Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

Chapter 11 / Adult CPR Training

11

199

Training Adults to Perform


Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation
What Works?

Barbara Riegel, DNSC, RN, CS, FAAN,


Lars Wik, MD, and Alidene Doherty, RN, BSN
CONTENTS
INTRODUCTION
HOW DO ADULTS LEARN BEST?
RELEVANCE
POSITIVE REGARD FOR THE INSTRUCTOR
FREEDOM TO DIRECT ONE'S OWN LEARNING
OVERCOMING BARRIERS
TRANSFERRING LEARNING TO REAL-LIFE EVENTS
RETRAINING
FUTURE DIRECTIONS
REFERENCES

INTRODUCTION
Cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR) is first-line therapy for sudden, unexpected,
cardiac arrest (CA). Laypersons are most likely to administer CPR because most CAs
occur in the home or in the community. Training laypersons to adminster CPR has
become a routine activity but some would argue that it is not an activity that we do
particularly well. Out-of-hospital resuscitation attempts have led to very low survival
rates (113)less than 10% survival in Europe (2,3,68) and the majority of urban areas
in the United States (1,913). Densely populated urban areas such as Chicago and New
York City have a particularly low rate of sudden CA survival (13) as do rural areas.
One reason for the universally low survival rates is that the frequency of CPR initiation
by bystanders remains extremely low (10,1217). Initiation of resuscitation by bystanders clearly increases survival (1,6,1719) but the rate of basic life support (BLS) initiation
by bystanders in the United States is typically less than 30% (916) and rarely greater than
50% in Europe (6,8,17).
Our ultimate challenge is to increase the number of bystanders initiating CPR. How
to manage this is not known for certain, but it is the thesis of these authors that low rates
From: Contemporary Cardiology: Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation
Edited by: J. P. Ornato and M. A. Peberdy Humana Press Inc., Totowa, NJ

199

200

Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation

of CPR are a direct consequence of our ineffective teaching methods. Much is known
about how to teach adults and traditional approaches violate most principles of adult
learning. Perhaps current approaches to CPR training should be largely abandoned in
favor of new methods. Many of the nationally recognized CPR training organizations
have modified their programs to emphasize student practice time and minimize instructor
lecture, a tactic consistent with adult learning principles. However, other, more radical
methods should be considered. In this chapter we discuss approaches to training adults
in CPR that work.

HOW DO ADULTS LEARN BEST?


Maximizing the acquisition of BLS knowledge and skills requires changing the traditional teaching model from instructor-to student-centered. Knowles (20) is usually credited with being the first to suggest that adults learn in a manner that is different from that
of children. However, others such as Rogers (21), have been influential in helping us to
understand that adults learn when the information is relevant, practical, and tied to existing knowledge. Adults must feel respected and free to direct their own learning if they
are to acquire new knowledge. Adults are typically busy so learning situations that are
fun and directed by someone who is likeable and worthy of respect facilitates learning.
Barriers to learning such as lack of time, money, and transportation must be minimized
if adults are to participate in learning events. These principles are described more fully
later along with practical examples of ways to address them in CPR training.

RELEVANCE
Adults learn best when they see the relevance of what they are learning. Scenariobased instruction has been recommended as a method of making CPR training relevant,
practical, and useful. Scenario-based instruction involves modifying the story surrounding each simulated event to fit the students individual situation. In this way, the instructor
maximizes relevance and allows the student to practice the CA scenario most like what
he or she will experience. Modification of the scenario allows the student to think through
an actual situation and build on existing knowledge. Scenario-based instruction is particularly helpful for students who learn primarily through observation.
Scenario modification is most effective when it is consistent with the experience of the
student. For example, if training police officers who will be responding after a call to 911,
it makes little sense to ask them to notify 911. However, if the scenario is modified to have
the officer update the dispatcher with information, it becomes consistent with the routine
practice of the officer. It is helpful if the scenario can be made as realistic as possible. For
example, continuing the police officer example, using a model phone or one that is not
plugged in instead of just pretending to call 911 would make the scenario more realistic
(22). Current thinking is that the pretend or acting surrounding many of the steps in
CPR training interfere with skill demonstration and perhaps with skill learning itself.
Only common, clinically relevant situations should be included in scenarios. For
example, it has been shown that when an instructor ends a class by practicing an unconscious obstructed airway scenario, students tend to confuse chest compressions and
abdominal thrusts (23). Additionally, if each scenario results in a return of spontaneous
circulation, the student leaves the classroom with the unrealistic expectation that all CA
victims will recover.

