 1974a,b). The connection between industrial capitalism and ruthless exploitation has been expressed famously in Frederick Taylor's phrase that the working man is no more than "a man of the type of an ox" (cited in Callahan, 1962). Lasch argues that modern capitalist production arose not for efficiency reasons but "because it provided capitalists with greater profits and power" (1978: 62). Marglin (1974a,b) suggests that the case for the factory system rested not on its technological superiority over handicraft production but on the more effective control of the labor force it allowed the employer. In Meyer and Rowan's (1977) analysis, organizations are depicted as assuming a sense of their own entitlement to continued successful existence, attempting to mold their institutional environments, artificially creating needs for their products, and seeking official protection. The potential consequences of these exploitative attitudes for employees also has received some attention. Beynon, for example, describes the Ford assembly line as "regulated tedium," and even "bondage," and the workers as "condemned" to carry out their work, which itself "traps," "brutalizes," and engenders "a terrible sense of isolation" (1973, 1984; see also Terkel, 1972). Anxiety The narcissistic personality is characterized by a sense of anxiety, which stems from a dependence on others to validate self-esteem. Analyses of the behavior of individuals in organizations strongly suggest that anxiety is a major feature of participants' lives (Berger, 1973; Kets de Vries, 1980; Kets de Vries & Miller, 1985; Schweiger & Denisi, 1991; Watson, 1994;

1997

Brown

663

Weick, 1995; Zaleznik, 1966). Many commentators have described the "internal suffering" of executives (Lasch, 1978: 44) and their need for stability and certainty to stave off anxiety (Varela, Thompson, & Rosch, 1991; Weick, 1995). Kets de Vries and Miller have identified those organizational leaders who experience "a sense of deprivation and emptiness" and "show a hypersensitivity to criticism, extreme insecurity, and a strong need to be loved" (1985: 588, 597). Zaleznik paints a picture of executives paralyzed by fear, whose esteem is constantly under threat, and who resort to fantasy and ritual in order to detach themselves "from personal anxiety and tension" (1966: 136). In his ethnography of a U.K. manufacturing company, Watson (1994) describes managers who are desperately attempting to maintain their fragile self-esteem in a difficult and chaotic world. A wealth of other studies link the prevalence of anxiety to dysfunctional outcomes, such as stress, job dissatisfaction, low trust and commitment, and intentions to leave the organization (Ashford, Lee, & Bobko, 1989; Bastien, 1987; Buono, Bowditch, & Lewis, 1985; Gil & Foulder, 1978; Marks & Mirvis, 1983; Schweiger & Denisi, 1991; Shirley, 1973; Sinetar, 1981). The salience of group anxiety in organizations has been illustrated by Menzies' (1970) research on nurses and Miller and Gwynnes' (1972) study of the social defense mechanisms at work in residential institutions. Menzies' (1970) study shows that nurses are subject to extremely high levels of tension, distress, and anxiety, which they seek to overcome through, for example, ritual task performance and denial of feelings of attachment. Miller and Gwynne's (1972) research suggests that residential institutions for the physically handicapped and young chronically sick are massively anxiety provoking for both staff and patients. These themes also figure prominently in the literature on organizations. Jacques (1955), for example, has contended that within the life of an organization, the defense against anxiety is one of the primary elements binding the individuals together (see also De Board, 1978). Anxiety and fear of loss of self-esteem are embraced either implicitly or explicitly by the Durkheimian concept of anomie, Marx's (1964) notion of alienation, and Gabriel's (1991) description of organizational myths as collective wish-fulfillment mechanisms. Durkheim's influence on theorists of organization has been especially important (Starkey, 1992). For Durkheim, "anomie" refers to a collective loss of self-esteem, which can "be remedied by providing the individual with a moral awareness of the social importance of his particular role" (Giddens, 1971: 230). Durkheim's (1912) emphasis on the "pathological" (anomic) state of organizations that results from the division of labor has been "reconceptualized as a psychological syndrome produced by the imbalance between technical and social needs within the organization of modern industry" by writers in the human relations tradition (Reed, 1985: 16). These theorists have responded to a perceived need for security and self-esteem by arguing, for example, for the creation of a shared moral order (Hill, 1981: 90), a sense of common purpose and cooperative relationships (Barnard, 1938), leadership attempts to secure

664

Academy of Management

Review

July

commitment (Selznick, 1957), and the broader distribution of work responsibilities (Walton, 1985). To summarize, participants in groups and organizations act individually, to preserve and enhance their personal identities, and collectively, to maintain their social identities. These behaviors involve the narcissistic regulation of self-esteem, and they take the form of the ego defensesdenial, rationalization, attributional egotism, ego aggrandizement, and sense of entitlement-which stave off anxiety. A largely implicit theme in the above review is that some groups and organizations will be more successful than others in regulating the self-esteem of their participants. This idea is central to our consideration of legitimacy. NARCISSISM AND LEGITIMACY The idea that the self-esteem of individuals is regulated partly through their participation in groups and organizations throws new light on the dynamics by which collectivities gain and maintain internal legitimacy. Because participants are involved in a reciprocal identity relationship with the groups and organizations in which they participate, they exhibit a normative acceptance of the rightness of these entities to exist. In other words, organizations offer individuals and groups the opportunity to share in the means by which their self-esteem may be continuously recreated and sustained in ways that make it motivationally compelling for them to accept their organization as desirable, proper, and appropriate-that is, as legitimate. This understanding of legitimacy is not at variance with conventional accounts of why legitimacy attributions are made, although it does suggest that such explanations are incomplete. Traditional accounts of the reasons why members attribute legitimacy to their organizations tend to suggest that these reasons are: (1) based on rational calculations of self-interest (Dowling & Pfeffer, 1975; Emerson, 1962; Pfeffer & Salancik, 1978; Wood, 1991); (2) a detection of congruence between the members' notions of what is right and good and the consequences, procedures, structures, and personnel associated with the organization (Aldrich & Fiol, 1994; Parsons, 1960; Scott, 1977; Scott & Meyer, 1991); and (3) because the organization offers explanations and models that allow participants to reduce anxiety and provide meaningful explanations for their experiences (Scott, 1991; Suchman, 1995; Wuthnow, Hunter, Bergeson, & Kurzweil, 1984). These arguments suggest that the provision of rewards to participants, the construction of a morally acceptable image with which members can identify, and the offer of explanations that make working life seem meaningful are valuable in and of themselves. I argue that this exchange relationship view of legitimacy is most plausible if we assume that these rewards function by reinforcing self-esteem. The crux of the argument is that legitimacy attributions are mediated by identification processes. Identification refers to "the degree to which a

