Sunteți pe pagina 1din 26

Folia Linguistica Historica XVI'/7-2 pp.

3-27
Societas Linguistica Europaea
BEFOREPIE:
MOTIVES FOR THEMATISM
N.E. COLLINGE
0. Studies of Proto-Indo-European morphology and morphosyntax have
lagged behind those making progress on its phonology. The latter enjoyed
the limelight in the nineteenth Century and a little later; and more recently
waves of the laryngeal theory and varieties of the glottalic theory have
maintained the impetus (not to mention more idiosyncratic ideas of diph-
thongal bases and freely varying consonant-features). Still, in the last quar-
ter-century much solid work has been done on PIE's grammatical history,
although the current fashion seems to turn to such morphophonological
matters s suffixal shape, paradigm dynamics, and the like.
In pursuit of genetic connections there has been also an urge to em-
brace super-divergence. Those attracted by Pedersen's notion (1924; see
also 1962: 338) of a 'nostratic' Ursprache have used macro-derivation to
link PIE with Dravidian, Kartvelian, Afro-Asiatic and Semitic - and even
that domain seems unduly parochial to some who would cast the net even
wider (like Greenberg; see Matisoff 1990 on this zeal for 'megalo-com-
parison'). To this end many traditional findings have been exploited, those
especially of phonology and lexical semantics. Shapes-with-meanings al-
ready assigned to PIE have been 'recognised' elsewhere. But PIE is no
more than a diagrammatic pro-language, summarising the reconciliation
of various reflexes seen or deducted in the evidential languages; and that
summary cannot long precede their first diaspora. When morphosyntax
has been brought in, nuclear sentence structures in IE (their transitivity
patterns) have been linked with 'active' or 'ergative' modes of grammar
- a tactic of long standing - wherever these are found. Even an improve-
ment-succession has been suggested, from active via ergative to accusative
type. But the unreal and achronic nature of PIE defeats this approach,
apart from the doubtful Status of individual proposals.
Yet what if we can get back to some firmer internal history of PIE
grammar? There should be traceable lines of cognitive development pre-
ceding and helping to shape the latest form of its morphosyntax. This
pre-morphosyntactic era may both constrain megalo-comparison and tidy
up bits of PIE morphology which have resisted attempts to fit them into
the overall picture so far. Here will be examined three similar, but distinct
(in time s well s function), marking-procedures in a proto-language
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
which was still morphologically immature. That is not yet PIE. As a name
for it when it is struggling to denote half-cognitive, half-grammatical cate-
gories let us use 'pre-morphological PIE' - in brief, PPIE. This title is
not to be confused with Rix's 'pre-Proto-IE' (1988: 101) which indicates
all pre-diaspora PIE; it is nearer to Georgiev's (1984) PIE I, or Frh-
indoeuropisch. Considering around twenty millennia, Georgiev saw no
morphology at all emerging in this first phase (aspect, personal pronouns
and unmarked locative being the first fruits of its successor, PIE II, when
grammar takes root and produces other pronouns, too, and 'precase' par-
ticles (this is Mittelindoeuropisch). This timetable is possible and ignores
larger genetic speculations. But unhappily it also ignores the three mech-
anisms examined below, though their traces need explanation and their
relative chronology consideration. And in these mechanisms the common
element of form is thematism.
PPIE deployed thematism in various ways both before and after its
Speakers became sensitive to a fundamental communicative distinction:
that between entities (which in a given context may or may not exist for
Speaker and hearer) and events or predications (which may or may not
be true s to fact). This is the essential basis for the evolving syntactic
dyarchy, of N(oun) and V(erb). The physical sign was the suffixation of
a lexical root by means of a vowel of middle height (front or back, these
later alternating). The root became a 'stem'; it received further, and later,
affixes to convey the 'mittelindoeuropisch' categories noted above and
then others. Possibly the early pronoun alone overlaps in emergence with
the thematic vowel. Thematism does not occur so s to set up the stem;
it merely joins with the instantiation of that structure by unextended con-
sonant-final, or (other) vowel-final, forms. Hence its name is unfortunate:
'thetic' would avoid misleading suggestions s to its role. But the term is
traditional and will be used here; of itself it explains nothing. ('Half-the-
matics', like Latin fer-t, where syncope or similar interference has obscured
the issue, are here ignored.)
Each of the three types of thematism is merkmaltragend (shaped by
the added vowel) and markiert (carrying a positive function which the
unextended form lacks). In order they are:
(I) basic root plus front vowel: (N
(II) entity-root plus back vowel:
(III) event-root plus alternating vowel: V +
e
/o.
(I) belongs to an epoch well predating the inflectional and derivational
phase of PIE; it is essentially of PPIE. (The other types reflect cognitive
revelations possibly peculiar to PPIE and early PIE; there is no need to
rely on finding them in, say, Kartvelian.) Indeed, PPIE's choice of +e s
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
the sign of (I) is idiosyncratic, and probably a bar to megalocomparison
even at the beginnings of speech.
There are problems with (II) and (III) if we accept notions of early
active or ergative in Indo-European. Form-function relations ought to
leave plenty of vestiges; and the chronology becomes awkward - because
if thematism has that sort of role it must be part of early PIE and not
PPIE at all. The evidence noted below suggests otherwise: and the only
language-group linked with PIE (even nostratically) which seems to be
ergative, southern Causasian, is really 'pseudo-ergative' (for this diagnosis
see Trask 1979). And activity remains in more than vestigial form only in
tongues s distant s Guarani or Eastern Porno. Comments on these syn-
tactic are offered below, in connection with the possible origins of this
or that shape of words. The scepticism here voiced s to their relevance
to (P)IE history receives some support from Matsumoto (1993), although
there an 'active' stage is accepted. If, however, thematism is divorced from
any such mode of grammar, the Charge of anachronism need no longer
be feared.
To pass to the mechanisms themselves, in their apparent relative
chronological order:
1. (N V)+e
An arbitrary starting point may be the pre-verbal base *g"
h
en + e set
up by Kurylowicz in 1964 (62-63) s underlying the later PIE perfect. This
was conceived to be a root (conveying action) enlarged by a suffix (of
state or undergoing) to produce an item of nominal Status; in later terms,
an adjective or participle. Watkins (1969: 105-118) charted its evolution
into the basic member of the PIE set of verbal forms which displayed
sensitivity to time, voice, person and number. The original form is, seem-
ingly, metanalysed: the structure l*g"
h
en + a plus (N-nominative) zero/
passes to /*g"
h
en- plus zero plus (verbal) e/, the final element assuming
the signalling of activity/predication. This result, no longer nominal, is then
seen s that non-person, non-number form of the verb which is sub-
sequently called 'third person Singular' and taken to be the platform on
which more complex forms are differentially built (on this concept, known
s Watkins' law, see Collinge 1985: 239-240). In succession, the precise
order being here irrelevant, the personal markers, the 'here and now* suffix
-l/, and the middle voice sign +o all fall into place. The early +e shrivels
into an adventitious and otiose piece of morphology. This history is
possible. It may miss much.
Kurylowicz glossed bis launch-pad s '(he is (being)/has been) killed'.
This verbal adjective was supposed to be reverbalised by accent shift or
metaphonic or apophonic ablaut: so *g*
h
onel*g"
h
onel*g"
h
n?lo, plus af-
fixes. It become the PIE perfect s well s its oldest form of medio-passive.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
Yet only a passive sense is first offered for it; and such suggested perfects
s *iouge are active. The function of +e is likened to that of later H - ;
but the latter is passive- intransitive rather than middle (cf. Skt. hata- and
gata-). The path which takes *g*
h
ene+i to active Gk. theinei 'kills' is
equally obscure.
