Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jcbs

ResearchBasic Empirical Research

Assessment of body image exibility: The Body Image-Acceptance


and Action Questionnaire
Emily K. Sandoz a,n, Kelly G. Wilson b, Rhonda M. Merwin c, Karen Kate Kellum b
a

University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Psychology Department, Girard 202-A, P.O. Box 43131, Lafayette, LA 70504, USA
University of Mississippi, USA
c
Duke University Medical Center, USA
b

art ic l e i nf o

a b s t r a c t

Article history:
Received 1 August 2012
Received in revised form
8 February 2013
Accepted 17 March 2013

Acceptance and mindfulness components are increasingly incorporated into treatment for eating
disorders with promising results. The development of measures of proposed change processes would
facilitate ongoing scientic progress. The current series of studies evaluated one such instrument, the
Body Image-Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (BI-AAQ), which was designed to measure body image
exibility. Study one focused on the generation and reduction of items for the BI-AAQ and a
demonstration of construct validity. Body image exibility was associated with increased psychological
exibility, decreased body image dissatisfaction, and less disordered eating. Study two demonstrated
adequate internal consistency and testretest reliability of BI-AAQ. Study three extended ndings related
to structural and construct validity, and demonstrated an indirect effect of body image dissatisfaction on
disordered eating via body image exibility. Research and clinical utility of the BI-AAQ are discussed. The
BI-AAQ is proposed as a measure of body image exibility, a potential change process in acceptanceoriented treatments of eating disorders.
& 2013 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of Association for Contextual Behavioral Science.

Keywords:
Body image
Psychological exibility
Acceptance
Mindfulness
Eating
Assessment

1. Introduction
A number of cognitive behavior therapies (CBTs) focus on
teaching clients to behave effectively in the presence of disturbing
thoughts and feelings (Hayes, 2004). This is particularly well
suited for clinical populations which tend to be cognitively rigid
(and therefore have thoughts that are difcult to change) or for
whom cognitions are consistent with a preferred self-image
(despite being harmful), such as in eating disorders like anorexia
nervosa. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Sandoz,
Wilson, & DuFrene, 2011), Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT; e.
g., Safer, Telch, & Chen, 2009), Mindfulness Based Cognitive
Therapy (MBCT; e.g., Baer, Fischer, & Huss, 2005b), and Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR; e.g., Kristeller & Hallet,
1999) have all been adapted for treatment of disordered eating.
Common to each of these approaches is an emphasis on building
awareness and openness toward experience (for example, cues for
hunger and satiation and dissatisfaction with the body) while
replacing automatic or reactive eating behavior with more intentional, committed behaviors.
This emphasis seems to be particularly well suited for treating
disordered eating (see Baer, Fischer, and Huss (2005a), Kristeller,

Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 3373715440.


E-mail address: emilysandoz@louisiana.edu (E.K. Sandoz).

Quillian-Wolever, and Baer (2006) for reviews), with recent


evidence that symptoms are maintained, at least in part, by nonacceptance or experiential avoidance. ACT has recently gathered
preliminary support in the treatment of eating disorders. A
randomized controlled trial comparing ACT to traditional cognitive
therapy (CT) for treatment of comorbid eating pathology found
that ACT produced larger reductions in eating pathology than CT
(Juarascio, Forman, & Herbert, 2010). A small case series investigating ACT as a treatment for unremitting anorexia nervosa
showed clinically signicant improvement in disordered eating
in all three clients (Berman, Boutelle, & Crow, 2009). A randomized
controlled trial investigating ACT as a treatment for obesity and
obesity-related stigma showed signicantly greater improvements
in obesity stigma, quality of life, psychological distress, and body
mass (Lillis, Hayes, Bunting, & Masuda, 2009).
2. Psychological exibility and disordered eating
In ACT, this combination of openness to experience with
constructive behavior is called psychological exibility. Psychological exibility is the ability to fully experience the present
moment while engaging in behavior that is consistent with one's
chosen values even when the present moment includes difcult
emotions, thoughts, memories or body sensations (Hayes, Luoma,
Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). From this perspective, healthy

2212-1447/$ - see front matter & 2013 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of Association for Contextual Behavioral Science.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.03.002

40

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

psychological functioning involves psychological exibility (Hayes


et al., 2006). A growing body of evidence supports psychological
exibility as central to psychological health (Kashdan &
Rottenberg, 2010), and psychological inexibility as central to
psychological vulnerability (Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, & Steger,
2006). Psychological exibility seems to be important in understanding the role of disordered eating-related cognitions in poor
psychological health (Masuda, Price, Anderson, & Wendell, 2010;
Merwin et al., 2011). Inexibility with distressing thoughts and
feelings has been associated with binge-eating (Barnes & TantleffDunn, 2010), anorectic (Merwin, Zucker, Lacy, & Elliott, 2010) and
bulimic symptoms (Lavender, Jardin, & Anderson, 2009).
Among the thoughts and feelings most strongly associated with
disordered eating is body image dissatisfaction (e.g., Polivy &
Herman, 2002). Conceptualizations of eating disorders have long
pointed to body image dissatisfaction (also called weight concerns) as a common precursor to disordered eating (e.g., Polivy &
Herman, 2002; Rosen, 1992; Shisslak & Crago, 2001; Slade, 1982;
Stice, 2002; Thompson, 1992). Meta-analytic reviews support
these theories, demonstrating the important role of body image
dissatisfaction in both the development and maintenance of
disordered eating (Jacobi, Hayward, de Zwaan, Kraemer & Agras,
2004; Stice & Shaw, 2002). Body image dissatisfaction seems
necessary to the development of disordered eating but does not
explain how some can remain dissatised without it impacting
their eating behavior (e.g., Polivy & Herman, 2002).

3. Psychological exibility and body image dissatisfaction


Body dissatisfaction is so pervasive that it has been described
as normative discontent (Rodin, Silberstein, & Streigel-Moore,
1985, p. 267). Thus, it cannot only be the presence of the
disturbing thoughts and feelings about the body that is of issue.
Rather, the key skill may be the individual's ability to have a
disturbing thought/feeling about the body and not let it impact
health and well being (Masuda et al., 2010). For example, a woman
may, upon regarding her reection, be lled with disgust and
think, I've gained so much weight. I look awful. If she is low in
psychological exibility, it is likely that she will engage in some
behaviors to manage her thoughts and feelings about her body
such as canceling social plans, changing her clothes, having a
drink, binge eating, or skipping a meal, even if these are contrary
to how she would like to live her life. If she is high in psychological
exibility, however, she is more likely to notice her thoughts and
the disgust she feels without making choices that are inconsistent
with her personal values. If her relationships are important to her,
it is likely she will keep her social plans, even on the days when it
makes her feel worse about her body to be around people.
The emerging acceptance and mindfulness-based treatments
for disordered eating are targeting precisely this kind of exibility
with promising results (see Baer et al. (2005), Kristeller et al.
(2006) for reviews). Continued progress in the development of
these treatments of eating disorders will require the demonstration of both positive outcomes (e.g., improvements to quality of
life and eating behavior) and the processes by which these
outcomes occur. Although processes of change have not been
demonstrated with respect to traditional cognitive-interventions
for body image disturbance, the assessments exist that would
allow for such research (e.g., Appearance Schemas Inventory
Revised; Cash, Melnyk, & Hrabosky, 2004; Assessment of BodyImage Cognitive Distortions; Jakatdar, Cash, & Engle, 2006). Body
image inexibility could not be assessed using existing measures
of body image as it is functionally dened while these measures
focus on formal properties of behavior. Body image inexibility is
not characterized by the contents of thoughts (e.g., What I look

like is an important part of who I am; Cash et al., 2004) or


topography of behaviors (e.g., I wear baggy clothes; Rosen,
Srebnik, Saltzberg, & Wendt, 1991), but on how body image is
impacting life (e.g., When I start thinking about the size and
shape of my body, it's hard to do anything else; Sandoz & Wilson,
2006).

