Sunteți pe pagina 1din 25

Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 www.brill.

nl/jocc

Metacognition of Problem-Solving Strategies


in Brazil, India, and the United States

C. Dominik Güss
Brian Wiley
Department of Psychology, University of North Florida
dguess@unf.edu

Abstract
Metacognition, the observation of one’s own thinking, is a key cognitive ability that allows humans
to influence and restructure their own thought processes. The influence of culture on metacognitive
strategies is a relatively new topic. Using Antonietti’s, Ignazi’s and Perego’s questionnaire on
metacognitive knowledge about problem-solving strategies (2000), five strategies in three life
domains were assessed among student samples in Brazil, India, and the United States (N=317),
regarding the frequency, facility, and efficacy of these strategies. To investigate cross-cultural
similarities and differences in strategy use, nationality and uncertainty avoidance values were
independent variables. Uncertainty avoidance was expected to lead to high frequency of decision
strategies. However, results showed no effect of uncertainty avoidance on frequency, but an effect
on facility of metacognitive strategies. Comparing the three cultural samples, all rated analogy as
the most frequent strategy. Only in the U.S. sample, analogy was also rated as the most effective and
easy to apply strategy. Every cultural group showed a different preference regarding what
metacognitive strategy was most effective. Indian participants found the free production strategy to
be more effective, and Indian and Brazilian participants found the combination strategy to be more
effective compared to the U.S. participants. As key abilities for the five strategies, Indians rated
speed, Brazilians rated synthesis, and U.S. participants rated critical thinking as more important
than the other participants. These results reflect the embedded nature and functionality of problem-
solving strategies in specific cultural environments. The findings will be discussed referring to an
eco-cultural framework.

Keywords
Metacognition, Problem Solving, Culture, Decision Making

Metacognition, thinking about one’s own thinking, is a key cognitive ability.


It allows humans to control and restructure their own thoughts and it plays
a crucial role in learning and problem solving (Akama & Yamauchi, 2004;
Dörner, Kreuzig, Reither, & Stäudel, 1983). The effects of metacognition on

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2007 DOI: 10.1163/156853707X171793

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 1 3/14/07 8:32:39 PM


2 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

problem solving have been widely studied in the school context with children
(e.g. Flavell, 1976). Many training programs have been developed and evaluation
of these programs shows positive effects of metacognitive activities on learning
(e.g., Lin, 2001) regardless of interindividual differences of participants (e.g.
regarding achievement see Mevarech & Kramarski, 2003) or certain disabilities
(e.g. regarding learning and attention disorders see Lauth, 1996).
Metacognition is especially important during the stages of problem solving:
realization that there is a problem, definition of goals, mental representation of
the problem, decision on overall strategy, information collection, prediction of
further developments, planning and evaluation of possible solutions, decision
making, monitoring problem solving, action, and evaluation of outcome
(Dörner, 1996; Duncker, 1945; Sternberg, 2003, p. 361). Metacognition is a
part of these problem-solving stages, especially during monitoring and evalua-
tion of solutions and outcomes. Stage theories often suggest a linear process of
problem solving: we proceed through stages in a sequential order, one stage at a
time. However, in every problem situation all of these stages are not equally
important and these stages are not followed sequentially in every problem situa-
tion. Rather, they are interconnected, and oftentimes problem solving requires
returning to a previous stage and resuming the process from that point or going
through those stages in a non-sequential, recursive order.
As the following brief summary shows, most theories on problem solving
focus on the stage of planning and evaluation of possible solutions. We follow
here Antonietti’s, Ignazi’s, and Perego’s (2000) discussion of five different prob-
lem-solving approaches. A first approach argues that problem solving consists
mainly of generation of many ideas (e.g. Johnson-Laird, 1993). A second
approach assumes that problem solving consists mainly of new combinations
of existing knowledge (e.g. Simonton, 1984). A third approach highlights
the power of analogies in problem solving (e.g. Vosniadou & Ortony, 1989).
A fourth approach sees problem solving as transforming an initial undesired
state into a desired goal state through a series of operators as described, for exam-
ple, in the means-end analysis (Newell & Simon, 1972). Finally, problem solving
can be viewed primarily as restructuring the representation of the problem situ-
ation (Wertheimer, 1959). The first four approaches focus on how to arrive at a
possible and promising solution. The last approach, the Gestalt approach, high-
lights the problem representation aspect in problem solving. In all of
these approaches, metacognition plays an important role: for generating new
ideas, producing new combinations of knowledge, thinking of analogies, coming
up with a specific combination of operators, and restructuring the problem rep-
resentation. Antonietti et al. (2000) developed a questionnaire assessing meta-
cognition of these five problem-solving strategies in three different problem

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 2 3/14/07 8:32:41 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 3

situations: interpersonal, practical, and study problems. The authors could


show that each strategy’s frequency, efficacy and facility of implementation are
highly related.
Metacognition can enhance problem-solving performance not only in well-defined
problems like the tower of Hanoi (e.g. Beradi-Coletta, Buyer, Dominowski, &
Rellinger, 1995) or puzzle problems (Akama & Yamauchi, 2004), but also in
complex and dynamic, ill-defined problems (e.g. Tisdale, 1998). Metacognition
becomes especially important in ill-defined problems as the problem solver can-
not rely as much on domain-specific knowledge (Land, 2004). Thus, the focus
on the problem-solving process becomes more relevant. To reflect on this pro-
cess leads to a deeper understanding of the problem and to a more flexible and
successful approach to solve the problem. For example, in the information col-
lection stage, Schmidt and Ford (2003) demonstrated that metacognitive activi-
ties go hand in hand with more successful acquisition of relevant knowledge.
They showed this using the real world problem of creating web pages. Chi, Bas-
sok, Lewis, Reimann & Glaser (1989) showed that successful problem solvers
more often reflect on their own problem solving. Experts compared to novices,
for example, are more skilled in allocating their time during problem solving and
realizing when they make errors (Carlson, 1997; Glaser & Chi, 1988). Engaging
in metacognitive activities, problem solvers become aware of their strengths, but
also of their limitations (Bransford, Brown, & Cooking, 1999) and suppressing
metacognitive processes during problem solving can lead to a decrease in perfor-
mance (Bartl & Dörner, 1998).