Let us rather shift our gaze to an actually documented (unaccented)
+ e. One proposed category exploiting that shape is the famous 'injunc-
tive', a PIE feature much in favour again, after its first proposal by Brug-
mann (1880- 2) whose examples of its simplest form were Skt. bharat, sthf,
Gk.phere, stf. It is only the final person- marker that is clear (- f# > - 0#
in Greek); unless crasis is proved, stht, stt do not show +e. And in
most examples quoted by adherents, aspect is already present via apo-
phony or reduplication (so, in Greek, leipe, mimne - cf. West 1989). H ence
the thematic vowel looks to be a vestige of some earlier positive Signal,
now reduced to being a stem- formant only. As to the injunctive in general,
H offmann (1967: 140) assigns to it the indication of iterative action or
general validity (re- cast by Dunkel (1992: 200) s the 'eternally valid').
More interesting is H offmann's claim, based on the absence of expected
augments in Vedic, that here we have a tenseless, moodless and non- re-
portive verbal category, which merely 'mentions' (erwhnt) the action.
That, s West says (1989: 135), looks like 'a primitive feature of the Indo-
European verb'. If so it is worth seeking out in its pre- personal stage
(and preablautal, pre- reduplicative stages, too). Dunkel first uses the sup-
posed injunctive to explain (hoping to improve on Szemerenyi's (1979)
metathesised phrases which may underline initial/?^ Variation in some
Greek lexemes) the left- hand members of compounds like Av. vanat-
p^sana, Ved. Bharad-vja- or Gk. heleptolis (*helet-polis). With perhaps
more cogency, he then (203- 206) analyses types like Ved. Trasa-dsya-,
Gk. Mene-laos s having s the first element imperative forms. (French
porte-monnaile, Eng. forget-me-not are adduced s evidential of the
process.) Further, for Dunkel these forms are of uncertain person s be-
tween second and third - which puts us into an era when even that dis-
tinction was not yet cristallised into a formal morphological Opposition.
If an imperative did stand there, a solution becomes possible which
is more compatible with an early stage of cognition, when in Speakers
urgency and imprecision went band in band. Why should the activity be
so economically but clearly specified at all? It may have been to expostu-
late, in a peremptory and jussive fashion, towards an entity (that it appear
or that it act) or towards an event (that it happen). In other words, that
N or V be manifested. Such a jussive, arising from otherwise thwarted
needs, is a likely enough origin at the envisaged point in the evolution of
the language - if not of all speech. Thereafter, when PIE has arrived, the
form will be 'imperative' of an event once V is morphologically furnished,
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
and 'vocative' of an entity once N is similarly paradigmatised. It is not a
real player in the emergent drama of nominal inflection (when argument-
role and spatial relation are formalised), but a vestigial element parked
there, and feit to be worth keeping. So much is indicated by:
(1) the betraying absence of crasis or contractual lengthening in the
o-stem vocatives: hence f -, added to the basic root, versus e.g. stem
extension dative -i < *-o + ei;
(2) the form's persistence (even, spottily, into modern Greek);
(3) the quite different processes invoked to supply the same signal else-
where: e.g. zero affix (Skt. rjan)\ truncation (Skt. tanu\ see Beekes
1985: 101; for j-loss see Winter 1969); accent shift (Gk. gfmai).
This makes understandable Beekes' paradoxical remark (1985: 106) on
'the "ending" of the vocative (which had no ending in PIE)'. There is,
however, no need to follow him (101) in equating the expostulatory suffix
e with the free-standing exclamatory particle . The former was a primeval
addition when N and V differed only crudely. Through history its am-
bivalence between them abides. It makes possible, for instance, the famous
Carpel jest of Trimalchio (Pertonus, Satyricon 36) who, in calling so upon
bis carefully named servant, fuses entity with event: eodem uerbo et uocat
et imperat. Winter rightly sees (1969: 212) that imperative and vocative
are mirror images of each other; he deplores the absence of proof thereof,
but what further proof is needed? An addressee is for both a prime re-
quirement. That thus the second person acquires a high degree of cen-
trality is a phenomenon encountered elsewhere: in most Algonquian lan-
guages a verbal construct has to have an initial k- if 'you' is included in
any nuclear role, agent or patient. So we seem to be justifying at last
Sommer's opinion (1937: 187-195) that here is a pre-inflectional element.
In V, s in N, the function broke loose from the simple form, and manifold
shapes of PIE imperatives then appear. No later development quite dims
the light of PPIE 4- e, despite the inclusion of third person forms in the
'mood' (not to mention first person hortatory markers). As a direct appeal
to the hearer, s existent or active, +e so began and so continued.
2. N + o
There has been unease in recent years s to what element is the head
of a noun-phrase. Deictics, some pronouns among them, have won many
votes s candidates for that Status, critical in any 'head-driven' grammar.
Hence the DP theory (replacing the NP), and the neoChomskyan DP-node
(cf. Webelhuth 1995: 89 fn 25; 398). This reassessment affects such ana-
lyses s that of Nichols (1986), which sets apart those languages (the ma-
jority) which signal modification within the phrase by a head-marker, those
which put the marker on the dependent item, and some which do both
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
(at least in the purest form of their syntax). And VPs may receive a similar
reconsideration soon. Now the sentence structure /N,V/ is not a phrase;
it is a complex of potential phrases. But the relation of the two members
justifies taking one s the head and the other s the dependent in the
syntagm; so does the visible marking in many languages. N provides the
entity which V maps on to the predication: N is/is not existent, /N,V/ is/is
not true (for which view, by no means new, see most recently Cresswell
1994: 20). It seems to follow that N is the reasonable candidate for head-
ship of the syntagm; V rather expresses a mathematical function and looks
to be dependent.
The IE languages, and PIE in its formative period, are dependent-
marking (except where outside influence is evident, s in Farsi) if one
works with the old NP: so Gk. omitlioz nZsos versus Tbrkish ku$ adasi
'bird island'. In a DP theory the terms reverse; but it is the modyfying
element which carries the sign in IE, of possession or location or province
or whatever. Therefore in the syntagm it should be on the V root that
the properties peculiar to the relationship are denoted - and later signals
of person and number are there, when their task is to tie N and V together.
So why does N carry any marker at all, if the feature conveyed is not
inherent? (The N in /N,V/ is in point: the N within V (VP or V) or
occurring s a non-nuclear argument is another, later, matter. To show
nominative case, s a sign of government of V? Now this is tied to animacy
(s opposed to semantic animateness), and shown by the suffix -ls in
most instances. (Chomsky has assigned this case to the syntactic feature
AGR since 1982 (52, 170) and equally in his 'parametic' and 'minimalist'
phases: but that is to equate explanandum with explicatio.) Nichols (1986:
115) accepts this marking of case on N s 'the sole head-marked pattern
of English' - it is actually common to all uneroded IE - but this is im-
material, after all. Our quarry is not the -s (in e.g. agro-o-s) but the -o.
Therefore thematism does not signal hierarchy in the syntagm (because
it is on the wrong item); nor argument-role within the sentence (because
another signal does that); nor some property common to all tokens of N
(because its distribution is limited to one declension). Its raison d'etre
must be otherwise, and early (pre-grammatical, in fact) if weight is given
to its proximity to the root. It looks increasingly like the sign of some
cognitive aspect, special to some entities.
