Sunteți pe pagina 1din 151

Flexibility in Early Verb Use: Evidence from a Multiple-N Diary Study

Author(s): Letitia R. Naigles, Erika Hoff, Donna Vear, Michael Tomasello, Silke Brandt, Sandra R.
Waxman, Jane B. Childers and W. Andrew Collins
Source: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 74, No. 2,
Flexibility in Early Verb Use: Evidence from a Multiple-N Diary Study (2009), pp. i-v, vii, 1-144
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/25580860
Accessed: 10-06-2015 02:47 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

FLEXIBILITYIN EARLYVERB USE: EVIDENCE


FROMA MULTIPLE-N DIARY STUDY

Letitia R. Naigles, Erika Hoff, and Donna Vear

WITH COMMENTARY BY
Michael Tomasello,
Silke Brandt,
Sandra

R. Waxman,

and Jane B. Childers

W. Andrew Collins
Series Editor

MONOGRAPHS
OF THESOCIETY
FORRESEARCH
INCHILD
DEVELOPMENT
Serial No. 293, Vol. 74, No. 2, 2009

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

(^PjWILEY
W
J PL/l%Jv

ELL

Boston,Massachusetts

Oxford,UnitedKingdom

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

EDITOR
W. ANDREW COLLINS
University

ofMinnesota

MANAGING EDITOR
DETRA
DAVIS
Society for Research

in Child Development

EDITORIALASSISTANT
ANTONELLA
CAIAZZA
Society for Research

in Child Development

Board of Advisory

Editors

Brian K. Barber
University
Michael
University
Glenn
University
Kathleen
University

of Tennessee
P.Maratsos
ofMinnesota
I. Roisman
of Illinois
Thomas
ofMinnesota

Manfred Van Dulmen


Kent

State University

Thomas Weisner
University

of California,

Los Angeles

PhilipDavid Zelazo
University

of Toronto

EDITORIALCONSULTANTS
Emma

Jane Childers

K. Adam

Northwestern

University

Trinity University

Mark Appelbaum
University

of California,

San Diego

Gregory Cook
University

of Wisconsin-Whitewater

University

of Wisconsin-Whitewater

RichardAslin
of Rochester

University
Marian

Bakermans-Kranenburg
Leiden University
John Bates
Indiana

University

Nazli Baydar
Koc University
Theodore
University

Beauchaine
of Washington

Susan
Utrecht

Branje
University

Celia Brownell
University

of Pittsburgh

Melissa M. Burch
Hampshire

College

Susan Campbell
University
Stephanie
University

of Pittsburgh
Carlson
ofMinnesota

Joan Cook

Susan

Crockenberg
of Vermont

University
Pamela
University

Davis-Kean
ofMichigan
Eccles

Jacquelynne
University

ofMichigan

James Elicker
Purdue

University

Michelle M. Englund
University

ofMinnesota

Kurt Fischer
Harvard
Doran

University
French

Illinois Wesleyan
Sarah

University

Friedman

CNA Corporation

Douglas Frye
University

of Pennsylvania

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Andrew Fuligni
University

of California,

Los Angeles

Darcia

of Calgary

Nelson

City University

of New

University

Megan

ofMinnesota

University

Susan

University

Robert Pianta
University

University

Mark

Lene Jensen

Ariel Kalil
University

of Chicago

Melissa Koenig
University

ofMinnesota

University

Karl Rosengren
of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

JudithG. Smetana

Atlantic

University

Eva Lefkowitz
Pennsylvania
Katherine
University

State University
Magnuson
Madison

of Wisconsin,
Ann Masten

University

ofMinnesota

KevinMiller
University
Ginger
Pennsylvania

ofMichigan
Moore
State University

of Rochester

University

Kathy Stansbury
Morehouse
Steve

Brett Laursen
Florida

of Virginia
Roosa

State University

Arizona

of Texas, Austin

Clark University

Santa Barbara

of California,

Aletha Huston
University

of Rochester

YukariOkamoto

Hespos

Vanderbilt

Davis

O'Connor

University

Paul Harris
Harvard

of California,

Thomas

Gunnar

University

York

LisaOakes

ElenaGrigorenko
Yale University

Dame

Katherine

Susan Graham
University

Narvaez
of Notre

University

University

College
Thoma
of Alabama

Michael Tomasello
Max

Planck

Institute

Deborah Vandell
University

of California,

Irvine

RichardWeinberg
University
Hirokazu
New

ofMinnesota
Yoshikawa

York University

Qing Zhou
Arizona

State University

David Moshman
University

of Nebraska

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

FLEXIBILITYIN EARLYVERB USE:


EVIDENCEFROMA MULTIPLE-N
DIARY STUDY
CONTENTS
ABSTRACT

vii

I. INTRODUCTION

II.PRESENTING
THEDIARY
METHOD

22

A GENERALDESCRIPTION
OF EARLY
III.
VERBGROWTHAND USE

32

IV.PRAGMATIC
AND SEMANTICFLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE

40

VERBUSE
AND GRAMMATICAL
FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
V PRODUCTIVITY

49

VERBGROWTHAND USEAS A FUNCTION


VI. DIFFERENCESINEARLY
OF
DEVELOPMENTAL
PERIOD,CHILD,AND VERB

68

VII.GENERALDISCUSSION

91

REFERENCES 105
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 112

COMMENTARY
FLEXIBILITY
INTHESEMANTICS
AND SYNTAX
OF CHILDREN'SEARLY
VERBUSE
Michael Tomasello and Silke Brandt

113

LEARNINGFROM INFANTS'FIRST
VERBS
Sandra

R. Waxman

VERBLEARNERS:
EARLY
CREATIVE
OR NOT?
Jane B. Childers

127

133

CONTRIBUTORS 140
STATEMENT
OF EDITORIAL
POLICY 142

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

ABSTRACT

are hallmarks
of human
and productivity
language use.
Flexibility
use
to serve a
to
have
the
the
words
know
speakers
Competent
capacity
they
to
new
to
of
refer
communicative
and
varied
functions,
variety
exemplars of
to which words refer, and in new and varied combinations
the categories
with other words. When
and how children achieve
this flexibility?and
when

they

are

truly

productive

language

users?are

central

issues

among

accounts

of language acquisition. The current study tests competing


hy
of
of flexibility
and some kinds of productivity
the achievement
potheses
verbs.
against data on children's first uses of their first-acquired
uses
mothers
recorded
their
children's
first
10
of 34 early
Eight
were
if
those
verbs
of the
within
the
window
verbs,
acquired
produced
were
The
16
children
between
and
months
when
the
20
study.
study began
on when the children started to
(depending
produce verbs), were followed
3 and 12 months,
for between
and produced
between
13 and 31 of the
a
verbs.
These
records
the
basis
for
of the
target
diary
provided
description
pragmatic,

semantic,

and

syntactic

properties

of

early

verb

use.

The data revealed that within this early, initial period of verb use, chil
dren use their verbs both to command and describe,
they use their verbs in
reference

to a

variety

of

appropriate

actions

enacted

by

variety

of actors

and

a variety of affected objects, and they use their verbs in a


variety of
semantic and grammatical
syntactic structures. All 8 children displayed
flexibility before 24 months of age. These findings are more consistent with
a model
of the language-learning
child as an avid generalizer
than as a
user.
conservative
Children's
language
early verb use suggests abilities and
to abstract from experience
inclinations
that may indeed begin in infancy.

with

vii

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

I. INTRODUCTION

list of words and


in the adult is not a memorized
knowledge
Language
sentences paired with the situations in which they have been experienced.
in their vocabularies
novel set
Adult speakers use the words
flexibly?in
to newly encountered
that
instances of the categories
tings and in reference
to form grammatical
can combine
their words
their words
label. They
sentences
in the study of
that they have never heard. A central question
context
is
the
when
children
achieve
independence,
language acquisition
of form that are the hallmarks of
and productivity
of meaning,
extendability
human

competence.

linguistic

One
to new

is that children

possibility
and

referents

produce

novel

are avid generalizers


sentences

very

who

early

in

extend
the

words

course

of

For example, a child who hears the verb jump in the


language development.
sentence
t jump on the bed would without
Don
further linguistic
imperative
to
use
to
in reference
in
declarative
also
be
able
sentences,
experience
jump
to actions as different
jumping done by other people, and even in reference
or jumping
into the
from the original as jumping up to reach something
use
to
the child would be able
jump in other structures,
pool. Furthermore,
such as I jump, he jumped, and Granny jumped into the pool. An alternative
is that children are initially conservative
learners who
possibility
language
to
close
A
the examples of language use they have already experienced.
stay
child who has heard jump only in Don tjump on the bed would not use jump to
refer to a variety of other sorts of jumping, by a variety of different jumpers,
or

in a range

of

syntactic

constructions.

These

two

positions

define

space

of the child as language learner.


that includes multiple possible descriptions
we present new data on children's first uses of their first
In this monograph,
this hypothesis
verbs and ask what theoretical positions within
space are
consistent with that evidence.
The

view that children

in their very
generalize beyond their experience
that children's
first uses of newly
productions
yields the prediction
a range of meanings
verbs should be flexible,
and
acquired
conveying
a
structures.
in
of
The
view that children are initially
appearing
variety
earliest

1
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

conservative
that children's first uses
language learners yields the prediction
of newly acquired verbs should be very repetitious
in terms of the events
to
structures
refer
and
the
in
because
they
they participate
they are used
to
contexts
structures
in
in
similar
those
which
and/or
only
they were first
encountered.

Verbs
development

are the focus of this investigation of the process of early language


for two reasons. First, verb acquisition has been less studied

noun

than

the

Thus,

acquisition.

and

rapid-generalizer

conservative

views of lexical development


have been tested more
frequently
on
nouns
the
of
than
data
and
verbs,
against
acquisition
questions about the
noun
are
to
which
and
verb
similar
processes have
learning
learning
degree
& Boroditsky,
&
been raised (e.g., Gentner
Hirsh-Pasek,
2002; Maguire,
learner

2006; Sandofer

Golinkoff,
use

tence

structure

Thus,
alizer
ment

the

is also

studying
and

8c Smith,
of

development

as

Second,

the development

grammar,

of verb
sen

because

accounts,

by many

in relation to the verb (Bloom, 1993).


is essentially defined
the acquisition of verbs provides a test of the rapid-gener
views

conservative-learner

as well

2000).

lexical

with

to

respect

grammatical

develop

development.

first 10 uses of
Eight mothers
kept detailed records of their children's
first verb
their first verbs over a period ranging from 3 to 13months. Those
uses were coded for the pragmatic,
semantic, and grammatical
flexibility
accounts of the child as language
learner
and competing
they evidenced,
were

evaluated

against

those

In

data.

the

remainder

of

this

introductory

to this study
chapter, we present the theoretical and empirical background
the extendability
of
in some detail, first considering
work that addresses
verb meaning,
the flexibility of verb
second considering work that addresses
the theoretical and empirical background
grammar, and third, considering
relations among verb vocabulary growth,
that yields predictions
regarding
and flexibility of verb grammar.
flexibility of verb meaning,

MEANING
LEARNING
VERB
Theories of Early Word Meanings
a new word used in a single utterance and a
first encounters
or
must
somehow figure out both what the word
she
single situation, and he
to
in that situation and what it can refer to in other situations. Current
refers
of how the child makes
accounts of word learning differ in their descriptions
The

child

that word-to-referent
not

to a

just

One
linguistic

single

mapping
referent

and
but

is that the child enters


that guide
understandings

view

in how,

to an

entire

and

when,

the

child

maps

words

category.

task equipped with


the word-learning
and extension
the mapping
processes.

2
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

Children

for

understand,

that

example,

words

and

refer,

this

understand

words (Golinkoff,
ing guides them to seek a referent for newly encountered
Mervis, Frawley, 8c Parillo, 1995; McShane,
Hirsh-Pasek,
1980). They also
that words refer to kinds of things or events, which allows them
understand
to extend words to new instances of the same kind (Golinkoff et al., 1995;
& Hoff,

Naigles
They

to

competence

syntactic

use

the

word-learning,

the correspondences
of

terpretation

8c Forbes,

2006; Poulin-duBois

the

newly

Children

especially

between

encountered

2006).

For

task.

verb-learning,

to guide

and meaning

syntax
verbs.

also bring

example,

in

a novel

hearing

verb in a structure such as X verbed Y, yields, via syntactic bootstrapping,


the
a
or
that
the
verb
is
causative
verb
hit
rather
than
(like
kicked)
interpretation
a verb like sing or laugh (Gleitman,
8c Swensen,
1990; Naigles
2007).
to the
Other
theories
attribute
less initial linguistic
sophistication
and
child
limited
from
language
learning
predict
generalizations
experi
ence in children's
that
early word use. A child who does not understand
words

refer

may

associate

a new

to what

word

with

the

context

entire

of

its use

(e.g.,

to the refrigerator
door;
"open" applies only
A
child
who
understands
that
refer
words
Dromi,
1982; Mervis,
1987).
only
but not that they refer to kinds will construe meanings
very narrowly. On
some

accounts,

children,

at

one's mother

some

early

does

point,

tend

to

words

map

onto

what

context and to extend words to other things


ismost salient in the concurrent
or events with similar
perceptual
properties
(e.g., "open" may apply to
lateral motions
of doors but not of vertical motions
of doors,
lids, or con
et al., 2006;
8c Golinkoff,
tainers; Hollich,
Hirsh-Pasek,
2000; Maguire
that children eventually use
Smith, 1999, 2000). All theories acknowledge
and syntactic information
in their word learning,
reference,
extendability,
but some models hold that these biases and useable sources of information
are not all available when word
for word mapping
and extension
learning
et
et al., 2000; Maguire
Hollich
et al., 2006). In
al.,
1995;
(Golinkoff
begins
this model,
early verbs (those learned during the 2nd year of life) are likely
to be used in specific contexts or extended
limited ways based on perceptual
to
the
the 3rd year of life can
similarity
original exemplar. Only during
the child use social and linguistic sources of information
for mapping
and
extension
to this Coalition-Emergentist
of verbs. Crucial
is the
model
assertion

that

word-learning

principles

emerge

earlier

for

nouns

than

for

and may not transfer directly from noun learning to verb


learning
et al, 2006).
(Golinkoff et al., 1995; Maguire
These
different models
of word learning make different
predictions
to which children will extend
about the degree
their first verbs to new
contexts of use and to new actions. A model
to the verb
that attributes
verbs

to identify new verbs as verbs


learning child sufficient syntactic knowledge
and sufficient lexical understanding
to map new verbs onto
categories of
actions predicts
that children will use their new verbs in utterances
that
3
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

serve a range of pragmatic functions and will extend the verbs


they acquire
to actions by a variety of actors or agents. A model
that attributes
less lin
to
the
child
less
extension
of
word
guistic knowledge
1-year-old
predicts
use. The emergentist model
et
et
1995;
al.,
al,
(Golinkoff
2006)
Maguire
verb use followed by later flexible verb use. One
predicts initial conservative
uses of
is by looking at children's multiple
way of testing these predictions
their first verbs. Do children first restrict the use of a new verb to a specific
setting, or do they use the verb in a variety of settings?
Evidence

From Studies of Word

Learning

about the extendability


of newly acquired words have more
Questions
on
nouns
with
been
addressed
data
than verbs. Children
be
frequently
tween 12 and 18 months
do sometimes
underextend
nouns,
restricting
their use to a single context
1987; Harris, Barrett, Jones, 8c
(e.g., Dromi,
children's use of nouns in spontaneous
Brookes,
1988). After 18 months,
not
context
is
bound
(Barrett, Harris, 8c Chasin,
1991), and in word
speech
learning
novel

children

experiments
to more

nouns

one

than

older
exemplar,

than
even

18 months
after

only

routinely
one

extend

presentation

& Maguire,
2004; Markman,
1989).
(Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, Hennon,
children might
restrict
If verb use also shows initial underextension,
newly

to

verbs

acquired

refer

to a

actor,

addressee,

single

action

type,

or

a child might use the verb blow only when


object. For example,
or
to blowing out a candle, but not to refer
to
in
Mom
reference
talking
only
to the dog's
to blowing one's nose. A child might know sit with reference

affected

sitting

but

no

one

else's,

or

eat

only

in reference

to

eating

pizza.

restricted by actor or affected


There
is evidence of early verb meaning
uses
of
verbs have been found to
Children's
earliest
conventional
object.
to their own rather than another's
8c
actions
refer primarily
(Childers
8c
8c
1983;
Tomasello,
2006; Huttenlocher,
Smiley
Charney,
Smiley,
Huttenlocher,
1995; but see Tomasello,
1992). Twenty-month-olds
taught
novel

verbs

via

video

were

unsuccessful

in

understanding

the

extension

of

these verbs to similar actions performed


by new actors (Maguire et al., 2006;
8c Forbes, 2002). Diary studies have revealed other kinds of
Poulin-Dubois
verb use, such as cut used solely when using a knife,
initial idiosyncratic
watch used solely when watching
television, push used solely in a game
to a pacifier
in the pool, and cry used solely in reference
of pushing
1995; Smith 8c Sachs, 1990; Tomasello,
1992).
(Braunwald,
the
verb meanings
Evidence of extendable
2nd year of life also
during
to use the same verb (rock and ride) to
exists. Children have been observed
of both objects and people over the 6-month period
refer to the movements
assess
from 13 to 19 months
1995). Laboratory
(Smiley 8c Huttenlocher,
found
ments of children's understanding
of familiar verbs have consistently
4

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

of a wide range of English verbs


demonstrate
comprehension
actors
unfamiliar
Hirsh-Pasek,
(Golinkoff,
Cauley, 8cGordon,
performed
by
et al., 1983; Naigles,
1987; Huttenlocher
1997; Naigles & Hoff, 2006), al
of verbs in comprehen
though other studies have found that extendability
sion is limited to children who had reached
either a certain age or
level. Only children over 19 months
and/or with high vocab
vocabulary
that toddlers

familiar verbs to actions by unfamiliar actors (Forbes


ulary counts extended
8c Poulin-Dubois,
of a verb
1997; Naigles 8cHoff, 2006); further extensions
to both new agents and new manners
of action were found only at 26
months
of age (Forbes 8c Poulin-Dubois,
conditions
also
1997). Exposure
to matter.

appear

were

who

Twenty-one-month-olds

two

taught

tran

novel

sitive verbs in a playroom


and
setting with two different actors?themselves
the researchers?were
able to distinguish
those two verbs when presented
on video with a disembodied
hand as the only visible actor (Naigles, Bavin,
et al. (2005) may have been
8c Smith, 2005), and the children
in Naigles
more accepting of yet another actor at test than children
in other studies
because the verbs had been taught with at least two actors.
Questions

to Be Addressed

Regarding Children's Acquisition

of Verb Meanings

The previous literature suggests some early limits on the extendability


of
verb meanings
and some increases in extendability as a function of age or level
of language development
achieved. There are suggestions
in the data from
in
to
Forbes and Poulin-Dubois
(1997) that,
comprehension,
extendability
new

agents
outcomes.
questions

is an
The

earlier

achievement

than

literature

about

leaves

existing
the
extendability

1. How

in the

early

of

process

unanswered,

verbs

as

of

learning

early in the course of learning


emerge?

current

The

study

to new

extendability

they

manners

however,
enter

children's

in

verbs

and

several

basic

lexicons:

how

and/or

general

individual verbs, does extendability


was

to

designed

track

children's

extendability of verbs from the beginning of verb use in the child


and from its first use for individual verbs to address this question.
2. Are
ments

some kinds of extendability


than

others?

tients, affected
(e.g.,

eating

cereal

If children

objects,
and

earlier developmental
use

or different
eating

a carrot;

their

verbs

with

but still appropriate


opening

a door

achieve

different

and

pa

actions
opening

is not closely
jar) itwould indicate that the basis of their meanings
tied to perceptual
et
al., 1995; Goodman,
similarity (Golinkoff
8c Brown, 1998; Maguire et al., 2006).
McDonough,
It is important to acknowledge,
we will not be investigating when

relative to both questions


1 and 2, that
children can extend
their verbs to novel
5

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

actors,

affected

and

contexts,

objects,

we

Because

actions.

do

not

have

access

we will be unable to
to all the verbs uses the children have experienced,
goes beyond the
specify the extent to which their demonstrated
extendability
in relation to
input given. However,
by noting when flexible use emerges
their age and number of uses of a given verb, we will be able to address
early verb use ismore

children's

whether

in the

verbs

among

spect

than their later verb use.

is the source of individual differences,

3. What
data

restricted

seem

studies

few

comprehension,

The

children.

verbs

children

Differences

all

of

are

and

comprehension
factors
with

as candidate

Across

idiosyncratic.

among

extension?

size
vocabulary
differences
among

more

of verb

scope

verb

and

age
suggest
to individual

verbs

of

re

among

the

experimental
understood
reliably

in more detail by
by all children at a given age. As discussed
a
and
Hoff
such
(2006),
Naigles
cross-study inconsistency is symp
tomatic of a deeper issue with respect to early verb acquisition: the
order

in which

children

acquire

verbs

particular

seems

quite

id

iosyncratic, probably because children's input is idiosyncratic in


the particular verbs it illustrates. Diary studies of production
overcome

this

verbs children
verb

uses

also

development
children.
many

problem

by

avoids
that

a wide

casting

learn. Analyses
assuming
can be found

net

of individual
that
by

to capture

children's

whatever

developing

course
is a single
data
averaged
studying
there

of verb
across

LEARNING
VERB
GRAMMAR
Theories of Early Verb Grammar
verbs first appear in children's speech, they frequently appear in
these
utterances. The nature of the representations
that underlie
multiword
a
with
of
conflict
words
is
first combinations
other
of verbs
among
point
accounts of verb development
and is also a central issue with respect to the
broader question of how children achieve grammar. The view that has been
reflects the
in linguistics for decades
is that word combination
standard
When

of

operation

productive

grammar

that

operates

over

abstract

categories,

or meaning
which
1975, 1981,
(Chomsky,
independent
their verbs because
combine
1995; Crain 8c Lillo-Martin,
1999). Children
of items in the category
they know something about the syntactic properties
in
VERB
subjects, that some verbs
English
require
(e.g., that all verbs
that
many verbs appear with the
require objects while others prohibit them,
to
the verbs in
and
indicate
they recognize
activity)
ongoing
"-ing" suffix
to that category.
their lexicons as belonging
are

of function

6
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

An alternative view, consistent with the child as a conservative


learner
in production,
who stays close to experience
is that children's combinations
of verbs with other lexical items reflect the operation
of lexically specific
to this view, there is no abstract category VERB to which
rules. According
individual verbs belong
1992, 2000, 2006). A child who has
(Tomasello,
that one verb can appear with a subject, direct object, "-ing"
discovered
this
suffix, and/or prepositional
phrase thus will have no basis for extending
same
to
at
other
verbs
the
in
time
2004;
Pine,
discovery
point
(MacWhinney,
8c Rowland,
Lieven,
1998; Theakston,
Lieven, Pine, & Rowland,
2001).
Most of these latter theorists agree
that children
attain an
eventually
abstract

grammar,

accounts

concerning

these theories
combinations

the

still disagree

they may

although

exact

or

nature

level

of

with

formal

the

abstraction.

generative
Thus,

need to include an account of how children's verbs and verb


are transformed
from lexically based to abstract.

Evidence From the Study of Verb Learning


Evidence for Early Abstract Grammar
The claim has been made from several quarters
that structural regu
are evidence
larities in children's earliest combinations
that children have
abstract grammatical
the
(Borer 8c
categories,
including
category VERB
8c
Fisher
Gleitman
&
Wexler,
1987;
2002;
Gleitman,
1995; Pinker,
Newport,
from comprehension
studies
1984; Valian,
1990). Two kinds of evidence
also

argue

that

young

children

know

more

about

verb

syntax

than

just

the

combinatorial
of individual verbs. First, 21-28-month-old
chil
possibilities
dren who have been taught a novel verb in one syntactic frame show they
can distinguish
that verb from another when both are presented
in a new
et
a
frame
children
know
al.,
Second,
2005).
syntactic
great deal
(Naigles
about sentence frames, independent
of any verb they contain, which also
as properties
of specific verbs.
suggests that frames are not just represented
For example,
children
understand
the
1-year-old
subject-verb-object
sentences when given noun-verb
(SVO) word order of simple English
noun
(NVN) sequences with novel verbs (Gertner, Fisher, 8c Eisengart,
2006). That is, they infer the first noun of an NVN
sequence
(e.g., "The
is the agent of a novel action presented
duck gorps the bunny")
in a video
clip.

Moreover,

2-year-olds

map

novel

verbs

in NVN

sentences

onto

caus

ative actions (in which the duck is doing something


to the bunny), and they
in NNV
sentences
map novel verbs presented
(e.g., "The duck and the
are
onto
noncausative
actions
the duck and
(in which
bunny
gorping")
are
same
as
the
side
action,
bunny
doing
independent
by side)?again,
scenes
to
at
evidenced
the
look
when
sentences
such
with
by
they prefer
7
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

to them
verbs are presented
1996;
1990).
Naigles,
Naigles,
novel

Further

evidence

that

(Fisher,

1996; Hirsh-Pasek,

represent

2-year-olds

with

meanings

not just with lexical items comes from studies in which children
act out sentences
in which frame meaning
and verb meaning
a sentence

given

as

such

*the

zebra

goes

the

lion,

infer

2-year-olds

&

Golinkoff,
frames

and

are asked
conflict.

to
So,

a causative

to the lion,
from the NVN frame and have the zebra do something
meaning
but given a sentence such as *the zebra brings), children infer a noncausative
of bring) and have the zebra do
meaning
(despite the causative meaning
without
affected
8c Gleitman,
any
something
object (Naigles, Gleitman,
et
al.
also
found
that
and
2128-month-olds
1993). Finally, Naigles
(2005)
could recognize novel verbs in unattested
frames; that is, verbs taught in the
in an intransitive
transitive frame were reliably recognized when presented
(see also Fernandes, Marcus, Di Nubila, & Vouloumanos,
2006).
These
of abstract-level
findings, taken together, support the existence
transitive and intransitive frames in the linguistic knowledge
bases of 1- and
learners.
do
indicate
the range of
not, however,
2-year-old
English
They
or
are
how
how
that
much
toddlers
(i.e.,
flexibility
early)
capable of, nor do
us
muster
of
will
in their own
tell
about
the
level
that
children
they
flexibility
frame

And

is
yet production matters, because production
flexibility
of adult language use in all theories (Chomsky, 1975; H. Clark,
turn to data from
Pinker,
1994; Tomasello,
2000). We therefore

productions.
the hallmark
1996;
children's

of

production

verbs

sentences.

and

Evidence for Early Lexically Specific Grammar


to

contrast

In

the

evidence

from

comprehension

studies

that

young

that allows them to in


possess abstract grammatical
knowledge
of
verbs
the
evidence
from
studies
terpret
production
flexibly,
yields a pic
ture of limited flexibility, which has been taken as support for the view that
is lexically based. Tomasello
(1992) argued for this view on
early grammar
verb uses during her 2nd year
the basis of his diary records of his daughter's
of life. This 1-year-old child tended to use her verbs in only one construc
she might
tion type per verb (i.e., one frame); for example,
say "Mommy
children

break"
Fewer

and
than

"Daddy
one

third

break"
of her

but not
verbs

were

"Break
used

cup" or "Break with


in more

than

two

stick."

construction

in her
the child was uneven
the span of her 2nd year. Likewise,
of
lexical
lexical
and
markers;
subjects
subjects
morphological
production
were produced
for some verbs (take, get) but not for others (put),
consistently
types over

and

some

verbs

received

tense

markers,

others

aspect

markers,

but

very

few

received both (see also Tomasello,


2000). Studies involving more children
have also found
limited productivity
of early verb use (e.g., Matthews,
8c Tomasello,
2005; Pine et al., 1998; Theakston,
Lieven, Theakston,
8
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

et al, 2001). These


latter findings
Lieven, 8c Tomasello,
2003; Theakston
are not independent,
come
much
of
these
from the
because
data
however,
same Manchester
in
which
children's
12
corpus,
spontaneous
speech during
at
for 6 months,
lunch time and toy play was recorded monthly
beginning
the onset of multiword
speech.
was "counted"
Across all of these studies, productivity
if a child used a
or
frame
marker
with
different
lexical
items
and/or the
given
morphological
same lexical item in different
frames or with different morphological
markers. Using
these criteria, these researchers
have found evidence
for
not
of
the
of
the
marker
but
for
the
present progressive
(-ing),
productivity
SV, or SVO frames (see also
past tense, third person
singular, auxiliaries,
8c Berman,

Armon-Lotem

learn
2003, for similar findings from Hebrew
for
limited
Theakston,
Lieven, Pine, and
arguing
productivity,
uses of go, that
when
children's
(2004) discovered,
analyzing
different forms were used in nonoverlapping
contexts, suggesting different
forms were associated with different meanings
(i.e., The train goes choo-choo
use of one meaning
and Mommy went to work) rather than productive
went
to
and
likes
work/outside
(i.e., Mommy
Johnny
going to the park). The flex
was
that
observed
individual
verbs
seemed
and/or children)
(with
ibility
to imitations of varied uses in the maternal
attributable
input (Theakston
et al., 2001), or emerged piecemeal
to
via one-word
additions or deletions
a
a
want
to
want
from
"I
"I
W"
book")
(e.g.,
previously
produced
phrases

ers). Also
Rowland

8c Tomasello,
Behrens,
(Lieven, 2006; Lieven,
2003). Finally,
Speares,
the children's verb use at Stage
McClure, Pine, and Lieven (2006) compared
I (MLU<2.0)
and Stage II (MLU between 2.0 and 2.5), finding that "old"
structures than
verbs (those used at both stages) appeared
inmore complex
"new" verbs (those first observed at Stage II). Thus,
it appears from these
data

that

children's

ability

to use

verbs

in more

structures

complex

is related

to the length of time they have known the verbs and hence children's
ability
to use a verb in a given frame is not predictable
from their use of other verbs
in that frame, as might be expected
if all verbs are treated as full members
of
the

same

abstract

category

studies

of elicited speech have yielded


similar findings:
Experimental
Children
under 2.5 years who were taught novel verbs for novel actions
verb in an unattested
frame. That
is,
rarely if ever used a just-learned
a
children
in
verb
the
intransitive
Ernie
chams) did not use it
taught
(e.g.,
in the transitive (Ernie is chamming Bert) (Akhtar 8cTomasello,
spontaneously
8c
Brooks
1997;
Tomasello,
1999; Olguin 8cTomasello,
1993; see Tomasello,
a
for
Tomasello
8cAkhtar, 2003).
2000,
summary, also, Naigles, 2002, 2003;
older
and
those
in frames dis
novel
verbs
children,
Only
especially
taught
over

tributed

able

to use

Lieven,

several

the novel

8cTomasello,

sessions

verbs

rather

than

in unattested

massed

frames

in a

single

(Ambridge,

session,

were

Theakston,

2006).
9

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

In

on

research

sum,

children's

early

use

verb

productive

(i.e.,

before

2.5

that young verb users do not routinely use their verbs in


forms and sentence frames, nor do they routinely use
multiple
grammatical
forms and frames with multiple
verbs. The research that yields
grammatical
these findings
is limited, however, by four major factors:
First, Tomasello's
(1992) verb diary study included only one partici
those findings can be generalized
is un
pant; thus, the extent to which
years) has found

in the current research).


(but will be investigated
Second and more
studies with larger sample sizes have
importantly,
the
children's
that the full
only sampled
speech, with the consequence
use
to
of
verb
is
have
been
in
the speech
flexibility
unlikely
represented
known

available for analysis. Given


that the recorded utterances
comprise just a
it seems unwarranted
very small portion of the speech the child produced,
to

draw

conclusions

about

those

words,

and

morphemes,

con

syntactic

structions that did not appear in the sample because these could have been
used by the child while not being recorded
1996; Naigles,
2002;
(Demuth,
8c Stahl, 2004). Many of the speech samples
Stromswold,
1996; Tomasello
were

also

in context

restricted

(i.e.,

same

the

recording

at each

setting

visit,

2006; Lieven et al., 2003) as well as time, thereby limiting the ob


that could be the topics of conversation.
For ex
and people
actions,
jects,
a
if
child
is
recorded
breakfast
and
after
ample,
consistently
during
Lieven,

breakfast
wanting

(food,

things

toys)

and

words

as,

perhaps,

system continues

production
utterances

her

not

would

would

vocabulary

to develop
be

necessarily

seen,

different times of the day and different


have

the

reported,

verbs

used

for

suggestions

time, the same types of conversations


more

is likely to involve expressions

free play, then the conversation

action

recur, with
and

grows

the

working

at

least,

vary

on

memory/

But very novel

is also

child

settings. As Naigles

by mothers,

of

"). Over

the child adding

her

(Lieven et al., 2003).


unless

. . .

("let's

at

recorded

and Hoff

(2006)
accord

dramatically

ing to setting; it is unlikely that children's verb use would be any different. And
given that mothers do not even always use every word and frame flexibly in
et al., 2003), when they,
their sampled speech (Lieven et al., 2003; Theakston
to
unarguably, have full command of productive grammar, it is unwarranted
attribute children's lack of flexibility to a less-than-full command of the same.
Because

children talk less than their mothers,


they would, in fact, demonstrate
less flexibility in their sampled speech by base rates alone.
Third,

the

"conservative-child"

assumes

argument

that when

children's

verb uses omit a subject or object, such omissions


reflect limited grammat
ical knowledge.
and Tomasello
However,
Matthews,
Lieven, Theakston,
(2006) and Valian, Prasada, and Scarpa (2006) have recently demonstrated
that toddlers' production
of subjects and objects in elicited production
tasks
varies

systematically

toddlers

according

to discourse

use fewer full NPs when

and

the referents

processing

constraints

(e.g.,

of those NPs have just been

10
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

these are part of


they imitate full NPs less frequently when
and
Allen
Schroder
have
also found that
(2003)
longer target sentences).
toddlers' spontaneous
of
lexical
and
production
subjects
objects is highly
context.
Because
constrained
the
discourse
the
by
linguistic and discourse
context affects whether or not children produce NPs, children's production
of a given verb with only one NP is not good evidence
that the child thinks
that verb has only one argument.
Such utterances may reflect pragmatic
sensitivity on the child's part, rather than limited grammatical
knowledge.
limited
Fourth, the import of the experimental
findings that suggested
is
associated
with
verbs
itself
limited
the
fact
that
grammatical
flexibility
by
most of the types of frame productivity
that have been tested have involved
meaning
changes as well as frame changes. That is, the use of specific verbs
in specific frames is partially governed
by each verb's semantics; for exam
not
causative
verbs
do
frames, motion
ple,
usually appear in intransitive
verbs do not appear with sentence complements,
and so on (Fisher, Gleit
and

labeled,

man, 8cGleitman,
1983, 1990; Levin, 1993). As described
1991; Jackendoff,
inmore detail by Naigles
to use a novel verb in
children's
reluctance
(2002),
an unattested
frame could be traced to their uncertainty
the
about whether
verb's

was

meaning

even

Thus,

when

for

suitable

the frame independently


8cAkhtar, 2003; Naigles,

the

children's

verb

uses

than

rather

frame,

of the verbs already


2003).

show

truly

not

their

knowing

in it (see also Tomasello

used

less

flex

grammatical

system would support, there are two possible


ibility than a fully productive
One
that
is
toddlers'
of simple sentence frames
interpretations:
knowledge
is abstract, but they use a limited range of grammatical
options in their verb
uses because they use their verbs to express only a limited range of mean
On

ings.

this

account,

it is the

limited

of

understanding

verb

meaning

that

the limited grammatical


in
flexibility of verb use. An alternative
is
that
toddlers'
of
sentence
initial
is
frames
terpretation
knowledge
simple
a child might know both
indeed lexically specific. For example,
and
drop
move can be causal, but having heard move but not
drop in the transitive
frame thus far, he only produces move in this frame. On this account,
it is
underlies

limited

syntactic

A Mechanism
Thus
dren

have

knowledge

causes

that

initial

conservative

of Transition From Lexically Specific

far we have been


an

abstract

grammar

considering
that

allows

use.

toAbstract Grammar

the evidence
newly

verb

regarding

acquired

verbs

when
to be

chil
rec

as instances

of a category and freely combined with inflectional


ognized
and
morphemes
syntactic frames. Another question, which is crucial to a full
account of verb development,
is how the child achieves such a grammatical
to
the
the grammatical
system. According
strongest generativist
position,
and
the
could
themselves,
categories
configurations
they
possibly appear in,
11
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

are innate (Chomsky, 1975, 1995; Crain 8c Lillo-Martin,


1999). At least some
of
this
innateness
be
rendered
if it could be shown
aspects
may
unnecessary
that children discover grammatical
and/or
syn
categories
language-specific
tactic configurations
from their input before combinatorial
speech begins,
and studies of infant learning of language structure make this a plausible
1993; Gerken,
2007; Gomez
2002; Mintz,
2003;
(see Brent,
suggestion
of
such
innateness is also a central tenet of
However,
Naigles, 2002).
rejection
to young children. Ac
that attribute
little syntactic competence
positions
to
the
children
cording
lexically specific grammar view,
begin to combine
verbs with other words before they have achieved grammatical categories and/
or abstract syntactic patterning
2000). Thus,
(MacWhinney, 2004; Tomasello,
a full account consistent with an early lexically
specific grammar requires an
account

of how

children

move

from

to

state

that

an

having

abstract

grammar.

A set of influential suggestions


revolves around the idea that a subset of
are
in
verbs
that
called
verbs,
frequent
input and have general meanings,
verbs,

"light"

serve

as

the

child's

entry

into

abstract

forms

grammatical

and

their meanings
(Chenu 8c Jisa, 2006; E. Clark, 1987, 1996, 2003; Goldberg,
et al., 2004; Goodman
8c Sethuraman,
1999; Goldberg
2006; Ninio,
1999;
but see Campbell 8cTomasello,
This
2001).
proposal that light verbs provide
system comes in several forms, three of which we
entry into the grammatical
summarize

here.

of Goldberg
and her colleagues
(2004) focuses on the
proposal
or
to
of
the
be
frames
constructions
meanings
acquired and relies on both
and
semantic
attributes
of
light verbs. Essentially, Gold
frequency
general
of
children's
the high-frequency
verb go
that
berg hypothesizes
hearings
or
to
the
the
followed by a prepositional
around
store,
go
phrase
locative?go
frame or verb+locative
and
the
table, go outside?(+PP
[VL] construction)
verb put in the + NP PP frame (or V+object+L
[VOL] con
high-frequency
on
to as
the
the
bowl
there?enables
them
bowl
the
table,
put
struction)?put
The

sociate the general meanings


of go and put (inchoative motion
and caused
VL
with
VOL
the
and
frames
motion,
1999; Gold
(Goldberg,
respectively)
et
&
Goodman
8c
Casenhiser,
2006;
2003;
Sethuraman,
al.,
Goldberg
berg
to
of the light verbs are "transferred"
2006). Thus, the children's meanings
of the frames or constructions
that they typically appear in so
the meanings
to be acquired
that the light verbs enable the frames' meanings
(see Case
nhiser 8c Goldberg,
evidence
from grade schoolers'
2005, for supporting
of nonsense
verbs). This theory does not predict,
though, that
acquisition
to
first
in
child
verbs
should
be
the
verbs
appear
speech in a given
light
frame?light

verbs

do

their

work

during

comprehension.

They

are

con

to be the engine of learning about construction meaning,


but not
in
of
the
this
observable
first
children's
consequence
necessarily
learning
(Goldberg et al., 2004). And the theory is silent on how the
productions
sidered

frame

or

construction

forms,

themselves,

are

acquired.

12

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

second version of the light verb proposal


does address children's
of
that
each
frame is first learned
acquisition
syntactic form, hypothesizing
to other verbs (E. Clark,
with a single light verb, and only later extended
rationale
1987, 2003; Chenu 8c Jisa, 2006; Ninio,
1999). These researchers'
most
for this special role for light verbs is similar to Goldberg's:
verbs
Light
or
encode
the
of
frames
constructions
(i.e., go is the
meanings
specific
closely
motion
the +PP frame, make is the canonical
verb, exemplifying
on
the
transitive frame, and put is the ca
verb, exemplifying
causal/acting
the + NP PP frame). The specific hy
nonical transfer verb, exemplifying
is that light verbs should be the first to appear in a given frame
pothesis
canonical

because
used

the

form-meaning
a PP or L because

with

is so

relation
go

Go

transparent.
encodes

transparently

to be

is

learned

motion,

so

any

ac

involve locatives (go home/to the store). Then


arguments must
companying
children's use of go with a variety of PPs or Ls enables them to abstract a
more general +PP or VL frame, which
they can then transfer for use with
more specific verbs such as run and
(eventually) cry.The idea is that the first
verb used in a given frame is that frame's pathbreaker (Ninio, 1999), which,
facilitates children's formalization
of that
by virtue of its general meaning,
frame and thus their use of other verbs in that frame.
from this light-verbs-as-pathbreakers
Further predictions
hypothesis
include that light verbs should be used in ways both more grammatically
flexible
than heavy verbs learned at the
complex and more grammatically
same time (E. Clark, 1987; Ninio,
et al., 2004). Both of
Theakston
1999;
these predictions
follow from the semantic generality of light verbs: Because
verbs such as go and put can apply to any type of inchoative or caused motion
in these mo
(respectively),
specifics about which entities are participating
tions

are

not

recoverable

always

from

the

context.

For

eat

example,

usually

only involves eatable things, whereas put can involve a wide range of things
in forward horizontal
put, and run usually only involves animates engaged
motion,

whereas

motion

in any

can

go
direction.

involve
Thus,

to be as clear as possible
appear

with

overt

either

arguments

(Grice,

or

animates
to

in response

the

inanimates

engaged

communicative

pressure

1989), go and put should be more

than

run

and

eat.

Furthermore,

in

likely to

semantically

in the adult language


(Levin,
general verbs are more
syntactically diverse
8c Hoff-Ginsberg,
their general mean
1993; Naigles
1995, 1998) because
ings allow them to fit into a wider variety of sentence frames (Snedeker 8c
Gleitman,
2004). For example, bring can be used both with and without a PP,
on whether
the speaker chooses to focus on the action or goal of
depending
What's
(e.g.,
bringing
Molly doing? She's bringing thepizza/Where isMolly going
so quickly? She's
bringing thepizza to the library), and go but not run can be used
with a preverbal P (e.g., Up you go! but not Up you runl?for
most speakers).
The current evidence
for light verbs as "pathbreakers"
to an abstract
is
mixed:
Ninio
and
Chenu
and
(1999)
grammar
Jisa (2006) report, based
13

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

on spontaneous
learning Hebrew,
speech samples from children
English,
and French, that many children first used a given frame with only one verb;
moreover,
(Chenu 8c
they took from 43 days (Ninio, 1999) to several months
to
a
with
verb.
Ninio
that
frame
second
However,
(1999)
Jisa, 2006)
produce
et al. (2004) have also found
and Theakston
were not always light verbs. Ninio
(1999)
finding with the light-verbs-as-pathbreakers
what
notion but broadening
"pathbreaking"
verb for a given frame. In particular, want,
is included as a canonical/general
frames,
does not encode "highly transitive" senses

verbs
that these pathbreaking
this divergent
has reconciled
the
by maintaining
hypothesis
counts as a light or "canonical"
a frequent first verb in SVO
it
transitive verb even though
such as change of state or po

8c Thompson,
8c Hopper,
sition (i.e., Hopper
1980; Thompson
2001).
et al. (2004) also point out, though, that because semantic gen
Theakston
are confounded,
it is not clear which factor
erality and input frequency
to children's
forms.
of grammatical
contributes more heavily
acquisition
no
in
numbers
and
verbs
their
between
found
difference
heavy
They
light
of different
frames, percent of utterances with subjects, nor percent of ut
terances with objects, once input frequency was partialled out. Apart from
the light-verbs
the mixed nature of the evidence, a problem with evaluating
is that all of the relevant data thus far have been
hypothesis
as-pathbreakers
drawn from samples of spontaneous
speech of limited duration and limited
are
tenuous for the sampling reasons dis
such
contextual
range;
findings
cussed

earlier.

