Sunteți pe pagina 1din 14

The Roots of Stereotype Threat: When Automatic Associations Disrupt Girls

Math Performance
Silvia Galdi and Mara Cadinu
University of Padova
Carlo Tomasetto
University of Bologna
Although stereotype awareness is a prerequisite for stereotype threat effects (Steele & Aronson, 1995), research
showed girls decit under stereotype threat before the emergence of mathgender stereotype awareness, and
in the absence of stereotype endorsement. In a study including 240 six-year-old children, this paradox was
addressed by testing whether automatic associations trigger stereotype threat in young girls. Whereas no indi-
cators were found that children endorsed the mathgender stereotype, girls, but not boys, showed automatic
associations consistent with the stereotype. Moreover, results showed that girls automatic associations varied
as a function of a manipulation regarding the stereotype content. Importantly, girls math performance
decreased in a stereotype-consistent, relative to a stereotype-inconsistent, condition and automatic associations
mediated the relation between stereotype threat and performance.
Stereotype threat research (Steele & Aronson, 1995)
has shown that both adults and children underper-
form in difcult tests when a negative domain-
relevant in-group stereotype is made salient (see
Inzlicht & Schmader, 2012, for a review). Despite
this overwhelming evidence, the specic require-
ments for childrens underperformance under ste-
reotype threat are still unclear. With the aim of
providing new insights into research on stereotype
threat, this study investigates automatic associations
as a potential requirement for the emergence of ste-
reotype threat underperformance in children.
Automatic associations are those associations
between concepts (e.g., mewoman), or between
concepts and attributes (e.g., owerpositive) that
come to mind unintentionally when an individual
encounters a relevant object, that are difcult to con-
trol once they have been activated, and that are not
necessarily explicitly endorsed (e.g., Gawronski &
Bodenhausen, 2006). Such automatic associations are
often contrasted with conscious beliefs, which are
those mental contents that an individual explicitly
endorses as accurate (e.g., Gawronski & Bodenhau-
sen, 2006). Most measures of automatic associations
are based on participants performance on com-
puter-based, speeded categorization tasks (see Gaw-
ronski & Payne, 2010, for a review). These measures,
commonly referred to as implicit measures, differ
from self-reports, described as explicit measures,
which assess conscious beliefs.
This study employed implicit and explicit
measures jointly to show that automatic associa-
tions consistent with a negative in-group stereotype
may lead to stereotype-induced underperformance
at early stages of development, even though
children do not possess the cognitive competencies
for stereotype awareness yet, and in the absence of
evidence that they endorse the stereotype content.
Prerequisites for Stereotype Threat Susceptibility
According to the stereotype threat model, for ste-
reotypes to affect performance, children need to (a)
have developed a concept of social categories (cate-
gory awareness), (b) be able to identify themselves as
members of a social category (self-categorization), and
(c) know that the in-group category is negatively
related to specic domains or attributes (stereotype
awareness; Aronson & Good, 2003). When children
enter elementary school, they possess the cognitive
competencies of category awareness and self-categoriza-
tion (e.g., Martin & Ruble, 2010) but not stereotype
awareness (e.g., McKown & Strambler, 2009).
Research has shown that, between the ages of 3
and 4 years, children become aware of social
categories, such as gender and race (e.g., Aboud,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Silvia Galdi, Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della
Socializzazione, Universita di Padova, Via Venezia 15, Padova
35131, Italy. Electronic mail may be sent to silvia.galdi@unipd.it.
2013 The Authors
Child Development 2013 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2014/8501-0017
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12128
Child Development, January/February 2014, Volume 85, Number 1, Pages 250263
1988; Katz & Kofkin, 1997), and are able to identify
themselves as members of these categories (e.g.,
Martin & Ruble, 2010; Quintana, 1998). Moreover,
by this age children develop personal stereotypic
beliefs about abilities and characteristics of ethnic
(Aboud, 1988) and gender groups. For example,
they hold personal stereotypic beliefs about gender
differences in toy preferences, dressing, and aggres-
sive versus prosocial behaviors (Martin & Ruble,
2010). However, personal stereotypic beliefs,
referred to as stereotype endorsement, differ from the
knowledge of stereotypes held by others (not neces-
sarily personally endorsed), which is dened as
stereotype awareness (e.g., Martinot & Desert, 2007;
McKown & Strambler, 2009). Thus, although in
many cases personal beliefs overlap with socially
shared stereotypes, this does not imply necessarily
that young children are aware of stereotypes.
Indeed, the literature on childrens social perspec-
tive taking (e.g., Selman, 1980), theory of mind
(e.g., Perner & Wimmer, 1985), and person percep-
tion (e.g., Rholes & Ruble, 1984) suggests that chil-
dren develop the cognitive competencies for
stereotype awareness only by middle childhood,
and that up to then they do not distinguish their
personal stereotypic beliefs from others beliefs.
Consistently, children have been shown to be aware
of common ethnic and gender stereotypes about
academic abilities by the ages of 8 and 9 years (e.g.,
McKown & Weinstein, 2003; Quintana, 1998).
The evidence that children do not possess stereo-
type awareness when they enter elementary school
has important consequences. As noted earlier,
stereotype awareness is a specic requirement for
stereotype threat. Specically, the stereotype threat
model posits that peoples negative stereotypes
hamper performance only in situations that induce
targets to become concerned about being judged by
others on the basis of relevant stereotypes (Steele,
1997). Consistently, research on performance decre-
ments induced by ethnic stereotypes conrms that
only children who are aware of broadly held stereo-
types are vulnerable to stereotype threat effects
(McKown & Strambler, 2009; McKown & Weinstein,
2003). Therefore, in principle we should not expect
declines in performance under stereotype threat
prior to 89 years of age, or even later (Aronson &
Good, 2003). Nevertheless, Ambady, Shih, Kim, and
Pittinsky (2001) showed that 5- to 7-year-old Asian
American girls underperformed on a math task
when their gender identity was made salient, as
compared to children in a control condition.
Similarly, Tomasetto, Alparone, and Cadinu (2011)
found that gender identity activation hampered
math performance among 5- to 7-year-old Italian
girls, and Neuville and Croizet (2007) obtained
similar ndings among 7-year-old French girls.
The last three studies raise a theoretical paradox:
How can stereotype-induced performance decre-
ments be found in girls who do not possess stereo-
type awareness yet? One could assume that for a
negative stereotype to affect performance in chil-
dren who have not developed stereotype awareness
yet, it is sufcient that children hold the personal
stereotypic belief that their in-group is less skillful
in the relevant domain. Indeed, given that young
children do not distinguish their own stereotypic
beliefs from others, and rather assume that their
beliefs are shared by others as well (e.g., Augousti-
nos & Rosewarne, 2001), in principle childrens
personal negative beliefs about abilities of their
in-group could be sufcient to trigger stereotype
threat. If this were the case, stereotype endorsement
would be the key to identify the sources of stereo-
type threat in young children.
To date, the potential role of childrens endorse-
ment of the mathgender stereotype as a prerequi-
site for stereotype threat effects does not t with
the available evidence. For example, Ambady et al.
(2001) found that 5- to 7-year-old American girls
believe that boys and girls are equal at math (for
similar results on Italian children, see Tomasetto
et al., 2011); on the contrary, 5- to 7-year-old Amer-
ican boys state that boys are better at math, thus
exhibiting in-group favoritism (e.g., Powlishta,
1995; Yee & Brown, 1994). Similarly, other research
showed no endorsement of the mathgender stereo-
type until 89 years of age among Italian and
French children (Martinot & Desert, 2007; Muzzatti
& Agnoli, 2007).