2.1. As V in /N,V/ may Sponsor derived forms which carry gender s new
Ns, and s gender is an essential ingredient of N in IE, it is worth con-
sidering whether gender was the original point of the marking by the-
matism. The reflexes suggest two stages: gender I and gender II. The latter
appears in the tripartite System of masculine, feminine and neuter. These
values do not reflect in clear and consistent shapings s between, or within,
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
thematic and athematic declensions. Besides, the System is 'recent' s
being post-Anatolian (pace Kammenhuber (1985: 449-450) on Luwian
possible /-feminines); Hittite had nominals which were lexically female
(cf. Brosman 1984: 346 and 1994), but not morphologically. Gender I,
however, is the prior dichotomy into plus/minus animate. Here a formal
plus marker is expected; but the facts are discouraging: (1) V-governing
+S is added to all animate (non-neuter) subjects - allowing for Szeme-
renyi's law (see Collinge 1985: 237-238) - and does not tie up notably
with +o-; nor is its presence in athematics the result of subsequent dif-
fusion, s far s one can see. (2) Neuters are the institutionalised Version
of inanimates, and are never animate in the early evidence. But their dis-
tribution is unhelpful: absent in diphthongal stems and in the minority in
/-stems and M-stems, they are (equally strangely) present among the ap-
parent founder-members of the thematic declension (cf. Brosman 1979:
61). They occur there fractionally more frequently than in Hittite high-
vowel stems. It is usual to accept s original in PIE *yiigo-, *pedo-, *wergo-
(and possibly *Hwerso-, *dno- *dro-, *petro-/ptero-). It is beyond belief
that athematic inanimates are all secondary; but conversely thematic in-
animates seem so numerous in Hittite (about 170, according to Brosman
1979), and thematic neuters so solidly testified s original, s to cast doubt
on any proposed stage (even 'Ur-PPIE') wherein existed only non-neuter,
animate, nominals with +o-. Of course, the later proliferation of o-stem
membership (especially in Hittite and Indic, less markedly in Greek and
Latin) - wherein neuters are very common (Brosman 1984: 357) - does
cloud the issue somewhat; and animacy and sex are always shifting affairs
in grammar (e.g. Greek loved to use feminine thematics for inanimate
objects). But the evidence does not add up to indicate that gender, of
either sort, was the reason behind thematism in N.
2.2. N-thematism, unlike verbal +
e
/o discussed below, does not spread
across sub-categories; nouns do not have e.g. voice or tense inherently.
Perhaps, therefore, its contribution was rather to some semantic property,
peculiar to that lexical class. But one may pause to wonder whether
4
pre-
morphosyntactic' might mean 'pre-dating accusative-type syntax
5
. That is,
whether thematism is among the vestiges of an earlier 'active' or 'ergative'
type of Operation. That ergativity was a stage of PIE has been suggested
intermittently from Uhlenbeck (1901), via Pedersen (1907, 1933) and Vail-
lant (1936), down to recent analysts like Schmalstieg (1980, 1986, 1988)
or Kortlandt (1983). Beekes (1985: 192) derives the entire o-stem para-
digm from the single +os ending (for such paradigm-creation see Plank
1991), s this is for him the ergative marker in the PIE hysterodynamic
inflection (but see Matsumoto 1993 on this). If here we have a declension
which is s a whole inimical to entities which do not readily serve s
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
10
agents, then indeed One would not expect neuters in the o-stems' (Beekes
1985: 194). Yet neuters and other inanimates occur there freely. And in
only ergativoid nominals are at home there, what is the function of the
+s marker? Again, if nouns not so marked are doing some other Job
than the ergative, why keep their +o? (Such paradigm build-up elsewhere
tends to be either retricted to levelling, s when Latin replaces gen. sg.
- with -/f around 25 BC to level it with -ins, -ium, -f; or is 'stepped',
s Tocharian first establishes an oblique case and then adds further suf-
fixes to it to provide an array of spatial cases.)
Now sporadic recourse to ergativity is common enough. Celtic has an
Innovation which signals alike the intransitive subject (S) and the transitive
object (P): Irish leabhar e 'it is a book' agrees with buailim e beat him'.
The Hindi type maine kitb likhl is a neo-ergative construct (on which
see Bubenik 1993) deriving from Sanskrit participal maya pustakarii likhi-
tam wherein 'by me the book is written' becomes (erg.) wrote the book
(absol.)' Garrett proposes (1990) that a Common Anatolian ergative
Operation arose similarly from transitive V with null subject plus neuter
N (instrumental ablative), the latter's ending (-anz(a)) coming ultimately
from *-anti. This split usage with neuters, like Hittite clitics conveying
only S, is post-PIE. All these examples show how ergativity can pop up
at any time; that rules them out s strong proof of early or inevitable
stages of evolving grammars. For ergativity s itself something led up to,
note Anderson 1977. PIE +to- seems to fuse S and P in e.g. Skt. gata-
'gone' but hata- 'killed'; but, pace Garrett 1990: 263, this means only that
it signals resultative Status (like potential result in Greek -tos; and co-oc-
curring with an agent marker in Skt, k-ta-va(n)t-). As for the deceptive
English ergativoid + ee, marking S/P versus A in escapeelemployee versus
employer, this attaches widely to any 'V-concerned' N. So in referee, am-
putee\ even in V-less lexemes like refiigee. Only special pairing preserves
-erl-ee where -ed is available, notably when three entities are involved (s
in mortgaging). To all this scepticism on the historical relevance of erga-
tivity one may add Villar's doubts (1983, 1984) on it s a PIE mechanism,
considering its forms.
Objections arise to the supposition of a PIE active epoch, too. It might
be reflected in case (so Matsumoto 1993). But that early IE behaviour
was of the active/stative sort described by Klimov (1977) has been rather
less frequently urged, despite the active -* ergative -* accusative develop-
ment noted above. (In the Klimovian model, nouns in pre-passive con-
structions or functioning s patients are Stative, and they become active
when they lead intransitive predications or act agents.) The tension be-
tween activity and state is probably universal; it is certainly endemic in
IE. Aspect enshrines resultative state (perfectivity), and even the mixed
idea of on-going action (imperfectivity) s against simple action. The
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
11
middle voice is often belived to relate at least partially to stativity: cf. e.g.
Jasanoff 1978: 119 (Rix 1988: 104, 166-167 sees them s partners syn-
cretising in late PIE - or to change of state, s by Hart 1988: 88). Indeed,
stativity was positively lexicalised by denominatives in +e(s(c))- (see
Watkins 1971: 90-91; Jasanoff 1978: 125). But it is not clear which is the
unmarked member of the pair in IE; and so relation to the Klimov model
is hard to state. In any case, none of this really matters because the timing
is wrong. Active-type grammar has its own prerequisites: it needs enough
morphology already in place to signal argument-relation, N-V concord or
government, and verbal diathesis. But the pre-morphological nature, and
the still evolving morphophonology, of thematism force our eyes back to
a more primitive era (PPIE) in which a more likely reason for this marking
is a fundamental cognitive appreciation of sub-types of items (here of N).
The recourse is not so much systemic s epistemic.
Two other facts about the o-stems should be borne in mind. (1) It has
long been noted that between the IE languages there is considerable dis-
agreement s to which lexical items belong in that declension. This dis-
agreement widens s the declension becomes more populous, and the lex-
emic matching is particularly sparse between Anatolian and the rest of
IE. (2) Those lexemes which are safely deduced to be original o-stems
are also very few (cf. e.g. Brosman 1972: 62). In seeking PPIE motivation
for marking off these words, one must allow for their being a minority
of oddities with local control on their selection.
There is another possible explanation for them based on an early kind
of sentence-architecture. The PIE pattern of case-marked roles in a tran-
sitivity System may have succeeded one in which, above all, one N was
limelighted s the topic of the communication. Sensitivity to the perspec-
tive of the event sets up Topics (and Comments) instead of, or in interplay
with, Subjects. (Note here Li 1976, and especially therein Lehmann and
Li - Thompson.) Given an epoch when one of these stood out s the
essential signandum, + o could be a vestige of the signalling. A topicalising
PPIE (or just a topic-prominent PPIE; see Lehmann 1976: 456) is sup-
ported by the absence of any constraint among the thematics arising from
inanimacy. A Speaker may set up anything s his Topic. That the original
thematics were so few, and neuters made up so high a proportion of them
even so, does not then matter. Moreover, diffusion of o-stem membership
is just s plausible from a starting point in topicalisation. On the other
band, topic-marking implies a choice between multiple arguments s to
which is to be the topic (without disruption of the event itself or the
various roles). Then the distinction between e.g. a topicalised agent/actor
and a topicalised patient/outer party will need signalling (s in Tagalog
both by particle and by verb affix). But no IE or PIE mechanism of that
sort has been identified; word order and possible intonational devices are
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
12
irrelevant to our search, even if the latter were accessible. Voice might
have been so used; but the most likely nuclear Opposition would have
demanded an active/passive contrast, while the obvious and agreed de-
duction from IE comparison is that the middle preceded the passive and
was the source (apart from some endings of basically Stative sense) of its
forms. This, again, is a false trail to the explanation of thematism in PPIE.