Moreover,

almost

all of

the

research

thus

far has

investigated

the behavior of light verbs as a class, even though the light-verbs-as-path


breakers proposal really targets several individual light verbs as pathbreak
ers for specific frames (i.e., go for the VL/+PP
frame and put for the VOL/
+ NP PP frame), and furthermore,
the particular
light verb serving the
for
children.
different
different
could
be
function
pathbreaker
that light verbs are the prod
and Gleitman
Snedeker
(2004) propose
ucts of syntactic acquisition
rather than its engines. They argue that light
that could be inferred from observation
verbs do not have specific meanings
as,

for

example,

the meaning

are more

of

eat could

be

inferred

from

observing

eaters.

on

syntax for their acquisition


dependent
from their
receives support in findings
argument
8c
Gleitman,
Lederer,
Gleitman,
1999),
(Gillette,
Paradigm
to
and
told
conversation
of
video
mother-child
clips
given
a
tone
and
verbs
sounds.
when
is
what
verb
go
put)
(e.g.,
guess
Light
produced
to be accurately
than the
more syntactic
information
guessed
required
One
run
8c
and
verbs
Gleitman,
2004).
throw)
(Snedeker
(e.g.,
"heavy"
verbs
this
is
that
that
from
view
follows
for
children's
light
speech
prediction
should appear less frequently than heavy verbs in verb-only phrases and more
frequently with sentence frames because the light verbs should be acquired
after (and because of) the sentence frames.

Thus,
light verbs
than heavy verbs.
Human Simulation
in which adults are

This

14
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

to Be Addressed

Questions

users

of

verb

their

children

become

is central

to

and how

when

Understanding
ductive

of Verb Grammar

Regarding Children's Acquisition

lexicons

pro

grammatically

and

when

understanding

users. The

of
foregoing
language
verb
about
literature reveals the following
early
questions
and thus about the flexibility of children's early verb use, that
productivity,
form a focus of the present study:
how

children

adult-like

become

review

the existing

1. How

in

early

course

the

at

ductivity?or
novel

verbs

sitive

versus

verb

1- and

that

demonstrated

of

does

learning

least flexibility?emerge?

intransitive

frames

novel
et

(Gertner

understand

in tran

verbs

al.,

have

studies

can

children

2-year-old

in SVO frames and distinguish

pro

grammatical

Prior

2006;

Naigles,

1998). Still, the existing studies of children of that age have not
in their spontaneous
found good evidence of such productivity
et
et
A big gap in the
Theakston
al.,
1998;
al.,
2003).
(Pine
speech
which

literature,
from

the

fact

that

the

current

no

studies

in spontaneous

ductivity

was
study
have
tracked
in a way

speech

to

designed
several
that

fill,

results

children's

includes

all

pro
uses

of

their very first verbs.


to delineate,
though, how much of the gap in the lit
can
fill.
The productivity
that is the hallmark of
study
one
utterances
human language is the capacity to produce and understand
has never heard before. The most unambiguous
evidence of this capacity in
It is important
erature the current

spontaneous

is the

speech

production

fall that on me"

(Bowerman,

have

heard

an

uous

evidence

by

testing

such

that
children

errors?utterances

1973)?because

utterance.

experimenter-created

such

the child

Comprehension
utterances
understand

children
with

of

can

studies
they
utterances

as

"don't

likely to ever

is not

elicit

unambig
never
heard

have
using

nonsense

the bunny"
words, such as "the duck is gorping
1990). Because
(Naigles,
our study relies on children's
of
verbs in their
spontaneous
productions
conventional

uses,

without

comprehensive

analysis

of

their

input,

we

will

not be able to make claims about this level of productivity. However, many
researchers have looked at spontaneous
speech for evidence of productivity
use
as
of
pro
by taking flexibility
indicating, albeit indirectly, underlying
1989;
Shirai,
1998; Tomasello,
1992). If a child can use a
ductivity (Ingram,
frames or a single frame with multiple
verbs, this
single verb in multiple
a
at
work.
Without
the
child's
system
suggests
productive
knowing
input, it
uses are all input-based
is always possible
that these multiple
rather than
the child demonstrates
the
anew, but the greater the flexibility
generated
less plausible
that account becomes. Using a metric
that the production
of
three to five verbs in a given frame displays some amount of productivity,
15

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

various
with

have reported productivity


in 1- to 2-year-old
children
such as V-ing, SV, VO, SVO (Ingram, 1981; Pine et al., 1998;
1998; Tomasello,
1992). In the present study we similarly adopt the
researchers

frames

Shirai,

and

three-verbs-per-frame

measures

five-verbs-per-frame

of

productivity

as less unambiguous
but still valuable indicators of the children's ability to
use their frames flexibly and independently
of specific verbs.
In this study we also introduce several additional measures,
which we
of
We
index
the
the
argue
system.
investigate chil
productivity
underlying
we
term gram
dren's ability to use their verbs with multiple
which
frames,
(1992) claimed that Travis's "inability"
in her grammar;
limitations
illustrated
to which our child subjects did use their
the appropriate
the first 10 instances?with
verbs?within
grammatical
to which these
We
and
also
arguments
morphemes.
investigate the degree
latter two types of flexibility are related to each other: Does it automatically
follow that a child who uses eat with both Ss and Os might also use the SVor

matical
to use

flexibility. That is, Tomasello


break with both Ss and Os
therefore, we investigate the extent

VO

frame with multiple

verbs?

2. If children do not initially have adult-like productivity,


process
idence

they
by which
of "pathbreaking"

that

seem

view

that

to

verbs

when

which

they
whether

over,
and

whether

the

earliest

tions

they
show

our

onset

driven,

first

grammatical

grammatical
will
be
which

uses

of

though,
a frame
first

children's
flexibility

Relations Among Different Aspects

of data
we

uses
than

function

not

occur
of
their

for

this

among

with

was
address

all

children,

in

the

driven

all

ques

current
rather
the

whether
verbs

are

and

are

verb

to

degree
More

function

productivity,
addressed

collection
will

differences

in the
speech
and
complexity.

serve

show

unanswered,

be

should

specifically,

ev

is, verbs

with

flexibility
serve
this

grammatical
same
verbs

light
to
verbs

study.
than frame

there

that

in children's

appear

verbs,

still

whether

first

the

Because

dren's

that

suggests

is the

found

speech;
a
The
frame.
given
into
abstract
gram
entry

to
the way
productivity
serve
as the child's
verbs

some

what

have

in children's

verbs

lead

frames

matical

studies

it? Several

achieve

but

light
verbs
demonstrate
light
first uses
of heavy
verbs.

chil

rather
more

of Early Verb Growth and Use

on the nature of early verb


positions
the
predictions
regarding
distinguishable
yield empirically
development
and flexibility of grammar as they
relations between flexibility of meaning
of both types of flexibility of
develop and the relation of the development
to generativist
verb use to growth in the verb lexicon. According
theories,
Some of the contrasting

theoretical

16
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

the grammar
is formal, abstract, and autonomous
1995, 1998;
(Chomsky,
Borer 8cWexler,
1987). Therefore,
acquisition of a verb lexicon, develop
ment
of semantically
flexible uses of those verbs, and development
of
uses
flexible
of
verbs
those
should
all
be
aside
unrelated,
grammatically
from

the

obvious

command

necessity
be able

would

that
to

children

produce

more

have

who

at

words

their

sentences.

longer

theories predict
there will be relations
Other
lexical size,
among
are
semantic flexibility, and grammatical
There
many arguments
flexibility.
that language development
is the product
of domain-general
learning
et al., 1996;
8c Synder,
mechanisms
1988; Elman,
(Bates, Bretherton,
Saffran 8c Thiessen,
of de
2007), and these predict that all the measures
number of verbs acquired and the semantic and gram
velopment?the
matical flexibility with which
be at least loosely
they are used?should
comes
related. A stronger prediction
relatedness
of
from the view that
to this view lexical devel
grammar
emerges from the lexicon. According
opment

must

precede

grammatical

because

development,

a certain

thresh

old or "critical mass" of vocabulary must be achieved before grammatical


can be abstracted
8c
2006; Marchman
(Childers 8c Tomasello,
patterns
Plunkett
8c
Robinson
8c
Bates, 1994;
Marchman,
1993;
Mervis,
1998) and,
in

some

zational
These

accounts,

grammar

processes
views yield

continues

to

develop

lexicon
by a growing
the prediction
that grammatical
caused

as

a result

of

reorgani

8c Thai,

2006).
(Conboy
should be pos
flexibility
of verbs. The syntactic bootstrap

itively correlated with children's number


also yields the prediction
that the size of the verb lexicon
ping hypothesis
com
should be positively related to the flexibility and overall grammatical
plexity

of

verb

use

because

grammar

is

the

source

of meaning-relevant

to learning verbs. Both the shared mechanism


information
that contributes
and syntactic bootstrapping
hypotheses
predict a positive relation between
semantic and syntactic flexibility of verb use. The shared-mechanism
hy
semantic and syntactic flexibility
pothesis predicts that the relation between
should be across children?children
who show more
semantic flexibility
should also show more grammatical
The
flexibility.
syntactic bootstrapping
hypothesis
additionally
predicts a relation between grammatical
flexibility
and semantic flexibility across verbs, because it is knowledge
of a particular
verb's structural possibilities
that supports its semantically flexible use.
Numerous
studies support the conclusion
that the size of children's
vocabularies
and the grammatical
of their speech are related
complexity
8c
8c Bates,
Goodman,
1999; Caselli, Casadio,
(Bates
1999; Jackson
8c
Maldonado,
Thai, Marchman,
Bates,
1993; Maital,
Guitierrez-Clellen,
& Bates,
Dromi,
2000; Marchman
1994; Marchman,
Sagi, & Bornstein,
8cDale, 2004; Ogura, Yamashita, Murase, & Dale, 1993;
Martinez-Sussman,
8c Mervis,
are less well sup
Robinson
1998). More
specific predictions
The
claim
that
lexical
ahead of
ported.
development
initially proceeds
17
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

is paced by the
and that grammatical development
grammatical development
on
studies
that
used
theWords
is
based
lexical
largely
preceding
development
form of the MacArthur-Bates
Communicative
and Sentences
Development
Inventory (CDI; Fenson et al, 1994). The CDI includes a checklist of over 600
words plus a section in which pairs of phrases (one closer to the adult form
than the other; e.g., two shoe vs. two shoes) are listed; the parent checks off the
phrase that is closer to the child's current level of speech. These studies have
more words
reported a curvilinear relation in which children who produce
are also reported to produce more complex grammatical forms, with smaller
in vo
associated with early increments
complexity
changes in grammatical
in
with
associated
size
and
grammatical
complexity
larger changes
cabulary
later,

in

increments

equal-sized

vocabulary.

to think that the statistical


a variety of reasons
There
are, however,
in these data do not necessarily
relations observed
imply that grammatical
on lexical development.
measurement
issues
First,
depends
development
et
Robinson
abound. In the studies of spontaneous
al.,
1988;
(Bates
speech
of grammatical
8c Mervis,
1998), MLU was used as the measure
develop
of grammar apart from the size of the
ment, and MLU is not a pure measure
in their study
lexicon. Indeed, as Rollins, Snow, and Willett
(1996) reported
14
of children's
speech, the MLU growth between
sampled spontaneous
and 32 months for a substantial percent of the children was accounted for by
or other grammatical
growth in content words alone, not in morphological
items.

a relation

Thus,

between

size

vocabulary

MLU

and

not

does

neces

de
lexical development
and grammatical
sarily reflect a relation between
it
the
reflect
Rather
greater
may
possibilities
expressive
velopment.
afforded by a larger vocabulary.
Two problems particularly apply to the many studies that have used the
CDI.

the measures

Because

same

report

parental

of

instrument,

vocabulary
they

and
may

grammar
share

the

are
same

taken

from

the
bias.

parental

curvilinear
of the observed
interpretation
critically, the ordering
on the
measures
and grammar
the vocabulary
relation between
depends

Most

assumption
grammar

that
were

the measurement
equally

sensitive

of vocabulary
measures
of

and
change

the measurement

of

across

of

the

range

and Marchman
(2007) have argued that this
development
function an
nonlinear
false. They performed
is demonstrably
assumption
CDI
section of the
mapped
alyses which revealed that, while the vocabulary
are uni
lexicon (i.e., increases in vocabulary
linearly onto the underlying
section
the grammar
in underlying
lexicon),
formly related to increases
are
in
such that early changes
grammar
underrep
nonuniformly,
mapped
resented in the CDI, relative to later ones. The curvilinear relation observed
studies. Dixon

and grammatical
between
lexical development
is, they argue,
development
an artifact of the differences
of lexical and gram
the measures
between
in their sensitivity to early growth.
matical development
18
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

to

are
the observed
(linear) correlations
or direction
source
of
The
they
causality.
that vocabulary
supports the induction of grammar
knowledge
possibility
and the alternate possibility
that grammatical
supports the in
knowledge
are different
duction of word meaning
mechanisms
that
language learning
size and gram
would both be reflected in a correlation between vocabulary
Finally,

matical

not

that

reveal

neither

Also,

usage.

neous

extent

the

still do

real,

the

the

use have

word

CDI

assessed what
that they use them?nor

use?only

or

argument

structure

bination

and some

sentence

nor

the

children
have

about

that

sponta

the words
children's

words

appear

they
use of

in com

to the degree
that vocab
Thus,
achievement
of grammatical
about vocabulary
is
knowledge

may
knowledge
it is not clear just what
understandings,
and what about grammatical
doing the supporting
ulary

of

analyses

know

they assessed

frames?only

are present.
the
support

inflections

previous

understanding

is being

supported.

Previous
the semantic flexibility
studies have not examined
of chil
dren's verb use, and thus have not captured possible differences
among
children and among verbs in what children know about the meanings
of the
verbs they use. A revised version of the grammar-from-lexicon
hypothesis
suggest that children who know more about the words that they use
might
to knowing more words,
(i.e., are more flexible with them), as opposed
or
to
sooner. Some support for
be
able
achieve
grammar
might
productivity
this revised version comes from an experimental
study that assessed tod
dlers' extendability
of verbs' grammatical
form as well as their extendability
of the verbs across situations (Naigles et al., 2005). As described
earlier, the
21-month-olds

in

erent

which

were

pairings,

on

presented

Moreover,

the

this

study

thus

video,

children

were

had been
were

shown

taught

demonstrating
also
shown

to be

able

to

recognize

in a playroom
one
to

recognize

form

verb-ref

setting, when
of
the

they

extendability.
verb-referent

pairings that had been taught in the transitive frame but tested in the in
transitive frame (e.g., children were taught you're gorping the ball and tested
on the ball is gorping).
it was the children who could distinguish
Strikingly,
in the transitive who were
(i.e., had learned) the verb-referent
pairings
more
them to the intransitive. These
likely to be able to extend
findings
there is some threshold
of semantic
before
suggest
required
learning
could
be
demonstrated.
grammatical
flexibility
to investigate
The current
these issues in more
study was designed
detail, asking whether children who use verbs more flexibly in terms of their
semantics also use them more flexibly
in terms of their grammar. We will
also examine
as a characteristic
the relations among types of flexibility
of
verbs. That is, we ask whether verbs that are used more
terms
in
of
flexibly
their semantics also more
likely to be used more flexibly in terms of their
grammar.

19

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

THEPRESENT
STUDY
The foregoing
review of the theoretical
and empirical
children's acquisition of verbs makes
it clear that a central and
is when in the course of verb development
children
question
verbs beyond
the functions,
and morphosyntactic
referents,

literature on
unanswered
extend their
structures
in

which
cause

is as yet unanswered
be
they have heard them used. The question
the research methodologies
that have been brought
to bear are lim
ited in several ways. Sampling
naturalistic
interactions
parent/child
(e.g.,
is likely to miss situation
collecting 3 hours of data per week for 4 months)
specific language use. Children use verbs in particular contexts that may not
be recorded,
such as washing in the bathtub, splashing in the pool, sleeping at
and
night,
riding in the shopping cart at the grocery store (Naigles 8c Hoff,
8c Hoff-Ginsberg,
2006; Naigles
1998). Sampling also does not capture the
full range of uses to which children put the verbs they use, thus rendering
about

questions

the

of

scope

and

meanings

of

forms

verb

use

unanswerable.

with one child (e.g., Dromi,


1987; Mervis,
1987;
Diary
Tomasello,
1992) may solve the sampling problems but at the cost of making
to all children from
it impossible to untangle evidence of processes common
uses that are
to
the
child
under
idiosyncratic
single
study.
An ideal data set for addressing
the issue of when and to what degree
children go beyond their input would include a record of all of the verb uses
studies

of many

conducted

children?in

than

that
of

verb uses

dren's
Ingram,
issues

verbs
some

address

these

are

the

used
as

that does

1981; Shirai,

discussed

make

ways are more

variety of different
use

to a

addition
we

If, however,

input.

yet

all
that

of

those

verbs

children's
that

likely to reflect productive

in

only
unanswered

not

of

record

assumption

include

1998)?provided

single

way,

questions

are

verb

used

in a

and extended

then

we

with

a record

can

begin
of chil

to

their input (e.g., E. Clark, 2003;


the record overcomes
the sampling

above.

In this study, we overcome


utility of the existing evidence
earliest uses
turing children's
attributes
of a diverse
positive
1987; Harris et
1993; Dromi,
We
included more
sello, 1992).

of the sampling issues that limit the


to the diary method
for cap
by returning
in the method
of verbs, incorporating
the
set of previous diary studies (e.g., E. Clark,
many

al, 1988; Robinson & Mervis,


1998; Toma
a
than
single child, asking the parent par
ticipants to record their children's uses of only 34 common verbs and only
the first 10 uses of those verbs. In this way, we were able to track early
our parent participants'
changes in verb use without exhausting
capabilities.
We targeted 34 of the most common verbs produced
by children early in
8cGelman,
(Goldin-Meadow,
language acquisition
Seligman,
man & Bates,
& Kruger,
Tomasello
1994; Tomasello,
1992;
next chapter we present
in detail. In the results
the method

1976; March
1992). In the
chapters that

20
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INTRODUCTION

we present
the method,
the description
of early verb development
that these data suggest, and we address the questions we have outlined with
nature of children's
semantic, and grammatical
respect to the pragmatic,
we
first verb uses. In a concluding
consider
what
the data imply
chapter
with respect to the nature of children's early linguistic understandings.
follow

21
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

II.PRESENTINGTHEDIARYMETHOD

PARTICIPANTS
The data in the present
study come from diary records kept by eight
uses of 34 common verbs. A
mothers
of their children's first 10 spontaneous
to
total of 18 mothers
but 9 mothers withdrew
initially agreed
participate,
from the study before having collected enough data for analysis (i.e., at least
10 uses each of 10 different verbs). For five of these mothers,
their reason
was that their child (always a boy) was not
for withdrawing
talking at all, and
to bring the child to clinical services for evaluation
and
they planned
treatment.

The

mothers

other

who

withdrew

the

gave

reason

their

that

child was talking so much


that they found they could not record or make
note of every relevant utterance
their child produced,
and so they could not
keep

year

the

diary

from

after

However,

One

accurately.

her

and

child
the

data

from

additional

mother

seven

collected

verbs

were

data

for

over

for a total of 31 verbs.

the diary

completed

collected,

she

lapsed

in her

diary keeping for 6 months. Given this lapse, we could not be certain that
the data from the last 24 verbs really reflected
the child's first 10 uses of
these

verbs,

and

this

child's

data

were

not

analyzed

further.

At the onset of the study, the children (5 girls and 3 boys) ranged in age
from 15 to 19months;
all were European American. Detailed questioning
of
established
the mothers
that none of the children had yet produced
any
verbs in the 20
verbs; this was supported by the absence of any spontaneous
min
the experimenter's
first visit. The
speech sample collected
during
children's
word
this
types during
spontaneously
produced
speech sample
were tabulated; on average,
the children produced
25.12 different words
=
(SD
11.47). At the first visit, the mothers were asked to fill a questionnaire
the child's siblings, the parents' education and occupation,
and
television and reading habits. Three of the children had older
=
= 3
(M 5 years, SD
years); one also had a younger
siblings
sibling
one
All
months
of
but
of the parents
(3
(mothers and fathers) had
age).
= 3.33
=
attended
1.97;
years, SD
college for at least a year (M [mothers]
= 4.0
=
were
M
SD
Their
[fathers]
2.28).
years,
occupations
generally
an
middle
class (the fathers
included an accountant,
several engineers,
concerning
the child's

22
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

THEDIARY
METHOD
PRESENTING

insurance

executive,

and

landscaper,

the mothers

manager;

computer

All of the
been accountants,
had previously
teachers, and bookkeepers).
The eight children
children were cared for at home by their mothers.
watched an average of 9.83 hr of television per week (SD = 7.54) and were
to the
read to for an average of 11.08hr per week (SD = 7.67), according
maternal

report.

MATERIALS
Each mother was provided with a bound diary with 34 individual pages.
the first
On each page was listed a different verb with 10 rows for recording
10 instances of that verb's use. The 34 verbs were chosen from prior data
et al., 1976;
sets of the children's
verb use (Goldin-Meadow
spontaneous
8c Kruger,
8c Bates,
Marchman
1992; Tomasello
1994; Tomasello,
1992);
these are all words that would be used as verbs in the adult language. They
included 8 light verbs, which have more general meanings
(H. Clark, 1996;
narrower
which
had
Nine
and
26
verbs,
1999),
heavy
meanings.
Goldberg,
of the 34 were obligatorily
transitive, 9 were obligatorily
intransitive, and 16
were

alternating

intransitive

verbs

(i.e.,

frames). They

they

can

in

appear

are listed, by category,

both

transitive

in Table

and

1. In the blank

TABLE 1
List

of

34 Common

Verbs

Transitive
Light

verbs

Intransitive

Alternating

come
bring
give

go
look

put
take
want
Heavy

verbs

hold

clap

bite
like
need
see

cry cut
fall
drop
run eat
sit

JumP
kiss

walk
wave

lay
move
open

pull
push
roll
stop
throw
wash

23

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE2
Diary
of Connecticut/Florida

University

Atlantic
Record

University
First Verbs
Child's

Page

ten uses

of first

of:

Study
Birthdate_ PULL

Name_Heather_

For commands,
who or what

is

command
addressed
of

Record
Date

Was

utterance

a command

complete
utterance

or

the

doing
verb action?

description?

1st

10/2

Pull

Command

Mommy

2nd

10/3

Pull

Command

I pulling
Uncle,
pull

Description
Command

Mommy
Heather

3rd

12/5

4th

12/8

5th

12/8

6th

12/8

Pulling
Auntie,

pull

7th

12/8

Auntie,

pull

8th

1/3

9th

1/13

10th

diary,

1/13

space

Mommy,
me
pull
I pull that
I pull

was

this

to?

For descriptions,
who or what
is

or what

Who

the
receiving
verb action?

Sled

Command

Uncle

Sled

Command

Aunt

Sled

Command

Aunt

Sled

Command

Mommy

Cart

Description

Heather

Blanket

Description

Heather

Blanket

to

record

the

Other
comments

Chair
Wagon
Car with

Uncle

provided

is

complete

handle

utterance,

the

date,

or
function
and addressee
of the
(i.e., command
pragmatic
description),
as well as the actor and the affected object (when relevant) of the
utterance,
verb's action. A sample diary page is reproduced
in Table 2.

PROCEDURE
Parents with children between
15 and 19 months of age were found by
of the local newspaper.
searching back issues of the birth announcements
the study were sent, followed by phone calls requesting
Letters describing
volunteers willing to keep detailed diaries of their child's verb development.
Extensive questioning
confirmed
that the child had not produced
any verbs
at that point and that the parent would be in primary contact with the child
at this point because
their child
20 families were excluded
(approximately
had already begun
verbs). All the volunteer
parents were
producing
mothers. The researcher
then visited the family to train the mother on diary
a
to
collect
20-min spontaneous
keeping and
speech sample.
24
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

THEDIARY
METHOD
PRESENTING

The training session was conducted


the goal of the study was to investigate
so

detail,

that

it was

of

crucial

first. The researcher explained


the child's language development
to make

importance

note

of

every

utterance

that
in
that

a target verb and (b) was not an immediate


of
(a) contained
repetition
then
introduced
the
and
The
researcher
another person's
diary
speech.
a separate list of the 34 target verbs. It was explained
that our
provided
were less well studied than nouns. The
focus was on verbs because
they
between nominal and
the difference
researcher discussed with the mother
a
vs.
in
bite"
"bite this"), described
bite
"take
verb uses of homonyms
(e.g.,
several

of

examples

uses

verb

in one-word

and

multiword

and

utterances,

then
that the study was about verb uses only. The researcher
emphasized
of possible
each column of the diary page, giving examples
discussed
utterances
in the diary. Specifically,
the
and how they were to be entered
were instructed on (a) what constituted
"When
commands
mothers
(e.g.,
to get you or someone
to do?or
else
child
is trying
your
stop
and descriptions
(e.g., "When your child is telling
doing?something")
an addressee
or
an
about
event,
relation"),
(b) what constituted
you
object,
actor
is
directed
is
the verb
the
command
to"),
("Who
("Whom
doing
or
what
is
the
of
the
verb
and
affected
action"),
patient
object ("Who
were
were
not
and
labeled
the
when
these
child, (c) recording
action")
by
the

as

utterance

as

exactly

inflections

including

possible,

such

as

"-ing,"

"-ed," and "-s" if heard, and (d) the use of the right-most column involving
notes (mothers were encouraged
to add notes at all times, but
contextual
when

especially
Then,

the

they

researcher

were

not

emphasized

sure
how

about

or

pragmatic

important

it was

semantic
for

roles).

the mother

to

note

the target verbs until 10 instances had been


every utterance using
told
the
mother
that every utterance meant
that
and
repeatedly
produced
of any target verbs should be
the child's exact and inexact self-repetitions
of
included, as well as new utterances with that verb, but not repetitions
other

people's

speech.

The

researcher

suggested

that

the

mother

put

the diary in an easily accessible place in the house and carry the diary
she and the child went out. The researcher
asked and
along whenever
noted which words the child was currently producing
(in case some verbs
had emerged
since the phone
researcher
then
call; none had). The
described her own role, which would be to phone the family every 2 weeks
until

the child began producing


the target verbs. Once verb production
the
would
researcher
began,
phone the family every week to check how data
collection was proceeding
and to answer any questions
the family might
have. The training session ended with a discussion
of the duration for data
collection; each mother was asked to keep the diary for at least 3 months.
The actual duration of the study varied from child to child, ranging from a
=
to a maximum
minimum
of 3 months
of 13 months
(M 8.625 months,
=
SD
3.62).
25
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

to play with
After the training session, the researcher asked the mother
so that the child's current
child for about 20 min
level of speech
could be recorded. The researcher
toys for them to
production
brought
own
could
also
with
and
read
their own books
their
with;
toys
they
play
play
her

little book reading was conducted),


(although
doing whatever
they usually
did at that time of day. The researcher
then started the audio recorder and
left the room for 20 min, staying outside or in another part of the house.
to minimize
Audio recording was used instead of video recording
feelings of
and

self-consciousness

concerns

about

privacy.

to each family
After this initial visit, biweekly phone calls were made
until the child began producing
the target verbs. Then,
the family was called
the parents of the diary procedure
weekly, reminding
(especially, to record
and
the
every utterance)
parents might have had in
answering any questions
out
to discuss
the
Mothers
used
conversations
these
diary.
filling
typically
new utterances
and the records
the children's
the
thereof, describing
in detail and receiving confirmation
and/or instruction concern
were
how
recorded.
Mothers
little difficulty with the
ing
they
reported
some
level of detail required by the records; those who did acknowledge
were
ones
soon
to
the
who
leave
the
Table
3
asked
study.
difficulty
displays
each child's age at the onset of the study, duration of the study, and the total
to the 10-instance criterion out of the 34.
number of target verbs produced
were recorded were included in
for
all
instances
which
10
those
verbs
Only
utterances

of

any

the

and

coding

analyses.

MEASURES
of the age at which each verb was
diary records provided measures
and the number of days elapsed from the 1st to the 10th instance of

The
used

TABLE3
and

Age
Age at
Onset
Child

of Verbs

Number

Length

of

Study

by Each

Produced
#

of Verbs

Child
Rate

Produced

(Out of a Possible

34)

1;8Carl

11 months

24

Carrie

1;7

7 months

30 4.42/month

Elaine

1;8

5 months

Heather

1;7

7 months

314.42/month
31

Stacey

19
3.8/month

Mae

13 months

1;5

Ned

3 months

14

4.67/month

1;6

Sam

11 months

31

2.82/month

1.5
SD

12 months
8.625

months

3.62

of Target
Growth

2.18/month

1;7

1;4
Mean
1;6

Verb

2.38/month

20
1.67/month
24.5

7.07

3.21/month

1.15

26
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

METHOD
THEDIARY
PRESENTING

use. In addition,
the following measures were taken from the diary records
or coded based on the information
the mothers
recorded:
Pragmatic Content
Each verb use was coded as a command or description
(these accounted
the addressee was
and for each command,
for over 98% of all utterances),
1 of Table 2, Heather
in Instance
noted. To illustrate,
says "Pull" as a
to pull a chair; in Instance 3 she says "I pulling" as
to her mother
command
a description
is the
of her own actions with the toy car. The addressee
utterance
to
Instance
in
this
the
child's
is
is
the
whom
directed;
1,
person
4 it is her uncle.

and in Instance

mother,

Semantic Role
The
person

recorded for each verb use were the particular


semantic properties
or objects filling the roles entailed by each verb. For all verbs, this
who
recording
For
transitive

involved
the

verb.

or what
and

as

served

the

actor,
who

verbs,

alternating

agent,
or what

or

experiencer
as
served

of
patient

or theme was also recorded. For example, when a child said "jumping" and
the child in the actor cell, then the child was coded as
her mother
recorded
the actor of the action. With regard to affected objects, an utterance of "Pull"
with "chair" recorded in the affected object cell would be coded with "chair"
as the affected object, whereas an utterance of "pull me" would be coded with
of actor and affected
"Heather" as the affected object. These
assignments
were

object
expressed

to which
multiple
actors

made
or not

regardless
because
the

of

whether

the

of

purpose

the

roles

semantic

coding

was

to capture

were
the

overtly
extent

actors and on
children extend these verbs to actions by multiple
were
on
to determine
mothers
instructed
how
The
carefully
objects.

and

affected

objects

when

these

were

not

overtly

expressed.

Action Referent
Each

instance

of

each

verb's

use

was

coded

as

the

same

or different

from

to. This
previous uses of the verb in terms of the physical action referred
uses
was
on
for
of
of
the
made
34
based
the
28
actor,
verbs,
target
judgment
affected object, and other notes recorded by the mother. Action referent
change was not coded for four internal event verbs (like, look, need, see, want),
and itwas not coded for the verb bring because the specific action referred to
by uses of this verb was difficult to discern from the diaries. In our first pass
of coding the action content of the remaining
verbs, utterance pairs of the
same verb were hypothesized
to refer to different actions if (a) the actors
were of different species (e.g., dog
running vs. child running) or kinds (e.g.,
child coming vs. TV show coming), (b) the affected objects were of different
27
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

kinds

(e.g.,

vs.

rocks

eating

an

eating

sizes

apple),

(e.g.,

a chair

washing

vs.

washing dishes) or configurations


(e.g., opening a bag vs. opening a plastic
or
to different
the
situations
locations
(c)
container),
pertained
(coming
vs. coming out of the house), directions
downstairs
(taking Legos off vs.
taking a Pop tart out), or activities (taking a nap vs. taking a shower). A total
were
of 93 distinct
action pairs were
then
initially identified. These
to
in
were
11
to
randomized
order
who
asked
presented
undergraduates,
rate them on a scale of 1-7, where
1 indicated the pairs described identical
that the pairs described completely different
physical actions and 7 indicated
were included to anchor the lower
An
actions.
additional
items
20
physical
end of the scale; these included action pairs performed
by similar actors to
similar affected objects in similar situations. The ratings were averaged for
each action pair and only those whose rating averaged 4.0 or higher (n = 76)
were included in the final
analysis (mean rating for these 76 pairs averaged
=
SD
The
list of action pairs and ratings can be
5.63,
0.84).
complete
obtained from the authors.
Frame

Grammatical
The

frame

grammatical

mothers'

records

coded

components

location

of

words,

For example,

the utterance

an

cracker";

"I"

subject

"down."
preposition
marker
"-ing"
gical
there"
includes
the

and

were

not

verb-containing

and

utterance

The

the

locative

"my

clapping"
marker
"no,"
Vocatives
frame

grammatical

and

phrases,

full

"I drop my
a

it encompasses

includes a subject "my" and a

"no

negative
form
"there."

as a

counted

object

"My fall down"

the
The

markers,

subjects, objects, morphological

(SVO) frames.
utterance

me)

utterance.

preposition/prepositional

full SVO frame. The

push

complete

overt

coded from

markers,

negative

includes

target verb use was

of each

children's

included

subject-verb?object
cracker"

the

includes
and

the
the

morpholo
utterance
in

(e.g., Mommy

"Go

Mommy,

component.

Flexibility
In order
spontaneous
in

instances

Changes

to calculate
speech
terms

are defined

flexibility

was
of

its

coded
pragmatics,

below

as

of verb use, each


same

the

semantics,

and examples

or

instance

of a verb

from

changed
and
grammatical

are provided

in Table

in

previous
frame.

4.

Pragmatic Flexibility
types of change were counted as instances of pragmatic
flexibility:
in
function and changes in addressee. A function change was coded
changes
or
to using descriptions,
when a child first switched from using commands
Two

28
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

THEDIARY
PRESENTING
METHOD

TABLE4
Flexibility

Type

Coding
Instance

Baseline

of Flexibility

Instance

Changed

Pragmatic
Pull
Function

(Command)
=
(Ad Mommy).

Pull

Addressee

pulling
(Description)
=
Uncle,
uncle)
pull (Ad

Semantic

Actor
(A)
Affected object (AO)

=
Eating (A daddy)
=
Daddy eating (AO pizza)

=
Eating (A dog)
=
Jill eating (AO bagel)

Pull

Pull

(AO

chair)

(AO

wagon)

Action
(A

Open
Come

object

Situation

Go
child)
= car
door) Open
(AO

ActorGoing
Affected

(A

car)
=

(AO
Come

(downstairs)

jar)
(out of the house)

Grammatical

Subject Pull I pulling


Object
Locative

versa,

first made

My

cup

my

drop

pulling

Doggie
eating
I pull that
Pull me

object

for

Pull

Morphology
Lexical
subject

vice

me

Pull

My fall
Myfall down
I biteNobite

Preposition
Negation

Lexical

Pull

SVO
Drop
Go Go there

verb.

given

a change

An

addressee

in addressee

with

Daddy

eating

was

change

a given

coded

a child

when

verb.

Semantic Flexibility
Three

types

of

semantic

change

were

coded.

An

actor

change

was

coded

when a child first made a change in actor during the 10 instances of a given
the change from the dog to the father as actor in Table 4
verb. For example,
instantiated this child's first actor change with eat. An affected object change
was noted for the instance with each verb when a child used a different
affected object from the first instance. For example,
the change from the
as
to
4
the
in
affected
Table
instantiated
this child's first
pizza
bagel
object
affected object change with eat. An action change was coded when a child
first referred to an action physically different from his or her initial action
referent

for that verb, according

to the ratings described

earlier.

Grammatical Flexibility
Nine types of changes were coded as instances
all consisted of the addition or subtraction of words

of grammatical
or morphemes

changes;
to/from a
29

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

attested
frame. Table 4 shows the nine forms followed
by
previously
The
first
six forms (subject, object, SVO, locative, preposition,
examples.
formed the category of syntactic flexibility. Morpho
collectively
negation)
included
any instance in which the child made a change in
logical flexibility
Past tense uses were vanishingly
verb morphology.
rare; therefore, we did
tense
not have to distinguish
forms
from regularized
past
irregular past
tense forms (e.g., "fell" vs. "failed"). In addition to the foregoing measures
in which
the target verbs were used,
of syntactic frames
of flexibility
measures
of the flexibility of verb use with respect to the lexical items filling
the subject and object roles were coded. Lexical subject and lexical object
in the
the child made a change
included any instance in which
changes
a
or
For
term
in
the
lexical
used
change
example,
object position.
subject
from saying "I pull" to "Baby pull" was coded as a change in lexical subject
a change in
use. In contrast, a change from "pull" to "I pull" constituted
syntax.

In sum, a total of 14 kinds of flexibility were coded for in the children's


of pragmatic
first uses of their first verbs. The measures
flexibility were
to which
narrow
to
the
of
the
address
relatively
degree
question
designed
use
uses
were
their
context
who
verbs
bound:
Children
verb
the children's
their mother
and/or only in addressing
(when other
only as commands,
as tied to a
were
these
verbs
understand
addressees
available), may only
were
measures
use.
of
semantic
The
flexibility
designed
specific context of
to

address

the

core

questions

of

extendability:

to what

extent

were

the

children able to use their verbs with a variety of actors and affected objects,
to different actions? Children who use each of
and to refer (appropriately)
to only one
their verbs with only a single actor or affected object, referring
instantiation of that verb, may not have acquired a principle of extendability
of grammatical
The measures
that applies to verb meanings.
flexibility were
core questions of productivity: To what extent were
to
the
address
designed
sentence
able to use their verbs in different
the children
(appropriate)
Children who use each of their
and/or with different morphology?
in only a single frame may not yet have abstract frames that are
or that operate independently
of their verbs. The semantic and
represented
were
measures
used to address the question of
also
flexibility
grammatical
to an adult/abstract
use light verbs as pathbreakers
children
whether
or grammatical
semantic
with
used
verbs
Are
greater
grammar:
light
were also the relevant
measures
latter
These
verbs?
than
heavy
flexibility
ones for the analyses concerning
how semantic and grammatical
flexibility
of verb use might be related during language development.
The coding was first performed
by the third author and then checked in
were resolved by discussion.
its entirety by the first author. Disagreements
For each verb for a given child, the following were calculated for each
in
the change
(a) which of the first 10 uses manifested
type of flexibility:
frames

verbs

30
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

THEDIARY
PRESENTING
METHOD

usage, (b) how many days elapsed between first use and first different use,
and (c) how many different uses for each category occurred within the 10
in how children use their first verbs can arise both
instances. Variability
from differences
among children and from differences
among verbs. In
order to investigate both sources of variability, all analyses were conducted:
for each
(1) treating children as the random factor and calculating measures
child by averaging across the verbs they produced
and (2) treating verbs as
the

random

factor

and

calculating

measures

for

each

verb

averaging

the children who produced


them (H. Clark, 1973). Means
are
for
both
types of calculations.
analyses
reported

and

across

statistical

31
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

III.A GENERALDESCRIPTION
VERBGROWTH
OF EARLY
AND USE

In this chapter, we describe the emergence


and early growth of verb use
in the eight typically developing,
children who were the
English-learning
in
this
On
the
children
the first
average,
participants
diary study.
produced
instance of one of the target verbs by 18 months
of age and added verbs
lexicons at a rate of 3.21
(from the list of target verbs) to their productive
verbs per month. On average, just over 1month elapsed from the 1st to the
10th instance of use. Table 5 presents
the data for each child, listing age at
first use of the first verb, mean age of first use of all verbs, number of target
over the course of the
verbs produced
study (i.e., between 3 and 13months;
see Table

3),

mean

and

to reach

days

10

instances.

As Table 5 shows, the children varied considerably


both in age of onset
of days to reach 10 instances of use. Some
of verb use and in number
children
(e.g., Carl) took very little time (e.g., on average, under 4 days) to
produce
took more
instances

once

instances

10

time
were

on

(e.g.,
produced

use
average,
on the

of

a verb
over

same

began;
2 months).
for at
day

other

children

For
least

one

four
verb;

Sam)

(e.g.,

all

children,
for

the

10

other

four children,
the shortest number of days to reach 10 instances ranged
1
from 2 to 18 days (Ned = 2, Carrie = 5, Stacey = 7, Sam = 18). Figure
presents

the

cumulative

children's

target

verb

vocabulary

over

the

course

of

the study. The children can be grouped


into early verb learners (Sam, Ned,
and Stacey) and later verb learners (Elaine, Carl, Mae).
Carrie, Heather,
Most of the early verb learners (except for Sam) were also fast learners, with
over time. Most of the late verb
steep increases in target verb vocabulary
learners (except for Elaine) were also slower learners, with more gradual
increases

in

target

verb

vocabulary.

The

correlation

between

the

age

at

their first of the target verbs and the average age


which children produced
at which they first produced
all the target verbs used during this study was
=
=
<
.001.
.81,
r(n
8)
p
significant,
the analogous
data by verb, also revealing much
Table 6 presents
across all verbs was 649.96
onset
The
of
average
age
variability.
days
32
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

VERBGROWTHAND USE
A GENERALDESCRIPTION
OF EARLY

TABLE5
Verb

Overall
Number
Child

Verbs

Carrie

30

of

Elaine

19
31

20

Stacey
Mean

24.63

Age of Onset
Across Verbs

of Days
10 Instances

Number

Reach

to

759.17 3.17

564

744.9320.03

602

674.8924.95

584

49.55
632.97

577 31
Mae

744.93

35.60

511 14
Ned

532.85

22.64

546 31
Sam

600.10

67.48

491

534.94 58.39

559.75

653.1035.23

41.15

6.48

SD

Mean

Mean

Age First Verb


Produced
(Days)
60324
Carl

Heather

by Child

Development,

92.76 21.73

38.47); open was on average the earliest produced whereas give was on
On average, verbs
the latest of these 34 verbs to be produced.
average
reached the 10-instance mark 37.39 days after their first use (SD = 16.71);
wave took the most,
clap took the fewest days to reach 10 instances and
on
wave
are
the
and
least
the
data
reliable,
being used by only
although
clap
two children. Two verbs were produced
all
8
children
(want, wash); on
by
=
5.79
children
(SD
1.59).
average, each verb was produced
by
the verb within the
Across verbs, the number of children who produced
with
the
window of this study correlated
average
age at which
negatively
(SD

35

-|-1

r-S

/J

& 25J**T
\
/ fi
y~r*I
IS
r-//
\ - 7
/
5/

\Z /* //

||

5J
0

r^ I I

J
*

i
'

'
*

;J

Carl//
* / Ned

F^H

Carrie
/
-^Heather

/
Elaine

?Mae

//

I /'

rJ

|
I"^-Stacey

Ii i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i I

Age
Figure

in days

1.?Cumulative

verb

vocabulary

by age.

33

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE6
Overall
Number
Verb

of

Verb

Mean

Age of Onset
Across Children

Children

Bite

Rank

Order

7 592.71

44.29

10
26.38
17
34.00
16
57.20
47.14 5

Fall 5 614.20

55.60 8

5741.60

34
40.60

Go 7 589.86
Hold 6663.67
Jump7643.14
Kiss 6661.33

26.00 2
23
23.00
15
61.00
19
50.50

Lay 4 599.25
Like 6 710.83

27.00

33
33.00

7 626.29

12
17.57

Move 3 696.00

30
35.67

Need 6638.33

14
27.67

Open 7 587.57
Pull 5671.20

17.29 1
25
77.60

Push

14.83

6 608.17

48.40
22

6 680.50

30.67
28
26
38.29

6 612.17

29.57

Sit 7616.57

11
13.83

Stop 5 700.00
Take 5661.80
Throw

5 663.00

Run 7672.71
See

to

27
65.86

Cut 7 645.57

Roll

of Days
10 Instances

Drop 5 645.20
Eat 7 604.71

Put

Number

Reach

24
11.50

Cry 7680.57

Look

Mean

32
47.33

Clap 2 668.50
Come 8 616.50

Give

of

Acquisition

3 704.67

Bring

by Verb

Development,

31
25.60
38.80
20

614.20

35.60

Walk7637.43

13
23.71

Want 8651.13

18
43.75

Wash 8 662.13

21
29.88

Wave 2 687.00

29
72.00

that verb first appeared


in the speech of the children who produced
it,
=
= ?
.39, p<.0b.
Thus, verbs that were produced
r(n
34)
early were the
verbs that were produced
by more children within the span of the study.
were
the
verbs
that
21
Also,
(bite,
produced
by at least six of the children
come,

cry, cut,

eat, go,

hold,

jump,

kiss,

like,

look,

need,

open,

push,

10 instances of use in an average


walk, want, wash) reached
= 14.42
the 13 verbs produced
whereas
(SD
days),
by fewer

run,

roll,

sit, see,

of 33.24 days
than six of the

34
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

A GENERALDESCRIPTION
OF EARLY
VERBGROWTHAND USE

children
reached

(bring, clap, drop, fall, give, lay, move, pull, put, stop, take, throw, wave)
10 instances of use in an average of 44.07 days (SD = 18.52), a
reliable difference,
Cohen's
one-tailed,
?(32) =1.91,
p<.05,
statistically

In

0.67.

those

sum,

verbs

that

were

the most

widely

acquired

were

also

the earliest acquired.


