Sunteți pe pagina 1din 25

10.

1177/1077727X03255900
FAMILY
Jung,
Lennon
AND/CONSUMER
BODY IMAGE
SCIENCES
AND MEDIA
RESEARCH
IMAGES
JOURNAL

ARTICLE

Body Image, Appearance Self-Schema,


and Media Images
Jaehee Jung
University of Delaware
Sharron J. Lennon
Ohio State University

This study examines the effects of womens appearance self-schemas and exposure to attractive
media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood. Womens response to media images was
expected to vary according to an individual difference variable, appearance self-schema, or cognitive representations of organized information about the self in relation to appearance. College
women volunteers (N = 168) were divided into two groups (schematic and aschematic) on the
basis of appearance self-schema; half of each group was exposed to photos of attractive images,
whereas the other half was not exposed to any images before completing measures of body image,
self-esteem, and mood. Women who are schematic on appearance exhibited lower body image,
lower self-esteem, and more negative mood than did those who are aschematic on appearance.
Neither exposure to media images in conjunction with appearance self-schema nor exposure
alone affected response on body image, self-esteem, and mood.
Keywords:

appearance self-schema; body image; mood; self-esteem; media images

Body image is a mental image of ones body (Garner & Garfinkel,


1981) and simply refers to how individuals perceive their own bodies.
Because society places a greater emphasis on the appearance of
women than that of men in establishing their identities, women are
often evaluated by their physical attractiveness (Locher, Unger,
Sociedade, & Wahl, 1993; Timko, Striegel-Moore, Silberstein, &
Rodin, 1987) rather than by their abilities or achievements (Drogosz &
Levy, 1996). This emphasis is evidenced by heightened interest in diet
industries and the increased use of advanced medical technologies to
improve physical appearance. Thus, researchers have documented
womens dissatisfaction with their bodies (Cash & Henry, 1995;
Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985) and an increasing
Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, Vol. 32, No. 1, September 2003 27-51
DOI: 10.1177/1077727X03255900
2003 American Association of Family and Consumer Sciences

27

28

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

tendency among women to diet (Mellin, Irwin, & Scully, 1992; Polivy,
Garner, & Garfinkel, 1986; Rosen, 1990).
In contemporary U.S. society, media images reflect cultural standards and emphasize unrealistically thin body shapes and attractive
physical characteristics. Because most women are not granted the
ideal body size and shape from nature, approximating the cultural
ideal of female beauty is difficult (Brownell, 1991). If women consider
appearance important in self-perceptions and perceptions about self
made by others, their body images may be affected by failure to measure up to the cultural ideal. Fallon (1990) has articulated a social-cultural explanation of body image that suggests that social and cultural
factors (e.g., media images) have a powerful influence on the development and maintenance of body image. However, there have been
mixed results for the effects of media images on body image and selfesteem, some possibly due to methodological differences. In Richinss
(1991) study, female college students exhibited lower satisfaction
with their own physical attractiveness when they were exposed to
advertisements containing idealized images. Thornton and Moore
(1993) found similar results for the effects of media images on physical attractiveness and social anxiety but not on self-esteem. On the
other hand, Cusumano and Thompson (1997) found no relationship
between simple exposure and indices of body image, eating dysfunction, and self-esteem.
Although sociocultural messages about womens bodies typically
glamorize an unrealistically slender body, only some women are
adversely affected by those messages. Some women are satisfied with
their bodies even when they deviate from the ideal (Rosen, 1996),
some women are more susceptible to media images than are others
(e.g., depressed women or bulimic women) (Mori & Morey, 1991;
Peterson, 1987), and some women are dissatisfied with their bodies
even when they do approximate the ideal (Hesse-Biber, 1996). Thus,
any explanation of body image must accommodate the heterogeneity
of body image experiences.
A few studies identified self-schema (Markus, 1977) or cognitive
representations of organized information about the self as an individual difference variable that may moderate the relationships between
sociocultural pressure and eating disorders (Vitousek & Hollon, 1990)
and between self-concept and eating disorders (Stein, 1996). Selfschemas are available in a variety of domains such as independence
(Markus, 1977), masculinity (Markus, Smith, & Moreland, 1985), and
body weight (Markus, Hamill, & Sentis, 1987). For example,

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

29

masculine schematics were those individuals who rated themselves


as extreme on most trait adjectives concerning masculinity (e.g.,
aggressive, dominant, and acts as a leader) and who indicated that
those characteristics were important to their self-evaluation (Markus
et al., 1985). Similarly, when individuals are highly invested in their
appearance (i.e., appearance is important, self-relevant, and central to
the individuals) and are actively concerned with it, they are said to be
appearance schematic or schematic on appearance. On the other
hand, when appearance is not important, self-relevant, and central to
the individuals, they are said to be appearance aschematic or
aschematic on appearance. Cash and Labarge (1996) found that college women who are schematic on appearance are more self-conscious in public and more socially anxious, express more symptoms
of depression and eating disorders, and have poorer social selfesteem than those who are aschematic on appearance. Presumably,
womens responses on measures of body image (e.g., body satisfaction or dissatisfaction, appearance satisfaction or dissatisfaction,
appearance evaluation, and appearance orientation) can be predicted
by whether they are schematic or aschematic with respect to appearance. Also, the extent to which media images affect womens body
image, self-esteem, and mood may depend on whether they are schematic or aschematic on appearance. Therefore, the purpose of this research was to examine the effect of appearance self-schema and exposure to attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and
mood.
THE RELATED LITERATURE
Body Image, Self-Esteem, and Mood

Many researchers have found that body image (Jackson, Sullivan,


& Rostker, 1988; Lennon, Rudd, Sloan, & Kim, 1999) or body satisfaction (Lerner, Orlos, & Knapp, 1976; Simmons & Rosenberg, 1975) is
positively related to self-esteem. For example, Jackson et al. (1988)
found that high self-esteem in women explained favorable evaluations of appearance and more appearance-directed behaviors, both of
which are measures of body image. Lower self-esteem, on the other
hand, was associated with abnormal eating behavior (Button, Loan,
Davies, & Sonuga-Barke, 1997). Although most of the previously
mentioned studies focused on European Americans, the same results

