Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

Skepticism and the Winter Management Accountant: 2008 Insights for Fraud Detection

VOL.9 NO.2

B Y K I M B E R L Y F. C H A R R O N , P H . D . , C M A ,

AND

D. JORDAN LOWE, PH.D.

SURVEY EXAMINES THE LEVEL OF PROFESSIONAL SKEPTICISM AMONG MANAGEMENT

ACCOUNTANTS.

WITH

INCREASED EMPHASIS BEING PLACED ON DETECTING AND PRE-

VENTING FRAUD, COMPANIES MAY BENEFIT FROM ENHANCING THE ABILITIES OF THEIR EMPLOYEES TO EXHIBIT SKEPTICISM.

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY Skepticism has long been held as a fundamental concept for the public accounting profes-

sion. Exhibiting skepticism is presumed to increase the likelihood of detecting fraud in the conduct of an audit. With increasing incidences of fraud within organizations, however, the accounting profession as a whole needs to embrace the concept of skepticism. This study examines skepticism within the management accounting profession. We measure the levels of skepticism within a group of management accountants and examine characteristics associated with higher levels of skepticism. We also provide insights into the implications of skepticism and suggestions on how firms can capitalize on this concept to increase fraud detection.

ebsters Dictionary defines skepticism as an attitude of doubt or a disposition to incredulity either in general or toward a particular object. In the accounting profession, skepticism has long been recognized as a fundamental principle of auditing. The first codification of auditing standards, Statement of Auditing Standards No. 1 (SAS 1), Codification of Auditing Standards and Procedures, promulgates the idea that auditors must use professional skepticism when engaged in an audit. Skepticism is defined in the auditing standards as an

attitude that includes a questioning mind, a critical assessment and objective evaluation of audit evidence, and a willingness to suspend judgment about the honesty of client management. In 2002, the American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA) issued SAS 99, Consideration of Fraud in a Financial Statement Audit, which addresses skepticism and emphasizes the auditors responsibility to explicitly consider the possibility of fraud on every engagement.1 The rationale behind these standards is that high levels of professional skepticism (i.e., assuming a more questioning attitude) enhance the ability to detect fraud.

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

Despite auditing standards that require professional skepticism, 60% of the Securities & Exchange Commission (SEC) cases against auditors cite a lack of professional skepticism as the cause for action.2 In response to the SECs concern about the quality of audits, the Public Oversight Board (POB) issued a recommendation that audit firms should teach the concept of professional skepticism more effectively.3 The implications are that professional skepticism is a learned skill that can be enhanced with training rather than strictly an innate quality. Lending support to the SECs and POBs concerns about the skepticism among auditors, a 2004 fraud survey by the Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE) revealed that only 11% of the frauds committed were detected by external auditors.4 The survey shows it is far more likely that fraud will be uncovered by employees of the organization rather than by the external auditors. While internal auditors faired significantly better, detecting 24% of fraud cases, nearly 40% were discovered through a tip. Of the tips that led to the discovery of fraud, 60% came from company employees. The importance of employees as a source of fraud detection may be understated in these findings. Another 16% of the tips came from anonymous hotlines, which are widely used by employees. The abundance of tips adds credibility to the Sarbanes-Oxley Act (SOX) mandate that audit committees establish internal reporting mechanisms such as hotlines.
TYPES
OF

uct for 2003 translates to approximately $660 billion in total losses, up from $400 billion in 1996 and $600 billion in 2002. Furthermore, once an organization has been defrauded, it is unlikely to recover its losses. The survey shows that victim organizations recover an average of 20% of the original loss, and 40% of the companies recover nothing at all. Small organizations represent the greatest proportion of reported fraud cases (nearly 46%), but the cost per incident of fraud is often higher in large organizations. While the true cost of fraud to any single organization is hard to measure, the magnitude and prevalence of fraud in our society should put all organizations on alert.
M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N TA N T S F R AU D D E T E C T I O N
AND

F R AU D

AND

THEIR COSTS

There are three general categories of fraud: asset misappropriation, corruption, and fraudulent financial statements. The majority of frauds (90% of reported cases) are in asset misappropriations, which involve the theft or misuse of an organizations assets, such as skimming, stealing inventory, and payroll fraud. Fraudulent financial statements make up the smallest percentage of reported fraud cases (7.9%), but they represent the highest median losses per case ($1,000,000) and are more likely to be publicized.5 The cost of fraud to an organization is staggering. The ACFE 2004 survey shows that the typical U.S. organization loses 6% of its annual revenue to fraud. Applying this statistic to the U.S. Gross Domestic Prod-