The mean age of first use for light (M = 645.43 days, SD = 96.40) and
=
=
83.19) did not differ, nor did these verb
heavy verbs (M 635.13 days, SD
in mean
number
of days to reach
10 instances
subclasses
differ
(M
=
=
=
=
tran
M
SD
SD
31.82,
18.08;
36.30,
23.74).
Similarly,
[light]
[heavy]
sitive (T), intransitive
(I), and alternating
(A) verbs did not differ in their
=
=
mean
SD= 114.53, M
663.85,
[I] 629.61,
ages of first use (M [T]
=
=
=
SD
90.87, M [A] 626.89, SD
81.33) nor in their mean number of days
to reach 10 instances (M [T] = 32.28, SD = 20.39, M [I] = 31.9, SD = 24.17,
M [A] = 37.19, SD = 24.37). Thus,
the data indicate that the variability on
these

measures

of

the

timing

of

emergence

and

the

rate

of

early

use

was

not

the heavy-light
distinction among verbs or to
systematically
the transitive, intransitive, and alternating distinction
among verbs.
at least one grammatical
All children produced
subject with at least one
=
were
of
the
verb
tokens
verb, but only 32% (SD
22%)
produced with overt
related

subjects.

Overt

direct

to either

objects

were

more

common,

with

produced

an

av

=
at least two
25%). Each child produced
erage of 46% of the tokens (SD
=
tokens of subject-verb-object
with
24%
frames,
(SVO)
(SD
19%) of tran
sitive or alternating
verb tokens in SVO frames. Similar findings were ob
tained when calculations were performed
by verb: 35% of verb tokens were
=
overt
with
20%), 45% of transitive or alternating
produced
subjects (SD
=
verb tokens were produced
with overt objects
(SD
30%), and 24% of
were
verb
tokens
in SVO
frames
transitive/alternating
produced
=
then, the children more
(SD
21%). On average,
frequently
produced
verbs with direct objects than with subjects, t(7) = 1.85, p = .052, one-tailed,
d = 0.59, and transitive and alternating
verbs appeared with direct objects
more
than they appeared with subjects, t(24) = 2.36, p = .013
frequently
d = 0.39. Six of the eight children,
and 18 of the 25 relevant
one-tailed,
verbs, followed this pattern. As expected, uses of verb inflections and prep
ositions were much
less frequent, with inflections included with 6% of chil
=
= 7% and
dren's
verb tokens
calculated
9%; when
(SD
by verb, M
=
SD
included with
11% of their verb tokens
11%) and prepositions
=
=
=
5%; when calculated by verb, M
24%, SD
(SD
25%).
As did the age of onset and rate of production measures
for these verbs,
the measures

dren

of

grammatical

use

revealed

wide

variation

among

the

chil

and

the verbs. The percent


of verb tokens produced
with
among
subjects varied from 74% (Carrie) to 1% (Ned), the percent of transitive or
verb tokens produced
with direct objects varied from 77%
alternating
verb
(Stacey) to 17% (Sam), and the percent of transitive or alternating
35
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

in SVO frames ranged from 59% (Carrie) to 2% (Ned).


tokens produced
Verbs also varied in the percent of tokens produced with subjects, from 70%
(wave) to 0% (look, stop), transitive and alternating verbs varied in the percent
of tokens produced with direct objects, from 94% (want) to 0% (jump, lay),
in SVO frames ranged from 73% (want)
and the percent of tokens appearing
to 0% (five verbs: jump, lay, stop, push, open). Variation
in morphological
and
was also observed: Only five of the children produced
use
any
preposition
verb tokens with the "-ing" suffix and the percent of such tokens ranged
from 23% (Carrie) to 1% (Ned). Fifteen of the 34 verbs appeared with
"-ing," and the percent of such tokens ranged from 0% (9 verbs) to 37%
included 4 uses of third person singular "-s"
(cry).Other verbal morphology
10 uses of the past tense (three children
children
and
(two
contributing)
the
children
verb tokens with prepositions
of
Six
produced
contributing).
and the percent of such tokens ranged from 17.7% (Sam) to 4.6% (Carrie).
Prepositions were produced with 15 of the 34 verbs, and the percent of such
the children's first verb
tokens ranged from 90% (lay) to 1% (look). Thus,
were
not
structures and typically
utterances
full
SVO
containing
frequently
utterance
the
with
consistent
lacked morphological
inflection,
length lim
nature
of early
with
this
and
the
characteristic
of
itations
age
telegraphic
was
a
in
such
the
chil
the
data
that
also
trend
There
word combinations.
dren who were older when they began using these verbs used them more
in full SVO frames than did the earlier verb users, r(n = 8) = .63,
frequently
=

.09.

7 displays the percent of tokens including subjects, objects, prep


ositions, "-ing," SVO frames, and verb-only utterances used with light and
to the timing of
heavy verbs. In contrast to the null findings with respect
Table

emergence

and

rate

of

production

measures,

these

measures

of

grammat

between
these two categories of
ical frame use revealed several differences
more
in SVO frames
verb. Light verbs appeared
frequently
significantly
less likely to appear
than did heavy verbs, and light verbs were significantly
both verb subclasses included verbs with
in verb-only utterances. However,
few verb-only uses (fewer than 30% of tokens [light verbs in boldface]: bring,
clap, cry,fall, give, hold, lay, like, need, put, take, want, wave), many verb-only
uses (>60% of tokens: bite, go, move, pull, push, see, stop, throw, walk), and
intermediate
verb-only uses (between 30% and 59% of tokens: come, cut, eat,
in the analyses both by chil
kiss,
look,
pull, roll, run, sit, wash). Trends
jump,
dren and by verbs suggest that "-ing" appeared more frequently with heavy
than with light verbs.
Table 8 displays for the transitive, intransitive, and alternating verbs the
"-ing," and SVO
percent of tokens used with subjects, objects, prepositions,
frames. Children used transitive verbs with both objects and SVO frames more
the by-verbs analysis revealed
than alternating verbs. Moreover,
frequendy
both
the
for
of
verb
effects
type
percent of tokens with subjects and
significant
36
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

A GENERALDESCRIPTION
OF EARLY
VERBGROWTHAND USE

TABLE7
of

Properties

Grammatical

Light
Item

Grammatical
I. By children
with
Tokens

Heavy

Verb

Use

Heavy
SD

Means

(Proportions)

and

Light

Means

df*

-0.36

SD

Cohen's

subjects

.33

.22

.34

.21

ns

.75

.23

.38

.24

5.31

.001

1.57

with

objects
SVO

.42

.30

.19

.16

2.88

.02

0.96

"-ing"

.03

.05

.08

.11

ns

.09

.10

.07

.07

1.09

ns

.25

.21

.40

.21

5.14

.001

subjects

.37

.24

.34

.19

0.37

32

ns

.73

.17

.38

.28

2.64

23

.01

.43

.23

.23

.26

1.52

23

ns

.04

.08

.08

.12

32

ns

.12

.19

.10

.21

.24

.24

.42

.26

Tokens

with

Tokens
Tokens

with

Tokens

with

Tokens

verb-only

//. By verbs
with
Tokens

Tokens

with

Tokens

with

objects
SVO

Tokens

with

"-ing"

Tokens

with

Tokens

verb-only

Note.?The
df decrease for the % tokens with objects and with SVO
because only the transitive and alternating verbs are included.

-1.46

-1.86
0.17
1.76

0.71

1.32

32

ns

32

.087

0.70
by items

(subject-verb-object)

the percent

of tokens with "-ing": Transitive verbs appeared with subjects more


than
frequently
alternating verbs, and intransitive verbs appeared with "-ing"
more frequendy
than either transitive or alternating verbs.
In sum, this group of typically developing
status
middle
socioeconomic
children

produced
utterances.
ports

to use

began

18 months

some

verbs

in their

spontaneous

speech

at an

average

the following
3-11 months,
(560 days). Within
an average of 25 of the
common
34
verbs
targeted
The

nature

of

generalizations

these

children's

about

early

first
verb

uses

of

development.

their

age

of

the children
in at least 10

first
Contrary

verbs

sup
to

the

picture of idiosyncratic development


suggested by studies that only sampled
children's
speech (see chapter I), these diary data indicate that some verbs
are consistently
earlier than others, and these early-appearing
acquired
verbs are also used by more children.
(We can reject sampling probability as
cause
a
the
of
these two variables because of the
spurious relation between
recorded all verb uses.) Children
unique nature of this data set?mothers
who produced
their first verb earlier tended to be faster learners overall.
at least some of their first uses of first verbs in
The children produced
multiword
utterances, but they varied widely in the percent of verb uses that
were in word combinations.
Consistently,
though, verbs were more likely to
be combined with direct objects than with subjects and combinations
with
prepositions

and

morphology

use

were

rare.

37

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

for
Negligible
Tokens
P
with
all
types
verb
2.03
22
4.94
.14
.13
.001
.25
.51
SVO
Tokens
with

Note.?The

the

df decrease

intransitive

for

the % tokens
with
Properties
Grammatical
Intransitive,
Transitive,
and
Alternating
Use
Verb
of
TABLE8

versus

objects

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating
transitive
post hoc

and with

comparison

SVO

yielded

by
(subject-verb-object)
a

significant
2.31
0.23
.13
.23
.10
.19
.18
.09
P
Tokens
with
ns
Tokens
with
"-ing"
.03
.01
.16
.15
.05
2.31
5.99
.08
.006**
2.31
.17
4.50
.26
Tokens
.01**
.20
.36
.18
subjects
with
.49

Tokens

Grammatical

with

Item

Tokens
objects

(Proportions)

2.21
2.24
.06
Tokens
.14
.11
.01
"-ing"
with
.01
.10
ns
Tokens
with
SVO
.35
.28
.12
.11
3.70
7.007
1.08
objects
Tokens
with
subjects
.40
.24
.30
.22
.27
.21
.78
2.21
ns.27
.64
.31

with

SD
Means

.27
.72
.21
.20

SD Means

items
subjects,
%
with
tokens
/(22)
difference,
transitive
hoc
yields
%
d
with
**For
post
alternating
significant
versus
comparison
3.13,
1.32.
tokens
For
jf?
.004,
a"-ing,"
=

SD Fit

.20

df*

because

t(\7)

= ? 2.83,

the

difference,

5.13

p Cohen's

22 5.33
.001

Means

p2.43,
*(23)
comparison,
d=
.02,
.98.

I.
By
children

only

By
//.
verbs

CO
00

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

.001

A GENERALDESCRIPTION
OF EARLY
VERBGROWTHAND USE

pattern of combinatorial
class. Light verbs were produced
This

SVO

and

frames,

heavy

verbs

verb use differed


with more objects
were

produced

somewhat
and more

with

more

by verb sub
in
frequently
"-ing"

forms.

that light verbs would serve as "path


However,
contrary to the hypothesis
we
to grammatical
found no evidence
that light verbs
breakers"
structures,
were produced
than
verbs.
Of the 11
earlier in children's
speech
heavy
some were light
before the age of 620 days (on average),
verbs produced
(come, go), but others were not (throw, sit, see, push, open, lay,fall, eat, bite). And
in verb-only utterances,
supporting
light verbs did appear less frequently
that light verbs are the products
Snedeker and Gleitman's
(2004) suggestion
Further analyses of this
rather than the engines of grammatical
acquisition.
V
in
and
will
be considered
VI, when we discuss the
chapters
hypothesis
as
a group and individually.
flexibility of light and heavy verbs
grammatical
verbs appeared with subjects, objects, and SVO frames
verbs did, while
intransitive verbs ap
than alternating
than either transitive or alter
peared with "-ing" forms more frequently
these patterns of differences
among
nating verbs. We further investigate
children and among verbs in chapter VI.
this general picture of verb emergence
and growth in
Having painted
these eight children's use of their first verbs, we turn now to a more detailed

Finally, transitive
more
frequently

description
meanings

of
that

the communicative
they

functions

these

verbs

served

and

the

expressed.

39

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

IV.PRAGMATIC
AND SEMANTICFLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE

In this chapter, we present analyses that address the


questions of how
extendable
first verb uses are in terms of function and meaning. The model
of the child as a conservative
language learner suggests that children should
restrict the functions of their first verbs to those they have heard in input
and thus, to the degree that input illustrates only some functions children's
verb uses, should show a restricted range of functions for each verb when it
et al, 2006; Tomasello,
(Golinkoff et al, 1995; Maguire
1992).
The conservative-child
model also suggests that children should initially use
some verbs with reference
to only the event that verb was first
paired with.
is first used

The

that

evidence

verb

use

in maternal

to children

speech

is

frequently

tied

to particular
settings (Naigles 8c Hoff, 2006) suggests that a conservative
would be similarly restrictive
learner
in verb production.
Such a
language
conservative

in

only

child

a ball

throw

or

reference

for
might,
example,
"eat"
in reference
only
to themselves
eating

use
to
but

"throw"
eating
not

only
cookies

their

as
but

parents.

a command
not

pizza,
If children's

to
or

initial uses of their early verbs are underextended


in the same fashion
as early noun uses, we should expect to find the
diary records of the chil
dren's initial verb instances replete with identical entries down the 10 rows.
are extended
In contrast,
if verb meanings
and functions
almost from
the beginning,
then we should expect to find the diary records containing
a varied set of entries even within
the first 10 uses of children's
very
verbs.

early
To

test

these

predictions,

we

describe

the

pragmatic,

semantic,

and

action flexibility
that the children demonstrated
in their first 10 uses of
course
the target verbs they produced
the
of this study, and we
during
measures
the
of
for
and
compare
flexibility
light
heavy verbs, and for tran
sitive,

intransitive,

and

alternating

verbs.

40

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INEARLY
PRAGAAATIC
AND SEMANTICFLEXIBILITY
VERBUSE

PRAGMATIC
FLEXIBILITY
and Descriptions

Commands
The

pragmatic

of

categories

and

"command"

"description"

together

utterances
in the diary records;
for 98.6 % of the verb-containing
in commands
in descriptions.
and 53.2% were
45.4% of verb uses were
are in line with other findings
These proportions
for this age group (e.g.,
2008). All of the children used at least
Vasilyeva, Waterfall, & Huttenlocher,
account

one

verb

sorts

in both

of

utterances.

On

the

average,

children

produced

with 53% of their verbs (SD = 13%); the


and descriptions
commands
commands only
range was from 33% (Stacey) to 71% (Carl). They produced
with an average of 21% of their verbs (SD = 16%) and descriptions
only with
an average of 25% of their verbs (SD = 14%). Four children used more verbs
than only in descriptions,
three children used more
only in commands
verbs only in descriptions
than only with commands,
and one child used the
same number of verbs only with commands
and only in descriptions
(see
children
tended to use language for different
Figure 2). Thus, different
both

communicative

functions;

however,

all

were

children

able

to use

at

least

some of their verbs in utterances


functions.
serving different
to
in
and others in
Some verbs were more
be
used
commands
likely
a
utterance
in
verb
both
the
of
children
average percent
using
descriptions;
=
were
was
first
10
Some
verbs
within
their
instances
53%
(SD
27).
types
all
the
children
whereas
used in both utterance
of
take),
types by
(bring,

35tl
30- f%

i^i n

.o

Sam

Stacey

Mae

Carl

Carrie

inB?thc&Di

c/2 0 Commands

Heather

Elaine

Ned

Children
Figure

2.?Communicative

functions

of early

verb

uses.

41

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

others never were

Seven verbs
(fall and drop were used only in descriptions).
and
(bite, bring, kiss, pull, push, take, and walk) were used both as commands
stop was used only in com
descriptions
by 80% of the children or more,
mands by 80% of the children, and like was used only in descriptions
by 80%
seem to follow from the meanings
and typ
of the children. These findings
in ref
ical usages of these verbs: Fall and drop were used most commonly
erence to the actions of inanimate objects, which are not likely to respond
as like is a verb of internal volition,
it is not
well to commands; moreover,
not
to
tell adults to like
be the subject of commands
(children do
likely
were
tied to specific functions the restricted use
things). Thus, where verbs
seems explicable
in terms of the verb's meaning
rather reflecting context
on the part of the child. The majority of verbs were
bound representation
and descriptions
used as both commands
by the majority of children, and
the second function ap
when verbs were used to serve both functions,
use of that verb (M = 4.32,
on
of
the
fourth
instance
average
by
peared
16.91 days (SD = 11.83).
SD = 0.89) and within
Some differences
light and heavy verbs in terms of
emerged between
these pragmatic properties. The children were more likely to use light verbs
than heavy verbs, Ms = 0.39 (0.30) and 0.14 (0.12), re
only as commands
=
=
=
1.09, and less likely to use light
.05, Cohen's d
spectively, ?(7) 3.01, p
=
0.08 (0.13), M (heavy) = 0.30 (0.17),
verbs only as descriptions, M (light)
=
=
= 1.92. Similarly, light verbs tended to be
.05, Cohen's d
3.00, p
t(7)
used

more

than

frequently

heavy

verbs

only

as commands,

Ms

0.33

(0.27)

=
=
and 0.16 (0.25), respectively,
.10, Cohen's d
0.67, and less
/(32)
1.66,p
= 0.32
=
as
M
M
0.10
(0.09),
(heavy)
descriptions,
(light)
only
frequently
=
=
= 0.79.
.10, Cohen's d
1.92, p
(0.31), t(32)
Flexibility (ForCommand

Addressee
When
more

using

than

one

Utterances Only)

to command,

verbs
addressee;

however,

used at least one verb with

all children
there

again,

was

considerable

variability

in the addressee flexibility


Figures 3a
among children
they demonstrated.
and 3b show the mean percent of verbs with which the children used more
in
the first change
instance at which
and the mean
than one addressee
flex
addressee
children
the
took
On
addressee
average,
displayed
place.
=
21%). For those verbs that were used
ibility with 50% of their verbs (SD
so on average before the fifth instance (M = 4.72,
flexibly, the children did
= 10.03
=
on
SD
0.58), and
average within 13.29 days (SD
days) of the first
instance.

Within

the

first

10

instances

of

a verb's

use,

the

number

from 1 to 4 (M= 1.86, SD = 0.66). Most of


dressees
ranged
from at least
were
with more than one addressee
used
(81.25%)
and
six
verbs
however,
(wave, like, jump, cry, clap,
bring) were used
were
on
used with
one addressee
Verbs
child.
each
average
by

of

ad

the verbs
one child;
with only
addressee

42
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

PRAGMATIC
AND SEAAANTIC
FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE

:?

100tT^)
90-

80"

11

Sam

llllllli
.

Sam

iiuiiii
Figure

Mae

Stacey

Carl

Carrie Heather

Elaine

Ned

Elaine

Ned

Children

9J(b)
8"

dressee

fl|

Mae

Stacey

Carrie Heather

Children

Percent
of verbs
in early verb use.

3.?(a)

flexibility

Carl

used

with

addressee

flexibility

and

(b) onset

of ad

=
flexibility by 41% of the children (SD 26%). Verbs in commands were used
with a different addressee from the first instance within, on average, 14.47 days
of the first use (SD = 19.96 days) and by the fifth instance of the use of that verb
=
the measures
of addressee
(M 4.36, SD= 1.25). When
flexibility are com
none
for
the
and
of
the
subclasses,
pared
light
heavy
analyses by child yielded
one
differences.
The
verb
difference: Light verbs
significant
analyses by
yielded
were used with significandy more addressees than heavy verbs, M (light) = 2.04
= 0.98.
= 1.58
=
=
.05, Cohen's d
(.55), M (heavy)
(.44), *(30) 2.36, p
In
do

not

sum,

when

seem

to

verbs

uniformly

first

enter

restrict

children's
these

verbs

productive
to a
single

lexicons,
utterance

children
function,

nor

to a single addressee. They use about half of their verbs in both com
mands and descriptions,
and they direct their commands, with about half of
their verbs, to multiple
addressees. One limitation of change of addressee as
a measure
of children's flexibility of verb use is that, over the course of a day,
43
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

have only a limited number of possible addressees: mother,


and
father. Thus,
children who reach their 10 instances of a
sibling, pet,
be
verb
less
addressee
given
quickly might
likely to demonstrate
flexibility
these children

because

fewer

different

are

addressees

available.

FLEXIBILITY
SEMANTIC
Action Flexibility
4a and 4b show

Figures

to more

reference

in action

change

more

one

than
referent

child

On

the percent

the mean

and

occurred.

than one action with

|
fev?i
Q

for each

action

average,

38% of their verbs

children

(SD

in

of verbs used
at which

instance

the

reference

made

first
to

19%). For those verbs

100-A
(a)
90 - K)
80

70-

I , 60-

mm

^ Sam

Stacey

Mae

Carl

Carrie

Heather

Elaine

Ned

Heather

Elaine

Ned

flexibility

and

Children

io -ri I
9- 0>)

iliililk
Figure
flexibility

Sam

Percent
4.?(a)
in early verb use.

Stacey

Mae

Carl

Carrie

Children
of verbs

used

with

action

(b) onset

of action

44

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

AND SEMANTICFLEXIBILITY
PRAGAAATIC
INEARLY
VERBUSE

showing

action

children's

flexibility,

first

new

action

use

occurred

av

on

=
=
15.54 days
0.85) and within
erage before the fifth instance (M 4.6, SD
=
first
after
instance.
the
The
of
number
actions
verb
within
(SD
13.50)
per
the first 10 instances ranged from one to four; six of the eight children
to at least three different
referred
actions with at least one verb. Across
=
an
of
of
the
children
33%
verbs,
(SD
20%) referred to more than
average
one action within the first 10 instances of
a verb. Come was used to
producing
refer to more than one action by six of the children; put and open were used
to refer to more than one action by five of the children. The first
change in
=
on average
action reference
occurred
the
fifth
instance
(M 5.1,
by
SD = 1.99) and on average within 27.14 days of the first use (SD = 35.77).
Actor Flexibility
5a and 5b show for each

Figures
reference

change

to more

in actor occurred.

Sam

actor

child

Stacey

Mae

the percent

the mean

and

All children

iluiiii
10
fl*> I

used

Carl

in

of verbs used
at which

instance

the

first

at least 50% of their verbs

Carrie Heather

Elaine

Ned

Carrie Heather Elaine

Ned

in

Children

<a>

te

8 -

Uilllll

Sam

ibility

one

than

Figure
5.?(a)
in early verb

Percent
use.

Stacey

Mae

Carl

Children
of verbs

used

with

actor

flexibility

and

(b) onset

of actor

flex

45

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

to more

reference

one

actor.

On

average,

children

made

reference

to more

than one actor with 73% of their verbs (SD = 13%). For those verbs showing
on average before
actor flexibility,
the first new actor use occurred
the
fourth instance (M = 3.9, SD = 0.77) and within
15.79 days (SD = 12.06)
after the first instance. The number of actors per verb within the first 10
instances ranged from 1 to 5 (M = 2.68, SD = 1.0). Across verbs, an average
=
of 76% of the children
than one actor within
20%) referred to more
(SD
a verb; eight of the verbs were used with
the first 10 instances of producing
actors by all the children. The first change
in actor reference
different
occurred on average before the fourth instance (M = 3.85, SD = 1.01) and
on average within
16.91 days of the first use (SD = 13.91).
percent of children's verb uses that referred to themselves as actors
from
11% (Stacey) to 70% (Carl). On average, 51% (SD = 18.1%) of
ranged
children's first 10 uses of these early verbs concerned
themselves as actors.
as
uses
actors ranged from
to
of
the
that
referred
self
verbs,
percent
Among
in
0% for the verbs cry, look, move, open, and pull (the latter four primarily
was
to
want.
for
used
exclu
the
verbs
and
100%
like,
commands)
Cry
clap,
to infant siblings or fictional characters. Mean percent of
sively in reference
self as actor uses calculated across verbs was 50.8% (SD = 30%).
The

AffectedObject Flexibility
used

6a
Figures
in reference

and

6b
to more

show

the
than

of
percent
one
affected

object

verbs
alternating
at
instance
the mean

and

transitive
and

their first change in affected object reference.


produced
re
verbs, of course, do not have affected objects.) Children
(Intransitive
affected objects on average with 89% of their verbs
ferred to different
=
(SD
9.8%). For those verbs that were used flexibly, the children referred
to a new affected object on average before the fourth instance (M = 3.33,
11.68 days (SD = 7.93) of the first instance. The
SZ) = 0.79) and within
to within the first 10 instances ranged
number of affected objects referred
of children who made
from 1 to 8 (M = 4.69, SD = 1.77). The proportion
to more
than one affected object averaged
reference
79% across verbs
=
verbs were used with
3l). Twelve of the transitive and alternating
(SD
different affected objects by all children, whereas only two verbs (jump, lay)
The
appeared with only one affected object in every child's productions.
on average before
the
in affected object reference
occurred
first change
fourth instance (M = 3.34, SD = 0.97) and within
12.40 days (SD = 7.61).
The above descriptions
(and compare Figures 5a, 5b and 6a, 6b) sug
gest that the children displayed a higher degree of affected object flexibility
for two of the
this difference
than actor flexibility. Paired t tests confirmed
a significant
tests
t
the
revealed
three flexibility measures:
children,
By
in the percent of verbs showing actor compared with affected
difference

which

the children

46
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INEARLY
PRAGMATIC
AND SEMANTICFLEXIBILITY
VERBUSE

iooji(a)

m m

Sam Stacey

Mae

Carl

Carrie

Heather

Elaine

Ned

Carrie

Heather

Elaine

Ned

Children

lllillll
10
iTcw
9

Qt

^ ^

llllilU

Sam Stacey

Figure
Percent
of verbs
6.?(a)
affected
in early verb
object flexibility

Mae

Carl
Children

used
use.

with

affected

object

flexibility

and

(b) onset

of

=
=
=
.0005, Cohen's d
1.39, and a significant
?(7) 6.02, p
object flexibility,
actor was produced
in when
difference
the first different
compared with
=
=
when
the first different
affected object was produced,
.01,
2(7) 3.42, p
=
d
0.73. Comparison
of the number of days to actor and affected object
=
d = 0.40.
t(7)
change showed a trend in the same direction,
1.599,/?<.10,
are
verbs
the t tests
included),
By verbs (only the transitive and alternating
t
revealed significant differences
for the mean
instance of change measure,
=
=
=
2.16, p
.04, d
0.62, and the number of days to first change mea
(22)
=
=
=
of children
sure, 2(22)
2.78, p
.01, d
0.37, but not for the percent
of freedom differ in these
2(24) <1, ns. (The degrees
showing flexibility,
analyses because two verbs displayed no affected object flexibility.)
We next compared
the different verb subclasses in terms of actor and
a significantly higher percent
affected object flexibility. Children produced
47

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

to different affected objects than that they


of their light verbs with reference
=
=
did for heavy
SD = 0.06, M
0.98,
verbs, M
0.81,
(heavy)
(light)
=
=
=
=
SD
0.20, t(7)
2.29, p
1.15, and light verbs tended
.055, Cohen's d
to be produced with different actors by a higher percent of children than
did heavy verbs, M (light) = 0.87, SD = 0.18, M (heavy) = 0.72, SD = 0.19,
=
= 0.80. There were no differences
=
1.88, p
.068, Cohen's d
?(32)
among
the categories of transitive, intransitive, and alternating
verbs in actor flex
transitive and alternating verbs in affected
ibility and no difference between
object flexibility.
in early
These data reveal a picture of pragmatic and semantic flexibility
verb use. For example,
these children were very likely to use eat in reference
to multiple
eaters. Eating, for them, was not a restricted
foods and multiple
a
to
that
category
single type of situation but one that applied across
applied
foods
different
(at least)
being eaten (which are, of course, eaten in different
are
ice cream is licked, cereal is slurped) and dif
chewed,
ways?cookies
ferent people and animals doing the eating (showing that the children were
to an inclusive rather than
able to generalize across different mouth motions
for eat). This picture suggests that children's first verb
restricted meaning
uses differ from their first noun uses, which can be quite inflexible (Dromi,
1987; Harris et al., 1988).
use

The present findings also differ from other descriptions


of early verb
et al.
to the predictions
in the literature. Contrary
of Huttenlocher
children's

(1983),

their own
someone

very

actions
else's

first

uses

verb

or relations;

actions

as

were

own.

their

not

were

children

just as likely
more

Interestingly,

to

in reference

consistently

to talk about
was

flexibility

ob

that this is be
served with affected objects than with actors; we conjecture
cause there is a greater range of possible affected objects in a child's home
items) than of possible actors (e.g., animates).
(e.g., toys, food, household
more varied affected objects than heavy
were
verbs
with
Light
produced
verbs, which is consistent with the notion that light verbs apply to a wider
than heavy verbs (usually,
range of items (one can bring just about anything)
one eats only food, kisses only animates, and rolls only round things; see also
8c Sethuraman,
Goodman
8c Gleitman,
2006; Snedeker
2004; Valian et al.,
and
these
of
the
functions
semantics of early
Overall,
then,
2006).
analyses
use

verb
are

not

nicative

suggest
to

that

when

tied

particular
to refer
functions,

and relations

they denote

verbs
uses
to a

enter

but

are

variety

with multiple

children's
available
of

actions,

agents

to

productive
serve

and

lexicons

multiple
to refer
to

and affected

they
commu

the

actions

objects.

48
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

IN
AND GRAMMATICALFLEXIBILITY
V. PRODUCTIVITY
EARLY
VERBUSE

In this chapter, we present analyses that address the question of how


and flexible children are in their initial uses of
productive
grammatically
model predicts
that children
The
conservative-child
verbs.
early-acquired
structures they have heard
will use their verbs only in the morphosyntactic
used with that verb. Each verb might have its own range of structures, but
than the full possible range allowed by the
that range would be narrower
their first verbs as items in
adult grammar. If, in contrast, children represent
an abstract category, with all the morphosyntactic
of occurrence
privileges
allowed by the adult grammar, then in early verb use, each verb should be
each structure should be used
used in a wider range of structures?and
the rapid-generalizer
model
with a wider range of verbs. Thus,
predicts
the conservative-child
verb use, whereas
and productive
early flexible
should
be achieved
and
model
that
only
flexibility
productivity
predicts
or years of input (see Tomasello's
claim
that children
after months
[2000]
with the subject-verb-object
[SVO] frame at 3
only become
productive
of verbs
after
have
heard
thousands
of
222-223],
age [pp.
years
literally
they
used in SVO in their input).
To index the productivity
of children's
syntactic frames, we calculated
verbs used each of the following
for each child the number of different
or particle),
and V-ing. Then,
frames: SV, VO, SVO, V+P
(preposition
we
tabulated
the number
and
Shirai
(1998),
(1981,
1989)
following Ingram
of frames that were used with at least three verbs and the number of frames
used with at least five different
percent
the

verbs. We

also calculated

of all of each child's verb types used

three-verbs-per-frame

(3-V)

and

for each frame the


the age at which

in that frame,

five-verbs-per-frame

(5-V)

levels

of

were

reached, and the number of frames that reached 3-V and


productivity
of age.
5-V productivity
when children were 24 and 30 months
with
To index the grammatical
which
children
used their
flexibility
of different
frames
verbs we counted
the number
(syntactic flexibility),
inflectional endings
different
(morphological
flexibility), different nouns in
49

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

nouns
in object
and different
(lexical subject flexibility)
subject position
in the
with
which
each
verb
(lexical
object flexibility)
position
appeared
10 uses. A frame was coded as different from previously appearing
recorded
frames if there was an addition or subtraction of a subject, object, locative,
term. Morphological
differences
included the
and/or negation
preposition,
or

addition

subtraction

of

or

tense/aspect

person

on

marker

the

verb.

in the lexical item used in the


involved changes
subject differences
Lexical
involved
in the lexical
differences
subject position.
object
changes
differ from the actor and
item used in the object position. These measures
in the previous
affected object flexibility measures
discussed
chapter be
cause those measures
to produce
did not require the children
the nouns
to
actors
whereas
the
lexical
and
affected
the
objects,
subject and
referring

Lexical

measures

object

depend

on

noun

in actual

changes

use.

INFIRST
VERBUSES
GRAMMATICAL
PRODUCTIVITY
Table 9 displays the mean
across children. The children

in each frame
of verb types produced
SV and VO frames with > 60% of
produced

percent

all the target verbs they used, and the SVO frame with just under half (40%)
these are not frames used with just 1 or 2
of the verbs they used. Thus,
the V+P and V-ing frames with <20% of
verbs. In contrast, they produced
in chapter III, these frames were
the target verbs they used; as described
used less frequently
overall. The children varied in the degree of gram
and Carrie were consistently
matical productivity
they displayed. Heather
the

most

productive

children

across

the

frames;

who

child

was

least

TABLE9
Productivity

of Verb

Use

Measures
Frame

SV
Mean % of V types (SD)
Highest

% of V

types

Highest child (# Vs)


Lowest

% of V

types

Lowest child (# Vs)

61 (28)
93.5

VO
62 (29)
100

SVO

VP

41 (26)

18 (15)

Ned (2)

15 (14)

3571.4
35

Carrie (29) Heather (21) Carrie (15)


14.3

V-ing

Sam (11) Heather (11)

40 0
29.4

Carl (5)

Sam (1)

Carl,

Carl

Elaine
Mean

age of 3-V

21.83(7)

22.28(8)

23.64(6)

22.45(3)

21.11(4)

(n)
productivity
Mean
age of 5-V

22.6(7)

23.1(7)

22.13(4)

23.43(5)

21.35(2)

productivity

(n)

50

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

PRODUCTIVITY
AND GRAMMATICAL
FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE

for a given frame varied across four different


children
(Carl,
productive
Elaine, Ned, Sam). Sam was clearly the most variable child, showing the
most productivity
with the V+P frame and the least with the SVO frame.
Table 9 also shows the children's mean age when they achieved
the 3-V
and 5-V levels of productivity
for each frame. All of these mean ages are
than 24 months. The number of children
included is fewer than
younger
seven or eight for the SVO, V+P, and
V-ing frames, indicating that these
frames manifested
less productivity
within the span of the study. The chil
dren also varied in the number of productive
frames (out of five) achieved at
24 and 30 months
(see Table 10). At 24 months, most children had achieved
at least 3-V productivity with the SVand VO frames (Ned left the
study at 20
so we know the least about his
to productivity).
months,
Carrie,
progression
and Stacey achieved 5-V productivity
before 24 months with al
Heather,
most all frames, whereas Carl achieved
less productivity
later, closer to 30
months.
Elaine, Mae, and Sam occupy the middle ground, having achieved
3-V productivity
with four or five frames by 30 months.
Because we do not
have records of the children's uses of their early verbs past the first 10
scores are very
to underestimate
the number
instances, the 30-month
likely
of frames used with multiple
verbs.
Researchers
have proposed
that children's
linguistic relations "go pro
ductive
in free combination
with other
[when] the pattern can participate
lexical items that satisfy its typed variables"
2004,
pp. 188
(Jackendoff,
cannot
be
189). Such "free combination"
usually
unequivocally
supported
from spontaneous
speech data, and we do not claim that we have found it
as operationalized
here. However, productivity
by Ingram (1989) and Shirai
(1998) can be addressed with spontaneous
speech data, and we have found
evidence of productivity
defined
this way in these children's
speech. Well
36 months
before
of age?and
even
before
months
24
of
frequently

TABLE 10
of

Number
At

Ned
Stacey

Frames

24 Months

Name
3-V level

Carl
Carrie
Elaine
Heather

Productive

0
5
55
14 2
5
55
Mae 0 0 4
10
Sam3 3 4
5
45

(Out

of

Five)

At
5-V

level

30 Months
3-V

level

5-V

level

11
5
2
5
4
10
3
4

51
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

children used a number of syntactic


age?the
thus showing that frames are not necessarily

frames with multiple


to specific

restricted

verbs,
lexical

items.

FLEXIBILITY
GRAMAAATICAL
INFIRST
VERBUSES
All eight children used more than one syntactic frame within the first 10
uses of at least one of their verbs. Figures 7a and 7b display for each child
the mean
instance, across verbs, of the first new frame and the percent of
verbs used with more than one frame. On average, the children used 65.6%
of their verbs (SD = 18%) in at least two different frames within their first 10
= 1
uses; their mean number of different frames per verb was 2.14 (range
this early period did not seem to be one characterized
5). Thus,
by frozen

100T71-1
- (a)

&??

90

-7AI

H ^m

^1 ^1 ^H ^H ^1 ^1 ^H ^H
^1 ^1 ^1 ^1 ^1 ^1 ^1 ^1
$3

Sam

Carl

Mae

Stacey

Carrie Heather

Elaine

Ned

Children

10-,-,

(b)

\j i

lllllii.
Sam

Figure
flexibility

Percent
7.?(a)
in early verb use.

Stacey

of verbs

i
Mae

Carl

Carrie

i
i
i
Ned
Heather Elaine

Children
used

with

syntactic

flexibility

and

(b) onset

of syntactic

52

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

AND GRAMMATICAL
FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE
PRODUCTIVITY

form use. For those verbs that showed flexibility, the first new use occurred
on average just after the fourth instance (M = 4.17, SD = 1.47) and within
15.37 days (SD = 12.58) from the first instance. Verbs varied in the percent
of children who used them in different
frames, from 100% (fall, give, take,
wave) to 0% (stop). On average, verbs were used with different frames by 66%
of the children (SD = 23%); the mean number of different frames per verb
was 2.20 (SD = 0.55). For those children who produced
different
frames,
on average before
use occurred
the first different
the fourth
instance
=
17.77 days (SD = 16.96) from the first in
(M 3.81, SD= 1.34), within
stance. Most of these instances of syntactic flexibility
involved the addition
or subtraction of subjects, objects, prepositions,
and locatives. Only 12% of
frame changes
included the use of negation
(range 0-36%). On average,
included negation.
1.8% of utterances
was included in our mea
The addition or subtraction of a preposition
sure of syntactic flexibility; however,
this measure
captured only whether
the preposition
slot was filled. It did not capture whether
the children used
more
than one preposition
with a given verb and/or a given preposition
with more
than one verb. Examination
of the particular prepositions
used
showed that of the six children who used prepositions
with their verbs, five
showed flexibility
in their use of prepositions.
That is, Ned used only one
one
with
verb
but
Mae,
Heather,
Carrie, Stacey, and
(down)
(lay),
preposition
Sam each used at least one verb with multiple
and/or used the
prepositions
same preposition
with multiple
verbs. For example, Mae used put and pull
each with two prepositions
and used in and over each with two different
and down,
verbs, and Heather used put and throw each with two prepositions
to, and away each with two different verbs. Sam was by far the most flexible
use: He used lay, give, pull, push, and put each with at least
in his preposition
two different prepositions
and down and in each with at least two different
verbs.

Consistent
with descriptions
of early combinatorial
speech in English
learners as telegraphic,
these young speakers produced
very little inflec
was
tional morphology,
and, not surprisingly, morphological
flexibility
much
than frame flexibility both across children
less pervasive
(appearing
in approximately
and across verbs (appearing
6% of children's utterances)
in approximately
7% of verb uses). Figures 8a and 8b show the percent of
verbs with which the children used more than one morphological
structure
none of their
and the instance of the first change; two children produced
verbs inmore than one morphological
form. On average, the children used
different morphology
with 15.69% of their verbs (SD = 14.5%). For those
use occurred
on average
verbs that showed flexibility,
the first different
=
before
the fifth instance
26.98 days
(M 4.55, SD= 1.91) and within
=
(SD
21.45) after the first instance. Verbs were used with different mor
=
structures by an average of 18% of the children
(SD
22%); the
phological
53
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

100-n

90

80

3 ?

||
2|

70

60
so

40

il

1
?

oil

Sam

II
i"
Stacey

Mae

i-iM
Carl

i*

Carrie Heather

i-M?t
Ned
Elaine

Children

10-rj

1
Q

ll.
Figure

8.?(a)

morphological

Sam

Stacey

Mae

Carl

ll

Came

Heather

Elaine

Children

Percent
of verbs used
in early verb use.

with

morphological

flexibility

Ned

and

(b) onset

of

flexibility

from 100% of children


(wave) to 0% of children (14 verbs).
range extended
The first new use occurred on average before the fifth instance (M = 4.34,
SD = 2.29) and within 22.78 days (SD = 23.66). Almost all morphological
in the
changes involved the addition of "-ing." A few verbs were produced
was
tense
one
and
with
the
third
wave,
past
produced
(drop,
give, go, fall),
were
more
No
"-s"
verbs
child
with
(need).
person singular
produced by any
than

one

overt

morphological

marker.

(ex
subject flexibility was the norm, with seven of the children
more
one
one
at
than
lexical
with
least
of
Ned)
cepting only
using
subject
their verbs. Figures 9a and 9b show for the children
the percent of verbs
with which they used more than one lexical subject and the instance of the
first new lexical subject. On average,
the children used more
than one
different
lexical subject with 30.65% of their verbs (SD ? 27.15). For those
verbs that showed flexibility, the first new use appeared on average before
the sixth instance (M = 5.2, SD = 1.06) and within 20.29 days (SD = 17.79)
Lexical

54
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

AND GRAMMATICAL
FLEXIBILITY
PRODUCTIVITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE

100-n-1

tS: c

<?

m B
Sam

Stacey

B
Mae

Carl

Carrie Heather

B_
Elaine

Ned

Children
10-rl

fl

fl

?~

3 -B

Sam

Stacey

Mae

fl

Carl

Carrie Heather

Elaine

Ned

Children
Figure
lexical

subject

9.?(a)

Percent
of verbs used
in early verb use.

with

lexical

subject

flexibility

and

(b) onset

of

flexibility

after the first lexical subject use. Verbs also varied in the percent of children
lexical subjects over the first 10 instances, from 100%
different
producing
(wave) to 0 (throw, stop, push, move, lay). On average, verbs were used with
=
lexical subjects by 35% of the children
(SD
26%). For those chil
multiple
new use on average
dren showing this flexibility,
their
first
they produced
=
=
the fifth instance
before
12.74 days
(M 4.77, SD
2.21) and within
=
16.17) after their first lexical subject use.
(SD
All 8 children used more than one lexical object with at least one tran
10a and 10b show for the children
sitive or alternating
verb. Figures
the
percent of verbs with which they used more than one lexical object and the
instance of the first new use. On average,
the children used at least two
lexical objects with 43.38% of their verbs (SD = 26.56%). For those verbs
that showed flexibility, the first different use occurred on average before the
fifth instance (M = 4.13, SD = 1.22) and 20.64 days (SD = 16.22) after the
first lexical object use. Among
the transitive and alternating
verbs (the only
55

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

"3 3

ioo -n-1

?t 901

Sam

Stacy

Mae

Carl

Carrie

Heather

Elaine

Ned

Children
*

10T1-1

8
ll
*>o
00

M
II
H

M
H

?
I
H

I
H

Sam

Stacy

I
H

I
H
Mae

I
^
H

fll
H

I
I

m
I
H

I
H

IH

I
H

IH

IH

Carl

Carrie Heather

Elaine

Ned

Children
Figure
lexical

object

10.?(a)
flexibility

Percent
in early

of verbs used
verb use.

with

lexical

object

flexibility

and

(b) onset

of

in the
variation
verb categories
that take objects) there was considerable
over
10
the
first
lexical
of
different
children
percent
objects
producing
to
tran
On
from
0%
100%
instances, ranging
average,
(stop, move).
(bring)
lexical objects in the
verbs appeared with multiple
sitive and alternating
=
(SD
28%). For those children showing this
speech of 46% of the children
on average before the fifth instance
new use occurred
the
first
flexibility,
=
=
=
SD
and
19.06
16.09) after the first lexical
(Af 4.74,
2.06)
days (SD
object

use.

