Sunteți pe pagina 1din 16

School Psychology Review,

2002, Volume 31, No. 3, pp. 3 13-32 7

Motivation as an Enabler for Academic Success

Elizabeth A. Linnenbrink
University of Toledo

Paul R Pintrich
The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor

Abstract. Student motivation as an academic enabler for school success is dis-


cussed. Contrary to many views, however, the authors conceive of student motiva-
tion as a multifaceted construct with different components. Accordingly, the ar-
ticle includes a discussion of four key components of student motivation including
academnic self-efficacy, attributions, intrinsic motivation, and achievement goals.
Research on each of these four components is described, research relating these
four components to academic achievement and other academic enablers isreviewed,
and suggestions are offered for instruction and assessment.

Psychologists and educators have long models of motivation (Pintrich & Schunk,
considered the role of motivation in student 2002). One of the mostimportant assumptions
achievement and learning (for a review see of social cognitive models of motivation is that
Graham & Weiner, 1996). Much of the early motivation is a dynamic, multifaceted phenom-
research on student achievement and learning enon that contrasts with the quantitative view
separated cognitive and motivational factors taken by traditional models of motivation. In
and pursued very distinct lines of research that other words, these newer social cognitive mod-
did not integrate cognition and motivation. els do not assume that students are either "moti-
However, since at least the 1980s there has vated" or "not motivated" or that student moti-
been a sustained research focus on how moti- vation can be characterized in some quantita-
vational and cognitive factors interact and tive manner between two endpoints on a single
jointly influence student learning and achieve- continuum. Rather, social cognitive models stress
ment. In more colloquial terms, there is a recog- thatstudents canbemotivatedin multiple ways
nition that students need both the cognitive sIdil and the important issue is understanding how
and the motivational will to do well in school and why students are motivated for school
(Pintrich & Schunk, 2002). This miniseries con- achievement. This change in focus implies that
tinues in this tradition by highlighting the con- teachers or school psychologists should not
tribution of both motivational and cognitive label students as "motivated" or "not moti-
factors for student academic success. vated" in some global fashion. Furthermore,
The integration of motivational and cog- assessment instruments that generate a single
nitive factors was facilitated by the shift in global "motivation" score for students may be
motivational theories from traditional achieve- misleading in terms of a more multifaceted
ment motivation models to social cognitive understanding of student motivation. Accord-

Address correspondence regarding this article to Elizabeth A.Linnenbrink, College of Education, The
University of Toledo, Mail Stop 923, Foundations of Education, 2801 West Bancroft Street, Toledo, OH
43606. Email: lisa.linnenbrink@utoledo.edu.
Copyright 2002 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015

313
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

ingly, in the discussion of motivation as an and eventual achievement. That is, students'
academic enabler, many aspects of student own thoughts about theirmotivation and learn-
motivation including self-efficacy, attributions, ing play a key role in mediating their engage-
intrinsic motivation, and goals are considered. ment and subsequent achievement.
A second important assumption of so- Following from these three general as-
cial cognitive models of motivation is that sumptions, social cognitive motivational theo-
motivation is not a stable trait of an individual, rists have proposed a large number of differ-
but is more situated, contextual, and domain- ent motivational constructs that may facilitate
specific. In other words, not only are students or constrain student achievement and learning.
motivated in multiple ways, but their motiva- Although there are good theoretical reasons for
tion can vary depending on the situation or some of these distinctions among different
context in the classroom or school. Although motivational theories and constructs, in many
this assumption makes it more difficult for re- cases they can be confusing and less than help-
search and assessment efforts, it means that ful in developing applications to improve stu-
student motivation is conceived as being in- dent motivation and subsequent learning in
herently changeable and sensitive to the con- school (Pintrich, 2000a). Rather than discuss-
text. This provides hope for teachers and school ing all the different motivational constructs that
psychologists and suggests that instructional may be enablers of student achievement and
efforts and the design of classrooms and learning, this article will focus on four key
schools can make a difference in motivating families of motivational beliefs (self-efficacy,
students for academic achievement. attributions, intrinsic motivation, and goal ori-
This situated assumption means that stu- entations). These four families represent the
dent motivation probably varies as a function currently accepted major social cognitive moti-
of subject matter domains and classrooms (e.g., vational theories (Eccles, Wigfield, &
Bong, 2001). For example, within social cog- Schiefele, 1998; Graham & Weiner, 1996;
nitive models, motivation is usually assessed Pintrich & Schunk, 2002) and, therefore,
for a specific subject area such as math, read- seem most relevant when thinking about
ing, science, or social studies and in reference how motivation relates to achievement and
to a specific classroom or teacher. In some other academic enablers. For each of the
ways, this also fits with teachers' and parents' four general components, the components
own perceptions and experiences as they find are defined, a summarization is given for
that some children are quite motivated for how the motivational component is related
mathematics, whereas others hate it, and also to student achievement and learning as well
observe these motivational differences with as the other academic enablers discussed in
other subject areas as well. However, this im- this special issue, and some implications for
plies that assessment instruments that assess instruction and assessment are suggested. Al-
general student motivation for school or aca- though these four families are interrelated, it
demics may not be as useful as more domain is beyond the scope of this article to present an
or context specific assessment tools. interrelated model of self-efficacy, attributions,
A third assumption concerns the central intrinsic motivation, and goal orientations.
role of cognition in social cognitive models of Readers interested in a more comprehensive
motivation. That is, itis notjust the individual's overview may refer to Pintrich and Schunk's
cultural, demographic, or personality charac- (2002) detailed discussion of motivationalpro-
teristics that influence motivation and achieve- cesses in schooling.
ment directly, or just the contextual character- Adaptive Self-Efficacy Beliefs as
istics of the classroom environment that shape Enablers of Success
motivation and achievement, but rather the
individual's active regulation of his or her mo- A common layperson's definition of
tivation, thinking, and behavior that mediates motivation is that it involves a strong personal
the relationships between the person, context, interest in a particular subject or activity. Stu-