2.3. If we return to the notion of early cognitive demands, various par-
ticularities suggest themselves. If an entity needs to receive special sig-
nalling it is presumably difficult to establish, in a sentence-usable value,
in its own right. It may be interpretable in speech only if:
(1) some other entity can be presumed s its anchor; or
(2) its purport is determinable from the speech-context; or
(3) it is delineated by, or cognitively derived from, some basic root in
the same semantic field.
These adjustments may be termed those of (1) (cor)relativity; (2) con-
text-sensitivity; (3) secondary definition. Common to them is the inderlying
sense of bafflement, and its dispersal, among early communicators.
In this connection (especially in considering the first and second con-
ditions) it is useful to recall a group of nominals noted by Schmidt (1986:
96). They are not quite homogeneous, but in all three sorts they combine
a lack of objective Status with a readiness to serve s agents. They are:
kinship terms, names and pronouns (let us say, the KNP set). For first
noticing their like behaviour credit is usually given to McLendon (1978,
reporting on Eastern Porno, an active-type Hokan language of northern
California; the example is also used by Mallison - Blake 1981: 52). Does
0-marking tie in with any of these explicative mechanisms?
2.3.1. With a strictly correlative solution the kinship terms fit well. Uncle
presupposes nephewlniece and implies relative ages; cousin is translative
between 2 + persons; no term is absolute. Formally the picture is cloudier.
PIE kinship terms can be thematic: Hitt. alias 'father' (cf. annas 'mother')
agrees with atta in Hellenic, Germanic and Slavic traces; Hitt. huhhaS
'grandfather' goes with Lat. auos\ and 'son' is often thematic s Gk.
hu(y)os, Celt. *makkos. But w-stems are rife and not all attributable to
analogy or attraction: Lat. nurus may derive from PIE snoru- 'son's wife'
(so Szemerenyi 1977: 68), the underlying form being sunu-i suyu and
swe are equally original. Besides, the need for a clear relational marker
was satisfied not by thematism but by creating the kinship marker par
excellence in +(t)er (on which, and the similar +w(y)
y
see Benveniste
1973: 171, 205-206; also Gamkrelidze - Ivanov 1984: 761-772).
Names are not correlative: one is not John simply in relation to
another's being Charles or Mary. Relativity may be imposed, by reference
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
13
to historical or literary patterns ('he played Hai to my Falstaff etc.) or
in legalistic formulae (s in the Roman wife's marital declaration ubi tu
Gaius ego Gaia)\ it is not inherent. Nor is there evidence that thematism
was essential in (P)PIE names, even s basic s those of gods or rivers;
and we lack a credible history of IE onomastic Formation in general.
Pronouns agree with other nominals s to some endings in the o-stem
declension. So in the nominative plural in -01, the ablative singular in -:d
(a significantly rare use of the plain voiced stop, or glottalic ejective, s
a PIE grammatical affix), and perhaps in the genitive singular (Beekes
(1985: 186) derives the Greek H- os-o from a post-Mycenaean pronominal
source). But these agreements are too sporadic to offset the fact that
while pronouns are often opposed in deixis (this versus that, or hiciste-ille,
in relation to the speaker's location) they are not correlative: that is, in
he buys these and sells those the verbs entail each other but the pronouns
do not (cf. Fillmore 1977: 72-73).
A more serious objection still is the existence among the thematics of
items like the widespread, and formally and semantically consistent,
lexeme *wlk
w
o-/luk
w
o-. It is hard to conceive any sense in which the wolf
is necessarily (cor)relative to anything eise. Yet it seems to be a founder
member of the paradigm.
2.3.2. The KNP set is more homogeneous in its sensitivity to the context
(s with type (2) above). For them, context presents itself s a series of
concentric cricles. The outermost is the world of discourse, in which
names operate. Then comes the social circle within which the kinship
terms make sense, with a smaller circle of local or familir referents. A
yet smaller circle, of immediate speech Situation, gives meaning to the
deictics; while the narrowest band of all lets T and *you' be understood,
this being each successive utterance. Yet again we face patchiness of for-
mal incidence in the various rings. It is quite understandable to desire to
mark those lexemes which lack a context-free Interpretation; even e.g.
*woik'o- 'dwelling' may convey no clear idea of its part in life, let alone
its local shape, except s an element of the speech Situation. But whereas
an approaching stranger, encountering the utterance give them to Hilary's
cousin, can attach no specific content to any of the three KNP terms, in
give them to Hilary's horse at least the last word conveys a clear sense
and possible Identification. Yet the PIE word for 'horse' is another early
o-stem, and defeats the contextual solution.
2.3.3. Let us test type (3), secondary deflnition. As is well known, in PIE
an added -o- may signify a derivation N -> N or A(dj): a Standard example
is *roteH2 'wheeF (Lat. rota) * *rotH2-o- Vheeled' (Skt. ratha- 'chariot').
Genitives in -o are relevant, given the adjectival value of the genitive (and
vice versa). Even the use of -o- s a linking device in compounds with
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
14
many stem-types may be a later extension of this sign of modification. It
is therefore attractive to suppose that already in PPIE some roots were
thus given an immediate extension of meaning; to put it more crudely,
some lexemes were usable only if they could be interpreted s sub-types,
or special manifestations, of existing roots which had cognitive acceptance.
Thus *yug+o is the agricultural application of *Vyug ('join' * 'yoke'),
and *woik' + o understood only in the light of *Vwk' ('dwelP -> 'dwelling').
Not many entities would seem so defeating s to need this epistemic lo-
cating; hence the low number of original N -l o lexemes. And the choice
would depend on the speech group; hence the sparseness of interlingual
cognates in that declension. To mark urgency at the N = V stage +e
was used; with separated N, recourse to the parallel back vowel is very
natural. Patchiness of adoption produces *ner in Greek ((a)ner-) but *nero
in Indic (Skt. nara-)\ 'man' and 'manlike' were rivals, and perhaps the
basic form seemed overstrong, so that Latin has it only onomastically, in
Ner-. Patchy KNP incidence is reasonable too, and o- marking may Start
so.
But a serious problem arises, none the less. One is obliged to find,
or cogently conjecture, an underlying root (N or V) in all cases. This is
not straightforward even with KNP items; with objective items, s many
undoubtedly are, the challenge is greater. For example, under the 'wolf
word there must lurk some root - presumably *VJk
w
/luk - with a credible
meaning which wolves can live up to. Perhaps it is *howP, an obvious pro-
pensity of the species; the activity is denoted by ul/ol in onomatopoetic
words elsewhere. This is just the sort of challenge to fire the ingenuity
of IE scholars.
[2.3.4. Let us take a speculative instance. One can easily belive that the
horse was noted s having a characteristically large and expressive eye.
Then we require a root *Vek
w
, balancing the known *Vk
w
so common
in semantically related forms. Now, in establishing the dorsal stops of PIE,
some award original Status to a unisegmental labiovelar (the phonic ele-
ments being later separated out in e.g. Anatolian or Tocharian B); others
think that *k
w
is a secondary contraction of bisegmental kw or k'w in
some languages (or in some words in those languages, s Sanskrit has
sacate but asva-). The Indo-Iranian evidence suggests PIE ek'wo-\ but
Celtic (OWel. epo) and Germanic (OE eoh) and Latin (s ?quo- is like
s&quor) justify an Urform with -k
w
-. That some degree of co-existence
(and squinting between them) is to be recognised is clear from Gk. hippos,
wherein (apart from the oddly high first vowel and the dialectal aspiration)
the -pp- needs explanation. (So it does in Boetian Thio-ppastos. In each
case the usual etymology is from forms with medial *-k'w-. Then Greek
should lose the labial with or without retention of syllabic weight, if the
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
15
history of other - C w > - sequences is our guide (so monos/monos from - H H > - ,
tettareslpisures from -tw-). The expected form is then *ikos or */:/coy or
ikkosj of w hich only the last is found (and only in Etymologicum Magnum
474. 12). The labiality must come from somew here eise, and the obvious
source is the rival -k
w
- w hich before back vow els gives -/?- in Greek. C on-
versely, if -k
w
~ is credited to the etymon the gemination needs excuse,
and this appears to be the influence of bisegmental -k'w-. Formal rivalry
seems to occur, then (see Szemerenyi 1990: 69 on the debated relation
of these consonantal combinations). If so, a root *Vek makes sensible
Szemerenyi's etymology ofblep (Laconian - glep) s from *g
w
l-ek
w
- 'cast
(my) glance at' (1969: 236-238; 1974: 145-146). (But the e- alternant is
hardly a 'retrograde' formation, s he says. If it reflects the PIE meta-
phonic stage it should be o (s it occurs in the noun element of the un-
derlying phrase); if the verb is a late unilingual creation, a clearcut Greek
equation of back vow el w ith noun but front w ith verb is not there; cf.
nouns like belos, tekos w ith verbs like boulomai, lou. And at least one
w ould look for a pairing of blep and *blopos.) If the horse w ere termed
the 'looking' or 'big- eyed' animal, then *ek
w
o-/ek'wo- is a ready formation.