To our knowledge, only one study found stereo-
type endorsement as early as 67 years of age in a
Western country (i.e., United States; Cvencek,
Meltzoff, & Greenwald, 2011). However, this dis-
crepancy from the other ndings might be due to
the type of measures used. Whereas all the studies
discussed elsewhere in the article assessed the
endorsement of the gender stereotype regarding
math ability, Cvencek et al. (2011) assessed the
endorsement of the stereotype that boys like math
and girls like language. Therefore, it is possible that
6- to 7-year-olds already hold the personal stereo-
type that boys like math, but still do not endorse the
stereotype about girls and boys math ability.
Consistent with this reasoning, Steele (2003) found
that 6- to 10-year-old children do not endorse the
stereotype regarding boys and girls abilities in math
(see also Martinot, Bages, & Desert, 2012), even
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 251
though they endorse the same stereotype about men
and women (stereotype stratication). Thus, to date,
there is no evidence that young children endorse
the mathgender ability stereotype, at least in
Western countries. Nevertheless, research shows
that it is specically the ability component of the
mathgender stereotype that affects womens per-
formance, whereas other stereotypical beliefs (e.g.,
women are less interested in math) do not trigger
stereotype threat effects (Thoman, White, Yam-
awaki, & Koishi, 2008).
To summarize, young girls show math underper-
formance before being aware of the mathgender
stereotype, and in the absence of evidence that they
endorse such belief as a personal stereotype. One
possibility to explain this paradox is that previous
ndings on childrens mathgender stereotype have
been based only on self-reports, which may not
capture all relevant aspects of the stereotype. Thus,
in this research, we used implicit and explicit mea-
sures jointly to investigate automatic associations as
an alternative route for the emergence of stereo-
type-induced underperformance in young children.
Insights From Social-Cognitive Research
Implicit measures, such as the well-known Impli-
cit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, &
Schwartz, 1998), have been developed to assess the
strength of automatic associations between concepts
or concepts and attributes. These procedures are
based on the rationale that individuals should be
faster at pairing concepts, or concepts and attributes
that are strongly associated in their cognitive map,
as compared to those that are weakly or not at all
associated. Research has shown that men and
women differ in the strength of their automatic
associations between gender and academic domains
(Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002) and that auto-
matic associations consistent with the mathgender
stereotype predict attitudes toward math, domain
identication, and math performance (e.g., Kiefer &
Sekaquaptewa, 2007; Nosek & Smyth, 2011).
Together, these results show that automatic associa-
tions play an important role in peoples academic
achievement, interest, and performance, and that
the combined use of implicit and explicit measures
may allow researchers to provide useful insights
that neither implicit measures nor explicit measures
alone would offer.
Theorizing on the meaning of implicit and
explicit measures posits that explicit measures
reect more recent as well as newly formed
representations that are not strong enough yet to
be automatically activated (e.g., Rudman, 2004;
Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000), whereas impli-
cit measures detect only those highly overlearned
associations between concepts whose activation has
become automatic over the course of long-term
experiences. According to this widespread assump-
tion, in terms of emergence, conscious beliefs reect
more recently formed representations, whereas
automatic associations reect only the older repre-
sentations. However, an emerging body of research
suggests that the opposite may be true as well.
Recent ndings showing experimentally induced
changes in implicit but not explicit measures (e.g.,
Gawronski & LeBel, 2008; Olson & Fazio, 2006)
suggest that automatic associations may reect
more recently formed representations as compared
to the corresponding conscious beliefs. These results
are also consistent with studies using implicit and
explicit measures in predicting future choices of
decided and undecided individuals (for a review,
see Gawronski & Galdi, 2011). These studies dem-
onstrate that well-structured automatic associations
about a specic object are present also in the cogni-
tive maps of those individuals who, on explicit
measures, report being undecided and do
not endorse well-dened conscious beliefs yet.
These ndings come from research on attitudes
(i.e., dealing with the affective evaluation of an
object; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993), whereas stereotypes
are cognitive beliefs about the link between a cate-
gory and an attribute (Hamilton & Trolier, 1986);
nonetheless they suggest that in terms of emer-
gence, automatic associations may precede conscious
beliefs. Thus, there is reason to believe that implicit
measures can also detect recently formed automatic
associations between concepts and stereotypical
attributes (e.g., boysmath) that are not reected on
explicit measures yet.
Insights From Research on Malleability of Automatic
Associations
Other research has demonstrated that external
cues (e.g., situational stimuli) may increase or
decrease the activation of automatic associations in
adults. For example, Dasgupta and Asgari (2004)
found that participants exposed to counterstereo-
typical women showed lower activation of auto-
matic associations consistent with genderrole
stereotypes, as compared to control participants.
Similarly, women exposed to gender-stereotypical
women in TV commercials showed increased acti-
vation of automatic associations consistent with
the traditional female stereotype, and this higher
252 Galdi, Cadinu, and Tomasetto
activation of automatic associations led to worse
math performance (Davies, Spencer, Quinn, & Ger-
hardstein, 2002). The last results are consistent with
research demonstrating that stereotypical automatic
associations affect working memory resources
needed to perform complex cognitive tasks. Speci-
cally, Forbes and Schmader (2010) found that
women trained via an IAT to associate their gender
with being good at math showed higher working
memory and increased math performance. Taken
together, these ndings suggest that it may be spe-
cically in automatic associations between group
membership and stereotypical attributes, and in the
malleability of such automatic associations, where
we should look not only to investigate stereotype-
induced underperformance but also to reduce
stereotype threat effects. Unfortunately, to date no
study has tested whether automatic associations are
malleable in children as well, or whether changes
in activation of stereotypical automatic associations
may reect changes in performance. Therefore, we
hypothesized that situational cues making a nega-
tive stereotype salient may automatically activate
corresponding automatic associations. The activa-
tion of such stereotypical automatic associations
should in turn affect the performance of children
who are stereotype targets, even in the absence of
evidence for stereotype endorsement. Conversely,
cues challenging the negative stereotype should
reduce the activation of corresponding automatic
associations, and such a reduction should lead to
improved performance.
Aims of This Study
To test the hypotheses mentioned above, we
employed explicit and implicit measures jointly to
assess two aspects of the mathgender stereotype,
stereotype endorsement and automatic associations,
and their differential relation with young childrens
stereotype threat vulnerability.
Research with children has already used implicit
measures to assess automatic associations (e.g.,
Baron & Banaji, 2006). Regarding mathgender ste-
reotypes, Steffens, Jelenec, and Noack (2010) found
stereotypical automatic associations in 9- to 14-year-
old girls and no automatic associations for boys at
any age. Conversely, Cvencek et al. (2011) found
stereotype-consistent automatic associations in both
girls and boys as young as 67 years of age. How-
ever, neither Steffens et al. nor Cvencek et al.
assessed either performance or automatic associa-
tions under stereotype threat. Therefore, these stud-
ies are not directly relevant to our main hypothesis.
To our knowledge, only Ambady and collabora-
tors used an implicit stereotype awareness task
(Ambady et al., 2001, p. 387) to assess the math
gender stereotype in a study on stereotype threat in
young children. The measure was a memory recall
task, in which the experimenter told a story about a
student especially good at math, and recorded
whether the participant, when repeating the story,
used the pronoun he or she to identify the protago-
nist. However, rather than a measure of stereotypi-
cal automatic associations, this task should be
considered as an indirect measure of childrens
stereotypes. Therefore, it is still unknown whether
automatic associations may be responsible for ste-
reotype threat performance decits in young chil-
dren.