4. Assessing body image exibility


Psychological exibility has typically been assessed using a
brief self-report measure, the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ; Hayes et al., 2004; Bond et al., 2011) or one of its
variations. Mediation of treatment outcomes may be better
assessed by measures of psychological exibility that target
specic areas of difculty. Domain-specic measures have been
found to be sensitive to changes in psychological exibility with
smoking urges (Gifford et al., 2004), diabetes-related thoughts and
feelings (Gregg, Callaghan, Hayes, & Glenn-Lawson, 2007), obesity
stigma (Lillis & Hayes, 2008), auditory hallucinations (Shawyer
et al., 2007), and pain-related thoughts and feelings (McCracken,
Vowles, & Eccleston, 2004). In some cases, the domain-specic
measures mediated treatment outcomes where the general AAQ
did not (e.g., Gifford et al., 2004; Gregg et al., 2007).
To date, two studies in this area have assessed body image
exibility as a proposed process of change (Berman et al., 2009;
Lillis et al., 2009). Berman et al., 2009 conducted a small case
series investigating ACT with previously treated, unremitted
anorexia nervosa, and demonstrated improvement in body image
exibility in all three participants. The design did not, however,
allow for a formal mediation analysis, and used an unpublished
version of a measure the authors developed (Sandoz & Wilson,
2006). Lillis et al., 2009 demonstrated weight-specic psychological exibility as a mediator of weight control and quality of life
following an ACT-based workshop. Their measure, the Acceptance
and Action Questionnaire for Weight-Related Difculties (AAQ-W;
Lillis & Hayes, 2008), may not be sensitive to body image inexibility in the form of controlled eating (e.g., avoidant restriction).
Although an item like, I am in control of my eating behavior
might indicate greater exibility for an individual struggling with
binge eating, it would likely indicate inexibility in an individual
engaging in harmful dietary restriction.
Further investigation of the role of body image exibility in the
prevention or treatment of disordered eating depends on the
development of a brief measure of body image exibility that is
psychometrically sound, easy to administer, and appropriate for
individuals with a range of weight concerns and a variety of
disordered eating behaviors. For example, disordered eating and
eating-related behaviors (e.g., purging and excessive exercise) are
exhibited by both male (e.g., Drummond, 2002; Lavender, De
Young, & Anderson, 2010; O'Dea & Abraham, 2002; Petrie,
Greenleaf, Reel, & Carter, 2008) and female (e.g., Berg, Frazier, &
Sherr, 2009; Napolitano & Himes, 2011; Reinking & Alexander,
2005) college students. In many cases, subclinical levels of disordered eating can be just as disruptive to functioning as eating
patterns meeting diagnostic criteria (Fairburn & Bohn, 2005).
However, measures developed in clinical populations may not be
appropriate for these populations, as they might be insensitive to
variations in the subclinical range.

5. The Body Image-Acceptance and Action Questionnaire


The current series of studies present an attempt to develop a
measure of body image exibility that could allow for research
that examines the role of body image exibility in preventing or

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

reducing disordered eating in a nonclinical population. The Body


Image-Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (BI-AAQ) was
adapted from existing measures of psychological exibility to
specically assess exible responding to body-related thoughts
and feelings. Three studies were conducted in a college student
sample so as to include individuals with varying degrees of
disordered eating. Pilot data suggested signicant disordered
eating in around ten to fteen percent of this particular
population.
Study one focused on the generation and selection of items for
the BI-AAQ, and an initial evaluation of its validity. Concurrent
validity was examined in terms of the relationship between body
image exibility and overall psychological exibility, body image
dissatisfaction, and disordered eating. Study two focused on an
evaluation of the reliability of the BI-AAQ. Study three focused on
replicating the results of study one.

6. Study one: initial validation


6.1. Participants
One hundred eighty-two (182) undergraduates were recruited for
participation in return for extra credit in their psychology classes.
Participants had an average age of 19.65 with 92% being between the
ages of 18 and 22. The sample was 70% female. They were 68.2%
Caucasian and 15.3% African American. Only one individual in the
sample reported having been diagnosed with an eating disorder at
one time. Sixty-ve percent of the sample had a calculated body
mass index (BMI) (from self-reported weight and height) within
normal range (18.5 kg/m2 oBMIo25 kg/m2), with 4.9% classied as
underweight, 18% classied as overweight, and 10.4% classied as
obese (WHO, 1995).
6.2. Measures
6.2.1. Body Image-Acceptance and Action Questionnaire
The BI-AAQ (see Appendix) was designed to measure body
image exibility. Body image exibility is dened as the capacity
to experience the ongoing perceptions, sensations, feelings,
thoughts, and beliefs associated with one's body fully and intentionally while pursuing chosen values. The original BI-AAQ item
pool included 46 items adapted from (1) the three published
versions of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes et al.,
2004; Bond & Bunce, 2003; Bond et al., 2011) and (2) the item
pools from which the published AAQ items were selected. The
authors rewrote these items to focus specically on body-related
content as opposed to psychological experiences in general. Items
worded in the direction of inexibility are reverse scored such that
higher summed scores are indicative of greater body image
exibility.
6.2.2. Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II; Bond et al.,
2011)
Psychological exibility involves both the ability to accept
difcult thoughts and feelings and to engage in valued activity in
their presence. This study employed the 10-item version of the
AAQ-II (Bond et al., 2011), an improved version of the earlier AAQ
(Hayes et al., 2004). Although the 7-item version has been recommended by the authors (Bond et al., 2011), the 10-item version was
what was available and in common use at the time that the study
was conducted. Higher scores are indicative of greater psychological
exibility. Scores on the AAQ-II have been shown to have good
reliability and construct validity (Bond et al., 2011). The internal
consistency of the AAQ-II in this sample was good, with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.84.

41

6.2.3. Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ; Cooper, Taylor, Cooper,


& Fairburn, 1986)
Body image dissatisfaction was assessed using the BSQ (Cooper
et al., 1986), a commonly used 34-item measure. Participants
respond on a ve-point scale from never to always, and responses
are scored such that higher scores indicated higher body image
dissatisfaction. The BSQ has demonstrated good testretest reliability, discriminant validity, concurrent validity, and criterionrelated validity (Cooper et al., 1986; Rosen, Jones, Ramirez, &
Waxman, 1996). The internal consistency of the BSQ in this sample
was excellent, with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.97.