Culture, Problem Solving, and Metacognition


Although metacognition has been studied widely in western cultures, it has
not yet been thoroughly studied cross-culturally (see Davidson, 1994). Several
cross-cultural studies on problem solving, coping with conflicts, planning, and
decision-making in different cultures (Mann, Radford, Burnett, Ford, Bond,
Leung, Nakamura, Vaughan, & Yang, 1998; see Weber, & Hsee, 2000 for
an overview on decision making and culture) highlight different strategic
approaches and thus would indicate differences in metacognitive problem-
solving strategies as well. For example, planning behavior in Brazil, India,
and the U.S. was compared in several studies using problem scenarios with
open-ended questions (Güss, 2000; Strohschneider & Güss, 1998). Brazilian
participants accepted the problem situations as they were. They developed short
plans following only one direction. Most of their outcome expectations were
hopeful, a result that goes hand in hand with the description of Brazilian opti-
mism in other studies (Stubbe, 1987; Scheper-Hughes, 1990). Brazilian planning

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 3 3/14/07 8:32:41 PM


4 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

and problem solving have also been described as creative (Fleith, 2002) often
using improvisation (see Brazilian term “jeito” in Stubbe, 1987).
Indian participants proved similar to the Brazilians in that they accepted the
problem situation and were optimistic with regards to the plans’ outcomes.
However, Indian plans were longer. Their optimism and detailed plans were
quite contrary to the western stereotype that describes Indians as “fatalistic.” In
a study on Indian problem solving, Indian students showed a flexible approach,
adjusting to the changes in the situation (Güss, 2002). This result goes hand in
hand with the description of India as a high context culture (Hall, 1976): behav-
iors are highly selected according to the specific context characteristics. Many
Western individualistic cultures are low context cultures, where behaviors are
less influenced by the context.
American students developed short plans, but their plans consisted of different
alternatives and many questions (Glencross & Güss, 2004). Americans were
more skeptical about their future expectations. In other studies, Americans
showed a preference for assertive tactics in dealing with problems (Ohbuchi,
Fukushima, & Tedeschi, 1999). The described cross-cultural difference in plan-
ning and problem solving suggest that Brazilians, Indians, and Americans also
use different metacognitive strategies during these processes.
It is about time we defined culture before further discussion on cross-cultural
research on planning and problem-solving. Culture is a term that is difficult to
grasp and has been defined in many different ways (see Kroeber & Kluckhohn,
1963). Under a relativistic perspective expressed by cultural psychologists, cul-
ture is often seen as a whole that cannot be divided into separate parts. Most
cross-cultural psychologists, on the other hand, see culture as a set of separate
variables (Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, 2002). In order to understand cul-
tural differences, such variables should then be studied in quasi-experimental
designs. Such studies would not only describe differences between cultures, but
also answer the question why certain cultures differ. For our further discussion,
culture can be defined as implicit and explicit shared knowledge that is transmit-
ted from generation to generation (Smith & Bond, 1998).
Cultural differences in problem solving might be due to differences in this
implicit and explicit knowledge; specifically in goals, means to achieve these
goals, and the transfer of goals and means to other situations (Saxe, 1994). Cul-
tural differences in the stability and predictability of the environment, and in
basic values such as individualism and collectivism might influence the selection
of goals and means, and the degree of metacognitive engagement (Strohsch-
neider & Güss, 1998). Whereas one cultural environment may allow for detailed
information collection and reflection, another cultural environment may
demand quick action. Whereas one culture requires strategic planning for nego-

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 4 3/14/07 8:32:41 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 5

tiation with in-group members before making decisions, another culture would
require individual decision making without much consideration of the opinion
of the in-group.
The use of different metacognitive strategies in different cultures can be a
result of different socialization processes. School institutions, parents, teachers,
and peers may play different roles in this socialization process in different
cultures (Carr & Borkowski, 1989). The authors also stress how different moti-
vational orientations in different cultures can influence when and how someone
engages in metacognitive activities. Three empirical studies highlight cross-
cultural differences in metacognition in the educational context. A first study
comparing German and American parents showed that Germans reported more
frequent instruction of metacognitive strategies at home, and German children
showed use of these strategies more than American children did (Carr, Kurtz,
Schneider, Turner, & Borkowski, 1989). Second, Davidson and Freebody (1988)
found differences on metacognitive knowledge about school learning of Austra-
lian children from varied ethnicities. Participants’ metacognitive knowledge
increased with the occupational status of participants’ fathers. Third, McCafferty
(1992) compared the metacognitive activity of students learning English as a
second language between two small samples of Hispanic and Oriental college
students. Hispanics showed more metacognition during communication in the
second language.
Although these three studies show cross-cultural differences in metacogni-
tion, they are not related to problem-solving processes. The current study then is
an attempt to investigate cross-cultural differences in metacognition, specifically
in metacognition of problem-solving strategies.

Situatedness or Generality of Metacognition

The fact that research shows cross-cultural differences in problem solving and
metacognition is important. Cross-cultural research previously described along
with other studies (e.g. Veenman, Wilhelm, & Beishuizen, 2004) suggest that
metacognitive processes are more general cognitive preferences. On the other
hand, one might argue that metacognitive strategies are influenced by the specific
demands of the situation (see “situatedness” described by Rohlfing, Rehm, &
Goecke, 2003). Whereas some people, for example, might engage in metacogni-
tion when confronted with a problem at work, they might not engage in meta-
cognition and problem solving when they are confronted with a private problem.
Dunlosky (1998) argued that further research should address this important
question of whether metacognitive processes are indeed different in different

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 5 3/14/07 8:32:41 PM


6 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

problem domains or more general cognitive styles as there is not yet enough
empirical evidence to support either side.

Uncertainty Avoidance and Problem-Solving Strategies


One cultural variable that might possibly explain differences in metacognitive
problem solving strategies is values. Basic value dimensions are one of the most
widely studied aspects of different cultures (e.g. Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz, 1994;
Smith, Peterson, Schwartz, 2002). A value can be defined as “a conception,
explicit or implicit, distinctive of an individual or characteristic for a group, of
the desirable which influences the selection from available modes, means and
ends of actions” (Kluckhohn & Murray, 1953, p. 59). Under a cognitive perspec-
tive, values can be seen as abstract goals acquired during the socialization and
enculturation processes. These goals are guiding principles for the selection of
subgoals and for the selection of means to achieving those subgoals (Rokeach,
1973). Therefore, values can guide the problem-solving process. One of the basic
value dimensions studied cross-culturally is uncertainty avoidance. Uncertainty
avoidance is “the extent to which the members of a culture feel threatened by
uncertain or unknown situations” (Hofstede, 2001, p. 161). Hofstede (2001)
distinguishes three components of uncertainty avoidance: rule orientation,
employment stability, and stress. High uncertainty avoidance is expressed in
strong rule orientation (Rule orientation stands for agreement and acceptance of
existing norms and rules), preference for employment stability, and high scores
for stress. High uncertainty avoidance might go hand in hand with high engage-
ment in metacognitive and problem-solving activities. To cope with the threat
of uncertainty, people might vary in the degree they engage in metacognitive
activities.