Of course, one might object that dogs, too, have struck humans, and no
doubt the Indo- Europeans, w ith their large and soulful eyes. Well, the
same etymon (but w ith zero grade and a nasal extension) is at hand: *k
w
o-
n/k'wo-n (s w as suggested by C ohen (1988-91) to the surprise of some:
see Polome 1994: 195). Greek ku-on-, Skt. svan-, Lith. su- all fit; Latin
w ould have *guon-(i)- *coni- (w hich w as revocalised to canis to avoid
collision w ith the other *coni~ w ord 'ash, dust' - cf. Gk. konis - w hich
w ent to cinis\ this seems more credible than that the - 0- derived analogi-
cally to catulus 'pup', s Szemerenyi (1987: 878) thinks). A corresponding
bisegmental *ok 'w- should accompany the common *ok
w
- in this meaning;
note its traces in Attic Greek triottis (-0C C -), prospon (-o:C -), H omeric
eis Spa\
2.4. After all, how ever, thematic neuters import problematic features of
their ow n:
2.4.1. PPIE Speakers w ould have had no difficulty in apostrophising in-
animates if, s is probable, their Weltanschauung w as animistic. Nor does
it ever trouble Speakers, to judge from Philoctetes' 8 toxon phflon (So-
phocles, Philoc. 1128), or Lear's blow, winds, and crackyour cheeks (III 2),
or a contemporar/s come on, car
t
start, will youl But, w hile inanimates
w hich are admitted to the masuline- feminine set do possess a vocative in
-e (s Roman poets address their opus s libelle - cf. Martial 4. 89), an-
imate referents w hich happen to have neuter forms never do. Even s
late s Plautus females w ith neuter names have o- less vocatives like mea
Gymnasium (several examples are in the scene at Cistellaria 51-110).
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
16
There is no warrant for recognising an e-vocative in Indo-Iranian or Ana-
tolian, where vowel histories obscure what is probably the Stern-final -o-
of the class. In other words, where the expoctulatory suffix is employed,
a vocative in -e entails a nominative in -os. After all, it is understandable
to call on (and appropriately mark) only an entity which is capable of
appearing and/or postively acting. Thus this original cognitive linking, once
established, becomes set s hard s a piece of morphological granite.
2.4.2. It is harder, however, even to approach an answer s to why neuters
in the o-stem paradigm, and there alone, possess a final -m in the lexical
(or non-oblique) case. At first they comprise only inanimates; these are
unlikely actors or instigators at the Start, and long remain equally unlikely
s subjects of sentences (s obviously in Hittite and Greek). Therefore
they do not need a nominative marker to separate their non-existent agent
role from their nominal patientive one. They do not need an accusative
sign either; the basic lexical shape will serve for nuclear grammatical pur-
poses. And, in all the other declensions, so it does (the traditional "Nom-
Voc-Acc" lay-out in manuals is quite otiose - only Acc exists). Certainly,
Hittite thematic adjectives do sporadically lose their final -n (<-m),
possibly under the influence of endingless neuters in other paradigms;
but that does not alter the problem.
Opinion divides on whether accusative form object function derives
from the form's use s a spatial signal (i.e. allative -* objective). So judged
Planudes (13th Century, following Heliodorus, 8th Century), and many re-
cent scholars agree. But Kurylowicz (1964: 183) led the opposite view
(i.e. objective -* allative). Yet neuters which are not o-stems do not have
the final -m in the spatial use either; nor is there any sign of analogical
incursion from the masculines or feminines.
Burrow tackled the difficulty with the ingenious Suggestion (1955: 173)
that a group of neuter roots with final (lexical) -m were mistaken for
o-stem case-marked forms, and gave rise to further taddhita creations. To
be inclined to accept this idea, one requires a fund of roots in final -m.
But they are extremely rare in PIE. And to say that this rarity is itself
due to their being common only in neuters, all of which have been metana-
lysed and lost all independent etymological traces when submerged among
the thematics, is to argue in circles. From the absence parallel forms in
**-/m, **-M W , or **-em we might deduce that a phonological constraint
operated: namely that unprotected final -o was eschewed in Indo-
European. It is certainly uncommon. But it occurs in secondary middle
endings of the verb (*-(e)so, *(e)to, *-(o)nto), even if only Greek stays
happy with it.
Frankly, no defensible satisfying explanation of neuter -m is at band.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
17
3. V + elo
The third strategy of thematism appears at a late stage of PPIE, over-
lapping with PIE. At that epoch one expects increasingly 'grammaticaP
tasks marked minimally (or not at all): the PIE endingless locative belongs
there. Suffixing by mid vowels now formed stems from event-roots. This
was not, however, to establish (s with N) a separate pre-morphological
sub-class. Some verbs remained athematic throughout (if 'defective');
others were only partially thematised, and none entirely so. The stative-
resultative perfect was never so marked. There was a corresponding
functional spottiness: by the time of PIE formal Opposition may mark no
functional difference (s when aorists are both thematic and athematic;
root and sigmatic aorists are the latter) while conversely clear Opposition
of aspectual values may be obscured by unwelcome likeness of form (cf.
the Sanskrit non-durative past asicat 'poured' and the durative past atudat
'was striking', both thematic).
Also, there has by now arrived an awareness of the usefulness of a
back/front contrast in mid vowels, morphophonically employable (this con-
trast may have been produced by differential placing of word-accent; but,
for the doubts, see Szemerenyi 1990: 125). This tactic was long-lived: cf.
English sing/sang etc., or the phonologically alike modern Russian elo con-
trast (see Itkin 1994, for example). In PIE it might signal the derivation
V -* V', where V' may be intensive or frequentative (so Gk. pher - >
phore etc.) or be realised s a deverbative N (s Gk. nomos/nomos -
nem, Lat. toga - tego etc.). Anomalies are rife: Greek has e in N (belos
y
tekos) and o in V (lou, oiomai)\ while PIE first person endings in dual
and plural are - wes or - wos, - mes or - mos. The root-syllable o in the
perfect is less predictable once the other signs, like reduplication, are in
place: cf. Gk. leloipa but pepheuga. It is possible that the odd Variation
in e.g. Gk. leg- o- men, leg- e- te arises from Manczak's rule (1960) that before
a sonant post-accentual PIE e became o\ but the colour contrast is ruined.
(For similar wobbliness in apophony, i.e. eC/0C, note highly variable
guna/zero relation between presents and aorists, or the unusual
vrddhi/guna Opposition for number in proterodynamic 'Narten presents'
or 'Insler aorists' (Narten 1968; Insler 1972).)
Vowel suffixation had obvious euphonic merits in an evolving mor-
phology if consonantal endings needed to be attached to consonant-fmal
roots. Yet it occurs equally after resonants and vowels (s in Greek con-
tract verbs). So it must have some early more semantic role. Was this the
marking of one polar term in an old 'active' System a la Klimov (1977)?