6.2.4. Eating Attitudes Test-26 (EAT-26; Garner, Olmsted, Bohr,


& Garnkel, 1982)
Disordered eating behavior was assessed using the EAT-26
(Garner et al., 1982). Participants respond to 26 items on a sixpoint scale from never to always. Responses were recorded such
that Always, Usually, or Often, are scored as 3, 2, or 1,
respectively. All other responses (i.e., Sometimes, Rarely, or
Never) were scored as 0. Responses result in scores on three
subscales: Dieting, Food Preoccupation, and Oral Control. The
Dieting subscale includes items that describe the extent to which
individuals restrict intake, obsess about thinness, and experience
eating related discomfort. The Food Preoccupation subscale
includes items that describe the extent to which individuals feel
controlled by food and food-related thoughts and engage in
bulimic behavior. The Oral Control subscale includes items that
describe the extent to which individuals exert highly-controlled
eating behavior and feel pressure to gain weight or eat more. On
all three subscales, higher scores indicate more disordered eating
behavior.
The EAT-26 has been shown to have good criterion-related,
discriminant, concurrent validity (Garner et al., 1982; Mintz &
O'Halloran, 2000; Koslowsky et al., 1992), internal consistency
(Garner et al., 1982; Mazzeo, 1999), and testretest reliability
(Mazzeo, 1999). It has also been adapted as a valid measure
disordered eating behavior in a number of populations (see
Garnkel & Newman, 2001 for a thorough review of the EAT).
Internal consistency of the EAT-26 in this sample was good, with a
Cronbach's alpha of 0.86.
6.3. Procedures
After consenting to participation, participants completed the
battery of measures including the BI-AAQ item pool, the AAQ, the
EAT-26, the BSQ, and a demographics questionnaire, in that order.
Participants received a debrieng form upon completion, providing more detailed information regarding the study's hypotheses
and references for supplementary information and mental health
services.
6.4. Study one results
6.4.1. Factor analysis
Body image exibility was dened to be actively contacting
perceptions, thoughts, beliefs, and feelings about the body without
attempts to change their intensity, frequency, or form. It was
expected to demonstrate a unidimensional structure. Seventeen
items were omitted based on low or negative item-total correlations ( o0.30). Omission followed an iterative procedure. All items
with negative item-total correlations were omitted. Then the item
with the lowest item-total correlation was omitted, and the
remaining item-total correlations examined. This was repeated
until all items had item-total correlations over 0.30. Internal

42

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

consistency of the remaining 29 items was good, with a Cronbach's


alpha of 0.93.
The remaining data were then examined to further determine
factorability. First, the KaiserMeyerOlkin (KMO) Measure of Sampling Adequacy was 0.91, above the recommended 0.6 (Kaiser, 1970;
Kaiser & Rice, 1974) and indicating enough common variability
amongst items to indicate factorability. Second, Bartlett's test of
Sphericity was signicant (2 (406)2596.45, po0.001), meaning
that the correlation matrix diverged signicantly from an identity
matrix in which the items would be uncorrelated. Third, the
diagonals of the anti-image correlation matrix (KMO for individual
items) were all over 0.8, supporting the inclusion of all 29 items in
the factor analysis based on partial correlations other items. Fourth,
the initial communalities ranged from 0.33 to 0.72, suggesting a
satisfactory amount of shared variance for each item with other
items. Taken together, these indices suggested that despite the
moderate sample size, factor analysis was appropriate for these data
(MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, & Hong, 1999).
Principal factor analysis was conducted to determine the latent
structure of the data with the intention of determining which items
best measure body image exibility. Direct oblimin rotation (with a
delta of 0) was employed so that multiple factors, if found, would be
allowed to correlate. Parallel analysis (Horn, 1965), a Monte Carlo test
for retention of factors based on size of eigenvalues, was conducted
to determine the number of factors corresponding to eigenvalues
signicantly greater than parallel random eigenvalues (i.e., eigenvalues generated from completely independent items). One hundred
sets of eigenvalues reecting sampling were generated based on
random data matrices representing 29 variables and 175 cases to
represent the effects of sampling error. Because of the tendency of
parallel analysis to overfactor, the 95th percentiles of the random
eigenvalues were used, instead of the means (Glorfeld, 1995;
Harshman & Reddon, 1983). As seen in Fig. 1, a comparison of the
actual eigenvalues obtained to those calculated from the random
data, suggested retention of two factors would be appropriate. The
rst factor accounted for 34.6% of the variance and the second factor
accounted for an additional 7.4% of the variance.
Examination of the rotated factor loadings revealed that all 29
items loaded above 0.30 on one factor. Eleven of these items also
loaded above 0.30 on both factors. Additionally, all of the items
loading on the second factor were those that were worded in the
direction of exibility, suggesting that this factor may be an
artifact of item wording. There were no other cross loadings.
Finally, the two factors were correlated 0.45. For these reasons,
the analysis was repeated and one factor extracted, as would be
theoretically consistent.
The single extracted factor accounted for 34.4% of the variance.
Twenty-six items had factor loadings above 0.40, suggesting a
more conservative criterion for retention could be used in order to

Fig. 1. Actual vs. randomly generated eigenvalues in study 1.

produce a measure of shorter length. Twelve items with factor


loadings above 0.60 were retained for further analyses.
6.4.2. Calculation and descriptive statistics
Body image exibility scores were calculated by reversescoring and summing responses on the 12-item BI-AAQ (see
Appendix). The distribution of scores was examined for central
tendency and spread. BI-AAQ scores ranged from 14 to 84, with a
mean of 66.56 and a standard deviation of 15.11. Thus, the
distribution was slightly negatively skewed, indicating a trend
toward body image exibility.
6.4.3. Internal consistency
The twelve-item version of the BI-AAQ was examined for
internal consistency. All item-total correlations were over 0.57.
Cronbach's alpha was 0.92, and decreased if any one item was
deleted.
6.4.4. Covariates
Correlational analyses were conducted to examine the relationships between body image exibility and theoretically relevant
criterion variables. The relationship between BMI and body image
exibility approached signicance, such that higher body mass
index (BMI) was associated with lower body image exibility
(r (176) 0.15, p 0.054). This suggests that it may be important
to control for BMI when examining the relationship between body
image exibility and disordered eating.
Signicant gender differences were also noted with the distribution of scores being signicantly less variable for males
(p 0.034) and males reporting signicantly higher body image
exibility than females (p o0.001). Examination of correlation
matrices of body image exibility with other covariates and
dependent variables revealed that this difference did not extend
to the multivariate level, meaning that the pattern of relationships
between body image exibility and other variables of interest was
similar enough between genders to allow for subsequent analyses
to be conducted on males and females as one group.
The pattern of correlations was wholly similar with one notable
exception. The correlation between body image exibility and BMI,
although nonsignicant in the female sample and in the sample as
a whole, was signicant and of moderate size in the male sample,
(r (53) 0.366, p 0.007). For this reason, BMI was controlled for
in subsequent analyses.
6.5. Construct validity
6.5.1. Concurrent validity
Body image exibility would be expected to relate negatively to
body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. As seen in Table 1,
partial correlational analysis supported these relationships.
BI-AAQ scores were highly negatively related to body image
dissatisfaction as assessed by the BSQ and disordered eating as
assessed by the EAT-26 and the BULIT-R. The sole exception were
responses on the Oral Control subscale, which were unrelated to
BI-AAQ scores, suggesting that body image exibility does not
predict oral control when BMI is controlled for. Body image
exibility was low when body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating were high.
If the BI-AAQ measures psychological exibility specic to the
body, BI-AAQ scores would be expected to have a moderately
positive correlation with measures of overall psychological exibility. As seen in Table 1, partial correlation coefcients provided
initial support for the hypothesized relationship. BI-AAQ scores
were moderately positively related to psychological exibility.