In this study, we focused on rst-grade children
for two reasons. So far, the only study showing the
presence of automatic associations consistent with
gender stereotypes about math and language in
young children (Cvencek et al., 2011) considered
children from 6 to 7 years altogether (Grades 1 and
2). Second, studies showing stereotype-induced
math performance decrements in young girls have
addressed 5- to 7-year-old children (Ambady et al.,
2001; Tomasetto et al., 2011) or 7-year-olds (Neuville
& Croizet, 2007). Thus, the focus on 6-year-olds
(Grade 1) allowed us to investigate stereotypical
automatic associations as well as stereotype threat
effects specically when children enter elementary
school, thus becoming acquainted for the rst time
with formal teaching of math (at least in the Italian
school system).
The study had four goals. First, using a child-
friendly version of the IAT (ChildIAT; Baron &
Banaji, 2006) we tested whether gender stereotypes
about math and language are present as automatic
associations in 6-year-olds. Consistent with Cvencek
et al. (2011), we expected stereotype-consistent auto-
matic associations for both girls and boys.
Second, we investigated the malleability of chil-
drens automatic associations. It was hypothesized
that childrens stereotypical automatic associations
would increase or decrease in activation depending
on the stereotype content of a prior manipulation
task. We predicted an increased activation of stereo-
typical automatic associations in a stereotype-consistent
condition than in a stereotype-inconsistent condition,
respectively, conrming or contradicting the tradi-
tional gender stereotype about math, with results in
the middle in a control condition, in which the math
gender stereotype was not salient.
Third, using an explicit measure, we investigated
whether 6-year-olds endorse the belief that boys are
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 253
better than girls at math. Three reasons led us to
assess stereotype endorsement: (a) children rst
develop stereotype endorsement and then stereo-
type awareness (e.g., Aboud, 1988; Martin & Ruble,
2010), (b) 6-year-olds do not possess stereotype
awareness yet (e.g., Rholes & Ruble, 1984; Selman,
1980), and (c) childrens personal stereotypic beliefs
could be sufcient to trigger stereotype threat, spe-
cically because at this age children assume that
their beliefs are shared by others as well (e.g.,
Augoustinos & Rosewarne, 2001). Consistent with
previous research (e.g., Ambady et al., 2001; Muzz-
atti & Agnoli, 2007), we expected no evidence of
endorsement of the mathgender stereotype.
Finally, like in past research on stereotype threat
including 6-year-olds (e.g., Tomasetto et al., 2011),
we expected girls math performance to decrease
under stereotype threat, that is, in the stereotype-
consistent as compared to the control condition and
the stereotype-inconsistent condition, which should
lead to the highest performance. Importantly, we
predicted the increased activation of stereotype-
consistent automatic associations to mediate the
decrease in girls performance under stereotype
threat.
Method
Participants
Two hundred and seventy-six rst-grade chil-
dren (143 girls) participated in the study. All of
them were born in Italy and were of typical age for
their grade (M = 77.60 months, SD = 0.28). Chil-
dren attended one of six elementary schools in one
of three small towns in the northeast of Italy. Per-
missions to conduct the study were granted by
school principals and parents. Six female experi-
menters were involved in the data collection, con-
ducted during school hours. All children were
tested in the same school period to avoid potential
effects of variability in mathematics curricula across
classes at the moment of data collection.
Procedure
Children were tested individually in a quiet room
of their school, while sitting at a desk facing the
experimenter. Each experimental session started by
asking participants to color one of three pictures,
depending on the experimental condition (stereo-
type consistent, control, and stereotype inconsistent)
to which they were randomly assigned. When chil-
dren nished coloring, the experimenter asked them
to describe the picture, and then left the picture on
one side of the desk. After the manipulation task,
children were invited to play a computer game (i.e.,
the ChildIAT) using a laptop computer. Partici-
pants were told that they would see pictures during
the game, and that they would press the red (A key)
or the green (L key) button of the computer board to
indicate which picture they saw. After the Child
IAT, children performed eight math calculations
(i.e., math test). The experimenter asked one calcula-
tion at a time (e.g., How much is 5 plus 5?) and
children had to respond within 5 s. For each ques-
tion, the experimenter noted whether the response
was correct or not (i.e., incorrect or no response by
5 s), without providing any feedback. Finally, chil-
dren performed the explicit stereotype-endorsement
task. They were shown a picture of a boy and a girl
side by side and were told: These are a boy and a
girl. They are 6-year-olds and they are good at
school. Is the boy better at math, the girl better at
math, or are they the same at math? Next, the
experimenter recorded the response, and partici-
pants were thanked, given a candy bar, and dis-
missed.
Thus, the administration of the implicit measure
always followed the experimental manipulation and
always preceded the math test followed by the expli-
cit measure. This order of the tasks was chosen
because we aimed at testing the effects of the experi-
mental manipulation on automatic associations
ruling out the possibility that counterbalancing
implicit and explicit measures might weaken the
effects of the experimental manipulation on auto-
matic associations. Moreover, there is no evidence
that performing the IAT before a self-report induces
reactance or assimilation tendencies in the subse-
quent self-report (Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005).
Materials
Experimental manipulation. According to the
stereotype threat model, it is the salience of a nega-
tive domain-relevant in-group stereotype that
impairs targets performance. A negative domain-
relevant in-group stereotype can be made salient in
three ways: (a) describing the task that targets will
subsequently perform as diagnostic of the ability
related to the negative in-group stereotype, (b) mak-
ing targets in-group salient, or (c) making the con-
tent of the negative stereotype salient (Inzlicht &
Schmader, 2012). In this study, the manipulation
choice was to make salient the stereotype content
(mathgender stereotype). Thus, participants were
randomly assigned to one of three experimental
254 Galdi, Cadinu, and Tomasetto
conditions by inviting them to color a picture, in
which: (a) a boy correctly resolves a math calculation
on a blackboard, whereas a girl fails to respond
(stereotype-consistent condition); or (b) a girl
correctly resolves the calculation, whereas a boy fails
to respond (stereotype-inconsistent condition); or
(c) a landscape was depicted (control condition).
Implicit measure: The math(language)-gender Child
IAT. A ChildIAT (Baron & Banaji, 2006) was used
to assess the relative strength of automatic associa-
tions between the target category boy and the
attribute category mathematics, and the target cate-
gory girl and the attribute category language, as
compared to the opposite pairings (i.e., boylan-
guage and girlmathematics).
Categories and stimuli for the ChildIAT. Four pic-
tures of math-related objects (i.e., numbers) and four
pictures of reading- and writing-related objects (i.e.,
letters), pretested with rst-grade children for famil-
iarity and comprehension, were used as stimuli for
the attribute categories mathematics and language.
Four face-pictures of boys and four face-pictures of
girls represented the target categories boy and girl.
Procedure of the ChildIAT. Children were instruc-
ted to respond fast and accurately to three simple-
categorization (practice) blocks and two (third and
fth) critical double-categorization blocks. Each
practice block included 16 trials; each critical block
included 32 trials. On each trial, a target or attribute
picture appeared on the screen until participants
gave their response. Children categorized each pic-
ture by pressing a key on the computer board, the
left-hand response A or the right-hand response L
key. To simplify the task, the left-hand key (A) was
colored in red, and the right-hand key (L) was col-
ored in green. The intertrial interval was 200 ms. A
red cross, which remained on the screen for 200 ms,
followed incorrect responses.