314
Motivation

dents who are interested are motivated and they found that self-efficacy beliefs are positively
learn and achieve because of this strong inter- related to student cognitive engagement and
est. Although interest as a component of stu- their use of self-regulatory strategies (similar
dent motivation wir be discussed later, one of in some ways to study skills) as well as gen-
the more important motivational beliefs for eral achievement as indexed by grades (e.g.,
student achievement is self-efficacy, which Pintrich, 2000b; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990;
concerns beliefs about capabilities to do a task Wolters, Yu, & Pintrich, 1996).
or activity. More specifically, self-efficacy has In summary, both experimental and cor-
been defined as individuals' beliefs about their relational research in schools suggests that self-
performance capabilities in a particular con- efficacy is positively related to a host of posi-
text or a specific task or domain (Bandura, tive outcomes of schooling such as choice,
1997). Self-efficacy is assumed to be situated persistence, cognitive engagement, use of self-
and contextualized, not a general belief about regulatory strategies, and actual achievement.
self-concept or self-esteem. For example, a stu- This generalization seems to apply to all stu-
dent might have high self-efficacy for doing dents, as it is relatively stable across different
algebra problems, but a lower self-efficacy for ages and grades as well as different gender and
geometry problems or other subject areas, de- ethnic groups (Bandura, 1997; Pintrich &
pending on past successes and failures. These Schunk, 2002). From these findings, it seems
self-efficacy beliefs are distinct from general clear that self-efficacy beliefs are related to
self-concept beliefs or self-esteem. several of the other academic enablers re-
Although the role of self-efficacy has viewed in this miniseries. In particular, self-
been studied in a variety of domains including efficacy has been associated with increased
mental health and health behavior such as cop- persistencerelating it to engagement. Evidence
ing with depression or smoking cessation, busi- has also been reviewed suggesting that self-
ness management, and athletic performance, a efficacy promotes adaptive strategy use such
number of educational psychologists have ex- as self-regulation suggesting that students with
amined how self-efficacy relates to behavior high self-efficacy beliefs will also be likely to
in elementary and secondary academic settings use adaptive and appropriate study skills. In
(e.g., Bandura, 1997; Eccles et al., 1998; terms of social behavior, less is known about
Pintrich, 2000b; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; the relation of academic self-efficacy to peer
Schunk, 1989a, 1989b, 1991). In particular, relations. However, recent research suggests
self-efficacy has been positively related to that both perceived social competence and the
higher levels of achievement and learning as endorsement of social responsibility goals (ad-
well as a wide variety of adaptive academic hering to social norms or rules) are associated
outcomes such as higher levels of effort and with higher reports of academic self-efficacy
increased persistence on difficult tasks in both (Patrick, Hicks, & Ryan, 1997). In summary,
experimental and correlational studies involv- having generally positive self-efficacy is adap-
ing students from a variety of age groups tive for school learning and achievement as
(Bandura, 1997; Pintrich & Schunk, 2002). well as other academic enablers, suggesting
Students who have more positive self-efficacy that schools should seek to develop positive
beliefs (i.e., they believe they can do the task) self-efficacy beliefs in their students.
are more likely to work harder, persist, and At the same time, two important caveats
eventually achieve at higher levels. In addi- need to be stressed about attempts to facilitate
tion, there is evidence that students who have positive self-efficacy beliefs in students. First,
positive self-efficacy beliefs are more likely as noted earlier, self-efficacy is not self-esteem
to choose to continue to take more difficult and the two constructs should not be confused.
courses (e.g., advanced math courses) over the Self-efficacy is a judgment of task-specific
course of schooling (Eccles et al., 1998). In capabilities and is based on actual accomplish-
our own correlational research with junior high ments and success and failures, whereas self-
students in Michigan, we have consistentiy esteem is a much more general affective evalu-

315
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

ation of the self. The second caveat relates to schooling, both Pintrich and his colleagues
the issue ofinaccuracy in self-efficacy beliefs. (Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993)
The generalization about the positive link be- and Midgley and her colleagues (Midgley et al.,
tween self-efficacy and achievement may sug- 1998) have developed self-report measures of
gest that self-efficacy should always be as high academic self-efficacy that can be used with a
as possible. However, it seems that it is more variety of students. In both the Motivated Strat-
adaptive to have self-efficacy beliefs that are egies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ,
relatively accurate or calibrated to actual ac- Pintrich et al., 1993) and Pattems of Adaptive
complishments (Bandura, 1997). For example, Learning Survey (PALS, Midgley et al., 1998),
a novice mountain climber should have self- students respond to a series of statements by in-
efficacy beliefs that match his actual skills, or dicating how true the statement is for them. Items
are a little higher than actual skills, but are not are typically rated using 5-point or 7-point Likert
so overly positive that he attempts a climb that scales. These self-report measures are reliable
is well beyond his capabilities. In the latter forabroadrange of students fromfourth grade
case, serious injury or even death could result through college.
from having overly positive beliefs about his Although these measures were designed
capabilities for mountain climbing. In the same for research purposes, they can also be used
manner, students should not overestimate or by individual instructors to get a sense of stu-
underestimate their capabilities for school- dents' efficacy beliefs in their classrooms.
work, rather they should have fairly accurate, However, it is important to note that these
but optimistic, beliefs about their efficacy to measures are not norm-referenced assessments
accomplish school work. This implies that such as standardized intelligence or achieve-
teachers and other school personnel should at- ment tests. It is expected that students' re-
tempt to foster positive, but accurate, self-ef- sponses differ depending on the context; there-
ficacy beliefs. fore, norms should be established within a par-
In terms of instructional implications, ticular context or classroom. Furthermore, as
self-efficacy is best facilitated by providing noted earlier, motivation is context-specific.
opportunities for students to succeed on tasks Therefore, teachers and school psychologists
within their range of competence and through should think about the context in which effi-
these experiences actually develop new capa- cacy is being assessed and adjust the items
bilities and skills. Self-efficacy is not fostered appropriately. For instance, if a teacher is in-
by providing inaccurate or effusive praise to terested in students' academic self-efficacy in
students in the absence of specific task accom- math class, she orhe should ask students ques-
plishments. This type of praise is meaningless tions about their feelings and beliefs in math,
and invalid and may foster inaccurate beliefs in not about school in general.
students who think they are capable of some task,
such as reading, when in fact they are not very Adaptive Attributions as Enablers of
good readers (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002). There- Success
fore, it is important that educators calibrate tasks Attribution theory, which focuses on at-
and assessments so that success is attainable. By tempts to understand why events occur, is an-
having a variety of tasks in the classroom and other important line of research on achieve-
multiple forms of assessments such as portfo- ment motivation (Graham & Weiner, 1996).
lios, essays, and project-based assessments, Similar to other motivational theories, research
classroom teachers may be able to provide all on attributions did not focus initially on aca-
students with opportunities to be successful, demic achievement. However, Weiner's (1985,
thus fostering self-efficacy among all students. 1986) research relating attributions to students'
Self-efficacy is typically assessed using behaviors and success in academic settings has
self-report questionnaires. For instance, as part done much to further an understanding of how
of larger survey instruments to assess motiva- attributions relate to learning in school. Al-
tion, strategy use, and general attitudes about though much of Weiner's research was con-
Motivation