11 and 12 present means,


standard deviations,
and statistical
measures
verb
subclass.
for
of
the
flexibility by
grammatical
comparisons
a
of
revealed
that
children
and
verbs
produced
heavy
light
Comparison
with
lexical
verbs
than
verbs
of
heavy
light
higher percent
significantly
of light verbs, and
object flexibility, consistent with the broader meanings
a significantly ear
at
in
first
lexical
their
also
change
object
they
produced
lier instance for light than for heavy verbs. The children tended to produce
a higher percent
lexical subject
of heavy verbs than light verbs with
was
not
this
difference
statistically significant. Similar
although
flexibility,
were produced
with
in
the
item
obtained
analyses: Light verbs
findings
lexical objects by a significantly greater percent of children than
different
Tables

56

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

11
TABLE
Grammatical
Use
Flexibility
of
for
Light
Versus
Heavy
Verbs

Mean

SD
Mean
SD
30.5021.34
Light
Heavy

Mean
instance
morphology
change5.133.66
5.73
2.43
Only
3children
contributed
^
t
df*
26.58
16.68
30.66
25.17

20.18
13.04

change
days

3.66
to

frame

change

^7Mean
3.82
0.71
instance
3.54
1.39
0.57
frame
change
ns
5.43

Number
of
days
to
frame
change
15.93
11.58
14.58
13.08
0.6
7ns
g

Only
Only

5.10
0.9
instance
32
1.13
1.23
3.53
1.04
Mean
4.08
frame
change
F=
ns

Cohen's
3 5 children
children

Only

children

contrib

15.31
21.35

NJ Note.?*df
different
with

Number
of of days days
Number
70.37
0.74
different
3.38
1.24
0.22
.01
0.36
with
Vs
Proportion
lexical
objects
O
different

subjects
lexical
Proportion
different
with
Vs
frames
0.72
0.14
0.69
0.21
0.76
7ns
morphology

By
I.children

Mean
instance
lexical
object
change
2.98
0.85
4.8
1.47
-9.19
6.001
1.52
>

contrib
contr

vary because not all children,


experi
verbs,
all
0.23
0.26
0.32
2 0.37
1.01
.02
24
2.24
different
0.26
0.32
0.61
Cs
Proportion
with
lexical
objectsobject
to to
lexical
nor
^-1.73
22.72
days
lexical
617.18
Number
16.43
18.25
of
object
to
change
ns
Number
of
to
days
subject
lexical
change
17.0
19.37
11.1
14.99
0.87
27
ns
m
lexical
different
32
0.34
Proportion
0.19
Cs
0.37
with
0.29
subjects
0.23
ns
r*
0.26
0.20
?0.20
different
32
-0.8
0.23
0.13
0.16
Proportion
with
Cs
morphology
ns0.16
0.75
1.31
.02
2.95
18
-2.54
2.02
5.33
Mean
instance
lexical
object
change
m
change
^
2.87
instance
2.13
19
1.26
5.62
4.04
Mean
change
morphology
ns
morphological
27
Mean
^
instance
2.83
lexical
2.01
4.71
4.94
0.24
subject
change
ns
change
Proportion
different
with
Cs
frames
0.76
0.18
0.67
0.23
1.02
32
ns
n

>//.
verbs
By

Ch

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

- ?

Intransitive
Transitive
Alternating
Flexibility
Transitive,
of
Use
Intransitive,
for
and
Alternating
Verbs
TABLE
12ooGrammatical
df*
p
Mean
SD
Fit
Grammatical
item
Mean
d
Cohen's

frame
instance
3.38
Mean
5.62
2.0
0.85
2.17
3.2
0.46
2,21
change
ns

different
0.78
Proportion
0.75
frames
0.23
0.30
0.60
0.14
Vs
1.35
2,21
with
ns

frame
instance
3.81
1.18
4.26
3.14
2,31
Mean
1.2
3.46
1.01
.04
change

days
frame
14.75
17.2
16.31
0.97
Number
16.14
14.65
2,21
12.84
of
to
change
ns

lexical
2.35
instance
Mean
2.0
5.62
0.87
4.99
5.26
1.01
2,14
subject
change
ns
2.87
3.67
instance
5.65
5.25
Mean
5.0
2.5
2,11
0.05
morphology
change
ns
26.15
36.83
30.42
24.68
Number
19.06
of
days
13.28
to
change
morphological
2,11
0.6
ns14.26
frames
different
0.76
0.77
Proportion
Cs
0.21
0.22
2.02
0.19
0.60
2,33
with
ns
17.17
18.95
Number
13.24
of
25.17
days
lexical
13.75
subject
0.66
to
2,14
change
ns

Note.?*df
different
0.09
0.19
Proportion
with
Vs
0.22
0.26
0.21
0.99
2,21
morphology
nsdifferent
0.29
0.36
0.38
0.28
Vs
Proportion
with
0.29
lexical
0.23
2,21
subjects
ns
lexical
different
0.27
0.32
0.56
Proportion
0.30
2.22
Vs
objects
with
7.06
0.88
5.63
0.8
Mean
lexical
instance
object
3.29
1.87
-2.96
61.63
change
.025

vary

days
14.75
Number
frame
21.13
13.93
19.92
17.37
16.63
0.38
2,31
of
to
change
ns

Mean
instance
morphology
change
4.3
2.99
4.15
1.62
4.62
0.07
2.81
2,18
ns
25.95
22.19
22.15
31.91
22.49
0.11
Number
of
days
19.4
2,18
to
morphological
change
ns
Number
of
days
to
lexical
subject
change
15.28
18.70
15.43
20.66
5.82
7.75
0.71
2,26
ns
lexical
instance
2.61
4.97
Mean
1.94
4.52
2.29
subject
4.8
0.09
change
2,26
ns

because
not all children,
different
0.24
Cs
0.52
0.19
Proportion
with
lexical
0.33
subjects
0.29
0.23
2,33
4.33
.02
different
0.15
4.77
Cs
Proportion
0.14
0.33
0.13
0.10
0.29
.01
2,33
with
morphology
Number
days
lexical
15.77
33.70
12.62
36.12
6-1.62
0.66
object
of
to
.12
change
lexical
different
2.86
Cs
Proportion
0.26
0.30
0.24
0.61
22
with
objects
.01
1.23
lexical
instance
3.41
Mean
1.94
5.83
1.31
18
-3.18
object
.005
change
1.43

verbs,
all
nor
22.55
9.69
14.81
Number
of
18
days
lexical
19.68
object
-1.07
to
change
ns

experienced

%
Cs
with
different
Imorphology:
versus
A:
-2.7,
*(23)
=
.012,
=
pd
=
1.11.
%
different
lexical
Cs
p
with
A:
If(23)
subjects:
2.66,
d
versus
=
.013,
1.10.
Mean
instance
Tframe
change:
versus
A:
=
t(22)
2.67,
.013,
=
pd
=
1.04.

other
All
comparisons
ns.
By
I.
children
//.
verbs
By

C/i

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE
AND GRAMMATICAL
PRODUCTIVITY

were heavy verbs, and the change in lexical objects was seen at a significantly
earlier instance for light than for heavy verbs.
of children's use of transitive, intransitive, and alternating
Comparison
lexical objects with
verbs revealed that children tended to produce different
a higher percent of transitive verbs than alternating
verbs (p = .06); they
earlier in
lexical objects at a significantly
also produced
those different
stance for transitive verbs than for alternating
verbs. Analyses
by verb
a greater number
Intransitive
verbs
of
ap
significant findings:
yielded
con
lexical subjects and in multiple morphological
peared with multiple
in more
than did alternating
verbs. Moreover,
children's
speech
transitive verbs appeared with lexical object changes for more children than
lexical
did alternating verbs; transitive verbs also appeared with a different
a
at
in
earlier
instance.
transitive
verbs
appeared
object
significantly
Finally,
new syntactic frames at a significantly earlier instance than did alternating
texts

verbs. Overall,
then, both transitive and intransitive verbs were associated
than were alternating
with more grammatical
verbs, and consis
flexibility
tent with the arguments
each type of verb takes, intransitive verb use was
more characterized
in the subject slot whereas
transitive verb
by flexibility
use was more characterized
in
the
slot.
by flexibility
object
some types of grammatical
To ask whether
flexibility were displayed
more

than others, two one-way repeated measures


ANOVAs
(by children
as a single factor within
with flexibility
and by verbs) were performed,
lexical subject, and
subjects factor with four levels (frame, morphology,
a
lexical object). The AN OVA by children
effect,
yielded
significant
to discover which
F (3,28) = 7.14,/? = .001. Pairwise t tests were performed
the Bonferroni
correction adjusted the re
categories differed
significantly;
= .05.
a
to
level
.0083
with
this correction,
quired significance
Using
only
one contrast was significant:
frame
Syntactic
flexibility was significantly
=
=
than morphological
7.72,
t(7)
greater
jfr .0001, Cohen's
flexibility,
=
a significant
d
AN OVA
3.04. The
also yielded
effect,
by verbs
=
were
contrasts
Five
F(3,120)
23.11,p<.0001.
significant using the same
Bonferroni
correction: by paired t tests, syntactic flexibility was significantly
than morphological
and
lexical
greater
flexibility
subject flexibility,
Cohen's
and
ds>1.25,
?s(33)>7.25,
ps<.00001,
significantly greater than
lexical object flexibility, ?(23) = 4.23,/? = .0003, d = 0.78. Both lexical subject
and lexical object flexibility
flexibility
(by paired t tests) were significantly
=
=
=
than
4.71, p
.00004, rf 0.71,
?(33)
greater
morphological
flexibility,
=
=
and t(23)
d
1.11, respectively.
5.5\,p<.00001,
In

sum,

the

above

analyses

converge

on

the

conclusion

that

the

eight

in this study all used their first verbs in multiple


syntactic envi
within the first 10 uses of those verbs. Many verbs were used with
different
frames?that
is, with subjects, with objects, in SVO frames, with
and
with
verbs were used with different
prepositions
negation. Moreover,
children
ronments

59
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

and

subjects

as well,

objects,
is, across

That

frames.

the

than

less frequently

although

first

instances

of

verb

in different
more

children

use,

fre

the frame entirely


(via adding or subtracting nouns, Ps,
quently changed
the nouns in the frame. In contrast, much
than changed
and/or negation)
more
at all, let
rarely did the children use their verbs with morphology
children
alone with morphological
these
Thus,
changes.
English-speaking
revealed
the flexibility of their early verb use and, by implication,
the ab
stractness
utterances
needed

of their grammars, more


in the syntax of their verb-containing
is
than in the morphology
of their verb use. Further research
more
to see whether
the same pattern holds in children
learning

rich languages
(e.g., Turkish).
morphologically
The light and heavy verb subclasses did not differ with regard to overall
light verbs were produced with more and
grammatical
flexibility; however,
with
earlier lexical object changes whereas
heavy verbs were produced
more and earlier lexical subject changes. Thus, counter to all versions of a
verb" hypothesis
8c Jisa, 2006; Ninio,
1999),
(e.g., Chenu
"pathbreaking
use and
in
verb
there was no consistent
light
advantage
early grammatical
the light and heavy verbs,
between
flexibility. More detailed comparisons
in
individual
the
variation
children, will be discussed
among
addressing
verb
also
did
VI.
subclasses
The
transitive/intransitive/alternating
chapter
not differ with regard to overall grammatical
transitive
flexibility; however,
in
their
of
verbs exceeded
verbs
alternating
achieving
speed
syntactic flex
amount
of
lexical
and
in
both
and
object flexibility.
achieving
speed
ibility
Intransitive

verbs

exceeded

in amount

verbs

alternating

of morphological

and

lexical subject flexibility.


that the obligatorily
transitive and intransitive
It may seem surprising
flexible"
verbs showed more flexibility
than the "permissibly
alternating
to our definition
verbs. This pattern of results may be partially attributable
of flexibility, which included any change in overt realization of grammatical
that apply equally
such as negation
elements and so also included elements
at
across verbs. More
the chil
of
this
development
early stage
important,
et
and
their
is quite constrained
dren's output
al.,
2006),
(Valian
typically
short

as to whether

unrevealing
(i.e.,

grammatical

from

removed

utterances?especially

tion?were

with

versus

verbs)

alternating

the

direct

context

objects

of

conversa

the

were

(i.e.,

processing

for

omitted

tran

for

nature of direct
It seems likely that the obligatory
sitive verbs) reasons.
to a greater use of objects with those
led
for
transitive
verbs,
then,
objects
in more flexibility. The
verbs than with alternating
verbs, and so resulted
subclasses of verbs reflect
children's different patterns of use with different
the

target

grammar.

alternating

verbs.

(i.e.,

possible

According

lacking

explanations

in children's

flexibility

morphological
minimal

Two

objects)

to

uses

processing

argument

also

exist

of intransitive
demand

structures

for

greater

with

compared

account,

of

the

the

intransitive

more

verbs

60
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

FLEXIBILITY
AND GRAMMATICAL
INEARLY
PRODUCTIVITY
VERBUSE

resources
to selecting
to devote more of their cognitive
to a se
inflections
for
these
verbs.
and
subjects
morphological
According
mantic account, the fact that the intransitive verbs in our data set tended to
verbs explains
the difference;
the
be activity rather than accomplishment
"
former verbs have already been shown to be used more and earlier with
1980; Vendler,
1972; Wagner,
2002). Further
ing" (Bloom, Lifter, 8cHafitz,
to distinguish
between
these explanations.
research is needed
children

allows

FINDINGS
COMPARISON
WITH PREVIOUS
Tomasello(1992)
(1992, 2000)
findings may seem at odds with Tomasello's
his daughter Travis's first verbs and with the Verb Island
of
diary report
on the basis of those data. However,
these and Toma
Hypothesis
proposed
are
sello's (1992) productivity
consistent:
Both Travis
findings
actually quite
and several of the children in the present study produced
the SV, VO, and
SVO frames with multiple
verbs before the age of 24 months
(see Table 8.4
were
in Tomasello,
were used
in
9
verbs
used
the
SV
33
verbs
1992;
frame,
in the VO frame, and 41 verbs were used in the SVO frame). Tomasello
(1992) states, "Travis did indeed begin using word order as a productive
The

above

syntactic device during the 18-24 month period" (p. 247). The findings that
motivated
the Verb Island Hypothesis
did not have to do with the use of
frames with different verbs but with two other findings
(see also Tomasello,
no
Travis
verbs
with
2000, p. 213): First,
produced
overlapping
privileges of
occurrence
same
time
the
and
almost
half of her
second,
during
period,
one third
verbs were produced
in just one construction
and
than
fewer
type
were produced
in two or more. Thus,
in her
individual verbs behaved
speech

as

"islands"

all different
construction.

of

grammar

and because
The

verb

because

their

privileges

each one was restricted


diary

records

collected

for

of occurrence

to being
the

used

present

were

in only one
study

do

not

the first finding regarding overlap among verbs in their


permit addressing
uses (i.e., because we have only 10 instances for each verb), but
they do
at
were
data
odds
with
Tomasello's
that
individual
verbs
provide
finding
limited in the range of constructions
in which they appear. The children in
the present study used their first verbs both with and without
subjects, with
and without

direct objects, with varied prepositions,


and so on. In fact, they
more
one
almost
of
their
in
verbs
than
70%
produced
syntactic frame by the
10th instance of use. What might account for this discrepancy
between our
and
Tomasello's?
One
is
that
both
Tomasello
and the
findings
possibility
present

study

participants

accurately

differ.

characterize

the

participants

It could be that flexibility

in each

study,

and

the

of early verb use varies among


61

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

one extreme
of this variability. An
that Travis represented
is that the different
other possibility
findings reflect the different ways in
which flexibility was coded in the two studies. We consider each of these
in the following analyses. First, we reanalyze the data from our
possibilities
to Tomasello's method
child participants
and compare the results
according
with those obtained
Travis.
this exercise
led us to our
from
Performing
second analysis, which involves a more detailed coding of argument use by
children

verb

and

subclass.
One

basis

for Tomasello's

characterization

of Travis's

grammar

as

con

islands comes

from his analysis of the arguments Travis used


taining verb
with her verbs. Tomasello
found that Travis was most likely to use her verbs
or with only a single argument.
utterances
either in single-word
Impor
struc
tantly for the claim that individual verbs did not appear in multiple
as being the same
utterances
treated all single-argument
tures, Tomasello
sort of structure
cannot

be

on the grounds

that children

to have

over

assumed

control

at this point

word

order.

in development

Thus,

"draw

dog"

structure
in grammatical
different
would not be considered
(verb+noun)
the same analysis to the present data,
from "I draw" (noun+verb).
Applying
we coded all 10 instances of our children's verb uses for whether
they ap
or holophrastically,
utterances
in
frames
argument
treating
single
peared
with arguments
in either the subject or object position as equivalent.
The results are shown in Table 13, together with data from Travis. We
of Tomasello
recoded Travis's data from the Appendix
(1992), using only
instances of the 34 target verbs of the present
study. The data are not
because Tomasello
did not consistently
report all to
perfectly comparable
use.
This inconsistency,
kens of single verb
though, could only have the
result

vanced

of

Travis

making

appear

than the children

rather

more,

in the present

than

less,

study. Higher

grammatically

percents

indicate

TABLE 13
Percent

of Verbs

Used

With

One

Argument

or

in

Holophrases

Child

%
79

Carl
Carrie

37

Elaine

84
52

Heather

53

Mae

Ned

86

Sam

90

Stacey
Travis

50
50

62

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

ad

that

FLEXIBILITY
VERBUSE
INEARLY
PRODUCTIVITY
AND GRAMMATICAL

The findings
verbs are "island like" or unproductive.
again reveal
to
Tomasello's
the
substantial variability: Applying
coding
subjects in the
a
in
which
the
four
of
present
present participants
study yields
picture
less flexibility
than Travis, using over 75% of their verbs in
demonstrate
frames or in single-word
three children
look
utterances;
single argument
or
to
of
their
verbs
in
about
half
similar
Travis, using
very
single-argument
one
more
us
child
looks
flexible
than
Travis,
single-word utterances;
only
or
utterances.
ing only 37% of her verbs in single-argument
single-word
of verb use.
Thus, Travis does not appear to be an outlier on this dimension
to
seven
in
of
Tomasello's
the
children
the present
criteria,
According
eight
to
et al.,
in
the
be
McClure
also
"verb
island"
also
appear
stage (see
study

more

2006).
treating

Beyond

ical structure,
we

would

there

argue

all

aspect
to

erroneously,

as

utterances

single-argument

is another

the

of Tomasello's

same

grammat

that leads,

procedure

that

conclusion

the

are

children

not

pro

in their early verb use. That is,Tomasello's


analysis did not take into
the differing requirements
of the individual verbs. His assessment
of whether
verbs are produced with multiple
and his inference
arguments
not
assume
of limited grammatical
when
do
that the verbs
they
knowledge
not.
In
all require multiple
But
do
intransitive
arguments.
they
particular,
to view a child who uses subjects but
verbs require only subjects; therefore,

ductive
account

not
over,

come

with

objects

of

many

the

or

verbs

go

as

is
More
arguments
inappropriate.
as transitive,
as draw,
are
such
regarded

"missing"

Tomasello

in

transitive and intransitive frames (Le


reality verbs that alternate between
use
such
the
that
of
these
verbs with just one argument
is per
vin, 1993),
can
a
For
"I
draw"
be
fectly appropriate.
example,
fully specified
if the context makes what is drawn obvious, and "draw dog"
description
can be a fully
if the context makes
ob
the addressee
specified command
more

The

vious.

revealing

is to compare

therefore,

analysis,

the

children's

to
argument use for each verb subclass (transitive, intransitive, alternating)
see if the children are using their arguments
for
the
different
differently
subclasses.
14 presents,

Table

ances

by

verb

class,

the

of

percent

that are verb only, verb plus one argument

or

such

arguments

source/goal
word
garding

order),

and

verb

as
plus

"look
two

ball"
or more

(including
and

utter

verb-containing

subject, object,
and

store,"

"go

arguments.

It

disre

is apparent

in these data that the use of holophrases


is high, particularly with intran
sitive and alternating verbs and that the frequency of one-argument
frames
one third of all uses) for all three subclasses.
is similar (approximately
as predicted,
the use of two-argument
frames is highest with the
However,
transitive

and
as

frequent

seemingly

the

verbs;

alternating
one-argument

anomalous

moreover,
frame

for

the

2-NP uses found with

the
two-argument
transitive
verbs.

intransitive

is as

frame
(Note

that

the

verbs arose because


63

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE 14
Percent

of Verb

Uses

Produced

in Different
Frame

Child/Subclass

1 Argument3

Holophrase

Frames,

by Verb

Subclass

Type

2 Arguments3

3 Arguments3

Otherb

Carl

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

0 40 70
64.3
82.31

0
27.14
5.38
4.62

0
na
na

0
8.6
0

10
64.44
10
30.83
49.2

4.44
na
na

0
11.11
0

0
42.85

0
na
na

0
0
0

Carrie

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

0
14.44
21.67

85.5

Elaine

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

45
20 35
57.14
51
41 8

Heather

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

3.75
24.44
25.38

48.75
43.75
0
47.77
50
22.31

1.25
na
na

2.5
32.2
10

Mae

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

35.56
24.44
11.25
12.5
30.77 29.23

11.11
na
na

5.56
6.25
0

10

0
na
na

0
0
12.85

42.2

41.1
6.67

10

50

4.3
31.43

na

14.3

na

11.3

0
na
na

0
10
16.67

12.22
70
32.3

Ned

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

65
305
97
30
77.14

Sam

Transitive
Intransitive

Alternating

0 9.3

79.3

Stacey

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

2 6632
47.1
35

042.9
11.67
36.67

Travis

Transitive
Intransitive

Alternating

10

64 22

33

25 na

na

14

78 3

na

2.1

2.26
10.31
6.35

43

Mean

Transitive
Intransitive
Alternating

16.89
53.05
50.51

39.4139.19
3.35
33.85
29.04

13.33

required objects, recipients, and locations.


Note.?aSubjects,
vocative uses (e.g., "Mom, come").
bAdverbs, prepositions,

64

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
AND GRAMMATICAL
VERBUSE
PRODUCTIVITY

their prepositions,
of uses of look and sit that were missing
e.g., "Grandma
look doggie" and "Dolly sit chair.")
The critical question for the Verb Island Hypothesis
these
is whether
in the number of arguments
data indicate systematic differences
appearing
with subcategories
of verbs that would be predicted on the basis of the adult
grammar. If children's grammars are lexically based, then children have no
of verbs. If, however,
category VERB and, by implication, no subcategories
children's very early verb uses show differences
related to verb subclass, this
of underlying
verb subclasses in children's
implies the existence
linguistic
our
from
data set, we do not know if these go
representations.
(Admittedly,
the input given.) This hypothesis was tested in a 3 (verb type) x 3
beyond
ANOVA, which yielded a sig
(number of arguments)
repeated measures
nificant interaction, F(4,28) = 7.46, p = .0005. Pairwise contrasts using the
Bonferroni
correction
(p< .005) revealed that within the intransitive verbs,
utterances

holophrastic

ment
were

were

utterances, Fs(\,7)>
used holophrastically

more

frequent

than

17, jfrs<.004, Cohen's


with less frequency

either

one-

ds>0.73.

or

two-argu

Transitive

than either

verbs
or

intransitive

ds> 1.35; the transitive verbs also


verbs, Fs(l,7)>
alternating
16,jf?s<.005,
to appear
in significantly more
tended
frames than the
two-argument
=
=
In
d=
the
intransitive
1.72.
sum, under
15.22, p
.006,
verbs, F(l,7)
all
of
the
children?even
Travis?demonstrate
knowl
appropriate
analysis,
in frames privileged
edge of the difference
by transitive and intransitive
verbs from the beginning
of their use of those verbs (see also Goldin
Meadow,

2003,

TheManchester

and McClure

et al., 2006,

for similar findings).

Corpus

The degree of syntactic flexibility observed


in this study is also greater
than that reported
in Lieven and colleagues'
studies of their Manchester
et al., 2001, 2004).
corpus (Lieven et al., 2003; Pine et al., 1998; Theakston
in particular, are at odds. First, the degree of lexical subject
Three findings,
flexibility with intransitive verbs in our data is considerably
higher than that
et
Pine
al.
for
their
25%
for ours;
36-52%
(1998);
reported by
namely,
study,
see Table
et al. (2004; Table 8) reported
12. Second, whereas Theakston
syntactic flexibility on the order of two frames per verb in their children
within, on average, 6 weeks (thus including many more instances of a given
verb than we could), we find comparable
numbers of flexibility within, on
It seems likely that we found more and earlier
average, just over 2 weeks.
grammatical
flexibility because the present data were, to use Lieven et al.'s
term, "denser." That is, every instance produced by our children (within the
first 10 for a given verb) was recorded; moreover,
children's utterances were
across a wide range of situations. As discussed
recorded
in chapter
I,
on
situation-restricted
to
make
relying
sampled
speech
judgments
65
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

toddlers'

concerning

grammatical

yield underestimates

may

productivity

of

productivity.
Lieven et al. (2003; see also Lieven, 2006) make a further claim that is
also relevant to the present analyses of the diary records. Lieven and col
are not
combinations
leagues have claimed that children's early verb+noun
a
indications of grammatical
because
number
of
these, in
flexibility
large
involved substitution of only one word at a time. For example,
or "Bring cooky" and then "Bring
saying "Doggie go" and then "Kitty go,"
as
not
to
count
Lieven et al. because a change of only
juice" does
flexibility
one word could simply indicate flexibility at the lexical rather than gram
matical
levels. In contrast, in the coding scheme used in the present study,
both of the above examples would have been counted as changes (in lexical
subject or object) and thus counted as indicators of flexibility. We have two
their corpus,

to Lieven

counters

et al.'s interpretation
is practical: Children

of this type of flexibility. The first


in the toddler years are at a stage of
in terms of length; they only produce
if the
1973; Valian et al., 2006). Thus,

counterargument
utterance production
that is limited
two to three words at a time (Bloom,
verb stays the same, only one other word could change over the course of
a time would be the only way
sequential utterances. Changes of one word at
these children could show syntactic flexibility, because of output constraints
not

and

because

one

from

vary

of

that young

finding

matical

than

of

absence

abstract

to
an

the

another

in the number
is a

next

Put

grammar.

are limited

children

utterance

rather

limitations

the

discovery

independent

that can
of

consequence

logical

their

regarding

the

way,

of words

length
gram

knowledge.

comes directly from the diary records. As


second counterargument
in the present
earlier in this chapter, the children
described
study were
more
to
frame
the
demonstrate
(i.e.,
syntactic flex
entirely
change
likely
or
which
included
and/or
nouns,
Ps,
negation)
subtracting
adding
ibility,
lexical subject or
than to change the nouns in the frame (i.e., demonstrate
individual children also demonstrated
lexical object flexibility). Moreover,
went beyond just changing nouns
that
considerable
flexibility
grammatical
in a given frame. The first example of such flexibility comes from one of the
she said "I coming"
"star" children, Heather. Early in her verb production,
to Lieven et al.,
and "Come here" within 30 days (starting at 18 months);
substitution and not real flexibility.
this might be coded as just a one-word
The

she

However,

dropped
"Take
users:

followed

"Drop,"

"I

"I

drop,"

span of time;

23 months

Heather

said

such
Carl

was

were

demonstrations
one

of

our

slower

not
learners,

by (on the same day) "Gavin walk,"

showing
a

a bite,"

yet

he

"I

syntactic
of

property
also

and "I don't

word

multiple

comprise

"take

"I

and

something,"

drop

these

this" all in the same day, clearly

Furthermore,
verb

said

the same
at

Moreover,

changes.

and

also

it" over

take

bite,"

flexibility.

the
only
said "I no

"star"
walk"

like" followed

66
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

by

FLEXIBILITY
PRODUCTIVITY
AND GRAMMATICAL
INEARLY
VERBUSE

(on the same day) "I like cheese." Both of these couplets involved changes of
more than one word, and thus would count for grammatical
flexibility even
on

et

Lieven

al.'s

We

ations/contexts.

set

data
would

we

above,

argued

grammatical

our

because

corpus

As

criteria.

more

demonstrated

flexibility

includes

argue,

believe

speech
in addition,

a wider

from

the

that

our

that

children

in the Manchester

than those

of

range

situ

scheme

coding

ap

biases the
corpus (and that used by Tomasello)
plied to the Manchester
et al.
That
little grammatical
Lieven
results toward finding
is,
productivity.
claim to have discovered
that children are not productive
in
and Tomasello
is not, however,
of
their use of verbs. This discovery
their
independent
to credit the child with little grammatical
coding decisions. Having decided
on an utterance-by-utterance
basis, it is not surprising that they
knowledge
then find

that the children


overall.

demonstrate

little grammatical

in

knowledge

their speech

CONCLUSIONS
In sum, the data presented
in this chapter support our contention
that
we
the
learners?at
have
in
studied
such
least,
English
eight
in their verb use from its very
detail?demonstrate
grammatical
flexibility
onset. Overwhelmingly,
the first 10 instances of these 34 common
verbs

American

frames. Moreover,
these first 10 uses show sensitivity to
appear in multiple
verb subclass, in that transitive verbs appear less frequently as holophrases
and tend to appear more frequently
than intransitive verbs in frames with
two

use

arguments.

in which
is an

young
"island"

each
These

unto

verb
data

of

descriptions

appearing
do

not

itself.

The

learners

data

children's

in its own

suggest

learners have underlying

language

even very young


their
of
input language.
in which

contradict

data

with

environments.

syntactic
verb

These

as restricted,

are more

are acquiring

an

account

of

grammars
consistent

early

limited

the grammatical

of

acquisition

in which

with

verb

range

an

each

account

patterns

67
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

VI. DIFFERENCES
INEARLY
VERBGROWTHAND USEAS A
FUNCTION
OF DEVELOPMENTAL
AND VERB
PERIOD,CHILD,

In

V,

chapter

we

presented

analyses

that

some

revealed

grammatical

in the first verb uses of eight children. All chil


and flexibility
productivity
dren used at least one syntactic frame with multiple
verbs; three children
used all five coded frames with multiple
verbs. All the children used at least
some of their first verbs with grammatical
flexibility, and each of these early
was
some children during the win
at
verbs
used
least
acquired
flexibly by
dow of this study. Within
that picture of early and pervasive flexibility, there
was variability among children and among verbs in the degree of
flexibility
It is also possible that there was variability across the duration of
observed.
in the analyses presented
thus far. In
the study that has been unexplored
this

chapter,

we

children,

among

interrelations

provide

examine
and

among

further

differences

among
the

verbs

measured

tests of the theories

among
in more
properties

of

periods
detail,
of

and

we

children's

of verb development

development,
examine
verb

we have

the

use

to

consid

ered.

DOES FLEXIBILITY
OF VERBUSEVARYBYPERIOD
OF VERBDEVELOPMENT?
to the conservative-child
in chapter I,
described
hypothesis
According
are
at
later
is
the
combinatorial
first
pos
grammar
lexically specific; only
in a grammar with an abstract category of
sibilities of verbs represented
VERB
1999, 2007; Tomasello,
2004; Ninio,
2000). The
(MacWhinney,
are
most
used
with
frames
and most
that
verbs
present findings,
multiple
verbs within the first 10 instances of verb use, are
frames used with multiple
It is still possible, however,
that there is a
inconsistent with this position.
such
in the abstractness of children's grammars
progression
developmental
than verbs that
that verbs that are learned first are used with less flexibility
are learned later. To address this proposal, we go back to the data to test the
verb use, suffi
that there is an early stage of nonproductive
hypothesis
68
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INEARLY
DIFFERENCES
VERBGROWTH

TABLE 15a
Age

Children's

for

(Months)

Each

Developmental

Period

Child
Carl

Period
1

20-21

Heather

Sam

Mae

16-17

19-20

Elaine

19-20

20-21

Carrie

Stacey

18-19

16-17

2 ends

23.5

22.5

19.5

22.5

23.5

21.5

19.5

3 ends

25.5

24.5

21.5

24.5

25.5

23.5

21.5

4 ends

27.5

26.5

23.5

26.5

5 end

29.5

28.5

25.5

28.5

6 ends

31.5

30.5

7 ends

33.5

32.5

Ned
17-18
20.5

TABLE 15b
Children's

MLU

for

Each

Developmental

Period

Child
Period

Carl

Sam

Mae

111
1

1.05

1.93

2.18

2.68

1.65

1.17

1.71

3.95

2.71

2.06

2.7

4.37

3.08

3.67

2.3

3.8

3.31

2.2

4.04

3.45

Ned

1.98

5.08

2.52

Stacey

2.58

1.15

= mean

Carrie

1.48

Note.?MLU

31

Elaine

2.32

2
4

1.2

Heather

5.72
length of utterances.

brief that it was obscured


and flexible
by the overall productive
of verb use during the long window of this study.
We analyzed each child's verb use across his/her participation
in the
We
child's
of
divided
each
into
span
study.
participation
developmental
as follows: Period 1 included each child's first 6 weeks of
periods,
partic
of the study,
ipation and thus, given the participant-selection
requirements
their very first verbs. Period 2 included each child's next 8 weeks of par
ticipation, Period 3 included the next 8 weeks, Period 4 included the sub
Some children
sequent 8 weeks, and so on to the end of their participation.
ciently
nature

seven periods
(Carl, Mae) participated
(see Table
15a), others
through
five
others (Elaine, Carrie,
(Sam, Heather)
participated
through
periods,
three periods,
and one (Ned) participated
Stacey) participated
through
two
It
to
is
through
periods.
important
point out that because we have
records only for each child's first 10 uses of each verb, the later periods
include data only from later-learned
verbs and not from additional uses of
69
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

? 70%
- IP

ll

iim mi

iiu

Period

Period

1(7)

2(9)
Period

Figure

early-learned

11.?The

verbs.

Period

^m ' Multiword

?& ^

iH

in

Period

Period

Period

Period

3(10) 4(14)

5(20)

6(22)

7(24)

of Development

development

Thus,

^|

the

asm?'e
frame

(Total Target Verbs)


of syntactic

crucial

flexibility

question

in newly

concerns

learned

whether

verbs:

Carl.

the

first

10 instances of later-learned
verbs consistently display more flexibility
than
the first 10 instances of early-learned
verbs. Tables 15a and 15b present the
for each
children's ages and mean length of utterances
(MLUs), respectively,
in which they participated.
of the periods
The MLUs are estimates, calcu
utterances
in the diaries.
lated using only the verb-containing
recorded
Figures 11 through 18 present each child's timeline individually; Figure
in a single figure. The figures are ordered begin
19 presents all children
for the greatest number of pe
ning with those children who participated
for the fewest
riods, Carl and Mae, and ending with Ned, who participated
11, Carl began with seven verbs in his first
periods. As shown in Figure
period and added new verbs slowly during his next two periods. All of his
verbs in these first three periods were in single-word utterances
and hence
not used grammatically
(gray bars). During his fourth period, he
flexibly
utterances
began to use verbs in multiword
(striped bars); however, both
were
one
verbs
frame (subject-verb
[SV]). It was during his
only used in
fifth period, when he was between 27.5 and 29.5 months of age, that he used
two verbs both in multiword
utterances
and in different
frames within the
first 10 instances (black bars). The frames included SV, verb-object
(VO),
and SVO. Thus, Carl began to show grammatical
flexibility during Period 5.
Mae, shown in Figure 12, displayed a similar although not identical pattern.
Mae began using two verbs in Period
1, then added eight more verbs in
Period 2 but only eight more verbs in Period 3. Unlike Carl, throughout
70

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

VERBGROWTH
DIFFERENCESINEARLY

1 80%|
1 %-i

? 60%-p
50%~

*
I I
I 40%"
^II i I
I
i^ 30%1
I
I
20%1I 1
, ill
0% I
| 111 I
Period

Period

1(2)

2(10)
Period

Figure

12.?The

these three periods


first

10

instances.

I
I
^
1
P
I, K^

._,
Multiword

I l I
I^H I^1 I^|

I
l
II "^ i y/ft IH

| | |

| I
I22? H
I I
P
I^,
I,

utterances
in

frames
multiple

0Multiword
u~swith

I
lH
I

asin?le
trame

rsingie-word
utterances
'-'

I
III
I^,
I,

Period

Period

Period

Period

3(12) 4(20)

5(27)

6(30)

7(31)

Period

of Development

of syntactic

development

she used
However,

(Total Target Verbs)

some verbs
as was

the

in newly

flexibility

inmultiword
case

for

Carl,

learned

utterances
each

verb

verbs: Mae.

within

her
in

appeared

one verb appeared with a preposition


(P)
only one frame: During Period 1,
and during periods 2 and 3, verbs appeared only in the SV frame. During
Period 4 (i.e., one developmental
period earlier than Carl), she used three
utter
utterances
and in different
frames of multiword
verbs in multiword
ances within
the first 10 instances. Frames of SV, VO, SVO, and verb
(VP) were all represented. Mae thus began to show grammatical
preposition
of age. Sam,
24.5 and 26.5 months
flexibility
during Period 4, between
this general pattern of (a) restricted use
shown in Figure 13, also displayed
utterances
for their first 10 instances during
of his target verbs inmultiword
use begin
use and multiframe
1-3 and then (b) mostly multiword
Periods
at
in
4.
the
At
Sam
used
his
verbs
Period
his
Period
SV,
SVO,
VO,
4,
ning
and VP frames. The timeline data from these three children (Carl, Mae, and
some support for a weaker version of the conservative-child
Sam) provide
in restricted frames (either single
First
verbs were produced
hypothesis:
word

utterances

or multiword

utterances

using

only

one

frame);

later

verbs

were

in flexible frames.
produced
not all of the children followed this pattern. Heather
However,
(Figure
in their
14) and Carrie
flexibility
(Figure 15) demonstrated
grammatical
verb use starting at Period 1. Both girls produced many verbs even during
in the study (n = 16 and 13, respectively),
their first 6 weeks of participation
utterances
half of these verbs in multiword
and they used approximately
and

in different

frames

within

the first

10 instances

during

Period

1.
71

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

100% -i

vm

vm

, 90%A |
-%1
? 70%-i
n
fc

| I
ill

j?gv
IB
j%

60%

HI

~
?>50%
II
HI
II
II r?3
Bl
|l|
H 40%
111
|

Wk
*o
H HI

Ml

30%"
S 20%1
?

Period

Period

Period

1(3)

2(9)

3(20)

4(29)

2 70%1 1111
I I
c*
,:]|
gg
g

60%-

-p
3 50%
p

?
!?
I
1":

Figure

5(31)

of syntactic

%Z

I
I
I

I
1
I

I
I

I
I
I

I
I

Period

Period

Period

Period

Period

2(22)

3(29)

4(31)

14.?The

development

in newly

flexibility

of Development

01Single-word
utterances

(Total Target Verbs)

Period

with
frame

Period

1(16)

Multiword

utterances
a
single

utterances in
multiple frames

of Development

development

Multiword

H
H

r
77/ ,
Zvy Wfk

Period

13.?The

._.

P;%> I^fl

II
II

Period

H^1

I
ll

III

Figure

learned

verbs:

Sam.

I ,_,Multiword
utterances in
multipleframes

P 0 Multiword

r;:?r
???
\

5(31)
(Total Target Verbs)

of syntactic

flexibility

in newly

learned

verbs:

Heather.

Heather
and Carrie used the SV, VO, SVO, VP, and V-ing frames all during
Period
flexible use continued
1, and grammatically
through each child's
next two periods. Neither
Carrie nor Heather
of
any evidence
provides
restricted verb use in terms of grammar, even for their very first verbs. They
72
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

VERBGROWTH
DIFFERENCESINEARLY

100%-.

90%-III
^1

^ 80%
o

70% _

^H

^fl
^H

^fl
^H

^1

^1

^1

^1 ^1

^1

^|

utterances
with
B^ 40%- S I I a singleframe
^^|

30%

Period

1(13)

Figure

15.?The

(Total Target Verbs)

of syntactic

I|

gg

I
-

'd
20%

10% IIII

II-1

pIH

i-1

Period

three

ered

development

point

Carrie.

%60% I multipleframes
[I P

H
Multiword
utterances
with
I ? 1 single frame
I 40%a
30%"
1
I I I?Si?gle-word
utterances

II

///.

rcfl

i-1-1

Period

3(19)

of Development

the ending

conservative-first

i-1

2(15)
Period

also reached

Hi

Period

1(3)

16.?The

III

verbs:

Multiword
utterances in

M
%6
H|

III
? fly

gz

Figure

learned

.-,-

S 50%

o% I

in newly

flexibility

^|

utterances

3(28)

of Development

development

90%I|
m%]
80%| 70% l|l
?

^| |

Period

2(23)

Period

ra Single.word

-p
20%

Period

I-;

utterances
frames
multiple
0 Multiword

~ ^|
^|
^|
-I ^| ^|
II

^0%
50%

fc
?

Multiword

(Total Target Verbs)

of syntactic

flexibility

of the study more

in newly

quickly

learned

verbs:

Elaine.

than two of the

children.

Elaine, Stacey, and Ned (Figures 16-18, respectively) may be consid


with respect to their speed of reaching grammatical
"intermediate"
73
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

100%-. _

g 80%- p

jj&f

||

Y/z\?PM 20%
B

Hn <u

rvv>j

io%-1

0%4-B?,-,_
Period

17.?The

i-1

i-1-1

3(20)

of Development

development

Multiword

Period

2(19)
Period

utterances in
H H multiple frames

with
5340%- utterances
III
a
frame
HH
single
t ^|
^H
30%n Single-word
III |
utterances
^1
"

_,-1

Period

1(9)

Figure

g2 60%-

Is

50%-

I Multiword

I^
1I 70%00
^|
^|

(Total Target Verbs)

of syntactic

in newly

flexibility

learned

verbs:

Stacey.

100%-i

o 90%"
I I
80%"

1=-:-1
I|

c/,

111
H

Is 60%"
||
1

2 70%
H

frames
multiple

- 50%
III

Multiword
utterances in
0

Multiword

utterances
with

40%- IH
I
?HI

|
0% I
*io%-1

,-,-

Period

20%-

a single frame

U I D Single-word
utterances
I w

-1-,-,

Period

2(14)

1(10)
Period
Figure

YM
1_'-'

30%"

18.?The

of Development

development

(Total Target Verbs)

of syntactic

flexibility

in newly

learned

verbs:

Ned.

frames within the first 10


flexibility of verb use: Each used 1 verb inmultiple
instances during Period 1. Elaine and Stacey both used their one multiframe
this
1 in the SV, VO, and SVO frames. Also during
verb during Period
one
two
frame
in
within
multiword
additional
verbs
used
just
period, Stacey
the first 10 instances (Elaine used none in this way). Leaps in grammatical
or Period 3 (Elaine).
flexibility were then displayed during Period 2 (Stacey)
74
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

DIFFERENCESINEARLY
VERBGROWTH

100
90-

-i-^--??-II
o

80| 70J
a 60"
c 50-

/
/ A

17

^ ^ ^. /
'

>r

jf i

Sam

Carrie
--*-Heather

~ /1

II I
y40
""*"""Elaine
|
/ / / /^f/l -"H"~~Mae
/
W

\ /
V

/
0 -I?It?i
16-

Carl
//
/' ^Ned

//

*-r*

^ 30 ///
20?

>.

J*

/ \ /

--*?Stacey

18-

i-r?i??
20-

22-

i X24-

19

21

23

25

'?i-1-1-1-1
2628-

30-

32

27

31

33

29

Age
Figure

19.?Change

in multiword

use by age.

than Carl, Mae, or Sam; however, their


they showed earlier flexibility
was
across
as
a number of verbs as was Carrie's
not
wide
flexibility
displayed
or Heather's.
Ned participated
for the fewest periods and, like Carl, Sam,
and Mae, used his first 10 verbs in only restricted single-word or multiword
utterances
1. During
for their first 10 instances during Period
Period 2,
new
two
in
he
four
verbs
and
used
of
them
multiword
though,
produced
utterances and in different frames within the first 10 instances. Ned's frames

Thus,

during Period 2 included SV, VO, and SVO.


to grammatical
that these children
The different pathways
flexibility
can be seen together
in Figure
the
19. Figure
19 displays
demonstrated
utterances
in different
in multiword
cumulative percent of verbs produced
the first 10 instances for each child across periods. As the
frames within
consider
and Ned produced
figure shows, Carrie, Stacy, Elaine, Heather,
utterances
in different
Periods
able multiword
frames during
2 and 3,
whereas Sam, Mae, and Carl reached these levels only during Periods 4-6 of
their

verb

use.

in the
children's
analysis of individual
developmental
changes
with
used
verbs
reveals
which
grammatical
flexibility
they
newly acquired
that the majority of the children, although not all children,
showed gram
even the
matical flexibility
close to the very onset of verb use. Moreover,
child who was slowest to achieve grammatical
Carl,
flexibility,
displayed
"slow" learner,
such flexibility
before the age of 2.5 years; Sam, another
before 24 months
of age.
grammatical
flexibility
actually demonstrated
and
Carrie
demonstrated
Heather,
Ned,
grammatical
flexibility be
Stacy,
This

75
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

fore 20 months
of age. Both these developmental
patterns and the ages of
are
at odds with those re
the first grammatical
demonstration
flexibility
other
who
claimed
that
there is little gram
have
researchers,
ported by
are
to
matical
children
close
until
2.5
years of age and that
flexibility
children's
verb
2000,

if not exclusively,
is predominantly,
et al., 2006; Tomasello,
2004; McClure

initial use of sentence

frames

specific (e.g., MacWhinney,


p. 213).