In the rst block of the task, children were pre-
sented with pictures for the target categories girl
and boy, with each picture randomly presented
twice. Participants had to indicate whether the pic-
ture on the screen was a boy or a girl by pressing
the red key for girl and the green key for boy. In
the second block, children were presented with pic-
tures for the attribute categories language and
mathematics, with each picture randomly presented
twice. Participants had to press the red key when
the picture was a reading- or writing-related object
and the green key when the picture was a math-
related object. In the third double-categorization
block, both pictures of boys and girls, and pictures
of math-related and reading- or writing-related
objects (with each picture representing each cate-
gory randomly presented twice) appeared on the
screen. In this case, children pressed the red key
when they saw either a girl or a reading- or writing-
related object, and the green key when they saw
either a boy or a math-related object. In the fourth
block, participants categorized again the pictures
representing the categories mathematics and lan-
guage. However, different from the key assignment
in the second block, participants had to press the
red key when they saw a math-related object and
the green key when they saw a reading- or writing-
related object. Finally, in the fth double-categoriza-
tion block, children categorized the same pictures of
boys and girls and the same pictures of math-related
and reading- or writing-related objects. However,
participants now pressed the red key when they saw
either a girl or a math-related object, and the green
key when they saw either a boy or a reading- or
writing-related object. Following the procedure
employed in the third block, each picture represent-
ing each of the four categories was randomly pre-
sented twice. The order of the two critical blocks,
third and fth, was counterbalanced across partici-
pants to avoid order effects.
Math test. Three days before the data collection,
a math test was developed in collaboration with all
teachers of the classes participating in the study, to
create a set of difcult math calculations taking into
account the mathematics achievement of all classes.
The math test was a retrieval of numerical facts
including ve additions and three subtractions (i.e.,
5 + 5, 8 4, 6 + 4, 10 5, 2 + 3, 2 + 2, 6 3, and
4 + 5). Children had to resolve each math calcula-
tion in 5 s.
Explicit stereotype endorsement. As in Ambady
et al.s (2001) study, children were presented with a
photograph of a boy and a girl described as good
at school, and had to choose whether the boy or the
girl is especially good at math or whether they are
equal at math. The two photographs as well as the
questions wording for the boy or the girl were
presented in counterbalanced order.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Twenty-three girls and thirteen boys were
excluded only because of 30% or higher error rates in
at least one critical (double-categorization) block of
the ChildIAT. Preliminary analyses conrmed that
excluded participants were evenly distributed across
conditions (i.e., 13 in the stereotype-consistent, 10 in
the control, and 13 in the stereotype-inconsistent
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 255
conditions), and that their responses did not differ
from those of retained children on both the math test
and the stereotype endorsement. The nal sample
included 240 children (120 girls).
Individual scores of automatic associations were
calculated by means of the D-algorithm, designed for
analyzing data with the IAT (Greenwald, Nosek, &
Banaji, 2003). The D-algorithm compares the extent
to which performance on the incompatible critical
block (i.e., girlmathematics and boylanguage shar-
ing the same response key) is impaired relative to
performance on the compatible critical block (i.e.,
girllanguage and boymathematics sharing the
same response key), taking into account participants
individual response latencies, standard deviations of
latencies, and error rates in each of the two blocks.
Scores were calculated so that positive scores reect
stronger boymathematics and girllanguage automatic
associations. To estimate reliability of the ChildIAT,
two ChildIAT scores were calculated: one using the
rst half and the other using the second half of the
two critical blocks. Internal consistency was satisfac-
tory (Cronbachs a = .84).
A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was
then conducted on scores of automatic associations
using conditions (stereotype consistent, control, ste-
reotype inconsistent), gender (male, female), and
the order of administration of the critical blocks
(1 = third block: girllanguage and boymath, fth block:
girlmath and boylanguage; 2 = third block: girlmath
and boylanguage, fth block: girllanguage and boy
math) as the independent variables. A main effect for
order of administration was found, F(1, 234) = 6.64,
p = .01, g
p
2
= .03. However, because order of
administration produced no signicant interactions
(all ps > .09), it is not further discussed.
For each participant, correct responses to the
math test (i.e., correct math calculation in 5 s) were
coded +1, and incorrect responses (i.e., no response
or incorrect math calculation in 5 s) were coded 0.
For each child, responses were then added up in a
single math score. To correct for potential variabil-
ity in mathematics prociency across classes, math
scores were standardized within each class of the
six schools. Internal consistency of the math test
based on all eight calculations was satisfactory
(Cronbachs a = .89).
Separate one-way ANOVAs were conducted on
scores of automatic associations and math scores,
using condition (stereotype consistent, control, ste-
reotype inconsistent), gender (male, female), and
the schools as independent variables. Schools did
not lead to any signicant result (all ps > .70), and
thus this variable is not further discussed.
Zero-order correlations between all dependent
variables (automatic associations, math performance,
and explicit mathgender stereotype endorsement),
separately for girls and boys across experimental
conditions, are presented in Table 1.
Automatic Associations and Their Malleability
We predicted stereotype-consistent automatic
associations for both girls and boys in the control
condition, in which the mathgender stereotype
was not made salient. In addition, we aimed at test-
ing for the malleability of childrens automatic asso-
ciations. Higher scores of automatic associations
were expected in the stereotype-consistent than in
the stereotype-inconsistent condition, with results in
the middle for the control condition.
A two-way ANOVA was conducted on scores of
automatic associations, with condition (stereotype
consistent, control, stereotype inconsistent) and
gender (male, female) as the between-participants
variables. Results showed a signicant Condition 9
Gender interaction, F(2, 234) = 6.35, p = .002,
g
p
2
= .05. To assess the effect of condition within
gender, simple-effect analyses were conducted sepa-
rately for girls and boys. Table 2 presents the aver-
age scores of automatic associations in each
experimental condition for both boys and girls,
together with information about the planned con-
trasts between the means.
No effect of condition emerged for boys
automatic associations (p > .30). Moreover, one-sam-
ple t tests against 0 showed no effect in any condi-
tion (all ps > .20). Conversely, simple-effect analysis
on girls automatic associations revealed a signi-
cant effect of condition, F(2, 117) = 7.75, p < .01,
Table 1
Zero-Order Correlations Between Automatic Associations, Math Per-
formance, and Explicit Stereotype Endorsement for Girls and Boys
1 2 3
Girls (n = 120)
Automatic associations
Math performance .267**
Explicit stereotype endorsement .115 .142
Boys (n = 120)
Automatic associations
Math performance .045
Explicit stereotype endorsement .000 .034
Note. Pearsons r coefcients are reported for automatic associations
and math performance. Spearmans r
s
coefcients are reported
for the categorical variable explicit stereotype endorsement.
**p < .01.
256 Galdi, Cadinu, and Tomasetto
g
p
2
= .06, linear trend, p < .001. Post hoc tests with
Bonferroni correction of the alpha level for multiple
comparisons showed that girls scores of automatic
associations were lower in the stereotype-inconsis-
tent as compared to the control (p < .02) and the
stereotype-consistent (p < .01) conditions. As shown
in Table 2, although girls automatic associations
were the highest in the stereotype-consistent
condition, scores in the control and the stereotype-
consistent conditions did not differ from each other
(p > .90). Moreover, one-sample t tests against 0
showed that girls scores of automatic associations
were different from 0 in the stereotype-consistent
and in the control (both ps < .02) but not in the
stereotype-inconsistent condition (p = .20).