ducted with college undergraduates, others external) can also be adaptive because it means
such as Borkowski (e.g., Borkowski,Weyhing, that the circumstances perceived to cause the
& Carr, 1988) have applied attribution theory failure may not be present in future situations.
to elementary and secondary students. Researchers who focus on children with
Attribution theory suggests that when a learning disabilities or underachieving children
failure or success occurs, such as failing a math find similar patterns of adaptive and maladap-
exam or doing particularly well on an assign- tive attributions; however, they further suggest
ment, individuals will analyze the situation to that effort attributions be associated with strat-
determine the perceived causes for the failure egy use (Carr, Borkowski, & Maxwell, 1991;
or success (Weiner, 1986). These causes may Licht, 1983). That is, rather than attributing
be environmental factors, such as a distracting success to effort, success should be attributed
testing environment or bias on the part of the to effortful strategy use. This may be especially
teacher, or personal factors, such as lack of important for children with learning disabili-
knowledge, ability, or failure to prepare ad- ties in that effort may not always lead to suc-
equately for the exam. These perceived causes cess. Furthermore, rather than attributing fail-
can be categorized into three causal dimen- ure to lack of effort alone, attributing failure
sions: stability (how stable the perceived cause to the lack of strategy use or use of inappropri-
is), locus (whether the cause is internal or ex- ate strategies helps dispel the inappropriate
ternal), and controllability (whether or not the belief that effort always leads to success, but
perceived cause can be controlled). Using these still helps to convey the idea that success is
three causal dimensions, a specific cause can possible.
be categorized into one of eight cells. For in- Although attribution theory does not sug-
stance, a student who fails an exam may say it gest a direct link of adaptive attributions to
is due to instructor bias (external, stable, con- academic achievement and other academic
trollable) or lack of ability (internal, stable, un- enablers, some indirect links can be made via
controllable). According to attribution theory, other psychological processes. For instance,
it is the individual's focus on why success or adaptive attributions are associated with higher
failure occurred that explains specific psycho- expectancies for success, enhanced academic
logical outcomes such as future expectancies, self-efficacy, and positive affect such as pride
self-efficacy, and affect (Weiner, 1986). These or hopefulness (Weiner, 1986). These psycho-
psychological outcomes have been further logical outcomes are in turn associated with
linked to behavioral outcomes such as engage- engagement (persistence and choice) and study
ment and achievement. skills (via self-efficacy), as well as actual
In general, research on attributions sug- achievement. Carr et al. (1991) also suggested
gests that for success it is adaptive to attribute that attributions to external factors hinders the
the success to stable, internal factors such as acquisition of strategies because external at-
ability, skill, or talent as these factors should tributions provide little reason for children to
also be present forffuture tasks (Weiner, 1986). learn strategies-suggesting that attributions
Attributions to unstable but controllable inter- are important for learning strategies.
nal factors such as effort are especially adap- Attribution theory is useful for school
tive in that effort can be modified based on the psychologists and practitioners in that individu-
demands of the situation. On the other hand, als' beliefs about the causes of events can be
for failure, attributions to factors that are un- changed through feedback and other environ-
stable are more adaptive. For instance, attrib- mental manipulations. In particular, teachers'
uting failure to lack of effort (unstable, con- reactions following success or failure can in-
trollable, internal) not only allows the student fluence students' attributions suggesting that
to protect his or her self-worth, it also helps the teacher plays an important role in the types
the student to see a way to avoid failure in the of attributions students make (Graham, 1984).
future (by exerting more effort). Attributing For example, a teacher's expression ofpity fol-
failure to bad luck (unstable, uncontrollable, lowing failure makes it more likely that a stu-

317
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

dent will attribute the failure to low ability (in- Intrinsic Motivation as Enabler of
ternal, stable, uncontrollable). Success
In addition to teachers' reactions, teach-
ers can influence students' attributions by pro- The concept of intrinsic versus extrinsic
viding feedback to students following success motivation is certainlyprevalent within social-
or failure (Licht, 1983; Pintrich & Schunk, cognitive models of motivation and is thus in-
2002). In doing so, it is first important for cluded in this review of motivation as an aca-
teachers to assess whether success or failure demic enabler. Intrinsic motivation is defined
has occurred for a particular student based on as motivation to engage in an activity for its
the student's judgments of success or failure. own sake, whereas extrinsic motivation refers
Following this, the teacher should consider the to motivation to engage in an activity as a
situation and help the student make adaptive means to an end (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002).
but accurate attributions. For failure settings, Given the space constraints of this article, it is
it is useful for students to attribute the failure not possible to discuss all the various lines of
to unstable causes as this helps to ensure that research that fall under the heading of intrin-
there is a possibility for success in the future. sic motivation. Therefore, this section focuses
For instance, it is often adaptive to attribute on personal and situational interest. However,
failure to lack of effort or inappropriate strat- it should be noted that there are many other
egy use. If the failure was due to lack of ap- multidimensional perspectives of intrinsic
propriate strategy use, it is not useful to tell motivation that deserve mention, such as Deci
the student to try hard. Rather, the teacher may and Ryan's (e.g., Deci, 1980; Deci & Ryan,
need to work with the student to help them 1985, 1991) self-determination theory. In this
develop the strategies and skills necessary to theory, one of the defining features of intrinsic
succeed in the future. For success, attributions motivation is high personal interest in the task
to effort are also adaptive, especially if the stu- or activity. Readers are urged to explore these
dent did indeed try hard. It is also adaptive to other aspects of intrinsic motivation. Sansone
attribute success to appropriate strategy use as and Harackiewicz's (2000) edited book on in-
this helps to encourage the use of strategies in trinsic and extrinsic motivation is an excellent
the future and is internal. However, success is starting place.
also possible without great effort, so when ap- As discussed earlier, interest reflects
propriate, it is also adaptive to attribute suc- what most laypeople think of when they think
cess to talent or skill. of motivation. However, similar to other con-
Research on attributions has used a vari- structs in motivation, interest is also multidi-
ety of methodologies to assess students' attribu- mensional and should not be thought of as sim-
tions including experiments, think-aloud proto- ply liking or not liking a particular task or do-
cols, and analysis of written material (Pintrich main. For instance, whereas interest in gen-
& Schunk, 2002). Some of these methodologies eral is defined as the interaction between the
could be adapted forinformal assessment by in- individual and his or her environment (Krapp,
structors. For instance, rather than creating ex- Hidi, & Renninger, 1992), interest theorists
perimental situations and assessing students' have distinguished between personal or indi-
attributions through think-aloud protocols or vidual interest and situational interest (Elidi,
rating scales, teachers or school psychologists 1990; Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000; Krapp et
could talk informally with students about why al., 1992).
they think they succeeded or failed in a spe- Personal interest reflects an individual's
cific situation. Based on these discussions, interest in a particular topic or domain (Hidi
teachers would be able to develop a better un- & Harackiewicz, 2000). It is often measured
derstanding of the attributional responses of by students' reports of how much they like or
students in their classes and determine whether enjoy a particular activity or domain. Personal
they should help certain students retrain their interest is thought to be somewhat stable over
attributional responses to success and failure. time and is partially a function of individuals'
Motivation