Preventing fraud is a big responsibility for all levels of financial management from staff accountant to CFO. As financial professionals, the accounting staff is often responsible for establishing cost-effective controls to prevent fraudulent behavior. It is unlikely that all frauds will be prevented with even the best internal controls. Therefore, every organization should have the goal of detecting fraud early in order to minimize its financial impact. Even though management accountants are not under the same regulations as external auditors, they should adopt an elevated attitude of skepticism to detect fraud within their respective organizations. Management accountants often have insights to details that may go unnoticed or unaudited by external or internal auditors. They certainly have intimate knowledge of financial activities, the corporate environment, and employee actions. This insider perspective can put them in a position to detect many symptoms of fraud that may not be as obvious to an outsider, such as an employee who never takes vacations or invoices that come from unknown companies. This is particularly important given that the typical fraudster may not provide obvious, discernable signs of fraud. The majority of white-collar crimes are committed by middle-aged men who are well educated, have been with the organization for several years, are in a position of trust, and have no criminal history. These individuals, however, exhibit noticeable changes in their behavior at work.6 Manage-

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

10

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

ment accountants would thus be in a better position to recognize some of these behavioral signs than an outsider who has limited contact. Because professional skepticism is the standard of behavior for fraud detection, it may be beneficial to understand how well management accountants utilize professional skepticism. The purpose of this study is to measure the professional skepticism of management accountants and compare it to the skepticism levels of internal and external auditors. This is the first study that we are aware of that has objectively measured the skepticism levels of management accountants. By having a better understanding of professional skepticism, the management accounting profession can develop guidelines to implement it more effectively. We also look at different demographic characteristics that may correlate with higher levels of professional skepticism. Finally, we draw from the literature to provide suggestions on how organizations can increase the skeptical behaviors of those who may be inherently less skeptical.
MEASURING SKEPTICISM

comprehensive measure of this construct.10 Hurtts skepticism scale draws from philosophy as well as research in accounting and other disciplines. Her scale is based on characteristics and behaviors that historically have been associated with skepticism. It presumes professional skepticism is a multidimensional construct composed of six individual characteristics falling within three general categories: examining evidence, understanding evidence providers, and acting on evidence. Examining Evidence Hurtts scale is drawn from the theoretical development of skepticism found in Professional Skepticism: A Model with Implications for Research, Practice, and Education, written by Hurtt, Martha Eining, and David Plumlee (HEP).11 HEP argue that the three characteristics of skepticism that deal with examining evidence are a questioning mind, suspension of judgment, and a search for knowledge. A questioning mind means skeptics are unlikely to accept information at face value; instead, they require proof or justification. Skeptics can see the other side of arguments and often enjoy the role of devils advocate. It has also been argued that skeptics suspend judgments in order to make additional inquiry and obtain evidence. Suspension of judgment means skeptics are slower to form judgments and not likely to jump to conclusions. Finally, a search for knowledge is equivalent to being generally curious. Skeptics are individuals who enjoy the learning process and seek knowledge for knowledges sake. Understanding Evidence Providers This aspect of skepticism is also referred to as interpersonal understanding, and it involves an individuals interest in the motivations and integrity of the evidence providers. HEP suggest a skeptic has the ability to recognize that different people will have different personal views of the same event. Skepticism aids individuals to see past the obvious to question the source of information even when a person appears completely trusting. In public accounting, auditors are advised to be cautious regarding management assertions. The implication is that managers are just looking out for their (or the firms) best interests and will misrepresent the facts.