30

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

have been found with African American women (Akan & Grilo, 1994;
Thomas, 1989), Asian women (Lee & Lee, 1996), and Asian American
women (Akan & Grilo, 1994).
Because a positive relationship exists between body image and
self-esteem, it is reasonable to expect that a poor body image would be
associated with other indicators of poor psychological health along
with low self-esteem. For example, as low self-esteem has been found
to be related to depressive symptomatology in women (Russo, Green,
& Knight, 1993), it might be expected that women with low scores on
body image variables would exhibit more symptoms of depression
than would women with higher scores on body image variables.
Koenig and Wasserman (1995) recently found that depression was
associated with a poor body image in their sample of male and female
college students. In a sample of 174 college women, Silverstein,
Caceres, Perdue, and Cimarolli (1995) found that depressive
symptomatology was associated with anxiety and poor body image.
These and other studies (Fabian & Thompson, 1989; Thompson &
Psaltis, 1988) seem to suggest relationships among poor body image,
depression, low self-esteem, anxiety, and other measures of negative
mood.
Much of the research on body image has studied either clinical
populations (e.g., Garner & Kearney-Cooke, 1996) or college students
(e.g., Keeton, Cash, & Brown, 1990). Although the importance of
appearance among women decreases somewhat with age (Pliner,
Chaiken, & Flett, 1990), concern with appearance and body image
remains important to women across the life span (Gupta & Schork,
1993; Pliner et al., 1990). Indeed, Pliner et al. (1990) found that the
importance of appearance to women of college age (M = 3.9) and
women aged 60 and older (M = 3.8) are quite similar. Likewise, in a
large national survey, Cash and Henry (1995) found no significant difference between college-age women and other women up to the age
of 70 on one measure of body image (body areas satisfaction).
Media Influences on Body Image

Media images seem to play a critical role in shaping the cultural


ideal of beauty and attractiveness through advertising, retailing, and
the entertainment industry. Although there are no perfect criteria that
people can use to judge their own or anothers attractiveness, a uniform standard of beauty was made possible by the rise of mass media
(Mazur, 1986). Previous studies suggested that the constructed

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

31

images of attractive models in the omnipresent media may influence


the general criteria for evaluating attractiveness to a considerable
degree and elevate our standards for attractiveness (Martin &
Kennedy, 1993; Richins, 1991). Beauty industries such as cosmetics,
diet foods, plastic surgery, and the fashion business have expanded
their economic investments and emphasized the need for appearance-enhancing products and behaviors. Perhaps this is because there
is increasing dissatisfaction with their bodies among women due to
increasing demand for a thin body size. For example, female college
students exhibited increased negative feelings and body dissatisfaction after exposure to ultra-thin models (Stice & Shaw, 1994).
The fashion and beauty industries appeal to female consumers to
use their products by presenting attractive images (e.g., young, slim,
and attractive models with flawless skin) through advertisements.
According to Shaw and Waller (1995), comparisons made with media
images through indirect contact such as looking at magazine photos
are likely to encourage women to use those images as reference points
in evaluating their own bodies. For example, the extent to which girls
reported magazines as an important source of information for their
appearances was correlated with the degree of investment in thinness, weight management behavior, and disturbed eating (Levine,
Smolak, & Hayden, 1994). Therefore, it would be important to examine the extent to which exposure to media images can trigger negative
responses on womens body image, self-esteem, and mood, especially
for those who are schematic on appearance.
Schema Theory

Self-schemas resulting from the process of acquiring knowledge


about the self (e.g., categorization and evaluation of behavior by oneself and others) influence information processing about the self in a
variety of domains, with more consistent judgments in ones schematic domains. Self-schemas allow one to attend selectively to information of greater relative importance while ignoring other information. For example, in a study by Markus et al. (1987), body weight
schematics and body weight aschematics were identified depending
on whether the term overweight was self-descriptive and was an
important dimension to individuals self-concept among overweight
people. Overweight people who were schematic on body weight
were more likely to perceive themselves as overweight than were
overweight people who were aschematic on body weight; the latter

32

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

were more likely to perceive themselves as of normal body weight.


Thus, people with well-defined appearance self-schemas are likely to
attend to appearance information (e.g., physical attractiveness and
thinness), yet those without well-defined appearance self-schemas
are likely to ignore or pay less attention to such information.
Attributes related to self-schemas are always in peoples minds as
well-developed self-structures and are ready to be activated in information processing as chronically accessible constructs. Unlike temporarily activated constructs that are not expected to last long, chronically accessible constructs are those that are cognitively available due
to persistent activation over time (Higgins, King, & Mavin, 1982).
Thus, exposure to attractive media images might temporarily activate
appearance-related information for most of the people, but the exposure can activate chronically accessible appearance-related information for those who have appearance self-schemas. For those who have
appearance self-schemas, appearance-related information can be
used in their self-evaluations and their evaluations of others. For
example, for those who are self-schematic on appearance, a variety of
stimuli can be evaluated with reference to their own appearances
(e.g., Does this hair style make me look younger? Is she more attractive than I am? Does this dress make me look slim?). Thus, appearance
may be an important part of schematics thoughts, emotions, and
behaviors, and appearance self-schema may be an important variable
explaining individual differences in body image, self-esteem, and
mood. Also, individuals who are temporarily exposed to attractive
media images may have negative evaluations of their bodies and
themselves, especially for those who are highly invested in their
appearances, as those images reflect cultural standards with unrealistically thin body sizes and attractive physical characteristics.
Research Hypotheses

It was expected that individuals who are schematic on appearance


are more likely to be dissatisfied with their bodies and self in general
and are more likely to devote their time and energy to taking actions
to improve their appearances than are those who are aschematic on
appearance. Similarly, individuals who are schematic on appearance
were expected to exhibit more negative mood than were those who
are aschematic on appearance. Finally, exposure to attractive media
images was expected to influence individual differences in body

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

33

image, self-esteem, and mood, especially for appearance schematic


individuals; individuals who are schematic on appearance were
expected to exhibit the most negative mood, self-esteem, and body
image after being exposed to attractive media images.
Therefore, based on the literature reviewed, the following hypotheses were proposed in this study.
Hypothesis 1: Women who are schematic on appearance will exhibit lower
self-esteem and body image scores than will those who are aschematic
on appearance.
Hypothesis 2: Women who are schematic on appearance will exhibit higher
scores on negative mood and lower scores on positive mood than will
those who are aschematic on appearance.
Hypothesis 3: Women who are exposed to attractive media images will
exhibit lower body image scores, lower self-esteem scores, and greater
negative mood scores than will those who are not exposed to media
images.
Hypothesis 4: Appearance self-schema will mediate effects of exposure to
attractive media images on body image, self-esteem, and mood; as a
result, women who are schematic on appearance and are exposed to
media images will exhibit the most negative mood, self-esteem, and
body image scores.