preferences as well as aspects of the task (e.g., achievement and other academic enablers is
Malone & Lepper, 1987). In contrast, situ- similar. Situational interest can enhance
ational interest is based entirely on the features achievement by engaging students in the task
of the learning context and may be short term or activity. Furthermore, both the catch and
or long lasting (Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000). hold components of situational interest should
For the most part, the researchers who have be associated with other. academic enablers
studied situational interest have been reading such as persistence and strategy use, at least
researchers who have focused on how differ- for the time during which situational interest
ent aspects of text can generate and sustain is activated (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002).
interest For teachers and school psychologists,
Building on this important work, these results suggest that academic achieve-
Mitchell (1993) sought to further expand the ment, study skills, and engagement can be in-
notion of situational interest beyond the read- creased by tapping into students' interests. This
ing domain. He suggested that situational in- can be done by building upon personal inter-
terest could be broken into two factors: catch est or creating situational interest. That is, al-
and hold. Catch factors are thought to stimu- lowing students to work on topics they find
late students (i.e., "catch" their attention; personally interesting may help them to engage
Mitchell, 1993). They include innovative or in such a way that they use better strategies for
novel instructional techniques such as using learning and ultimately achieve at higher lev-
an exciting computer program, having students els. One way to capitalize on personal interest
leam course material by playing a game, or al- is to allow students to pick topics for class
lowing students to work on a group project to- projects or reports. Admittedly, it is difficult
gether. In contrast, hold factors are thought to to design classroom activities that capitalize
empower students by making the content mean- on the personal interest of all students in the
ingful so students view the content as useful, or classroom; therefore, educators should also
by encouraging students' involvement in the consider ways to enhance situational interest.
task (Mitchell, 1993). For instance, emphasiz- Teachers trying to enhance situational
inghow aparticular mathematics lesson is use- interest should think about how to promote
ful for bookkeeping or planning a budget may both catch and hold factors. For instance, when
help to make the material more meaningful to designing classroom activities, having an ex-
students. Providing activities that encourage citing experiment in science or using an inno-
active student involvement such as small group vative computer program in social studies may
work or discussions rather than lectures is also spark situational interest and engage students
usefulforpromotingtheholdfactorof situational at that particular moment. This is useful for
interest. Interestresearchers have given increased engaging students in a specific activity, but may
attention to the hold factor as it seems to be a not translate into interest in future activities.
better predictor of continuing interest than the Therefore, teachers should also consider how
catch factor (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, to promote the hold factor of situational inter-
Carter, & Elliot, 2000). est, such as trying to make the topic meaning-
Both personal and situational interest ful to students. This may be done by capitaliz-
have the potential to influence academic ing on the utility of what is being learned (e.g.,
achievement and other academic enablers. For helping students see that the material is useful
instance, personal interest is positively asso- for things outside of school or for future goals).
ciated with achievement (see Krapp et al., In terms of assessment, measures of per-
1992) as well as with the use of deeper cogni- sonal interest tend to be behavioral, such as
tive strategies (Schiefele, 1991) for children continued engagement during free time or a
and adults. As might be expected, personal in- few self-report items assessing liking or en-
terest is also associated with increased atten- joyment (e.g., Barron &Harackiewicz, 2001).
tion and persistence (Hidi & Harackiewicz, For situational interest, researchers are begin-
2000). The relation of situational interest to ning to develop self-report measures of catch

319
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

and hold (e.g., Harackiewicz et al., 2000); how- The MSLQ, developed by Pintrich and
ever, these measures are still under develop- his colleagues (Pintrich et al., 1993) also in-
ment for college students and are not yet use- cludes intrinsic and extrinsic goal orientations
ful for elementary and secondary classroom and can be used to assess intrinsic and extrin-
educators. Teachers wishing to assess the de- sic motivation for junior high through college
gree of situational interest in their classes may students. A task value scale, also included in
want to observe informally students' reactions the MSLQ, consists of interest, importance,
to different instructional techniques or talk with and utility. Although task value is not the same
students about what aspects of the activities as personal interest, it does contain a personal
they find exciting or meaningful. interest component. Therefore, this scale may
There are also more general measures of be of use to educators who wish to assess stu-
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation that may be dents' interest in a particular topic area. Again,
useful to school psychologists and teachers. For it is recommended that these scales be used to
instance, Harter (1981) developed a self-report assess students' motivation in aparticular con-
scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation text.
in the classroom that can be used as part of a All of the self-report scales described
diagnostic battery to assess students' motiva- thus far use students' reports of their intrinsic
tional orientation. This scale consists of five motivation. Although there are measures that
subscales; three scales assess motivational use teacher ratings such as the Teacher Rating
components (preference for challenge, cu- of Academic Achievement Motivation
riosity/interest, mastery) and two scales as- (TRAAM; Stinnett, Oehler-Stinnett, & Stout,
sess cognitive-informational structures (in- 1991), we believe that the internal nature of
dependent judgment, internal criteria). The intrinsic motivation (as well as the other mo-
survey was designed to be used with upper tivational constructs discussed in this article)
elementary students and consists of a 4-point makes students' own self-reports more accu-
forced-choice format in which students must rate assessments of their motivation. Thus, al-
indicate whether they are similar to the in- though teacher ratings may correlate with stu-
trinsically or extrinsically motivated stu- dent ratings, the students' own ratings should
dents. Teachers who want to assess personal provide more accurate descriptions of students'
interest may find the third subscale (curiosity/ intrinsic motivation (Gottfried, 1985).
interest) particularly useful. However, as this
Adaptive Goal Orientations as Enablers
scale measures motivation at a global level,
of Success
those using this scale may wish to consider
making the items specific to a particular class The final perspective for motivation dis-
or subject area. cussed in this article is achievement goal
Gottfried's (1985) Children's Academic theory. Achievement goal theory is one of the
Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (CAIMI) can most prominent theories within motivational
also be used to measure children's intrinsic research today (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002). Goal
motivation. This self-report scale consists of theory proposes that there are two general goal
five subscales. Four of those subscales mea- orientations that concern the purposes individu-
sure intrinsic motivation separately for four als are pursuing when approaching and engag-
topic areas (reading, math, social studies, sci- ing in a task. Achievement goal theorists have
ence) and the fifth subscale assesses intrinsic used a variety of labels to refer to these two
motivation more generally. In contrast to goals including learning and performance goals
Harter's (1981) scale, the CAIMI does not (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), task and ability
measure intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation. goals (Maehr &Midgley, 1996), task-involved
Rather, this scale assesses high versus low in- and ego-involved (Nicholls, 1984), and mas-
trinsic motivation. As with other self-report tery and performance goals (Ames, 1992;
measures, this scale can be used for upper el- Elliot, 1997; Elliot & Church, 1997; Elliot &
ementary through college-aged students. Harackiewicz, 1996; Harackiewicz, Barron, &