The concept of skepticism has been discussed by philosophers throughout time. Despite all the concern with skepticism in the accounting profession, however, there is no universal way of measuring the construct. Accounting literature defines skepticism in a variety of ways. For example, Jeffrey J. McMillan and Richard A. White suggest that an auditor that uses an error-based explanation of questionable evidences (as opposed to naturally occurring) is more skeptical.7 Skepticism has also been defined by the outcomes it manifests: confronting a client or performing extra work.8 Other studies support the contention that skeptical behaviors can be induced. Researchers found that auditors who were told to stress professional skepticism were more apt to increase their audit hours and perform a higher-quality audit than those encouraged to be efficient or objective.9 The inconsistent measurement of skepticism has certainly hampered research efforts and the accounting professions ability to apply findings to practice.
HURTT SKEPTICISM SCALE

Kathy Hurtt developed a skepticism scale to explicitly measure an individuals level of skepticism, providing a

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

11

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

Figure 1:

The Hurtt Scale

Understanding Evidence Providers


x Interpersonal Understanding (0.883)
q I am interested in what causes people to behave the way that they do q Other peoples behavior doesnt interest me* q I like to understand the reason for other peoples behavior q I seldom consider why people behaving in a certain way* q The actions people take and the reason for those actions are fascinating

Examining Evidence
x Questioning Mind (0.638)*
q I often reject statements unless I have proof that they are true q My friends tell me that I often question things that I see or hear q I frequently question things that I see or hear

x Suspension of Judgment (0.832)


q I wait to decide on issues until I can get more information q I take my time when making decisions q I dislike having to make decisions quickly q I dont like to decide until Ive looked at all of the available information q I like to ensure that Ive considered most available information before making a decision

Acting on Evidence
x Self-confidence (0.933)
q I feel good about myself q I am confident of my abilities q I am self-assured q I dont feel sure of myself* q I have confidence in myself

x Search for Knowledge (0.958)


q The prospect of learning excites me q Discovering new information is fun q I think that learning is exciting q I like searching for knowledge q I enjoy trying to determine if what I read or hear is true q I relish learning

x Self-determination (0.779)
q I often accept other peoples explanations without further thought* q I tend to immediately accept what other people tell me* q I usually accept things I see, read, or hear at face value* q I usually notice inconsistencies in explanations q Most often I agree with what the others in my group think* q It is easy for other people to convince me* * These questions were reverse coded.

The truth may be that they simply see things differently. A skeptic understands the other point of view and can investigate accordingly. Acting on Evidence The last portion of the skepticism scale focuses on personal initiative to act on information. HEP define the characteristics in this section as self-confidence and self-determination. By exhibiting higher levels of selfconfidence, a skeptic is more likely to take the initiative to act on the information he or she finds questionable.

With high levels of self-determination, a skeptic likely will continue to collect information until satisfied that there is sufficient information. Self-confidence and selfdetermination make the skeptic better able to value his or her personal insights and have the courage to challenge the positions of others. Hurtts scale, which uses these characteristics as the basis to measure professional skepticism, consists of a series of 30 questions designed to elicit respondents self-assessment of their skepticism characteristics (see Figure 1). The scale has been exhaustively pilot tested

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

12

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

and refined to arrive at its final state. It was also tested and retested several weeks later to provide validation for intrasubject test-retest reliability. Researchers using the Hurtt scale have provided consistent evidence of its robustness across internal auditors, external auditors, and accounting students. The use of the skepticism scale also has been validated in studies that link the level of skepticism with expected behaviors associated with skepticism. In this study, we focus our attention on the professional skepticism levels among management accountants.
T H E S T U DY

were women. Respondents had an average of 19.4 years of experience and an average age of 46.6. Twenty six of the respondents were certified as a Certified Management Accountant (CMA), Certified Public Accountant (CPA), or both. Other demographic details can be seen in Table 1.
R E S U LT S

The Hurtt Skepticism Scale was administered to 59 members of the Institute of Management Accountants (IMA) in the southwestern United States. Participants were directed to the survey website through a link on their chapter website. The questionnaire was completed entirely online. This allowed respondents to maintain anonymity and enabled them to take the survey at their convenience, ensuring they had adequate time to respond to the questions. Management accountants who participated in this study held a wide variety of positions ranging from staff accountant to CEO, with the majority in the controller position (34%). Sixty-six percent of the respondents

Respondents answered the 30 skepticism scale questions shown in Figure 1 in random order using a sixpoint scale where one was strongly disagree and six was strongly agree. The eight questions that are reverse coded are adjusted accordingly. Factor analysis was run on the 30 questions, and the individual loading factors did not exactly correspond with the preidentified constructs that Hurtt defined. But Hurtt indicates that the focus of the professional skepticism model should be on the measure as a whole and that the subscales are not necessarily designed to be used independently. As such, the responses are then summed to generate an overall skepticism score that ranges from 30 (low skepticism) to 180 (high skepticism). In assessing the inter-item reliability of the 30question scale, our analysis provides a Cronbachs alpha score of 0.88. This indicates a high inter-item reliability for the scale as a whole. In addition, Cronbachs alpha is