TOGRAMMATICAL
FLEXIBILITY?
VERBSPATHBREAKERS
ARE LIGHT
Previous

comparisons

of

the

grammatical

environments

surrounding

III and V)
first verb uses (in chapters
light and heavy verbs in children's
more
with
direct
revealed that light verbs appeared
significantly
frequently
in
and
verbs
and
frames
than
verbs
that
SVO
light
appeared
objects
heavy
earlier and more frequently with different direct objects than
significantly
evidence for the claim
did heavy verbs. These
findings might be considered
of abstract
that light verbs function as facilitators of children's acquisition
sentence
E.
&
frames
Clark, 1987; Gold
(Chenu
Jisa, 2006;
phrase-level
8c
2005b;
1999, 2005a,
Casenhiser,
2006; Ninio,
1999; Goldberg
berg,
the
the
Rice
8c
because
verbs,
Pinker, 1989;
Bode, 1993)
among
target
light
verbs

in

this

study,

did

seem?overall?to

show

more

and

earlier

direct

for light verbs


the finding of greater flexibility
object flexibility. However,
was limited to the direct object slot; light verbs did not show greater gram
matical flexibility
overall
(Table 14). Furthermore,
light verbs may have
more varied direct objects not because of
and
with
more,
earlier,
appeared
of light verbs are general
any syntactic privilege but because the meanings
the direct objects is necessary for communicative
and thus expressing
pur
or
event
is
to
the
child
relation
that
the
talking
poses
specify
sufficiently
et al., 2004).
about (e.g., Snedeker & Gleitman,
2004; Theakston
that these averaged data missed potential path
It is possible, however,
individual
that
functions
light verbs might have served for indi
breaking
vidual children. As listed inTable 1, the light verbs among the target verbs in
this study were fairly diverse, and some of these (go, put, want) have figured
than others
more
of the pathbreaker
in proposals
hypothesis
heavily
2006; Ninio,
1999; Goldberg & Casenhiser,
(Chenu & Jisa, 2006; Goldberg,
such potential functions for each
1999). The following analyses investigated
each light verb that each child produced.
child individually,
considering
we investigated whether
et
individual light
al.
Theakston
(2004),
Following
in a wider range of different frames than verbs with more
verbs participated
the
that were acquired at the same age. We compared
specific meanings
frame flexibility of each light verb with (a) the heavy verb that appeared just
76
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

DIFFERENCES
INEARLY
VERBGROWTH

TABLE 16
CARL's

Verb

Light/Heavy

for

Comparison

Frame

Flexibility

Age

(Days)
Preceding

Light

Verb(s)

Verb

1st

Following

Use

Verb(s)

Push658
GO680
715

Kiss

Sit 808

COME

of Frames

Number

10th Used in 10
Use

Instances

680
680
715

1 (V)
1 (V) Go
1 (V) Kiss

(Frame)

Examples

Push

Me

sit, No

808

2(SV,

800
Cry
814

800
814

2 (V,SV)
1 (V)

Cry, Baby cry


Come
Cut,

Cut

paper,

Mama

cut

Cut,
Give

Cut

paper,

Mama

cut

me,

Sissy

noV)

827

827

Cut 827

827

GIVE828

828

3 (V, VO, SV)


3 (V, VO, SV)
2 (VIO, SVIO)

861

1 (SVO)

Cut 827

827

838

840

3 (V, VO, SV)


2 (V, V locative)

Cut

Hold

861

LOOK

861

1 (SVO)

Like 911

911

928

932

2 (SVO,
2 (SVO,

861

Hold
WANT

932

932

me

give

I hold (flash)light
cut
paper, Mama
Look punch bag

Cut

Cut,
Look,

I hold (flash)light
SV-neg

O)

I don't

3 (SVO, VO,

Me

V)

like,

want

Me

SV+S)

to do
Wash

sit

I like cheese

it, Me

want

it

wash

table, Wash

floor, Wash
=
=
=
=
Verb; S
subject; SV
subject-verb; VO
verb-object;
talized verbs are highlighted
relative to "regular" verbs.

Note.?V

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

before this light verb's first use and (b) the heavy verb that appeared just
after this light verb's first use. We also looked across the range of earliest
verbs used by each child to see which verb(s) first appeared with which
frames.

Given

that

our

data

set

was

verb

centered

rather

than

frame

centered,
though, we are unable to address the related question of whether
the very first uses of a frame appeared with light verbs. The findings are
in Tables
16-23, described below in detail for Carl and summa
presented
rized for the rest of the children.
Carl produced
six light verbs. Each was preceded
in development
by a
more
none
verb
was
Table
and
of
the
verbs
(see
16),
heavy,
specific
light
more flexible in its use than the
verb.
Moreover,
just-preceding
heavy
light
verbs were not consistently
the first ones to be produced
in new construc
at 800 days, was Carl's 13th verb but the
tions. For example,
cry, produced
first verb used with a subject. Cut was the first one used with an object, and
hold was the first one used in the SVO frame. Carl's light verbs were the first
to be used in the verb 4-locative (VL) (inchoative motion)
and VOL (caused
motion)

frames/constructions;

however,

his "first-in-frame"

light verbs were


77

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE 17
CARRIE'S

Verb

Light/Heavy

for

Comparison

Frame

Flexibility

Age
(Days)
Number
Preceding
Verb(s)

Light
Verb

Following

1st

10th Used

Use

Use

Push

564

574

Wash

565

583

Verb(s)

of Frames
in 10

Instances

(Frame)

568

582

3 (V, SV, VO)


3 (V, SV, VO)
3 (V, V L, SVL)

569

644

3 (V, SV, SV-ing)

Cry 590
593

601

2 (SV, SV-ing)
2 (V, SV-ing)

GO
Walk

Examples
Push, Arty push, Push mommy
I wash, Wash my hands
Wash,
Go, Go bye-bye, Daddy go work
Walk, Mom walk, Doggie
walking

COME
Run

Open
WANT
Kiss
Hold
LOOK
Pull

BRING
Wave

TAKE
Wave

Roll
GIVE

594

603

585

600

593

626

595

616

604

622

608

625

622

660

628

660

PUT 644

670

See
Need

603

652

679

Need 652

679

659

683

672

691

2 (SV, SV-ing)
3 (V, SV, VO)
1 (SVO)
3 (no V, SV, VO)
3 (SVO, neg, -ing)
2 (V, V locative)
3 (SVO, neg, -ing)

No kiss, Kiss baby, Daddy


No Mom hold it, Daddy

SVing)

I bring my baby
Dad wave, I wave

679

659

681

1 (SVO)
3 (Neg VO,

672

691

NegSVO)
3 (SV, SVIO,

687

709

3 (SVO, SVP, SVing)

My waving
My roll my hands, Wagon
rolling down, all rolling

690

708

3 (VOP, SVOIO,

Give mom

No

SVO,

SVing)

VO)

take it, No my

take my nap,

Daddy take my sippy


Dad wave, I wave at A,

hug, I give my
to A, Give that back

after

V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb-object;


relative to irregular verbs.
talized verbs are highlighted

Note.?

at A,

My waving
My need my binka

Need 652

produced
Give

kiss

it
holding
Look, Look my shoe
No pull my shirt, Daddy
pulling it

cookie
No verbs were

open

My see doggie
put shoes
Mommy
My need my binka
My need my binka

1 (SVO)
1 (SVO)
1 (SVO)
1 (SVO)
1 (SVO)
3 (SV, SVIO,

Baby cry, Arty crying


Come, Granny coming
Arty run, My running
teeth, Daddy
Open, Open
I want my daddy

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

the ones predicted: Look was his first verb to be used in the VL frame
(lookpunch bag) instead of the predicted go, and give was his first verb used in
frame (give me) instead of the predicted/^.
the V(0)L
Admittedly,
given the
of
of go and his 1st production
time elapsed between Carl's 10th production

not

78
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

INEARLY
VERBGROWTH
DIFFERENCES

TABLE 18
ELAINE'S

Verb

Light/Heavy

for

Comparison

Frame

Flexibility

Age

(Days)
Preceding

Light
Verb

Verb(s)

1st

Following

Use

Verb(s)

WANT
LOOK
Open
Sit

10th
Use

of Frames

Number
Used

in 10

Instances

(Frame)

See

658

672

Wash

Open
I sit

2 (V, SV)

I see

See,

716

1 (V)

Roll

696

716

696

741

3 (V, VV, SV)


2 (SVO, VO)

Come,
I wash

Roll 695
COME

Examples

I want veggies
Go, I go
Look

602 613 1 (SVO)


GO613 710 2(V, SV)
633 634 1 (V)
651 651 1 (V)
653 656 1 (SV)

V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb-object;


Note.?
talized verbs are highlighted
relative to "regular" verbs.

Come
hands,

roll, My come
Wash hands

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

look, it is probably not the case that his VL production with look was his very
first in that frame; however, this caveat does not hold for his 1st production
of a verb in the VOL frame. That is, when Carl produced give me he had not
even one instance of put, bring, or take-, therefore, it is very likely
yet produced
that give me was one of his first, if not his very first, use of the VOL frame.
Carrie, Ned, and Heather each showed a similar pattern to Carl's, in that
(a) none of their light verbs was used more flexibly than concurrently
pro
duced heavy verbs and (b) only some frames were used first with light verbs,
whereas others were used first with heavy verbs (Tables 17, 19, and 21). For
Mae, Sam, and Stacey, some proportion
(one quarter to two thirds) of their
light verbs were used more flexibly than their concurrently
produced heavy
verbs; however, they were no more likely to produce specific frames first with
the predicted
light verbs (e.g., VL with go or VOL with put) (Tables 20, 22,
and 23). Note that the first 10 uses of make were (voluntarily) recorded by
Mae's

and

Stacey's

mothers;

make

has

been

proposed

as

pathbreaker

verb

for the VO frame, yet itwas neither the earliest nor the most flexible verb in
this frame for either child. In fact, only Elaine followed the predicted pattern,
such that both her first uses of grammatical forms and her first flexible uses of
such forms appeared with light verbs (Table 18). These data, then, corrob
orate those of Theakston
et al. (2004), who found no consistent advantage
in
grammatical flexibility for light verbs at this early point in development.
Table 24 presents a summary for seven commonly
frames,
appearing
whether
they were first used by the children with light or with heavy verbs.
for the light verbs here either, as
Clearly, there is no consistent advantage
were
as frequently
at
verbs
the
least
for the S, O, -ing,
heavy
pathbreakers
79
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE 19
HEATHER'S

Verb

Light/Heavy

Comparison

for

Frame

Flexibility

Age
(Days)
Number
Preceding
Verb(s)

Light
Verb

Following

10th

Used

of Frames

in 10

Use

Use

Instances

584

629

Cut

585

590

2 (VO, SVO)
1 (V)
Go

585

590
590

593

634

595

629

1 (V P)
1 (V P)
2 (SV-ing, V locative)
4 (V, SV, SVO, SVedO)

Sit down

585

589

708

593

701

4 (V, SVO, VO, SV)


4 (V, V locative,

Push

588

600

SVP, VOP)
2 (V, VO)

Drop

595

629

Wash 593

652

595

624

611

615

618

631

633

653

634

652

Verb(s)
Cut

GO
Sit
Sit
COME
Drop
Kiss
PUT

WANT
Like
Need
LOOK
Eat
Throw

1st

669

(Frame)

Examples
this, I cut this

Sit down
I coming, Come
Drop, I dropped

here

it, I drop
I kiss it,Mommy
kiss, Kiss that
Put here, Put top on, I put on

Push, Push this


4 (V, SV, SVO, SVedO) Drop, I dropped
it, I drop
2 (VO, V)
Wash, Wash it
I want it, I don't want this
2 (SVO, SV+negO)
Like this, I like this
2 (VO, SVO)
2 (VO, SVO)
2 (V, V locative)
3 (V, SV, SVO)

701

5 (SVO, SV, V P,
SVP SVOP)

Me need

this, need this


Look, Look here
eat, Heather
Eat, Heather

eat this

I throw that, throw away

GIVE

687

708

5 (VO, VIO,
VPP, SVPP SVOPP)

I threw down, I throw it away


M give it, Give me, Give to D,
A gave to me

TAKE

701

701

Wave

702

827

2 (VO, SVO)
3 (V bye, V PP VL)

A gave coin to me
Take a bite, Daddy take this
Wave bye, We need to wave

Stop

710

710

1 (V-I-gerund)

to D, I waved M

V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb-object;


Note.?
talized verbs are highlighted
relative to "regular" verbs.

Stop eating

the flowers

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

first appear with light verbs;


and P frames. Some frames did consistently
the pathbreaking
verbs that were observed were not consistently
however,
the first one used
the ones predicted. That is, a light verb was consistently
for the VL frame; however, for only two children (Carrie, Stacey) was this, as
was come (one
the pathbreaker
go. For the other four children,
predicted,
or
a
was
used
look (three children). Similarly,
child)
light verb
consistently
first in the VOL frame, but for only one child (Heather)
this was the pre
was give (two), take
the pathbreaker
dicted put. For the other five children,
or
a
stative rather than action verb, was the
(two),
bring (one). Finally, want,
first verb produced
in the SVO frame for six of our children; need, another
80
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

VERBGROWTH
DIFFERENCES
INEARLY

TABLE20
MAE's

Verb

Light/Heavy

for

Comparison

Frame

Flexibility

Age
(Days)
of Frames

Number
Preceding
Verb(s)

Light
Verb

Following
Verb(s)
Push

COME
Throw

1st
Use

10th Used

in 10

Use

Instances

1 (V) Push
1 (Von)

577

589

616

625

624

630

1 (V)
1 (V)

(Frame)

Examples

Come

on

Throw

654

669

GO 661

684

673

756

1 (V)
2 (V, SV)

Run 731

750

2 (V, VP)

Cut 740

819

757

757

742

776

3(V, SV, VO)


1 (V loc)
3 (V, SV, V-neg)

765

898

5(VO, VOP,
SVOP SVO)

727

733

3 (V, SV, VO)

Open,

Open
Me want

Eat
Fall

LOOK
Cry
Pull

Eat
Push
fall
Fall, Mommy
Run, Run away
Cut, Me cut, Cut it
Look me
Cry, Jill cry, Don't cry
Pull feet, This one pull
off, Me pull it over,
Me pull

Open
WANT

it

Mommy
this

open,

769

838

3 (SVO, VOS-bar,

765

898

5 (VO, VOP, SVOP, SVO)

773

777

791

834

2 (SVO, SV)
1 (SVO)

Me hold her, Me hold


Me bring rock

789

820

3 (VO, SVO, NegVO)

Me need

795

863

3 (VOP, SVOP, SVO)

Drop 802

818

2 (SVO, VO)

812

856

4 (VP, VOP, VOPP,

2 (SVO, S-neg VO)


2 (SVO, S-neg VO)
3 (SVO, SVOIO, SVOPP)

Pull
Hold
BRING
Need
TAKE

PUT
Like

833

910

Like 833

910

840

928

GIVE

VO)

cheese, Want
Panda watch me, I want that
Pull feet, This one pull off,
Me pull it over, Me pull it

that, No need
that plate
Take that off, Me take
my pie out, I take it

SVPP)

Drop my Teddy, Me drop


my Bandaid
Put on, Put me over,
Put me up Jill's table
I like that, I don't
I like that, I don't

like pears
like pears

ring to Jill,
give Jill a toy
I made, Me made tower
Me wash it, wash my hands

Me give my
Me

MAKE
Wash

851

880

855

868

3 (VO, SVO, past)


2 (SVO, VO)

V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb-object;


talized verbs are highlighted
relative to "regular" verbs.

Note.?

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

desire verb, was the SVO pathbreaker


for the seventh child, and cut or hold
for the eighth.
The finding that the particular light verb used first in a frame varied across
children

is contrary

to

two

crucial

parts

of

the

light-verbs-as-pathbreakers

81

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE21
NED's

Verb

Light/Heavy

for

Comparison

Frame

Flexibility

Age
(Days)
of Frames

Number
Preceding

Light
Verb

Verb(s)

1st

Following

10th

in 10

Used

Use

Use

Instances

Roll
519

549

COME
526

549

2(V, Ving)
2 (V, V P)

526

580

549
Lay
Like
556

611

Verb(s)

Bite

593

580

Examples
Roll, Rolling
Come, Come

1 (V)
Bite
2 (V, V P)
2 (V, VO)

568

WANT
578
Need

(Frame)

Lay, lay down,


Like, Like it
Want some

1 (VO)
2 (VO, SVO)

615

a cookie,
a pen

Need
needs

Note.?V
highlighted

=
=
Verb; S
subject; VO
verb-object;
relative to "regular" verbs.

SVO = subject-verb-object.

on

lay back

Pop

verbs are

Capitalized

verb's trans
targets the pathbreaking
hypothesis. One part of the hypothesis
to
to
is
enable
children
conceive
of or to
which
parency of meaning,
proposed
8c Sethuraman,
That
abstract the frames (Goldberg, 1999; Goodman
is,
2006).
children learn how go encodes inchoative motion, or put encodes caused mo
tion,

or make

involves

caused

action,

and

then

deduce

the

from

verb's

meaning

that go is to be followed by locatives, make is to be followed by object names, and


put is to be followed by object names and locatives. From these "semantic
frames VL (or VPP), VO, and VOL (or
frames," they abstract the grammatical
and
all
be considered equally representative
VOPP). Bring, take,give,
put might
of the caused motion
construction VOL, such that the variation here is not
a
look could not possibly be considered
particularly problematic. However,
transparent
look

involves

representative
a more
abstract

of

the
sense

inchoative

motion
with

of motion,

construction
someone's

VL,
gaze

because
being

di

to
toward a ground rather than an object or person actually moving
Mae's
and
Sam's
&
ward a ground
1985;
Gleitman,
Levin,
1993).
(Landau
innovative use of the VL frame first with look seems inexplicable in terms of an
argument based on semantically transparent underpinnings.
rected

verbs is
variability with the SVO frame pathbreaking
If se
for
the
light-verbs-as-pathbreakers
hypothesis:
similarly problematic
of
children's
ab
is
the
action
of
transitive
mantic
transparency
instigator
as Hopper
and
straction of the SVO frame, then want hardly qualifies;
(1980) and others have discussed at length, verbs such as make or
Thompson
The

push,

which

children's

capture

caused

changes

of

state

or

position,

are

better

candidates.

et al, 2004) have already grappled


Ninio (1999) and others (e.g., Theakston
so early in the SVO frame
with the inconsistency of verbs like want appearing
82
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

DIFFERENCES
INEARLY
VERBGROWTH

TABLE22
SAM's

Light/Heavy

Verb

for

Comparison

Frame

Flexibility

Age
(Days)
Number
Preceding
Verb(s)

Light
Verb

1st

Following

10th

Used

of Frames
in 10

Use

Use

495

566

1 (V)

GO 531

567

2(SV, V)
1 (V)

Verb(s)
Open
Cut
Cut

LOOK
Eat
Eat
COME
Hold
Drop
WANT

540

583

540

583

553

592

569

660

Instances

(Frame)

Open
Go, I go
Cut

1 (V) Cut
1 (VPP)
1 (V)
1 (V)

Examples

Look

at me

Eat

569

660

576

602

571

607

602

783

4 (V, SV, VO, VPP)

632

722

3 (SVO, VO,

Eat

1 (V)
1 (V)

Come
Hold

SVS-bar)

M drop, Drop
Drop in tub
I want
I want

Roll

644

711

602

783

634

663

644

711

Need 646

674

664

739

Drop
TAKE
Roll
BRING

Lay
PUT
Run

cup, Want
to go

2(V, VO)
4 (V, SV, VO, VPP)

Roll ball, Roll


M drop, Drop
Drop in tub

1 (V)
2 (V, VO)
2 (VO, SV)

Take

4 (VO, VOP,

Bring

diaper,

that, Bring my
plate over, No bring,

673

705

3 (V, VO, negVO)

679

739

3 (SV, SVP, SVPP)

694

732

697

750

2 (V P, VOP)
5 (V, Ving, SVPP,
SV, VPP)

Note.?
V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb-object;
talized verbs are highlighted
relative to "regular" verbs.

cup,

Roll ball, Roll


I need, Need this

negV, Ving
Like

diaper,

Bringing
Like those, M like,
Don't like you
Teddy
lay, Lay down,
Lay down with me
Put away, Put that back
I run in water

Running,

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

but not encoding highly transitive actions, and


they have proposed different
resolutions. Ninio
that an even more abstract
(1999) suggested
("prototyp
the subject's/agent's
relation is more
ical") notion of transitivity, in which
facilitates children's abstraction of the SVO frame, and Ned's use
prominent,
of need fits this notion. However, Carl's use of cut does not (cut
clearly changes
the patient and ismore similar to the
and Thompson
version); thus,
Hopper
there is still individual variation unaccounted
for.
83
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE23
STACEY's

Verb

Light/Heavy

Age

for

Comparison

Light
Verb

Verb(s)

1st

Following

Use

Verb(s)

10th
Use

of Frames
in 10

Used

Instances

(Frame)

529

1 (V) Sit

491

498

495

624

2(WhSV,
V)
5 (V, Ving,

503

533

VingO)
1 (V)

512

611

2(VO,

516

611

503

533

2 (Ving, SVing)
1 (V)

COME

518

600

TAKE

520

595

Sit 491
GO
Eat

Open

Run
Open

Hold
LOOK
Jump

Nanny go? Go
Eat pizza, I'm eating, I eat it

Where

Open
I want juice, Want
some more

SVO)

Running,
Open
Come

3 (V, V P, SV P)
2 (VO, VOP)
2 (VO, SVO)

540

593

572

621

559

584

2 (V, V PP)
4 (Ving, SVing,

581

SVed, V)
2 (SVing, VO)

555

Wash

Examples

SVO,

SVing,

WANT

Flexibility

(Days)
Number

Preceding

Frame

PUT

572

598

MAKE

598

675

1 (VOP)
3 (SVO,

(5th)

VO,

Hold

in, You come out


it, Take it off
that duck, I hold this

Look,

Look

Take

in mirror

Kitty jumping,

Kitty jumped

Mommy's
washing,
Wash my hands
Put it on, Put phone
Daddy make it,
making messes

-ing)

V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb-object;


Note.?
talized verbs are highlighted
relative to "regular" verbs.

Girl running

back

SVO = subject-verb-object.

Capi

The

second
part of the
light-verbs-as-pathbreakers
hypothesis,
between
the hypothesis
resolution
of the inconsistency
potential
frames is highly
and the data, is that children's abstraction of grammatical
on
with
which
the
particular
light verbs appear
frequency
dependent
and

in

variety

abstraction
or

put

encodes

in

frames

of

the

caused

that go is frequently
followed by object
frequently
names

and

locatives.

Then,

That

is,

an

alternative

learn how go encodes


or

motion,

observe

object

input.

is that after children

make

followed
names,
over

involves

caused

action,

by locatives,
and put is frequently
hearings

motion,
they

next

(or want) is
followed by

make

numerous

of

process

inchoative

of

these

verbs

with a wide variety of locatives and object names in the relevant positions
in the input, the children
abstract
the VL
(or VPP), VO, and VOL
not rely on the meaning
does
latter
This
frames.
(or VOPP)
procedure
but does rely on the frequent
the abstraction
of the verb to enable
(with varied lexical items) of a single verb in a given frame
appearance
84
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

DIFFERENCES
INEARLY
VERBGROWTH

TABLE24
First

of Grammatical

Uses

Forms

by Verb

for

Subclass

Each

Child

Child
Grammatical

Form

SVCarrie,
Elaine*

Heather,

VO
Heather*,

Sam*,
Carrie

VP
Sam,
VL Carl
Mae
VOL Carl
Mae

Sam,

First Use With

Verb

Stacey

Elaine*

SVO Carrie*,

"-ing"

a Light

First Use With

Heather*,

Elaine*,

Mae*

Carl,

Carrie,

Mae,

Ned

Carl,

Carrie,

Mae,

Ned,

Sam,

Stacey
Ned

Carl,

Verb

Heavy

Stacey*
Heather
Carrie,
(come)
Stacey, Ned
(look), Carrie
(come)
(go), Heather
(look), Sam (look), Stacey
(go)
(give), Carrie
(give), Heather
(put)
(take), Sam

(bring), Stacey

Heather,

Mae,

Ned

(lay)

(take)

Note.?*Child's
first use was with the verb want.
V = Verb; S = subject; SV = subject-verb; VO = verb?object;

SVO = subject-verb-object.

et al., 2003; Chenu


8c Jisa, 2006; Goldberg,
(e.g., Casenhiser
8c Sethuraman,
Goodman
8c Tomasello,
2006; Kidd,
Lieven,
et al., 2004).
Theakston

1999;
2006;

This frequency-based
does indeed account for the predom
procedure
inance of want as the children's pathbreaker
for the SVO frame; however,
it encounters
in accounting
difficulties
for the occurrences
of look or
come

as Mae's,

and

Heather's,

Sam's

pathbreakers

for

the VL

frame,

because

look and come are much


less frequent
verbs than go in children's
input
and output (Naigles 8cHoff-Ginsberg,
the highly differential
1998). Moreover,
of put, bring, take, and give (put appears
2.5-3
frequencies
approximately
times more frequently
than the other verbs; Goldberg,
8c
1999; Naigles
in
of
and
children's
make
1998)
corpora
Hoff-Ginsberg,
caregivers'
speech
this process
less viable as an explanation
of children's acquisition
of the
so
VOL frame/construction:
is
in
the
VOL
construction, why do
Ifput
frequent
Sam, Stacey, Carrie, Mae, and Carl all use other verbs with their first VOL
frame?
In other

words,

the

semantic

transparency-based

version

of

the

light

hypothesis may account


but it does not account

for the children's first uses of


for their first uses of the VL
version of this hypothesis may ac

verbs-as-pathbreakers
the VOL construction,
and SVO frames. The frequency-based
count for the children's first uses of the SVO frame, but it has difficulties
for their first uses of the VL and VOL frames. Our data, then, do
accounting
not support the
as a process
pathbreaking-verbs
hypothesis
by which chil
dren

learn

to abstract

sentence

frames.

85

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

HOWAREDIFFERENT
MEASURES
OF VERBDEVELOPMENT
AND USERELATED?
to generativist
that underlies
theories, the abstract grammar
is
the
that
children's
autonomous,
language productivity
yielding
prediction
use
to the
of
flexible
verb
should
be
unrelated
development
grammatical
use
or
a
to
of
flexible
verb
the
of
verb
development
growth
semantically
lexicon (e.g., Chomsky,
includ
1981). Other theories of verb development,
results from domain-general
ing theories that grammatical
development
According

theories

processes,

learning

that

grammar

from

emerges

the

lexicon,

and

the syntactic bootstrapping


hypothesis,
predict that the acquisition of a verb
of semantically flexible verb use, and the devel
lexicon, the development
flexible verb use should all be positively
related
opment of grammatically
across children. The syntactic bootstrapping
pre
hypothesis
additionally
are
dicts that the grammatical
with
which
different
verbs
used
flexibility
should be related to the semantic flexibility with which those verbs are used
because
semantic understanding
of a verb is based on syntactic analysis of
verb's

that

environments.

One way to investigate these hypotheses


with the current data might be
to compare the degree and onset of flexibility demonstrated
by the children
in the different domains. Although
of
it is not a given that our measures
are
so
and
semantic,
(i.e.,
syntactic flexibility
directly comparable
pragmatic,
as a change in syntactic frame?),
is a change in actor of the same magnitude
we

flexibility

syntactic

of

number

t tests

pairwise

performed

using

between

four

number

instances,

of

days,

affected

actor,

addressee,

indices

of flexibility
of

percent

(percent
and

verbs)

object,

and

of children,

the

Bonferroni

to set alpha at .0083. Of all 16 comparisons,


only 1 reached
significance: Children were significantly more flexible with their affected
=
= .0018. Im
object use than with their syntactic frame use, ?(7) 4.83, p
correction

none

portantly,

viding
earlier
A

of

the

no evidence
than
better

time-related

reached

comparisons

for the notion

pro

significance,

in one domain

that flexibility

emerges

in another.
way

to address

these

proposals,

we

feel,

is to examine

intercor

and use. To reduce the num


relations among properties of verb development
on
we
of descriptive
focused
selected measures
ber of correlations calculated,
and

theoretical

interest,

and,

where

possible,

we

created

composite

scales.

OF CHILDREN'S
VERB
AMONGMEASURES
INTERCORRELATIONS
AND USE
DEVELOPMENT
measures
were
Composite
use
to
verbs and
dren started

created to index the age at which the chil


the semantic and grammatical
flexibility of

86
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

DIFFERENCES
INEARLY
VERBGROWTH

TABLE25
Intercorrelations

Among

of Children's

Measures
for

all

use

composite

4. Number

composite
flexibility
of verbs at 21 months

5. Number

of frames

6. Number

of productive

frames
*p<.05

5.

.042

.527+

Use

-.456

.749**

.624*

.537+

?
? ?

at 21 months

(one-tailed),

4.
-

3. Grammatical

Note.?+^?<.10

and

3.

2.

-.670*
?

begins

flexibility

Development

(n

= 8

Analyses)
1.

1. Age verb
2. Semantic

Verb

6.
-.189

.110

.586+

.222

.796**

.119

.839**

.183
?

at 24 months
(one-tailed),

**p<.01

(one-tailed).

their first 10 uses of early-acquired


verbs. The scales were created as follows:
was
use
at
verb
which
the
average of two measures?the
Age
age of first
begins
verb use and the mean age of first use of all the target verbs the child used in
a for this scale was .75. Semantic
the window of this study. Cronbach's
flex
use
was
measures:
verb
the
of
three
the percent of verbs used
average
ibility of
with different actors, the percent of verbs used with different affected ob
jects, and the percent of verbs used to refer to different actions (Cronbach's
oc= .89). Grammatical flexibility
of verb use was indexed by the average of four
measures:
of
verbs
used
with different frames (excluding vocatives),
percent
of
verbs
used
with
different
percent
percent of verbs used with
morphology,
different
lexical subjects, and percent of verbs used with different
lexical
=
of
.87). We also included two snap-shot measures
objects (Cronbach's a
at 21 months,
children's verb lexicons and verb grammars
which was the
oldest age at which we have data for all participants:
the number of target
verbs in the child's vocabulary and the number of frames (out of 5: SV, VO,
SVO,

VP,

productive
frame

V-ing)

in the

child's

utterances.

Last,

frames each child had at 24 months,

criterion.

For

each

correlation,

we

expected

we

counted

employing
that

the

of

number

the 5-verbs-per

if the measures

were

related they would be positively related, except that age would yield a neg
ative correlation
because a younger
of verb use is a
age at the beginning
of
indicator
verb
all
tests were
therefore,
positive
development;
significance
one-tailed. The correlation matrix
is presented
in Table 25.
The correlations
of children's verb use in Table 25
among measures
that children who began verb use at a younger
age showed more
in their first verb uses, and they also tended to have
semantic flexibility
at 21 months
larger verb vocabularies
compared with children who began
verb use at an older age. The semantic flexibility with which children used
their first verbs was also significantly related to the grammatical
flexibility of
their verb use, to the size of their verb vocabularies
at 21 months,
and
to
the
of
number
at
frames
had
months.
24
(marginally)
productive
they
reveal

87
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

TABLE26
intercorrelations

among

verbs'

of use

properties
of Verbs

in early

1.
1. Age

onset

of

child

speech

2.

4.

3.
.122

-.277+

children

3. Percent

using

children

4. Percent

using

children

5. Grammatical
Note.?+p<A0

using

flexibility
(one-tailed),

to refer
with
with

different
different

of use
*p<.05

to different

actions

actors
affected

.100

5.

-.036

(28) (34) (25)

2. Percent

(number

in Analysis)

.373+

.336*

(34)

.294+

(28) (19)

(28)
.349*

.020
?

(25)

(34)
.307+

objects

(25)

?
composite
(one-tailed).

at 21 months
verb vocabularies
showed a marginally
with
correlation
overall
grammatical
significant positive
flexibility of their
first verb uses and a significant positive
correlation
with the number
of
at
frames
had
months.
The
24
they
grammatical
flexibility of
productive
children's verb use also was positively related to children's number of pro
that flexibility of use is, in fact, a
ductive frames at 24 months,
suggesting

The

good

size of children's

index

of

grammatical

productivity.

AND USE
OF VERBS'DEVELOPMENT
AMONGMEASURES
INTERCORRELATIONS
of verbs, as they are
To analyze the intercorrelations
among properties
of grammatical flexibility
first used in children's speech, a composite measure
was created by averaging
four measures:
the percent of children using the
one
more
more
inflec
with
than one morphological
than
verb with
frame,
one
more
more
one
lexical
and
with
than
lexical subject,
than
tion, with
=
indices of semantic flexibility did
.79). The multiple
object (Cronbach's a
not form an internally consistent scale?as Table 26 shows, the three indices
of semantic flexibility were not significantly related to each other. Thus, the
were entered
the per
into analysis individually:
three measures
following
cent of children using the verb to refer to more than one action, the percent
than one actor, and the percent of
of children using the verb with more
children using the verb with more than one affected object. The final mea
sure of how verbs appear in children's speech was the average age at which
the
the verb first appeared. As was the case for the by child correlations,
predictions

were

that positive

indicators would

be positively

related,

and all

88
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

DIFFERENCESINEARLY
VERBGROWTH

tests were one-tailed. The correlation matrix


in
is presented
significance
Table 26.
The correlations
of how verbs were used within these
among measures
first instances of children's verb production
show that later-appearing
verbs
an
to
to
with
of
verbs
older
tended
be
used
children
fewer
onset)
(i.e.,
age
by
refer to varied actions and were used by significantly more children with
grammatical
flexibility There were also trends toward the number of chil
dren using a verb to refer tomultiple
actions being positively associated with
the number of children using that verb to refer to different affected objects
and negatively associated with the number of children showing grammatical
flexibility of use. Verbs used by more children with different actors were also
used by more
The

children

correlations

verbs were

with grammatical
with

age

of

onset

flexibility
indicate

that,

on

average,

early

used with

grammatical
appearing
flexibility by fewer children
than later-appearing
verbs. Such correlations might appear to be consistent
with hypotheses
that grammar
later, and thus perhaps as a con
emerges
Bates
of
lexical
8cGoodman,
1999). However,
sequence,
development
(e.g.,
these correlations were only significant in the by-verb analysis; across chil
to their
dren, the age at which children began to use verbs was unrelated
the analyses
of individual
grammatical
flexibility.
composite
Similarly,
of
earlier
in
described
this chapter,
growth patterns by period
development,
indicated that some of the children used their very first verbs with gram
matical flexibility. This appears to be a case in which averaged data (in this
case average properties
of verbs rather than of individuals) present a pic
ture that is true for some individuals but not for others (see Molenaar,
2008,
for discussion).

CONCLUSIONS
In this chapter, we looked at the grammatical
flexibility of early verb use
in more detail, asking whether
the overall grammatical
flexibility we ob
served might obscure a limited, very early period of less flexible verb use
and whether
the lack of average differences
between
light and heavy verbs
obscure a pathbreaker
role played by some light verbs for some
might
children. We found that a small number of children did manifest
conser
vative before

flexible verb use, but the majority of children demonstrated


to the beginning,
close
if not at the beginning,
of verb use. We also
flexibility
a light verb was the first to appear with a
found that sometimes
particular
in syntactic
syntactic frame, but that sometimes
heavy verbs appeared
frames before light verbs. These findings
that early
support the conclusion
flexible verb use is widespread
and not limited to a late stage of develop
89
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

ment

or a particular
out that
kind of verb. These
findings also pointed
not
of
do
reflect
all
average developmental
necessarily
descriptions
paths
individual developmental
paths.
We also asked whether different aspects of early verb use were related.
Direct comparisons
of the degree and onset of grammatical,
semantic, and
indication
of
differences
these
little
between
pragmatic
flexibility
yielded
domains;

the

however,

were

correlations

more

informative.

Across

children,

grammatical
flexibility and semantic flexibility of verb use were related to
each other; similarly, verbs showing actor flexibility with more children also
children. These
showed grammatical
flexibility with more
findings are
lexical
about the interrelation
between
consistent with many hypotheses
reflect
the
of a
The
correlations
effect
and grammatical
may
development:
cor
common
common underlying
of
and
the
effects
and/or
ability
input,
effects of syntactic under
relations may also indicate mutually
supportive
standings

on

and

semantics

versa.

vice

The

that

hypothesis

grammar

a particular
threshold
size received only
emerges
a
the
correlation
between
weak
very
significant
support?in
marginally
and their overall
number of the target verbs children used at 21 months
we found that those verbs
flexibility of verb usage. Although
grammatical
were
at younger
with
used
that appeared
ages
flexibility by
grammatical
once

fewer

do not

the lexicon

on

children,

average,

interpret

who

children

flexible

in their verb uses


children

were

than

this as reflecting

because

those

reaches

who

verb

began

began

those

use

than children
to

use

verbs

verbs

earlier

were

later

less

later. We

showed

we

ages,

of verb

period
not

who began
earlier

at

acquired

a pregrammatical

use

grammatically

did find that

greater

semantic

verbs may be used with less grammatical


flexibility. Thus, earlier-learned
seman
but
earlier
children are likely to be more
verb-learning
flexibility,
in
the
of the
discussion
these
flexible.
We
consider
again
findings
tically
theoretical
implications of these data.

90
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

VII.GENERALDISCUSSION

is children's first uses of their first verbs.


the study of verb development
has bur
of early verb comprehension
geoned,
including experimental
investigations
and verb learning,
studies of production
using samples of spontaneous
a
one
and
of
child's
speech,
early verb use (e.g., Fisher & Gleit
diary study
8cGolinkoff,
man, 2002; Hirsh-Pasek
2006; Lieven, 2006; Tomasello,
1992;
The topic of this monograph
In the past decade and a half,

8cMerriman,
Tomasello
1995). There have not, however, been studies of
in the very earliest stage of verb production
several children
that would
a
more
use
can
a study
of
verb
than
provide
generalizable
early
description
of a single child. This absence of detailed documentation
of the beginning
of
verb use was a significant gap in the data on verb development
because
there

are

disputes

the

regarding

nature

of

early

language

representation

and, indeed, the nature of the language acquisition process addressable


by
was
data on how children use their very first verbs. The present
study
to fill this gap.
designed
of any verbs, eight mothers
Starting before their children's production
kept records of their children's first 10 uses of 34 target verbs that the
literature suggested
should be among children's first verbs. For
previous
each instance of verb use, the mothers
in which
recorded
the full utterance
and also noted whether
the verb appeared
the utterance
the
containing
verb

served

the

function

of

a command

or

who

description,

or

was

what

the verb action, and who or what was the object of the verb
performing
action. These diary records were coded to provide measures
of the prag
uses. Our
and
of
children's
first
verb
matic, semantic,
grammatical flexibility
discussion
of these data and of their theoretical
is
implications
organized
around the following questions:
1. To

what

flexible
2. To

what

flexible

extent

are

in pragmatics
extent

are

children's

early

verb

uses

conservative

versus

verb

uses

conservative

versus

and semantics?
children's

early

in grammar?
91

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

3. To what extent is early flexibility particularly


some (i.e., light, or
verbs?
pathbreaking)
are the flexibility

4. To what extent
are

5. What

the

theoretical

of meaning
of

implications

semantic, and grammatical

flexibility

the

characteristic

of only

and form related?


observed

of children's

pragmatic,

first verb uses?

ARECHILDREN'S
EARLY
TOWHAT EXTENT
VERBUSESCONSERVATIVE
VERSUS
FLEXIBLE
INPRAGMATICS
AND SEMANTICS?
Pragmatic Flexibility
data of this study suggest that children's early verb use is prag
at least some of their first
flexible. All of the children produced
matically
verbs in both commands
and descriptions,
and the majority
of the verbs
were used as both commands and descriptions,
same
the
child
(and these
by
two functions captured >98% of the childrens recorded utterances). Thus,
to be completely
context bound in
neither any child nor any verb appeared
The

use

and,

ation

in mental

inference,

by

verbs

among

terances,

not

variation

by

such

no

commands;
can

verb

some

that

be

There

representation.
were
verbs
were

verbs

used

used

in

only
in

descriptive
commands.

the

verbs'

verbs

Furthermore,

addressees,

that

suggesting

the

as

used

children

were

commands
had

ut
The

inherent

and the suitability of these meanings


for the command
meanings
For all of the target
is not a sensible command).
(e.g., "Need"
are plausible
which both commands
and descriptions
functions,
within
the first 10
both commands
and descriptions
produced
recorded.

vari

however,
in

only

to differences

attributed

was,

function
verbs for
children
instances

with

used

of

representations

multiple
verb mean

we
actors. Although
ing that were sufficiently abstract to apply to multiple
did not explicitly assess the situations in which children used their verbs, the
fact that the verbs with the highest number of addressees were the ones with
more general meanings
(i.e., the light verbs come, go, look, put) suggests that
the children were applying the verbs across situations. This flexibility of use
across

situations,

combined

with

the

verbs'

use

to

multiple

addressees,

an account

ar

of early verb use as context bound. This discovery


gues against
use
across addressees
of the flexibility of verb
and, probably, situations was
the researcher
employed. Unlike
possible only because of the diary method
in a single setting, the mother keeping a
recording a sample of conversation
con
settings with multiple
diary observes children's verb uses in multiple
of
the
the
addressees
versational partners and potential addressees.
Indeed,
utterances

children's
in television

shows,

included
and

parents,

siblings,

other

relatives,

characters

pets.

92

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

GENERALDISCUSSION

Semantic Flexibility
from the be
data from this study also reveal semantic flexibility
use of a verb
as
is
defined
the
where
verb
semantic
use,
flexibility
ginning of
actors
and on
different
to refer to multiple
enacted
actions
by
appropriate
child's
10
of
the
first
instances
within
the
affected
different
produc
objects
in
tion of that verb. (The actors and objects need not have been expressed
or
use
her
that
his
indicates
child's
the point is that the
the utterance;
that
of the verb is not restricted to something
of the meaning
representation
a
to
a single actor does or something
that is done
single object.) Six of the
one
to
of their verbs in reference
in this study used at least
eight children
one
more
more than one action, and
third of the verbs produced
than
(38%)
were used to refer to multiple
actions. A total of seven verbs (come, cut, go,
to multiple
in reference
actions by at
open, put, take, wash) were produced
The

least half of the children.


in this study used at least some of their verbs with
All eight children
actors within the first 10 uses of those verbs, and most of the target
multiple
actors
verbs produced
(73%) were used with multiple
by these children
the children's very first use of approx
within the first 10 uses. Moreover,
was in reference
to an actor other than them
imately half of their verbs
was limited, the limitations
to
actor extendability
seemed
selves. Where
the child.
limitations within
rather than linguistic
reflect verb meaning
have

Children

and
may

like were
reflect

access

little

used

to others'

primarily

to use

occasions

restricted

wants,

needs,

in self-reference.
particular

and

Other
verbs

so want,

likes,

restricted
(e.g.,

roll

need,

actor uses
is used

only

there is a ball and thus ball is the only actor to appear with
(Naigles 8cHoff, 2006).
than actor flexibility:
Affected
object flexibility was even greater

when

at

some

children

used

affected

object, and 90% of the transitive

refer

to

an

action

least

on

more

verbs

in reference

than

one

to actions

and alternating
affected

object.

with

more

than

roll)
All
one

verbs were used


There

were

to

more

affected objects per verb in children's first 10 instances of use than there
were actors per verb; the time elapsed between first verb use with a different
affected object was less than the time between first verb use and first use
with a new actor, and all children showed a higher degree of affected object
than actor flexibility. Thus, most of the time, children did not
flexibility
relations to a single patient or theme;
restrict their talk about two-argument
to up to eight different patients or
in
used
their
verbs
relation
instead, they
in the early verb-learning
themes within 10 uses. These are the first findings
in produc
such a high level of semantic flexibility
literature to document
tion. They are consistent with the evidence of early flexibility from studies of
or novel
verb comprehension
unconventional
affected
using
objects
&
It
is
that
the
between
Hoff, 2006).
asymmetry
(e.g., Naigles
possible
93
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

actor and affected object flexibility


in the underlying
reflects a difference
actor
of
the
and
affected
roles
such
that children are
representations
object
more

a verb

to extend

reluctant

to a new

actor

to a new

than

or

patient

we

think a practical explanation


is more
However,
likely: In the
a
there
exists
wider
of
affected
child's world,
variety
possible
objects than
actors. That is, actors in children's worlds are mostly animate beings and
these consist of family and friends. In contrast, affected objects are primarily
theme.

inanimate objects, and these include all kinds of food, toys, household
items,
and so on. The set of possible affected objects is just bigger than the set of
actors.

possible

actor flexibility
and affected object flexibility
patterned
Importantly,
measure
were
in the composite
of semantic
combined
together?they
in the analyses of the intercorrelations
among verb use measures;
flexibility
was
both
and
their
zero-order
correlation
positive
significant,
=
=
=
that children who demonstrated
.831, jb .011. The
S)
r(n
finding
more
more
also demonstrated
and
and earlier affected object flexibility
earlier actor flexibility
resources
other.

the

Moreover,

that there was no tradeoff

suggests

to one

allocated

area

in fewer

resulted

children

were

who

early

in which

resources
verb

cognitive
for

available

learners

the

were

overall

that semantic
semantic flexibility,
the ones showing more
indicating
a
a
at
not
of
later
is
verb
stage.
property
developmental
learning
flexibility
actor
in
first
verb
children's
of
The
present
finding
flexibility
also

uses

is

at

preferred

odds

with

Huttenlocher

to use

verbs

in reference

et

al.'s

to themselves;

(1983)

that

report
the

however,

toddlers

discrepancy

et al.
inmethods
in terms of the difference
used. Huttenlocher
a
thus
in
and
mother-child
interaction
only
single setting,
dyadic

is explicable
sampled
two

possible

actors

were

available.