Research Questions
This study investigates the following questions. Do people from different cultures
differ in their metacognitive problem solving strategies? More specific questions
relate to our cultural samples: Do American participants show little variance
in metacognitive styles across situations, as the U.S. has often been characterized
as an individualistic (Hofstede, 2001), low-context culture (Hall, 1976)? Do
Indian participants show high variance in metacognitive problem solving
styles across situations, as India can be characterized as a high-context culture?
Do Brazilian participants show lower frequencies of metacognitive strategies
(Güss, 2000) as Brazilian thinking has been described as optimistic and more

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 6 3/14/07 8:32:42 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 7

focused on the present (Güss, Glencross, Tuason, Summerlin, & Richard, 2004;
Strohschneider & Güss, 1998)? Do cultures understand metacognitive problem-
solving strategies in a similar way and are the required abilities for a strategy the
same across cultures? Do cultural values, specifically uncertainty avoidance,
influence selection of problem-solving strategies? Does high uncertainty avoid-
ance lead to more frequent use of problem-solving strategies? Are frequency,
efficacy, and facility of reported strategy use highly related as shown by Anto-
nietti et al. (2000)? Are reported problem solving-strategies situation-specific or
similar across situations?

Method

Participants
Participants were sampled from three countries: the U.S.A., Brazil, and India. As
this study is a part of a larger project, the selection of these countries was
influenced by differences on the individualism-collectivism dimension. If we can
couch it in oversimplified terms, it can be said that the U.S. represents an indi-
vidualistic society, Brazil represents a collective society (Hofstede, 2001), and
India represents the middle of the individualistic-collectivistic continuum (Sinha
& Tripathi, 1994). Hofstede’s (2001) data from the late 1960s and early 1970s
assessing uncertainty avoidance in 50 countries and three regions with three
questions show average uncertainty avoidance for Brazil (76), and low uncer-
tainty avoidance for the U.S. (46) and for India (40). In the rank order of uncer-
tainty avoidance among 53 countries, Brazil has rank 21/22, India rank 45, and
the U.S. rank 43.
We intended to gather comparable samples across cultures regarding age,
study subject, and gender. Participants were 133 U.S., 97 Brazilian, and 97 Indian
students. The U.S. sample had a gender breakdown of 66% female and 34% male,
with an average age of 22.5. The Brazilian sample had a gender breakdown of
72% female and 28% male with an average age of 23.8. The Indian sample had a
gender breakdown of 52% female and 48% male with an average age of 22. Over
all three samples, 68% were studying psychology, 12% were studying business,
12% were studying social sciences, and 8% were studying natural sciences.

Measures
Uncertainty avoidance was measured with six items derived from Hofstede
(2001, p. 150). They were slightly modified since participants in this study
were students and not managers. The items referred either to the educational or

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 7 3/14/07 8:32:42 PM


8 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

the work environment (see Appendix A), e.g. “Company rules should not be
broken – even when the employee thinks it is in the company’s best interests” or
“How often do you feel nervous or tense at class?” Responses were scored on a
five-point scale. Several alpha coefficients for this scale were calculated including
only two items, three, four or five items for the overall sample and for the cul-
tural subsamples. The alpha coefficients vary from .05 to .50. Combining the
items referring to anxiety, work preferences, and rule orientation leads to low
alpha coefficients. One reason for this finding might be cross-cultural differences
regarding work experience. In fact, 82% of the U.S. participants, 64% of the Bra-
zilian participants, and 22% of the Indian participants have work experience
longer than one year (summarizing answers that indicate one to two years work
experience and answers indicating more than two years work experience). Eigh-
teen percent of the U.S. participants, 36% of the Brazilian participants, and 78%
of the Indian participants have work experience of less than one year. These
differences between the samples regarding work experience are statistically
significant, χ2 (6, N = 313) = 78.34, p < .001.
The highest alpha coefficient of .80 was found by calculating the reliability
of the first two items only (see Appendix A: “Company rules should not be bro-
ken – even when the employee thinks it is in the company’s best interests.” “Uni-
versity rules should not be broken – even when the student thinks it is in the
university’s best interests.”). These two items refer to rule orientation in compa-
nies and universities. Rule orientation is only one of the three uncertainty avoid-
ance dimensions of Hofstede. The other two dimensions were employment
stability and stress. For the two items assessing rule orientation, the alpha
coefficient was found to be .80 overall, .88 for the U.S. sample, .79 for the Brazil-
ian sample and .67 for the Indian sample. The same alpha coefficients result
when using grand mean centered item scores. These alpha coefficients are satis-
factory and further data analysis refers to uncertainty avoidance as assessed by
these two items.
Five different metacognitive strategies: free production, analogy (for an in-
depth case study on analogy and scientific discovery see Spranzi, 2004), step by
step, visualization, and combination were assessed with items that described
each strategy. Then the participants were asked to rate the frequency, efficacy,
and facility of that strategy on a scale from one to five across three situations:
interpersonal, study, and practical problems (Antonietti, Ignazi, & Perego, 2000).
Frequency refers to how often the strategy is used, efficacy refers to how effective
the strategy is, and facility refers to how easy it is to apply the strategy. Partici-
pants were also asked to indicate which of eight mental abilities were associated
with the metacognitive strategy. These mental abilities were creativity, speed,

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 8 3/14/07 8:32:42 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 9

synthesis, critical thinking, accuracy, memory, analysis, logical reasoning (see


Appendix B for an example).
In order to make data from the U.S., the Brazilian, and the Indian sample
comparable, they were standardized using grand mean centering (Fischer, 2004).
The overall mean score of all 45 responses of each cultural sample was calculated
and subtracted from every single individual item score. The overall mean score of
all answers was 3.29 in the US sample, 3.35 in the Brazilian sample, and 3.38 in
the Indian sample. Although the three groups did not differ significantly, F(2,
324)=1.04, p = .36, we still thought that the following statistical comparisons
using grand mean centered scores would be more accurate.

Procedure
Participants were recruited from universities through announcements in classes
and posted flyers. Participants were scheduled to complete the questionnaires in
groups. An experimenter was available to answer any questions that the partici-
pants might have had about the research materials.

Results

Age, Gender, Metacognitive Strategies, and Uncertainty Avoidance.


An alpha level of .05 was used for all statistical tests. A significant difference in
the age of participants was found, F(2, 326) = 4.59, p = .01, with Brazilian stu-
dents (M=23.8) being older than Indian participants (M=22.0). The U.S. sam-
ple had an average age of 22.5 years. Differences in age and gender are related to
the make-up of student populations in the different countries. However, no cor-
relation between age and the 45 metacognitive strategy variables was statistically
significant. Age did also not correlate significantly with uncertainty avoidance.
The gender distribution in India was significantly different from the distribu-
tions in the United States and Brazil, F(2, 326) = 7.51, p = .001. Comparing 204
female and 116 male participants, significant gender differences were found in
five out of 45 variables, namely free production-interpersonal-frequency,
t(318)=2.98, p = .003, Mm=2.91, SDm=1.23 and Mf =3.32, SDf = 1.19 before
grand mean centering; analogy-practical-easiness t(316)=2.67, p = .008;
Mm=3.43, SDm=1.10 and Mf=3.75, SDf = .98 before grand mean centering;
analogy-study-frequency, t(319)=2.30, p = .022, Mm=3.56, SDm=1.11 and
Mf =3.85, SDf = 1.10 before grand mean centering; analogy-study-usefulness,
t(319)=2.75, p = .006, Mm=3.59, SDm=1.13 and Mf =3.93, SDf = 1.06 before

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 9 3/14/07 8:32:42 PM


10 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

grand mean centering; and step-by-step-study-usefulness, t(317)=2.29, p = .02,


Mm=3.68, SDm=1.02 and Mf =3.95, SDf = 1.02 before grand mean centering; In
these five variables, female participants always had higher scores than male par-
ticipants. We included gender as a covariate in further analyses of those five vari-
ables where significant differences were found. No significant gender differences
were found in the cumulated scores across problem situations and across fre-
quency, efficacy, and facility. No significant gender differences were found
regarding uncertainty avoidance.