Table 1:

Demographic Profile of Respondents


Mean Std Dev 10.9 10.0 N 20 39 13 38 8 37 22 4 13 9 Min 26 1 Max 76 42 % 33.9 66.1 22.0 64.4 13.6 62.7 37.3 6.8 22.0 15.3

CONTINUOUS VARIABLES

Age (years) Accounting Experience (years)

46.6 19.4 Level

Gender Marital Status

Male Female Single Married Divorced Yes No CMA CPA CMA & CPA

Children Certification

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

13

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

greater than 0.70 for five of the six constructs, with two constructs having scores greater than 0.90. These results are similar to the results found for samples of internal and external auditors, suggesting the scale is consistent and reliable for use across the accounting profession. Overall, the skepticism score for management accountants is 140.2, ranging from a low of 103 to a high of 172. This score is significantly higher than that of business students (132.7).12 The score is very similar to the aggregate score of internal auditors (140.3) and slightly above the scores of external auditors (138.7) from other studies. This suggests that the skepticism of management accountants is similar to that of auditorswhether internal or external. Moreover, this study shows that while others may conjecture that external and/or internal auditors have greater skepticism skills in detecting fraud or questioning red flags, this clearly is not the case. We also examined demographic characteristics to determine whether they have any relationship (correlation) with levels of skepticism (see Table 2). For example, other researchers have found that, as a result of having little exposure to fraud over time, auditors tend to become less skeptical as they advance in their careers.13 The results of our study find no relation between experience and skepticism. The respondents above and below the median experience levels had sim-

ilar skepticism scores (similar results were also found for age). One interesting result, however, it that respondents with a CMA, CPA, or both have higher skepticism scores than those without certification. This result is consistent with the study of internal auditors by Cindy Durtschi and Rosemary Fullerton.14 We also found that women scored higher than men, and respondents with children scored higher than those without children. Single, unmarried individuals scored lower than married or divorced respondents. While there is no preconceived expectation that gender, children, or marital status would impact skepticism, these results indicate that life situations can alter the degree of skepticism within individuals. Future research should examine the relationship between demographic characteristics and skepticism to provide additional insights.
I M P L I C AT I O N S

Table 2:

Skepticism Results
MEAN 140.2 103172 0.88

OVERALL RESULTS Score Range Cronbachs Alpha DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS Experience (above median) Experience (below median) Certified (CMA, CPA, or both) Not Certified Women Men With Children Without Children Married or Divorced Single

139.4 140.1 142.6 137.5 140.4 137.5 142.2 135.9 141.0 135.1

The level of professional skepticism displayed by employees likely will vary. Yet these differences do not imply that employees who are less skeptical do not have the potential to improve their abilities. While researchers have found that higher levels of skepticism are associated with individuals questioning data and collecting more evidence to support decisions, individuals who score low on the skepticism scale can be trained to exhibit behaviors consistent with those who score higher.15 Understanding the differences in skepticism levels can help firms in developing their training strategy. Staff members who are less skeptical may need additional training at regular intervals to reinforce the types of skills needed to detect fraud. Ongoing fraud training may help employees identify risk factors and help them become more sensitive to clues and red flags that suggest fraudulent behavior. Research shows that even novices who receive practice and feedback with fraud detection exhibit higher levels of skepticism and are better able to detect fraud when it exists.16 Unlike auditors, who have a responsibility to explicitly consider fraud in every engagement, management accountants have no similar requirement. It is important to note, however, that increasing fraud awareness can facilitate fraud detection. Accountants can be made aware of fraudulent activities that are discovered within