Chapter 11 / Adult CPR Training

201

An important element influencing perceived relevance is the message that the learner
receives regarding importance of the training. In one study testing the effectiveness of
CPR training by videotape, about half of the 8659 recipients of the videotape did not even
view it although the tape was sent to homes of patients at risk for sudden cardiac death
(24). An equal number of CAs occurred in each group, but the bystander CPR rates did
not differ (47% video vs 53% controls), nor did hospital discharge rates (n = 3 vs n = 2).
Clearly, the video recipients were not convinced that the tape would be relevant for them.

POSITIVE REGARD FOR THE INSTRUCTOR


Positive regard for the instructor facilitates learning. In 1991, Kaye (25) published a
paper titled The Problem of Poor Retention of Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation Skills
May Lie With the Instructor, Not the Learner or the Curriculum, making this point.
Others (26) have found that CPR instructors had limited knowledge of the courses they
taught, did not understand or follow recommended teaching practices, did not read their
instructor manuals, and could not even pass a CPR provider test. If this is the case, it is
not surprising that one author (27) found that 10% of instructors were considered by
students to be incompetent, 9% of the students stated they would not perform CPR after
the course, and 23% stated mistakenly that they could be legally prosecuted if they did
CPR on a stranger.
Most of the information that CPR instructors teach and the style used to teach it has
been passed down by tradition. Instructors typically model an instructor they admired.
Few have heard how their words and stories sound to the students. Few realize how they
make the students feel, and how their well-intentioned words distract the learners from
the important skills that they have come to learn. When instructors provide anecdotes and
depart from the script, a decrease in written test scores has been demonstrated (28,29).
For every 2 minutes of anecdote, cognitive scores dropped 1% overall. So, adding just
20 minutes of additional material (anecdotes and stories), as most instructors do, could
lower students test scores by 10%. Clearly, more emphasis is needed on helping instructors to be better teachers, if the traditional model of instructor-based courses continues.

FREEDOM TO DIRECT ONES OWN LEARNING


Another important principle of adult learning is that adults learn best when they are
free to direct their own learning. Several investigators have developed and tested various methods of self-learning that maximize individuals freedom to direct their own
learning experience. In the 1970s, Berkebile and colleagues (30,31) compared five
training methods in suburban schools in Pittsburgh. A traditional instructor-led CPR
class lasting 3 hours and an untrained control group were compared to (a) self-practice
on recording mannequins, coached by audiotapes and flip charts, (b) repeated (16
times) viewing of a new 10-minute CPR demonstration film over a 3-month period, and
(c) viewing the new 10-minute film plus self-practice on mannequins (32). The recording mannequin and flip charts demonstrating proper technique were available throughout the study period in the learning laboratory. Knowledge and skill performance at 1
and 12 months were superior in the group who viewed the film and did self-practice
when compared to the traditional instructor-led course. Self-practice alone was superior to repetitive film viewing alone. Repetitive film viewing alone was superior to no
training. More students passed heart compressions than ventilations. In another study,

202

Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation

Kaye and colleagues (33) demonstrated superior skill performance after interactive
computerized self-training compared to those given the traditional instructor-led course.
These studies demonstrate that students free to direct their own learning performed
better than those sitting through the traditional, instructor-led course.
An innovative approach to self-learning was developed and tested by Braslow and
associates (23,24) called video self-instruction or watch and practice. Instead of
watching a video and then practicing, the watch and practice approach was designed
to include synchronous practice. That is, viewers practiced and learned along with the
videotaped expert demonstrationjust like a Jane Fonda exercise video. To increase
learning, the video contained no lectures, no information on anatomy and physiology, no
rates and ratios, and no complicated methods for locating the compression point and
opening the airwayto name a few. Information on heart attack care and airway obstruction was removed. This approach allowed for more than 25 minutes of continuous CPR
practice, compared with approx 2.5 minutes of practice in the traditional 4-hour CPR
course. When the video self-instruction was compared to the traditional instructor-based
course, it worked well, even in persons over age 50, a group that often has lower skill
retention than others. Participants learned CPR in only 30 minuteswithout an instructor
or textbookand outperformed students who had just completed the traditional course.
The efficacy of self-training has been known since the 1970s (30,31,3537). Courses
by instructors remain the accepted method of training in CPR despite this. Instructors are
not the obstacle, but a fixed time available for practice is less effective than unlimited time
for self-practice.