Relation of Frequency, Efficacy, and Facility of Problem-Solving Strategy


To investigate if frequency, efficacy, and facility of problem-solving strategy are
related, Pearson correlations between these different scores in each strategy and
type of problem were calculated (see Table 1). All correlations were significant at
the 1% level and ranged between .32 and .72. The average of the Frequency-
Efficacy correlations was .63, the average of the Frequency-Facility correlations
was .54, and the average of the Efficacy-Facility correlations was .48. The results
show that the frequency of strategy use is related to its perceived efficacy and its
perceived facility of implementation.

Table 1
Correlations of Frequency, Efficacy, and Facility Scores
for Each Type of Problem and Each Strategy.
N=323 to 327 Frequency- Frequency- Efficacy-
Efficacy Facility Facility
Free production
Interpersonal problems .60*** .40*** .32***
Practical problems .69*** .46*** .43***
Study problems .62*** .46*** .47***
Analogy
Interpersonal problems .53*** .51*** .44***
Practical problems .62*** .47*** .48***
Study problems .67*** .58*** .57***
Step-by-Step
Interpersonal problems .68*** .71*** .57***
Practical problems .59*** .58*** .45***
Study problems .54*** .53*** .41***
Visualisation
Interpersonal problems .72*** .61*** .58***

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 10 3/14/07 8:32:42 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 11

Table 1 (cont.)

N=323 to 327 Frequency- Frequency- Efficacy-


Efficacy Facility Facility
Practical problems .68*** .56*** .51***
Study problems .72*** .59*** .60***
Combination
Interpersonal problems .65*** .59*** .41***
Practical problems .60*** .48*** .43***
Study problems .61*** .54*** .50***
* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001

Cross-Cultural Use of Metacognition


Statistical analyses revealed differences in the use of metacognitive strategies
across cultures. Over all five strategies the differences in mean frequency were
not significant, F(2, 308) = 2.65, p = .07, ηp2=.02). The means were 3.52
(SD=.72; grand mean centered M=.14, SD=.71) in the Indian sample, 3.34
(SD=.52; grand mean centered M=−.01, SD=.52) in the Brazilian sample and
3.25 (SD=.55; grand mean centered M=−.04, SD=.55) in the U.S. sample.
An ANOVA with a Tukey post-hoc test to determine mean score differences
was used to detect differences in how frequently participants reported using
specific strategies across cultures. The independent variable was culture with
three levels: India, Brazil, and the United States. The dependent variables were
free production, analogy, step by step, visualization, and combination summa-
rized across situations. The free production method (F(2, 314) = 4.76, p = .01,
ηp2=.03) was used most frequently in India, followed by Brazil and the United
States. Indian participants had significant higher scores then Brazilian and U.S.
participants. The combination method (F(2, 314) = 5.75, p = .004, ηp2 =.02)
was used significantly more frequently in India and Brazil then in the United
States. The analogy method (F(2, 314) = 3.04, p = .05, ηp2 =.03) was used most
frequently in the United States, followed by Brazil and then India with significant
differences between Unites States and India (see Figure 1). No significant
differences regarding frequency were found in the step by step and visualization
strategy for all situations.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 11 3/14/07 8:32:43 PM


12 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

Frequency of Strategy Use Across Cultures

4.00

3.50

3.00

2.50
USA (n=133)
2.00 Brazil (n=97)
India (n=96)
1.50

1.00

0.50

0.00
Free Analogy Step-by-Step Visualization Combination
Production

Figure 1. Frequency of Strategy Use Across Cultures

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 12 3/14/07 8:32:43 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 13

An ANOVA with a Tukey post-hoc test to determine mean score differences


was also used to detect differences in the efficacy (“usefulness”) of strategy
use across cultures. The free production method (F(2, 311) = 4.33, p = .01,
ηp2 =.03) was rated to be most useful in India, compared to Brazil and the United
States. The analogy method (F(2, 311) = 13.53, p < .001, ηp2 =.08) was regarded
as the most useful in the United States, followed by Brazil and India with
significant differences between all three countries. The step by step method (F(2,
311) = 3.46, p = .02, ηp2 =.03) was viewed most useful in Brazil, followed by the
United States and India with significant differences between Brazil and India.
The combination method (F(2, 311) = 9.41, p < .001, ηp2 =.06) was also viewed
most useful in Brazil, followed by India and the United States with significant
differences between Brazil and the United States (see Figure 2). No differences
were found in the visualization method.
The only significant difference in the facility of strategy use (“easy to apply”)
across cultures was found in the analogy category. Similar to the efficacy cate-
gory, the analogy method was regarded as the easiest to apply in the United
States compared to Brazil and India, F(2, 320) = 10.33, p < .001, ηp2 =.06.

Uncertainty Avoidance and Metacognitive Strategy Use


The three cultures differed significantly in the uncertainty avoidance scores,
F(2, 321)=3.26, p = .04, ηp2 =.02. As the scale we used consisted of two items
referring to rule orientation and did not assess the two other dimensions of
uncertainty avoidance, i.e. employment stability and stress, as described by
Hofstede (2001), we will from now on refer to rule orientation instead of uncer-
tainty avoidance. The mean scores for the two rule orientation scores were
5.18 (SD=2.20) (grand mean centered −.35, SD=1.10) for the U.S. sample; 6.51
(SD=2.01) (grand mean centered −.03, SD=1.01) for the Brazilian sample; and
5.17 (SD=2.34) (grand mean centered −.39, SD=1.17) for the Indian sample.
High scores stand for low rule orientation. Although Brazilian participants
showed the lowest rule orientation, a Tukey post-hoc test comparing grand mean
centered scores shows only marginal significant differences between Brazil and
India (p = .06) and between Brazil and the United States (p = .07) regarding
rule orientation.
Rule orientation correlated significantly with facility over all strategies r=.13,
p = .02; with step-by-step-efficacy over all situations r = .11, p = .04; with
analogy-practical-facility r = .15, p = .01, and with visualization-study-facility,
r = .11, p = .05. Low rule orientation goes hand in hand with high facility of
several strategies and high efficacy of the step by step strategy in all situations.
The Pearson correlations of rule orientation and other metacognitive variables
varied between −.018 and .086.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 13 3/14/07 8:32:43 PM