320
Motivation

Elliot, 1998). Although there are slight varia- In our own empirical research at Michi-
tions in the interpretation of these goals under gan, we have found similar patterns in our data
these various labels, they will be referred to for mastery goals. That is, mastery goals have
here as mastery and performance goals for sim- been positively related to cognitive strategy use
plicity. Mastery goals orient learners to "de- and self-regulation as well as performance.
veloping new skills, trying to understand their These studies have shown thatjunior high stu-
work, improving their level of competence, or dents who report higher levels of mastery goals
achieving a sense of mastery based on self-ref- are more likely to use elaboration and organi-
erenced standards" (Ames, 1992, p. 262). In zational strategies as well as to be more
contrast, performance goals orient learners to metacognitive and regulating (Pintrich, 2000b;
focus on their ability and self-worth, to deter- Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). The assumption
mine their ability by outperforming others in that performance goals are maladaptive for all
competitions, surpassing others in achieve- outcomes, however, has not been confirmed
ments or grades, and receiving public recog- in our work with junior high school students
nition for their superior performance (Ames, (Pintrich, 2000b; Wolters et al., 1996) or the
1992). work of others with college students (e.g.,
In the literature on mastery and perfor- Harackiewicz et al., 1998).
mance goals, the general theoretical assump- This changing perspective for perfor-
tion has been that mastery goals foster a host mance goals stems in part from a change in
of adaptive motivational, cognitive, and the way performance goals are defined. In par-
achievement outcomes, whereas performance ticular, achievement goal theorists have begun
goals generate less adaptive or even maladap- to distinguish between approach and avoidance
tive outcomes (Ames, 1992). Moreover, this performance goal orientations (Elliot &
assumption, particularly the adaptive mastery Harackiewicz, 1996; Middleton & Midgley,
goal assumption, has been supported in a large 1997). Performance-approach goals reflect a
number of empirical studies on goals and focus on trying to outperform others, whereas
achievement processes (Ames, 1992; Dweck performance-avoid goals reflect a focus on try-
& Leggett, 1988; Pintrich, 2000c; Pintrich & ing to avoid looking incompetent in compari-
Schunk, 2002). The logic of the argument is son to others. Based on this recent develop-
that when students are focused on trying to ment, it seems clear that performance-avoid
learn and understand the material and trying goals are related to maladaptive patterns of
to improve their performance relative to their learningformiddle school and college students
own past performance, this orientation will (e.g., Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996; Middleton
help them maintain their self-efficacy in the & Midgley, 1997; Skaalvik, 1997). For per-
face of failure, ward off negative affect such formance -approach goals, the competitive
as anxiety, lessen the probability that they will urge or performance goal seems to be posi-
have distracting thoughts, and free up cogni- tively related to actual performance at least in
tive capacity, thus allowing for more cogni- terms of final course grade for college students
tive engagement and achievement. In contrast, (Harackiewicz et al., 1998). In addition, stud-
when students are concerned about trying to ies conducted with secondary and college stu-
be the best, get higher grades than others, and dents suggest that there is not necessarily a
do well compared to others under a perfor- decrement in cognitive engagement or self-
mance goal, there is the possibility that this regulation as a function of adopting a perfor-
orientation will result in more negative affect mance goal (Pintrich, 2000b, 2000c). Finally,
or anxiety, increase the possibility of distract- studies with students in junior high classrooms
ing and irrelevant thoughts (e.g., worrying also have shown that students high in perfor-
about how others are doing rather than focus- mance-approach goals and high in mastery
ing on the task), and that this will diminish goals are not more anxious, do not experience
cognitive capacity, task engagement, and per- more negative affect, and are equally motivated
formance. as those low in performance-approach goals

321
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

and high in mastery goals (Pintrich, 2000b). formance, or both, and that this focus can then
This recent research is leading to some be linked to other academic enablers as well
reconceptualization of the general theoretical as subsequent achievement in school. In mak-
assumption that mastery goals are adaptive and ing suggestions for promoting mastery and
performance goals are maladaptive, but there performance goal adoption, the focus here will
is still a need for more research on the stability be on how to emphasize mastery goals. Al-
of these findings for performance-approach though there is evidence suggesting that per-
goals. formance-approach goals may be adaptive for
In summary, empirical evidence suggests achievement, itis still unclear whetherperfor-
that the adoption of mastery goals relates posi- mance-approach goals are adaptive for all stu-
tively to school learning as well as other aca- dents in all contexts, especially when mastery
demic enablers such as study skills and engage- goals are not also adopted (Midgley, Kaplan,
ment. Forperformance goals, the picture is less & Middleton, 2001). Furthermore, many typi-
clear. There seems to be increasing evidence cal classroom contexts, especially at the sec-
that performance-approach goals are linked to ondary and college levels, tend to emphasize
academic achievement, although there is less performance goals. Therefore, the following
evidence to suggest that this link is due to an suggestions for instruction focus on pro-
increase in study skills or engagement, at least moting mastery goal adoption.
when performance goals are adopted in isola- A number of achievement goal theorists
tion from mastery goals. In relating achieve- have stressed how various structures in the
ment goals to social enablers, preliminary evi- classroom environment may influence stu-
dence suggests that a number of social vari- dents' perceptions ofthe classroom goal struc-
ables such as a sense of belonging in school, ture, which in turn is thought to relate to stu-
the endorsement of social responsibility goals dents' own adoption of mastery versus perfor-
(adhering to social norms or rules), a desire to mance goals (e.g., Ames, 1992; Blumenfeld,
be accepted by the peer group, and attempts to 1992; Meece, 1991). Classroom structure re-
achieve social status help to predict the types fers to the way the teacher establishes routines,
of achievement goals students adopt (e.g., sets up rules, assigns tasks, and evaluates stu-
Anderman & Anderman, 1999). In particular, dents. The classroom structure is often catego-
Anderman and Anderman found that students rized along the dimensions of task, authority,
who felt a sense of belonging were more likely and recognition or evaluation structures and
to adopt a mastery goal orientation and less these structures can be altered in classrooms
likely to adopt a performance goal orientation. to promote either a mastery or performance
Furthermore, endorsing social reasonability orientation (e.g., Ames, 1992; Blumenfeld,
goals positively predicted the adoption ofmas- 1992; Maehr & Anderman, 1993; Maehr &
tery goals. In contrast, students who focused Midgley, 1991, 1996; Meece, 1991).
on developing social relationships and achiev- The types of tasks used in the class-
ing social status were more likely to endorse room can convey very different messages
performance goals. to students regarding the general goal struc-
A few instructional suggestions can be ture of the classroom. To encourage mas-
made based on the situated nature of achieve- tery goal adoption, Ames (1992) recom-
ment goals. That is, achievement goal theory mends using tasks that are meaningful and
suggests that differences in the way the class- appropriately challenging. For instance,
room or school environment is perceived are rather than having students complete a se-
linked to students' goal adoption (e.g., Maehr ries of worksheets, having students engage
&Anderman, 1993; Roeser, Midgley, &Urdan, in hands-on, applied activities in math and
1996; Urdan, Midgley, & Anderman, 1998). science can help to challenge students and
This has important implications for teachers, help them see how what they are learning
namely that teachers can shape the classroom in school relates to things outside of school.
environment to focus students on mastery, per- Furthermore, using a variety of tasks not