METHOD
Instrument Development

To test research hypotheses, the following four main variables


were considered in this study: appearance self-schema, mood, selfesteem, and body image. The four variables were measured using 12
instruments. Participant characteristics were assessed for preliminary analyses as well as descriptive purposes.
The Appearance Schemas Inventory was used to assess appearance self-schema, that is, the importance, self-relevance, and centrality of appearance in individuals lives (Cash & Labarge, 1996). This
measure consists of 14 items in a Likert-type rating scale format (1 =
strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Higher scores indicate that an
individual is more schematic toward appearance than are others.
Example items are What I look like is an important part of who I am
and If I could look just as I wish, my life would be much happier.
The internal consistency of the Appearance Schemas Inventory has
been established as .82 for college women (Cash, 1992). Its 1-month

34

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

test-retest stability for female college students has been reported to be


.71 (Cash, 1992).
Two subscales (Joy and Distress) of the Differential Emotions Scale
(Izard, 1972) were used to examine the level of mood distress after
being exposed to attractive media images. These measures are composed of rating scale items on which people indicate how they are
feeling regarding each of the items at that time (1 = very slightly or not
at all to 5 = very strongly). For the Joy scale, ratings of the adjectives
happy, joyful, and delighted are summed and used as the measure of joy.
For the Distress scale, ratings of the adjectives discouraged, sad, and
downhearted are summed and used as the measure of distress. Measures of internal consistency for the Joy and Distress measures have
been found to be .80 and .90, respectively (Izard, 1972).
Heinberg and Thompsons (1992, 1995) visual analogue scales
(VAS) were used to measure immediate response in mood and body
satisfaction (an affective measure of body image) following exposure
to attractive images. Participants were asked to indicate their disturbance level on a 100-mm line by placing a short vertical slash to reflect
their current mood state. The anchors of the 100-mm lines were no
anxiety or extreme anxiety, no depression or extreme depression, no anger or
extreme anger, no body dissatisfaction or extreme body dissatisfaction, and
no overall appearance dissatisfaction or extreme overall appearance dissatisfaction. Thus, each of these measures was a 101-point scale, scored
such that higher scores indicated greater disturbance. All measures of
mood (VAS-anxiety, VAS-depression, and VAS-anger) and body satisfaction (VAS-body dissatisfaction and VAS-overall appearance dissatisfaction) have demonstrated convergent validity (Heinberg &
Thompson, 1995).
The revised Janis-Field Self-Esteem Scale (Eagly, 1967; Janis &
Field, 1959) was used to measure participants social self-esteem following exposure to attractive images. The instrument contains 20
items, 10 of which are keyed in a positive direction and 10 of which are
keyed in a negative direction to balance for response-bias (Robinson &
Shaver, 1973). Participants rated (1 = very often to 5 = never) their comfort level in various social situations in which higher scores are indicative of high social self-esteem. Sample items are How often do you
feel that you have handled yourself well at a social gathering? and
How often do you worry about whether other people like to be with
you? This scale has reported split-half reliabilities of .72 and .88
(Robinson & Shaver, 1973) and a test-retest reliability of .92
(Campbell, Chew, & Scratchley, 1991).

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

35

Rosenbergs (1965) Self-Esteem Scale is a reliable (.85) and valid


instrument that measures several areas of global self-worth (Silber &
Tippett, 1965). It is composed of 10 items presented in a Likert-type
format (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). Half of the items are
keyed in a positive direction, and the other half are keyed in a negative direction, which should be reverse scored so that higher scores
indicate higher levels of global self-esteem. Example items are I feel
that I have a number of good qualities and I am able to do things as
well as most other people.
Two subscales (Appearance Evaluation and Appearance Orientation) of the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire, a
well-validated body-image measure (Keeton et al., 1990), were used
to measure participants body images in relation to appearance.
Appearance Evaluation assesses the extent to which individuals like
and are satisfied with their physical appearances. This subscale is
composed of seven statements concerning appearances. Example
items are Most people would consider me good-looking and I like
my looks just the way they are. Appearance Evaluation has a
reported internal consistency of .88 and a 1-month stability of .91
(Cash & Szymanski, 1995). The 12 statements in Appearance Orientation assess cognitive importance of and behavioral improvement or
maintenance of ones appearance. Thus, Appearance Orientation
measures the time, mental energy, and behavioral activity devoted to
ones appearance. This subscale has an internal consistency of .85 and
a stability of .90 (Cash & Szymanski, 1995). Sample items are I am
always trying to improve my physical appearance and I check my
appearance in a mirror whenever I can.
Stimulus Materials

Photo slides were used to trigger participants cognitive and emotional vulnerabilities to attractive media images. Slides featured
images of relatively unknown female models scanned from fashion
magazines and catalogs (e.g., Glamour, Marie Claire, Harpers Bazaar).
White models were chosen for this study as they are dominantly featured in the media. As the result of a pilot test of female clothing and
textiles graduate students, 16 photos were selected for use as stimuli.
Photos used received average ratings between 6 and 7 on a 1 to 7 scale
on the dimension of physical attractiveness. The 16 photos consisted
of head-and-torso images and full-length images. All the models in
the photos were judged to be thin by the pilot sample.