14 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

Efficacy of Strategy Use Across Cultures

4.00

3.50

3.00

2.50
USA (n=133)
2.00 Brazil (n=97)
India (n=96)
1.50

1.00

0.50

0.00
Free Analogy Step-by-Step Visualization Combination
Production

Figure 2. Efficacy of Strategy Use Across Cultures

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 14 3/14/07 8:32:43 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 15

Impact of Situation on Metacognitive Strategy


Analyses determined that strategy selection was influenced more by culture than
by situation. The questionnaire assessed metacognition in three different situa-
tions: interpersonal problems, practical problems, and study problems. An
ANOVA with situation as independent variable and a Tukey post-hoc test to
determine mean score differences revealed that differences in situation are only
significant in the step by step method, F(2, 955) = 27.73, p < .001, ηp2 =.06.
Participants indicated that they use the step by step method more in study prob-
lems than in interpersonal and practical problems. Overall, only one significant
interaction effect between country and situation was found in the free produc-
tion method, F(4, 964) = 3.48, p = .01, ηp2 =.01. The interaction effect refers to
study problems. Compared to the other two situations, frequency in free pro-
duction in the United States was decreasing; while it was increasing in Brazil and
India (see Figure 3). These results regarding situation-specificity of metacogni-
tive strategies speak more for a general metacognitive style than for domain
specificity.

Strategy Use Across Situation and Culture

12.00

10.00

8.00
India
6.00 Brazil
USA
4.00

2.00

0.00
Combination

Combination

Combination
Visualization

Visualization

Visualization
Production

Production

Production
Step-by-

Step-by-

Step-by-
Analogy

Analogy

Analogy
Step

Step

Step
Free

Free

Free

Interpersonal Practical Study

Figure 3. Strategy Use Across Situation and Cultures

Composition and Abilities of Metacognitive Problem-solving Strategies


The understanding of metacognitive problem-solving strategies was studied
in the three different cultural samples. For every strategy, participants indi-
cated which of eight mental abilities was involved. The average of eight possible

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 15 3/14/07 8:32:43 PM


16 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

abilities in each of the five strategies was 18.82 (SD=6.52) in the U.S. sample,
18.80 (SD=6.30) in the Brazilian sample, and 17.91 (SD=9.35) in the Indian
sample, indicating 3 to 4 abilities marked per strategy. The main effect of culture
on the number of marked composition aspects was not significant, F(2, 322) = .50,
p = .60, ηp2 =.003. The following statistical analysis refers to the raw data. These
data were not grand mean centered. Regarding every strategy, differences were
found in at least three of the eight abilities associated with each strategy across
cultures. With respect to the free production strategy, creativity was more
important in Brazil than in India, F(2, 324) = 5.01, p = .01, ηp2 =.03. Speed was
more important in India than in the United States, F(2, 324) = 5.23,
p = .01, ηp2 =.03. Synthesis was more important in Brazil than the United States,
F(2, 324) = 3.67, p = .03, ηp2 =.02. Logical reasoning was more important in the
United States than in Brazil, F(2, 324) = 6.32, p = .002, ηp2 =.04.
With respect to the analogy strategy, creativity was more important in India
than in Brazil and the United States, F(2, 324) = 9.37, p < .001, ηp2 =.06. Speed
was more important in India than in Brazil and the United States,
F(2, 324) = 11.06, p < .001, ηp2 =.06. Synthesis was more important in Brazil
than in India and the United States, F(2, 324) = 5.89, p = .003, ηp2 =.04. Accu-
racy was more important in the United States than in Brazil and India,
F(2, 324) = 6.38, p = .002, ηp2 =.04. Memory was more important in the United
States than in Brazil and India, F(2, 324) = 18.42, p < .001, ηp2 =.10. Analysis
was more important in the United States than in India, F(2, 324) = 6.19,
p = .002, ηp2 =.04.
With respect to the step by step strategy, creativity was more important in
India than in the United States, F(2, 324) = 3.32, p = .037, ηp2 =.02. Speed was
more important in India than in Brazil and the United States, F(2, 324) = 7.55,
p = .001, ηp2 =.05. Synthesis was more important in Brazil than in the United
States, F(2, 324) = 3.34, p = .04, ηp2 =.02. Critical thinking was more important
in the United States than in India and Brazil, F(2, 324) = 14.614,
p < .001, ηp2 =.08. Accuracy was more important in Brazil than in India,
F(2, 324) = 5.09, p = .01, ηp2 =.03. Analysis was more important in Brazil than
in the United States and India, F(2, 324) = 8.98, p < .001, ηp2 =.05. Logical rea-
soning was more important in Brazil than in India, F(2, 324) = 7.88,
p < .001, ηp2 =.05.
With respect to the visualization strategy, creativity was more important in
the United States than in India, F(2, 324) = 6.90, p = .001, ηp2 =.04. Speed was
more important in India than in Brazil and the United States, F(2, 324) = 11.25,
p < .001, ηp2 =.07. Critical thinking was more important in the United States
than in India and Brazil, F(2, 324) = 5.23, p = .07, ηp2 =.03.
With respect to the combination method, creativity was more important in
Brazil than in the United States and India, F(2, 324) = 4.49, p = .01, ηp2 =.03.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 16 3/14/07 8:32:44 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 17

Speed was more important in India than in Brazil and the United States,
F(2, 324) = 5.58, p = .004, ηp2 =.03. Critical thinking was more important in the
United States than in Brazil, F(2, 324) = 3.10, p = .05, ηp2 =.02. Accuracy was
more important in India than in the United States, F(2, 324) = 3.05, p = .05,
ηp2 =.02.
In comparing the importance of specific mental abilities in the five different
strategies between the three different cultural groups, it is interesting to note
that Indian participants rated speed as more important than Brazilian and U.S.
participants in all five strategies. Brazilians rated synthesis as more important
than Indian and U.S. participants in the three strategies where significant
differences between cultural groups were found. U.S. participants rated critical
thinking as more important than did Indian and Brazilian participants in the
three strategies where significant differences were found.