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

6.5.2. Incremental validity


If body image exibility is important in predicting disordered
eating, then body image exibility would be expected to (a) improve
prediction of disordered eating behavior above and beyond body
shape dissatisfaction and (b) moderate the predictive value of body
image dissatisfaction for disordered eating. Hierarchical regression
analyses were conducted predicting disordered eating behavior from
BSQ and BI-AAQ scores after controlling for BMI, and with BI-AAQ
entered last. As indicated in Table 2, BI-AAQ scores remained a
signicant predictor of EAT-26 scores even after controlling for body
dissatisfaction and actual body size (BMI). In addition, the model
including both BSQ and BI-AAQ accounted for over half of the
variance in disordered eating behavior scores.
Next, hierarchical regression analyses were repeated, including
the BSQ  BI-AAQ interaction term to test for moderation effects of
body image exibility on the relationship between body shape
dissatisfaction and disordered eating behavior. The interaction
between body shape dissatisfaction and body image exibility
was a signicant predictor of disordered eating behavior, indicating that body image exibility moderated the relationship
between body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating.
(p o0.001). As indicated in Fig. 2, the slope of the regression line
for predicted disordered eating at different levels of body image
dissatisfaction was signicantly greater when body image exibility was low (p o0.001). This suggests that body image dissatisfaction had a decreased impact on disordered eating when
body image exibility was high.
Additionally, if body image exibility is to be a useful construct,
it would be expected to improve prediction of disordered eating

43

behavior over existing measures of general exibility. Hierarchical


regression analyses were conducted predicting disordered eating
behavior from BSQ, AAQ, and BI-AAQ scores after controlling for
BMI and with BI-AAQ scores entered last. The BI-AAQ remained a
signicant predictor of EAT-26 scores even after controlling for
BMI, body shape dissatisfaction, and general exibility (p o0.001).
Even when general exibility was a signicant predictor of
disordered eating behavior, BI-AAQ scores still contributed
signicantly to the overall predictive power of the model.

7. Study two: testretest reliability


7.1. Participants
Two hundred thirty-four (234) undergraduates were recruited
for participation in return for extra credit in their psychology
classes. Participants were unique to this study (i.e., potential
participants were restricted from participation if they had participated in study one). Participants had an average age of 19.42
years with 97% being between the ages of 18 and 22. The sample
was 68.4% female. The sample was 84.2% Caucasian and 12.8%
African American.

Table 1
Correlations between body image exibility, psychological exibility, and eatingrelated difculties.
Body image exibility

Psychological exibility (AAQ)


Body image dissatisfaction (BSQ)
Bulimic symptoms (BULIT-R)
Dieting (EAT-26)
Food preoccupation (EAT-26)
Oral control (EAT-26)

Study 1
rpartial

Study 3
r

0.48nnn
0.73nnn
0.70nnn
0.69nnn
0.60nnn
0.09

0.30nnn
0.80nnn
0.70nnn
0.61nnn
0.27nnn

Note: Study 1 correlations were calculated after controlling for Body Mass Index.
n
p o0.05, nnp o0.01, nnnp o 0.001..

Fig. 2. Predicated disordered eating from body image dissatisfaction by body image
exibility.

Table 2
Hierarchical regression model predicting disordered eating (EAT-26 total) from body shape dissatisfaction and body image exibility after controlling for BMI.
Std. beta

R2

R2

BMI

0.030

0.391 (0.149)

0.001

0.001

BMI
Body image dissatisfaction

0.184
0.761

3.571 (0.102)
14.750 (0.015)nnnn

0.556

0.555nnnn

BMI
Body image dissatisfaction
Body image exibility

0.184
0.537
0.309

3.743 (0.097)nnnn
7.633 (0.020)nnnn
4.437 (0.041)nnnn

0.601

0.045nnnn

BMI
Body image dissatisfaction
Body image exibility
Body image dissatisfaction  body image exibility

0.172
1.121
0.107
0.434

3.591
5.996
0.758
3.357

(0.094)nnnn
(0.054)nnnn
(0.083)
(0.001)nnn

0.626

0.025nnn

Note: Values in parentheses are standard errors.


n
p o0.05, nnp o0.01, nnnp o 0.005, nnnnp o 0.001.

44

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

Table 3
Hierarchical regression equation predicting disordered eating (EAT-26 scores) from body image dissatisfaction and body image exibility.
Std. beta

R2

R2

0.712

16.834 (0.013)nnn

0.507

0.507nnn

Body image dissatisfaction


Body image exibility

0.392
0.401

nnn

5.972 (0.021)
6.104 (0.046)nnn

0.565

0.059nnn

Body image dissatisfaction


Body image exibility
Body image dissatisfaction  body image exibility

1.417
0.320
0.667

7.910 (0.057)nnn
2.399 (0.094)n
6.093 (0.001)nnn

0.617

0.052nnn

Body image dissatisfaction

Note: Values in parentheses are standard errors.


n

p o 0.05,

nnn

p o0.001.

7.2. Measures
In study two, the Demographic Questionnaire and the twelveitem BI-AAQ developed in study one were included in a packet of
measures of psychological exibility and related constructs (e.g.,
mindfulness). None of the other measures are of interest to the
reliability of the BI-AAQ and will not be described further.
7.3. Procedure
After consenting to participation, participants completed the
battery of 12 brief questionnaires including the Demographic
Questionnaire and 12-item BI-AAQ. This took participants no
longer than 60 mins. Participants returned between two and three
weeks later and completed a shorter battery of three questionnaires including the BI-AAQ. This took participants no longer than
20 min. Participants received a debrieng form upon completion,
providing more detailed information regarding the study's hypotheses and references for supplementary information and mental
health services.
7.4. Study two results
7.4.1. Calculation and descriptive statistics
The distributions of scores were examined for central tendency
and spread. For the rst administration, BI-AAQ scores ranged
from 15 to 84, with a mean of 59.9 and a standard deviation of
17.4. For the second administration, BI-AAQ scores ranged from 22
to 84, with a mean of 63.3 and a standard deviation of 16.1.
Consistent with study one, both distributions were negatively
skewed, indicating a trend toward body image exibility.
7.5. Reliability
7.5.1. Internal consistency
The 12-item version of the BI-AAQ was examined for internal
consistency in both administrations. For the rst administration,
all item-total correlations were over 0.54. Cronbach's alpha was
0.92, and decreased if any one item was deleted. For the second
administration, most item-total correlations were over 0.62, with
one item (If I start to feel fat, I try to think of something else)
correlating with the total 0.39. Cronbach's alpha was 0.93. Cronbach's alpha decreased if any item except this item was deleted, in
which case it stayed the same.
7.2.2. Testretest reliability
The Pearson productmoment correlation between the two
administrations of the BI-AAQ was 0.80 (p o0.01). Correlations
between administrations for individual items ranged from 0.30 for
one item (I will have better control over my life if I can control

negative thoughts about my body) and 0.68 for another item


(I care too much about my body weight and shape), but all were
signicant at 0.01 level.