The

diary

method,

in contrast,

was

able

in which other
the day and across different
settings
instances
of actor flex
that
available.
The
fact
be
many
participants may
are
or
not
our
set
who
in
involved
data
pets,
usually included
siblings
ibility
to
credence
this
in recording
adds
further
sessions,
methodological
expla
actor flexibility with at least one
nation. In fact, each child demonstrated
the present
the action of his/her pet. Furthermore,
verb by describing
are
actor
of
with
the
of
consistent
comprehen
findings
flexibility
findings
sion studies, which show that toddlers can extend familiar and newly taught
et al.,
verbs to new actors and/or agents (Naigles 8c Hoff, 2006; Naigles
to tap uses

2005;

across

8c Forbes,

Poulin-Dubois

However,

the

children's

2006).
of

extension

new

verbs

to new

actors

was

not

On average, children had a verb in their lexicon for half a


instantaneous.
it to a new actor. Thus, it is
month and used it three times before extending
and Forbes's
that Poulin-Dubois
still possible
(see also
(2006) finding
not
match
et
do
that
20-month-olds
al., 2006)
just
consistently
Maguire
learned

verbs

to their

actions

when

these

are

performed

by

new

actors

94

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

may

GENERALDISCUSSION

an initial reluctance
children have (within the 10 minutes
a
to
extend
novel
verb's action to a new actor. And
usually provided)
et
contrast
between
al.'s (2005) findings
that children
the
Naigles
perhaps
and other researchers'
that
do extend verb meanings
findings
they do not
as
that
the
verbs
with
different
actors,
suggests
Naigles et al. did
presenting
indeed

during

reflect

training,

make

children

helps

their

own

In

extensions.

the

real

world, most children would experience most common verbs with multiple
actors well before
it seems that if such
the onset of speech; therefore,
actor
is
the
crucial
factor
for
then most children
experience
extendability
to new actions
would have had this experience
and be able to generalize
one
were
actor.
in the present
So
with
the
why
presented
1-year-olds
only
more
in
than
in com
flexible
others
have
demonstrated
study
production
are
A
is that 1-year-olds
prehension?
likely possibility
simply less efficient
learners, such that (in the absence of experiencing
actors) they
multiple
need more time with the teaching stimulus (more than the 18 or so seconds
in experimental
time after
usually provided
settings), or possibly more
to
consolidate
the representation
before extending
the verb to an
teaching,
action

performed

by

a novel

actor.

Context-bound

use,

then,

may

be more

or time with particular verbs rather


function of the amount of experience
a
course
than
of verb learning.
property of the overall
et al.'s (1995) proposal
These findings also cast doubt on Golinkoff
that
the principle
of extendability
that children apply to new nouns must be
relearned for verbs. The speed with which our eight children demonstrated
action, actor, and affected object flexibility
(within 10 to 16 days of the first
use of that verb) suggests that whatever
principle was acquired for noun
to use with verbs. Stated inmore
learning was easily and rapidly transferred
general terms, the picture of early flexible verb use suggested by the present
data contrasts with those descriptions
in the literature that paint early word
use,
attested,

in

as
general,
underextended

context

bound.
usage

may

While
be more

instances

of

characteristic

are

underextension
of nouns,

whose

acquisition begins earlier in English, than of verbs. Our data show that early
verb use is not typically context bound.

TOWHAT EXTENT
ARECHILDREN'S
EARLY
VERBUSESCONSERVATIVE
VERSUS
FLEXIBLE
INGRAMMAR?
The data from this study show clearly that within their first 10 instances
a verb in spontaneous
of producing
speech, children demonstrate
flexibility
in the syntactic environments
in which
those verbs appear. The children
used multiple
frames with two thirds of their verbs, and these first verbs
were used in different frames, on average,
by two thirds of the children. On
children's
first
in
average,
change
syntactic frame took place within half a
95
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

month
of the first verb use; thus, there was no extended
period of frozen
form use. On average, only 12% of such frame changes included the use of
in
1.8% of utterances
(range 0-36% across children; on average,
negation
most
of
frame
observed
the
cluded negation markers);
therefore,
flexibility
structure of the verb. Mor
in the argument
involved children's flexibility
reflecting just the children's uses of verbal suffixes,
flexibility,
phological
at a much
lower rate. Some children did not use any inflections
occurred
with
"-ing")

their verbs
and

examples

(most of the morphological


of

past

tense

or

forms

third-person

that were
singular

use

used were
were

rare.

close to a
addition or subtraction of such a morpheme?took
Changes?the
to be observed after the first use.
month
in verb use was the extent to which verbs
of flexibility
Another measure
were used with different
slot or in the subject
lexical items in the preposition
or object positions.
in filling all these
Children
demonstrated
flexibility
In
the children
lexical
items.
with
slots
particular,
multiple
grammatical
in which a verb was used with at least two
showed lexical subject flexibility,
one third of their verbs; just over one
different subjects, with approximately
with
used
verbs
the
third of
children
subjects. Lexical object flex
multiple
was
half of the children and by
for
under
verbs
demonstrated
just
ibility
by
was seen in
half
of
their
verbs.
children for just under
flexibility
Preposition
at
these
all.
five of the six children who produced
Tellingly,
prepositions
measures
of
cohered with the other
indicators of lexical flexibility
gram
matical

flexibility

our

in forming

composite

grammatical

flexibility

score.

in syntactic flexibility
individual differences
analyses
Finally,
course
of syntactic flexibil
the
developmental
(Figures 10-18) investigated
to
fewer than
in
the
conservative-child
child.
each
hypothesis,
Contrary
ity
initial conservative
half of our children (n = 3) demonstrated
verb-only use
4-5 months).
for
verbs
after
followed by later syntactic flexibility
(i.e.,
using
use of verbs with arguments
demonstrated
five
children
More
tellingly,
for two of these children,
from the start of verb acquisition;
syntactic flex
was evident within the first 6 weeks of verb use.
verbs
with
ibility
multiple
in the use of one verb appeared during
For the others, syntactic flexibility
the first period and more verbs came to be used with flexibility during the
almost always before the age of 2 years.
8 weeks?and
subsequent
of grammatical
In addition to these measures
flexibility, we also applied
measures
that have been
of productivity
to the present data corpus-based
We
found that
literature
in
the
Shirai,
1989;
1998).
(Ingram,
employed
a frame must be used with five
more
criterion?that
the
stringent
using
of productive
number
verbs to be considered
different
productive?the
frames children had at 24 months was positively correlated with the overall
is
measure
of their verb use. This correlation
of grammatical
flexibility
is an indicator of productivity.
that flexibility
consistent with the argument
3-Verb or 5-Verb
of the children who achieved
We found
that many
of

96
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

GENERALDISCUSSION

with a given frame (e.g., using VO with three or five different


productivity
so
did
before the age of 24 months;
three children even achieved
verbs)
such productivity with almost all of the relevant frames before 24 months of
age. Across the entire corpus of first uses of first verbs, we found that the
children used the SV and VO frames with > 60% of their target verbs, the
SVO frame with almost half of their target verbs, and the VP and V-ing
frames with fewer, but still multiple
verbs. Thus, well before 36 months
of
even
of
before
months
children
24
age?and
frequently
age?the
provided
evidence
that their frames were not necessarily
restricted to specific lexical
items.

The measures
of productivity
are
measures
indirect
only
study

and flexibility employed


in the present
as we were un
of hallmark productivity,
able to assess how any of the children's utterances
from their
diverged
the
children
had
heard
these
verbs
used
Indeed,
input.
likely
flexibly by the
adults around
them. The picture of early verb use that these measures
is in stark contrast to the description
of early verb use that
reveal, however,
has been the basis for claims that young children are not productive
verb
users.
use

With
across

a
of different
variety
frames
others
and

measures,
assessing

some
children's

assessing
frame

children's
use

across

verb
verbs,

these eight children have demonstrated


that their early verb use was flexible
rather than restricted
and thus more
consistent with an account of chil
dren's early verb use as reflecting abstract underlying
than with
knowledge
a description
of early verb use as strict imitations of input. The abstract
need not be innate, as the traditional generativist position would
knowledge
hold. Rather, children's flexible verb usage may reflect children's
learning
that flexibility
is general characteristic
of verb use from the evidence
of
flexible uses of particular verbs in their input. Put another way, children's
flexible verb uses may reflect learning from input, but it is learning that is
more abstract than learning the
uses of individual verbs modeled
multiple
in the input.

TOWHAT EXTENT
ISEARLY
FLEXIBILITY
PARTICULARLY
CHARACTERISTIC
OF ONLY
SOME (i.e., LIGHT,
OR PATHBREAKING)
VERBS?
The

picture of early grammatical flexibility of verb use suggested by the


was not limited to only some verbs. There was no evidence of a
data
present
role for light verbs in early grammatical
special pathbreaking
development.
were
verbs
neither
the earliest verbs produced,
nor were
Light
they, as a
flex
class, the verbs with the earliest or greatest syntactic or morphological
Of
11
the
verbs
before
the
of
620
age
ibility.
produced
days (on average),
some were light (come, go) but others were not (throw, sit, see,
push, open, lay,
when the children are considered
fall, eat, bite). Moreover,
individually, only
97
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

an advantage for light verbs, such that


one child consistently demonstrated
her first use of each frame and first instances of grammatical
flexibility were
shown for light verbs. Only sporadic evidence of a light verb advantage was
found for three other children, and for the other four children none of their
than heavy verbs that
light verbs showed more
grammatical
flexibility
at the same age. When
the frames were considered
emerged
individually,
the
SVO
first with a single verb, and this
frame
only
consistently
appeared
verb (want for six of the eight children) was hardly the most semantically
first varied widely
transparent. For all other frames, the verbs that appeared
across children and were neither consistently
transparent nor
semantically
our findings
consistently highly frequent in the input. Taken together, then,
not
do
1999;
support the proposals
(e.g., Chenu & Jisa, 2006; Goldberg,
of grammatical
Ninio,
1999) that light verbs are the primary promoters
development.

in our data that the children


There were, however,
several indications
did use light verbs differently
than heavy verbs. Light verbs appeared more
in descriptions
in commands
and less frequently
than did heavy
frequently
addressees more than
verbs. Moreover,
light verbs were used with multiple
heavy

verbs;

heavy

verbs,

also used with more

they were
and

there

actors

with

was

a trend

in the

different

same

affected
a

for

direction

than

objects

use

greater

of

light than with heavy verbs. Finally,


light verbs
more
in
SVO
frames
than
with
direct
and
objects
frequently
appeared
more
were
with
earlier
and
lexical
and
used
verbs
did,
object
they
heavy
than heavy verbs. All of these findings derive in a straightforward
flexibility
of light and heavy verbs. The
in the meanings
fashion from differences
can be attributed
to the behavior
association of light verbs with commands
different

of

three

monly

in

verbs
as

used

included more
not

be

come,

particular,
commands

in

as commands.

used

In

contrast,

com

to be

likely
the

subclass

heavy

states (like, need, cry), which

to internal

the more

Furthermore,

seem

look, which

as well.

input

verbs referring

and

go,

verbs

general

could

in the

light

situations and so can be used with


verb subclass can apply to more different
more different
That
and
addressees.
affected
actors,
is, anyone
objects,
can go or come and anything can be brought or taken, whereas only eatables
can

eaten

be

and

animates

only

can

eat.

And

because

heavy

refer

verbs

to specific events in the world, their direct objects can


directly and obviously
common
of
between
because
be omitted
speaker and hearer
ground
more
to
with
be
retained
forms
need
these
whereas
general verbs. The
to have
some
at
the
addressee
need
of
of
the
children seemed aware,
level,
some

affected

al., 2006).
of

common

objects

made

If such objects
ground,

then,

more

explicit

than

are required more


because

of base

others

with
rates,

(see

also

et

Matthews

light verbs by the dictates


more

different

NPs

should

of light verbs, appearing


the grammatical
be used. Thus,
advantage
more with objects and in SVO frames, is directly traceable to the semantic/
98
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

GENERALDISCUSSION

of those verbs in usage, not to their unique map


of specific frames. This is consistent with Snedeker
that light verbs, because of their appli
and Gleitman's
(2004) suggestion
a
to
of
such
wide
situations, must be the products rather than
cability
variety
the engines of early grammatical
acquisition.

pragmatic
ping onto

requirements
the semantics

ARETHEFLEXIBILITY
OF MEANINGANDOF FORMRELATED?
TOWHAT EXTENT
Semantic flexibility and grammatical flexibility of verb use were related,
of verbs. The children who
both as properties
of children and as properties
in their first verb uses (indexed by a
semantic flexibility
showed more
also showed more grammatical
composite of multiple measures)
flexibility
a
the target verbs in this study,
indexed
(also
score). Among
composite
by
those that were used with grammatical flexibility by more children were also
actors by more children. Thus,
these findings
used with multiple
extend
accounts
theoretical
of the relations between
early verb syntax and verb
semantics in a couple of ways. The findings
suggest that it is not just know
a
certain number of verbs (Marchman 8c Bates, 1994) but also having
ing
that underlie
the understandings
semantic flexibility of verb use that sup
ports the acquisition of verb grammar
(Naigles et al., 2005). The findings
also suggest that the relationship
between
semantic flexibility and syntactic
is reciprocal. In particular, the finding that semantic flexibility and
flexibility
of verbs, in addition to being
syntactic flexibility were related as properties
related as properties
of children,
supports the syntactic bootstrapping
hy
pothesis:
elaborate

Verbs used with more


semantic

grammatical

are verbs with more

flexibility

as well.

representations

The
semantic and grammatical
of verb use did, however,
flexibility
differ in their relations to the age at which the child began to use verbs and
to when the verb appeared
in the children's
speech. Children who began
verb

use

at a younger

age

showed,

on

average,

greater

semantic

flexibility

in

their verb use. Children may become early verb learners, then, if they have
the ability to extract verb referents
from their observed
early acquired
contexts.

In

contrast,

these

earlier

verb

learners

did

not

show

greater?or

in their early verb uses, nor did they achieve


lesser?grammatical
flexibility
a greater?or
of productive
lesser?number
frames by the age of 24
use
to
months
with
children
who
verbs later. Earlier verb
began
compared
in their ability to extend their
learners, then, may be specifically precocious
verb meanings in multiple ways, but they are neither advanced nor behind
other children in their ability to use those verbs inmultiple ways in terms of
the picture is a bit different: Those verbs
grammar. Among verbs, however,
that appeared
earlier in the children's speech were used with grammatical
(in their first 10 uses) by fewer children than were later-appearing
flexibility
99
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

of chil
showed no relation to the proportion
verbs, but age of appearance
dren who used a verb with semantic flexibility. Thus,
the data present a
use
use
to
in
which
children who begin
verbs earlier
their first verbs
picture
with a similar degree of grammatical
flexibility as children who begin later,
but verbs that are used earlier than other verbs are used with grammatical
flexibility by fewer children. Less grammatical
flexibility, then, is a property
of

earlier-learned

verbs

but

not

of

children.

earlier-verb-learning

It

is im

verb (open) was used


portant to point out, though, even the earliest-learned
we
with grammatical
almost
of
the
children.
30%
Overall,
by
flexibility
found

no

children,

evidence

that

there

exist

or

verbs,

early-learned

early-starting

that are consistently inflexible.

THEORETICAL
IMPLICATIONS
OF THE
AND
OBSERVED
PRAGMATIC,
SEMANTIC,
FLEXIBILITY
OF CHILDREN'S
FIRST
GRAAAAAATICAL
VERBUSES
The data from this diary study indicate that 1-year-old children use
in multiple
their newly acquired verbs flexibly,
situations, with multiple
actions, actors, affected objects, and paths or locations. These
findings are at
are ini
in which children's verb meanings
odds with models of acquisition
et
et
The
Golinkoff
conservative
1995;
al.,
al.,
2006).
Maguire
(e.g.,
tially
present data show that 1 -year-old children also use their verbs in different
sentence

frames,

with

different

subjects,

objects,

and

and

prepositions,

all within the first 10 instances of


(somewhat) with different morphology,
use. Such flexible use suggests as well that children younger
than 2 years do
the restricted use that inspired the Verb Island Hy
not, in fact, manifest
pothesis.

These

findings

are

more

consistent

with

the

generativist

view

of

8c Fisher, 2002)
child language acquisition
1981; Gleitman
(e.g., Chomsky,
that children are conservative
than with the positions
language users and
do not have abstract syntax (e.g., Goldberg,
that 2-year-olds
1999; Lieven,
2006; Tomasello,
2000).
verb use was
in the present data, fully flexible and productive
However,
not evident in all children from the moment
verb use began. On average,
the percent of verbs that children used flexibly varied from 16% (mor
to 30% (lexical subjects) to 38% (actions) to 46% (lexical objects) to
phology)
to 66% (syntax) to 73% (actors) to 90% (affected objects).
50% (addressees)
on the
some verbs in the same way (i.e., conservatively)
Every child produced
measures
10
their
first
for
all
of
lexical, and/or grammatical
pragmatic,
instances.

Even

when

verb

uses

were

flexible

across

the

first

10

instances,

they

were rarely flexible by the second instance. Thus, children were swift to show
all kinds of
some kinds of flexibility and productivity but did not demonstrate
some
children
and
Moreover,
ap
instantaneously.
productivity
flexibility
more flexible and productive
than others.
peared to be swifter and/or

100
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

GENERALDISCUSSION

There

are

two

and

possible,

not

sources

exclusive,

mutually

of

this

in the flexibility with which


children use their first verbs. A
variability
had full grammatical
would
that
the
children
argue
knowledge
generativist
and con
but that a variety of personality,
from the beginning
pragmatic,
of that underlying
textual factors limited children's expression
grammatical
more interested than others in talk
knowledge. That is, some children are
than other children
(Nelson,
ing about new things, and to new people,
some
8c
verbs afford more flexible
1973; Reznick, Corley,
Robinson,
1997);
use than others and sometimes
the occasions for the use of a verb may be
with this position,
limited. Consistent
studies, where such
comprehension
and

semantic-,

pragmatic-,

influences

personality-based

should

not

apply,

of most
that most toddlers demonstrate
significant generalization
et
et al.,
items
Gertner
introduced
lexical
al., 2006; Naigles
(e.g.,
newly
Future
8c
Poulin-Dubois
2005;
Forbes, 2006).
comprehension
investigations
could further solidify these findings by extending
the semantic and gram
matical properties
studied
(e.g., to novel affected objects, and to [in the
relevant languages] PPs and case markers).
indicate

in which
the present data, though,
is a model
not
is
from
Some
the
grammatical
knowledge
fully present
beginning.
are
use
achieved
in
before
verb
any
grammatical
understandings
produc
un
in the flexibility
tion resulting
that is observed,
but some grammatical
after
The
clear
frame
differences
derstandings
develop
begins.
production
in these data suggest that the correct description
and individual differences
as verb learners will contain
of children
elements
of both the rapid
measure
in
and
conservative-child
different
for differ
accounts,
generalizer
ent children, different verbs, and different frames (see Maratsos,
2007, for a
in a different domain). When all the supportive factors are
similar argument
in place (e.g., the verbs have been used flexibly
in the input, the child is
new
situation
the
is
and
feeling talkative,
interesting but not too new and
more
and
the
frame
is
children are more
interesting,
possibly
transparent),
to
be
flexible
and
likely
swiftly
productive?and
possibly even show gen
Also

consistent

to novel

eralization

with

instances,

such

although

latter

are

demonstrations

un

in spontaneous
the supportive
factors
likely to be manifest
speech. When
are not in place, children may be more
to
some
be
and
conservative,
likely
more
some
factors
be
available
for
verbs
than
for
others.
may
supportive
in their speech processing
Differences
abilities may also
among children
in the onset or rate of flexibility, productivity,
lead to variation
and gen
consonant with other recent findings
that early perceptual
fac
eralization,
tors predict later language measures
8c
Perfors,
Marchman,
2006;
(Fernald,
8c Pruitt,
Padden, Nelson,
8c
Dow,
2006).
Jusczyk,

Kuhl, Conboy,
Jusczyk,
Thus,

categories

one

position

are learned

consistent

from

with

our

2005; Newman,
data

is that

input, but the process

Bernstein

abstract

of learning

Ratner,

grammatical

begins

early,

101
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

at all or before production


of the relevant
lin
speech production
or
see
As
frame
construction.
summarized
Gerken
also
(2007;
guistic
by
have demonstrated
2002; Saffran 8c Thiessen,
2007), researchers
Naigles,
that considerable
about
the
patterns?at
knowledge
varying levels of ab
a given language are accrued during the first 12-18 months
stractness?of
before

of life. If children have indeed been actively processing


their input language
it is
their first verbs, then perhaps
for over a year by the time they produce
not surprising
that they should show the level of productivity
and flexibility
view from a
that our data reveal (see also Snyder, 2007, for a concurring
more generativist
if
awaits
framework).
Similarly,
grammatical development
it is a threshold that
the achievement
of a threshold of lexical development,
children achieve very early.
The present findings have some further implications for accounts of the
The observed
that underlie
process or processes
language development.

common

the grammatical
flexibility and semantic flexibil
and
25
26, chapter VI) suggest there may be some
(Tables
common experiences
that support both
and/or
learning processes

semantic

and grammatical

between

intercorrelations
ity of verb use

ticular,

suggest

that

syntactic

development.
properties

The
of

verbs

correlations
reveal

their

by verb,

in par

semantic

pos

sibilities, and vice versa. For example,


learning quickly that open can be used
affected objects might enable children to quickly understand
with multiple
that it should also appear with grammatical
objects; learning quickly that
run

does

not

involve

affected

objects

effectively

limits

its grammatical

uses

them from those of open. Another way to think about this


and distinguishes
to be learned quickly also
is that whatever
input enables verbs' semantics
is
relevant for meaning
information
enables their grammatical
aspects?the
is consistent with the
also relevant for grammar. The input interpretation
and Hoff-Ginsberg
(1998), who found that verbs used
findings of Naigles
are the ones used more frequently
with more syntactic diversity by mothers
10 weeks later.
and with more syntactic diversity by children
for future re
the
relevant
that
The present
question
findings suggest
even?can
be flexible and
search, then, is not whether children?toddlers,
lexical semantic meanings.
with syntactic frames and extend
productive
can.
in
is
how
The
issue
early
development, and how quickly
really,
Clearly, they
a
new
this
and
what
factors influence
do
this
do
verb,
they
upon meeting
process.

LIMITATIONS
This study is, of course, limited by the fact that we have investigated
of only a small number
the development
(34) of verbs and have tracked
for only their first 10 uses. In particular, our exclusion of
their development

102
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

GENERALDISCUSSION

about the gram


make and do from the targeted verbs limits our conclusions
as the
of light verbs, as these have been proposed
contributions
transitive
frame
for
the
1999;
Ninio,
1999). How
(Goldberg,
pathbreakers

matical
ever,

our

verbs

were

chosen

because

data

previous

indicated

that

these

34 were

among the earliest produced by 1-year-old English learners (e.g., Naigles 8c


1998); therefore, we do not anticipate that many other verbs
Hoff-Ginsberg,
would elicit earlier use and/or flexibility. Moreover, when the first uses of make
were examined for two of the children, whose mothers recorded them at their
own initiative (Mae and Stacey; see Tables 20 and 23, respectively),
there was
for either one.
that make functioned as a pathbreaker
is also limited by the fact
The generalizability
of the present findings
that we have only included eight children, although an n of 8 is large for the
in this study. Because
individual data analytic approach
the
employed
were
current method
for
different
both
children
who
reasons,
excluded,
talkative and those who talked very little (see chapter II), the
extremely
toddlers at large is probably
range of behavior of American English-learning
wider than seen here. It is likely that both more and less swiftly flexible
children exist. Our conclusions might also be limited by the fact that we
relied on the parents to note each of the first 10 instances of these 34 verbs,
no evidence

we have reason to believe


that these
However,
including
self-repetitions.
we
out
to
mothers
carried
this
task.
talked
each
First,
parent every
eight
just
week, once their child had begun producing
verbs, giving them weekly
reminders of how to report the relevant aspects of their children's verb use
to the diary pages. Mothers who were not able to do this were quite open to
telling us. Only the children whose mothers were able to carry out the task
in

the manner

prescribed

are

in

included

the

study.

Moreover,

if our

doc

umented
child variations reflected only maternal
variation
in keeping
the
or
then
children
should
in
how
slow
differ,
diary,
consistently,
speedy
they
were

to

That

is,

reach
if some

10

instances,
mothers

or
were

in how

flexible

consistently

they
more

were

on

conscientious

all measures.
or

more

attuned to their child's speech, then some children should show precocity
on all measures
whereas
other children
should be consistently
slow. In
all 10 instances of at least
contrast, though, all of our children produced
some verbs in a very short
period of time and all also took a very long time to
reach 10 instances of other verbs (chapter III).
Finally, it is possible that, because they were listening for their children's
use of these verbs, the mothers
might have been more
likely to use those
verbs, themselves. More frequent use by mothers,
though, would only have
led to earlier use by the children, not to more or less flexible or productive
use. That is, there is no reason to expect that more
frequent use by mothers
would have also meant more diverse use by mothers?in
fact, we would
that
the
of
folk
theories
held
argue
language "training"
by most middle
class mothers
would
lead them to less diverse uses (i.e., repetitions
of the

103
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

same verb phrases). Thus,


it seems quite unlikely
of diary records altered the development
under

that the mothers'

keeping

study.

CONCLUSIONS
The overall picture of the functions, meanings,
and grammar of verbs
one of flexible use. Children
is
when they first appear in children's
speech
use their verbs in utterances
that serve multiple
communicative
functions,
actors and affected objects, and in multiple
with multiple
syntactic frames.
in the literature of early verb use as
This picture contrasts with descriptions
for these differing
restricted and context bound. Part of the explanation
use may be in the unique nature of the database
of
verb
descriptions
early
the completeness
of sam
for the present analyses. Our database provided
our database included several
pling that is characteristic of diary studies, but
individual children can have
children. Thus, our data reveal that whereas
restricted uses of individual verbs, restricted use is not a stage of language
through which all children pass. Like children's overexten
development
sions (e.g., calling strange men Daddy; Rescorla,
1980) and over regular
Rosen, & Xu,
Hollander,
(e.g., goed) (Marcus, Pinker, Ullman,
verb use
restricted
and
of
context-bound
instances
1992),
grammatically
are
more
than
salient
be
may
they
frequent.
izations

The

present

findings

argue

that

children

are

not

as

conservative

lan

guage learners as some theories of language acquisition would have them


or in structure. At least for verb learning, they do not
be, either inmeaning
start small, with verb-specific
but instead begin in the middle,
grammars,
with some flexibility and productivity manifested
by the time they begin to
from the very
use verbs at all. Children quickly match their input, observing
in the target
distinctions
of language use the subcategorization
beginning
uses
of newly acquired verbs children show
their first 10
language. Within
include categories of verbs, which are differentiated
that their grammars
by
occurrence. They do not appear to have a single
of
privileges
grammatical
seem to
category, VERB, in which all verbs are treated equally, nor do they
To
reveal these
have 34 separate and isolated verb-specific
representations.
a
new
we
to
method?a
had
diary that targeted only verbs
develop
findings,
to
record only their child's first 10 instances of use.
and that asked mothers
the diary tractable for
the aspects of language use, we made
By restricting
more
children could be studied. By using the diary
data collectors such that
in which
we
were
to
collect uses in almost all of the contexts
able
method,
we
were
able to
talk. And by asking for 10 successive
children
instances,
demonstrate

that?and

track

how?children's

very

first

uses

changed.

104
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

REFERENCES

with word
children's
M. (1997). Young
N., & Tomasello,
productivity
Psychology, 33, 952-965.
morphology.
Developmental
M. (2000). The
of words
and word
social nature
Akhtar, N., & Tomasello,
& L. Bloom
K. Hirsh-Pasek,
Golinkoff,
(Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A
Press.
New York: Oxford
University
(pp. 115-135).
acquisition
structure
in
H.
Preferred
S. E. M., & Schroder,
Allen,
(2003).
argument
E.
L.
In
W.
&
data.
DuBois,
W.J.
Ashby
spontaneous
J.
Kumpf,
speech

Akhtar,

argument

structure: Grammar

as architecture for function

(pp. 301-338).

order

and verb

In R.
learning.
debate on lexical

early

Inuktitut

(Eds.), Preferred
Amsterdam:
John

Benjamins.
M. (2006). The distributed
A. L., Lieven,
E. V. M., 8c Tomasello,
B., Theakston,
Ambridge,
of an abstract
construction.
effect for children's
Cognitive
syntactic
acquisition
learning
21, 174-193.
Development,
R. A. (2003). The
of grammar:
Armon-Lotem,
S., 8c Berman,
emergence
Early verbs and
30, 845-877.
of Child Language,
beyond. Journal
for clues to word
reference.
D. A. (1993).
the speaker
Infant's
ability to consult
20, 395-418.
of Child Language,
Journal
and maternal
Barrett, M., Harris, M., 8c Chasin, J. (1991). Early lexical development
speech:
uses of words.
initial and subsequent
A comparison
of children's
Journal
of Child Lan

Baldwin,

18, 21-40.
I., 8c Snyder, L. (1988). From first words togrammar: Individual differences
E., Bretherton,
Press.
and dissociable mechanisms. New York: Cambridge
University
In B.
of grammar
from the lexicon.
the emergence
Bates,
E., 8c Goodman,
J. (1999). On
New Jersey: Erlbaum.
(Ed.), The emergence of language
(pp. 29-80).
MacWhinney
guage,

Bates,

Mouton.
One word at a time: The use of single word utterances. The Hague:
UK: Cambridge
Uni
(1993). Language
development from two to three. Cambridge,
versity Press.
of verbs and the development
of verb
L., Lifter, K., 8c Hafitz,
Bloom,
J. (1980). Semantics
in child language.
inflection
386-412.
56,
Language,
K. (1987). The maturation
of syntax. In T. Roeper
8c E. Williams
Borer, H., 8cWexler,
(Eds.),
Parameter
Reidel.
Dordrecht:
123-187).
setting (pp.

Bloom,

L.

Bloom,

L.

(1973).

M.
Structural
Bowerman,
(1973).
relationships
In T. E. Moore
semantic?
(Ed.), Cognitive

in children's
and

utterances:

Syntactic

or

the acquisition

development
of language
Press.
New York: Academic
197-214).
in the acquisition
of early verbs: Evidence
from diary data
S. (1995). Differences
In M. Tomasello
8cW. Merriman
from sisters.
(Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young
children's acquisition of verbs (pp. 81-111).
Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.

(pp.
Braunwald,

to lexicon:
grammar
Unsupervised
243-262.
19,
Linguistics,
Computational
M. (2001). The
of dative
A., 8c Tomasello,
acquisition
Campbell,
253-267.
22,
cholinguistics,
Brent,

Caselli,

M.

R.

(1993).

From

learning

of

constructions.

lexical

syntax.

Applied

Psy

M. C, Casadio,
of the transition
from first words
P., 8c Bates, E. (1999). A comparison
to grammar
in English
and Italian. Journal
Child
69-111.
26,
Language,
of

105
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

a phrasal
A. E. (2005). Fast mapping
between
form and mean
D., & Goldberg,
Science, 8, 500-508.
ing. Developmental
in early
constructions
and semantic
F., & Jisa, H. (2006). Caused motion
Chenu,
generality
In E. Clark & B. Kelly
of French.
in acquisition
(Eds.), Constructions
(pp. 233
acquisition
Casenhiser,

CA: CSLI Publications.


261). Stanford,
M. (2006). Are nouns
easier to acquire
Childers,
J., & Tomasello,
children
Pasek & R. Golinkoff
(Eds.), Action meets word: How
New York: Oxford
Press.
University
Chomsky,

N.

(1975).

N.

(1981).

In K. Hirsch

than verbs?
learn verbs

(pp. 311-335).

on
Books.
Reflections
language. New York:Pantheon
Lectures on government
and binding. Holland:
Foris Publications.
on
A constraint
The
of contrast:
principle
language
acquisition.
(Ed.), Mechanisms
of language
acquisition
(pp. 1-33). Hillsdale,

Chomsky,
E. V. (1987).
Clark,
B. MacWhinney

In
NJ:

Erlbaum.
E. V.

Clark,

The

(1993).

E. V.

Clark,

(2003).

in acquisition.

lexicon

First

language

Cambridge,

acquisition.

UK:

Cambridge,

Press.
University
Cambridge
UK:
University
Cambridge

Press.
Press.
UK: Cambridge
(1996). Using language. Cambridge,
University
A
statistics
H. H. (1973). The
of
fallacy:
language
language-as-fixed-effect
critique
and Verbal Behavior,
research. Journal
12, 335-359.
of Verbal Learning
psychological
the lexicon and grammer:
Cross-sectional
B. T, & Thai, D. J. (2006). Ties between
Conboy,
H.

Clark,
Clark,

toddlers.
studies of bilingual
longitudinal
D. (1999). An introduction
S., 8c Lillo-Martin,
UK: Blackwell
Publishers.
Oxford,
and

Crain,

in

Child development,
77, 712-735.
to
and
theory
language acquisition.
linguistic

K. (1996). Collecting
spontaneous
production
S. Cairns
(Eds.), Methods for assessing children's
Press.
MIT

Demuth,
H.

data.

In D. McDaniel,

syntax

(pp.

3-22).

C. McKee

Cambridge,

8c
MA:

in
V. A. (2007). Grammar
and the lexicon: Developmental
ordering
J., & Marchman,
Child
190-212.
78,
Development,
language
acquisition.
Press.
UK: Cambridge
E. (1987). Early lexical development. Cambridge,
Dromi,
University
K.
M. H., Karmiloff-Smith,
E. A., Johson,
A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett,
Elman,
J. L., Bates,
on
MA:
innateness: A connectionist perspective
(1996). Rethinking
Cambridge,
development.

Dixon,

MIT

Press.

in early
S. (1994). Variability
Fenson,
L., Dale, P., Reznick,
J., Bates, E., Thai, D., & Pethick,
in Child Development,
communicative
of the Society for Research
Monographs
development.
59(5, Serial No. 242).
Fernald,

V. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding:


A., & Marchman,
Speech
across the second year. Developmental
and
Psy
growth
vocabulary
efficiency
98-116.

A., Perfors,

processing
chology, 42,
K. J., Marcus,
Fernandes,
to syntax and back

G. E, Di Nubila,
again: Argument

J. A., & Vouloumanos,


structure
in the third

A.
year

(2006). From semantics


of life. Cognition,
100,

B10-B20.
Fisher,

C.

(1996).

Structural

limits

of sentences.

on

verb mapping:

The

Cognitive Psychology,
interpretations
L. (1991). On
H., 8c Gleitman,
C, Gleitman,
zation frames. Cognitive Psychology,
23, 331-392.
L. R. (2002).
8c Gleitman,
Fisher, C,
Language
acquisition.
R. (Vol. Ed.), Steven's handbook of experimental
8c Gallistel,
Fisher,

role

31, 41-81.
the semantic

in children's

of analogy
content

In H.

of

subcategori

Pashler

(Series Ed.)
Vol. 3. Learning,
8c Sons.
and emotion (3rd ed., pp. 445-496).
Hoboken,
motivation,
NJ: John Wiley
in young
D.
8c
Poulin-Dubois,
N.,
Forbes,
(1997).
change
Representational
J.
verb meaning.
of familiar
children's
24,
of Child Language,
Journal
understanding
psychology:

389-406.

106
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

REFERENCES

Gentner,
M.

L. (2002).
D., 8c Boroditsky,
Individuation,
Bowerman
& S. Levinson
(Eds.), Language

and early word


In
relativity,
learning.
acquisition and conceptual development
(pp.
Press.
University

UK: Cambridge
215-256).
Cambridge,
L. (2007). Acquiring
structure.
Gerken,
linguistic
handbook of language development
(pp. 173-190).

In E. Hoff

Oxford,
words
Learning

Y, Fisher, C, &: Eisengart,


Gertner,
J. (2006).
in early sentence
of word order
comprehension.
H., Gleitman,
L., 8c Lederer,
Gillette,
J., Gleitman,
73, 135-176.
ulary learning.
Cognition,

A.

8c M.
UK:

Shatz

Publishing.
and rules: Abstract
knowledge

Science, 17, 684-691.


Psychological
of vocab
simulations
(1999). Human

sources of verb meanings.


L. (1990). The
structural
Language
E. L. (1995). The
L. R., 8c Newport,
of language
invention
Gleitman,
on the
ronmental
and biological
influences
of language.
acquisition
to
M. Liberman
invitation
(Eds.),^4n
cognitive science: Vol. 1. Language
MA: MIT
Press.
Cambridge,
Gleitman,

A. (1999). The
Goldberg,
In B. MacWhinney

Blackwell

(Eds.),

Blackwell

of the semantics
of argument
emergence
(Ed.), The emergence
of language
(pp.

1, 3-55.
Acquisition,
Envi
by children:
In L. R. Gleitman
(2nd ed., pp.

structure
197-211).

8c

1-24).

constructions.
Mahwah,

NJ:

Erlbaum.
D. (2006). Learning
A., 8c Casenhiser,
argument
Goldberg,
Clark & B. Kelly
in acquisition
(Eds.), Constructions
(pp.
Publications.

structure

constructions.

185-204).

Stanford,

In E.

CA: CSLI

D. M., 8c Sethuraman,
A. E., Casenhiser,
N. (2004). Learning
structure
argument
15, 289-316.
generalizations.
Cognitive Linguistics,
S. (2003). The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf children can tell
Goldin-Meadow,
us about how all children learn
Press.
language. New York: Psychology
Goldberg,

M. E., 8c Gelman,
R. (1976). Language
in the two-year-old.
S., Seligman,
4, 189-202.
Cognition,
L. (1987). The eyes have it: Lexical
and
R., Hirsh-Pasek,
Golinkoff,
K., Cauley, K., 8c Gordon,
in a new paradigm.
14, 23-45.
syntactic
comprehension
Journal
of Child Language,
R. M., Hirsh-Pasek,
C. B., Frawley, W,
8c Parillo, M. (1995). Lexical
Golinkoff,
K., Mervis,
can be extended
to
In
of
verbs.
the
M.
Tomasello
& W. Merriman
principles
acquisition
Goldin-Meadow,

(Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children's acquisition of verbs (pp. 185-222).
Erlbaum Associates.
NJ: Lawrence
R. L. (2002). Variability
structure.
and detection
of invariant
Gomez,
Psychological
431-436.

Hillsdale,
Science,

13,

context
role of semantic
and
L., 8c Brown, N. B. (1998). The
J. C, McDonough,
in the acquisition
of novel nouns.
Child Development,
69, 1330-1344.
memory
8c Sethuraman,
N. (2006).
Interactions
in the development
of constructions
Goodman,
J. C,
In E. Clark & B. Kelly
and the acquisition
of word meanings.
in
(Eds.), Constructions
Goodman,

Grice,

Stanford:
acquisition
(pp. 263-281).
H. P. (Ed.) (1989). Further
notes

CSLI Publications.
on
Studies in the way of words
logic and conversation.
MA:
Harvard
Press.
from Syntax and
41-57).
(pp.
Cambridge,
University
(Reprinted
semantics: Vol. 9. Pragmatics,
1978, New York: Academic
pp. 113-128,
by P. Cole, Ed.,

Press).
S. (1988). Linguistic
M., Barrett,
M., Jones,
D., & Brooks,
input and
15, 77-94.
meaning.
Journal
of Child Language,
R. (Eds.),
K. & Golinkoff,
children
Hirsh-Pasek,
(2006). Action meets word: How
Oxford
UK: Oxford
Press.
University
Harris,

early

R. M., Hennon,
E. A., & Maguire,
K., Golinkoff,
Hirsh-Pasek,
(2004). Hybrid
M.J.
at the frontier
of developmental
The emergentist
coalition model
psychology:
as a case in point.
a lexicon
In G. Hall & S. Waxman
(Eds.), Weaving
learning
204).

Cambridge,

MA: MIT

word

learn verbs.
theories
of word
(pp.

173

Press.

107
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

use of syntactic frames


L. (1996). Young
children's
Hirsh-Pasek,
K., Golinkoff,
R., 8c Naigles,
to derive meaning.
In K. Hirsh-Pasek
8c R. Golinkoff
(Eds.), The origins of grammar:
Evidence
MA: MIT
123-158).
from
early language
comprehension
(pp.
Cambridge,
Press.
G. J., Hirsh-Pasek,
R. M. (2000). Breaking
the language
barrier: An
K., & Golinkoff,
for the origins of word
coalition model
emergentist
learning. Monographs
of the Society for
in Child Development,
Research
65(3, Serial No.262).
P. J., & Thompson,
in grammar
S. A. (1980). Transitivity
and discourse.
Hopper,
Language,
56,251-299.
R. (1983). The emergence
of action categories
in the
Huttenlocher,
J., Smiley, P., 8c Charney,
Hollich,

from verb meaning.


child: Evidence
Review, 90, 72-93.
Psychological
of grammatical
In R. Stark (Ed.) Language
Ingram, D. (1981). Early patterns
development.
New York: Elsevier North-Holland.
behavior in infancy and early childhood (pp. 327-352).
Press.
UK: Cambridge
Ingram, D. (1989). First language acquisition. Cambridge,
University
R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Jackendoff,
Jackendoff,

R.

(1990).

Semantic

Jackendoff,
Oxford

R.

(2004).

Foundations

Cambridge,
Brain,

of language:

MA: MIT

Press.
evolution.

grammar,

meaning,

New

York:

Press.

University

D., Thai,

Jackson-Maldonado,
lexical
Early

structures.

V. (1993).
D., Marchman,
V, Bates, E., 8c Guitierrez-Clellen,
in Spanish-speaking
infants and toddlers. Journal
of Child

development
20, 523-549.
Language,
M.
the role
E., Lieven,
E., 8c Tomasello,
Kidd,
(2006). Examining
children's
and processing
of sentential
complements.
acquisition

of

lexical

frequency

Cognitive

in

Development,

21, 93-107.
and
P., Conboy,
D., Nelson,
T, & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception
B., Padden,
for
the
"critical
later language
Language
Learning
Implications
period".
development:
and Development,
1, 237-264.
and experience: Evidence from the blind child.
L. R. (1985). Language
B., 8c Gleitman,
Landau,
MA: Harvard
Press.
University
Cambridge,

Kuhl,

The
B. (1993). English verb classes and alternations: A preliminary
investigation. Chicago:
of Chicago
Press.
University
utterances.
In E. Clark 8c B. Kelly
E. (2006). Producing
multiword
Lieven,
(Eds.), Construc
CA: CSLI Publications.
tions in acquisition
Stanford,
(pp. 83-110).
A
M. (2003). Early syntactic
H., Speares,
E., Behrens,
Lieven,
creativity:
J., 8c Tomasello,
Child
333-367.
30,
Journal
of
Language,
usage-based
approach.
Levin,

B.

MacWhinney,

(2004).

A multiple
solution
process
883-914.
Child
31,
Language,
of

Journal
acquisition.
M. J., Hirsh-Pasek,
Maguire,
verb
learning:
Putting
Action meets word: How

to the

logical

problem

of

language

R. M.
of word
& Golinkoff,
(2006). A unified
theory
R. Golinkoff
context.
In K. Hirsh-Pasek
in
8c
(Eds.),
acquisition
New York: Oxford
children learn verbs (pp. 364-391).
University
K.,

Press.
S., Dromi,

Maital,

development
tions. Journal
Maratsos,

M.

(2007).

E.,

Sagi, A.,

8c Bornstein,

M.

inventory:
Language
specific
27, 43-67.
of Child Language,
Commentary.

Monographs

H.