Discussion

The main question investigated refers to cultural differences in metacognitive


problem-solving strategies. As the results show, we indeed find cross-cultural
similarities and differences in frequency, efficacy, and facility of metacognitive
strategies. In each of the five strategies frequency, efficacy, and facility were
significantly correlated. Antonietti et al. (2000) found the same result. Over all
five strategies, no cultural differences regarding frequency of strategy were found.
It was expected that Brazilians would show lower frequencies. However, Brazil-
ian presence orientation (Strohschneider & Güss, 1998) and improvisation do
not necessarily go hand in hand with lower metacognitive activity. The time
frame, but not the amount of metacognitive activity, might be different.
Analogy was the most frequently reported strategy in all three samples. Indian
participants reported more frequent use of the free production strategy, U.S. par-
ticipants more frequent use of the analogy method, and Brazilian and Indian
participants more frequent use of the combination method. The more frequent
use of the free production method in the Indian sample might be associated with
a higher context-sensitivity (Hall, 1976). The combination strategy might reflect
the Brazilian “jeito” (Stubbe, 1987), that in a specific problem situation, creative
and improvised solutions have to be found.
Every cultural group also had a different preference regarding the efficacy of
metacognitive strategy. Indian participants reported higher scores than the other
two cultural samples for the free production method, U.S. participants reported
higher scores for analogy, and Brazilian participants reported higher efficacy
scores for the step by step and combination strategy. Regarding the facility of
metacognitive strategies, all samples rated the analogy method as the most easy

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 17 3/14/07 8:32:44 PM


18 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

to apply. However, U.S. participants found it easier to apply than Indian and
Brazilian participants. In the U.S. sample, the analogy strategy was found most
easy to apply, most useful, and most frequently used. This was not the case in the
Brazilian and Indian sample. This result could be related to the often-described
American pragmatism. In philosophy, American pragmatism is a popular school
(Gross, 2002; Perry, 2001) that goes back to Charles Sanders Peirce and John
Dewey, and was further developed by William James (Pajares, 2003). This tradi-
tion stresses the importance of the practical outcome of ideas in the practical
culture. In a popular sense “get things done” reflects such a pragmatic attitude. A
pragmatic problem-solving approach would favor those strategies that have been
proven to work well and easy to apply.
To explain cultural differences, uncertainty avoidance was assessed. The two
questions used for further analysis only refer to rule orientation. The scale could
and should be improved. Items have to be added and the validity has to be
assessed. Besides the methodological problems with the survey, there are also
theoretical problems related to the construct of uncertainty avoidance. India has
often been described as a society in which contradictions exist and are not per-
ceived as contradictions (e.g. Sinha and Tripathi, 1994). Therefore, what uncer-
tainty means to an Indian might be quite different from what uncertainty means
to an American or a Brazilian (see Nisbett, et al. 2003, for a description of
differences in Western and Eastern thinking). An Indian worker, for example,
put a picture of Jesus next to her pictures of the Hindu Gods at her altar at home
(Güss, 2000). In many Western households, Christian and Hindu beliefs might
not fit together, and the person would choose either one of the two religious
worldviews.
The second theoretical problem is related to the two-item scale in this study.
It only refers to rule orientation and does not include the other two dimensions
of Hofstede, namely employment stability and stress. In our samples, Brazil
showed slightly less rule orientation than India and the United States. In Hof-
stede’s study (2001), assessing uncertainty avoidance with three items, Brazil
showed more uncertainty avoidance than India and the United States. Possible
reasons for these contradicting results are different samples, different times of
data gathering, and different uncertainty questions. Hofstede’s data are based on
IBM managers in the late 1960s and early 1970s, whereas our samples consist of
students who participated in 2003 and 2004. Although our questions were
derived from Hofstede’s questions, they focused only on rule orientation. His
questions referred to rule orientation, employment stability, and stress. A fur-
ther problem regarding the uncertainty scale in this study was the focus of the
questions on work and study context. The three samples differed significantly
regarding work experience and many students don’t have work experience, which

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 18 3/14/07 8:32:44 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 19

could explain the low overall alpha coefficients. Further studies on uncertainty
avoidance in Brazil using a multi-method approach with different samples from
different parts of the country and from different professional backgrounds
would shed further light on these contradicting results.
It was hypothesized that uncertainty avoidance might influence frequency of
metacognitive strategies. This hypothesis was not confirmed due to the reasons
discussed above. Results show that low rule orientation correlated with facility
of several strategies and situations and with facility across all situations. Low rule
orientation also correlated significantly with step by step efficacy across all situa-
tions. Thus the less concerned participants were with certain rules, the easier to
implement certain strategies were perceived.
Another question investigated was whether or not metacognitive strategies
are situation-specific or more general cognitive styles. Regarding the cultural
samples, we expected the United States as an individualistic low-context culture
to show little variance across situations and we expected situational variability in
the Indian and Brazilian samples. The U.S. sample indeed showed no situational
differences, but, contradicting our hypothesis, the Brazilian and Indian partici-
pants also did not describe situation specific strategies. With the three different
problem domains – interpersonal, practical, and study – only one out of the five
strategies, the step by step strategy, varied across situations. It was most frequently
applied in study problems and least in interpersonal problems. This result might
be due to our samples which consisted of college students. For this sample, study
problems might be especially important and require a step by step approach.
Another possible explanation for the variation across situations in the step by
step approach could be related to the problem type. Study problems can be
defined more clearly than interpersonal and practical problems. In well-defined
problems, a step by step approach works better than in ill-defined problems.
Interpersonal and practical problems might be more uncertain thus not allowing
for a step by step approach.
Results indicate a tendency for metacognitive strategic preferences to hold
across situations in all countries, indicating a general metacognitive style rather
than a situation specific approach. However, the instrument did not give specific
interpersonal, practical, and study problems, which can be regarded as a strength
or as a weakness – as a strength, because participants can imagine relevant prob-
lems; as a weakness, because responses can differ according to the problems
imagined. We don’t know what specific problems participants imagined while
they were answering the questionnaire. It might be that the problem context
plays an important role for the selection of specific problem-solving strategies.
Further research investigating metacognitive strategies in more specific, concrete
situations is necessary before drawing conclusions. In a similar way, criticism that

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 19 3/14/07 8:32:44 PM


20 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

refers to survey data in general applies specifically to this survey. The chief con-
cern is whether or not ratings on such a questionnaire, where relatively abstract
strategies are imagined, reflect real problem-solving behavior. It would be inter-
esting to compare results from this metacognitive questionnaire with, for exam-
ple, thinking aloud protocols (Ericsson & Simon, 1980, 1984/1993) of concrete
problem-solving behavior in different situations. Strategic and metacognitive
preferences in these situations could then be compared with those indicated in
the questionnaire.
Results that might explain cross-cultural differences in metacognitive strate-
gies are the differences in abilities associated with each strategy in each country.
Out of eight abilities assessed for each of the five strategies, we find cross-cultural
differences in at least three of the eight abilities associated with each strategy.
Overall, Indian participants rated speed as more important than Brazilian and
U.S. participants in all five strategies. In the three strategies where significant
differences between cultural groups were found, Brazilians rated synthesis as
more important than Indian and U.S. participants, and U.S. participants rated
critical thinking as more important than Indian and Brazilian participants.
Apparently the individual skills required for specific metacognitive strategies
differ between cultures. Applying an eco-cultural framework (Berry, 2004),
skills are acquired in a specific cultural context. Those skills seem functional and
beneficial for success in a given environment. According to our respondents,
speed is a crucial factor in problem solving and decision making in India, synthe-
sis in Brazil, and critical thinking in the United States. It would be interesting to
investigate the differences in the cultural environment in these countries in more
detail in order to better understand the required individual skills. Regarding
Brazil, for example, Dessen & Torres (2002) highlight the economic instability
and many dynamic changes in the environment of many Latin American coun-
tries. In what way are the dynamic changes unique in Brazil and in what way do
they differ from those in India? And how does speed in one and synthesis in the
other culture respond to these changes?
Research about cultural preferences in problem-solving strategies is not only
relevant for cultural, cross-cultural, and cognitive psychology, but it is also rele-
vant for practical reasons. For instance, work-teams consisting of members from
different cultures might encounter difficulties in working together due to
different preferences in problem solving. Knowledge of these differences in
problem-solving strategies could lead to better mutual understanding and to
smoother work-relations.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 20 3/14/07 8:32:45 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 21