8. Study three: validation, replication and extension


8.1. Participants
Two hundred eighty-eight (288) undergraduates were
recruited for participation in return for extra credit in their
psychology classes. Participants were unique to this study (i.e.,
potential participants were restricted from participation if they
had participated in study one or two). Participants had an average
age of 19.5 years with 95.2% being between the ages of 18 and 22.
The sample was 59.9% female. The sample was 77.4% Caucasian
and 17.5% African American.
8.2. Measures
In study three, the AAQ-2, the EAT-26, BSQ and the BI-AAQ
described above were included in a packet of measures of depression, anxiety, and education-related difculties. Other measures
were not of interest to the validity of the BI-AAQ and will not be
described further.
8.3. Procedure
After consenting to participation, participants completed the
battery of 16 brief questionnaires. This took participants no longer
than 60 min. Participants received a debrieng form upon completion, providing more detailed information regarding the study's
hypotheses and references for supplementary information and
mental health services.
8.4. Study three results
8.4.1. Calculation and descriptive statistics
The distribution of scores was examined for central tendency
and spread. BI-AAQ scores ranged from 15 to 84, with a mean of
66.2 and a standard deviation of 15.8. Consistent with studies one
and two, the distribution was negatively skewed, indicating a
trend toward body image exibility.
7.4.2. Internal consistency and structural validity
The 12-item version of the BI-AAQ was examined for internal
consistency. The scale was found to have good internal consistency. All item-total correlations were over 0.52. Cronbach's alpha
was 0.93, and decreased or stayed the same if any one item was
deleted. The principal factor analysis and the parallel analysis
conducted in study one were replicated, providing additional

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

45

Table 4
Logistic regression equation predicting eating disorder risk (EAT-2611) from body image exibility and body shape dissatisfaction.
Predictor

Std. beta

SE

Wald's 2

Odds ratio

Constant

0.672

0.127

28.066

N/A

Constant
Body image exibility

5.483
0.096

0.775
0.012

50.057
64.125nnn

N/A
0.908

Constant
Body image exibility
Body image dissatisfaction

2.021
0.036
0.040

1.565
0.016
0.008

1.668
5.384nn
25.158nnn

N/A
0.964
1.041

Constant
Body image exibility
Body image dissatisfaction
Body image exibility  body image dissatisfaction

3.087
0.113
0.007
0.001

3.199
0.046
0.026
0.000

0.931
6.018nn
0.069
3.307n

N/A
0.893
0.993
1.001

Evaluation of nal model

128.254nnnn

Likelihood ratio test


HosmerLemeshow goodness-of-t test

131.179nnnn
6.392

p o 0.10.
p o 0.05.
nnn
po 0.01.
nnnn
p o 0.001.
nn

support for the stability of the unidimensional model. This single


factor accounted for 54% of the variance in responses.
8.4.3. Concurrent validity
Body image exibility was related to overall psychological
exibility and disordered eating in study one. As seen in Table 1,
these relationships were replicated in the current study. BI-AAQ
scores were positively related to psychological exibility. They
were also negatively related to body shape dissatisfaction and
disordered eating.
8.4.4. Incremental validity
In study one, body image exibility improved prediction of
disordered eating behavior above and beyond body shape dissatisfaction and moderated the predictive value of body image
dissatisfaction for disordered eating. As seen in Table 3, hierarchical regression analyses replicated this nding. Notably, a full 61%
of the variance in disordered eating was predictable from body
image dissatisfaction, body image exibility, and the interaction
term. As in study 1, body image dissatisfaction had less of an
impact on disordered eating when body image exibility was high.
8.4.5. Criterion-related validity
Ninety-six participants (33.3%) were classied as at-risk for
eating disorders using the recommended cutoff for nonclinical
samples of 11 on the EAT-26 (Orbitello et al., 2006). If body image
exibility is important in predicting disordered eating, BI-AAQ
scores should discriminate those at-risk for eating disorders from
the rest of the sample. Logistic regression was performed to
explore the extent to which increases body image exibility
correspond to an increase in the likelihood of eating disorder risk.
Body image dissatisfaction and the interaction term were included
as predictors in Blocks 2 and 3, respectively.
As seen in Table 4, all three models were signicant. Body
image exibility improved prediction over the null model, 2(1)
99.204, p o0.001. Body image exibility alone correctly classied
91.5% of those with eating disorder risk, and 54.3% without. Body
image dissatisfaction improved prediction when included along
with body image exibility, 2(2) 128.254, p o0.001. Body image
exibility and body image dissatisfaction correctly classied 92.6%
of those with eating disorder risk, and 63.6% without. The nal

model, including the body image exibility  body image dissatisfaction interaction, was also signicant, 2(2) 128.254,
po 0.001. The full factorial model correctly classied 92.6% of
those with eating disorder risk and 63.6% without, for a total of
72.7% correct classication. Interestingly, body image dissatisfaction was not a signicant predictor once the interaction term was
included.

9. Discussion
The current series of studies attempted to develop and provide
initial psychometric support for a brief self-report measure of
body image exibility, the Body Image-Acceptance and Action
Questionnaire (BI-AAQ). To be useful, such an instrument would
need to demonstrate both that it does, indeed, measure body
image exibility (validity), and that it does so consistently
(reliability).
9.1. Reliability
The twelve-item BIAAQ was found to assess body image
exibility with good reliability between items and administrations. Internal consistency was high (0.920.93) in all four administrations across three different samples. Temporal stability was
good (0.80) for overall BI-AAQ scores.
8.2. Validity
Body image exibility was dened as the capacity to experience
the perceptions, sensations, feelings, thoughts, and beliefs about the
body fully and intentionally while pursuing effective action in other
life domains. Although conceptually, body image exibility could be
distinguished into different capacities (e.g., capacity to experience
body image vs. capacity to pursue effective action), each item was
generated to refer to both acceptance and action. In other words,
the BI-AAQ was intended to measure body image exibility as a
unidimensional construct. Results of principal factor analysis in the
rst study supported the retention of 12 items that loaded on a
single factor with a conservative retention criterion. Studies 2 and
3 replicated a single, highly homogenous factor. Since data were
rst collected for these studies, the item selection and

