Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

The Molecular Genetics of ADHD: A View From the IMAGE

Project
By Stephen V. Faraone, Ph.D., and Philip Asherson, MRCPsych, Ph.D.

Psychiatric Times August 2005 Vol. XXII Issue 9

Although attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is frequently misunderstood as caused by


normal childhood energy, boring classrooms, or overstressed parents and teachers, several
decades of research show ADHD to be a valid disorder with a neurobiological basis (Faraone, in
press). Genetic studies have played a leading role in clarifying the biological basis of the
disorder. Family studies have documented familial transmission; adoption studies show this
transmission occurs through biological, not adoptive relationships; and twin studies show that
ADHD is highly heritable such that genes account for about 75% of the disorder's variability in
the population (Faraone et al., in press). With a prevalence of 8% to 12% (Faraone et al., 2003),
it is among the most common of psychiatric disorders.

Given this strong evidence from epidemiologic studies, molecular genetic studies have begun the
search for genes that increase susceptibility to ADHD. Two general approaches have been used.
Genome scan linkage studies scan the entire genome in search of regions that might harbor
susceptibility genes. They do not require a prior hypothesis about which genes cause the
disorder. In contrast, candidate gene studies nominate specific genes based on a biological theory
about their putative role. They use the method of association to test these prior hypotheses.

To date, there have been three research groups performing genome linkage scans of ADHD
(Arcos-Burgos et al., 2004; Bakker et al., 2003; Fisher et al., 2002; Ogdie et al., 2004, 2003).
Table 1 summarizes their main results and gives their highest LOD scores (a statistic that
measures the evidence for linkage). With the exception of chromosome 17p11, genomic regions
implicated by these studies do not overlap. Because three of the implicated regions (17p11,
16p13 and 15q15) have also been implicated in studies of autism, these findings may suggest a
shared genetic susceptibility for these disorders.

For ADHD, the modest replication across studies completed so far suggests that genes of
moderately large effect are unlikely to exist. For this reason, further studies are needed to
increase the power of available linkage data. Difficulties in replication may also be due to
genetic heterogeneity (i.e., there are different forms of ADHD influenced by different genes).

In contrast to the small number of linkage studies, numerous candidate gene studies have used
association to determine if biologically relevant genes influence the susceptibility to ADHD.
Much of this work has focused on genes in catecholaminergic systems because the drugs that
effectively treat ADHD are either dopamine reuptake inhibitors (e.g., stimulants) or
norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (e.g., tricyclic antidepressants, atomoxetine [Strattera]).
Compared with dopaminergic and noradrenergic systems, serotonergic systems have received
relatively little attention in ADHD research. This is because measures of serotonin metabolism
are minimally related to the clinical efficacy of the medicines that treat ADHD. Nevertheless,
molecular genetic studies have examined serotonergic genes due to the well-known role of
serotonin in impulsivity, one of the core symptoms of ADHD (Brunner and Hen, 1997).

Another biological pathway to ADHD was discovered through study of coloboma mice, which
have a hemizygous deletion of chromosome 2q (Wilson, 2000). This deletion region includes the
gene encoding SNAP-25, a neuron-specific protein implicated in exocytotic neurotransmitter
release. Coloboma mice show spontaneous hyperactivity, delays in achieving complex neonatal
motor abilities and learning deficiencies. These problems are not seen if the mice are given a
functioning SNAP25 gene through a transgenic procedure. Treatment with mixed amphetamine
salts (Adderall) but not methylphenidate (Concerta, Metadate, Ritalin) reverses the hyperactivity,
which is consistent with the mechanism of action of these medications.

Methylphenidate treats ADHD by blocking the dopamine transporter. Mixed amphetamine salts
block the dopamine transporter but also facilitates the non-vesicular release of dopamine through
reverse transport, which would be expected to reverse the deficits in exocytotic neurotransmitter
release caused by the coloboma mutation.

To test genes associated with these biological hypotheses, candidate gene studies have used case-
control or family-based designs. Case-control designs compare allele frequencies between
patients with ADHD and non-ADHD controls. Alleles that confer risk for ADHD should be more
common among patients with ADHD. The family-based design compares the alleles that parents
transmit to children with ADHD to alleles parents do not transmit. If an allele increases the risk
for ADHD, it should be more common among the transmitted alleles than the nontransmitted
alleles. From both study designs, it is possible to derive an odds ratio (OR) that assesses the
magnitude of the association between ADHD and the putative risk alleles. An OR of 1.0
indicates no association; >1.0 indicates the allele increases risk for ADHD; and <1.0 indicates
the allele decreases the risk for ADHD.

Faraone et al. (in press) reviewed the ADHD candidate gene literature and examined pooled ORs
for candidate genes that had been examined in at least three case-control or family-based
association studies. Table 2 shows seven genes that provide statistically significant evidence of
association with ADHD. Other genes have been studied, showing either negative (catechol O-
methyltransferase, norepinephrine transporter) or unclear (D2 and D3 dopamine receptors;
tyrosine hydroxylase; noradrenergic receptors ADRA2A, 2C and 1C; monoamine oxidase A;
serotonin 2A receptor; the A4 and A7 acetylcholine receptor subunits, and the 2A glutamate)
results.