1997

Brown

665

member defines him- or herself by the same attributes that he or she believes define the organization" (Dutton, Dukerich, & Harquail, 1994: 239). Individuals identify with groups and organizations to the extent that they define these social category memberships as enhancing their self-esteem; indeed, they adjust their degree of identification with organizations and groups in order to maximize self-esteem (Brewer, 1991). Although the degree of individual identification with a social category usually has not been linked to the concept of legitimacy, it has been associated with the internalization of its goals, values, norms, and traits (Ashforth & Mael, 1989); with the degree of commitment (Mowday, Porter, & Steers, 1982); with displays of cohesion, cooperation, and altruism toward other members of the organization (Billig & Tajfel, 1973);and with the diminished likelihood of absenteeism and psychological withdrawal from the organization (Tsui, Egan, & O'Reilly, 1992). Part of the reason why the connection with legitimacy usually has not been made is that whereas identification is a psychological concept, legitimacy principally is a concern of sociologists. Notable exceptions in this respect are Tajfel and Turner and their associates (Tajfel, 1972; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Turner, 1985; Turner et al., 1987; Turner & Oakes, 1989), on whose work this article draws substantially. My argument is that the degree of identification between an individual and a social category will depend crucially on the collective regulation of individuals' self-esteem through the narcissistic ego-defense processes reviewed above. Processes of denial, rationalization, attributional egotism, self-aggrandizement, and sense of entitlement are not just identity maintaining but legitimacy enhancing. In acknowledging the legitimacy of the groups and organizations with which they identify, individuals are tacitly reaffirming their sense of self. Seen in the context of this argument, the pragmatic, moral, and cognitive rewards offered by social categories to their participants are the means by which individual and collective self-esteem are reinforced narcissistically. These rewards are, in short, the vehicles through which the legitimacy/self-esteem exchange relationship is transacted. Pragmatic rewards encourage positive self-esteem directly, by promoting selfaggrandizement and sense of entitlement, and indirectly, by allowing individuals to deny their faults, rationalize their actions, and attribute success to their own efforts. For example, rewards in the form of higher salaries or increased influence are self-aggrandizing for recipients, who are thus encouraged to think of themselves as unique and invulnerable. Over time, individuals can come to believe that they have a right, or sense of entitlement, to the rewards offered to them by groups and organizations. Continually receiving rewards allows individuals to deny their faults ("if I were not competent, I would not receive these rewards") and to engage in both rationalization ("my actions are justified by the rewards I receive") and attributional egotism ("success is due to me, and failure is the fault of other individuals or factors").

666

Academy of Management Review

July

Moral rewards, which an individual accrues as a result of a perceived correspondence between his/her morality and the several aspects of the organization, are similarly associated with positive self-esteem. These rewards encourage the individual to self-aggrandize ("because I am a member of a virtuous or worthwhile organization, I too am virtuous or worthwhile"), to deny moral improprieties and questioning of the individual's social utility ("since I participate in a good organization, my actions must be good"), to rationalize actions ("my actions are prompted by virtuous motives"), to possess a sense of entitlement ("the virtuous should receive"), and to engage in attributional egotism ("since I am good, so are the consequences of my actions"). Cognitive rewards bolster participants' self-esteem by offering models and conceptual frameworks that incorporate attributionally egotistic and rationalized explanations. These explanations often permit the denial of ignorance ("there is nothing important that I do not understand"). They also enhance a sense of entitlement ("I know that I am deserving") in the sense of overestimating one's and promote self-aggrandizement knowledge ("I understand my organization and my role in it"). I represent the interconnections among types of reward, ego defenses, self-esteem, identification, and legitimacy attributions in Figure 1. The figure makes it clear that although all organizations offer some rewards (to at least some of their participants) that engage their ego defenses, the degree to which they do this varies. In fact, I identify three basic categories of organization: (A) those in which the self-esteem of participants is adequately regulated, (B) those in which the self-esteem of participants is less than adequately regulated, and (C) those in which the self-esteem of participants is more than adequately regulated. The second and third categories are likely to be associated with pathological conditions, one symptom of which will be extremely low (in the former category) and high (in the latter category) levels of internal legitimacy. One may hypothesize that low levels of internal legitimacy will be associated with relatively low levels of participant loyalty and commitment and with high levels of staff turnover. Conversely, it seems reasonable to suggest that high levels of internal legitimacy will be associated with relatively high levels of participant loyalty and commitment and with low levels of staff turnover. Both extremes may well be associated with a relatively high incidence of organizational demise-in the former case because of paralysis brought about by anxiety and withdrawal, and in the latter case through too great an insulation from "reality." I pursue these themes in more detail in the next section, Implications for Research. The external projection of narcissism is also important for organizations, which must achieve legitimate status in their environments in order to secure resources and to avoid claims that they are negligent, irrational, or unnecessary (Dowling & Pfeffer, 1975; Meyer & Rowan, 1977; Parsons, 1960). The idea that organizations must exhibit "congruence" (Dowling & Pfeffer, 1975) or "isomorphism" (Meyer & Rowan, 1977) with the social

1997

Brown

667

E 2

o~~

0~~~~~~~~~~
o
0
._ -)