Importantly, simple-effect analyses on the main
effect of gender within conditions revealed that
boys scores were lower than girls scores of
automatic associations in the stereotype-consistent,
F(1, 234) = 5.90, p < .03, g
p
2
= .02, and control con-
dition, F(1, 234) = 5.84, p < .03, g
p
2
= .02. Although
this difference fell short of signicance, boys scores
of automatic associations tended to be higher than
those of girls in the stereotype-inconsistent condi-
tion, F(1, 234) = 3.79, p = .053, g
p
2
= .01.
In sum, although automatic associations consis-
tent with math(language)gender stereotypes were
not found for boys, girls revealed stereotype-consis-
tent automatic associations in both the control and
the stereotype-consistent conditions. Importantly,
girls automatic associations showed the expected
malleability, thus differing in activation as a function
of the experimental condition: They were higher in
the stereotype-consistent and the control conditions
than the stereotype-inconsistent conditions. No effect
of the experimental manipulation was found for
boys automatic associations.
Math Performance
For girls, we expected performance decrements
in the stereotype-consistent condition as compared
to the control and the stereotype-inconsistent condi-
tions. Conversely, no effect of condition on boys
math performance was hypothesized.
An ANOVA on math scores, with condition
(stereotype consistent, control, stereotype inconsis-
tent) and gender (male, female) as the between-
participants variables, was conducted. A Condi-
tion 9 Gender interaction emerged, F(2, 234) = 4.69,
p = .010, g
p
2
= .04. Therefore, the effect of condition
was tested separately for girls and boys. Simple-
effect analyses on boys math scores (Table 2)
showed that boys performed equally well across
conditions (p > .30). To the opposite, an effect of
condition was found for girls, F(2, 117) = 3.66,
p < .03, g
p
2
= .03, linear trend, p < .01: Girls under-
performed in the stereotype-consistent as compared
to the stereotype-inconsistent condition (p < .03). On
the contrary, math scores in the control and both in
the stereotype-consistent and in the stereotype-
inconsistent conditions were not different from each
other (both ps > .20). Thus, whereas the salience of
the negative in-group stereotype led girls to perform
worse on the math test, the removal of the same
stereotype led girls to perform better in the
stereotype-inconsistent condition as compared to
the stereotype-consistent condition.
Explicit Stereotype Endorsement
To assess stereotype endorsement, participants
were shown a picture of a boy and a girl and were
asked to say whether the boy or the girl is espe-
cially good at math, or whether they are equal. For
each child, stereotype-inconsistent response (i.e., the
girl is better at math) was coded 1, neutral choice
(i.e., the boy and the girl are equal) was coded 0, and
stereotype-consistent choice (i.e., the boy is better at
math) was coded +1. We expected no evidence that
children endorsed the mathgender stereotype.
A logistic regression for ordinal dependent mea-
sures was performed on childrens choices at the task
(1 = stereotype-inconsistent, 0 = neutral, +1 = stereo-
type-consistent choice), with gender (0 = male,
1 = female), condition (1 = stereotype inconsistent,
0 = control, +1 = stereotype consistent), and the product
Table 2
Average Scores of Automatic Associations and Math Scores as a Func-
tion of Condition (Stereotype Consistent, Control, Stereotype Inconsis-
tent) for Girls and Boys
Stereotype
consistent Control
Stereotype
inconsistent
Girls (n = 120)
Automatic
associations
**0.28
a
(SD = 0.45)
*0.19
a
(SD = 0.78)
0.10
b
(SD = 0.43)
Math test 0.36
a
(SD = 0.73)
0.01
ab
(SD = 0.49)
0.16
b
(SD = 0.92)
Boys (n = 120)
Automatic
associations
0.03
a
(SD = 0.44)
0.05
a
(SD = 0.30)
0.10
a
(SD = 0.50)
Math test 0.28
a
(SD = 1.03)
0.05
a
(SD = 1.02)
0.03
a
(SD = .75)
Note. Means within rows not sharing the same subscript are sig-
nicantly different from each other at the p < .05 level (Bonfer-
roni test).
*p < .02. **p < .001.
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 257
term as predictors. A main effect of gender emerged,
Wald v
2
= 11.49, p < .001, indicating that the proba-
bility of a counterstereotypical versus neutral versus
stereotypical response was not equal for boys and
girls. Regardless of condition, 57% of boys and 57%
of girls favored their own gender and indicated the
boy or the girl, respectively, as the most talented for
math. Only 12% of boys and 21% of girls responded
that the boy and the girl were the same. Of the
remaining children, 22% of girls identied the boy
as being better at math, whereas 31% of boys
believed that the outstanding math student was the
girl. Thus, consistent with predictions, 6-year-olds
did not endorse the mathgender stereotype, but
simply manifested gender in-group favoritism.
Mediation Analysis
Because of null ndings for boys on both auto-
matic associations and math scores, a mediation
analysis was conducted only for girls to test
whether automatic associations mediated the rela-
tion between condition and math performance.
Given that the predictor (i.e., condition) was a cate-
gorical variable with three levels, we created two
dummy-coded variables to conduct the mediation
analysis (Hayes & Preacher, 2012) with the stereo-
type-consistent condition as the reference group.
Specically, Contrast 1 tested the effect of the ste-
reotype-consistent (coded 0) versus stereotype-
inconsistent (coded 1) condition, with the control
condition coded 0. Contrast 2 tested for the residual
difference between the stereotype-consistent (coded
0) and the control (coded 1) conditions, with the
stereotype-inconsistent condition coded 0.
Consistent with the univariate analyses reported
above, the effect of Contrast 1 on performance was
signicant, b = .55, t(111) = 3.38, p < .01, whereas
the effect of Contrast 2 fell short of signicance,
b = .34, t(111) = 1.91, p = .06. Similarly, the effect of
Contrast 1 on automatic associations was signicant,
b = .37, t(111) = 3.56, p < .001, whereas the effect
of Contrast 2 was not, p > .40. Moreover, when
automatic associations and the two contrasts were
entered simultaneously in the model predicting
performance, the effect of automatic associations
was signicant, b = .36, t(111) = 2.32, p < .03, indi-
cating that automatic associations negatively affect
performance. Importantly, the effect of Contrast 1
(i.e., stereotype-consistent vs. stereotype-inconsistent
condition) was reduced, b = .38, t(110) = 2.06,
p < .05, whereas the effect of Contrast 2 (i.e., stereo-
type-consistent vs. control condition) remained not
signicant, b = .31, t(111) = 1.77, p = .07. Figure 1
summarizes results for Contrast 1. To test for media-
tion, we calculated a bias-corrected 95% condence
interval for the indirect effect (.13, SE = .06) using a
bootstrapping technique (Preacher & Hayes, 2008).
As the null hypothesis of no mediation states that
the indirect effect is 0, the null hypothesis is rejected
when the condence interval does not include 0. In
the present analysis, the condence interval (with
5,000 resamples) for the estimate of the indirect
effect of Contrast 1 on performance did not include
0, 95% CI [0.02, 0.28], thus conrming that auto-
matic associations mediated the relation between
condition (stereotype consistent vs. stereotype
inconsistent) and girls performance.