Acknowledgement
This study is part of a bigger project and based on work supported by the
National Science Foundation under Grant 0218203 to the first author from
2002 to 2006 with the title “Cultural influences on dynamic decision making.”
This research would not have been possible without the support of friends
and colleagues abroad. We would like to thank especially Prof. Cristina Ferreira,
Prof. Cilio Ziviani, Prof. Nadia, and Dr. Miguel Cal in Brazil; Prof. Krishna
Prasaad Sreedhar, Dr. S. Raju, Dr. Ajay Kesavan, and Mr. Ibrahim Syed in India,
and the many students who participated. We also would like to thank Paul Go
for his comments on an earlier version of this article.
Portions of the material were presented at the 25th Annual Convention of the
Society for Judgment and Decision Making in Minnesota, MN, November 2004.

References
Akama, K., & Yamauchi, H. (2004). Task performance and metacognitive experiences in problem-
solving. Psychological Reports, 94(2), 715-722.
Antonietti, A., Ignazi, S., & Perego, P. (2000). Metacognitive knowledge about problem-solving
methods. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 1-16.
Bartl, C., & Dörner, D. (1998). Sprachlos beim Denken – zum Einfluß von Sprache auf die Prob-
lemlöse- und Gedächtnisleistung bei der Bearbeitung eines nicht-sprachlichen Problems
[Speachless while thinking – on the influence of language on problem solving and memory per-
formance in a non-language problem]. Sprache & Kognition, 17(4), 224-238.
Beradi-Coletta, B., Buyer, L. S., Dominowski, R. L., & Rellinger, E. R. (1995). Metacognition and
problem solving: A process-oriented approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, & Cognition, 21(1), 205-223.
Berry, J. W. (2004). An ecocultural perspective on the development of competence. In R. J. Stern-
berg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Culture and competence (pp. 3-22). Washigton DC: American
Psychological Association.
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology.
Research and applications (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cooking, R. R. (1999). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience,
and school. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Carlson, R. A. (1997). Experienced cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carr, M., & Borkowski, J. G. (1989). Kultur und die Entwicklung des Metakognitiven Systems
[Culture and the development of the metacognitive system]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psy-
chologie, 3(4), 219-228.
Carr, M., Kurtz, B. E., Schneider, W., Turner, L. A, & Borkowski, J. G. (1989). Strategy acquisition
and transfer among American and German children: Environmental influences on metacogni-
tive development. Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 765-771.
Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations. How
students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145-182.
Davidson, G. R. (1994). Metacognition, cognition and learning. Old dubitations and new direc-
tions. South Pacific Journal of Psychology, 7, 18-31.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 21 3/14/07 8:32:45 PM


22 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

Davidson, G. R., & Freebody, P. R. (1988). Cross-cultural perspectives on the development of


metacognitive thinking. Hiroshima Forum for Psychology, 13, 21-31
Dessen, M. A., & Torres, C. V. (2002). Family and socialization factors in Brazil: An overview. . In
W. J. Lonner, D. L. Dinnel, S. A. Hayes, & D. N. Sattler (Eds.), Online Readings in Psychology and
Culture (Unit 13, Chapter 2), (http://www.wwu.edu/~culture), Center for Cross-Cultural
Research, Western Washington University, Bellingham, Washington USA.
Dörner, D. (1996). The logic of failure. New York: Holt.
Dörner, D., Kreuzig, H. W., Reither, F., & Stäudel, T. (1983). Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit
Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität. [Lohhausen. Dealing with Uncertainty and Complexity].
Bern, Switzerland: Huber.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5). Whole No. 270.
Dunlosky, J. (1998). Epilogue: Linking metacognitive theory to education. In D. J. Hacker,
J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 367-
381). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87(3), 215-251.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1984/1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge:
MIT Press.
Fischer, R. (2004). Standardization to account for cross-cultural response bias: A classification of
score adjustment procedures and review of research in JCCP. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychol-
ogy, 35(3), 263-282.
Flavell, J. (Ed.) (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. The nature of intelligence. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Fleith, D. S. (2002). Creativity in the Brazilian culture. In W. J. Lonner, D. L. Dinnel, S. A. Hayes,
& D. N. Sattler (Eds.), Online Readings in Psychology and Culture (Unit 5, Chapter 3), (http://
www.wwu.edu/~culture), Center for Cross-Cultural Research, Western Washington University,
Bellingham, Washington USA.
Glaser, R., & Chi, M. T. H. (1988). Overview. In M. T. H. Chi, R. Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.), The
nature of expertise (pp. XV-XXXVI). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Glencross, E., & Güss, C. D. (2004, November). Indian fatalism? American pragmatism? Culture,
values, and planning. Poster presentation at the 25th Annual Convention of the Society for Judg-
ment and Decision Making to be held in Minnesota, MN, November 19 to 22, 2004.
Gross, N. (2002). Becoming a pragmatist philosopher: Status, self-concept, and intellectual choice.
American Sociological Review, 67(1), 52-76.
Güss, D. (2000). Planen und Kultur? [Planning and culture?]. Lengerich, Germany: Pabst.
——. (2002). Decision Making in Individualistic and Collectivist Cultures. In W. J. Lonner,
D. L. Dinnel, S. A. Hayes, & D. N. Sattler (Eds.), OnLine Readings in Psychology and Culture,
Western Washington University, Department of Psychology, Center for Cross-Cultural
Research. Web site: http://www.wwu.edu/~culture
Güss, C. D., Glencross, E., Tuason, M.T., Summerlin, L., & Richard, F. D. (2004). Task complexity
and difficulty in two computer-simulated problems: Cross-cultural similarities and differences.
In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Conference
of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 511-516). Mahwah, NJ: Cognitive Science Society and Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. New York: Doubleday.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1993). Human and machine thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Kluckhohn, C., & Murray, H. A. (1953). Personality formation: The determinants. In C. Kluck-
hohn & H. A. Murray (Eds.), Personality in nature, society, and culture (2nd edition, revised and
enlarged) (pp. 53-67). New York: Knopf.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 22 3/14/07 8:32:45 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 23