00 0

0 *
C

0Uo ,-

~~~~~~~~-o X0

.-

o N

*P

o%

c n

f
>

-r

n 0

C:

0U

o
o

~ ~~~~~~~Q
- o
-2
r

9c

39

U7

~
c

~
r

*~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ ~
Uo

U,

00

- o00

-:

o
g*
.

g s
-oo
U. o

~
0

~ ~ ~ ~~~~~C
0

T 0

0~~~~~~~~~~~~

0~~~~~
:3

668

Academy of Management

Review

July

values and norms of acceptable behavior in the larger social system is well established (Beyer, 1981; Sproull, 1981; Suchman, 1995). The argument that ours is a culture of narcissism characterized by ego aggrandizement, denial, rationalization, a sense of entitlement, and so on also has received some attention (Alvesson & Berg, 1992; Cooper, 1986; Gendlin, 1987; Lasch, 1978, 1984; Richards, 1989). That organizations are, in fact, required to incorporate and project narcissistic values and behaviors has not, however, been tackled explicitly by theorists of legitimacy, although it is a theme running through much written on the subject. Organizations generally seek to present an external "image" (Dutton & Dukerich, 1991) or "reputation" (Fombrun & Shanley, 1990) that is overtly narcissistic. Organizations are expected to be acutely sensitive to what outsiders think of them and to make the sort of strategic decisions and information disclosures that create a favorable impression (Caves & Porter, 1977; Dutton & Jackson, 1987; Fombrun & Zajac, 1987; Wilson, 1985). A favorable reputation can generate excess returns for a firm by inhibiting the mobility of rivals (Caves & Porter, 1977; Wilson, 1985), signaling product quality (Klein & Leffler, 1981; Milgrom & Roberts, 1986), attracting better applicants (Stigler, 1962), enhancing the firm's access to capital markets (Beatty & Ritter, 1986), and attracting investors (Milgrom & Roberts, 1986). Organizations are therefore encouraged to make optimistic projections about their performance, deemphasize the financial risks associated with holding their stock (Fombrun & Shanley, 1990), rationalize problems and errors (Staw, 1980), deny faults (Elsbach, 1994), and conceal negative information (Abrahamson & Park, 1994). Indeed, it may even be argued that the expression of narcissism through organizational architecture is "a purposeful adaptation to postmodern society with its emphasis on appearance and mass communication" and its insistence on individuality as a token of credibility (Berg & Kreiner, 1990: 43). In presenting a positive organizational identity, a firm's use of networks of interpersonal relations, corporate ties, press articles, and the mass media is key (Fombrun & Abrahamson, 1988; Mizruchi & Schwartz, 1987). Particularly notable is the role of advertising, which can help organizations develop strategic positions that "provide them with a measure of goodwill from consumers and other stakeholders" (Fombrun & Shanley, 1990: 241; see also Rumelt, 1987, and Weiss, 1969). Equally important are annual reports and letters to shareholders, through which organizations seek to represent themselves in ways that are both socially acceptable and often blatantly narcissistic (Graves et al., 1996; Hager & Scheiber, 1990; Preston et al., 1996). IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH In this article I have argued that organizations regulate the selfesteem of their participants through collective ego-defensive behaviors, with implications for identification and legitimation processes. This ar-

1997

Brown

669

gument has five important ramifications for ongoing research in the field of management studies. First, it suggests a need to review our literature to evaluate the extent to which our current understanding of collective behavior confirms the perspective on organizations outlined here. A review of this nature would encourage debate on the importance of the concept of self-esteem in explanations of patterns of organized activity. It also would help focus attention on the issue of agency, especially the extent to which behaviors are the result of conscious calculation or subconscious motivation. Clarification on this point would be especially welcome in the area of attributional egotism, where these competing explanations have prompted some investigation (Bettman & Weitz, 1983; Riess et al., 1981; Staw et al., 1983). As yet, researchers have paid little attention to behaviors linked to denial, rationalization, expressions of entitlement, and ego aggrandizement as discrete categories. Research is also evidently required on whether self-esteem preservation is always the best explanation for these behaviors or whether conscious calculation of other net benefits is sometimes or always associated with such behaviors. Second, there is a need to direct future research efforts at the differing extents to which organizations exhibit the narcissistic traits predicted by this paper. One possibility would be to use the organizational climate survey research popular in the 1970s as a model for investigating organizational narcissism (see, for example, Denison, 1996; Payne, Fineman, & Wall, 1976; Schneider, 1990). Use of the narcissistic personality profile outlined in this paper would yield a survey instrument with six dimensions. Although the precise wording of the questions that could validly and reliably be employed in such a tool would require further research, the sort of questions that might be used to investigate, say, denial, are as follows: Do people admit responsibility for their errors? Are important issues dodged around here? Does the organization refuse to acknowledge problems? Research of this kind will help us to determine whether different sorts of organization are prone to different types of narcissistic delusion or whether the sorts of delusion to which organizations are prone vary over time in any predictable pattern. It will also elicit any associations there may be between industry sector, length of history, size, geographic spread, dominance in the marketplace, and so forth, and different forms and degrees of narcissism. This type of research makes the collective both the subject and the object of evaluation and yields information on those characteristics participants consensually attribute to their organization. The approach assumes that organizations, like individuals, have traits. There are at least three important points to note here. In the first place, these climate-style survey questionnaires assume that individuals both know and are willing to tell the truth, as they see it, about their organization. The reality, however, is that most organizations have some participants who, through a combination of lack of perceptiveness and lack of interest, are ignorant about their organization and/or are concerned to describe their

670

Academy of Management

Review

July

organization in more favorable terms than they privately hold. To provide valid and reliable results, therefore, researchers must devise means of screening out respondents who are unable or unwilling to comment meaningfully on their organization. In the second place, survey tools of this kind will not generate independent (third-party) measures of organizational narcissism if they are completed by organizational participants. They can, though, be used to gain such information if completed by industry analysts, management consultants, and academic researchers who are sufficiently familiar with the organization to make meaningful evaluations. Comparisons of the results of surveys completed by participants and independent analysts would be particularly interesting, allowing us not only to distinguish organizations in terms of their self-assessed degree of narcissism but the "accuracy" of the self-perceptions. Such research would inform us whether high and low narcissism organizations differ in the extent of their self-insight. In the third place, climate-type research of this kind would not really help us to identify the narcissism of individuals acting as the collective (i.e., under the influence of a selfimage based on the organization). It also would reveal little information on identification and legitimation processes. Accomplishing this requires the development of a tool disclosing the extent to which participants derive their social identity from an organization. One possibility would be to refine existing tools for investigating collective identities (Greenwald, Breckler, & Wiggins, 1987; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992) so that they refer to a particular organization. Another possibility would be to refine existing self-esteem inventories, such as the Revised Janis-Field Self-Esteem Scale (Eagly, 1967; Robinson & Shaver, 1973), so that they elicit information on the self-esteem individuals feel as organizational participants. Instruments like these would provide direct measures of organizational identification and indirect measures of organizational narcissism. Combined with other survey instruments for measuring the degree of legitimacy participants accorded their organization, this tool could test the positive correlation between identification and legitimacy posited by this article. In addition, survey research of this type, perhaps using standard self-esteem inventories (Brockner, 1988), also could be used to test the hypothesis that the predispositions of organizational participants (in this instance relating to self-esteem) tend to aggregate over time under the influence of forces for homogeneity-that is, the attraction, socialization, and retention/attrition of participants (Nemeth & Staw, 1989; Schneider, 1987; Staw, 1991). Longitudinal studies of this nature additionally would yield insights into the tendencies of high and low self-esteem organizations to perpetuate themselves over time. Third, I have argued here that participation in a social category affects the centrality and evaluative importance of it to an individual's self-definition through involvement in collective ego-defense behaviors, and I have illustrated the role of these collective ego-defense behaviors in the processes by which individuals identify with and attribute legitimacy

1997

Brown

671