The difference between e.g. Gk. - men and bain- o- men Ve are going' is
then unclear (time reference apart); and if the latter means really 'we are
actively moving' (essentially the view of Eichner 1975: 77 fn 3, on limited
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
18
evidence), how does Homeric be d'ienai 'made a move' fit in? True,
medio-passive perfects are athematic and resultative-stative in sense, so
that thematism seems to be the sign of /+ active/. But Renou (1925) was
sure, from Vedic, that /+ active/ was the unmarked term, in both senses;
and that thematism denoted / + Stative/ gains some support from the en-
during use of that value and the re-elaboration of positive indicators (such
s IE +e(s(c)) - for Stative-inchoative, identified by Watkins, e.g. 1971:
86-89). But the polar terms are not easy to apply to e.g. Gk. eimi 'be'
but eimi '(will) go', where form and function disagree. And subject-activity
verbs are thematic in some dialects of Greek and not in others (s Attic
agre 'capture' versus Aeolic agremi) or differently in dialects of the same
group (s Attic didsi 'he gives' versus Miletian didoi, or Attic histanai
'to stand' versus Euboean kathist ffn) or even between tenses (cf. Attic
present tithesi with imperfect ethei, not *etithe). On all this consult Tbcker
1990: 73 fn 83. In effect, the reason behind e/o root-suffixation in only
some verbal forms remains to be found.
For Kurylowicz (1956: 74) its starting point was in aorist formations,
already so fashioned; but that simply shifts the question elsewhere. The
origin for Watkins (1969: 65) was the middle voice; and Hart (1990: 448,
462) supposes it, at least in Class I presents, to come from the middle 3
sg. ending -o, although its position within the verbal construct seems to
rule that out. In any case, to locate its start-point is not enough: what
was it meant to do? To that question many answers have been offered;
their variety is illustrated in Figure 1:
name date Suggestion
Thieme
Meillet
Renou
Knobloch
(so Kortlandt 1983)
Risch (so Rix 1986)
Eichner
Schmalstieg (cf. idem
1980, 1986, 1988)
Shields (cf. idem 1992)
1929
1931
1932
1953
1965
1975
1978
1989
partial marker of middle voice ('une
tendance vers la voix moyenne')
(1) component of morphs + sko/ + no
(2) subjunctive (from athematics)
marker of indeterminate dimension
('eventueP ...'independant de toute
categorie verbale')
marks that verb has object (so does
nominal thematism)
marker of subjunctive (itself source of
indicative)
partial marker of continuous action
concordal marker of ergative-type actor
(= pronoun)
marker of non-present (for fuller -yo-)
Note: see below on Valliant (1936, 1937) and Kurytowicz (1956).
Figure 1. Functional diagnoses of (P)PIE verbal thematism
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
19
It is relevant that Plank (1980) has demonstrated that confusion be-
tween the marking of subject and object in a transitive System (or, pre-
sumably, S/P and A in an ergative one) is a relatively tolerable ambiguity.
This ruling could commend the solution of both Knobloch and Schmal-
stieg: but they are intended to preclude each other. Again, transitivity has
numerous diagnostics and in its career has had many unexpected Signals
(see Hopper - Thompson 1980). In IE, Tocharian was described by Winter
(1980) s so using (a) stem-initial consonant palatalisation (in the pre-
terite), and (b) a nasal formant (in the present - he notes that all Latin
nasal-infixed verbs are transitive with the solitary exception of the -cum-
bere dervatives), and (c) initial syllable accent (in second subjunctive and
causative formations). Jamison (1993) thought Sanskrit -ay- on old -0-pre-
sents to be transitiviser. These fmdings promote the notion that thematism
is an object-sign; by their wide differences they reduce its inevitability.
Indeed, long ago Kurylowicz provided (1956: 74 fn 47) the simplest
counter-argument to the Knobloch-Kortlandt position, employing IE data
where the arguments of bis opponents rested first on Kabardian and then
on Hungarian. He noted that there are plenty of clearly transitive verbs
in (P)IE which stay athematic: he listed many from the gveda (atti,
degdhi, dvesti... and could have added all the Greek 'second wave' group,
tithemi, ollttmi etc.). Presumed early PIE ergativity enters here. To the
suspicions aired above one may add disbelief that so careful a marking
of the so careful a marking of the S-V-O (or A-V-P) syntagm should so
soon fade to a scarcely visible and purely paradigmatic affair. Such fading
is more serious than the common loss within a single category of some
indistinct or arbitrary Opposition.
(In passing, Vaillant is omitted from Figure l because his 1936 paper
was not really concerned with verbal thematism s such, while his 1937
article sought to relate Class I and VI thematic verbs to Hittite -M and
-mi conjugations.)
Kurylowicz once suggested (1956: 73) that one function of thematism
s in *leik
w
elo- was to show the subjunctive mood. This idea can be traced
to Meillet (1931), and perhaps arose independently in Renou (1932) s
a development of his 'eventueP. It was revived and revised by Risch in
1965; he sought to establish the priority of the subjunctive s a mood over
the indicative (cf. Rix 1986). Within PPIE this construct may be called
the 'pre-subjunctive' - s 'pre-modaP might mislead, and such demo-
dalised derivations s Tocharian B imperfect < PIE optative are not
directly relevant, although they do demonstrate possibilities. Now, many
languages are less sensitive to (for example) time than to a fundamental
differentiation of verbal predications s between the actual and the con-
tingent. Thus in Hopi the verbal diatheses are basic, habitual (consuetudi-
nal), and contingent; and the contingent value often contextually entails
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
20
/+ future/. Tagalog meshes the parameters of aspect and reality, s in
Figure 2:
(Tagalog)
aspect
punctual
durative
reality
actual
(past) event
(present) event
process
contingent
hypothetical event
l possible
[ fut ure
process
Figure 2.
The Latin 'ideal second person' subjunctive falls in here: cLdelem haud
ferme midien inuenias uirum (Terence, Andria 460) 'you hardly ever find
a man faithful to a woman'. It is therefore quite thinkable that the e/o
suffix was an early marker of a deep but simple distinction. Delicacy s
to persons, number and so on came later (cf. Schlerath -Rittner 1985,
passim); and the markers were placed further from the root, s being
syntactical-inflecting whereas thematism was concerned with assessment
of the event's degree of reality in the context (and so, more adjacent to
lexical values). Merely to 'mention' the verb's action may be a doubtful
thing; but to assess the event s not actual but 'happenable' seems natural
enough. If anything clear emerges from sociolinguistic enquiry it is that
the schema * if p then q', or 'possibly q, given p', is well established even
in mentally naive speech communities. One may commend again Gonda's
old formula (1956: 69-70) concerning a 'process... not yet having a higher
degree of being than mental existence... "visualisation"...' (That Gonda
actually contrasts this value with contingency is due to bis reserving the
latter term for the precise pragmatic force of what is said, doubt s op-
posed to fear, etc.)
The next thing was for the marked form to be used to convey the
unmarked sense. This sort of linguistic Inflation is awkward but common.
It was aided in this case by the convenience of the linking vowel in con-
sonant-bound sequences like *ya(n)g+te. The sense of contingency (ap-
plicable in respect of wanting or urging or supposing the event) abides
s the subjunctive; its early date and temporal insensitivity permit its
having s further affixes both old 'secondary* endings and new 'primary*
ones (with -/). The mood value was so attached to -e/o-, however, that
once thematic forms had faded into simple declaratives they had the con-
tingent function re-imposed upon them, by repeated thematism: so, in
Greek, s -men had been extended to -o-men, faded bain-o-men spawned
*bain-o-o-men > bainmen. Some find it hard to accept that these pre-
subjunctives could come to have a non-contingent (faded) role at all, s
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
21
does Hart (1990: 446-447) on the ground that e.g. the future is not em-
ployable in a present, non-modal, sense (whereas the converse is frequent,
s in Eng. / am leaving tomorrow). But the future is so used, being poised
between tense and mood; so a character in Terence (Phormio 801) defends
a previous Statement by saying sie erit\ non temere dico 'it is so; Fm sure
of it'; and when Seneca declares that glory always accompanies courage
he uses comitabitur (Epi. Mor. 79.13). There is an extra implicature that
corroboration is still to seek; but then the present used of a future event
also carries an implicature, that the process has s good s begun.