322
Motivation

only allows students to choose among tasks terested in assessing the goal orientations of
they find personally interesting but also helps their own students or students' perceptions of
to decrease the opportunity for social compari- the classroom can administerPALS to students
son. The authority structure of the classroom in their own class or school. This might be par-
also sends important messages regarding the ticularly useful if one is interested in seeing if
achievement goal-orientation of the classroom. students perceive changes to the classroom or
By allowing students to have autonomy in the school goal structure. However, itis important
classroom and reducing the authority of the to note that reports of mastery goals and mas-
teacher, mastery goals can be promoted (Ames, tery goal structures tend to be high, making it
1992). For instance, giving students a set of more difficult to detect changes in the class-
assignments and allowing them to choose the room or school goal structure based on reform
order in which they will complete them helps efforts.
to grant autonomy to the students and thus en-
Future Directions and Limitations
courage mastery. Evaluation and recognition
are often quite salient to students and play a Although a great deal of research has
large role in setting the "tone" of the classroom already been conducted on students' motiva-
as emphasizing mastery or performance. To tion in academic settings (elementary through
fostermastery goal adoption, evaluation should college), there is a clear need for additional
focus on individual improvement as well as research. This last section focuses on four ar-
mastery of ideas. For instance, evaluating stu- eas for future research. First, although the cur-
dents on their improvement over several drafts rent theories of motivation are social cogni-
of a written assignment can help to focus stu- tive and emphasize both individual and con-
dents on learning rather than on performing textual influences, much of the research within
better than others. In addition, private rather achievement motivation has focused on moti-
than public recognition of students' efforts and vation at the individual level. Educational re-
improvement helps to promote a mastery-ori- searchers are urged to further examine how
ented learning environment. specific features of the classroom environment
To assess achievement goals, it is impor- might affect a variety of aspects of students'
tant to consider students' personal goal orien- motivation. In doing so, researchers need to
tations as well as the classroom or school goal move beyond self-report measures of motiva-
structure. One assessment that has been used tion and consider other methodologies such as
in a variety of cultures and age ranges is PALS, classroom observations. For instance, a num-
which was developedbyMidgley and her col- ber of achievement goal theorists (e.g., Patrick,
leagues (Midgley et al., 1998). As noted ear- Anderman, Ryan, Edelin, & Midgley, 2001;
lier, PALS is a self-report questionnaire that Turner et al., 2002) have used classroom ob-
can be administered to students from fourth servations to enhance or supplement self-re-
grade through college. To assess individual port data and this certainly seems to be pro-
achievement goals, students respond to a se- viding richer descriptions of how motivation
ries of questions about their orientations to- plays out in classroom contexts.
wards achievement situations. As these orien- A second related direction for future re-
tations are thought to be situation-specific, it search is the use of intervention studies. That
is recommended that the items ask students to is, if the context plays a central role as sug-
focus on a particular context or subject area gested by the theories discussed in this article,
such as math class. The classroom goal struc- it should be possible to design intervention
ture can be assessed through self-reports from studies to promote the development of adap-
students (i.e., what students think is empha- tivemotivationalbeliefs such as increased self-
sized in the classroom) as well as teacher's self- efficacy; attributions to internal, controllable
reports of their instructional practices. Al- factors; interest in academic tasks; and mas-
though these measures were designed for the tery-goal orientations. However, much of the
purposes of classroom research, educators in- research within the field of achievement moti-

323
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

vation is correlational or conducted in lab-like models to consider affective processes