46

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

unidimensional structure of the BI-AAQ has been replicated using


conrmatory factor analysis in a large sample with more diverse
body sizes (Ferreira, Pinto-Gouveia, & Duarte, in press). Together,
these ndings suggest that the unidimensional structure is stable.
Body image exibility was the focus of these studies because of
its relationship with disordered eating, and purported role as a
process of change in prevention and treatment of disordered
eating. Study one and study three both supported a strong
negative relationship between body image exibility and a range
of self-reported disordered eating behaviors. Dieting, bulimic
symptoms, and preoccupation with food were all strongly associated with inexibility with body image after controlling for BMI.
Oral control, however, was only associated with body image
exibility when BMI was not controlled for. Consistent with the
extant literature, this may reect more variability in the functions
of oral control than with other facets of disordered eating
(Masuda, Price, & Latzman, 2012). For some individuals, controlling dietary intake may actually reect increased exibility. This
also may be an artifact of the homogenous samples common in
this literature.
Body image exibility also proved important in correct identication of individuals at risk for eating disorders. Nearly 73% of
participants' risk status was accurately predicted by body image
exibility alone, and body image dissatisfaction did not contribute
signicantly to the predictive model once the interaction term was
added. Body image exibility seems to be more important than
body image dissatisfaction when it comes to predicting who is at
risk for development of eating disorders.
Construct validity depends, however, not only on relationships
with the outcome of interest, but also on relationships with other
predictors. Body image exibility was positively related to overall
psychological exibility. Body image exibility improved prediction of disordered eating, however, above and beyond overall
psychological exibility even after controlling for body image
dissatisfaction and BMI. This suggests that the AAQ might be less
sensitive to differences in the kind of psychological inexibility
important in eating disorders. This also provides additional support for the continued development of measures of psychological
exibility that are domain specic.
Body image exibility was strongly negatively related to body
image dissatisfaction. This was not surprising, as many items on
the nal BI-AAQ included specic reference to how people relate
to aversive experiences with the body (e.g., feeling fat, or
negative thoughts about the body). Notably, these items were
empirically selected. In study one, the items in the pool that did
not refer specically to dissatisfaction (e.g., When I think about
my body) did not meet the criterion for retention. This suggests
that body image exibility may be more important when people
feel badly about their bodies than when they feel good or neutral.
Despite the strong relationship between body image exibility
and body image dissatisfaction, body image exibility predicted
disordered eating above and beyond body image dissatisfaction.
Consistent with the theoretical model (e.g., Sandoz, Wilson, &
Kellum, 2009) and with extant literature (see Hayes et al., 2006),
body image exibility moderated the relationship between body
image dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Body image dissatisfaction had less of an impact on disordered eating when body
image exibility was high. This was consistent in linear and
categorical models (i.e., when predicting EAT-26 scores and when
predicting risk status).
These data complement previous research indicating that
mindfulness moderates the relationship between disordered eating cognitions and disordered eating behaviors (Masuda et al.,
2012). Masuda et al. (2012) have posited that overall psychological
exibility is not as important to predicting disordered eating as it
is general psychological health. Flexibility specic to body image,

however, seems to make a considerable contribution. It is likely


that the moderation effect of body image exibility extends to the
relationship between disordered eating cognitions (of which
body image dissatisfaction is an aspect) and disordered eating
behaviors.
9.3. Methodological limitations
There were methodological issues that limit the conclusions
that can be drawn from this series of studies. For one, all three
samples were drawn from the same population of undergraduate
students. The participants were mostly Caucasian females around
nineteen years of age. The homogenous samples limited range, and
thus, statistical power in such a way as to prevent meaningful
analyses examining individual differences in body image, disordered eating, or the relationships between body image and
disordered eating. Generalizability of these ndings will depend
on the BI-AAQ's validation with diverse genders, ethnicities, age
groups, cultures and clinical groups. This work is already underway, and with promising initial ndings (e.g., Ferrerira et al., in
press; Merwin, France, & Zucker, 2009; Rabitti, Manduchi, Miselli,
Presti, & Moderato, 2010).
In addition, the order of administration of the questionnaires
was not randomized between subjects. This means that responses
on the BI-AAQ could be affected by the participants' experience of
having just completed other questionnaires. Order effects seem
unlikely, however, as the BI-AAQ was administered following
different questionnaires in all three studies with similar univariate
and multivariate results.
Finally, all data were collected via concurrent self-report.
Anonymity reduces the likelihood of dishonesty. It does not
ensure, however, that the participants understood the questions
and successfully discriminated the correct answers. The validity of
these ndings also rests on the assumption that these relationships do not tend to vary signicantly without intervention. These
conclusions would be strengthened if BI-AAQ scores were found to
predict performance on behavioral tasks or trends in behavior
over time.
9.4. Future directions
In addition to replications of this work in various samples and
with suggested methodological improvements, future work should
focus on further specication of body image exibility as a
construct. For example, efforts should be made to examine how
body image exibility might contribute to the overall multidimensional construct of body image. Traditional cognitive behavioral
approaches have focused on body image as including three
dimensions: perceptions, attitudes, and overt behavior (Cash &
Pruzinsky, 2002). The current studies addressed only the relationship between body image exibility and body image dissatisfaction, and in a limited way. Future research might integrate current
ndings with existing research by examining how body image
exibility relates to traditional conceptualizations of body image.
Of particular interest might be longitudinal studies that examine
body image exibility and other body image variables over time,
along with associated difculties. It may be that body image
exibility changes an individual's susceptibility to body image
disturbances in perception, attitudes, and overt behavior.
Future research might also focus on outcomes of body image
exibility interventions on different components of body image.
Interventions that target body image exibility may effect some
improvement of body image distortion, body image dissatisfaction,
body image investment and body image avoidance. For example,
cognitive interventions have traditionally focused not only on
body image dissatisfaction but also on body image investment

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

(e.g., Cash, 1997; Rosen, 1997). This is an important area of


development as body image investment predicts treatment
response, treatment outcome, and long-term remission (e.g.,
Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003; Fairburn, Peveler, Jones, Hope,
& Doll, 1993; Masheb & Grilo, 2008; McFarlane, Olmsted, &
Trottier, 2008; Spoor, Stice, Burton, & Bohon, 2007). There has
been some evidence, however, that targeting exibility with
thoughts can produce bigger reductions in believability of
thoughts than direct cognitive disputation (e.g., Zettle & Hayes,
1986; Bach & Hayes, 2002). It could be that body image dissatisfaction and investment may be better targeted through interventions on body image exibility.
Finally, the current studies provided support for body image
exibility as a moderator of the relationship between body image
dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Body image dissatisfaction
seems to have less of an impact on disordered eating if body image
exibility is high than when it is low. Other approaches have
supported body image exibility as a mediator of the relationship
between disordered eating cognitions, such as body image dissatisfaction, and disordered eating behavior (Wendell, Masuda, &
Le, 2012). This slightly different model suggests that body image
exibility may account for the association between body image
dissatisfaction and disordered eating. This does not, however,
speak specically to the role of body image exibility as a potential
mediator of changes to disordered eating or risk of disordered
eating following mindfulness-based prevention or treatment
efforts. Now that the BI-AAQ has demonstrated reliability and
validity, future research should include direct examination of
validity and utility in the context of response to interventions
over time in both clinical and nonclinical samples.
The BI-AAQ was developed with the ultimate goal of facilitating
continued prevention and treatment development in the area of
disordered eating. Mindfulness-based treatments of eating disorders are quickly gaining support for their effectiveness (see Baer
et al. (2005), Kristeller et al. (2006) for reviews). To date, however,
there has been no way to demonstrate that they are effective by
changing the processes they target. The ndings of these studies
provide not only psychometric support for a measure that could be
used in precisely this way but also provide further support for the
importance of including body image exibility in the conceptualization and treatment of disordered eating. The current ndings
suggest that interventions targeting body image exibility may
prevent or reduce disordered eating, and offers the BI-AAQ as an
assessment of purported mechanisms of change in such
interventions.