The pre-subjunctive split into sub-categories of which future-marking
was only one (to invert the sequence proposed in Hahn 1953). One cannot
imagine that the thematic form was the original, unless by supposing Gk.
i-men, kei-tai or Skt. as-ti, se-te to have been produced by subtraction.
Hence some additional task must have been imposed on the basic verb,
and yet the new form assigned to that task was able to lose its force by
attrition or Inflation. (So Trkish verbal affix -mi$- loses its inferential
value in written, third person, past tense constructs; see Lewis 1967: 122,
162 versus 101, 140.)
It is harder to decide which of two ultimately contrasting systemic
terms is 'prior'. The formal origin and the system's invention may be at
variance. Within the IE modal System, s elsewhere, the athematic shape
is in place first. While it represents the indicative (non-contingent) value,
historical and systemic priority are congruent. Even when, in some verbs,
the thematic shape has been misapplied so s to convey the unmarked
term, the thematism is repeated to make a physical sign of the plus term;
so that the indicative form is still the earlier of the two. Yet the value
'indicative' in a mood-system becomes the ground-term only after the
emergence of a thematic shape with a marked modal value; so, in a sense,
the indicative does derive from the subjunctive. (The nearest parallel in
outer history which comes to mind is in the evolving collegiate structure
of the University of Durham (founded 1832). From 1833 to 1845 the entire
body of members were the university. In 1846, with the founding of Hat-
field Hall (later College), the prior group only then became the senior
College (University College) in the new dispensation.)
4. This paper
1
has made several interlinked suggestions. One is that a
period we may call PPIE was characterised by a series of cognitive leaps.
These were increasingly sophisticated and abstract; and they ended in a
morphological overlap with the organised paradigmatic marking, in PIE,
of conventionalised categories. Another is that, within an era of imprecise
but permansive pre-morphology, two revelations co-occurred. The first
1. Of which an earlier version was given at the Perceval Maitland Laurence seminar
in Cambridge, 25 May 1994.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
22
was a philosophical grasp of the difference between entity and predication;
the second was a phonological appreciation of the balanced nature of
front and back vocalism, and of its capacity of signalling that items are
related but opposed. (This use of metaphony went along with an equally
patchy reliance on the grades of apophony to perform slightly more reg-
ularised tasks.) This picture of historical stages is summarised in Figure 3:
stages ofPPIE
1. early -*
2. middle -
3. late -
(post-Anatolian?)
what is to be signalled
expostulation
secondary definition
contigency of event
on what
element
N = V
N
V
with what
vowel
W
[o]
[e/o]
Figure 3.
The late overlap with PIE when it had lost the Anatolian group of lan-
guages (which show no sign of a subjunctive) is balanced by an early
possible overlap with an epoch of nostratic adherence, even with a quite
embryonic speech stage. To face this latter notion is to reject the imitation
of reconstruction to (s Kurylowicz (1964: 58) proposed) 'stages bordering
the historical reality*. We should pursue thematism rather further back
than that, and recognise it s one process of shaping which had successive
varieties of form and of meaning. To say so is simply to put two and two
together; to be sure that the semantic details proposed above are correct
may be to make the answer five. But Indo-Europeanists should not neglect
this area of enquiry, and can no doubt get the sum right in the end.
Neville Collinge
31 Stansgate Av.
CAMBRIDGE CB2 2QZ
UNITED KINGDOM
REFERENCES
Anderson, S.R.
1977 "On mechanisms by which languages become ergative", in: C.N. Li (ed.),
317-363.
Beeks, R.S.R
1985 The origfns of the Indo-European nominal inflection. Innsbruck: Innsb. Bei-
trge zur Sprachwissenschaft 46.
Benveniste, E.
1973 Indo-European language and society. [Iranslation by E. Palmer of (1969) Le
vocabularie des institutions indoeurop ens.] London: Faber & Faber.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
23
Brosman, P.W. Jr.
1979 "The Hittite neuter -stems", KZ 93: 54- 63.
1984 "The IE cognates of the Hittite ai- and au stems", JIES 12: 345-365.
1994 " The PIE r/i/-, /uu- and f^ -sterns", Folia Linguistica Historica XV: 3-19.
Brugmann, K. H. Osthoff
1880 Morphologishe Untersuchungen. Vol. 3. Leipzig: THibner.
Bubenik, V.
1993 "Morphological and syntactic change in late Middle Indo-Aryan", JIES 21:
259-281.
Burrow, T.
1955 The Sanskrit language. London: Faber & Faber.
Chomsky, A.N.
1982 Lectures on govemment and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.
1995 "Bare phrase structure", in: G. Webelhuth (ed.), 385-439.
Cohen, G.L.
1988 Etymology and linguistic principles. (2 vols.) Rolla, MO: University of Missouri.
Cole, P. J.M. Sadock (eds.)
1977 Syntax and Sematics 8: Grammatical relations. New York: Academic Press.
Collinge, N.E.
1985 The laws of Indo-European. Amsterdam Philadelphia: Benjamins.
1993 Review of Plank 1991. Studies in Language 17: 264-269.
Cresswell, M.L.
1994 Language in the world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, R.M.W. (ed.)
1976 Grammatical categories in Australian languages. Canberra: Australien Institute
of Aboriginal Studies.
Dunkel, G.E.
1992 "Two old problems in Greek: and 5'\ Glotta 70: 197-
223.
Eichner, H.
1975 "Die Vorgeschichte des hettitischen Verbalsystems", in: H. Rix (ed.), 71-104.
Etter, A. (ed.)
1986 O-o-pe-ro-si. (Fs. Risch.) Berlin New York: \\folter de Gruyter.
Fillmore, CJ.
1977 "The case for case reopened", in: P. Cole J.M. Sadock (eds.), 59-81.
Fisiak, J. (ed.)
1980 Historical morphology. The Hague: Mouton.
Gamkrelidze, TV. V.V. Ivanov
1984 Indoevropejskij jazyk i indOeiropeicy. (2 vols.) Tbilisi: Tbilisi State University.
[Translated by J. Nichols (1995) s Indo-European and the Indo-Europeans
(Trends in Linguistics 80.) Berlin New York: Mouton de Gruyter.]
Garett, A.
1990 "The origin of NP split ergativity", Language 66: 261-296.
Georgiev, V.l.
1984 "Die drei Hauptperioden des indoeurop ischen", Diachronica 1: 65-87.
Gonda, J.
1956 The character of the Indo-European moods. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
24
Hahn, E.A.
1953 Subjunctive and Optative: Their origin s futures. New York: American Philo-
logical Association.
Hart, G.R.
1988 "Anatolian evidence and the origins of the Indo-European medio-passive",
BSOAS 51: 69-95.
1990 "'Class l present', subjunctive and middle voice in Indo-European", BSOAS
53: 446-468.
Heesterman, J.C. G.H. Schokker VI. Subramaniam (eds.)
1968 Pratidnam. (Fs. Kuiper.) The Hague: Mouton.
Heinz, A M. Karas* (eds.)
1965 Symbolae linguisticae. (Fs. Kurytowicz.) Wroclaw Warszawa Krakow:
Polska Akademie Nauk.
Hoffman, K.
1967 Der Injunktiv im Veda. Heidelberg: Winter.
Hopper, PJ. S.A. Thompson
1980 "Transitivity in grammar and discourse", Language 56: 251-299.
Insler, S.
1972 "On proterodynamic root present inflection", MSS 30: 55-64. (But see also
Language 48 (1972): 557.)
Itkin, I.B.
1994 "Jee raz o Ceredovanij e/ v sovremennom russkom jazyke", VJa 1994. 1:
126-133.
Jamison, S.
1983 Function and form in the -aya- formations of the Rig veda and Atharva Veda.
Gttingen: KZ Ergnzungsheft 31.