experimental contexts that do not reflect typi- (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2002). Although
cal learning environments. Educational re- emotions are central to attribution theory, and
searchers need to collaborate with teachers and research certainly considers both the affective
administrators to implement the suggested and value components of interest, it is antici-
changes to the context and evaluate the effec- pated that other, more cognitive, motivational
tiveness of these changes. Although this is cer- constructs such as achievement goals and self-
tainly a large task and, based on reports of those efficacy will expand to include emotions as
who have attempted this (e.g., Maehr & well as cognitionsin their understanding of mo-
Midgley, 1996; Midgley &Maehr, 1999), does tivation. Considering the role of affective pro-
not always provide the type of empirical data cesses in motivational theories should aid in
one may desire, it is essential that these theo- the development of more nuanced and accu-
ries of motivation are directly applied to stu- rate models of motivational processes in
dents' learning. school. The inclusion of affective processes
A third direction for future research is also may be particularly important for students
more careful study of how motivational pro- who are having serious problems in school
cesses may differ for various ethnic groups. (e.g., those with learning disabilities or more
Much of the initial research on motivation was severe emotional and behavioral adjustment
based on Caucasian college students, and mo- problems).
tivational researchers have made efforts to ex-
tend and test various motivational theories for Conclusion
elementary and secondary students with dif- This article has focused on four keys
ferent ethnic and cultural backgrounds (Gra- components of student motivation. Based on
ham, 1994; Zusho & Pintrich, in press). How- research on these four components, sugges-
ever, there is a need to continue this type of tions have been offered regarding what teach-
research and to expand it to more carefully ers and school psychologists can do to promote
consider whether motivational processes dif- students' motivation and several motivational
fer for students from varied cultural back- assessments such as PALS (Midgley et al.,
grounds. The key issue in this type of research 1998) and the MSLQ (Pintrich et al., 1993)
is not general comparisons of the different have been suggested. These suggestions are
groups in terms of mean-level differences, but based on the multidimensional view of mo-
rather that the relations among the motivational tivation as well as the idea that motivation
constructs and other outcomes of academic is not a stable trait but reflects an interac-
success such as engagement, self-regulation, tion between the context and what the stu-
study skills, and achievement are similar within dent brings to the context. Accordingly, it
the groups. If the relations are similar, then is inappropriate to label students as "moti-
the motivational models and constructs are vated" or "unmotivated"; rather, school psy-
proving useful in understanding the groups' chologists and other educators are urged to
motivation and achievement. If the relations consider ways in which the learning envi-
are not similar, the models may need to be ronment can be altered to enhance all stu-
changed or supplemented with other con- dents' motivation based on a variety of mo-
structs. tivational constructs including academic
Finally, at a more theoretical level, so- self-efficacy, attributions, intrinsic motiva-
cial cognitive models of motivation have tion, and achievement goals. Although there
tended to weigh heavily on cognition. Thus, is certainly a need for additional research
although motivation and affect have been on how specific changes to the classroom con-
linked historically, many current social cogni- text influence multiple aspects of students' mo-
tive theories downplay the role of affect or tivation in school, it is clear from what is
emotions. It is hoped that future motivational known that the context shapes students' moti-
researchers will expand these social cognitive vation, engagement, strategy use, and achieve-

324
Motivation

ment. Therefore, teachers and school psycholo- Elliot,A. J., &Church, M.A. (1997). Ahierarchical model
of approach and avoidance achievement modvation.
gists are urged to focus on changes that can be Joumal ofPersonalityand SocialPsychology, 72, 218-
made to the school or classroom environments 232.
to help all students, rather than citing lack of Elliot, A. J., &Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and
motivation for a particular student as a reason avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivadon:
A mediational analysis. Joumal of Personality and
forlower than expected academic performance. Social Psychology, 70,461-475.
Gottfried, A. E. (1985). Academic intrinsic motivation in
References elementary and junior high school students. Journal
of EducationalPsychology, 77,631-645.
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and stu- Graham, S. (1984). Communicating sympathy and anger
dent motivation. Joumalof Educational Psychology, to black and white students: The cognitive
84, 261-271. (attribudonal) consequences of affective cues. Jour-
Anderman, E. M., &Anderman, L.H.(1999). Social pre- nalof Personalityand Social Psychology, 47,40-54.
dictors of changes in students' achievement goal ori- Graham, S. (1994). Motivation in African Americans.
entadons. ContemporaryEducationalPsychology, 25, Review of EducationalResearch, 64, 55-117.
21-37. Graham, S., &Weiner, B. (1996). Theories and principles
Bandura,A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. of motivadon. In D. C. Berliner &R. Calfee (Eds.),
New York: Freeman. Handbookofeducationalpsychology(pp. 63-84). New
Barron, K E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achieve- York Macmillan.
ment goals and optimal motivation: Testing multiple Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., &Elliot, A. J. (1998).
goal models. Joumal of Personalityand Social Psy- Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adap-
chology, 80,706-722. tive for college students and why? Educational Psy-
Blumenfeld, P.(1992). Classroom learning and motiva- chologist, 33,1-21.
tion: Clarifying and expanding goal theory. Joumalof Harackiewicz, . M., Barron, K E., Tauer, J. M., Carter,
EducationalPsychology, 84, 272-281. S. M., &Elliot, A. J. (2000). Short-term and long-term
Bong, M. (2001). Between- and within-domain rela- consequences of achievement goals: Predicdng inter-
tions of academic motivation among middle and est and performance over time. Joumal ofEducational
high school students: Self-efficacy, task value, and Psychology, 92,316-330.
achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psy- Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic ver-
chology, 93, 23-34. sus extrinsic orientadon in the classroom: Motivadonal
Borkowski, J.G., Weyhing, R.S., &Carr, M. (1988). Ef- and informational components. Developmental Psy-
fects of attribudonal retraining on strategy-based read- chology, 17,300-312.
ing comprehension inlearning-disabled students. Jour- Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental
nal of EducationalPsychology, 80,46-53. resourceforlearning.Revie;v ofEducationalResearch,
Carr, M., Borkowski, J. G., & Maxwell, S. E. (1991). 60, 549-571.
Motivational components of underachievement. De- Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivadng the
velopmental Psychology, 27, 108-118. academically unmoivated: A critical issue for the 21st
Deci, E. L. (1980). Thepsychology ofself-determination. century.ReviewvofEducationalResearch,70,151-179.
Lexington, MA: D.C.Heath. Krapp, A., Hidi, S., &Renninger, K. A. (1992). Interest,
Deci, E. L., &Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation learning, and developmenL In K A. Renninger, S. Hidi,
andself-determinationin human behavior.NewYork: &A.Krapp (Eds.), The role of interestin leamingand
Plenum. development (pp. 3-25). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Deci, E. L., &Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational ap- Erlbaum Associates.
proach to self: Integration in personality. In R. A. Licht, B. G. (1983). Cognitive-motivational factors that
Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraskasymposium on motivation contribute to the achievement of learning-disabled
1990 (Vol. 38, pp. 237-288). Lincoln, NE: University children.JoumalofLeamingDisabilities,16,483-490.
of Nebraska Press. Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Achieve-
Dweck, C., &Leggett, E. (1988). A social-cognitive ap- ment goal theory and affect: An asymmetrical bidirec-
proach to motivation and personality. Psychological tional model. EducationalPsychologist, 37, 69-78.
Review, 95,256-273. Maehr, M. L., &Anderman, E. (1993). Reinventing
Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motiva- schools for early adolescents: Emphasizing task goals.
tion to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. ElementarySchool Joumal,93, 593-610.
Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of childpsychology: Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1991). Enhancing student
Vol. 3. Social, emotional, andpersonalitydevelopment motivadon: Aschoolwideapproach.EducationalPsy-
(5"' ed., pp. 1017-1095). New York: Wiley. chologist, 26, 399427.
Elliot, A. (1997). Integrating the "classic" and "contem- Maehr,M.L., &Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school
porary" approaches to achievement motivation: A hi- cultures. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
erarchical model of approach and avoidance achieve- Malone, T. W., &Lepper, M. R. (1987). Making learning
ment motivation. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich fun: A taxonomy of intrinsic motivations from learn-
(Eds.), Advances in motivation andachievement (Vol. ing. InR. E. Snow &M. J. Farr (Eds.),Aptitude, leam-
10, pp. 143-179). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. ing, andinstruction:Vol3: Conativeand affectivepro-