36

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

Procedure

Participants were recruited through (a) advertisements posted on


bulletin boards of different university buildings or (b) oral solicitation
in several classes. Participants were given incentives of cash prize
drawings for their participation. Participants responded twice, once
during an initial questionnaire session and then during a follow-up
experimental session, with an interval of 2 weeks between sessions to
prevent any carry-over effects from the initial session. Tasks were
completed in small groups of 5 to 10. In the initial session, participants
were given an informed consent form and asked to complete a questionnaire packet containing the Appearance Schemas Inventory and
items assessing demographic information (age, major, height, weight,
and ethnicity). Participants level of appearance self-schemas was
based on a median split of scores; they were blocked into two groups
(high appearance self-schema scores versus low appearance selfschema scores). For convenience, participants with high appearance
self-schema scores are referred to as schematics on appearance,
whereas participants with low appearance self-schema scores are
referred to as aschematics on appearance. Half of each group was randomly assigned to exposure to the media images experimental condition, and the other half of each group was randomly assigned to the
no exposure control condition for the follow-up session.
After 2 weeks, participants were asked to return for a follow-up
session, which did not require a parallel task. Separate sessions of
small groups were held for those who were assigned to the exposure
condition and for those who were assigned to the control condition.
Those who were assigned to the exposure condition were presented
with 16 media images in slide photos and given a filler task by which
they assessed the formality and fashionability of models clothes. Participants were exposed to each slide for about 8 seconds. After exposure to the attractive images of stimulus photos, participants completed dependent measures of mood (joy, distress, depression,
anxiety, and anger), self-esteem (social and global), and body image
(body dissatisfaction, overall appearance dissatisfaction, appearance evaluation, and appearance orientation). Participants completed questionnaire packets within 25 to 30 minutes. Participants
who were assigned to the control condition completed dependent
measures without being exposed to media images.

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

37

RESULTS
Participant Characteristics

This study used a college student sample for convenience; it was


also reasonable given the nature of the association between age and
measures of body image. A convenience sample of 175 college women
enrolled in various majors at two large Midwestern universities volunteered for this study. Missing values resulted in usable data from a
final sample of 168 women. Participants ages ranged from 18 to 28,
with a mean of 21. The sample included 134 European Americans, 13
African Americans, 15 Asian Americans, 3 Hispanic Americans, and 3
other ethnicities. The majority of the participants (n = 104) were clothing-related majors such as textiles and clothing and apparel merchandising; other participants (n = 64) came from a variety of other majors
such as business, nutrition, and the arts.
Preliminary Analyses

Cronbachs coefficient alpha measured the internal consistency of


the instruments used in this study (see Table 1). Reliabilities resulted
for the Appearance Schemas Inventory, both social (Janis-Field SelfEsteem) and global (Rosenbergs Self-Esteem) measures of selfesteem, two subscales (Appearance Evaluation and Appearance Orientation) of the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire,
and two subscales (Joy and Distress) of the Differential Emotions
Scale. Reliabilities of the VAS could not be calculated due to the nature
of the measures, which required participants to mark a slash on a horizontal line rather than use a numerical value to indicate disturbance
level.
Because appearance is often important to clothing and merchandising students, an analysis of variance (ANOVA) determined if
major (apparel major or nonapparel major) was related to level of
appearance self-schemas with major as the independent variable and
appearance schema scores as the dependent variable. Major was
unrelated to level of appearance schemas in this sample, F(1, 166) =
.123, p > .05. In an attempt to rule out the possible effects of major on
the dependent variables of interest in this study, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was calculated. Results revealed that the

38

TABLE 1:

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

Means, Standard Deviations, and Minima and Maxima for All


Variables Used in the Analyses

Measures
Appearance self-schema
Appearance Schemas
Inventory
Mood
Joy scale
Distress scale
VAS-anxiety
VAS-depression
VAS-anger
Self-esteem
Janis-Field SelfEsteem Scale
Rosenbergs SelfEsteem Scale
Body image
Appearance evaluation
Appearance orientation
VAS-body dissatisfaction
VAS-overall appearance
dissatisfaction

Scale

Mean

SD Minimum Maximum Reliability

14 to 70

37.99

7.97

18

55

.81

3 to 15
8.39 3.03
3 to 15
5.74 2.58
0 to 100 44.43 28.73
0 to 100 25.59 22.65
0 to 100 16.82 20.22

3
3
0
0
0

15
15
100
85
100

.89
.80

20 to 100 68.93 10.81

36

97

.89

10 to 50

7.13

17

50

.89

7 to 35 23.33 5.17
12 to 60 44.57 8.41
0 to 100 43.01 29.20

10
20
0

35
60
98

.84
.89

0 to 100 36.69 24.20

92

39.39

overall multivariate effect for major on the dependent variables was


not significant, F(11, 156) = .265, p > .05. Therefore, in this sample,
major was unrelated to both the independent variable of interest and
to all of the dependent variables.
In a similar way, measures of body fat (body mass index [BMI])
were examined for their relationships to body image, mood, and
appearance self-schema. There is a common perception that body fat
is tied to body image such that smaller women have more positive
body images than do larger women. Research results are mixed on the
subject. Research with a variety of populations and age groups demonstrates that BMI or weight is sometimes negatively related to body
image in the United States (Faith, Leone, & Allison, 1997; McCabe &
Ricciardelli, 2001), particularly among women (Cash & Hicks, 1990).
However, losing weight does not necessarily improve body image
(Cash, Counts, & Huffine, 1990), and sometimes body image
improves without a change in weight (Rosen, 1996). Because of these
mixed results, it is possible that BMI could explain relationships
among the variables of interest in this research. Based on a regression

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

39

analysis with BMI as the independent variable and scores on appearance self-schema as the dependent variable, these two variables were
found to be unrelated, F(1, 166) = .001, p > .05. The effects of BMI on the
dependent variables in this study were assessed using multivariate
analysis of variance using the standardized categorization scheme of
BMI: A BMI of less than 20 as underweight (N = 49; 29%), a BMI
between 20 and 25 as normal weight (N = 105; 63%), a BMI of more
than 25 as overweight (N = 14; 8%). The overall effect of BMI on the
dependent variables was not significant, F(11, 156) = 1.22, p > .05.
Thus, results from preliminary analyses revealed that significant
effects on the variables of interest in this study can be attributed neither to students choice of major nor to their BMI.
Multivariate Analysis of Variance

Research hypotheses were tested by a 2 (appearance self-schema)