Appendix. BI-AAQ
Directions: Below you will nd a list of statements. Please rate the
truth of each statement as it applies to you. Use the following rating
scale to make your choices. For instance, if you believe a statement is
Always True, you would write a 7 next to that statement.
Never Very
Seldom Sometimes Frequently Almost Always
true seldom true
true
true
always true
true
true
1

1. Worrying about my weight makes it difcult for me to live a


life that I value.
2. I care too much about my weight and body shape.
3. I shut down when I feel bad about my body shape or weight.
4. My thoughts and feelings about my body weight and shape
must change before I can take important steps in my life.

47

5. Worrying about my body takes up too much of my time.


6. If I start to feel fat, I try to think about something else.
7. Before I can make any serious plans, I have to feel better about
my body.
8. I will have better control over my life if I can control my
negative thoughts about my body.
9. To control my life, I need to control my weight.
10. Feeling fat causes problems in my life.
11. When I start thinking about the size and shape of my body, it's
hard to do anything else.
12. My relationships would be better if my body weight and/or
shape did not bother me.

References
Bach, P., & Hayes, S. C. (2002). The use of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy to
prevent the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: a randomized controlled
trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 11291139.
Baer, R. A., Fischer, S., & Huss, D. B. (2005a). Mindfulness and acceptance in the
treatment of disordered eating. Journal of Rational Emotive and Cognitive
Behavioral Therapy, 23, 281300.
Baer, R. A., Fischer, S., & Huss, D. B. (2005b). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy
applied to binge eating disorder: a case study. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice,
12, 351358.
Barnes, R. D., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2010). Food for thought: examining the relationship between food thought suppression and weight-related outcomes. Eating
Behaviors, 11, 175179.
Berg, K. C., Frazier, P., & Sherr, L. (2009). Change in eating disorder attitudes and
behavior in college women: prevalence and predictors. Eating Behaviors, 10,
137142.
Berman, M. I., Boutelle, K. N., & Crow, S. J. (2009). A case series investigating
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy as a treatment for previously treated,
unremitted patients with anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review,
17, 426434.
Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2003). The role of acceptance and job control in mental
health, job satisfaction, and work performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,
88, 10571067.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R., Carpenter, K., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H., Waltz, T., &
Zettle, R. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and
Action Questionnaire II: a revised measure of psychological exibility and
experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42, 676688.
Cash, T. F. (1997). The body image workbook. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Cash, T. F., & Pruzinsky, T. (2002). Body image: a handbook of theory, research, and
clinical practice. New York: Guilford Press.
Cash, T. F., Melnyk, S. E., & Hrabosky, J. I. (2004). The assessment of body image
investment: an extensive revision of appearance schemas inventory. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 305316.
Cooper, P. J., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (1986). The development and
validation of the Body Shape Questionnaire. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 6, 485494.
Drummond, M. (2002). Men, body image and eating disorders. International Journal
of Men's Health, 1, 7993.
Fairburn, C. G., & Bohn, K. (2005). Eating disorder NOS (EDNOS): an example of the
troublesome not otherwise specied (NOS) category in DSM-IV. Behavior
Research and Therapy, 43, 691701.
Fairburn, C. G., Peveler, R. C., Jones, R., Hope, R. A., & Doll, H. A. (1993). Predictors of
12-month outcome in bulimia nervosa and the inuence of attitudes to shape
and weight. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 696698.
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive-behavior therapy for
eating disorders: a transdiagnostic theory and treatment. Behavior Research
and Therapy, 41, 509528.
Ferreira, C., Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Duarte, C. (2011). The validation of the Body Image
Acceptance and Action Questionnaire: Exploring the moderator effect of
acceptance on disordered eating. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 11, 327345.
Garnkel, P. E., & Newman, A. (2001). The Eating Attitudes Test: twenty-ve years
later. Eating and Weight Disorders, 6, 124.
Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garnkel, P. E. (1982). The Eating Attitudes
Test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 12,
871878.
Gifford, E. V., Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., Antonuccio, D. O., Piasecki, M. M.,
Rasmussen-Hall, M. L., et al. (2004). Applying a functional acceptance based
model to smoking cessation: an initial trial of Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy. Behavior Therapy, 35, 689705.
Glorfeld, L. W. (1995). An improvement on Horn's parallel analysis methodology for
selecting the correct number of factors to retain. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 55, 377393.
Gregg, J. A., Callaghan, G. M., Hayes, S. C., & Glenn-Lawson, J. L. (2007). Improving
diabetes self-management through acceptance, mindfulness, and values: a

48

E.K. Sandoz et al. / Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 2 (2013) 3948

randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,


336343.
Harshman R. A., & Reddon J. R. (1983). Determining the number of factors by
comparing real with random data: a serious aw and some possible corrections. In Proceedings of the classication society of North America at Philadelphia
(pp. 1415).
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy and the new behavior
therapies: mindfulness, acceptance, and relationship. In: S. C. Hayes, V.
M. Follette, & M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance: expanding the
cognitive-behavioral tradition. New York: Guilford.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., &
Zettle, R. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: a preliminary test of a
working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553578.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy: model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 44, 125.
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis.
Psychometrika, 30, 179185.
Jacobi, C., Hayward, C., de Zwaan, M., Kraemer, H. C., & Agras, W. S. (2004). Coming
to terms with risk factors for eating disorders: application of risk terminology
and suggestions for a general taxonomy. Psychology Bulletin, 130, 1965.
Jakatdar, T. A., Cash, T. F., & Engle, E. K. (2006). Body-image thought processes: the
development and initial validation of the Assessment of Body-Image Cognitive
Distortions. Body Image: An International Journal of Research, 3, 325333.
Juarascio, A. S., Forman, E. M., & Herbert, J. D. (2010). Acceptance and commitment
therapy versus cognitive therapy for the treatment of comorbid eating
pathology. Behavior Modication, 34, 175190.
Kaiser, H. F. (1970). A second generation Little Jiffy. Psychometrika, 35, 401413.
Kaiser, H. F., & Rice, J. (1974). Little Jiffy, Mark IV. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 34, 111117.
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological exibility as a fundamental
aspect of health. Clinical Psychological Review, 30, 865878.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Experiential
avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: comparisons with
coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44,
13011320.
Koslowsky, M., Scheinberg, Z., Bleich, A., Mordechai, M., Apter, A., Danon, Y., &
Solomon, Z. (1992). The factor structure and criterion validity of the short form
of the Eating Attitudes Test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 2735.
Kristeller, J., & Hallet, B. (1999). An exploratory study of a meditation-based
intervention for binge eating disorder. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 357363.
Kristeller, J., Quillian-Wolever, R., & Baer, R. A. (2006). Mindfulness-based
approaches to eating disorders. In R. A. Baer (Ed.), Mindfulness-based treatment approaches: Clinicians guide to evidence base and applications (pp. 7591). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Lavender, J. M., Jardin, B. F., & Anderson, D. A. (2009). Bulimic symptoms in
undergraduate men and women: contributions of mindfulness and thought
suppression. Eating Behaviors, 10, 228231.
Lavender, J. M., De Young, K. P., & Anderson, D. A. (2010). Eating Disorder
Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q): norms for undergraduate men. Eating
Behaviors, 11, 121191.
Lillis, J., & Hayes, S. C. (2008). Measuring avoidance and inexibility in weight
related problems. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4,
372378.
Lillis, J., Hayes, S., Bunting, K., & Masuda, A. (2009). Teaching acceptance and
mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: preliminary test of a theoretical
model. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 5869.
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor
analysis. Psychological Methods, 4, 8499.
Masheb, R. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2008). Prognostic signicance of two subcategorization methods for treatment of binge eating disorder: negative affect
and overvaluation predict, but do not moderate, specic outcomes. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 46, 429437.
Masuda, A., Price, M., Anderson, P. L., & Wendell, J. W. (2010). Disordered eatingrelated cognition and psychological exibility as predictors of psychological
health among college students. Behavior Modication, 34, 315.
Masuda, A., Price, M., & Latzman, R. D. (2012). Mindfulness moderates the
relationship between disordered eating cognitions and disordered eating
behaviors in a non-clinical college sample. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 34, 107115.
Mazzeo, S. E. (1999). Modication of an existing measure of body shape concern
and its relationship to disordered eating in female college students. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 46, 4250.
McCracken, L. M., Vowles, K. E., & Eccleston, C. (2004). Acceptance of chronic pain:
component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain, 107, 159166.
McFarlane, T., Olmsted, M. P., & Trottier, K. (2008). Timing and prediction of relapse
in a transdiagnostic eating disorder sample. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 41, 587593.
Merwin R. M., France B. E., & Zucker N. L. (2009). Eating disorders and the new
wave of cognitive-behavioral therapy: does acceptance matter. In Presentation
at the annual meeting for the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy.
New York, NY.
Merwin, R. M., Zucker, N. L., Lacy, J. L., & Elliott, C. A. (2010). Interoceptive
awareness in eating disorders: distinguishing lack of clarity from nonacceptance of internal experience. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 892902.