OVERCOMING BARRIERS
Common barriers to learning CPR are lack of time and/or transportation. Standard
group classes with mannequin training are 3 to 4 hours in length, a significant time
commitment for adult learners with other responsibilities. Another barrier is lack of
interest in learning a skill one may never use. Additionally, learning style, speed, and
physical agility vary widely among adults, which can be a barrier to those concerned that
they may not keep up with others in a formal class. Societal cost of training large numbers
of laypersons is a major barrier as well.

Motivating the Learner


Individuals who are not interested in learning CPR may be motivated to learn CPR to
help others. Research into the psychology of helping behavior (what makes some
people act when confronted with an emergency, whereas some do not act) tells us that
before someone can perform in a CA or other emergency situation, psychologically, they
must feel able and ready to do so (38). Yet, most people are not. Historically, this prerequisite has been ignored perhaps because CPR and other emergency cardiac care training
began as training for physicians and has not been tailored to lay people. Depending on
the instructor, some trainees feel badgered by instructors and others feel supported to
learn. Self-efficacy, or perceived ability to perform the skill of CPR must become part of
the training program if the rate of bystander CPR is to increase (39).

Cost to Society
The societal cost of training is a major obstacle for widespread use of CPR. In order
to lower the cost, Wik et al. (40) introduced peer CPR training based on the belief that

Chapter 11 / Adult CPR Training

203

a handful of lay people trained as CPR instructors could train their coworkers, who in turn
would train their relatives, and so on. The effect of this approach is similar to a domino
effect potentially resulting in a significant increase in the number of skilled CPR providers at a low cost. An inexpensive take-home mannequin together with a flip chart and a
20-minute videotape were used. When those trained using the peer approach were compared to those trained with the traditional method, third-generation trainees proved equally
effective at CPR as those trained directly with the traditional method.
In an extension of this approach, Wik (40) trained people in CPR and then sent a
mannequin and videotape home with them to train their family members on their own.
Training of family members was accomplished in under 60 minutes. How well did they
do? The family members CPR performance was equal to, or most often better than the
performance of students coming out of traditional instructor-based CPR courses. Why?
We believe that peer-to-peer learning and modeling are learning methods with which
laypersons feel comfortable. In fact, peer-to-peer learning and modeling reflect a natural and universal method of learning. Throughout life, important learning takes place
in the home and this is the environment in which most will use CPR if they ever have
to use the skill.

TRANSFERRING LEARNING TO REAL-LIFE EVENTS


CPR acquisition includes both cognitive learning and skill retention. Cognitive
learning regarding the correct sequence to perform occurs by seeing (reading or mimicking) and by hearing. The learner needs to remember the algorithm at the same time
as it is performed with quality and speed. No study has shown that lecturing about
physiology, medicine, and risk issuescommon content in traditional CPR courses
improves skill performance (25,29,4143). In fact, lecturing and reading may distract
the learner and result in information overload. Braslow (23) found that when the hearthealthy information was eliminated from the class, practice time increased and skill
improved.
Clarity of the message (i.e., focus on essential information only) is important but
controversy continues regarding what to teach the lay public about CPR. Should we
teach abdominal thrusts (44) or back blows for suspected foreign body obstruction
(45,46)? Is moderate backward tilt of the head plus jaw thrust and separation of the lips
(the triple airway maneuver) the best airway control measure for comatose trauma
victims (4749)? How should we position patients in coma with spontaneous breathing
(horizontal supine vs stable side position, both with head tilted backward [45])? What
should we teach about when the lone rescuer is justified to leave the individual temporarily to call for help?
Pathophysiology must guide instructional content. Complete airway obstruction (as in
drowning, brain trauma, intoxication) results in CA within 5 to 10 minutes (5052). In
many situations, complete recovery can be achieved with CPR steps A and B alone (50
54), which reinforce the teaching of steps A, B, and C together. For normothermic sudden
CA, we know the time limits for preventing permanent damage to the brain (4 minutes
[5557]) and the heart (20 minutes [57,58]). Therefore, teaching must stress that only
resuscitation initiated within seconds of collapse can provide the necessary oxygen
delivery to maintain viability of the individual until arrival of advanced life support
personnel. Teaching the importance of continuous chest compression and ventilation
with a minimum of pauses (no-flow periods) is essential.