to the groups and organizations to which they belong. My illustrations were drawn, however, from a wide range of organization studies literature primarily designed to pursue arguments other than those presented here. Although these studies have provided suggestive evidence for our understanding of organization, specific empirical research is required to verify the putative linkages between individual identification and collective processes of denial, rationalization, and so forth. This requires that measures of the effectiveness of collective ego-defensive behaviors be devised and correlated with independent measures of individual identification. As with the other forms of research suggested above, these studies will need to be conducted across the range of different types of organization, at different points in their life cycles, and across different cultures in order to provide a full picture. Since the criteria and standards used for self-evaluation vary enormously across national and regional cultures, research sensitive to international cultural differences seems especially important (Allport, 1960; Azuma, 1984; Erez & Earley, 1993; Geertz, 1973, 1979; Kojima, 1984; Miller, 1984; Schweder & Bourne, 1982; Weisz, Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984). It is, for example, likely that individualistic cultures, which reinforce formation of the independent self and encourage self-reliance and the expression of one's unique self, exhibit a different pattern of identification/legitimation dynamics than collectivist cultures, which exhibit a normative emphasis on interdependence and connectedness (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1989). Such research is needed to provide us with a clearer understanding of the variety of the dynamics of identification and legitimacy attributions worldwide. Fourth, future research should explore the role of self-esteem regulation in the life of organizations through in-depth, inductively derived case studies based on key informant interviews, document analysis, and histories gleaned from press accounts. These interpretive approaches are best suited for exploring the extent to which the need for self-esteem influences, for example, decision making (Janis, 1989; Lamb, 1987; Staw, 1980) and communication through stories (Boje et al., 1982; Harvey et al., 1990). They are also an appropriate means for investigating whether the political behavior of individuals and groups, which is often described as being prompted by careerism (Burns & Stalker, 1961; Dalton, 1959; Knights & Murray, 1994; Mills, 1956; Pettigrew, 1973), might be linked to self-esteem regulation. Qualitative research also may be the most appropriate means for empirical inquiry into the degree to which the exercise of power in organizations involves the manipulation of individuals' self-esteem and social identity. Case studies of charismatic leaders will be especially useful in formulating explanations of the dynamics by which they elicit support from followers by maintaining and promoting their followers' self-esteem. Key informant interviews and observations are likely to be particularly valuable in teasing out the relationship between the means by which collectivities regulate the self-esteem of their participants (denial, ration-

672

Academy of Management Review

July

alization, attributional egotism, sense of entitlement, ego aggrandizement) and organizational adaptation, creativity, and learning. Content analyses of the biographies and autobiographies of those whose lives are identified closely with particular organizations, as well as corporatesponsored histories, also may be useful sources of information in the investigation of narcissism in organizations. Fifth, future research should focus on the distinction made earlier between healthy and pathological narcissism, which allows us to make some tentative predictions about collective levels of self-esteem and organizational outcomes. In an organizational context, healthy narcissism implies that the collective defense of self-esteem is effective without being excessive. It is descriptive of organizations with, for example, moderate levels of internal legitimacy and a relatively stable workforce, and those organizations aware of and responsive to their environments and able to learn and adapt to changing circumstances. Pathological narcissism may be characterized either by excessive self-esteem and reliance on ego-defense mechanisms or weak selfesteem and inadequate engagement of ego-defense mechanisms. Those organizations where self-esteem is excessive will be symptomized, for instance, by extremely high levels of internal legitimacy, low levels of staff turnover, little external monitoring, and the idiosyncratic interpretation of information in ways that defend self-esteem and resist change (see, for example, Dutton & Dukerich, 1991; Janis, 1972; Schwartz, 1987a,b, 1990a,b). Organizations characterized by very low collective self-esteem will exhibit low levels of internal legitimacy and high levels of anxiety and hostility in interpersonal interactions (Coyne, 1976; Strack & Coyne, 1982), and they may be less able and willing to take advantage of such worker participation schemes as quality circles (Brockner & Hess, 1986; Griffin & Wayne, 1984), may be less characterized by norms encouraging individuals to seek feedback on performance (Ashford, 1986; DeWhirst, 1971), might suffer from lower self-efficacy (Hollenbeck & Brief, 1987), and may be differentially prone to stress (Mossholder, Bedeian, & Armenakis, 1981). Low self-esteem organizations, however, may also be more willing to search for information prior to decision making. Both pathological forms are likely to be associated with lower than normal rates of organizational survival over time. Empirical investigation of these hypothesized connections between collective levels of selfesteem and organizational outcomes may be accomplished using the sorts of survey research methods mentioned previously. Research of this nature will need to construct measures of relative "health" and "pathology" for each of the six dimensions of the narcissistic personality. Once robust measures have been devised, these should have predictive power.

CONCLUSION
In this article I have argued that the self-esteem motive may be offered as a plausible explanation for group and organizational, as well as

1997

Brown

673

individual, behavior and have suggested that narcissism is a convenient label for grouping ego-defensive behaviors that combat anxiety and thus maintain and promote self-esteem at all levels. Employing the work of Tajfel and Turner and colleagues on social categorization, I have shown how a psychoanalytic concept could be applied at the collective level without reification, and I have devoted considerable space to illustrating the extent to which the collective defense of self-esteem is an implicit theme in the literature on organizations. With respect to legitimation dynamics, I then demonstrated the potential utility of this approach to understanding organizations. Finally, I note here that there are limits to the applicability of the narcissism concept to organizations. Most fundamentally, much recent research on the self and identity has implicated not one but two general sets of motives: self-esteem and self-knowledge (Banaji & Prentice, 1994; Kunda, 1990; Schlenker & Weigold, 1992; Strube, 1990; Swann, 1990). This article has focused on the self-esteem maintenance motive in organizations to the exclusion of the self-knowledge motive. As the self-esteem motive underpins much organizational behavior, so too does the self-knowledge motive-symptomized by organizations' attempts to reduce uncertainty and predict and control their environments. Precisely how the motivations for self-esteem and self-knowledge interact at the group and organizational levels is yet another area for further research. In short, although the idea that organizations are narcissistic entities allows us to see more in organizations (Weick, 1987), the scope of the idea is inherently partial.

REFERENCES
Abrahamson, E., & Park, C. 1994. Concealment of negative organizational outcomes: An agency theory perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 37(5): 1302-1334. Abravanel, H. 1983. Mediatory myths in the service of organizational ideology. In L. R. Pondy, P. J. Frost, G. Morgan, & T. C. Dandridge (Eds.), Organizational symbolism: 273-293. London: JAI Press. Akhtar, S., & Thompson, J. A. 1982. Overview: Narcissistic Journal of Psychiatry, 139: 12-20. personality disorder. American

Aldrich, H. E., & Fiol, C. M. 1994. Fools rush in? The institutional context of industry creation. Academy of Management Review, 19: 645-670. Allport, G. W. 1960. The open system in personality theory. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61: 301-310. Alvesson, M., & Berg, P. 0. 1992. Corporate culture and organizational Walter de Gruyter. symbolism. Berlin:

American Psychiatric Association. 1980. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. 1986. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Ash, M. K. 1981. Mary Kay. New York: Harper & Row. Ashford, S. J. 1986. Feedback-seeking in individual adaptation: A resource perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 29: 465-487.

674

Academy of Management Review

July

Ashford, S. J., Lee, C., & Bobko, P. 1989. Content, causes, and consequences of job insecurity: A theory-based measure and substantive test. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 803-829. Ashforth, B. E., & Gibbs, B. W. 1990. The double-edge nizational Science, 1: 177-194. of organizational legitimation. Orga-

Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. 1989. Social identity theory and the organization. Management Review, 14: 20-39.

Academy of