2 (exposure condition) between-subjects multivariate analysis of variance. Appearance self-schema (appearance schematic versus appearance aschematic) and exposure condition (exposure to attractive
media images versus no exposure) served as independent variables.
Based on a median split of scores, 83 respondents were classified as
schematic on appearance and 85 were classified as aschematic on
appearance. In all, 38 participants were classified as appearance schematic with exposure, 45 participants were classified as appearance
schematic with no exposure, 50 participants were classified as
appearance aschematic with exposure, and 35 participants were identified as appearance aschematic with no exposure. Thus, 88 participants received the experimental condition, and 80 received the control condition. Dependent variables were responses to the measures
of body image (VAS-body dissatisfaction, VAS-overall appearance
dissatisfaction, Appearance Orientation, and Appearance Evaluation), self-esteem scales (global self-esteem and social self-esteem),
and the measures of mood (Joy scale, Distress scale, VAS-depression,
VAS-anger, and VAS-anxiety). Variable means and standard deviations as well as their minimum and maximum scores are presented in
Table 1.
MANOVA revealed that neither the overall interaction between
appearance self-schema and exposure nor the overall effect for exposure was significant. Contrary to predictions, appearance self-schema
did not mediate effects of exposure to attractive media images on
body image, self-esteem, and mood. In addition, no significant

40

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

differences were found between women who are exposed to and


those not exposed to attractive media images in terms of their body
image, self-esteem, and mood scores. Thus, neither Hypothesis 3 nor
Hypothesis 4 was supported.
However, the MANOVA detected significant effects for appearance self-schema on the dependent variables, F(11, 156) = 9.68, p <
.0001. To assess the cause of the significant overall effect, univariate
analyses were performed for appearance self-schema on the dependent variables.
Univariate Analyses of Variance

Separate univariate analyses of variance for each of the dependent


variables revealed that the effect for each of the dependent variables
as a function of appearance self-schema significantly contributed to
the multivariate main effect for appearance self-schema (ps < .01; see
Table 2). Results revealed that women who subscribed to schematic
beliefs about the importance, meaning, and effects of appearance in
their lives received lower scores on social self-esteem, global selfesteem, and appearance evaluation than did those who did not hold
such beliefs (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). Similarly, the former group exhibited higher scores on body dissatisfaction, overall appearance dissatisfaction, and appearance orientation than did the latter group. As expected, women who are schematic
on appearance had lower self-esteem and body image than did those
who are aschematic on appearance. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was
supported.
It was also predicted that women who are schematic on appearance would exhibit greater negative mood and lower positive mood
than would those who are aschematic on appearance. According to
the analyses of variance, participants who held schematic beliefs
regarding the importance, meaning, and effects of appearance in their
lives expressed significantly greater mood distress, depression, anxiety, anger, and lower scores in positive mood (joy) than did those who
did not hold such beliefs (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). Thus, Hypothesis 2 was also supported.
Multiple Regression Analyses

Multiple regression analyses were used to examine relationships


among appearance self-schema, body image, self-esteem, and mood.

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

TABLE 2:

41

Means and Standard Deviations for Each of the Dependent Variables


as a Function of Appearance Self-Schema

Dependent Variable
Joy
Distress
Anxiety
Depression
Anger
Body dissatisfaction
Overall appearance
dissatisfaction
Social self-esteem
Global self-esteem
Appearance evaluation
Appearance orientation

Low
Appearance
Schema
(N = 85)

High
Appearance
Schema
(N = 83)

F(1, 166)

8.99
5.01
38.34
16.76
12.76
29.16

(2.97)
(2.15)
(29.14)
(19.47)
(16.92)
(24.37)

7.77
6.49
50.66
34.63
20.96
57.19

(2.97)
(2.78)
(27.09)
(22.21)
(22.46)
(26.95)

7.66**
13.92***
7.90**
29.29***
6.98**
50.76***

25.93
72.98
42.80
25.11
41.18

(20.53)
(9.23)
(5.43)
(4.68)
(8.84)

47.71 (22.76)
64.78 (10.80)
35.90 (7.02)
21.51 (5.04)
48.04 (6.31)

41.16***
28.18***
52.37***
25.59***
31.05***

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

For instance, is attention to appearance information directly related to


mood and body image outcomes, or does self-esteem mediate the
relationship between appearance self-schema and mood and
between appearance self-schema and body image? To assess possible
relationships between appearance self-schema and measures of body
image and mood mediated by self-esteem, nine multiple regression
analyses were conducted, one for each dependent measure of body
image (i.e., appearance evaluation, appearance orientation, body dissatisfaction, and overall appearance dissatisfaction) and for each
dependent measure of mood (joy, distress, anxiety, depression, and
anger). Independent variables were appearance self-schema and both
social and global self-esteem.
Appearance self-schema accounted for variance in all measures of
body image except appearance evaluation (see Table 3 for regression
statistics). Although there was a significant correlation between social
and global self-esteem, r(168) = .73, p < .01, more body image variables
were accounted for by global self-esteem (i.e., appearance evaluation,
body dissatisfaction, and overall appearance dissatisfaction) than by
social self-esteem (i.e., appearance evaluation and overall appearance
dissatisfaction) (see partial R2s to estimate the percentage of variance
accounted for by each of the three independent variables). Both

42

TABLE 3:

Multiple Regressions for Appearance Self-Schema and Social and Global Self-Esteem on Measures of Body Image and
Mood

Dependent Variable
Body image
AE

df

R2

Independent Variable
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE
ASS
SSE
GSE

3,164

28.49***

.59

.34

AO

3,164

22.54***

.54

.29

BD

3,164

20.87***

.53

.28

OAD

3,164

25.89***

.57

.32

3,164

10.25***

.40

.16

Distress

3,164

13.95***

.45

.20

Anxiety

3,164

3.56*

.25

.06

Depression

3,164

17.47***

.49

.24

Anger

3,164

4.59**

.28

.08

Mood
Joy

Beta

Partial R

Partial R2

.04
.16
.18
.61
.02
.12
1.10
.15
1.04
.57
.44
.94

.05
.34
.25
.58
.02
.10
.30
.06
.25
.19
.20
.28

.71
3.65***
2.63**
7.34***
.20
1.02
3.77***
.58
2.51*
2.42*
2.08*
2.83*

.06
.27
.20
.50
.02
.08
.28
.05
.19
.19
.16
.22

.0036
.0729
.04
.25
.0004
.0064
.0784
.0025
.0361
.0361
.0256
.0484

.01
.06
.08
.03
.06
.07
.74
.31
.22
.82
.62
.05
.65
.15
.13

.03
.22
.19
.10
.23
.19
.21
.12
.06
.29
.29
.02
.26
.08
.05

.29
2.10*
1.74
1.23
2.23*
1.75
2.26*
1.03
.48
3.52**
2.92**
.14
2.85**
.74
.40

.02
.16
.14
.10
.17
.14
.17
.08
.04
.27
.22
.01
.22
.06
.03

.0004
.0256
.0196
.01
.0289
.0196
.0289
.0064
.0016
.0729
.0484
.0001
.0484
.0036
.0009