325
School Psychology Review, 2002, Volume 31, No. 3

cess analysis (pp. 223-253). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W.
Erlbaum. (1993). Predictive validity and reliability of the Moti-
Meece, J. L. (1991). The classroom context and students' vated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ).
motivational goals. In M. L. Maehr &R R. Pintrich EducationalandPsychologicalMeasurement,53,801-
(Eds.),Advances in motivation and achievement(Vol. 813.
7, pp. 261-286). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Roeser, R., Midgley, C., &Urdan, T. (1996). Perceptions
Middleton, M., &Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the dem- of the school psychological environment and early
onstration of lack of ability: An under-explored aspect adolescents' psychological and behavioral functioning
of goal theory. JoumalofEducationalPsychology,89, in school: The mediating role of goals and belonging.
710-718. Joumal of EducationalPsychology,88,408-422.
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., &Middleton, M. (2001). Perfor- Sansone, C., &Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds). (2000). Intrin-
mance-approach goals: Good for what, for whom, un- sic and extrinsic motivation: The searchfor optimal
der what circumstances, and at what cost? Joumal of motivation and performance. San Diego: Academic
EducationalPsychology, 93, 77-86. Press.
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., Middleton, M., Maehr, M. L., Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation.
Urdan, T., Anderman, L. H., Anderman, E., &Roeser, EducationalPsychologist, 26, 299-323.
R. (1998). The development and validation of scales Schunk, D. H. (1989a). Self-efficacy and achievement
assessing students' achievement goal orientations. behaviors.EducationalPsychologyReview1,173-208.
ContemporaryEducationalPsychology, 23,113-131. Schunk, D. H. (1989b). Self-efficacy and cognitive skill
Midgley, C., &Maehr, M. L. (1999). Using motivational learning. In C. Ames &R. Ames (Eds.), Research on
theory to guide school reform. In A. J. Reynolds, H. J. motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 13-44). San Di-
Walberg, &R. R Weissberg (Eds.), Promotingposi- ego: Academic Press.
tive outcomes: Issues in children's andfamilies'lives Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic moti-
(pp. 129-159). Washington, DC: CWLA Press/Child vation. EducationalPsychologist, 26,207-231.
Welfare League of America. Skaalvik, E. M. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeat-
Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted ing ego orientation: Relations with task and avoidance
structure in the secondary school mathematics class- orientation, achievement, self-perceptions, and anxi-
room.Joumal ofEducationalPsychology,85,424-436 ety. Journal ofEducationalPsychology,89,71-81.
Nicholls, L.(1984).Achievement motivation: Conceptions Stinnett, T. A., Oehler-Stinnett, J., &Stout, L. J. (1991).
of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and per- Development of the teacherrating of academic achieve-
formance. PsychologicalReview, 91, 328-346. ment motivation: TRAAM. SchoolPsychologyReview,
Patrick, H., Anderman, L. H., Ryan, A. M., Edelin, K. C., 20, 609-622.
& Midgley, C. (2001). Teachers' communication of Turner, J. C., Midgley, C., Meyer, D. K., Gheen, M.,
goal orientations in four fifth-grade classrooms. El- Anderman, E. M., Kang, Y., &Patrick, H. (2002). The
ementary School Joumal, 102,35-58. classroom environment and students' reports of avoid-
Patrick, H., Hicks, L., &Ryan, A. M. (1997). Relations of ance strategies in mathematics: A multimethod study.
perceived social efficacy and social goal pursuit to self- Jourmal of EducationalPsychology,94, 88-106.
efficacy for academic work. Journalof EarlyAdoles- Urdan, T. C., Midgley, C., &Anderman, E. M. (1998).
cence, 17, 109-128. The role of classroom goal structure in students' use
Pintrich, P. R. (2000a). An achievement goal theory per- of self-handicapping.AmericanEducationalResearch
spective on issues in motivation termiinology, theory, Joumal, 35,101-122.
and research. ContemporaryEducationalPsychology, Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement
25, 92-104. motivation and emotion. Psychological Reviewv, 92,
Pintrich,P. R. (2000b). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: 548-573.
The role of goal orientation in learning and achieve- Weiner, B. (1986). An attributionaltheory of motivation
ment JourmalofEducationalPsychology,92,544-555. and emotion. New York. Springer-Verlag.
Pintrich, R R. (2000c). The role of goal orientation in self- Wolters, C., Yu, S., &Pintrich, P.(1996). The relation be-
regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, &M. tween goal orientation and students' motivational be-
Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Theory, liefs and self-regulated learning. Leaming and Indi-
research and applications (pp. 451-502). San Diego, vidualDifferences, 8,211-238.
CA: Academic Press. Zusho, A., &Pintrich, P. R. (in press). A process-oriented
Pintrich, P. R., &De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and approach to culture: Theoretical and methodological
self-regulated learning components of classroom aca- issues in the study of culture and motivation. In E Salili
demic performance. Joumal ofEducationalPsychol- & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Research in multicultural edu-
ogy, 82, 33-40. cation and internationalperspectives: VoL 3. Teach-
Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. (2002). Motivation in edu- ing, leaming andstudentmotivationin amulticultural
cation: Theory, research,and applications(2nd ed.). context.
Upper Saddle, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

326
Motivation

Elizabeth A. Linnenbrink received her Ph. D. in Education and Psychology from the Uni-
versity of Michigan, Ann Arbor in 2002. She is an Assistant Professor of Educational
Psychology in Foundations ofEducation at the University of Toledo. Her research focuses
on the way achievement motivation is linked to cognitive processing and emotional well-
being in school contexts.

Paul R. Pintrich received his Ph.D. in Education and Psychology from the University of
Michigan,AnnArborin 1982. He is aProfessor ofEducation andPsychology and Chair of
the Combined Program in Education and Psychology at the University of Michigan, Ann
Arbor. His research focuses on the development of motivation and self-regulated learn-
ing.

327
COPYRIGHT INFORMATION

TITLE: Motivation as an Enabler for Academic Success


SOURCE: The School Psychology Review 31 no3 2002
WN: 0200300759004

The magazine publisher is the copyright holder of this article and it


is reproduced with permission. Further reproduction of this article in
violation of the copyright is prohibited. To contact the publisher:
http://www.nasponline.org/index2.html.

Copyright 1982-2002 The H.W. Wilson Company. All rights reserved.

S-ar putea să vă placă și