Auletta, K. 1985a. The fall of Lehman Brothers: Part 1. Power, greed and glory on Wall Street. The New York Times Magazine, February 17: 29-36, 40, 43, 59-61, 92. Auletta, K. 1985b. The fall of Lehman Brothers: Part 2. The men, the money, the merger. The New York Times Magazine, February 24: 36-42, 48, 54-58, 70-71. Auletta, K. 1986. Greed and glory on Wall Street: The fall of the house of Lehman. New York: Random House. Azuma, H. 1984. Secondary control as a heterogeneous 970-971. category. American Psychologist, 39:

Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. 1994. The self in social contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 45: 297-332. Barley, S. R. 1986. Technology as an occasion for restructuring: Observations on CT scanners and the social order of radiology departments. Administrative Science Quarterly, 31: 78-108. Barnard, C. I. 1938. The functions of the executive. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Barthes, R. 1972. Mythologies. New York: Hill & Wang. Bass, B. M. 1981. Stodgill's handbook of leadership. New York: Free Press. Bass, B. M., & Coates, C. H. 1953. Forecasting officer potential using the leaderless discussion. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 47: 321-325. Bastien, D. T. 1987. Common patterns of behavior and communication and acquisitions. Human Resource Management, 26(1): 17-34. group

in corporate mergers

Baumeister, R. F., 1986. Identity: Cultural change and the struggle for self. New York: Oxford University Press. Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, regulation failure-negative 61(1): 49-61. T. F., & Tice, D. M. 1993. When ego threats lead to selfconsequences of high self-esteem. Journal of Personality,

Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Hutton, D. G. 1989. Self-presentational motivations and personality differences in self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57: 547-579. Baumgardner, A. H., Kaufman, C. M., & Cranford, J. A. 1990. To be noticed favorably: Links between private self and public self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(4): 705-716. Beatty, R. P., & Ritter, J. R. 1986. Investment banking, reputation, and underpricing of initial public offerings. Journal of Financial Economics, 15: 213-232. Becker, E. 1973. The denial of death. New York: Free Press. Bell, J. 1984. The effect of presentation form on the use of information in annual reports. Management Science, 30(2): 169-185. Berg, P. O., & Kreiner, K. 1990. Corporate architecture: Turning physical settings into symbolic resources. In P. Gagliardi (Ed.), Symbols and artifacts: Views of the corporate landscape: 41-67. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

1997

Brown

675

Berger, P. L. 1973. The social reality of religion. Harmondsworth, United Kingdom: Penguin. Bettman, J. R., & Weitz, B. A. 1983. Attributions in the board room: Causal reasoning in corporate annual reports. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 165-183. Beyer, J. M. 1981. Ideologies, values and decision-making in organizations. In P. Nystrom & W. H. Starbuck (Eds.), Handbook of organizational design, vol. 2: 166-197. London: Oxford University Press. Beynon, H. 1973. Working for Ford. London: Allen Lane. Beynon, H. 1984. Working for Ford (2nd ed.). Harmondsworth, United Kingdom: Penguin. Billig, M. G., & Tajfel, H. 1973. Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3: 27-52. Blau, P. M. 1955. The dynamics of bureaucracy: A study of interpersonal government agencies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Boje, D. M., Fedor, D. B., & Rowland, K. M. 1982. Myth-making: A qualitative terventions. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 18(1): 17-28. relations in two step in OD in-

Bolton, R. 1989. In the American East: Richard Avedon Incorporated. In R. Bolton (Ed.), The contest of meaning: 261-282. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Bowman, E. H. 1976. Strategy and the weather. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 49-62. of the

Bradley, G. W. 1978. Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination fact or fiction question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36: 56-71.

Brewer, M. B. 1991. The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5): 475-482. Brockner, J. 1988. Self-esteem at work. Boston: Lexington.

Brockner, J., & Hess, T. 1986. Self-esteem and task performance in quality circles. Academy of Management Journal, 29: 617-622. Bromberg, P. 1986. The mirror and the mask: On narcissism and psychoanalytic growth. In A. Morrison (E.), Essential papers on narcissism: 438-466. New York: New York University Press. Brown, A. D. 1994. Politics, symbolic action and myth making in pursuit of legitimacy. ganization Studies, 15(6):861-878. Or-

Brown, J. D., Collins, R. L., & Schmidt, G. W. 1988. Self-esteem and direct versus indirect forms of self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55: 445-453. Brown, J. D., & Rogers, R. A. 1991. Self-serving attributions: The role of physiological Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5): 501-506. Buono, A. F., Bowditch, J. L., & Lewis, J. W. 1985. When cultures collide-the merger. Human Relations, 38(5): 477-500. Burns, T., & Stalker, G. M. 1961. The management Burrell, G., & Morgan, G. 1979. Sociological Heinemann Educational Books. arousal.

anatomy of a

of innovation. London: Tavistock. analysis. London:

paradigms and organizational

Callahan, R. E. 1962. Education and the cult of efficiency. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Campbell, J. D. 1990. Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(3): 538-549. Caves, R. E., & Porter, M. E. 1977. From entry barriers to mobility barriers. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 91: 421-434.

676

Academy of Management

Review

July

Chatman, J., Bell, N., & Staw, B. M. 1986. The managed thought. In H. P. Sims & D. A. Gioia (Eds.), The thinking organization: 191-214. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Cohen, A. R. 1959. Some implications of self-esteem for social influence. In C. I. Hovland & I. L. Janis (Eds.), Personality and persuasibility. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Cohen, P. S. 1969. Theories of myth. Man, 4: 337-353. Conger, J. A. 1990. The dark side of leadership. Organizational Dynamics, 19(2): 44-55.

Cooper, A. M. 1986. Narcissism. In A. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism: 112-143. New York: New York University Press. Coyne, J. C. 1976. Depression and the response of others. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85: 186-193. Dalton, M. 1959. Men who manage. New York: Wiley. Davis, S. M. 1984. Managing corporate culture. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. De Board, R. 1978. The psychodynamics of organizations. London: Tavistock.

Denison, D. R. 1996. What is the difference between organizational culture and organizational climate? A native's point of view on a decade of paradigm wars. Academy of Management Review, 21(3): 619-654. DeWhirst, H. D. 1971. Influence of perceived information sharing norms on communication channel utilization. Academy of Management Journal, 14: 305-315. Douglas, M. 1987. How institutions think. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Dowling, J., & Pfeffer, J. 1975. Organizational legitimacy: Social values and organizational behavior. Pacific Sociological Review, 18: 122-136. Dunning, D., & Cohen, G. L. 1992. Egocentric definitions of traits and abilities judgement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3): 341-355. in social

Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., & Holzberg, A. D. 1989. Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6): 1082-1090. Dunning, D., Perie, M., & Story, A. L. 1991. Self-serving prototypes of social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(6): 957-968. Durkheim, E. 1912. Les formes elementaries australie. Paris: Alcan. de la vie religieuse: Le systeme totemique en

Dutton, J. E., & Dukerich, J. M. 1991. Keeping an eye on the mirror: Image and identity in organizational adaptation. Academy of Management Journal, 34(3): 517-554. Dutton, J. E., Dukerich, J. M., & Harquail, C. V. 1994. Organizational identification. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39: 239-263. images and member

Dutton, J. E., & Jackson, S. B. 1987. Categorizing strategic issues: Links to organizational action. Academy of Management Review, 12: 76-90. Eagly, A. H. 1967. Involvement as a determinant of response to favorable and unfavorable information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7: 3. Elsbach, K. D. 1994. Managing organizational legitimacy in the California cattle industry: The construction and effectiveness of verbal accounts. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39: 57-88. Emerson, R. M. 1962. Power-dependence relations. American Sociological Review, 27: 31-40.

Emmons, R. A. 1987. Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52: 11-17.

1997

Brown

677

Erez, M., & Earley, P. C. 1993. Culture, self-identity, and work. New York: Oxford University Press. Evered, R. 1983. The language of organizations: The case of the Navy. In L. Pondy, P. J. Frost, & T. C. Dandridge (Eds.), Organizational symbolism: 125-143. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Ewen, S. 1988. All consuming images. New York: Basic Books. Feinberg, M. R. 1953. Relation of background experience Abnormal Social Psychology, 48: 206-214. to social acceptance. Journal of

Ferguson, H. 1936. Modern man: His belief and behavior. New York and London: Knopf. Festinger, L. 1954. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7: 117-140. Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. 1956. When prophecy fails. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Filby, I., & Willmott, H. 1988. Ideologies and contradictions in a public relations department: The seduction and impotency of living myth. Organization Studies, 9(3): 335-349. Fine, R. 1986. Narcissism, the self, and society. New York: Columbia University Press. Fombrun, C., & Abrahamson, E. 1988. Weaving the institutional fabric of organizational environments. Working paper, Graduate School of Business, New York University, New York. Fombrun, C., & Shanley, M. 1990. What's in a name? Reputation building and corporate strategy. Academy of Management Journal, 33: 233-258. Fombrun, C., & Zajac, E. J. 1987. Structural and perceptual influences on intraindustry stratification. Academy of Management Journal, 30: 33-50. Freud, S. 1914. On narcissism: An introduction. Reprinted in Rickman, J. (Ed.). 1937. A general selection from the works of Sigmund Freud. Psycho-Analytical Epitomes, no. 1: 118-141. London: Hogarth Press. Freud, S. 1955. Group psychology and the analysis of the ego (standard ed., vol. 18). London: Hogarth Press. Frosh, S. 1991. Identity crisis, modernity, psychoanalysis Macmillan. and the self. Basingstoke, England: of