(2000).

properties

The
and

Hebrew
cross-linguistic

of the Society for Research

communicative
generaliza

in Child Development,

72,121-126.
A
in lexical and morphological
V, 8c Bates, E. (1994). Continuity
Marchman,
development:
test of the critical mass hypothesis.
21, 339-366.
of Child Language,
Journal
nature
V A., Martinez-Sussman,
C, & Dale, P. S. (2004). The
Marchman,
language-specific
learners. Developmental
from bilingual
Evidence
of grammatical
language
development:
Science,

7, 212.

108
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

REFERENCES

F. (1992).
T. J., 8c Xu,
G. E, Pinker,
M., Rosen,
M., Hollander,
S., Ullman,
in Child
in language
of the society for Research
acquisition. Monographs
Overregularization
57(4, Serial No. 228).
Development,
in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge,
E. (1989). Categorization
and naming
Markman,
MA: MIT Press.

Marcus,

M. (2005). The
in
role of frequency
A., 8c Tomasello,
order. Cognitive Development,
20, 121-136.
M. (2006). The
effect of perceptual
A., 8c Tomasello,
use of
on young
children's
Ap
referring
expressions.

E., Theakston,
Matthews,
D., Lieven,
of English word
the acquisition
E., Theakston,
Matthews,
D., Lieven,
and prior discourse
availability

27, 403-422.
plied Psycholinguistics,
E. V. M. (2006).
of chil
the abstractness
K., Pine, J. M., 8c Lieven,
McClure,
Investigating
structure. Journal
of argument
dren's early knowledge
33, 693-720.
of Child Language,
Press.
to talk. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge
McShane,
University
J. (1980). Learning
In U.
and early lexical development.
object
categories
in
and intellectual factors
Ecological
conceptual development:
Press.
New York: Cambridge
University
(pp. 201-233).
categorization
as a cue for
in child directed
T
frames
Mintz,
(2003).
categories
grammatical
Frequent
90, 91-117.
speech. Cognition,
of
theorems:
P. C. M. (2008). On the implications
of the classical ergodic
Molenaar,
Analysis

C. B. (1987). Child-basic
Mervis,
Neisser
(Ed.), Concepts and

variation.
has to focus on intra-individual
processes
Developmental
developmental
60-69.
50,
chobiology,
use syntax to learn verb meanings.Journal
L. (1990). Children
of Child Language,
Naigles,
357-374.
L.

Naigles,

verb learners,
too? In E. Clark
one-year-olds
English-speaking
research
the
annual
child
28th
Stanford,
199-212).
forum
(pp.
language
of
and Information.
for the Study of Language

(1997).

Proceedings
The Center

Are

in the use

L. (1998). Developmental
Naigles,
changes
L. Lipsitt, & H. Hayne
Rovee-Collier,
London:
Ablex.
298-318).

L., Bavin, E., 8c Smith, M. (2005). Toddlers


sentences.
Science, 8, 424-431.
Developmental
L. R. (1993).
L., Gleitman,
H., 8c Gleitman,
Naigles,
In E. Dromi
from syntactic
evidence.
ponents
Naigles,

(pp.
opmental perspective
L., 8c Hoff-Ginsberg,
Naigles,
of syntactic bootstrapping.
L., & Hoff-Ginsberg,
Naigles,
Effects

of

Language,
L. R.
Naigles,
language.
L. R.
Naigles,

input frequency
25, 95-120.
(2002).

Form

86,
Cognition,
(2003). Paradox

of

structure

(Eds.), Advances

17,

(Ed.),
CA:

In C.
learning.
research (Vol. 12, pp.

in verb

in infancy
verbs

recognize

Psy

in novel

situations

and

com
acquire word meaning
and cognition: A devel
(Ed.), Language
Norwood,
104-140).
NJ: Ablex.
E. (1995).
for the plausibility
Input to verb learning: Evidence
Developmental
Psychology, 31, 827-837.
are some verbs
E. (1998). Why
learned before
other verbs?
and

structure

on

is easy, meaning
157-199.

Children

children's

is hard:

early

Resolving

verb
a

use. Journal

of Child

in early

paradox

child

to Tomasello
lost? No, paradox
found! Reply
8cAkhtar
(2003).
88, 325-329.
Cognition,
L. R., 8c Hoff,
E. (2006). Verbs at the very beginning:
between
Parallels
compre
Naigles,
In K. Hirsh-Pasek
and input.
& R. Golinkoff
hension
(Eds.), Action meets word: How
children
L. R.,

Naigles,
M.

Shatz

learn verbs

New York: Oxford


Press.
(pp. 336-363).
University
L. D. (2007). Syntactic
8c Swensen,
for word
supports
learning.
New
(Eds.), The handbook of language development
(pp. 212-231).

In E. Hoff
York:

&

Black

well.
K. (1973). Structure
and
Nelson,
in Child Development,
Research

strategy
38(1-2,

to talk. Monographs
in learning
No.
149).

of the Society for

Serial

109
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Newman,
R.,
Infants'
later

Bernstein
early

to

ability

language

N.,

Ratner,

Jusczyk,

A. M., Jusczyk,
conversational

P. W.,

the

segment
A retrospective

development:

analysis.

8c Dow,

speech

K. A.

(2006).

signal

Developmental

predicts
42,
Psychology,

643-655.
A.

in syntactic development
and the question
Child
26, 619-653.
of
Language,
typical transitivity. Journal
in syntactic development.
the role of semantic
Ninio, A. (2005a). Testing
similarity
Child Language,
32, 35-61.
Ninio,

(1999).

verbs

Pathbreaking

of proto
Journal

A. (2005b). Accelerated
semantic
Indirect
similarity:
learning without
objects.
nitive Linguistics,
16, 531-556.
A. (2007). Language
and the learning curve: A new theory of syntactic development.
Ninio,
York: Oxford
Press.
University
Ninio,

of

Cog
New

from the Japanese


Yamashita,
T, 8c Dale, P. S. (1993). Some findings
Y, Murase,
the
communicative
inventories.
Memoirs
early
of
development
Faculty
of Education,
Shimane University,
27, 26-38.
(In English).
M. (1993). Twenty-five
month
do not have a gram
old children
R., 8c Tomasello,
Olguin,
T,

Ogura,

of verb. Cognitive Development,


8, 245-272.
category
different
C. (1998). Comparing
of the development
models
E., 8c Rowland,
J., Lieven,
of the English
verb category.
36, 807-830.
Linguistics,
MA: Harvard
S. (1984). Language
Pinker,
Cambridge,
learnability and language development.
Press.
University
matical

Pine,

S. (1989). Learnability
Pinker,
MA: MIT Press.
Pinker, S. (1994).
410.

How

could

and

The acquisition

cognition:

a child use verb

syntax

of argument

to learn verb

structure.

semantics?

Cambridge,

Lingua,

92,377

to system building: Acquiring


V (1993). From rote learning
verb
K., 8cMarchman,
nets. Cognition,
in children
and connectionist
48, 21-69.
morphology
to intentions-in-action
in learning
attention
D., 8c Forbes, J. N.
Poulin,
(2002). Toddlers'
novel action words. Developmental
Psychology, 38, 104-114.
Plunkett,

model
and action: A two-tiered
D., 8c Forbes, J. N. (2006). Word,
intentions,
In K. Hirsh-Pasek
8c R. Golinkoff
word
(Eds.), Action meets word: How
learning.
Press.
New York: Oxford
children learn verbs (pp. 262-285).
University
L. A.
in early
Rescorla,
(1980). Overextension
development.
Journal
of Child
language
Poulin-Dubois,
of action

7, 321-335.
Language,
in
twin study of intelligence
Reznick,
J. (1997). A longitudinal
J. S., Corley, R., & Robinson,
in Child Development,
the second
62(1, Serial
year. Monographs
of the Society for Research
No. 249).
in the verb lexicons of children with specific
L., 8c Bode, J. (1993). GAPS
language
First Language,
13, 113-131.
impairment.
Mod
C. B. (1998). Disentangling
B. E, 8c Mervis,
Robinson,
early language
development:
an extension
of case-study
using
methodology.
acquisition
eling lexical and grammatical

Rice,

M.

Psychology, 34, 363-375.


Developmental
P. R., Snow, C, 8cWillett,
Rollins,
J. (1996). Predictors
First Language,
16, 243-259.
developments.
E. D. (2007). Domain-general
Saffran, J. R., & Thiessen,
Shatz

(Eds.),

Blackwell

handbook

of

language

of MLU:

learning
development

Semantic

and morphological

capacities.
68-86).

(pp.

In E. Hoff

& M.

Oxford,

UK:

Blackwell

Publishing.
C. M., 8c Smith,
Sandhofer,
different
frequencies,
Shirai,

(1998).

disposition?

L. B.
kinds

The
emergence
First Language,

nouns and verbs in the input: Differential


(2000). Counting
of learning? Journal
of child language, 27, 561-585.
in Japanese:
Universal
of tense-aspect
pre
morphology
18, 281-309.

no
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

REFERENCES

Smiley, P., 8c Huttenlocher,


and
events, objects,

J. (1995).
persons.

and
Conceptual
development
In M. Tomasello
8cW. Merriman

for
early words
(Eds.), Beyond names for
Erlbaum.
NJ: Lawrence
the child's

things: Young children's acquisition of verbs (pp. 21-61). Hillsdale,


8c Sachs, J. (1990). Cognition
and the verb lexicon
in early
C,
11, 409-424.
Applied Psycholiguistics,

lexical

Smith,

L.

Smith,

Children's

(1999).

ized

noun

learning: How
In B. MacWhinney

mechanisms.

learning
80). Hillsdale,
Smith, L. (2000).

development.

make
processes
learning
(Ed.), The emergence of language

general

special
(pp. 29

NJ: Erlbaum.
crane.
how to learn words: An associative
In R. Golinkoff,
Learning
8c L. Bloom
(Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition
York: Oxford
Press.
University

Hirsh-Pasek,
New
51-80).

K.
(pp.

L. R. (2004). Why
it is hard to label our concepts.
In G. Hall 8c S.
Snedeker,
J., 8c Gleitman,
a lexicon
Waxman
MA: MIT
Press.
(Eds.), Weaving
(pp. 257-293).
Cambridge,
Press.
(2007). Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Snyder, W
K.
In D. McDaniel,
children's
Stromswold,
(1996).
spontaneous
Analyzing
speech.
8c H. S. Cairns
(Eds.), Methods
MA: MIT Press.
Cambridge,
Theakston,
A., Lieven,
E., Pine, J., 8c Rowland,
itations in the acquisition
of verb argument
Child Language,
28, 127-152.
C. McKee,

A.

Theakston,

generality,
61-99.

L.,

children's

assessing

syntax

(pp.

23-54).

C. (2001). The
role of performance
lim
structure: An alternative
account.
Journal
of

E. V. M.,
C. F. (2004).
Semantic
Pine, J. M., & Rowland,
and the acquisition
of syntax. Journal
31,
of Child Language,

Lieven,

input

for

frequency

A. L., Lieven,
M. (2003). The
Theakston,
E., 8cTomasello,
of third person
in English.
verbs
singular
Journal
Research, 46, 863-877.

role of the input


of Speech,

Language,

S. A., 8c Hopper,
Thompson,
P.J.
Evidence
from conversation.

structure,

and argument

clause
(2001). Transitivity,
In J. Bybee 8c R Hopper

in the acquisition
and Hearing
structure:

(Eds.), Frequency and the emergence of


the Netherlands:
linguistic structure (pp. 27-60). Amsterdam,
John Benjamins.
M. (1992). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical
UK:
Tomasello,
development. Cambridge,
Press.
Cambridge
University
M.
children
have adult syntactic
Tomasello,
(2000). Do young
74,
competence?
Cognition,
209-253.
Tomasello,

M.,

8cAkhtar,

N.

(2003). What

paradox?

to Naigles

A response

(2002).

Cognition,

88,317-323.
on actions:
verbs in ostensive
Tomasello,
M., & Kruger, A. (1992). Joint attention
Acquiring
and non-ostensive
contexts.
Child
19, 311-333.
of
Journal
Language,
W. (1995). Beyond names for
Tomasello,
M., & Merriman,
things: Young children's acquisition of
verbs. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
M., 8c Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling
31,
enough? Journal
of Child Language,
Valian, V. (1990). Null
subjects: A problem
35, 105-122.
quisition.
Cognition,

Tomasello,

Valian,

V, Prasada,
S., 8c Scarpa, J.
children's
imitation
of sentences.

M., Waterfall,
H.,
Vasilyeva,
across
and differences
Z. (1972).
Vendler,
Press.
Wagner,

L.

Language,

(2002).

Res

spontaneous

How

speech:

much

is

for

parameter-setting

models

of

ac

language

on young
Effects
object predictability:
Child
247-269.
33,
Journal
of
Language,
8c Huttenlocher,
of syntax: Commonalities
J. (2008). Emergence
children.
Science, 11, 84-97.
Developmental

cogitans: An

Aspectual
28, 661-682.

children's
101-121.

(2006).

Direct

essay in rational psychology.

influences

on

early

tense

Ithaca,

NY: Cornell

comprehension.

Journal

University
of Child

Ill
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

in part by the Large Research Grant Pro


This research was supported
of Connecticut.
gram of the University
We thank Cynthia Fisher, James Magnuson,
and our reviewers for their
constructive

comments

on

previous

versions

of

the

manuscript.

We

are

to Danielle

also

Popp, Roberta Golinkoff, Aylin Kuntay, Marilyn Shatz,


grateful
Lars Bergman,
Yuriko Oshima-Takane,
and Whitney Tabor for helpful dis
the work. This research would not have been possible
cussions regarding
without
the dedication
of the parents who kept these diaries of their chil
Eliane Ramos,
dren's first verbs. Special thanks go to Jennifer Coughlan,
for their assistance during data collection and manu
and Ashley Maltempo
script

preparation.

112
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

EARLY
FLEXIBILITY
AND SYNTAX
OF CHILDREN'S
VERBUSE
INTHESEMANTICS
Tomasello and Silke Brandf

Michael

are where the


Objects are boring; they just sit there. Events and actions
that probably 90% of all of
That is why it is so puzzling
action is?literally.
the work

done

on

children's

early

word

on children's

the current monograph

on

focuses

learning

acquisition

and

nouns,

why

is so welcome.

of verbs

OF VERBS
INPRAISE
Linguistic

almost

communication

some

event,

or

action,

concerns

always

or informatives

states of affairs. Declaratives


state

of

affairs,

and

events,

or

imperatives

or

actions,

invite the listener

to attend

directives

to

enjoin

to bring about a desired action or state of


the listener to do something
affairs. Thus, even when young children are using object labels as single
word
whole

from

utterances,
there

is almost

the
always

point
some

of view

of

underlying

the

communicative
event

or

action

as a

intention
at

issue.

When

to attend to it or
the infant exclaims "Airplane!" she is exhorting her mother
to notice its presence,
and when the infant requests
'Juice!" she is rousing
into action to satisfy her desire. One could argue that the ap
her mother
is something
along the lines of "Look at
propriate gloss of such utterances
the airplane!"
(or "The airplane is therel") and "Get me some juice!" The
action or state of affairs intended, and its corresponding
verb, is implicit; the
utterance
is what has been called a holophrase.
The one potential
is naming objects. But naming objects is
exception
a
kind
of
actually
metalinguistic
speech act. It is not using language but
rather

"mentioning"

it, mostly

teaching

it.Western,

middle-class

parents

do

this with some regularity with their children, and their children
learn the
names and then show off by using them in return. But in many other
or demonstrative
cultures the pedagogical
naming of objects is a very rare
act
of
in the acquisition of language.
and
little
role
type
speech
plays very

113
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

AND SILKE
MICHAELTOMASELLO
BRANDT

And even in Western,


culture parents only rarely explicitly
middle-class
teach words other than object labels. With verbs and other types of words,
children must in almost all cases learn them within the ongoing flow of social
and

interaction

discourse?on

their

so

own,

to

on

speak?based

un

their

of the intentional actions of others in the social interaction. For


derstanding
children are able to infer the referent action of a novel verb
young
example,
even when they hear it only as a directive
to act or as an anticipation
of an
even
never
see
at
if
action
the
referent
action
they
impending
actually
on the adult's accompanying
all?based
behavior and context
(Tomasello,
in the acquisition
is so interesting.

1992, 1995, 2001). This social-cognitive


complexity
cess is another reason why the acquisition of verbs
a

From
are

as

referents,

are

of

point

more

nouns

sponding
Even

semantic

much

also

complex

children's

or

causative

not

(e.g.,

their

are

nouns

vs.

for

for other
concrete

corre

kinds

in whether

have),

they

of

objects).

learn and use vary in whether

that children
give

used

mostly

verbs

corresponding
and
their
objects

than

for the moment

nouns

early

the very earliest verbs

and

conceptually

aside

(setting

events

view,

pro

they
results

designate

(e.g., clean the table vs. wipe the table), in whether


they designate
of motion or not (e.g., roll vs. move), in whether
they are defined by a
they are about spe
particular objects or not (e.g., hammer vs. hit), in whether
or more abstract changes of state (e.g., lick, involving
cific bodily movements
or not

manner

movements

specific

of

on what

depending

different
vs.

events

tongue,
from
the

vs.

events

completed

cut,
to a

that may

of actions

aspects

vs.
lawn

the

is cut,

different
very
to mention

involving
finger)?not

be designated

grammatically
vs.

events

impending

events

past

actions
all of

the

(ongoing
vs.

future

events, etc.). Indeed, Gentner


(1982) famously stressed that inmany cases it is
to
to
know how
difficult
"package" the ongoing flow of expe
conceptually
into

rience

discrete

Further
one or more

as

events,

to

referred

by

typically designated
participants,
event
not in any way "given" by the phenomenal
can be used so as to highlight different participants
use,

e.g.,

John

broke

the vase,

at all.

verbs,

is the fact that events

in this direction

two

with

participants

and verbs always involve


linguistically by nouns and
itself (indeed, some verbs
on different occasions of
vs. The

indicated,

vase

broke,

with only one indicated). And so, in a sense, events incorporate objects but
not the reverse. And this is the final way in which verbs are especially
in the study of child language acquisition. As
important and interesting
from all theoretical backgrounds
stressed by researchers
(e.g., Pinker, 1989;
the participants
Tomasello,
1992), verbs have as part of their very meaning
if there is
famous
Blake's
involved
observation,
(paraphrasing William
a
must
of verbs
the
be
there
dancer). This makes
acquisition
dancing
a

already

verbs
that

step

involves
form

the

on

the

way

the kind
backbone

to

grammatical

of verb-argument
of mature

sentence

competence,

as

the

(event-participant)

learning

of

structures

structure.

114
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

is so
It is thus for all of these reasons that the current monograph
in the field. Verbs are arguably the most complex and important
and they have been sorely
type of word in early language development,
in
8c
8c
the
Hirsh-Pasek
Golinkoff,
2006; Tomasello
(see
papers
neglected
some
notable
for
Merriman,
1995,
exceptions).
welcome

METHOD
INPRAISE
OF THEDIARY
in other areas of developmental
Researchers
sometimes
psychology
over
use
of
of
the
in
the
diaries
parent
puzzle
study
language acquisition.
But there are very good reasons for the use of this method,
and the current
monograph
child pairs.

involves

the innovative

extension

of using multiple

parent

is that children talk too much. A child is awake and


basic problem
around
but the normal sampling regime in
talking
lOhr/day, 7 days/week,
a
of one of those 70 hr/week.
research
is
maximum
language acquisition
This is a sample of 1-2% of what the child says. The reason researchers do
not sample more
is because
it takes from 10 to 20 hr to transcribe
1hr of
sam
conversation.
this
this
1-2%
But, despite
taped
good practical reason,
The

ple is inherently limited, potentially biased in all the ways that small samples
may be biased in all scientific inquiry, and it very likely misses completely
most
And so, for example,
it has been quan
low-frequency
phenomena.
a
demonstrated
that
with
small
the age at which
titatively
sample estimating
or
the child acquires low-frequency
structure
items
is highly un
linguistic
reliable (Rowland 8c Fletcher, 2006; Tomasello
8c Stahl, 2004).
Diaries
have their own limitations
of course. Most obviously,
they
one
involve
external validity?and
par
prototypically
only
child?limiting
ents are not trained scientific observers. But with the collection of
multiple
diaries
study?the

of

comparable
first
problem

children?again,
is to some

an
degree

innovation
overcome.

of
And,

the
in

current
terms

of

the limitations of parents as observers,


it has been established by researchers
the
MacArthur
Communicative
(a paper
using
Development
Inventory
assessment
of children's vocabulary
filled out by parents)
that
and-pencil
are
at
overt
reliable
their
child's
behaviors
parents
actually highly
recording
true if parents are directed what to
(Fenson et al., 1994). This is especially
look for. And the current study did an especially good job of this
by focusing
parents' attention on a smallish number of specific verbs that their children
might be using.
to diaries
that is often not
Finally, there is one further advantage
stressed. Because
the parent
is recording
(of some
basically everything
one
can
focal
conclude
with
reasonable
designated
phenomena),
certainty

115
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

MICHAELTOMASELLO
AND SILKE
BRANDT

the child does not do something, which can never be concluded with
small- or even medium-sized
samples. In the study of language acquisition,
to which
this is often important since one of the key issues is the degree
children use their early language flexibly and productively,
and to deter
mine
that we must know not only what they have done but also what they
is con
might have done but did not. And indeed, the current monograph
and productivity,
and so the
cerned precisely with the issues of flexibility
when

diary method

is especially

appropriate.

FLEXIBILITY
INEARLY
VERBUSE
data of the current monograph
provide us with by far the most
in
of how English-speaking
the
literature
picture
complete,
quantitative
the limitations of the diary
children learn and use their early verbs. Within
focused parents on a subset
method
(including the fact that the researchers
we
now
at our disposal a vast and
have
of 34 preselected
target verbs),1
The

array

important

of

new

facts.

Flexibility in Verb Use: Pragmatics and Semantics


Parents
of

each

kept the diary until they had recorded

targeted

verb,

that

is,

10 utterances

containing

10 actual
the

instances of use
verb

in question.

Across all of the verbs used by all of the children, the average time it took for
the 10 instances to be produced
Prag
by the child was a bit over 1month.
as commands or
either
of
their
verbs
used
about
half
the
children
matically,
as descriptions
only; the other half were used in both functions. Foreshad
an
issue that will come up later with regard to syntax, we do not know
owing
in which of these instances children heard their parents using the verb in
and so we do not know if children are simply
both pragmatic
functions,
use
or
the
adult
doing something more creative in using these
following
there are no ex
with
verbs. And because
functions
particular
pragmatic
we
in
this
the literature addressing
simply do not know.
question,
periments
current
found that
the
From the point of view of semantics,
monograph
the children used their newly acquired verbs relatively flexibly from rel
was not a foregone conclusion. Although
research has
atively early. This fact
shown that children use their object labels in fairly flexible ways from early
on (Harris, Barrett, Jones, & Brookes,
1988), a number of different nat
of young children using
have provided
uralistic observations
examples
more
context
in
bound
verbs and other types of words
ways for some time
never
But
been clear whether
it
has
1988; Tomasello,
1992).
(e.g., Gopnik,
or
are
reflect some
whether
those
they might
examples
unrepresentative
more systematic difference with object labels.

116
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

study, children did not confine their use of verbs to a


defined
action type; they used almost 40% of their verbs to
single, narrowly
refer to different
action types (e.g., a person coming vs. a TV show com
if "different action
this number would undoubtedly
be higher
ing)?and
In the current

had

type"

been

more

defined

new

These

loosely.

uses

on

occurred,

average,

after about four uses of the verb in question. And children quite often used
their verbs for different actors and patients of these actions as well (whether
these were
for something
around
three
lexically realized or not). Thus,
to
events
actors
of
their
children
referred
different
verbs,
quarters
involving
on

different

events
actors

question.
or

Although

other diary
to novel

patients

are

about

different

involving
and/or

usage

and

occasions,

being

studies

referents

we

same

the

patients

occurred

after

cannot

be sure

more

creative

in

around

In both

Bowerman,

uses

three

if the children

their

semantic

in the past have noted many


(e.g.,

were

proportion

of the actions.

to refer

used

to

cases,

the new

of

verb

the

are following
in all

extensions

cases of creative

in

adult
cases,

extensions

1982).

use much

of their early language in recurrent social


seem fairly high, and
situations and routines, these percentages
they would
seem to refute any proposal
that children are inflexible in their early use of
verbs to refer to events and actions in the world. One might ask how this
flexibility compares with children's early use of object labels, but the answer
is that we do not know because we do not have quite such detailed data in
this case. There are some diary studies (especially Dromi,
1987) that have
focused on the acquisition of object labels in enough detail to provide some
relevant data for comparison,
but since the two types of word are so differ
ent semantically,
it is difficult to think how such a comparison
could be done
Given

that children

quantitatively.

In

any

case,

the

general

conclusion

would

seem

to be

that

children use both their early nouns and their early verbs
English-speaking
in reasonably flexible ways to refer to all kinds of objects and events in the
world.

This having been said, it is unclear that?despite


the literature review
in the current monograph?anyone
any
espousing
theory of word learning
would predict that children in the second half of their 2nd year of life would
still be using any kinds of words in highly context bound ways. Virtually all
of the proposals for early context bound use have focused on the first half of
the 2nd year of life or even earlier.

Flexibility in Verb Use: Syntax


The most

contentious
issue in the monograph
is children's
flexibility
and productivity
in using their early verbs syntactically. We deal first with
the issue of flexibility, which,
in the current context, means children using
their verbs in syntactically diverse contexts.
In the following section, we will

117
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

MICHAELTOMASELLO
AND SILKEBRANDT

deal with the issue of productivity,


in the current context, means
which,
children using their verbs in syntactic contexts
that go beyond
those in
which they have heard them being used. Productivity
is an especially con
tentious issue as it involves the degree to which young children's language is
underlain
by abstract, lexically general,
linguistic categories and construc
tions (rules). But the two issues are tied together; hence, let us first sort them
a bit.

out

Several authors,
have proposed
that children do
including ourselves,
not have innate and abstract grammatical
that
categories
apply equally
across all words as part of their beginning
basic
linguistic competence?the
Chomskian

proposal.
revolves
around

petence

Instead,
specific

of

much

or

verbs

their
other

early
items

grammatical
as
such

com
pronouns

& Baldwin,
1997; Tomasello,
1992, 2000, 2003). Thus,
with
constructions?some
of which have
begin
lexically specific
been
called verb
island constructions
because
revolve
around
they
verbs?with
In
abstractions.
this
local,
only
lexically
specific
theory,
if the young child says "Doggie kiss me," the item-based construction
in
to such
volved might be something
like kisser KISS kissee, without reference
less subjects and objects.
things as agents and patients, much
verb-general
With this item-based construction,
the child could learn a new object label
use it as either "kisser" or "kissee"
and immediately
without
productively,
on
an
others
do
based
of
all
the
items
involved.
this,
hearing
understanding
Pine,

(Lieven,
children

But

what

she

could

not

do

is to

learn

a new

verb,

say

hug,

and

immediately

an analogy to kiss and use it with a


"hugger" and a "hugee" without
is a structural
having heard others do it. Each lexically specific construction

make

island.

comes from both naturalistic


The evidence
for this proposal
observa
tions and experiments.
Many studies of natural samples of children's early
language show restricted use of words and lexically specific schemas (e.g.,
Lieven, Behrens,
2003; Lieven et al., 1997), and this
Speares, & Tomasello,
restricted use has been taken to reflect a relative lack of lexically general
that can be used totally productively.
But most
categories and constructions
to
current
the
data isTomasello's
(1992) original diary study. In
comparable
that study there were two basic findings relevant to issues of verb flexibility
and productivity.
The first was that the majority of this diary child's early verbs were used
in fairly simple sentence frames, often only one frame per verb, and differ
ent verbs most often had different frames?suggesting
that they were each
an

and

island

graph
studies

not

addresses,
are

very

and, as noted

generally

for both. The

criteria
half full, depending

flexible.

very

These

are

the

data

by the authors,

comparable,

once

one

that

the

current

mono

the data of the two diary


adopts

the

same

coding

outcome
is the story of a glass either half empty or
on one's perspective.
Here
is a quick summary of the

118
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

(based on the diaries

relevant data from the current monograph


children that were fully analyzed).

Of the few verbs the children used at 18 months,


in more

used

children's

were

verbs
of

55%

months,
sentence

one

than

sentence

the

children's

the entire period


verbs

Across

more

with

the

16% of

entire

their
or

present
the

of diary collection,
one

than

point

of

view.

children

used

as one

used

intransitive

verbs

word

the perspective
the eight

one

children

morphological

object.

used

only

form

(e.g.,

over

course

the

or with

utterances

word

of

the

lexically ex

subject and object

one

only

with

five

four

children

The

authors

the

children

three

the

claims of lexical

ground general
pretty flexible.
But the alternative

the

course

of

the

(53%

lexically expressed

one

to five

current

at 24 months

frames,

not

have
child

such

single
had one

of age,

frame
such

that

or not
depends

and

on one's
as

monograph?taking

specificity

they

frame,

frames.

this syntactic flexibility


of

over

used

sentence
did

or more,

verbs
had

children

the subject

utterances).
of

of

used

that

used with

as one

used

one considers

Whether

collection,

and

subject,

expressed).

the

three

expressed

diary
than

used with both

study, 30% were

From

that

verbs

(56% were

argument

lexically

children used 31% of

tense).

past

transitive

pressed

were

24
one

than

than one lexically expressed

of
period
with more

verbs

study, 35% were

Of

the

At

frame.

in more

used

of

44%

sentence

were

verbs

43% of their verbs with more

Of

20 months,

one

than

15% were

only

frame.

Across
their

in more

used

At

frame.

of the eight

back

in early grammars?think

it is

of these kinds of data by proponents


interpretation
of lexical specificity has typically used as background
the expectation
that
children's language is structured by a set of abstract syntactic categories and
constructions
that apply across all relevant
lexical items equally. In this
one
use
one third of their transitive
wonders
children
about
context,
why
and intransitive verbs with all of the syntactically required arguments,
but
the other two thirds without. And why do children at around 2 years of age
still use almost half of their verbs in only one sentence frame? And why
are

there

perspective,

so

few

sentence

although

frames

children

used

across

multiple

show some flexibility,

verbs?

it does

not

From

this

look even

119
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

MICHAELTOMASELLO
AND SILKE
BRANDT

like all of their utterances

remotely

neutral,

lexically

syntactic

derive
and

categories,

from a set of preexisting,

abstract,

rules.

this disagreement
about flexibility
is really the issue of
In
the
verb
island
children
may be as flexible as
productivity.
hypothesis,
use
in
like
their
of
verbs
in
you
particular
syntactic frames and morpho
as
logical paradigms?so
long
they have heard all of these uses in the lan
around
With
them.
each verb, they learn various ways of talking from
guage
the discourse
interactions
is, for each verb
they have with adults?that
is cut
(1992) diary, a paradigm
separately. And so, in Tomasello's
example
Underlying

versus

draw.

frame,

whereas

similar

was

Cut

draw

used
(learned

was

semantics)

by

used

Tomasello's
at about
in many

child
same

the
different

one
in
basic
only
time
and
having
ones.
The
general

sentence
somewhat
explana

to many
tion is that, for whatever
reason, the child either heard or attended
different syntactic uses of draw, but not so for cut. Thus, flexibility of use for a
particular verb is purely a function of the language the child has heard and
to with that verb, along with her motivation
attended
for speaking about
these kinds of events.
And so flexibility with particular verbs does not signal lexically general
across all verbs, unless one has evidence
that the flexibility
is
productivity
a
to
due
child's creative generalization
and not simply to a reproduction
of
adult flexibility with each particular verb individually. Children will become
across verbs
in all kinds of ways at some point?including
productive
and

constructions?it

is just

that

initially

they

are

not.

This

is

important

If children begin lexically based and become


it speaks to process.
because
more productive
only gradually, then the likelihood is that the process is not
one

of

constructing

general
from

activating
ever

cognitive

mature

innate
more

syntactic
general

processes

language

and

categories
syntactic

(e.g.,

categories

statistical

rules,

but

and

rules

learning,

rather
on

analogy)

the

one

of

basis

of

and

input

users.

INEARLY
PRODUCTIVITY
VERBUSE
in the
is thus the key theoretical
issue. Indeed,
Syntactic productivity
on
was
we
the
entire
focus
first draft of this monograph
reviewed)
(which
reason
from
theoretical
for
this
derives
The
Chomsky's
mainly
productivity.
children go be
If from the beginning
poverty of the stimulus argument.
ways, this
yond the language they have heard in creative and productive
and
schemas
have
abstract
that
(or
suggests
categories
they
preexisting
that the
is the argument
their early language. This
rules) that generate
authors tried to make in the first draft. But the reviewers simply noted that
without any data about what language the children had heard, one can talk
about flexibility but not about productivity. One cannot say that children are

120
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

going beyond what they have heard if one does not know what they have
is indeed the issue these authors wish to
heard. That syntactic productivity
near
in
the end of the monograph:
is
evident
their
quote
push
These

are more

findings

guage acquisition
the positions
olds do not

sello, 2000).

that
have

consistent

(e.g., Chomsky,
children
abstract

are
syntax

with

the

view

generativist

of

child

lan

1981; Gleitman & Fisher, 2002) than with

conservative
(e.g.,

Goldberg,

language
1998;

users
Lieven,

and

that

2-year

2006;

Toma

(p. 107)2

as well as in
in the original verb island formulation,
But, to reiterate,
are
other item-based
children
approaches,
syntactically productive?but
only in limited
Thus, as alluded
teaches a child
diately stick it
terms
predicate

within
their item-based
constructions.
ways constrained
to above, from as early as they are combining words, if one
a novel noun in an experiment
(e.g., the wug), they imme
into the syntactic slot of many of their existing verbs and
straightaway,
saying such productive
things as "Wug gone"
never
and "Find wug"?even
before heard wug com
though they have
bined with any other word (Tomasello, Akhtar, 8c Rekau,
1997). Early in
most sentence frames are structured by verbs (like gone and
development
slots freely and pro
find), each of which fills new words into its argument
children are productive with their language in some ways
ductively. Hence,
from near the beginning
of multiword
speech.
It is just that transfer of structure across verb island constructions
does
not happen as the result of the pregiven
and abstract categories of formal
as a result of both what the child hears and her
linguistics but develops
across initially very low-scope schemas. This is
generalizations
developing
illustrated by another analysis performed
in Tomasello's
(1992) diary study.
The
the child learned one of her verbs in a full
logic was this. Suppose
for example, saying many things of the form x FINDy.
transitive construction,
If this construction was fully abstract and verb general, one would expect to
see her using other verbs in the same frame pretty soon after, that is,
given the
so.
to
not
one
But
do
that
is
what
the
data
If
showed.
looked at
opportunity
the development
of the sentence frames used with a particular verb across
time, the child added grammatical
complexity only gradually and incremen
never happened
for
each
verb
It
almost
that a verb that was at
tally
separately.
one point used in a simple sentence frame all of a sudden was used in a more
complex one, for example, based on some kind of transfer from another verb
that had recently been used in this more
frame. The summary
complex
statement was as follows: By far the best predictor of this child's use of a
given
verb on a given day was not her use of other verbs on that same day, but
rather her use of that same verb on immediately preceding days. Hence,
the
evidence here was that the overall developmental
initial
pattern suggested
for each verb.
syntactic independence

121
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

MICHAELTOMASELLO
AND SILKEBRANDT

on individual children
in the
comparable
analysis was performed
study. The analyses of the individual children (displayed in Figures
In terms of the number of sentence
11-19) basically showed the following.
frames used with each verb (flexibility), across time two children showed U
two children
showed a several
(Heather, Elaine);
shaped development
month
followed by a relatively rapid increase in
period with no flexibility
the
four children showed relatively quick
and
other
Sam);
(Carl,
flexibility
on.
This
kind
of
analysis does not look for any kind of
flexibility
early
No
current

across

transfer

sentence

frames.

recent

In another

study,

on

based

samples

of children's
(2006) claims to find
speech (not on a thorough diary), Ninio
lies the rub. Even in this case one cannot claim
such transfer. But herein
if one does not know what language
anything about transfer or productivity
the children have heard, or how their other cognitive abilities are devel
it could be that once a child's parents hear her using tran
oping. Hence,
sentences

sitive

more

sentences

with

one

often,

or more

verb,

this
saliently,

them

encourages
with

other

verbs.

to

use

Or

transitive

perhaps

the

the indi
this time, making
is expanding
child's working memory
during
of more complex verb island constructions
vidual acquisition
possible for
the first time.
di
The fundamental
point is that one cannot investigate productivity
have
the language the child has heard. Researchers
rectly without knowing
it of special importance when children overgeneralize
therefore considered
and

say

tion

that

things
this

like
sentence

"She

giggled
must
be

me"
generated

1982)?on

(Bowerman,
by

productive

the
schema,

assump
as the

have heard such a sentence with that verb in their ev


errors usually do not occur
(these productive
eryday linguistic environment
before age 3 years; Pinker, 1989). The other approach
is, of course, exper
in
iments. We and others have performed
literally dozens of experiments
see
a
we
in
various ways and
novel verb (e.g., tamming)
which
teach children
like the transitive or intransitive
if they then use it in existing constructions
to do so.
or passive?when
and motivation
opportunities
given multiple
as the authors of the monograph
is that children
The outcome,
note,
with newly
than 2.5-3 years of age show very little productivity
younger
child would

never

learned novel verbs?even


though older children show much productivity
with these same verbs learned in these same ways (see Tomasello,
2000,
2003, for reviews).
focus their critique only on the
The authors of the current monograph
we
and others have also done similar exper
but
experiments,
production
un
iments tapping the child's comprehension
(e.g., to see if children can
derstand who is doing what to whom in "The dog is tamming the cat") (e.g.,
8c Tomasello,
8c Tomasello,
Akhtar
2009; Dittmar,
1997; Chan, Lieven,
8c Tomasello,
Abbot-Smith,
Lieven,
2008a), as well as structural priming
using familiar English verbs (e.g., Savage, Lieven, Theakston,
experiments

122
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

8cTomasello,
across
ing

verb
the

in the

constructions
of

existence

we are testing for productivity

In all of these paradigms

2003).
island

abstract,

of

absence

relevant

input?suggest
in all cases

schemas?but

verb-general

the

before about 2.5-3 years of age.


finding is of very little syntactic productivity
The current authors focus instead on a recent preferential
looking study
with English-speaking
children by Gertner,
Fisher, and Eisengart
(2006),
in children under 2 years of age.
showing syntactic productivity
supposedly
of how to interpret
the results of a looking
aside the question
Leaving
in terms of the nature of underlying
that
discrimination
representations,
a
the
before
children
entered
the
used
experiment
study
training technique
that taught them key aspects of the construction
(i.e., it used the same actors
nouns
in both training and testing in the same semantic
and associated
When
children were tested without
such an initial
roles).
German-speaking
not
at this young
did
show
any syntactic productivity
training period,
they
so it seems
And
8c
Abbot-Smith,
Lieven,
Tomasello,
2008b).
age (Dittmar,
very

to us

clear,

at

that

least,

the

data

experimental

children across
and production,

in young

syntactic productivity
in both comprehension
ologies,
miliar verbs. One can always criticize
valid, but in this case there are many

show

several

a consistent

of

lack

different

method
both novel and fa

using
as not being ecologically
experiments
different methodologies
involved.
The main point is that there is no evidence
in the current monograph
for syntactic productivity
in 1- and 2-year-old
children?in
the sense of
across

generalizing

to

verbs

create

verb-general

construc

syntactic

we do not know what


tions?because
language
they have heard being
used around them, and there is no other indirect evidence either. Children
are flexible from the beginning,
as they learn from adults multiple
frames
for a given verb. And they are productive
in the limited sense that they can
for one another
in the syntactic slots of each
freely substitute participants
thus creating schemas such as x
verb-specific
syntactic frame individually,
on
But
based
Tomasello's
(1992) longitudinal analysis failing to find
FIND)).
transfer
children

across

verbs,

remain

on

and

conservative

the

extensive

with

their

syntactic

is that the grammatical


time, our own conclusion
children's
is not
competence
early linguistic
but rather
is concrete
and item-based?leading
productivity.
Perhaps
dren's

early

one reason
syntax

is that

for the slow buildup


to make

literature

experimental

generalizations

constructions

in which
some

for

organization
underlying
abstract and rule-based,
to highly
constrained

of productivity
across

such

in young
things

chil

as verb

island constructions
children must make complex analogies between com
structures
plex
(e.g., aligning agents with agents and patients with patients
in utterances with different
transitive verbs). The age at which they seem to
do this most readily in nonlinguistic
domains
corresponds
fairly well with
the age at which they seem to be doing itwith their language (see Gentner
8c

123
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

AND SILKEBRANDT
MICHAELTOMASELLO

Markman,
1997). And it is perhaps worth noting as well that psycholin
research
has shown that even adults' representations
of argument
guistic
structures are still fairly closely tied to specific lexical items (e.g., Trueswell
8cTanenhaus,
1994).

CONCLUSION
current monograph
is extremely
valuable and advances
the field
in
the
level
of
detail
and
thor
ways.
By exploiting
multiple
significantly
across
of
method?and
then
it
the
multiple
oughness
diary
employing
The

children?the

were

authors

to

able

collect

data

on

children's

early

acqui

assessments
sition of verbs that enabled all kinds of quantitative
simply not
now know new and important facts
in
We
other
kinds
of
studies.
possible
that
about children's
semantic flexibility with verbs in early development
that are
will help us to better formulate
theories of lexical development
inclusive of all word types. And we now know new and important facts about
children's syntactic flexibility with verbs that will help us to formulate better
come to generate
structure
and grammatically
theories of how children
we
in
critical
And
have
been
their early multiword
although
productions.
this commentary
about the claims of certain
in the current data set?we
actually believe
as

experiments?nevertheless,

flexibility
children's
dren

the

kinds of syntactic productivity


that that is impossible without

authors

out

point

in

several

places,

of
and so the investigation
is a precondition
for productivity,
a
is very important first step in figuring out how chil
flexibility
construct

ultimately

their

abstract

syntactic

constructions.

NOTES
of
for the exclusion
of verbs to focus on is well justified,
selection
except
early verbs, make, do, and have.
important
on p. 109 we get the somewhat
with
consistent
"One position
2. Although
contradictory:
are learned from the input, but the process
of
our data is that abstract grammatical
categories
or before
at
of
the
relevant
all
before
production
early,
speech production
learning
begins
frame or construction."
linguistic
1. The

three

authors'

fairly

References

Akhtar,

N.,

8cTomasello,

morphology.
M.
Bowerman,
late speech
146). New

M.

(1997).

children's

productivity
Developmental
Psychology, 33, 952-965.
to language
to talk worse: Clues
(1982). Starting
errors.
In S. Strauss 8c R. Stavy (Eds.), U-shaped

York: Academic

Young

with word

order

and word

acquisition
behavioral

from

children's

growth

(pp.

Press.

124
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

101?

COMMENTARY

M. (2009). Children's
of the agent-patient
A., Lieven,
E., & Tomasello,
understanding
Canton
between
in the transitive
construction:
relations
comparisons
Cross-linguistic
and English.
ese, German
20, 267-300.
Cognitive Linguistics,
com
M. (2008a). German
children's
K., Lieven,
E., 8c Tomasello,
Dittmar, M., Abbot-Smith,
Chan,

of word
prehension
1152-1167.
79(4),

order

K.,

M., Abbot-Smith,
Dittmar,
dren's
early syntactic
23, 48-66.
E.

Dromi,
Fenson,

case marking

and

Lieven,

competence:

in causative

Early lexical development. Cambridge,


P. S., Reznick,
J. S., Bates, E., Thai,
communicative
Monographs
development.
(1987).

L., Dale,

early

M.

E., 8c Tomasello,
A preferential

looking

sentences.

(2008b).

Child Development,
chil

German

Young

Science,

study. Developmental

UK: Cambridge
Press.
University
in
S. (1994). Variability
D., & Pethick,
in Child Devel
of the Society of Research

opment, 59(5).
versus natural
nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic
D. (1982). Why
Gentner,
relativity
In S. A. Kuczaj
Vol. 2. Language,
(Ed.), Language
thought and
development:
partitioning.
Erlbaum.
culture (pp. 301-334).
Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence
A. B. (1997). Structure
in analogy
and similarity. Amer
Gentner,
D., & Markman,
mapping
ican Psychologist,
52, 45-56.
words and rules: Abstract
8c Eisengart,
Gertner,
Y, Fisher, C,
J. (2006). Learning
knowledge
in early sentence
of word order
Science, 17(8), 684-691.
Psychological
comprehension.
A. (1988). Three
Social words,
words
and
types of early word:
Gopnik,
cognitive-relational
names
to cognitive
First Language,
and their relation
8, 49-70.
development.
S. (1988). Linguistic
M., Barrett, M., Jones,
D., 8c Brookes,
input and early word
15, 77-94.
meaning.
of Child Language,
Journal
R. M. (Eds.). (2006). Action meets word: How children learn verbs.
K., 8c Golinkoff,
Hirsh-Pasek,
New York: Oxford
Press.
University
Harris,

usage-based
Lieven,
E., Pine,
Ninio,

M.
H., Speares,
(2003). Early syntactic
J., 8c Tomasello,
creativity: A
333-367.
30(2),
of Child Language,
approach.
Journal
and early grammatical
G. (1997). Lexically
based
J., 8c Baldwin,
learning
187-219.
24(1),
of Child Language,
Journal
and the learning curve: A new theory of syntactic development. Oxford,
Language

E., Behrens,

Lieven,

development.
A. (2006).
UK: Oxford

University
S. (1989). Learnability
Pinker,
MA: MIT Press.