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

14

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

the firm or in other firms through newsletters or other forms of communication. Providing all employees with the common signs of individuals who commit fraud can help them identify potential fraud activities. Even subtle reminders at staff meetings or company-wide events that emphasize the importance of having a skeptical attitude may help deter fraud. Another fraud prevention and detection strategy organizations may want to consider is encouraging their finance and accounting employees to earn a certification. Accountants who study for certification expand their knowledge base, which in turn may make them more cognizant of irregularities when they encounter them and increase their levels of skepticism. Certification may also increase self-confidence, giving these individuals more courage to question situations, transactions, or the behavior of colleagues. In conclusion, research on professional skepticism has been a difficult construct to administer and measure. With the Hurtt Skepticism Scale, however, an objective measure of skepticism can be obtained. This study is the first to examine the skepticism levels of management accountants. While it offers some initial findings and conclusions, future research should expand on these results. For example, research should examine whether management accountants who are identified as being highly skeptical would also rate high in certain behaviors such as information search, contradiction detection, alternative generation, and expanded scrutiny of interpersonal information. That is, does a higher level of skepticism among management accountants lead to increased fraud detection? Are management accountants who are more skeptical also more likely to work slower than their counterparts? Finally, which teaching and training methods have the most potential for instilling an understanding of and ability to exhibit appropriate levels of skepticism? s Data used in this study is available from the authors upon request. We wish to thank the Institute of Management Accountants for their cooperation in obtaining participants for this research study.

Kimberly F. Charron, Ph.D., CMA, is an associate professor at the University of Nevada, Las Vegas. You can contact her at (702) 895-3975 or kim.charron@unlv.edu. D. Jordan Lowe, Ph.D., is a professor at the School of Global Management and Leadership at Arizona State University. You can contact him at jordan.lowe@asu.edu.
E N D N OT E S 1 American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA), SAS No. 99, Consideration of Fraud in a Financial Statement Audit, AICPA, New York, N.Y., 2002. 2 Mark S. Beasley, Joseph V. Carcello, and Dana R. Hermanson, Top 10 audit deficiencies, Journal of Accountancy, April 2001, pp. 63-66. 3 The Panel on Audit Effectiveness, Report and Recommendations, Public Oversight Board (POB), Stamford, Conn., August 31, 2000. 4 Association of Certified Fraud Examiners (ACFE), 2004 Report to the Nation on Occupational Fraud and Abuse, ACFE, 2004. 5 Ibid. 6 W. Steve Albrecht, Gerald W. Wernz, and Timothy L. Williams, Fraud: Bringing Light to the Dark Side of Business, McGraw Hill, New York, N.Y., 1995. 7 Jeffrey J. McMillan and Richard A. White, Auditors Belief Revisions and Evidence Search: The Effect of Hypothesis Frame, Confirmation, and Professional Skepticism, The Accounting Review, July 1993, pp. 443-465. 8 Michael K. Shaub and Janice E. Lawrence, Ethics, Experience and Professional Skepticism: A Situational Analysis, Behavioral Research in Accounting, Volume 8 Supplement, 1996, pp. 124-157. 9 Audrey A. Gramling, External Auditors Reliance on Work Performed by Internal Auditors: The Influence of Fee Pressure on this Reliance Decision, Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, Supplement 18, 1999, pp. 117-135. 10 Kathy Hurtt, Development of an Instrument to Measure Professional Skepticism, University of Wisconsin, working paper, 2003. 11 Kathy Hurtt, Martha Eining, and David Plumlee, Professional Skepticism: A Model with Implications for Research, Practice, and Education, University of Wisconsin, working paper, 2003. 12 Hurtt, Development of an Instrument to Measure Professional Skepticism, 2003. 13 Michael K. Shaub and Janice E. Lawrence, Differences in Auditors Professional Skepticism across Career Levels in the Firm, Advances in Accounting Behavioral Research, September 1999, pp. 61-83. 14 Cindy Durtschi and Rosemary Fullerton, The Effects of Professional Skepticism on the Fraud Detection Skills of Internal Auditors, Utah State University, working paper, 2006. 15 Cindy Durtschi and Rosemary Fullerton, Teaching Fraud Detection Skills: A Problem-Based Learning Approach, Journal of Forensic Accounting, June 2005, pp. 187-212. 16 Tina Carpenter, Cindy Durtschi, and Lisa M. Gaynor, The Examination of the Short-Term and Long-Term Effects of Fraud Training on Skepticism and Fraud Related Judgements, Utah State University, working paper, 2008.

M A N A G E M E N T A C C O U N T I N G Q U A R T E R LY

15

WINTER 2008, VOL. 9, NO. 2

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

S-ar putea să vă placă și