Gabriel, M. T., Critelli, J. W., & Ee, J. S. 1994. Narcissistic illusions in self-evaluations intelligence and attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 62(1): 143-155.

Gabriel, Y. 1991. Turning facts into stories and stories into facts: A hermeneutic exploration of organizational folklore. Human Relations, 44(8): 857-875. Gecas, V. 1982. The self concept. Annual Review of Psychology, 8: 1-33. Geertz, C. 1973. The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books. Geertz, C. 1979. From the native's point of view: On the nature of anthropological understanding. In P. Rabinow & W. M. Sullivan (Eds.), Interpretive Social Science: 225-241. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Gendlin, E. T. 1987. A philosophical critique of the concept of narcissism. The significance of the awareness movement. In D. M. Levin (Ed.), Pathologies of the modern self, postmodem studies on narcissism, schizophrenia, and depression: 251-304. New York: New York University Press. Gergen, K. J. 1971. The concept of self. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Giddens, A. 1971. Capitalism and modern social theory. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Gil, J., & Foulder, J. 1978. Managing a merger: The acquisition and the aftermath. Personnel Management, 10(1): 14-17.

678

Academy of Management

Review

July

Gilchrist, J. C. 1952. The formation of social groups under conditions of success and failure. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 47: 174-187. Goffman, E. 1959. The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Gonzalez, J. A. 1996. Organizational identification: An approach based on social identity. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Cincinnati, OH. Goren, E. R. 1995. Review essay: Narcissism and the interpersonal self. Psychoanalytic chology, 12(2): 329-342. Grafton-Small, R. 1985. Making meaning concrete: Exchange processes determination of physical space. Cebes, 1(1): 62-75. Psy-

and the cultural

Graves, 0. F., Flesher, D. L., & Jordan, R. E. 1996. Pictures and the bottom line: The television epistemology of US annual reports. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 21(1): 57-88. Greenwald, A. G. 1980. The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35(7): 603-618. Greenwald, A. G., Breckler, S. J., & Wiggins, E. C. 1987. Public, private, and collective selfevaluation: Measurement of individual differences. Cited in Brockner, J. 1988. Selfesteem at work. Boston: Lexington. Griffin, R. J., & Wayne, S. J. 1984. A field study of effective and less effective quality circles. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Boston. Hager, P. J., & Scheiber, H. J. 1990. Reading smoke and mirrors: The rhetoric of corporate annual reports. Journal of Technical Writing and Communication, 20(2): 113-130. Hampden-Turner, C. 1990. Corporate culture: From vicious to virtuous circles. London: Economist Books. Harvey, J. H., Weber, A. L., & Orbuch, T. L. 1990. Interpersonal accounts. Cambridge, Blackwell. MA:

Harvey-Jones, J. 1988. Making it happen: Reflections on leadership. London: Harper-Collins. Heynes, R. W. 1950. Factors determining influence and decision satisfaction in conference requiring pooled judgements. Proceedings of the Administrative Conference, Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Hill, S. 1981. Competition and control at work. London: Heinemann. Hill, T., Smith, N. D., & Lewicki, P. 1989. The development of self-image bias: A real-world demonstration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15(2):205-211. Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. 1988. Social identifications: relations and group processes. London: Routledge. A social psychology of intergroup

Hollenbeck, J. R., & Brief, A. P. 1987. The effects of individual differences and goal origin on the goal setting process. Cited in Brockner, J. 1988. Self-esteem at work. Boston: Lexington. Holm-Lofgren, B. 1980. Ansvar Avund Arbetsgladje. Stockholm: Askild and Karnkull. Hovland, C. I., & Janis, I. L. (Eds.). 1959. Personality and persuasibility. University Press. New Haven, CT: Yale

Howard, E. 1991. Preparing annual reports in the 1990s. Public Relations Journal, May: 26-27. Ingram, R. W., & Frazier, K. B. 1983. Narrative disclosures ness Research, 11: 49-60. in annual reports. Journal of Busi-

Jacques, E. 1955. Social systems as a defence against persecutory and depressive anxiety. In M. Klein, P. Heimann, & R. Money-Kyrle (Eds.), New directions in psychoanalysis. London: Tavistock.

1997

Brown

679

Jakobson, R. 1962. Selected writings, vols. 1-4. The Hague: Mouton. Jakobson, R., & Halle, M. 1956. The fundamentals of language. The Hague: Mouton. and crisis management. New

Janis, I. L. 1989. Crucial decisions, leadership in policymaking York: Free Press.

Janis, I. L. 1972. Victims of groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Jennings, E. E. 1971. Routes to the executive suite. New York: McGraw-Hill. Judd, V. C., & Tims, B. J. 1991. How annual reports communicate Industrial Marketing Management, 20: 353-360. Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. 1966. The social psychology of organizations. a customer orientation. New York: Wiley.

Keller, M. 1989. Rude awakening: The rise, fall, and struggle for recovery of General Motors. New York: William Morrow. Kernberg, 0. 1966. Cited in Frosh, S. 1991. Identity crisis, modernity, psychoanalysis self. Basingstoke, England: Macmillan. Kets de Vries, M. F. R. 1980. Organizational London: Tavistock. and the

paradoxes, clinical approaches to management.

Kets de Vries, M. F. R. 1996. Family business, human dilemmas in the family firm. London: Thompson. Kets de Vries, M., & Miller, D. 1985. Narcissism and leadership: An object relations perspective. Human Relations, 38(6): 583-601. Kipnis, D., & Lane, W. P. 1962. Self-confidence 46: 291-295. and leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology,

Klein, B., & Leffler, K. 1981. The role of market forces in assuring contractual performance. Journal of Political Economy, 89: 615-641. Kleinfield, S. 1981. The biggest company on earth: A profile of AT&T.New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kluckhon, C. 1942. Myths and rituals: A general theory. The Harvard Theological Review, 35: 45-79. Knights, D., & Murray, F. 1994. Managers divided, organization politics and information technology management. Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley. Knorr-Cetina, K. D. 1981. The microsociological challenge of macro-sociology: Toward a reconstruction of social theory and methodology. In K. Knorr-Cetina & A. V. Cicourel (Eds.), Advances in social theory and methodology: 1-47. Boston: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Kohut, H. 1971. The analysis of the self. New York: International Universities Press. Kojima, H. 1984. A significant Psychologist, 39: 972-973. stride toward the comparative study of control. American

Kramer, R. M. 1991. Intergroup relations and organizational dilemmas: The role of categorization processes. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol 13: 191-228. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Kunda, Z. 1987. Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53: 636-647. Kunda, Z. 1990. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychology Bulletin, 108(3):480-498. Lacan, J. 1966. Ecritis. Paris: Seuil. Lamb, R. B. 1987. Running American business: Top CEOs rethink their major decisions. New York: Basic Books.

680

Academy of Management Review

July expecta-

Lasch, C. 1978. The culture of narcissism, American life in an age of diminishing tions. New York: W. W. Norton. Lasch, C. 1984. The minimal self: Psychic survival in troubled times. London: Pan. Laughlin, H. P. 1970. The ego and its defenses. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Lax, R. F. 1975. Some comments on the narcissistic aspects of self-righteousness: Defensive and structural considerations. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 56: 283-292. Leary, M. R., & Kowalski, R. M. 1990. Impression management: component model. Psychological Bulletin, 107: 34-47. A literature review and two-

Lewis, R. 1984. Annual reports of the future. Communication Arts, September/October: 24-28. Lichstenstein, H. 1977. The dilemma of human identity. New York: Aronson.

Lippitt, R., Thelen, H., & Leff, E. Unpublished memorandum, undated. Cited in Bass, B. M. 1981. Stodgill's handbook of leadership. New York: Free Press. Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. 1992. A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18: 302-318. of one's social

Macoby, M. 1976. The gamesman: The new corporate leaders. New York: Simon & Schuster. Mangham, I., & Overington, M. 1983. Dramatism and the theatrical metaphor. In G. Morgan (Ed.), Beyond method: 219-233. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Marglin, S. 1974a. What do bosses do? Review of Radical Political Economics, 6: 60-112. Marglin, S. 1974b. What do bosses do? Review of Radical Political Economics, 7: 20-37. Marks, M. L., & Mirvis, P. H. 1983. Psychological consequences of a corporate acquisition and the factors that moderate them: A longitudinal study. Paper presented at the 91st Convention of the American Psychological Association, Anaheim, CA. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. 1991. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98: 224-253. Markus, H. R., & Wurf, E. 1987. The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological Annual Review of Psychology, 38: 299-337. Martin, J. 1992. Cultures in organizations: Three perspectives. Press. perspective.