Press.
and cognition:

The acquisition

of argument

structure.

Cambridge,

on estimates
C. E, 8c Fletcher,
S. L. (2006). The
effect of sampling
of lexical
and error rates. Journal
33(4), 859-877.
of Child Language,
specificity
M. (2003). Testing
the abstractness
of
A., 8c Tomasello,
E., Theakston,
Savage, C, Lieven,
con
children's
Lexical
and structural
of syntactic
linguistic
representations:
priming

Rowland,

structions
Tomasello,

M.

Cambridge
M.
Tomasello,
Merriman

in young
(1992).

University
(1995).

Cambridge,

York:

contexts
In M. Tomasello
for early verb learning.
& W.
names for
children's
verbs
acquisition
of
Beyond
things: Young
(pp. 115?
Erlbaum.
NJ: Lawrence
Pragmatic

(2000).

Do

young

M. (2001). Perceiving
Tomasello,
M. Bowerman
8c S. Levinson
132-158).
M.
Tomasello,

New

(Eds.),

146). Hillsdale,
M.
Tomasello,
209-253.

Science, 6(5), 557-567.


Developmental
verbs: A case study of early grammatical
development.
Press.

children.
First

children
intentions

have

adult

and

syntactic

learning

words

competence?
in the second

acquisition and conceptual


Press.
University

(Eds.), Language

UK: Cambridge
Cambridge,
a
(2003). Constructing
language. A usage-based
MA: Harvard
Press.
University

Cognition,

74(3),

year of life. In
development

theory of language

(pp.

acquisition.

125
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

MICHAELTOAAASELLO
AND SILKEBRANDT

L. (1997). Differential
in
K., 8c Rekau,
productivity
and verbs. Journal
24, 373-387.
of Child Language,
W.
Tomasello,
M., 8c Merriman,
(Eds.).
(1995). Beyond names for things: Young children's ac
Erlbaum.
quisition of verbs. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence
children's
is
Tomasello,
M., 8c Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling
spontaneous
speech: How much
101-121.
31(1),
enough?/oMrna/
of Child Language,
Tomasello,

young

M., Akhtar,
N., Dodson,
use of nouns
children's

a lexicalist
M. K. (1994). Toward
8c Tanenhaus,
framework
of constraint
In C. Clifton
8c K. Rayner
resolution.
(Eds.),
Jr., L. Frazier,
syntactic
ambiguity
on sentence
Erlbaum.
Hillsdale,
Perspectives
processing
NJ: Lawrence
(pp. 155-179).

Trues well,
based

J. O,

126
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

FIRST
FROMINFANTS'
VERBS
LEARNING
Sandra

R.Waxman

the centuries, people have been fascinated with infants' first words.
of parents of young children,
is not a special characteristic
fascination
or psycholinguistics.
Instead, this fascination
developmental
psychologists,

Across
This

is
widespread,
discussions

and
of

topics

infants'
as

far

first
ranging

can

words
as

innate

serve

as

entry

knowledge,

to heated
points
the nature
of in

of national character.
telligence, and the development
we can trace
to the writings of the Greek historian Herodotus,
Thanks
an
to the time of Psammetichus,
the fascination with infants' first words
to
BO
in
the
who
According
reigned
seventh-century
Egyptian pharoah
were
to
the
belief
that
the
the
held
Psammetichus
Egyptians
legend,
firmly
most
in the world, but this was disputed
ancient people
hotly by the
were
To
in
the
settle this
that
fact
who
they
originals.
argued
Phrygians,
to
their
claim
the
(and
rightful place), Psamme
dispute
Egyptian people
a passionate
interest in infants' first words, a passion that
tichus developed
the origin of human
stemmed from a desire to discover
language and that
on language development
in
led him to conduct the first known experiment
to bring two newborn
children. Apparently,
he somehow managed
infants
to a shepherd,
living alone among his flock of sheep. The protocol for this
was simple and clear: itwas the shepherd's
responsibility
proto-experiment
to feed and care for the infants, to make sure that they heard absolutely no
human
language, and to wait patiently and listen carefully for the infants'
was equally clear: he reasoned
first words. The hypothesis
that in the ab
sence of any linguistic input, the first word uttered
these
infants would
by
reveal which language was the origin of all human languages. As it turned
that the first word uttered by the children was
out, the shepherd reported
and excitedly with their arms out
"becos," a word they uttered repeatedly
stretched. When Psammetichus
learned that "becos" was the Phrygian word
for bread, he accepted for the first time that Phrygian,
and not Egyptian,
was the original language of humankind.

127
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

SANDRA R.WAXMAN

In
but

the

stems

21st
from

century,
a different

our

interest
source.

in
We

infants'
are

first

no

longer

words

remains

strong,
with

consumed

dis

is the lingua franca of humankind,


but instead
covering which
language
with what infants' first words can reveal about the nature of the human
mind and how it is shaped by experience.
In their monograph,
Naigles, Hoff, and Vear (2009) provide an out
a careful analysis of infants' first words,
how
of
and
standing example
their first verbs, can inform current
theories and debates
in
especially
on
to
from
16
20
language acquisition.
Focusing
eight infants, ranging
at the start of the investigation,
months
traces each infant's
the monograph
first uses of their earliest acquired verbs. The diary records, compiled with
the apparently
tireless support of the infants' mothers,
offer a rich depiction
of the pragmatic,
and
of
infants' early verb
semantic,
syntactic properties
contexts
in
Infants'
and
the
which
productions.
productions,
they occur,
then serve as an empirical base against which competing
about
hypotheses
the flexibility and productivity
of infants' early-acquired
verbs can be tested.

RICHLY
DESCRIPTIVE
fills an important niche. In essence,
it represents
the
This monograph
It provides an impor
first of what we might call a "focused diary design."
tant counterpoint
to more
that the
traditional diary studies, illustrating
diary

of

one

any

child,

we need

erything
this

monograph
in middleraised

no

matter

how

were
to

drawn

from

cannot

comprehensive,

to know. After all, although

the eight children

a rather

homogenous
families
by majority

upper-middle-class

tell

ev

us

in

included
(e.g.,

population
culture,

stay-at

even within
this population
and although
home mothers)
they represent
a
to language acquisition
of
the midrange
continuum
approaches
along
too much or too little were excluded),
their di
(e.g., those who produced
a
of
distinct
aries nonetheless
range
vary considerably,
revealing
approaches
to early verb learning. Importantly,
then, this richly descriptive monograph
serves

as

resource

to which

we

can

turn

to

later

to

pursue

a host

of

questions.

DIARY
DATA
GUIDEDBYAND APPLIED
TOTHEORY
is its strong commitment
Another outstanding
feature of this monograph
to theory. The data were guided by, and then applied to, theory. In addition,
and
the monograph
offers insight into the tight coupling between method
on
on
one
the
other.
and
and
the
hand,
interpretation
theory
coding

128
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

TENDENCIES
AND INDIVIDUAL
DIFFERENCES
CENTRAL
developmental
analyses of these eight diaries not only uncover
within
individual
the group as a whole, but also identify potential
patterns
the importance of bearing inmind that not all
differences. This underscores
in the same way. An
children go about the process of language production
means
of
and
offers
evidence
of the central ten
group patterns
analysis
to discern different
the surface, it is often possible
dencies, but beneath
to these individual
individual styles (Bloom, 1973; Nelson,
1973). Attending
differences
is instructive, and in the case of this monograph,
it permits the
It
authors to describe different developmental
also
underscores
that
paths.
a
children
of
task
the
with
may approach
although
language acquisition
set of linguistic competences,
universal
these alone do not determine
the
The

child's path of production.


This path is also shaped by linguistic character
istics of the input (e.g., Greek vs. English),
the amount of input (rich vs.
and
of
the
individual
child (cautious vs. intrepid)
sparse),
personalities
&:
8c
2005; Gleitman
Trueswell,
(Gleitman, Cassidy, Papafragou,
Nappa,
Fisher, 2005; Huttenlocher,
man 8c Lidz, 2006).

Vasilyeva,

Cymmerman,

& Levine,

2002; Wax

THE
OF "ABSTRACTNESS"
MATTERS
AND "FLEXIBILITY"
to focus on the first 10 uses was motivated
by the goal of
the
breadth
of
children's
first
of
verb use and
identifying
representations
verb meaning.
This is a key question, because
there is currently consider
over the breadth or abstractness
able controversy
of infants' early verb
an
and
For
if
infant
the verb
meanings
applications.
example,
produces
The

wave

decision

in the

context

of

flag-waving

event,

what

can

we

say

about

her

rep

resentation? Does she construe the verb narrowly, applying it to


flag-waving
events only? Does she construe
it too abstractly, applying
it to any and all
events that involve moving
one's hand while grasping an object? The au
thors of the monograph
this question
the
approached
directly, bringing
to bear on key theoretical perspectives
toddlers' productions
(Fisher, 1996;
& Golinkoff,
Hirsh-Pasek
2006; Lidz 8c Gleitman,
2004; Lieven, Behrens,
&
2003; Naigles,
2002; Tomasello,
2000; Waxman
Speares, 8c Tomasello,
Lidz,

2006).
In addition

to recording each infant's first 10 uses of a series of verbs,


and researchers
the mothers
also worked
together to record the range of
utterances
in which the newly produced
verbs appeared
and the range of
were
to which
situations
on the con
to
The
decision
focus
they
applied.
ditions under which the verb was produced
is also motivated
by theory.

129
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

SANDRA R.WAXMAN

At issue here is the question of flexibility of verb use. Of course, although


it
utterances
is not a simple matter to operationalize
with
flexibility, especially
as short as those produced
the authors were
by these very young children,
careful to guide their decisions by theory.
Another
strength of the analysis is the authors' efforts to tease apart
several (potentially) distinct elements of flexibility
(e.g., pragmatic flexibil
But
in this
semantic
there is a wrinkle
ity,
flexibility, grammatical
flexibility).
a
can
occur
in
of
frames
which
verb
because
kinds
the
analysis
grammatical
are not unrelated
to their semantics
1990;
(Fisher, 1996, 2002; Gleitman,
to bear in
8c Gleitman,
Lidz, Gleitman,
2004). As a result, it is important
mind that measures
of semantic and grammatical
flexibility are, of necessity,
not independent.
Although
focusing on flexibility and abstractness of early verb use is an
advance,

important

at

same

the

it raises

time,

several

thorny

of

questions

its

own. Chief

among them, does the child's flexibility of verb use, as measured


It might,
of the underlying
grammar?
really reveal the abstractness

here,
but

at

this

and

point,

based

on

the

current

data,

we

cannot

rule

strong

are merely
that in using verbs flexibly,
children
hypothesis:
to
the
which
have
been
(i.e., the various
exposed
mirroring
input
they
frames in which they have heard these very verbs in the ambient language).
the input lan
After all, if children have indeed been actively processing
a
more
first verbs,
their
for
than
then
the
time
guage
year,
by
they produce
As a
in
of
these
verbs
different
frames.
would
have
each
heard
many
they
even
uses.
to
in
first
To
them
be
10
flexible
their
result, we might
expect
alternative

resolve

this important matter,

itwill be important

to search for and analyze

errors.

children's

infants'
What this suggests is that although carefully cataloguing
is an important step, itmay not take us far enough. The first
to resolve some of the finely
may prove to be too blunt an instrument
Still, this first step certainly
questions of early language acquisition.
uses

far

enough

to

see where

we

should

step

first 10
10 uses
honed
takes us

next.

ENDEAVORS
OPENINGTHEDOORTONEW RESEARCH
opens the door to several creative lines of ad
Certainly, this monograph
the importance of analyzing the
it highlights
ditional research. For example,
even
to
before
children,
young
they
produce their first verbs, and pro
input
vides guideposts for the kinds of input analyses that are most likely to bear fruit.
It also sets the stage for a careful analysis of how the view we obtain from diary
in general relates to the view that we obtain
studies and language production
from

more

standard

experimental

work

based

on

language

comprehension.

130
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

the monograph
makes
clear the importance
of a more
uses
10
the
first
these
of
comprehensive
analysis,
looking beyond
early
verbs. How can we best characterize
the trajectory of these verbs?
produced
Do children exhibit a steep increase, or explosion,
in flexibility or abstract
a
characterized
reliance on the early
ness, or is development
by deepening
to
frames?
it
will
be
pursue focused diary studies
acquired
Finally,
important
of infants acquiring
other
than
languages
English, focusing especially on
in
that
differ
ways.
languages
theory-relevant
In addition,

The

strongest

for

signature

a "classic-in-the

is not

making"

whether

the

work answers all of our questions, but whether


it offers to carry us forward
in our inquiries. There
is no doubt that when considering
the contribution
to their monograph,
of Naigles, Hoff, and Vear, the answer is "yes." Thanks
we can move beyond asking whether young children are
capable of learning
verbs.

Clearly

they

are. We

of representing

capable
fashion.

Apparently

can

also

language
can.

they

move

beyond
we

Instead,

whether

asking

in an abstract

and grammar
now

can

move

they

are

or flexible
to

forward

pin

that best support the acquisition of abstract knowledge


point the conditions
to identify how early in development
and the use of flexible representations,
this abstract knowledge
becomes evident, and to discover how quickly it is
a new verb.
when young children encounter
apprehended

ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Preparation

of the commentary

was

by NIH

supported

HD030410.

References

L. (1973).
Bloom,
Mouton.
Fisher,

C.

(1996).

interpretations
Fisher, C. (2002).

One word at a time: The use of single-word


Structural

utterances

before syntax. The

Hague:

on verb
The
role of analogy
in children's
mapping:
Cognitive Psychology, 31, 41-81.
limits on verb mapping:
The
role of abstract
structure
in 2.5
of novel verbs.
Science, 5, 56-65.
Developmental
limits

of sentences.
Structural

year-olds'
interpretations
L. R., 8c Fisher, C. (2005). Universal
Gleitman,
to Chomsky
(Ed.), The Cambridge Companion
Press.
University

aspects
(pp.

of word

123-142).

learning.

Cambridge,

In J.
McGilvray
UK:

Cambridge

L. (1990). The
sources of verb
structural
meanings.
Language Acquisition: A Journal
1(1), 3-5 5.
of Developmental
Linguistics,
L. R., Cassidy,
K., Papafragou,
Gleitman,
A., Nappa,
R., 8c Trueswell,
(2005). Hard
J. T
words. Journal
and Development,
1, 23-64.
of Language
Learning
K. 8c Golinkoff,
R. (Eds.). (2006). Action meets word: How children learn verbs. New
Hirsh-Pasek,
York: Oxford
Press.
University
Gleitman,

Huttenlocher,
J., Vasilyeva,
child syntax. Cognitive

M.,

E., 8c Levine,
Cymmerman,
45, 337-374.

S. (2002).

Language

input

and

Psychology,

131
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

SANDRA R.WAXAAAN

Lidz, J., Gleitman,


the mental

L. (2004). Kidz in the


H., 8c Gleitman,
In D. G. Hall & S. R. Waxman
lexicon.

'hood:

Syntactic

(Eds.), Weaving

and
bootstrapping
a lexicon
(pp. 603

MA: MIT
Press.
636). Cambridge,
L. (2004). Argument
structure
and the child's contribution
J., 8c Gleitman,
Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 157-161.
learning.
M.
E., Behrens,
H., Speares,
Lieven,
(2003). Early syntactic
J., & Tomasello,
30, 333-367.
approach.
of Child Language,
usage-based
Journal
Lidz,

to

language

creativity:

a
L. R. (2002).
Form
is easy, meaning
is hard: Resolving
in early child
paradox
86, 157-199.
Cognition,
language.
L. R., Hoff,
in early verb use: Evidence
from
E., 8c Vear, D. (2009).
Naigles,
Flexibility
a
in Child Development,
multiple-Af
study. Monographs
of the Society for Research
diary
Serial No. 293).
74(1-111,
to talk. Monographs
K. (1973).
in learning
Structure
and strategy
Nelson,
of the Society for
Research
in Child Development,
Serial No.
38(1-2,
149).
Naigles,

Tomasello,

M.

(2000).

Do

young

children

have

adult

syntactic

competence?

Cognition,

74,

209-253.
In D. Kuhn
S. R., 8c Lidz, J. L. (2006). Early word
Waxman,
learning.
Handbook
child
6th
2,
ed.,
Hoboken,
(Vol.
299-335).
pp.
of
psychology

8c R. Siegler
NJ: Wiley.

132
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

(Eds.),

COMMENTARY

OR NOT?
EARLY
CREATIVE
VERBLEARNERS:
Jane B.Childers
describes a longitudinal
study of eight children's first
an analysis of the variety of words used in conjunction
and the patterns
with 34 targeted verbs, the variety of utterances produced,
uses
in
10
the first
of these verbs. These data are
of developmental
change
a
most
important because
diary studies have included very few children at
on
not
time and have
focused
the beginnings
of verb learning. Thus,
these
of an early stage of verb learning that
results advance our understanding
This monograph
verb uses including

has received
The main
the

verb

young

kinds

learner

one. Verb

servative
debate

relatively little attention.


discussed by the authors
controversy

or

the

other

of studies
there

However,

as

a creative

tend

researchers
and,

of

conducted
are

dangers

user

course,

and
to

these

to view

concerns
or

language

the nature
as

a more

the child on one

differing

the ways
both

of

world

in which

views

data
or

overestimating

are

of

con

side of this
influence

the

interpreted.

underestimating

If we assume more
children's
spontaneous
knowledge.
creativity by the
child than we should, we may begin down a path of experimentation
that
will ultimately be less fruitful than it would be, while ifwe ignore creativity
that is present, we will miss revealing a capacity of the human mind that is
the debate is likely
profound. How to resolve this issue is unclear; however,
to rage

for

some

time.

of how to view children's productions


in this study
Putting questions
an important contribution
aside for a moment,
of this study is that it sheds
that is relatively unexplored.
light on a stage of verb development
Although
a new focus of research is directed at understanding
event understanding
in
8c
much
of
what
is
known
about
verb
Lakusta, 2009),
infancy (e.g., Wagner
concerns

learning

children's

comprehension

and/or

production

after

24 months

Lieven,
Pine, 8c Rowland,
(e.g., Fisher, 2002; Theakston,
a
of
the
better
initial
2004). Thus,
stages of verb learning is
understanding
to produce
It is difficult to get children younger
needed.
than 24 months

133
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

JANEB. CHILDERS

verb utterances

reliable

record)

parent

scarce.

been

speech

On

the

other
can

a researcher

is that

in the laboratory, and so sampling (or having their


in everyday contexts provides data that to date have
a

hand,
control

great

how

of

advantage

many

experimental
a child
has

exposures

in which that particular verb has


verb, the specific sentences
In one study in which these
and the timing of those sentences.
about a week of daily
children
needed
controlled,
2.5-year-old
a verb to produce
it (Childers 8cTomasello,
2002). A limitation

studies
to a

given

been

heard,
factors were

to
exposure
of laboratory
abilities
children's productive
is that they probably underestimate
the child has to produce
the new verb at the right time in an

studies
because

these kinds of studies (e.g., Abbot-Smith,


setting. However,
experimental
Akhtar
8c Tomasello,
&
Tomasello,
2004;
Lieven,
1997) are important for
to
be
that is, their
about
children's
productive,
ability
answering
questions
a verb they have not heard in a particular sentence type to a
to
extend
ability
new

sentence

type.

ARETHESE
CHILDREN'S
PRODUCTIONS?
HOW CREATIVE
Flexibility in Verb Uses
An

important

in verb

question

dividual

verb's

children

are initially context

in

the

particular

early

meaning

situational

in

learning

concerns
A

development.

bound
contexts

the scope of an in
view

prevailing

in their verb uses, producing


and

to

failing

is

a new

extending

that

verbs only
verb

1983); recent exper


very widely
Smiley, & Charney,
(e.g., Huttenlocher,
this view (Forbes & Poulin-Dubois,
imental evidence has partially supported
8c Brandone,
Golinkoff,
Hirsh-Pasek,
1997; Maguire,
2008). This type of
idio
would be very useful to children given the somewhat
conservativism
an individual verb as well as the less
nature
of
of
the
meaning
syncratic
structure of the verb category compared with the hierarchical
organized
in this study included
structure of concrete nouns. Some of the analyses
semantic flexibility, or the use of a verb with different nouns or in conjunc
tion with different actions. These analyses are important because they show
that young children are more flexible in the use of different affected object
than in their use of different agents and that both of these types of
across different
are more common
actions (al
than is flexibility
flexibility
to code from these kinds of records).
uses
was
across
difficult
actions
though
levels of flexibility,
that there are different
these results demonstrate
Thus,
names

even
what
cause

it is still unclear
in the semantic domain, which is important. However,
be
is like for these early verb representations
the scope of meaning
the

analyses

likely affected

rest

on

by a myriad

children's

of factors

productions

(e.g., objects

of

the new

verbs,

which

available, motivation).

134
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

are

In

COMMENTARY

addition, even though new objects and agents were produced with the verbs
(and these were the most common ways in which children were semantically
it is possible
that children were still using the verb in particular
flexible),
contexts
situational
(and, thus, were
bound), while mixing
situationally
which particular
entities
in that context were named. That
is, it is still
somewhat unclear from these data how widely these new verbs were used
across

situational

or how

contexts,

"portable"

these

verbs

were.

In my

view,

this is the kind of contextual boundedness


in early verb
others are reporting
et
we
are exam
In
Huttenlocher
al, 1983).
my laboratory,
learning (e.g.,
an
how
children
achieve
of
level
(or
ining
might
appropriate
"portability"
new verb by
of a particular
the range of
extension)
actively comparing
situations
(Gentner, 1983, 1989) in which that verb is heard (Childers, in
Childers
8c Paik, 2009).
press;
Because all of the semantic flexibility exhibited
in these children's ut
terances could have stemmed from uses by the adult, what that means
in
terms of children's verb representations is unclear. If children are
verbs
using
to refer to different agents and to more
than one action, but these are all
initiated by the adult, could it be that a more
imitative process
superficial
underlies

this flexibility? That


this semantic
is, if the process underlying
is
could
their
verb
remain
imitative,
flexibility
representations
fairly small,
seems possible. Studies
restricted, and context bound? This interpretation
of verb use in everyday contexts are important because
can
they
potentially
reveal the breadth and depth of a young child's developing
representation
some
of a particular
verb. This study provides
helpful data relevant for
are like, but a detailed
inferences
about
what
those
making
representations
a
of
situations
in
which
verb is produced
the
(and perhaps whether a
coding
was
could
verb
have
been
to reveal
but
not) is needed
particular
produced
more

fully

the

Obviously,
point

in

scope

of

children

development.

children's

early

verb

representations.

are likely to be productive


Adults

are

productive

verb

in their verb uses at some


users,

creating

new

verbs

(e.g., "texting") and extending


existing patterns of use to those new words
centers on how early children
are
("I'll text you later"). The debate
creative
in
that
the
(i.e.,
ways
productive
spontaneously
go beyond
input)
because later productivity
allows for the possibility
that children could learn
to be productive, while early
that support
suggests mechanisms
productivity
that creativity could be innate. These
data do not bear directly on this
the input, we are unable to discern how these
question because, without
are tied to specific utterances
children's flexible productions
they may have
heard from an adult. That is, the question of
productivity
requires an eval
uation of the strength of the link from parent
speech to child speech.
this limitation does not mean
that these data are not important.
However,
The question of whether
children can go beyond the input in the early
of
verb
will
be
difficult to test. It will be difficult to record all
stages
learning

135
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

JANEB. CHILDERS

it with the child's


verb and then compare
input for a particular
new productions
to
not
heard.
Other argu
of
that
verb
reveal
production
ments of creativity in productions
have included a discussion of errors made
that would not fit the input and of errors that are possible but are never
of verb errors in older
made. For example, Bowerman's
(1982) description
children
is important because, by these ages, her children were producing
of the

errors

an adult English
that did not fit anything
say. In
speaker would
to
number
and
in
which
the
studies
addition,
exposure
types of
laboratory
sentences
that are sen
in which a novel verb is heard, with methodologies
it possible to control the input and address
sitive to toddler abilities, make
that we currently have very little of
Given
the question
of productivity.
a
either type of data (diary or laboratory) for this stage of verb acquisition,
it
of
demonstrates
the
kinds
is
that
of
the
contribution
present study
major
in the early stages of verb learning, even
children can produce
flexibility
links to specific utterances
created by
have
direct
this
flexibility may
though
an

adult.

Flexibility in Utterances
is not
though flexibility
examined
that these researchers
Even

evaluated

for

them

dren

were

most

of

the

as well.

flexibility
verbs

producing
data
reported

the same as productivity,


children
the utterances
These

in more
here

than

concern

one-

that

show

analyses
one

it is important
and
produced

of

utterance.

type
and
two-word

these

chil

However,
and

utterances,

are scaffolded by an underlying


these utterances
thus it is unclear whether
and
Martin Braine's
structured grammar. Lois Bloom's
(1971) work
(1993)
the

demonstrate

same

as

tensions

reflected

in

this

current

work.

For

ex

to
are too few words in the utterances
ample, Braine pointed out that there
or
not.
does
the child has a grammatical
be sure of whether
Thus,
category
some of the differences
the present
between
(1992)
study and Tomasello
utter
a rich (L. Bloom-like)
of
reflect the tension between
interpretation
ances

as

seen

here

and

a more

conservative

analysis

of utterances

as used

by

in
and Tomasello.
(And, although we usually prize conservatism
own
as
has
its
conservative
limitations.)
science,
noted, being overly
to command
and to de
In addition,
children were using utterances

Braine

scribe,

but,

at

the

same

time,

there

are

many

more

communicative

functions

as
This could be viewed as pragmatic
that could be expressed.
flexibility
as
a
set
or
of
be
restricted
it
viewed
could
and
argue,
colleagues
Naigles
functions. These are just the type of utterances parents are
communicative
of
and descriptions
likely to use frequently with young children: commands
to use these forms could be evi
events. Thus,
the tendency for children
dence of a link between parent and child speech in this study.

136
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

Are

Individual Differences

Important?

is that it provides
Another major contribution made by this monograph
as
a sense of individual variation in verb learning in this sample. However,
not
is
of
the
and
this
note,
range
colleagues
fully representative
Naigles
or even of the range of parents who initially
actual range of variation,
to participate
in this labor-intensive
agreed
study. Instead, a surprising
of families initially agreed but found
verbs. In addition,
several families
producing
number

drop
sented

out

their

because

here

children
a

represents

were
of

midrange

too

that their children were not


began the study but had to

verbal.

children

Thus,

who

were

the
neither

repre

range
too

verbal

nor not verbal enough at the time of the study, and the interpretation
of the
in mind. Per
results should be made with these participant
characteristics
in this study is even more
in development
haps the variability
surprising
then, given the fact that children at each end of variation were not included.
The

careful analysis by these researchers


information
provides
important
in
children
take
verb
about different
which
is impor
trajectories
learning,
tant for researchers,
and
alike
for
parents,
speech therapists
understanding
unfolds.
ways in which normal language development
Are There Specific Verbs That Provide a Particular Benefit in Verb Learning?
in this monograph
addresses questions
Finally, the evidence presented
about whether
the acquisition of some verbs ismore useful for breaking into
the

verb

refer
Thus,

lexicon.

to concrete
researchers

An

nouns

advantage

objects
can

have

can be ordered

examine

the

over

verbs

is that

hierarchically

acquisition

of nouns

the

nouns

that

within

a category.

at different

levels

of

a hierarchy and have found an


early tendency for children to learn nouns
or subordinate
to the superordinate
for entities at the basic level as opposed
levels (e.g., Rosch, 1978). Verbs in a language do not appear to have a clear
structure. There
basic level or a single hierarchical
is a small set of verbs that
is fairly general
As
and
note, there are
verbs").
("light
Naigles
colleagues
researchers who have proposed
that this set is especially
important in verb
can
these
the
child
because
verbs
discover
patterns of
learning
general
help
it has been unclear whether
syntax that hold for groups of verbs. However,
these verbs actually serve this function of "pathbreaking,"
and there is no
evidence
that they do so here. Even if this small set of very general
(light)
data to the child, this is obviously not the same
instead of thinking of verb
learning. Thus,
as
a
in a hierarchy, children
dimension
learning
proceeding
along
particular
to
more
be
both
and
appear
learning
general
specific verbs at the same time,
as the study reported
in this monograph
In ad
demonstrates
effectively.

verbs does provide important


as the "basic level" in noun

seem

to

dition,

verbs

share

sets of syntactic

group

into

frames,

clusters

instead

of

meaning,

of being

or

clusters

hierarchically

of verbs

that

structured

137
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

JANEB. CHILDERS

across

the verb category, and thus it is unclear whether general extension


across different verbs would actually be useful. Children may be
strategies
in verb learning, given
the major
better
served by being conservative
in the
in the meaning
of individual verbs and the differences
differences
of

range

syntactic

frames

that

verbs

particular

allow.

CONCLUSION
The
dren,

study reported

focuses

across

semantic

on

early
and

here
verb

is important

uses,

syntactic

and

contexts.

it includes

because

reveals

information

Future

studies

several

about
are

chil
uses

those

needed

to

clarify

be
and the relationship
the scope of children's early verb representations
tween parental
this
and
children's
However,
study
productions.
input
to the field by revealing more about an
makes an important contribution
this study
Thus,
early stage of verb learning that is poorly understood.
event
an
and
of
infants'
link
studies
between
processing
important
provides
a link that is important for un
studies of verb learning after 24 months,
the entirety of verb development.
derstanding

References

M. (2004).
E. V. M., & Tomasello,
K., Lieven,
Abbot-Smith,
The role of frequency,
construction:
duce the transitive
Science, 7, 48-55.
Developmental
syntactic distribution.

Training
semantic

2-6-year-olds
similarity

and

to pro
shared

with word order and verb


M. (1997). Young
children's
N., 8cTomasello,
productivity
Psychology, 33, 952-965.
Developmental
morphology.
UK: Cambridge
Uni
L. (1993). Language
Bloom,
development from two to three. Cambridge,
versity Press.

Akhtar,

M.
Bowerman,
L. Gleitman

In
in lexical and syntactic development.
(1982). Reorganizational
processes
art (pp. 320-346).
& E. Wanner
(Eds.), Language
acquisition: The state of the
Press.
UK: Cambridge
University

Cambridge,
In D. I.
of grammars.
of the internalization
Braine, M. D. S. (1971). On two types of models
Press.
New
York:
Academic
The
Slobin
153-186).
(Ed.),
(pp.
ontogenesis of grammar
new
two
events helps
extend
to multiple
Childers,
1/2-year-olds
J. B. (in press). Attention
verbs.

First Language.
and
8c Paik, J. H.
(2009). Koreanto learn novel predicate
information

Childers,
J. B.,
situational

English-speaking
terms. Journal

children

use

of Child Language,

cross
36,

201-224.
M. (2002). Two-year-olds
learn novel nouns,
verbs, and con
Childers,
J. B., 8c Tomasello,
or
actions from massed
ventional
Psychology, 38, 967
Developmental
spaced exposures.
978.
in 2.5
structure
The
role of abstract
limits on verb mapping:
Fisher, C. (2002). Structural
year-olds'

interpretations

of novel

verbs. Developmental

Science,

5, 55-64.

138
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

COMMENTARY

Forbes,

J. N.,

D. (1997). Representational
in young
children's
change
verb meaning.
24, 389-406.
Journal
of Child Language,
A theoretical
framework
for analogy. Cognitive Sci
Structure-mapping:

8c Poulin-Dubois,
of familiar

understanding
D. (1983).
Gentner,

ence, 7, 155-170.
D. (1989). The mechanisms
(Eds.), Similarity and analogical
sity Press.

Gentner,

of analogical
reasoning

P., 8c Charney,
Huttenlocher,
J., Smiley,
child: Evidence
from verb meanings.
M. J., Hirsh-Pasek,
K., Golinkoff,
Maguire,
facilitate
relation:
Fewer
examples

R.

(pp.

learning.
199-241).

In S. Vosniadou
New

York:

8c A. Ortony
Univer

Cambridge

of action
in the
Emergence
categories
72-93.
Review,
90,
Psychological
on the
A. C. (2008). Focusing
R. M., 8c Brandone,
De
initial verb learning
and extension.
children's
(1983).

velopmental Science, 11, 628-634.


In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd
E. (1978). Principles
of categorization.
(Eds.), Cognition and
Erlbaum Associates.
Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence
categorization
(pp. 27-48).
A. L., Lieven,
E. V. M., Pine, J. M., 8c Rowland,
C. F. (2004). Semantic
Theakston,
generality,
of syntax. Journal
and the acquisition
31, 61-99.
input frequency
of Child Language,
Rosch,

M. (1992). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical


Press.
Cambridge
University
to navigate
8c
L.
L.,
Lakusta,
(2009).
Wagner,
Using
language
Science, 4, 177-184.
Psychological
Tomasello,

development.

Cambridge,

the infant mind.

Perspectives

UK:
on

139
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

CONTRIBUTORS

R. Naigles
of Pennsylvania)
is Professor of
(Ph.D. 1988, University
at
Her
the
of
Connecticut.
research
the
Psychology
University
investigates
in children with autism and compares
of language acquisition
processes
across languages and cultures.
language development

Letitia

Erika Hoff
of Michigan)
is Professor of Psychology
(Ph.D. 1981, University
at Florida Atlantic University. Her research investigates
the role of input in
to
the
of
and
relation
memory
early language development
phonological
in
lexical
and
children.
early
monolingual
bilingual
development
Donna Vear (M.A. 2002, University
of Connecticut)
at NRI Community
Rhode
Services, Woonsocket,
Michael

is the Director

Tomasello

Department
tionary Anthropology
in Germany. His major
Psychology
social

social

cognition,
and

comparative,

cultural

learning,

is a children's
Island.

of the Max Planck


of Developmental
research
interests
communication

and

Institute

for Evolu

and Comparative
are in processes
of
from

aspects

perspectives?especially

clinician

developmental,
to
related

lan

theoretical
focus is on processes
of
involves mainly human chil
research

current

guage and its acquisition. His


shared intentionality. His empirical
dren from 1 to 4 years of age and great
is a postdoctoral

apes.
at the Max

Planck Institute for


Dr. Brandt's research interests are in language
Evolutionary Anthropology.
and processing
of complex
in German and English, acquisition
acquisition
learning mechanisms.
syntax, language and theory of mind, and general
Silke Brandt

researcher

at
in the Department
of Psychology
is a professor
on
between
the relation
Northwestern
University. Her research is focused
across development
and across
and conceptual
development
language
on
and
of biological
and
the acquisition
reasoning
knowledge
languages,
Sandra

R. Waxman

140
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

CONTRIBUTORS

across

cultures. She is a recent recipient of a Guggenheim


Fellowship,
Ann L. Brown Award for Excellence
in Developmental
Research
form
of Illinois, and a James McKeen Cattell Award.
University

the
the

of Texas-Austin)
is an Associate
(Ph.D., 1998, University
Jane B. Childers
at Trinity University,
Professor of Psychology
San Antonio. The main focus
of her research
is to examine
children's early verb learning, both in lab
studies

oratory

research
son

and

examines

processes,

may

contexts

in naturalistic

how domain-general
contribute

to

the

across

cultures.

mechanisms,
acquisition

of

Her

including

current

compari

verbs.

141
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

STATEMENT
OF EDITORIAL
POLICY

The Monographs
series aims to publish major reports of developmental
new findings and uses these to foster a
research that generate authoritative
or integration of findings on some
fresh perspective
conceptually
significant
issue.

Submissions

from

research

programmatic

are welcomed;

projects

these

or group-authored
may
reports of findings from a
or
a
from
sequence of experiments
single large-scale investigation
centering
on a particular question. Multiauthored
sets of independent
studies that
center on the same underlying
a critical
question may also be appropriate;
consist

of individually

in

requirement

issues and

such

and

substantial,

well-integrated.

research

multidisciplinary
including
contexts
advance

instances

that the contribution

evidence
are

of

from

particular

knowledge

on

is that

the

various

from

arising
Manuscripts

on significant

reporting

cultural,

racial,

interest.

Because

the
topics

aim
but

also

be unique,
or

and those

questions

ethnic,
of

common

address

interdisciplinary

developmental

diverse

specialized

authors

the set as a whole

the

or

national,
series
to

is not

enhance

other
only
cross

to

or subfields,
fertilization
the links between
the specific
among disciplines
issues under study and larger questions relating to developmental
processes
should emerge clearly for both general readers and specialists on the topic. In
short, irrespective of how itmay be framed, work that contributes
significant
data or extends developmental
thinking will be considered.
to be members
Potential authors are not required
of the Society for
or affiliated with the academic discipline of
in Child Development
Research
to submit a manuscript
for consideration
psychology
by the Monographs.
of the work in extending
significance
developmental
theory and in
new
information
is
crucial
the
consideration.
contributing
empirical
a minimum
should
of 80 manuscript
Submissions
contain
pages
The
of
is
tables
and
150-175
references).
upper
pages
(including
boundary
more flexible, but authors should try to keep within this limit. If color artwork
is submitted, and the authors believe color art is necessary to the presentation
of their work, the submissions letter should indicate that one or more authors
or their institutions are prepared
to pay the substantial costs associated with
The

142
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

color art reproduction.


Online
Monographs

Please

to the SRCD
submit manuscripts
electronically
at
and Review
Site (MONOSubmit)
contact the Monographs
office with any

Submissions
Please
www.srcd.org/monosubmit.
at

questions

The
letter,

monographs@srcd.org.

warrant

manuscript.
coauthors,
reviews

author

corresponding
that

all

for any manuscript

coauthors

are

in agreement

must,
with

in the submission
the

content

of

the

The corresponding
author also is responsible
for informing all
in a timely manner, of manuscript
editorial
submission,
decisions,

received,

and

any

revisions

recommended.

Before

publication,

the

author must warrant in the submissions


letter that the study
corresponding
was conducted according to the ethical guidelines of the
Society for Research
in Child Development.
Potential authors who may be unsure whether
the manuscript
they are
an
are
to
would
make
submission
invited
draft an
planning
appropriate
outline of what they propose and send it to the editor for assessment. This
as well as a more detailed description
of all editorial policies,
mechanism,
and format requirements,
evaluation processes,
is given in the "Guidelines
for the Preparation
of Publication Submissions," which can be found at the
or by contacting
SRCD website by clicking on Monographs,
the editor, W.
Andrew Collins, Institute of Child Development,
of
51
Minnesota,
University
E. River Road, Minneapolis,
MN 55455-0345;
e-mail: wcollins@umn.edu.

Note

to NIH Grantees

to NIH mandate,
will post the
Society through Wiley-Blackwell
version
of
to
Contributions
authored
NIH
accepted
by
grantholders
PubMed Central upon acceptance.
This accepted
version will be made
see
after publication.
For further information,
publicly available 12 months
Pursuant

www.wiley.com/go/nihmandate.

143
This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

CURRENT
in Early Verb

Flexibility
Marital

Conflict

and Children's

293,

Behavior:

Externalizing

Diary

Multiple-Af

Vear (SERIAL NO.

and Donna

Erika Hoff,

From

Evidence

Use:

R.

Study?Letitia

Naigles,

2009)
Between

Interactions

and

Parasympathetic

Sympathetic Nervous System Activity?Mona El-Sheikh, Chrystyna D. Kouros, Stephen


Erath, E. Mark Cummings, Peggy Keller, and Lori Staton (SERIAL NO. 292, 2009)
The

of Early

Effects

and Relationship

Social-Emotional

St. Petersburg-USA

Orphanage Children?The

on

Experience

the Development

Orphanage

of Young

Team

Research

(SERIALNO. 291,2008)
Mother-Adolescent

Understanding

Conflict

Discussions:

and Across-Time

Concurrent

Prediction

From Youths' Dispositions and Parenting?Nancy Eisenberg, Claire Hofer, Tracy L. Spinrad,
Elizabeth T Gershoff Carlos Valiente, Sandra Losoya, Qing Zhou, Amanda Cumberland,
(SERIAL NO. 290, 2008)
Jeffrey Liew, Mark Reiser, and Elizabeth Maxon
and Gary Lupyan
The

in Infancy:

Concepts

Object

Developing

and Environmental

Genetic

An Associative

(SERIAL NO.

Learning

H.

Perspective?David

Rdkison

289, 2008)
of Learning

Origins

Years?Yulia Kovas, Claire M. A. Haworth,

Abilities

in the Early

and Disabilities

S. Dale,

Philip

School

and Robert Plomin

(SERIALNO. 288, 2007)


The

of Two

Preservation

Children's

Kagan,

Adolescence?Jerome

and Sara Towsley (SERIAL NO.

A Mechanism

Questions:

Into

Infant Temperaments

Vali Kahn,

Snidman,

Nancy

for Cognitive

287,

2007)

M.

Development?Michelle

Chouinard

(SERIALNO. 286, 2007)


Best

in Quantitative

Practices

R. Burchinal,
Foundations

for

Methods

and Kristen L. Bub


An

for Self-Awareness:

Developmentalists?Kathleen

(SERIAL NO.

Exploration

285,

Autism?R.

Through

Parental
Time,

A History
of the First 25 Years of the Black
T.
1973-1997?Diana
Slaughter-Defoe,

(SERIAL NO.

Harrison-Hale

(SERIAL NO.

282,

283,

Control,
K.

Psychological
Support,
and Method?Brian
Culture,

Peter

284,

2006)

in
Caucus
of the Society for Research
Aline M.
O.
Garrett,
Algea

2006)
Across
Control:
Relevance
Assessing
E. Stolz,
A. Olsen
and
Joseph

and Behavioral
Heidi
Barber,

2005)

Accounts
and Moral
Narrative
and Hurting
Others:
Children's
A. Brehl,
and
Conflicts?Cecilia
Interpersonal
Beverly
Wainryb,

of Their
Judgments
Sonia Matwin

Hurt

Being
Own

Margaret

Hobson,

(SERIAL NO.

Gayathri Chidambi, Anthony Lee, and Jessica Meyer


Too:
Our Children
Child Development,

McCartney,

2006)

(SERIALNO. 281,2005)
Childhood

Sexual

Assault

Victims:

After Testifying

Outcomes

Long-Term

in Criminal

Court?

Jodi A. Quas, Gail S. Goodman, Simona Ghetti, Kristen W. Alexander, Robin Edelstein,
Allison D. Redlkh, Ingrid M. Cordon, and David P. H.Jones
(SERIAL NO. 280, 2005)
The

of Social

Emergence

Malinda

Carpenter
of Physical

Trajectories

in Three

Cognition

(SERIAL NO.

Young

279,

Correlates, and Outcomes?NICHD

and

2005)

From Toddlerhood

Aggression

Tomasello

Chimpanzees?Michael
to Middle

Childhood:

Predictors,

Early Child Care Research Network

(SERIALNO. 278, 2004)


Constraints
Folksociological

on Conceptual
Knowledge

Carey (SERIAL NO.


Origins

Heron

A Case
Development:
in Madagascar?i&ta

277,

Body

Knowledge?

Virginia

Conversations

About

Susan A. Gelman, Marrianne


Development

Ulrich Muller,

of Executive

Douglas

and
Susan

and Michelle

Slaughter

in Collaboration with Linda Jenkins and Elizabeth Tilse (SERIAL NO.

Mother-Child

The

of Folkbiological
and
Solomon,

2004)
of Human

and Early Development

Study of the Acquisition


E. A.
Astuti,
Gregg

Gender:

Understanding

the Acquisition

Taylor, and Simone Nguyen


Function

in Early

Childhood?

Frye, and Stuart Marcovitch

of Essentialist

(SERIAL NO.
Philip

David

(SERIAL NO.

276, 2004)
Beliefs?

275, 2004)

Zelazo,

274,

2003)

This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Wed, 10 Jun 2015 02:47:42 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

S-ar putea să vă placă și