Supplementary Analyses
Although main analyses showed that stereotype
endorsement did not vary as a function of
condition, supplementary analyses were conducted
to test whether the relations among condition,
girls automatic associations, and girls math per-
formance were further moderated by stereotype
endorsement. Two-way analyses of variance were
conducted on both scores of automatic associations
and math performance, with condition (stereotype
consistent, control, stereotype inconsistent) and
explicit stereotype endorsement (stereotype-consis-
tent, neutral, stereotype-inconsistent response) as
the between-participants variables. In both
ANOVAs, explicit stereotype endorsement yielded
no signicant effects: The effect of condition was
signicant on both scores of automatic associa-
tions, F(2, 111) = 7.76.12, p < .001, and math per-
formance, F(2, 111) = 4.12, p < .02, and was not
further qualied by explicit stereotype endorse-
ment (all ps > .15). However, given that the rela-
tively low number of girls who provided a neutral
response or indicated the other gender group as
Math Performance
Experimental
Condition
Automatic Associations
.38* (.55**)
-.36**
-.37***
Figure 1. Results of mediation analyses testing indirect effects of
experimental condition (stereotype consistent = 0, stereotype incon-
sistent = 1) on math performance via automatic associations in
girls (n = 120).
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
258 Galdi, Cadinu, and Tomasetto
better at math resulted in unbalanced cell frequen-
cies, this nding could be a consequence of low
statistical power. To rule out this possibility, we
repeated the same analyses by recoding explicit
stereotype endorsement into a dichotomous vari-
able, comparing girls indicating their gender group
as better at math with those who either indicated
boys as better or provided a neutral response.
Results were very similar: The effect of condition
remained signicant on both automatic associa-
tions, F(2, 114) = 6.83, p < .01, and math perfor-
mance, F(2, 114) = 4.59, p < .01, and was not
further qualied by explicit stereotype endorse-
ment (all ps > .09).
We also tested whether automatic associations may
act as a moderator, rather than a mediator, of the
effect of stereotype activation on girls math perfor-
mance. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was
performed on math performance with condition
(stereotype consistent, control, and stereotype incon-
sistent), automatic associations, and the interaction
between condition and automatic associations as
predictors. The main effect of automatic associations
was signicant, F(1, 114) = 5.57, p < .02, whereas the
interaction between condition and automatic associa-
tions was not (p > .20), thus conrming that auto-
matic associations acted as a mediator of the relation
between condition and performance.
Finally, we tested whether automatic associations
and stereotype endorsement may interact with each
other in determining girls vulnerability to stereo-
type threat. We carried out a three-way ANCOVA
on math performance with condition (stereotype
consistent, control, stereotype inconsistent), auto-
matic associations, and explicit stereotype endorse-
ment response (stereotype consistent, neutral,
stereotype inconsistent) as predictors. Again, only
the main effect of automatic associations was signif-
icant, F(1, 114) = 4.01, p < .05, whereas no other
main effect or higher order interaction attained sig-
nicance (all ps > .10).
Although boys showed null ndings on all
dependent variables, the same supplementary anal-
yses were conducted for them as well. In all analy-
ses, neither main effects nor interactions reached
signicance (all ps > .08).
Discussion
Although the stereotype threat model identies ste-
reotype awareness as a requirement for stereotype
threat effects, research has shown that gender iden-
tity activation affects girls math performance before
the emergence of stereotype awareness, and in the
absence of endorsement of the mathgender stereo-
type (Ambady et al., 2001; Neuville & Croizet,
2007; Tomasetto et al., 2011). The present research
disentangles this paradox and extends the stereo-
type threat model by demonstrating that automatic
associations consistent with a negative in-group ste-
reotype represent a key factor that may trigger
stereotype threat in young children.
Consistent with research showing that automatic
associations can precede conscious beliefs (e.g., Gaw-
ronski & Galdi, 2011), we demonstrated for the rst
time that girls acquire the mathgender stereotype as
automatic associations before its emergence at the
explicit level: Despite the absence of evidence that
children endorsed the mathgender stereotype,
6-year-old girls, but not boys, even in the control
condition (i.e., in the absence of any experimental
manipulation aimed at increasing or decreasing the
salience of the stereotype) showed stereotype-consis-
tent automatic associations between boy and mathe-
matics, and girl and language. Moreover, consistent
with research showing malle-ability of automatic
associations in adults (e.g., Dasgupta & Asgari,
2004), for the rst time, malleability of girls auto-
matic associations was found. After coloring a draw-
ing of a boy correctly solving a math problem, girls
stereotypical automatic associations were activated,
as compared to the control condition. At the same
time, after coloring a picture of a girl succeeding in
math, stereotypical automatic associations were
reduced.
Importantly, the activation of automatic associa-
tions in the stereotype-consistent condition ham-
pered girls math performance as compared to the
stereotype-inconsistent condition, in which, con-
versely, the reduced activation of automatic associa-
tions led girls to the highest performance.
Furthermore, highlighting the role of counterstereo-
typical information in counteracting stereotype
threat effects, these ndings are parallel to research
showing that providing information about equal
gender abilities in math (i.e., stereotype-inconsistent
information) is an effective strategy to prevent
womens performance decits (e.g., Cadinu, Maass,
Frigerio, Impagliazzo, & Latinotti, 2003).
An open issue of this study concerns the poten-
tial processes underlying girls underperformance in
the stereotype-consistent versus -inconsistent condi-
tion. Consistent with Forbes and Schmader (2010),
one may speculate that activated stereotypical auto-
matic associations in the stereotype consistent con-
dition may have burdened working memory capacity,
thus disrupting subsequent math performance.
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 259
Conversely, the reduced activation in the stereo-
type-inconsistent condition may have freed up
working memory resources, thus enhancing subse-
quent performance. However, no such conclusions
can be drawn as this study did not test how girls
automatic associations affect performance. There-
fore, a direct test of the working memory hypothe-
sis should be the goal of future studies.
Another potential process underlying girls worse
performance in the stereotype-consistent than the
stereotype-inconsistent condition could be a mere
priming effect, which would lead individuals to
behave automatically consistent with a cognitively
activated image (e.g., Wheeler & Petty, 2001). How-
ever, if a priming effect were at work, one should
expect underperformance even for boys in response
to stereotype-related stimuli. To the contrary, nei-
ther boys automatic associations nor performance
were affected by the activation of the mathgender
(counter)stereotype. Thus, differently from a prim-
ing effect, we argue that in this study it was the
membership in a negatively stereotyped group
(whose stereotype has been acquired via automatic
associations) that was responsible for girls perfor-
mance decit under stereotype threat.
Although girls automatic associations increased
and decreased in activation depending on the ste-
reotype content of the experimental condition, no
changes emerged at the level of endorsement of ste-
reotypical beliefs. Consistent with research showing
gender in-group bias in young children, the major-
ity of children across conditions indicated their gen-
der as superior in math. We argue that this lack of
endorsement of the mathgender stereotype further
strengthens the role of automatic associations in the
process of stereotype acquisition in children.
Relative to the latest point, the present pattern of
ndings raises the tricky issue concerning the
sources and mechanisms underlying the formation
of automatic associations. Up to date, highly inuen-
tial theorizing has posited that automatic associa-
tions stem from attitudes and conscious beliefs that
have become overlearned and automatized over time
(e.g., Rudman, 2004; Wilson et al., 2000). However,
recent research (e.g., Gawronski & Galdi, 2011), as
well as the present results, suggests that also other
underlying mechanisms may be responsible for the
formation of automatic associations. Drawing on the
distinction between associative and propositional
learning (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006), we
argue that the automatic formation of stereotype-con-
sistent associative links in childrens cognitive map
could stem from repeated co-occurrences of objects
or stimuli in childrens social environment. For
example, ample evidence shows that children are
sensitive to adults nonverbal behavior and that such
behaviors, often occurring in automatic and uncon-
scious ways, may represent an important channel
through which attitudes are transmitted (e.g., Rud-
man, 2004; Walden & Ogan, 1988). Thus, even
though adults may explicitly encourage nonstereo-
typical interests in children, they may exhibit differ-
ent patterns of nonverbal behaviors: Parents may
purchase more games or manipulative materials
related to math and science for their sons than for
their daughters (Jacobs & Bleeker, 2004), or intrude
more often in their daughters than in their sons
math homework to offer unsolicited help (e.g.,
Bahnot & Jovanovic, 2005). As a result, such repeated
co-occurrences of objects and events might promote
the automatic formation of stereotype-consistent
associative links in childrens cognitive maps.