NOTE: AE = appearance evaluation; AO = appearance orientation; BD = body dissatisfaction; OAD = overall appearance dissatisfaction; ASS =
appearance self-schema; SSE = social self-esteem, GSE = global self-esteem.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

43

appearance self-schema and social self-esteem accounted for variance


in three out of five measures of mood (i.e., anxiety, depression, and
anger by appearance self-schema and joy, distress, and depression by
social self-esteem). Global self-esteem did not explain any variance in
the mood variables.
Pearson correlations revealed that appearance self-schema was
significantly related to both social, r(168) = .50, p < .01, and global
self-esteem, r(168) = .53, p < .01, in the negative direction. Higher
scores on appearance self-schema were associated with lower scores
on social as well as global self-esteem. Although correlated, the three
measures did not always predict body image and mood (see Table 3).
Therefore, appearance self-schema is a significant individual variable
but is not entirely responsible for explaining participants responses
on measures of body image and mood. Participants self-esteem is
also a mediating factor on measures of body image and mood.
DISCUSSION

The purpose of this research was to examine the effects of womens


appearance self-schemas and exposure to media images on their body
images, self-esteem, and mood. First, it was hypothesized that
women who are schematic on appearance would exhibit lower selfesteem and body image scores than would those who are aschematic
on appearance. The self-esteem scores of women schematic on
appearance were lower than were the self-esteem scores of women
aschematic on appearance for both social and global self-esteem.
Analyses also revealed significant main effects for appearance selfschema on measures of body image. In each case, women schematic
on appearance exhibited lower appearance evaluation scores, higher
appearance orientation scores, higher body dissatisfaction scores,
and higher overall appearance dissatisfaction scores than did those
aschematic on appearance. Body image is a multidimensional construct for which four different measures were used (appearance evaluation, appearance orientation, body dissatisfaction, and overall
appearance dissatisfaction). In general, body image is thought to be
negative when individuals indicate dissatisfaction with their
appearances and bodies and when they evaluate their appearances
unfavorably. Results indicate that the more participants considered
appearance important, central, and self-relevant in their cognitions
(appearance self-schema), the lower they evaluated their own

44

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

physical appearances (appearance evaluation). Similarly, the more


participants considered appearance important, central, and self-relevant in their cognitions, the less satisfied they were with their bodies
(body dissatisfaction) and overall appearances (overall appearance
dissatisfaction), with much time spent on improving their appearances (appearance orientation). According to Cash (1995), appearance orientation (i.e., the devotion of time, mental energy, and behavioral activity to appearance) amplifies affective body image. Thus, if
people are dissatisfied with their overall appearances and bodies or if
they evaluate their appearances unfavorably, then higher scores in
appearance orientation indicate a more negative body image. This is
precisely how the appearance schematic women scored. Thus,
women who were schematic on appearance had more negative body
images than did women who were aschematic on appearance.
It was also hypothesized that women who are schematic on
appearance would exhibit higher scores on negative mood and lower
scores on positive mood than would those who are aschematic on
appearance. The analyses revealed significant effects for appearance
self-schema on all five measures of mood (joy, distress, depression,
anxiety, and anger). In each case, those who were appearance schematic exhibited higher scores on negative mood (distress, depression,
anxiety, and anger) and lower scores on positive mood (joy) than did
those who were appearance aschematic. Perhaps, young women who
define themselves (in part) in terms of appearance (i.e., self-schematic
on appearance) feel greater pressure to emulate the cultural ideal of
beauty and are more emotionally distressed than those who define
themselves otherwise. As a result, more negative mood responses
were assessed from those who consider appearance an important
domain of self-relevance and central to the self. As mentioned earlier,
poor psychological health such as depression is associated with a
poor body image, and thus, women who are schematic on appearance
may be more likely to develop and maintain negative body images
than are those who are aschematic on appearance.
The third hypothesis suggested that women exposed to attractive
media images would exhibit lower body image scores, lower selfesteem scores, and greater negative mood scores than would those
not exposed to media images. However, findings revealed no statistically significant effects for exposure to media images on body image,
self-esteem, and mood. Presumably, participants did not compare
themselves to media images (as they evaluated mood and body
image) or if they did, it had no significant effects on them. Participants

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

45

were asked to assess the formality and fashionability of models


clothes by the filler task given along with the stimulus photos. It is
also possible that the nature of the filler task elicited a search for information from the images rather than comparison with them. In such a
case, exposure to media images may not trigger cognitions about
appearance.
Finally, it was hypothesized that appearance self-schema would
mediate effects of exposure to attractive media images on body
image, self-esteem, and mood scores. The overall interaction effect
between appearance self-schema and exposure condition on measures of mood, body image, and self-esteem was not significant. Thus,
women who are schematic on appearance and were exposed to media
images did not exhibit the most negative body image, self-esteem,
and mood scores. Nevertheless, appearance self-schema has proven
to be a critical individual difference variable moderating the relationship between variables such as body image, self-esteem, and
mood.
Multiple regression analyses revealed that self-esteem might be an
important variable mediating the relationships between appearance
self-schema and body image as well as between appearance selfschema and mood. Self-esteem accounted for variance in most of the
measures of body image (more so by global self-esteem than by social
self-esteem) and mood (by social self-esteem only) when the effects of
appearance self-schema were controlled. However, further studies
are necessary to clarify the relationships among appearance selfschema, body image, self-esteem, and mood.
CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS

In conclusion, appearance self-schema was a significant factor in


assessments of womens body image, self-esteem, and mood. According to this study, women who placed high cognitive importance on
appearance exhibited lower body image, lower self-esteem, and more
negative mood than did those who placed low cognitive importance
on appearance. Although this study examined whether appearance
self-schema alone or appearance self-schema in conjunction with
exposure to media images is responsible for the negative effects on
women, it may be a moot point in that people are daily bombarded
with a multitude of idealized images in the media. Exposing individuals briefly to a relatively small number of media images and