Kroeber, A. A., & Kluckhohn, C. (1963). Culture. A critical review of concepts and definitions. New
York: Vintage Books.
Land, S. (2004). A conceptual framework for scaffolding ill-structured problem-solving processes
using question prompts and peer interactions. Educational Technology, 52, 5-22.
Lauth, G. (1996). Effizienz eines metakognitiv-strategischen Trainings bei lern- und aufmerksam-
keitsbeeinträchtigten Grundschülern. [The effectiveness of a metacognitive strategies training
program among children with learning and attention disorders.] Zeitschrift für Klinische Psy-
chologie. Forschung und Praxis, 25(1), 21-32.
Lin, X. (2001). Designing metacognitive activities. Educational Technology, 49, 28-40.
Mann, L., Radford, M., Burnett, P., Ford, S., Bond, M., Leung, K., Nakamura, H., Vaughan, G., &
Yang, K.-S. (1998). Cross-cultural differences in self-reported decision-making style and
confidence. International Journal of Psychology, 33(5), 325-335.
McCafferty, S. G. (1992). The use of private speech by adult second language learners: A cross-
cultural study. Modern Language Journal, 76(2), 179-189.
Mevarech, Z., & Kramarski, B. (2003). The effects of metacognitive training versus worked-out
examples on students’ mathematical reasoning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 449-471.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I, & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holis-
tic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291-310.
Ohbushi, K.-I., Fukushima, O., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1999). Cultural values in conflict management:
Goal orientation, goal attainment, and tactical decision. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30,
51-71.
Pajares, F. (2003). William James: Our father who begat us. In B. J. Zimmerman (Ed.), Educational
psychology: A century of contributions (pp. 41-64). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Perry, D. K. (Ed.) (2001). American pragmatism and communication research. Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Associates.
Rohlfing, K. J., Rehm, M., & Goecke, K. U. (2003). Situatedness: The interplay between context(s)
and situation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3(2), 132-156.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.
Saxe, G. B. (1994). Studying cognitive development in sociocultural context: The development of
a practice-based approach. Mind, Culture, & Activity, 1(3), 135-157.
Scheper-Hughes, N. (1990). Mother love and child death in Northeast Brazil. In J. W. Stigler,
R. A. Shweder & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural Psychology. Essays on comparative human development
(pp. 542-565). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schmidt, A., & Ford, J. (2003). Learning within a learner control training environment: The inter-
active effects of goal orientation and metacognition instruction on learning outcomes. Personnel
Psychology, 56, 405-419.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism/collectivism. New cultural dimensions of values. In
U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç. Kâgitçibasi, S.-C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and collec-
tivism. Theory, method, and applications (pp. 85-119). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Simonton, D. K. (1984). Genius, creativity, and leadership. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sinha, D., & Tripathi, R. C. (1994). Individualism in a collectivist culture. A case of coexistence of
opposites. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç. Kâgitçibasi, S.- C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individual-
ism and collectivism. Theory, method, and applications (pp. 123-136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Smith, P. B., & Bond, M. H. (1998). Social psychology across cultures (2nd ed.). London: Prentice
Hall.
Smith, P. B., Peterson, M. F., & Schwartz, S. H. (2002). Cultural values, sources of guidance, and
their relevance to managerial behavior. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 33(2), 188-208.
Spranzi, M. (2004). Galileo and the mountains of the moon: Analogical reasoning, models and
metaphors in scientific discovery. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(3), 451-483.

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 23 3/14/07 8:32:45 PM


24 C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25

Stubbe, H. (1987). Geschichte der Psychologie in Brasilien. Von den indianischen und afro-brasilianischen
Kulturen bis in die Gegenwart [History of psychology in Brazil. From Indian and Afro-Brazilian
cultures to the present]. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.
Tisdale, T. (1998). Selbstreflexion, Bewußtsein und Handlungsregulation. [Selfreflection, conscious-
ness, and action regulation.] Weinheim: Beltz.
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Cognitive psychology. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson.
Strohschneider, S., & Güss, D. (1998). Planning and problem solving: Differences between Brazil-
ian and German students. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29(6), 695-716.
van de Vijver, F., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis of comparative research. In J. W.
Berry, Y. H. Poortinga, & J. Pandey (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology. Volume 1:
Theory and method (pp. 257-300). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Veenman, M., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuizen, J. (2004). The relation between intellectual and metacog-
nitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Construction, 14, 89-109.
Vosniadou, S., & Ortony, A. (Eds.) (1989). Similarity and analogical reasoning. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Weber, E. U., & Hsee, C. K. (2000). Culture and individual judgment and decision making. Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 49(1), 32-61.
Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking. New York: Harper.

Appendix A

Uncertainty avoidance questions


1. Company rules should not be broken – even when the employee thinks it is in the company’s
best interests.
strongly agree 1—2—3—4—5 strongly disagree.
2. University rules should not be broken – even when the student thinks it is in the university’s
best interests.
strongly agree 1—2—3—4—5 strongly disagree.
3. How often do you feel nervous or tense in class?
I always feel this way. 1—2—3—4—5 I never feel this way.
If you work, answer questions 4 and 5a. If not, go to question 5b
4. How often do you feel nervous or tense at work?
I always feel this way. 1—2—3—4—5 I never feel this way.
5a. How long do you think you will continue working for this company?
a) one year at the most
b) from 1 to 2 years
c) from 3 to 5 years
d) more than five years
e) until I retire
If you do not work
5b. How long would you like to work in a company?
a) one year at the most
b) from 1 to 2 years
c) from 3 to 5 years
d) more than five years
e) until I retire

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 24 3/14/07 8:32:46 PM


C. D. Güss, B. Wiley / Journal of Cognition and Culture 7 (2007) 1-25 25

Appendix B*

I try to recall problems successfully solved in the past which are similar to the current problem. I
look for previous situations which share some aspects, elements or features with the current
problem so that I can transfer some ideas from the former ones to the latter one. 1 stands for very
little and 5 stands for very much.

Think of the application of this strategy to interpersonal problems:


*how frequently I apply this strategy 1 2 3 4 5
*how useful I think this strategy is 1 2 3 4 5
*how easy I think this strategy is to apply 1 2 3 4 5

Think of the application of this strategy to practical problems:


*how frequently I apply this strategy 1 2 3 4 5
*how useful I think this strategy is 1 2 3 4 5
*how easy I think this strategy is to apply 1 2 3 4 5

Think of the application of this strategy to study problems:


*how frequently I apply this strategy 1 2 3 4 5
*how useful I think this strategy is 1 2 3 4 5
*how easy I think this strategy is to apply 1 2 3 4 5

Which of the following mental abilities do you think are involved when the strategy is applied?
[ ] creativity [ ] speed [ ] synthesis [ ] critical thinking
[ ] accuracy [ ] memory [ ] analysis [ ] logical reasoning

* Reproduced with permission from The British Journal of Educational Psychology, (c) The British Psycho-
logical Society

JOCC 7,1-2_f2_1-25.indd 25 3/14/07 8:32:46 PM

S-ar putea să vă placă și