In contrast to girls, boys did not reveal stereotypi-
cal automatic associations in any conditions.
Consistent with research demonstrating that elemen-
tary school boys tend to manifest in-group favoritism
regarding both math and language (Steele, 2003),
these results could simply reect the fact that boys
showed in-groupserving automatic associations
about both math and language domains (Steffens
et al., 2010), regardless of the stereotype content con-
veyed by the experimental condition. Indeed, the
presence of in-group-serving boymathematics and
boylanguage automatic associations would result in
fast response latencies in both critical blocks of the
ChildIAT. Thus, given that the D-algorithm com-
pares the extent to which performance on the incom-
patible (i.e., girlmath and boylanguage sharing the
same response key) is impaired relative to the
compatible critical block (i.e., girllanguage and
boymath sharing the same response key), in-group
serving automatic associations could have led to the
small or null score of automatic associations that was
found for boys across conditions. However, because
both gender stereotypes regarding math and
language contribute to the ChildIAT score, and can-
not be separated (Nosek et al., 2005), this possibility
could not be tested in this study. To our knowledge,
only one study used an implicit measure to assess
mathgender and languagegender stereotypes
separately (i.e., Go-No-Go association task [GNAT];
Nosek & Banaji, 2001) in a sample of 14-year-olds
and university students. Consistent with the in-
groupserving explanation, Steffens and Jelenecs
(2011) male participants revealed both boysmath
stereotypical automatic associations and boyslan-
guage counterstereotypical automatic associations.
However, as Steffens and Jelenec noted, the internal
260 Galdi, Cadinu, and Tomasetto
consistency of the GNAT was low . . . and the IAT
clearly appears more sensitive (Steffens & Jelenec,
2011, p. 332). Thus, a goal of future studies should be
to test whether other implicit measures that combine
the measurement quality of the IAT with the separate
measurement of math and language gender stereo-
types, such as the GNAT, can be implemented and
adapted for use with young children.
Studies on the development of cognitive compe-
tencies offer alternative explanations for boys lack
of automatic associations. For example, previous
research has shown earlier knowledge of gender
categories (Zosuls et al., 2009) and earlier achieve-
ment of gender constancy (Ruble et al., 2007) by
girls than boys. At the same time, differences in
gender categorization abilities between girls and
boys could also be the result of socialization pro-
cesses. For example, women show stronger auto-
matic associations between the self, their gender,
and the in-group gender stereotype as compared to
men (Cadinu & Galdi, 2012), and children from
low-status groups are more likely to be aware of
self-relevant stereotypes than children from high-
status groups (McKown & Weinstein, 2003).
Although consistent with Steffens et al. (2010),
the result that boys did not manifest stereotypical
automatic associations is inconsistent with Cvencek
et al.s (2011) ndings, showing stereotypical auto-
matic associations between gender and academic
subjects in both 6- to 7-year-old boys and girls.
However, this discrepancy might reect cross-
cultural variations in the strength of gender stereo-
types at the societal level, which have been found
to be associated with either smaller or bigger gen-
der differences in the strength of stereotypical auto-
matic associations (Nosek et al., 2009), as well as in
the endorsement of gender stereotypes about
academic abilities (e.g., Evans, Schweingruber, &
Stevenson, 2002). Moreover, in a study involving
Chinese, Japanese, and American children, Lummis
and Stevenson (1990) found that whereas American
and Chinese 6-year-olds do not believe that there
are gender differences in math, 6-year-old Japanese
children tend to believe that boys are better than
girls at math (see also Del Rio & Strasser, 2013).
These results show that cultural differences in the
strength of gender stereotypes about academic abili-
ties, in schooling, and in out-of-school experiences,
may lead to different acquisition ages of the
endorsement of these stereotypes, and suggest that
the same could be true regarding the acquisition of
stereotypical automatic associations.
In conclusion, this study has the potential to con-
tribute to the stereotype threat model by suggesting
automatic associations as a novel prerequisite for
stereotype-induced underperformance in young
children. Importantly, by showing also that chil-
drens automatic associations are malleable, these
ndings are promising in terms of interventions to
promote gender equality in math and science
because they suggest that girls can be protected
from the deleterious impact of mathgender stereo-
types. Relevant to this claim is a study (Dasgupta &
Greenwald, 2001) demonstrating that Caucasian
participants exposed to images of admired Black
and disliked White exemplars showed lower pro-
White automatic associations than participants
exposed to images of admired Whites and disliked
Blacks. Interestingly, such a decrease in pro-White
automatic associations lasted for 24 hr, suggesting
that relatively enduring changes in automatic associ-
ations can be obtained. If so, repeatedly presenting
girls with exemplars of successful women in math
and science could promote the reduction in stereo-
typical automatic associations, long before girls have
acquired any awareness or endorsement of the ste-
reotype favoring males in math. This study suggests
that this strategy could protect girls performance in
stereotype-threatening situations and potentially
help them to expand their interests toward tradi-
tionally male domains.
References
Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. New York:
Basil Blackwell.
Ambady, N., Shih, M., Kim, A., & Pittinsky, T. (2001).
Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity
activation on quantitative performance. Psychological
Science, 12, 385390. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00371
Aronson, J., & Good, C. (2003). The development and
consequences of stereotype vulnerability in adolescents.
In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and educa-
tion: Vol. 2. Academic motivation of adolescents (pp. 299
330). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Augoustinos, M., & Rosewarne, D. L. (2001). Stereotype
knowledge and prejudice in children. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 19, 143156. doi:10.1348/
026151001165912
Bahnot, R., & Jovanovic, J. (2005). Do parents academic
gender stereotypes inuence whether they intrude on
their childrens homework? Sex Roles, 52, 597607.
doi:10.1007/s11199-005-3728-4
Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. (2006). The development of
implicit attitudes. Evidence of race evaluations from
ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17,
5358. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x
Cadinu, M., & Galdi, S. (2012). The cognitive accessibility
of womanhood: Implicit gender self-categorization
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 261
leads to stronger self-stereotyping by women than men.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 546551. doi:
10.1002/ejsp.1881
Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Frigerio, S., Impagliazzo, L., &
Latinotti, S. (2003). Stereotype threat: The effect of
expectancy on performance. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 33, 267285. doi:10.1002/ejsp.145
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011).
Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school children.
Child Development, 82, 766779. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.
2010.01529.x
Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing:
Exposure to counter-stereotypic women leaders and its
effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereo-
typing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 642
658. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.003
Dasgupta, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the
malleability of automatic attitudes combating automatic
prejudice with images of admired and disliked
individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
81, 800814. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.800
Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M., & Gerhard-
stein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television
commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain
women academically and professionally. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 16151628. doi:10.1177/
014616702237644
Del Rio, M. F., & Strasser, K. (2013). Preschool childrens
beliefs about gender differences in academic skills. Sex
Roles, 68, 231238. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0195-6
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of atti-
tudes. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Evans, E. M., Schweingruber, H., & Stevenson, H. W.