46

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

monitoring any changes in body image satisfaction or dissatisfaction


and mood may be an unreasonable approach in revealing the medias
effect, which takes place over time and is influenced by individual
variables such as appearance self-schema as shown in this study.
Given that this is the case, it seems reasonable to suspect methodological limitations in designing the exposure procedure employed in this
study. Future researchers may find it necessary to use a more intense
presentation of images that has external validity. As another limitation of this study, we were not able to measure ethnic differences on
body image and self-esteem (e.g., higher levels of self-esteem and a
more positive body image being reported by African American
women than by Caucasian women; Molloy & Herzberger, 1998) upon
their exposure to attractive images. Because only Caucasian models
were used in this study, it is possible that women of color would have
been less distressed by the images featuring ideals for Caucasian
women. Studies in the future should work to refine the current experiment to compare ethnic groups for the effect of media images on their
body images and self-esteem.
Some may consider appearance to be an important dimension of
self-evaluation, whereas others may place cognitive emphasis on
other dimensions (e.g., sports or politics). Focusing on other schematic domains rather than appearance has been suggested as a coping strategy to overcome negative effects of appearance (Cash, 1995).
To put it more simply and in the words of Cash (1995), Negative
body image has little to do with outward appearance; its a state of
mind (p. 27). Because appearance self-schema was significantly
related to measures of body image, self-esteem, and mood in this
study, appearance self-schema is demonstrated to be an important
individual difference variable moderating the extent of individuals
satisfaction with their bodies and selves in general.
Studies found that attractiveness in the media sells products
(Chaiken & Eagly, 1983; Kamins, 1990); however, attractive images
seen may also reinforce viewers unrealistic expectations for how they
should look, especially for young children. Fortunately, these results
suggest that not everyone is affected by cultural pressure to approximate ideal female beauty, and efforts to counteract the effects of unrealistic images may be successful. For example, the need to educate
children about media images is important because they are exposed
to media images from an early age. Children may not realize that
media images are artificially manipulated by makeup or computer
graphics. To ground expectations in reality, children should be

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

47

educated about unrealistic media images through class discussions


and other activities. They should also be encouraged to develop nonappearance-related self-defining cognitive areas, such as athletic
prowess, intellectual achievement, or musical ability. For example,
female high school students who participated in a media literacy program teaching skills for challenging the media have reported less
internalization of the thin beauty standard and lower perceived realism of media images (Irving, DuPen, & Berel, 1998). Educators, particularly family and consumer science educators, should help students
to develop and maintain positive physical and mental health, including body image. Results of this study suggest that when young
women define themselves in terms of their aesthetic value, they may
experience more negative mood and lower self-esteem and may be
dissatisfied with their appearances. Therefore, teaching children to
value themselves and others for their unique capabilities (e.g., goalie,
band member, or community volunteer) and attributes (e.g., perseverance, thoughtfulness, or friendliness) might be a useful strategy to
encourage the development of self-schemas with a nonappearance
focus.
REFERENCES
Akan, G. E., & Grilo, C. M. (1994). Sociocultural influences on eating attitudes and
behaviors, body image, and psychological functioning: A comparison of AfricanAmerican, Asian-American, and Caucasian college women. International Journal of
Eating Disorders, 18, 181-187.
Brownell, K. D. (1991). Personal responsibility and control over our bodies: When
expectation exceeds reality. Health Psychology, 10, 303-310.
Button, E., Loan, P., Davies, J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (1997). Self-esteem, eating problems,
and psychological well-being in a cohort of schoolgirls aged 15-16: A questionnaire
and interview study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 21, 39-47.
Campbell, J. D., Chew, B., & Scratchley, L. S. (1991). Cognitive and emotional reactions
to daily events: The effects of self-esteem and self-complexity. Journal of Personality,
59, 473-505.
Cash, T. F. (1992). Initial development of the Appearance Schemas Inventory. Unpublished
research, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia.
Cash, T. F. (1995). What do you see when you look in the mirror? Helping yourself to a positive
body image. New York: Bantam Books.
Cash, T. F., Counts, B., & Huffine, C. E. (1990). Current and vestigial effects of overweight among women: Fear of fat, attitudinal body image, and eating behaviors.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 157-167.
Cash, T. F., & Henry, P. (1995). Womens body images: The results of a national survey in
the U.S.A. Sex Roles, 33, 19-28.

48

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

Cash, T. F., & Hicks, K. L. (1990). Being fat versus thinking fat: Relationships with
body image, eating behaviors, and well-being. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14,
327-341.
Cash, T. F., & Labarge, A. S. (1996). Development of the Appearance Schemas Inventory: A new cognitive body-image assessment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20,
37-50.
Cash, T. F., & Szymanski, M. L. (1995). The development and validation of the BodyImage Ideals Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 466-477.
Chaiken, S., & Eagly, A. (1983). Communication modality as a determinant of persuasion: The role of communicator salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
45, 241-256.
Cusumano, D., & Thompson, J. K. (1997). Body image and body shape ideals in magazines: Exposure, awareness, and internalization. Sex Roles, 37, 701-721.
Drogosz, L. M., & Levy, P. E. (1996). Another look at the effects of appearance, gender,
and job type on performance-based decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20,
437-445.
Eagly, A. (1967). Involvement as a determinant of response to favorable information.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 1-15.
Fabian, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1989). Body image and eating disturbance in young
females. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 63-74.
Faith, M. S., Leone, M. A., & Allison, D. B. (1997). The effects of self-generated comparison targets, BMI, and social comparison tendencies on body image appraisal. Eating
Disorders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 5, 128-140.
Fallon, A. (1990). Culture in the mirror: Sociocultural determinants of body image. In
T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change
(pp. 80-109). New York: Guilford.
Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1981). Body image in anorexia nervosa: Measurement,
theory and clinical implications. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 11,
263-284.
Garner, D. M., & Kearney-Cooke, A. (1996, March/April). Psychology Today, pp. 55-56.
Gupta, M. A., & Schork, N. J. (1993). Aging-related concerns and body image: Possible
future implications for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14,
481-486.
Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1992). The effects of figure size feedback (positive vs.
negative) and target comparison group (particularistic vs. universalistic) on body
image disturbance. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 12, 441-448.
Heinberg, L. J., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Body image and televised images of thinness
and attractiveness: A controlled laboratory investigation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14, 325-338.
Hesse-Biber, S. (1996). Am I thin enough yet? The cult of thinness and the commercialization
of identity. New York: Oxford University Press.
Higgins, E. T., King, G., & Mavin, G. (1982). Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 35-47.
Irving, L., DuPen, J., & Berel, S. (1998). A media literacy program for high school
females. Eating Disorders, 6, 119-132.
Izard, C. E. (1972). Patterns of emotions. New York: Academic Press.
Jackson, L., Sullivan, L., & Rostker, R. (1988). Gender, gender role, and body image. Sex
Roles, 19, 429-443.