Merwin, R. M., Timko, C. A., Moskovich, A. A., Ingle, K., Bulik, C. M., & Zucker, N. L.
(2011). Psychological inexibility and symptom expression in anorexia nervosa.
Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 19, 6282.
Mintz, L. B., & O'Halloran, M. S. (2000). The Eating Attitudes Test: validation with
DSM-IV eating disorder criteria. Journal of Personality Assessment, 74, 489503.
Napolitano, M. A., & Himes, S. (2011). Race, weight, and correlates of binge eating in
female college students. Eating Behaviors, 12, 2936.
O'Dea, J. A., & Abraham, S. (2002). Eating and exercise disorders in young college
men. Journal of American College Health, 50, 273278.
Orbitello, B., Ciano, R., Corsaro, M., Rocco, P. L., Taboga, C., Tonutti, L., & Balestrieri,
M. (2006). The EAT-26 as screening instrument for clinical nutrition unit
attenders. International Journal of Obesity, 30, 977981.
Petrie, T. A., Greenleaf, C., Reel, J., & Carter, J. (2008). Prevalence of eating disorders
and disordered eating behaviors among male collegiate athletes. Psychology of
Men and Masculinity, 9, 267277.
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Review of
Psychology, 2002, 187213.
Rabitti E., Manduchi K., Miselli G., Presti G.,& Moderato P. (2010). The validation of
the Italian version of the Body Image Acceptance and Action Questionnaire. In
Presentation at the annual meeting for the Association for Contextual Behavioral
Science. Reno, NV.
Reinking, M. F., & Alexander, L. E. (2005). Prevalence of disordered-eating behaviors
in undergraduate female collegiate athletes and nonathletes. Journal of Athletic
Training, 40, 4751.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L. R., & Streigel-Moore, R. H. (1985). Women and weight: a
normative discontent. In: T. B. Sondregger (Ed.), Psychology and gender:
Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1984 (pp. 267307). Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press.
Rosen, J. C. (1992). Body image disorder: denition, development and contribution
to eating disorders. In: J. H. Crowther, D. L. Tennenbaum, S. E. Hobfoll, & M. A.
P. Stephens (Eds.), The ideology of bulimia nervosa: the individual and familial
context. Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing Corp.
Rosen, J. C. (1997). Cognitive-behavioral body image therapy. In: D. M. Garner, & P.
E. Garnkel (Eds.), Handbook of treatment for eating disorders (2nd ed.). New
York: The Guilford Press.
Rosen, J. C., Srebnik, D., Saltzberg, E., & Wendt, S. (1991). Development of a body
image avoidance questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 3, 3237.
Rosen, J.C, Jones, A., Ramirez, E., & Waxman, S. (1996). Body Shape Questionnaire:
studies of validity and reliability. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 20,
315319.
Safer, D. L., Telch, C. F., & Chen, E. Y. (2009). Dialectical behavior therapy for binge
eating and bulimia. New York: Guilford.
Sandoz E. K., & Wilson K. G. (July, 2006). Body Image Acceptance Questionnaire:
embracing the Normative Discontent. In Paper presented at the Association for
Contextual Behavioral Science ACT/RFT World Conference. London, England.
Sandoz E. K., Wilson K. G., & Kellum K. K. (2009). The effects of ACT for body image
disturbance on eating behavior and valued living. In Presentation at the meeting
for the Association for Behavior Analysis International. Phoenix, AZ.
Sandoz, E. K., Wilson, K. G., & DuFrene, T. (2011). Acceptance and commitment
therapy for eating disorders: a process-focused guide to treating anorexia and
bulimia. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
Shawyer, F., Ratcliff, K., Mackinnon, A., Farhall, J., Hayes, S. C., & Copolov, D. (2007).
The voices acceptance and action scale (VAAS): pilot data. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 63, 593606.
Shisslak, C. M., & Crago, M. (2001). Risk and protective factors in the development
of eating disorders. In: J. K. Thompson, & L. Smolak (Eds.), Body image, eating
disorders, and obesity in youth. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Slade, P. D. (1982). Towards a functional analysis of anorexia nervosa and bulimia
nervosa. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 167179.
Spoor, S. T. P., Stice, E., Burton, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Relations of bulimic symptom
frequency and intensity to psychosocial impairment and health care utilization
in a community-recruited sample. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40,
14505.
Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: a meta-analytic
review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825848.
Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and
maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research ndings. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 53, 985993.
Thompson, J. K. (1992). Body image: extent of disturbance, associated features,
theoretical models, assessment methodologies, intervention strategies, and a
proposal for a new DSM-IV category-body image disorder. In: M. Hersen, R.
M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modication, Vol. 28 (pp. 3
54). Sycamore, IL: Sycamore Publishing Inc.
Wendell, J. W., Masuda, A., & Le, J. K. (2012). The role of body image exibility in the
relationship between disordered eating cognitions and disordered eating
symptoms among non-clinical college students. Eating Behaviors, 13, 240245.
World Health Organization (1995). Physical status: the use and interpretation of
anthropometry. Report of a WHO Expert Committee. Geneva: World Health
Organization WHO Technical Report Series 854.
Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1986). Dysfunctional control by client verbal behavior:
the context of reason-giving. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 4, 3038.

S-ar putea să vă placă și