(2002). Gender differences in interest and knowledge
acquisition: The United States, Taiwan, and Japan. Sex
Roles, 47, 153167. doi:10.1023/A:1021047122532
Forbes, C., & Schmader, T. (2010). Retraining attitudes
and stereotypes to affect motivation and cognitive
capacity under stereotype threat. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 99, 740754. doi:10.1037/a0020971
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative
and propositional processes in evaluation: An integra-
tive review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 132, 692731. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.
132.5.692
Gawronski, B., & Galdi, S. (2011). Using implicit mea-
sures to read the minds of undecided voters. In M.
Cadinu, S. Galdi, & A. Maass (Eds.), Social perception,
cognition, and language in honour of Arcuri (pp. 203216).
Padova, Italy: CLEUP.
Gawronski, B., & LeBel, E. P. (2008). Understanding pat-
terns of attitude change: When implicit measures show
change, but explicit measures do not. Journal of Experi-
mental Social Psychology, 44, 13551361. doi:10.1016/
j.jesp.2008.04.005
Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of
implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applica-
tions. New York: Guilford Press.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L.
(1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cog-
nition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.74.6.1464
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003).
Understanding and using the implicit association test: I.
An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 85, 187216. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.85.2.197
Hamilton, D. L., & Trolier, T. K. (1986). Stereotypes and
stereotyping: An overview of the cognitive approach. In
J. Dovidio & S. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination,
and racism (pp. 127163). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Hayes, A. F., & Preacher, K. J. (2012). Statistical mediation
analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. Man-
uscript under review. Retrieved November 10, 2012,
from http://www.afhayes.com/public/hp2012.pdf
Inzlicht, M., & Schmader, T. (Eds.). (2012). Stereotype
threat: Theory, process, and application. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Jacobs, J. E., & Bleeker, M. M. (2004). Girls and boys
developing interests in math and science: Do parents
matter? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Develop-
ment, 106, 521. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.97
Katz, P. A., & Kofkin, J. A. (1997). Race, gender, and young
children. In S. S. Luthar, J. A. Burack, D. Cicchetti, & J.
Weisz (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Perspectives
on adjustment, risk, and disorder (pp. 5174). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). Implicit stereo-
types, gender identication, and math-related out-
comes: A perspective study of female college students.
Psychological Science, 18, 1318. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.
2007.01841.x
Lummis, M., & Stevenson, H. W. (1990). Gender differ-
ences in beliefs and achievement: A cross-cultural
study. Developmental Psychology, 26, 254263. doi:
10.1037/0012-1649.26.2.254
Martin, C., & Ruble, D. (2010). Patterns of gender devel-
opment. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 353381.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100511
Martinot, D., Bages, C., & Desert, M. (2012). French chil-
drens awareness of gender stereotypes about mathe-
matics and reading: When girls improve their
reputation in math. Sex Roles, 66, 210219. doi:10.1007/
s11199-011-0032-3
Martinot, D., & Desert, M. (2007). Awareness of a gender
stereotype, personal beliefs and self-perceptions regard-
ing math ability: When boys do not surpass girls. Social
Psychology of Education, 10, 455471. doi:10.1007/
s11218-007-9028-9
McKown, C., & Strambler, M. J. (2009). Developmental
antecedents and social and academic consequences of
stereotype-consciousness in middle childhood. Child
Development, 80, 16431659. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.
2009.01359.x
262 Galdi, Cadinu, and Tomasetto
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. (2003). The development
and consequences of stereotype consciousness in mid-
dle childhood. Child Development, 74, 498515. doi:10.
1111/1467-8624.7402012
Muzzatti, B., & Agnoli, F. (2007). Gender and mathemat-
ics: Attitudes and stereotype threat susceptibility in
Italian children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 747759.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.747
Neuville, E., & Croizet, J. (2007). Can salience of gender
identity impair math performance among 78 years old
girls? The moderating role of task difculty. European
Journal of Psychology of Education, 22, 307316. doi:
10.1007/BF03173428
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go associ-
ation task. Social Cognition, 19, 625666. doi:10.1521/
soco.19.6.625.20886
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002).
Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a
demonstration web site. Group Dynamics, 6, 101115.
doi:10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005).
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test:
II. Method, variables, and construct validity. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 166180. doi:10.1177/
0146167204271418
Nosek, B. A., & Smyth, F. L. (2011). Implicit social cogni-
tions predict sex differences in math engagement and
achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 48,
11251156. doi:10.3102/0002831211410683
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F., Sriram, N., Lindner, N., Devos,
T., Ayala, A., et al. (2009). National differences in
gender-science stereotypes predict national sex
differences in science and math achievement. Proceed-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 106, 1059310597. doi:10.1073/pnas.
0809921106
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically
activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative
conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
32, 421433. doi:10.1177/0146167205284004
Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). John thinks that Mary
thinks that: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to
10-year old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
ogy, 39, 437471. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7
Powlishta, K. K. (1995). Gender bias in childrens percep-
tions of personality traits. Sex Roles, 32, 1728. doi:
10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.781
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and re-
sampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect
effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research
Methods, 40, 879891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
Quintana, S. M. (1998). Childrens developmental under-
standing of ethnicity and race. Applied and Preventive
Psychology, 7, 2745. doi:10.1016/S0962-1849(98)80020-6
Rholes, W. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1984). Childrens under-
standing of dispositional characteristics of others. Child
Development, 55, 550560.
Ruble, D., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K., Lurye,
L. E., & Shrout, P. (2007). The role of gender constancy
in early gender development. Child Development, 78,
11211136. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01056.x
Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Cur-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 7982. doi:
10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00279.x
Selman, R. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understand-
ing: Developmental and clinical analysis. New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes
shape intellectual identity and performance. American
Psychologist, 52, 613629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and
intellectual test performance of African Americans.
Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797811.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797
Steele, J. (2003). Childrens gender stereotypes about
math: The role of stereotype stratication. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 33, 25872606. doi:10.1111/
j.1559-1816.2003.tb02782.x
Steffens, M. C., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit
gender stereotypes regarding math and language:
Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and
man, but not for girls and women. Sex Roles, 64, 324
335. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x
Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., & Noack, P. (2010). On the
leaky math pipeline: Comparing implicit math-gender
stereotypes and math withdrawal in female and male
children and adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 102, 947963. doi:10.1037/a0019920
Thoman, D., White, P., Yamawaki, N., & Koishi, H.
(2008). Variations of gender-math stereotype content
affect womens vulnerability to stereotype threat. Sex
Roles, 58, 702712. doi:10.1007/s11199-008-9390-x
Tomasetto, C., Alparone, F. R., & Cadinu, M. (2011).
Girls math performance under stereotype threat: The
moderating role of mothers gender stereotypes. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 47, 943949. doi:10.1037/a0024047
Walden, T., & Ogan, T. (1988). The development of social
referencing. Child Development, 59, 12301240.
Wheeler, S. C., & Petty, R. E. (2001). The effects of stereo-
type activation on behavior: A review of possible mech-
anisms. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 797826. doi:10.1037/
0033-2909.127.6.797
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A
model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101
126. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101
Yee, M., & Brown, R. (1994). The development of gender
differentiation in young children. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 33, 183196. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.
tb01017.x
Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout,
P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The
acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for
sex-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 688701.
doi:10.1037/a0014053
Stereotype Threat Before Stereotype Awareness 263

S-ar putea să vă placă și