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

49

Janis, I. L., & Field, P. B. (1959). Sex differences and personality factors related to
persuasibility. In C. I. Hovland, I. L. Janis, P. B. Field, H. Linton, E. Graham, & A. R.
Cohen, et al. (Eds.), Personality and persuasibility (pp. 55-68). New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Kamins, M. A. (1990). An investigation into the match-up hypothesis in celebrity
advertising: When beauty may be only skin deep. Journal of Advertising, 19, 4-13.
Keeton, W. P., Cash, T. F., & Brown, T. A. (1990). Body image or body images?: Comparative, multidimensional assessment among college students. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 54, 213-230.
Koenig, L. J., & Wasserman, E. L. (1995). Body image and dieting failure in college men
and women: Examining links between depression and eating problems. Sex Roles,
32, 225-249.
Lee, A. M., & Lee, S. (1996). Disordered eating and its psychosocial correlates among
Chinese adolescent females in Hong Kong. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
20, 177-183.
Lennon, S., Rudd, N., Sloan, B., & Kim, J. (1999). Attitudes toward gender roles, selfesteem, and body image: Application of a model. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 17, 191-202.
Lerner, R. M., Orlos, J., & Knapp, J. (1976). Physical attractiveness, physical effectiveness, and self-concept in late adolescents. Adolescence, 11, 313-325.
Levine, M., Smolak, L., & Hayden, H. (1994). The relation of sociocultural factors to eating attitudes and behaviors among middle school girls. Journal of Early Adolescence,
14, 471-490.
Locher, P., Unger, R., Sociedade, P., & Wahl, J. (1993). At first glance: Assessibility of the
physical attractiveness stereotype. Sex Roles, 28, 729-743.
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.
Markus, H., Hamill, R., & Sentis, K. P. (1987). Thinking fat: Self-schemas for body
weight and the processing of weight relevant information. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 17, 50-71.
Markus, H., Smith, J., & Moreland, R. (1985). Role of the self-concept in the perception
of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1494-1512.
Martin, M., & Kennedy, P. (1993). Advertising and social comparison: Consequences for
female preadolescents and adolescents. Psychology & Marketing, 10, 513-530.
Mazur, A. (1986). U.S. trends in feminine beauty and overadaptation. Journal of Sex
Research, 22, 281-303.
McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2001). Parent, peer, and media influences on body
image and strategies to both increase and decrease body size among adolescent
boys and girls. Adolescence, 36(142), 225-240.
Mellin, L., Irwin, C., & Scully, S. (1992). Prevalence of disordered eating in girls: A
survey of middle-class children. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 92, 851853.
Molloy, B., & Herzberger, S. (1998). Body image and self-esteem: A comparison of African-American and Caucasian women. Sex Roles, 38, 631-643.
Mori, D., & Morey, L. (1991). The vulnerable body image of females with feelings of
depression. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 343-354.
Peterson, R. T. (1987). Bulimia and anorexia in an advertising context. Journal of Business
Ethics, 6, 495-504.

50

FAMILY AND CONSUMER SCIENCES RESEARCH JOURNAL

Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. L. (1990). Gender differences in concern with body
weight and physical appearance over the life span. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 16, 263-273.
Polivy, J., Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1986). Causes and consequences of the
current preference for thin female physiques. In C. P. Zanna & E. T. Higgins (Eds.),
Physical appearance, stigma and social behavior: The Third Ontario Symposium in Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 89-112). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Richins, M. L. (1991). Social comparison and the idealized images of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 18, 71-83.
Robinson, J. P., & Shaver, P. R. (1973). Measures of social psychological attitudes (2nd ed.).
Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women and weight: A normative
discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed)., Psychology and gender: Nebraska symposium on
motivation (Vol. 32, pp. 267-307). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Rosen, J. (1990). Body-image disturbances in eating disorders. In T. F. Cash &
T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance and change (pp. 190-214).
New York: Guilford.
Rosen, J. (1996). Improving body image in obesity. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Body image,
eating disorders, and obesity: An integrative guide for assessment and treatment (pp. 425440). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Russo, N. F., Green, B. L., & Knight, G. (1993). The relationship of gender, self-esteem,
and instrumentality to depressive symptomatology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 218-236.
Shaw, J., & Waller, G. (1995). The medias impact on body image: Implications for prevention and treatment. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 3, 115123.
Silber, E., & Tippett, J. (1965). Self-esteem: Clinical assessment and measurement validation. Psychological Reports, 16, 1017-1071.
Silverstein, B., Caceres, J., Perdue, L., & Cimarolli, V. (1995). Gender differences in
depressive symptomatology: The role played by anxious somatic depression
associated with gender-related achievement. Sex Roles, 33, 621-636.
Simmons, R., & Rosenberg, F. (1975). Sex, sex roles, and self-image. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 4, 229-258.
Stein, K. (1996). The self-schema model: A theoretical approach to the self-concept in
eating disorders. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 10, 96-109.
Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (1994). Adverse effects of the media portrayed thin-ideal on
women and linkages to bulimic symptomatology. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 13, 288-308.
Thomas, V. G. (1989). Body-image satisfaction among Black women. Journal of Social
Psychology, 129, 107-112.
Thompson, J. K., & Psaltis, K. (1988). Multiple aspects and correlates of body figure
ratings: A replication and extension of Fallon and Rozin (1985). International Journal
of Eating Disorders, 7, 813-817.
Thornton, B., & Moore, S. (1993). Physical attractiveness contrast effect: Implications
for self-esteem and evaluations of the social self. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 19, 474-480.

Jung, Lennon / BODY IMAGE AND MEDIA IMAGES

51

Timko, C., Striegel-Moore, R. H., Silberstein, L. R., & Rodin, J. (1987). Femininity/
masculinity and disordered eating in women: How are they related? International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 6, 701-712.
Vitousek, K., & Hollon, S. (1990). The investigation of schematic content and processing
in eating disorders. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 14, 191-214.

S-ar putea să vă placă și