New York: Oxford University paradox in orga-

Martin, J., Feldman, M. S., Hatch, M. J., & Sitkin, S. B. 1983. The uniqueness nizational stories. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 438-453. Marx, K. 1964. The economic and philosophical Publishers.

manuscripts of 1844. New York: International

Maslow, A. H. 1954. Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Row. Menzies, I. E. P. 1970. The functioning of social systems as a defence against anxiety. London: Tavistock. Meyer, J. W., & Rowan, B. 1977. Institutionalized organizations: Formal structure as myth and ceremony. American Journal of Sociology, 83: 340-363. Milgrom, P., & Roberts, J. 1986. Price and advertising Political Economy, 94: 796-821. signals of product quality. Journal of

Miller, A. 1986. Depression and grandiosity as related forms of narcissistic disturbances. In A. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism: 323-347. New York: New York University Press. Miller, D., & Droge, C. 1986. Psychological and traditional determinants ministrative Science Quarterly, 31: 539-560. of structures. Ad-

1997

Brown

681

Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. 1975. Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction. Psychology Bulletin, 82: 213-225. Miller, E. J., & Gwynne, G. V. 1972. A life apart.... London: Tavistock. Journal of

Miller, J. G. 1984. Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Personality and Social Psychology, 46: 961-978. Mills, C. 1956. White collar. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mizruchi, M. S., & Schwartz, M. (Eds.). 1987. Intercorporate relations: The structural analysis of business. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Morgan, G. 1980. Paradigms, metaphors and puzzle solving in organization theory. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25: 605-622. Morgan, G. 1983. More on metaphor: Why we cannot control tropes in administrative Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 601-607. Morgan G. 1986. Images of organization. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Mossholder, K. W., Bedeian, A. G., & Armenakis, A. A. 1981. Role perceptions, satisfaction, and performance: Moderating effects of self-esteem and organizational level. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 28: 224-234. Mowday, R. T. 1979. Leader characteristics, self-confidence, and methods of upward influence in organizational decision situations. Academy of Management Journal, 22: 709725. Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. 1982. Employee-organization linkages: The psychology of commitment, absenteeism and turnover. New York: Academic Press. Nader, R., & Taylor, W. 1986. The big boys: Power and position in American business. New York: Pantheon. Nemeth, C. J., & Staw, B. M. 1989. The tradeoffs of social control and innovation in groups and organizations. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 22: 175-210. New York: Academic Press. Niskala, M., & Pretes, M. 1995. Environmental reporting in Finland-a note on the use of annual reports. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 20(6): 457-466. Ortony, A. 1975. Why metaphors are necessary 45-53. and not just nice. Educational Theory, 25: science.

Palmer, I., & Dunford, R. 1996. Conflicting uses of metaphors: Reconceptualizing their use in the field of organizational change. Academy of Management Review, 21(3): 691-717. Parsons, T. 1960. Structure and process in modern societies. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Payne, R. L., Fineman, S., & Wall, T. D. 1976. Organizational climate and job satisfaction: A conceptual synthesis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16: 45-62. Pettigrew, A. M. 1973. The politics of organizational Pettigrew, A. M. 1979. On studying organizational terly, 24: 570-581. decision-making. London: Tavistock. Science Quar-

cultures. Administrative

Pettit, G. 1990. Creating an annual report that's on the money. Across the Board, June: 62-63. Pfeffer, J. 1981. Management as symbolic action: The creation and maintenance of organizational paradigms. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior: 3(1): 1-52. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Pfeffer, J., & Salancik, G. 1978. The external control of organizations: A resource-dependence perspective. New York: Harper & Row. Piccardo, C., Varchetta, G., & Zanarini, G. 1990. Car makers and marathon runners: In pursuit

682

Academy of Management Review

July

of culture through the language of leadership. In P. Gagliardi (Ed.), Corporate artifacts, views of the corporate landscape: 255-270. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Preston, A. M., Wright, C., & Young, J. J. 1996. Imag[in]ing annual reports. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 21(1): 113-137. Pulver, S. 1970. Narcissism: The term and the concept. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 18: 319-341. Reed, M. 1985. Re-directions in organizational analysis. London: Tavistock.

Reed, M., & Hughes, M. 1992. Rethinking organization. London: Sage. Reich, A. 1960. Pathologic forms of self-esteem regulation. In A. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism: 44-60. New York: New York University Press. Rhodewalt, F., & Morf, C. C. 1995. Self and interpersonal correlates of the narcissistic personality inventory: A review and new findings. Journal of Research in Personality, 29(1): 1-23. Rhodewalt, F., Morf, C., Hazlett, S., & Fairfield, M. 1991. Self-handicapping: The role of discounting and augmentation in the preservation of self-esteem. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 61(1): 122-131. Richards, B. 1989. Images of Freud. London: Dent. Riess, M., Rosenfield, P., Melburg, V., & Tedeschi, J. T. 1981. Self-serving attributions: Biased private perceptions and distorted public descriptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41: 224-231. Ring, P. S., & Van de Ven, A. H. 1989. Formal and informal dimensions of transactions. In A. H. Van de Ven, H. L. Angle, & M. S. Poole (Eds.), Research on the management of innovation: The Minnesota studies: 171-192. New York: Ballinger. Robinson, J. R., & Shaver, P. R. 1973. Measures of social psychological MI: Institute for Social Research. attitudes. Ann Arbor,

Rosen, M. 1985. Breakfast at Spiro's: Dramaturgy and dominance. Journal of Management, 11(2): 31-48. Ross, J., & Staw, B. M. 1993. Organizational escalation and exit: Lessons from the Shoreham Nuclear Power Plant. Academy of Management Journal, 36(4): 701-732. Rothstein, A. 1980. The narcissistic pursuit of perfection. New York: International Universities Press. Rowlinson, M. C. 1987. Cadbury's new factory system. Unpublished University of Aston, Birmingham, England. doctoral dissertation,

Rowlinson, M. C., & Hassard, J. 1994. The invention of corporate culture: A history of the histories of Cadbury. Human Relations, 46(3): 299-326. Royko, M. 1971. Boss, Richard J. Daley of Chicago. Chicago: Signet. Rumelt, R. P. 1987. Theory, strategy, and entrepreneurship. challenge: 137-158. Boston: Ballinger. In D. Teece (Ed.), The competitive

Salancik, G. R., & Meindl, J. R. 1984. Corporate attributions as strategic illusions of management control. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29: 238-254. Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. 1978. A social information processing approach to job attitudes and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23: 224-253. Sankowsky, D. 1995. The charismatic leader as narcissist: Understanding the abuse of power. Organizational Dynamics, 23(4): 57-71. Schlenker, B. R. 1980. Impression management: The self-concept, social identity, and interpersonal relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.

1997

Brown

683

Schlenker, B. R., & Weigold, M. F. 1992. Interpersonal processes involving impression regulation and management. Annual Review of Psychology, 43: 133-168. Schlenker, B. R., Weigold, M. F., & Hallam, J. R. 1990. Self-serving attributions in social context: Effects of self-esteem and social pressure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(5): 855-863. Schneider, B. 1987. The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40: 437-453. Schneider, B. 1990. Organizational climate and culture. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Schonbach, P. 1980. A category system for account phases. European Journal of Social Psychology, 10: 195-200. Schwantzer, B. 1986. Die bedeutung der architektur fur die corporate identity eines unternehmens. Wien: Modulverlag. Schwartz, H. S. 1985. The usefulness of myth and the myth of usefulness: A dilemma for the applied organizational scientist. Journal of Management, 11(1):31-42. Schwartz, H. S. 1987a. On the psychodynamics of organizational disaster: The case of the space shuttle Challenger. Columbia Journal of World Business, 22(1): 59-67. Schwartz, H. S. 1987b. Anti-social actions of committed organizational participants: An existential psychoanalytic perspective. Organization Studies, 8(4): 327-340. Schwartz, H. S. 1987c. On the psychodynamics Management, 13(1): 41-54. of organizational totalitarianism. Journal of