The ORs for the positive associations range from 1.13 to 1.45 (Table 2). These small ORs are
consistent with the idea that many genes of small effect mediate the genetic vulnerability to
ADHD. Moreover, they suggest one explanation for the frequent failure to replicate initial
reports of association: Many individual studies are underpowered to find significant associations
if the effects are modest (Lohmueller et al., 2003). These small and sometimes inconsistent
effects emphasize the need for future molecular genetic studies to implement strategies that will
provide enough statistical power to detect small effects.

To address this project, we and colleagues from multiple sites in Europe conceived the
International Multi-site ADHD Genetics (IMAGE) project. The main aim of IMAGE is to
generate a clinical and genetic resource of 1,400 sibling pairs and their biological parents. The
sibling pair design will enable the use of linkage analysis to identify chromosomal regions
containing genes of moderate-to-large effect and association strategies to identify genes of small
effect. The IMAGE group uses a novel approach by including ADHD probands and all available
siblings in one dataset.

In order to increase confidence in the diagnosis and decrease potential genetic and etiological
heterogeneity, probands are recruited from ADHD treatment centers and selected for DSM-IV
combined subtype. All siblings of probands are included in the study, whether or not they have
ADHD, and continuous rating scale measures of symptoms will be used to map genes.

The use of quantitative measures to map genes for common disorders is known as quantitative
trait locus (QTL) analysis and reflects the view that genetic influences on ADHD are
continuously distributed throughout the population (Asherson and IMAGE Consortium, 2004).
This means that genes that increase risk for ADHD are also expected to influence individual
differences in ADHD symptoms throughout the entire population. Similar approaches have been
used for other common traits such as blood pressure (Harrap et al., 2002), cholesterol level (Lin,
2003) and dyslexia (Cardon et al., 1994).

The use of quantitative trait measures for ADHD genetic research is based on findings from
numerous population-based twin studies that show high heritability for parent and teacher ratings
of ADHD symptoms (Thapar et al., 1999). We know, for example, that correlations for parent-
and teacher-rated ADHD symptoms are 70% to 75% for identical (monozygotic [MZ]) twins
(who share 100% of their genes) and 30% to 35% for nonidentical (dizygotic [DZ]) twins (who
share 50% of their genes).

In order to examine the genetic correlation between ADHD diagnosis and continuous rating scale
measures, a method called DF analysis estimates group heritability from the differential
regression of identical and nonidentical co-twin trait scores to the population trait mean where
twin probands are selected for extreme scores (DeFries and Fulker, 1988). In one study of 6,000
preschool twins, the group heritability for twins where probands were selected for extreme scores
ranged from 0.83 to 0.93 (Price et al., 2001).

Using the IMAGE dataset, we estimated the familial correlation between combined-type ADHD
probands and continuous measures of ADHD symptoms among siblings. As expected, both
inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms show familial associations to combined-type
ADHD of around 0.2 to 0.3, which is similar to sibling correlations for DZ twins estimated from
population samples (Table 3).

There are several potential advantages to our study design. The collection of DNA from a large
sample of ADHD probands allows us to perform powerful tests of association that can identify
small genetic effects, such as those seen for candidate genes listed in Table 2. At the same time,
we can investigate whether alleles associated with ADHD correlate with quantitative ratings in
siblings and we can increase the power of our analysis by combining within- and between-pair
tests of association.

We previously tested the utility of this approach by investigating whether some of the genes
associated with ADHD showed QTL associations in a population twin sample with parent ratings
of ADHD behavior (Mill et al., 2005). We found significant correlations with previously
identified risk alleles from SLC6A3 and DRD5 and a protective allele from SNAP-25. Our
analyses found positive correlations using within-pair tests of association that are independent of
genetic stratification and are robust to potential stratification effects generated by parent raters
who may scale similar behaviors differently. In other words, within-pair differences for
behavioral scores are more reliable than across-pair comparisons. This was reflected in the
strength of the genetic associations we observed. The QTL linkage also takes advantage of the
reliable nature of within-pair differences by looking for correlations between the similarity
between siblings and the number of parental chromosomes they share at a particular genetic
locus.

The IMAGE dataset contains a subset of affected sibling pairs that can be used for more
traditional tests of association and linkage using diagnostic status alone. In our initial set of 608
ADHD combined-type probands, we found a sibling concordance rate for combined-subtype
ADHD of 22.1% for males and 5.2% for females. This gives rise to similar sibling risk ratios for
the combined subtype for males (9.2%) and females (11.5%) when expressed as a ratio against
recent prevalence rates in a European population (2.4% males, 0.4% females).