Jasanoff, J.
1978 Stative and middle in Indo-European. Innsbruck: Institut fr Sprachwissen-
schaft der Universitt Innsbruck.
Kammenhuber, A.
1985 "Zum Modus i nj unkt i v und zum Drei-genus-System im Urindoger-
manischen", in: U. Pieper G. Stickel (eds.), 435-466.
Klimov, G.A.
1977 Tipologija jazykov aktivnogo stroja. Moskva: AN SSSR.
Knobloch, J.
1953 "La voyelle thematique -e/o-: serait-elle un indice d'objet indoeuropen?",
Lingua 3: 407-420.
Kortlandt, F.
1983 "Proto-Indo-European verbal syntax", JIES 11: 307-324.
Kurytowicz, J.
1956 L'apophonie en indO-europoen. Wroctaw: Polska Akademia Nauk.
1964 The inflectional categories of Indo-European. Heidelberg: Winter.
Lehman, W.P.
1976 "From topic to subject in Indo-European", in: C.N. Li (ed.), 445-456.
Lewis, G.L.
1967 Turkish grammar. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Li, C.N. (ed.)
1976 Subject and topic. New York: Academic Press.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
25
1977 Mechanisms of syntactic change. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.
Mallinson, G. BJ. Blake
1981 Language typology. Amsterdam New York Oxford: North Holland.
Manczak, W.
1960 "Origine de Tapophonie e/o en indo-europen", Lingua 9: 277-287.
Matisoff, J.A.
1990 "On megalocomparison", Landtage 66: 106-120.
Matsumoto, K.
1993 "Problem of ergativity in Indo-European", JIES 21: 303-329.
McLendon, S.
1978 "Ergativity, case, and transitivity in Eastern Porno", UAL 44: 1-9.
Meillet, A.
1925 Review of Fs. Wackernagel (1923). BSL 25: 46-49.
1931 "Caractere s^condaire du type thomatique indo-europe"en", BSL 32: 193-203.
M hlestein, H E. Risch (eds.)
1979 Colloguium Mycenaeum. Neuchatel: Universite* de Neuchatel.
Narten, J.
1968 "Zum 'proterodynamischen' Wurzelpr sens", in: J.C. Heesterman et al.
(eds.), 9-19.
Nichols, J.
1986 "Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar", Language 62: 56-119.
Pedersen, H.
1907 "Neues und nachtr gliches", KZ 40: 129-217.
1924 [1962J Sprogsvidenskaben i det nittende Aarhundrede: metoder og resultater. Copen-
hagen: Gyldendalske Boghandel. [Translation by J.W. Spargo, The discovery
of language. Bloomington, In.: Indiana University Press.]
1933 "Zur Frage nach der Urverwandtschaft des indoeurop ischer mit dem ugro-
fionischen", Mem. Soc. Finno-Ougrienne 67: 308-326.
Pieper, U G. Stickel (eds.)
1985 Studia linguistica diachronica et synchronica. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Plank, F.
1980 "Enconding grammatical relations: Acceptable and unacceptable non-dis-
tinctness", in: J. Fisiak (ed.), 289-325.
1991 Paradigms: The economy of inflection. Berlin New York: Mouton de Gruy-
ter.
Plank, F. (ed.)
1979 Ergativity. New York: Academic Press.
, E.G.
1994 Review of Cohen 1988-91. JIES 22: 195-196.
Puhvel, J. (ed.)
1969 Substance and structure of language. Berkeley Los Angeles: University of
California Press.
Renou, L.
1925 "Le type vedique tudati". Mtlanges Vendryes. Paris: Champion, 309-316.
1932 "A propos du subjonctif vedique", BSL 33: 5-30.
Risch, E.
1965 "Zum Problem der thematischen Konjugation", in: A. Heinz M. Karas*
(eds.): 232-242. (Also in Kleine Schriften (1981): 701-709.)
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
26
Rix, H.
1986 Zur Entstehung des indogemianishen Modussystems. Innsbruck: Innsbrucker
Institut fr Sprachwissenschaft.
1988 "The Proto-Indo-European middle: Content, forms and origin", MSS 49:101-
119.
Rix, H. (ed.)
1975 Flexion und Wonbildung. (Vth Fachtagung der indogermanischen Gesells-
chaft.) Wiesbaden: Reichert.
Schlerath, B. V. Riltner (eds.)
1985 Grammatische Kategorien: Funktion und Geschichte. Wiesbaden: Reichert. (=
Vllth Fachtagung der indogermanischen Gesellschaft.)
Schmalstieg, WR.
1978 "The middle voice re-visited", Folia Linguistica 12: 349-366.
1980 Indo-European linguistics. University Park PA: Pennsylvania State University
Press.
1986 "The ergative function of the Proto-indo-European genitive", (Remarks on
Kortlandt 1983.) JIES 14: 161-172.
1988 "The ergative syntax of the Indo-European middle aorist", Lingua Posnanien-
sis 31: 11-22.
Schmidt, K.H.
1986 "Zur Vorgeschichte des prdikativen Syntagmas in indogermanischen", in:
A. Etter (ed.), 90-103.
Shields, K.C. Jr.
1989 "The origins of the thematic vowel", IF 94: 7- 20.
1992 A history of Indo-European verb morphology. Amsterdam Philadelphia:
Benjamins.
Silverstein, M.
1976 "Hierarchy of features and ergativity", in: R.M.W. Dixon (ed.): 112-171.
Sommer, F.
1937 "Ahhijawa und kein Ende", IF 55: 168-297.
Szemernyi, O.
1969 "Etyma Graeca II", in: Studio classica et orientaliaA. Pagliaro oblata. (3 vols.)
Rome; 3. 233-259. (Also 1987: 1367-1369.)
1974 "The origins of the Greek lexicon: ex orience lux". Journal ofHellenic Studies
94: 144-157.
1977 Studies in the kinship terminology of the Indo-European languages. (= Acta
Iranica 7) Leiden: Brill.
1979 "The consonant alternation pt/p in early Greek", in: H. Mhlestein E.
Risch (eds.), 323-340.
1987 Scripta Minora (edited by P. Considine J.T. Hooker). Innsbruck: Inns-
brucker Institut fr Sprachwissenschaft.
Thieme, P.
1929 Das Plusquamperfektum im Veda. Gttingen: Vanderhoeck & Ruprecht.
Trask, R.L.
1979 "On the origin of ergativity", in: F. Plank (ed.): 385-404.
Tucker, E.F.
1990 The creation of morphological regularity: Early Greek verbs in - , -a, - 6,
-u and ( . Gttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht. (HS (KZ) Ergnzungsheft
35.)
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
27
Uhlenbeck, C.C.
1901 "Agens und Patiens im Kasussystem der indogermanischen Sprachen", IF 12:
170-171.
Vaillant, A.
1936 "Cergatif indo-europoen", BSL 37: 93-108
1937 "Lorigine des presents thmatiques en - e/o", BSL 38: 89-101.
Villar, F.
1983 Ergatividad, acusadividad y gfnero en la familia lingnfstica indoeuropea.
Salamanca: University de Salamanca.
1984 "Ergativity and animate/inanimate gender in Indo- European", KZ 97: 167-
196.
Watkins, C.
1969 Indogermanische Grammatik III/I. Heidelberg: Winter.
1971 "Hittite and Indo-European studies: Denominative statives in -e-\ TPhS 70:
51-93.
Webelhuth, G. (ed.)
1995 Government and binding theory and the minimalist program. Oxford Cam-
bridge, MA: Blackwell.
West, ML.
1989 "An unrecognised injunctive usage in Greek", Glotta 67: 135-138.
Winter, W.
1969 "Vocative and imperative", in: J. Puhvel (ed.), 205-223.
1980 "Morphological signaling of selection properties: Transitiveness in Tocharian
B and A verbs", in: J. Fisiak (ed.), 421-442.
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM
Brought to you by | University of Sussex
Authenticated | 139.184.30.135
Download Date | 8/23/12 10:50 PM

S-ar putea să vă placă și