Schwartz, H. S. 1990a. The symbol of the space shuttle and the degeneration of the American dream. In P. Gagliardi (Ed.), Symbols and artifacts: Views of the corporate landscape: 303-322. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Schwartz, H. S. 1990b. Narcissistic process and corporate decay, the theory of the organizational ideal. New York: New York University Press. Schweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. 1982. Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In A. J. Marselaa & G. White (Eds.), Cultural concepts of mental health and therapy: 97-137. Boston: Reidel. Schweiger, D. M., & Denisi, A. S. 1991. Communication with employees following a merger: A longitudinal field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 34(1): 110-135. Scott, W. R. 1977. Effectiveness of organizational effectiveness studies. In P. S. Goodman & J. M. Pennings (Eds.), New perspectives on organizational effectiveness: 63-95. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Scott, W. R. 1991. Unpacking institutional arguments. In W. W. Powell & P. J. DiMaggio (Eds.), The new institutionalism in organizational analysis: 164-182. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Scott, W. R., & Meyer, J. W. 1991. The organization of societal sectors. In W. W. Powell & DiMaggio (Eds.), The new institutionalism in organizational analysis: 108-140. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Sculley, J. 1987. Odyssey, Pepsi to Apple ... a journey of adventure, ideas and the future. New York: Harper & Row. Seiler, J. A. 1984. Architecture at work. Harvard Business Review, September-October: 120. Selznick, P. 1957. Leadership and administration. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson. 111-

Shengold, L. 1995. Delusions of everyday life. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. 1953. Groups in harmony and tension. New York: Harper & Row.

684

Academy of Management Review

July produced groups. Ameri-

Sherif, M., White, B. J., & Harvey, 0. J. 1955. Status in experimentally can Journal of Sociology. 60: 370-379. Shirley, R. C. 1973. Analysis of employee and physician Academy of Management Journal, 16: 465-480.

attitudes toward hospital merger.

Sinetar, M. 1981. Mergers, morale and productivity. Personnel Journal, 60: 863-867. Sproull, L. S. 1981. Beliefs in organizations. In P. C. Nystrom & W. H. Starbuck (Eds.), Handbook of organizational design, vol. 2: 167-202. New York: Oxford University Press. Starbuck, W. H. 1983. Organizations as action generators. American Sociological 91-102. Review, 48:

Starbuck, W. H., Greve, A., & Hedberg, B. T. 1978. Responding to crisis. Journal of Business Administration, 9: 111-137. Starkey, K. 1992. Durkheim and organizational 13(4): 627-642. analysis: Two legacies. Organization Studies,

Staw, B. M. 1980. Rationality and justification in organizational life. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, 2: 45-80. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Staw, B. M. 1991. Dressing up like an organization: When psychological organizational action. Journal of Management, 17(4): 805-819. theories can explain perfor-

Staw, B. M., McKechnie, P. I., & Puffer, S. M. 1983. The justification of organizational mance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 582-600.

Steele, C. M. 1988. The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 21: 261-302. New York: Academic Press. Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. 1991. Motivation and work behavior (5th ed.). New York: McGrawHill. Stigler, G. J. 1962. Information in the labor market. Journal of Political Economy, 70: 49-73. Strack, S., & Coyne, J. C. 1982. Social confirmation of dysphoria: Shared and private reactions. Unpublished manuscript, University of California. Strube, M. J. 1990. In search of self: Balancing the good and the true. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(4): 699-704. Suchman, M. C. 1995. Managing legitimacy: Strategic and institutional emy of Management Review, 20(3): 571-610. approaches. Acadimage

Sutton, R. I., & Callahan, A. L. 1987. The stigma of bankruptcy: Spoiled organizational and its management. Academy of Management Journal, 30: 405-436.

Swann, W. B. 1990. To be adored or to be known: The interplay of self-enhancement and self-verification. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition, vol. 2: 408-450. New York: Guilford. Tajfel, H. 1972. La categorization sociale. In S. Moscovici (Ed.), Introduction a la psychologie sociale, vol. 1: 272-302. Paris: Larousse. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1986. The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations, vol. 2: 7-24. Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Teicholz, J. 1978. A selective review of the psychoanalytic literature on theoretical conceptualizations of narcissism. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 26: 831861. Tennen, H., & Herzberger, S. 1987. Depression, self-esteem, and the absence of self-protective attributional biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(1): 72-80.

1997

Brown

685

Terkel, S. 1972. Working. New York: Ballantine Books. Tobacyk, J. J., & Mitchell, T. E. 1987. Out-of-body experience status as a moderator of effects of narcissism on paranormal beliefs. Psychological Reports, 60: 440-442. Triandis, H. C. 1989. The self and social behavior in differing social contexts. Psychological Review, 96: 506-520. Trice, H. M., & Beyer, J. M. 1985. Using six organizational rites to change cultures. In R. H. Kilmann, J. S. Saxton, & R. Serpa (Eds.), Gaining control of the corporate culture: 370-399. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Trice, H. M., & Beyer, J. M. 1993. The cultures of work organizations. Prentice-Hall. Trist, E. L., & Bamforth, K. W. 1951. Some social and psychological wall method of coal getting. Human Relations, 4: 3-38. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: of the Long-

consequences

Tsoukas, H. 1991. The missing link: A transformational view of metaphors in organizational science. Academy of Management Review, 16(3): 566--585. Tsoukas, 1993. Analogical reasoning and knowledge generation in organization theory. Organization Studies, 14(3): 323-346. Tsui, A. S., Egan, T. D., & O'Reilly, C. A. 1992. Being different: Relational demography organizational attachment. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37: 549-579. and

Turner, J. C. 1985. Social categorization and self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior. Advances in Group Process, 2: 77-122. Turner, J. C. 1987. Rediscovering land: Blackwell. the social group, a self-categorization theory. Oxford, Eng-

Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. 1987. Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford, England: Blackwell. Turner, J. C., & Oakes, P. J. 1989. Self-categorization theory and social influence. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), The psychology of group influence, vol. 2: 233-275. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Van Zelst, R. H. 1951. Worker popularity and job satisfaction. 412. Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. 1991. The embodied human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Personnel Psychology, 4: 405mind: Cognitive science and

Walton, R. E. 1985. From control to commitment in the workplace. Harvard Business Review, 63(2): 76-84. Watson, T. J. 1994. In search of management, London: Routledge. culture, chaos and control in managerial work.

Wegner, D. M. 1987. Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior: 185-208. New York: SpringerVerlag. Weick, K. E. 1979. The social psychology of organizing. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Weick, K. E. 1987. Organizational culture as a source of high reliability. California Management Review, 29(2): 112-127. Weick, K. E. 1988. Enacted sensemaking 24: 305-317. Weick, K. E. 1995. Sensemaking in crisis situations. Journal of Management Studies, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

in organizations.

Weick, K. E., & Roberts, K. H. 1993. Collective mind in organizations: Heedful interrelating on flight decks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38: 357-381.

686

Academy of Management

Review

July

Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kulka, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. 1971. Perceiving the causes of success and failure. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Weiss, L. W. 1969. Advertising, profits, and corporate taxes. Review of Economics and Statistics, 51: 421-429. Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. 1984. Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and Japan. American Psychologist, 39: 955-969. Westen, D. 1990. The relations among narcissism, egocentrism, self-concept, and self-esteem: Experimental, clinical, and theoretical considerations. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 13: 183-239. Whetton, D. A. 1980. Organizational decline: A neglected Academy of Management Review, 5: 577-588. topic in organizational science.

Whyte, W. F. 1943. Street corner society: The social structure of an Italian slum. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Wilson, H. W. 1985. Reputations in games and markets. In A. E. Roth (Ed.), Game-theoretic models of bargaining: 65-84. New York: Cambridge University Press. Wiseman, J. 1982. An evaluation of environmental disclosures made in corporate annual reports. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 7(1): 53-63. Wood, D. J. 1991. Corporate social performance revisited. Academy of Management Review, 16: 691-718. Wood, J. V. 1989. Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychology Bulletin, 106(2): 231-248. Wright, J. P. 1979. On a clear day you can see General Motors. Grosse Pointe, MI: Wright Enterprise. Wuthnow, R., Hunter, J. D., Bergeson, A., & Kurzweil, E. 1984. Cultural analysis. Boston: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Zaleznik, A. 1966. Human dilemmas of leadership. New York: Harper & Row. Zaleznik, A. 1989. The mythological 28(2): 267-278. structure and its impact. Human Resource Management,

Zuckerman, M. 1979. Attribution of success and failure revisited, or: The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory. Journal of Personality, 47: 245-287. Andrew D. Brown received his Ph.D. in organizational behavior from the University of Sheffield. He is a lecturer in organizational behavior at the University of Cambridge. His current research interests focus on issues of organizational identity.

S-ar putea să vă placă și