As we discover more genes, the goal of ADHD genetic research will shift from gene discovery
toward gene functionality. Quantitative genetic findings have shifted perception of ADHD
toward that of a quantitative trait sharing etiological influences with other developmental,
behavioral and cognitive traits. Molecular genetics has confirmed a priori hypotheses of
dopamine system dysregulation and promises to identify additional genes in the coming decade.
Combining genetic, environmental and neurobiological research has the potential to delineate
causal links between ADHD and the developmental course of the disorder, including persistence
of ADHD symptoms into adulthood and comorbidity with associated psychiatric disorders/traits.

Acknowledgements

The IMAGE project is a multi-site, international effort supported by National Institutes of Health
grant R01MH62873 to Dr. Faraone. IMAGE Project principal investigators are Philip Asherson,
MRCPsych, Ph.D.; Tobias Banaschewski, Ph.D.; Jan Buitelaar, M.D., Ph.D.; Richard P. Ebstein,
Ph.D.; Stephen V. Faraone, Ph.D.; Michael Gill, M.D., MRCPsych; Ana Miranda, Ph.D.;
Fernando Mulas, Dr.D.; Robert D. Oades, Ph.D.; Herbert Roeyers, Ph.D.; Aribert
Rothenberger, M.D.; Joseph Sergeant, Ph.D.; Edmund Sonuga-Barke, Ph.D.; Eric Taylor, M.D.,
Ph.D.; and Hans-Christoph Steinhausen, M.D., Ph.D.

Dr. Faraone is professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences at the State University of New
York Upstate Medical University in Syracuse. He is also director of medical genetics research
and head of child and adolescent psychiatry research at the Center for Neuropsychiatric
Genetics at Upstate Medical University.

Dr. Asherson is senior lecturer in molecular psychiatry and honorary consultant psychiatrist in
the Institute of Psychiatry at King's College in London.

References

Arcos-Burgos M, Castellanos FX, Pineda D et al. (2004), Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder


in a population isolate: linkage to loci at 4q13.2, 5q33.3, 11q22, and 17p11. Am J Hum Genet
75(6):998-1014.

Asherson P, IMAGE Consortium (2004), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in the post-


genomic era. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 13(suppl 1):I50-I70.

Bakker SC, van der Meulen EM, Buitelaar JK et al. (2003), A whole-genome scan in 164 Dutch
sib pairs with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: suggestive evidence for linkage on
chromosomes 7p and 15q. Am J Hum Genet 72(5):1251-1260.

Brunner D, Hen R (1997), Insights into the neurobiology of impulsive behavior from serotonin
receptor knockout mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci 836:81-105.

Cardon LR, Smith SD, Fulker DW et al. (1994), Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on
chromosome 6. [Published erratum Science 268(5217):1553.] Science 266(5183):276-279 [see
comment].

DeFries JC, Fulker DW (1988), Multiple regression analysis of twin data: etiology of deviant
scores versus individual differences. Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) 37(3-4):205-216.

Faraone SV (in press), The scientific foundation for understanding attention-deficit/hyperactivity


disorder as a valid psychiatric disorder. Eur J Child Adolesc Psychiatry.

Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE et al. (in press), Molecular genetics of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry.

Faraone SV, Sergeant J, Gillberg C, Biederman J (2003), The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: is
it an American condition? World Psychiatry 2(2):104-113.

Fisher SE, Francks C, McCracken JT et al. (2002), A genomewide scan for loci involved in
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Hum Genet 70(5):1183-1196.

Harrap SB, Wong ZY, Stebbing M et al. (2002), Blood pressure QTLs identified by genome-wide
linkage analysis and dependence on associated phenotypes. Physiol Genomics 8(2):99-105.
Lin JP (2003), Genome-wide scan on plasma triglyceride and high density lipoprotein cholesterol
levels, accounting for the effects of correlated quantitative phenotypes. BMC Genet 4(suppl
1):S47.

Lohmueller KE, Pearce CL, Pike M et al. (2003), Meta-analysis of genetic association studies
supports a contribution of common variants to susceptibility to common disease. Nat Genet
33(2):177-182.

Mill J, Xu X, Ronald A et al. (2005), Quantitative trait locus analysis of candidate gene alleles
associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in five genes: DRD4, DAT1,
DRD5, SNAP-25, and 5HT1B. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 133(1):68-73.

Ogdie MN, Fisher SE, Yang M et al. (2004), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: fine
mapping supports linkage to 5p13, 6q12, 16p13, and 17p11. Am J Hum Genet 75(4):661-668.

Ogdie MN, Macphie IL, Minassian SL et al. (2003), A genomewide scan for attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder in an extended sample: suggestive linkage on 17p11. Am J Hum
Genet 72(5):1268-1279.

Price TS, Simonoff E, Waldman I et al. (2001), Hyperactivity in preschool children is highly
heritable. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40(12):1362-1364 [letter].

Thapar A, Holmes J, Poulton K, Harrington R (1999), Genetic basis of attention deficit and
hyperactivity. Br J Psychiatry 174:105-111.

Wilson MC (2000), Coloboma mouse mutant as an animal model of hyperkinesis and attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24(1):51-57.

S-ar putea să vă placă și