204

Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation

Practice Time
Practice time has been shown to be the most important factor in acquisition of BLS for
adult learners (23). Maximizing the practice time for the students is currently the responsibility of the instructor. However, instructors vary greatly in how they set up courses and
the use of videotapes, work books, and information provided. Kaye et al. (25) observed that
in 3-hour courses, the actual mannequin practice time ranged between 2 and 16 minutes
per student. Additionally, Brennan and colleagues (29) noted that most trainees are not
even minimally competent following training; most instructors simply pass or coach
students to pass. Testing by an independent instructor on a fully computerized system
resulted in failure of all students whom the instructor earlier had considered competent.
Kaye (33) suggests that lack of skill retention may reflect lack of initial skill acquisition.
Minimizing nonessential information that uses up practice time is essential. If the
instructor has had personal experience in prehospital resuscitation it can add credibility
to the training course but the sharing of personal experience also uses valuable time
needed for skill practice. Actual personal experience relayed to students often results in
students remembering the story and missing the underlying message.
Electronic devices such as CPR skill-prompting devices are effective in encouraging
students to practice more effectively during the available time and for longer periods of
time (59). If a prompting device is used at the point in the class when the students are
beginning to loose interest in skill practice, even the most experienced adult students will
be willing to practice for longer periods of time. Patterns (i.e., linked content) are retained
and more easily accessed in memory than isolated facts or complex algorithms, even
under stress. The prompting device will help to cement the pattern by consistently repeating phrases such as, head tiltchin lift. Students retrained at 2 years will report remembering the phrases repeated by the prompting device.

Positive Reinforcement
Positive reinforcement is essential if individuals are to develop self-efficacy, a key
predictor of performance, as discussed above. According to psychological research on
helping behavior (6067), issues inherent in the decision to act arise from the initial
response to threatening, unfamiliar and/or complex situations. The decision to act depends
on acknowledging that the situation exists and having confidence in ones ability to handle
the emergency (self-efficacy). Helping behavior research has focused on laypersons
response to public assault, medical emergencies such as heart attack, and trauma such as
uncontrolled bleeding, involving strangers. Research on laypersons response to CA in
a family member is nearly nonexistent.

Skill Retention
Skill retention is directly affected by the amount of practice available during the
learning process because the acquisition of psychomotor skills greatly depends on
repetition (68). Overtraining has shown to improve retention (6971). Overtraining is
defined as continuing to practice a task after having achieved the performance criterion
(72). Overtraining has been claimed to be of particular value in the retention of skills
in which the individual has no chance to warm up (71), as is indeed the case during
clinical CPR.
Other strategies recommended for improving retention include sensory input or feedback (73). Feedback received at the end of skill performance appears to be less effective

Chapter 11 / Adult CPR Training

205

than that occurring during performance (74). Qualitative is not as effective as quantitative
feedback (72). Improvement in performance depends on the frequency of feedback (72).
The person giving the feedback can be important as well. Feedback from a peer may be
less threatening than correction from the instructor. If the observer is given a written
checklist with the steps outlined, he or she can provide feedback and learn while observing his or her peers.
Why do learners not perform CPR correctly after an interval when most people have
no difficulty riding a bicycle several years after initial training? The answer is feedback.
When riding a bicycle, you receive instant feedback on how you are bicycling and consequently wrong performance is corrected. When performing CPR, little feedback is
received. Feedback was more salient in the early training efforts. In the early training
performed by Safar et al. (47) curarized nonintubated human volunteers would lie supine
on the floor to demonstrate open airway and mouth-to-mouth ventilation. If the learners
did CPR wrong, the patient would turn blue in a few seconds. That feedback changed
their behavior and performance so that their technique created pink patients. In 1960,
Lind (75) of Norway introduced the use of mannequins (instead of curarized patients) in
the training of B-CPR steps A and Ba safer approach but one with little feedback (68).
Could we create a CPR bicycle or could an instructor play that role? Later, we propose
the potential uses of technology to address these issues.

RETRAINING
Retraining of students 3 to 6 months following the initial CPR/automated external
defibrillator (AED) course will result in better retention of skills. Review of skills can be
as simple as asking students to demonstrate what they remember from their training class
and then providing reinforcement for skill mastery and instruction in areas not mastered.
This approach was used in the Public Access Defibrillation (PAD) Trial and found to
require only 5.3 0.1 minutes for CPR and 7.8 0.1 minutes for CPR + AED to test and
retrain lay volunteers (75a). If the skill review is done individually with the instructor,
the student is given individualized attention and peer pressure is eliminated. The skill
review session also gives the instructor the opportunity to debrief students if any medical
emergencies have occurred since the original training session.
Retention of CPR skills decreases significantly in a short period after training, even
in medical personnel who are not routinely involved in resuscitation (33). Although
skill decrement may reaches low levels, it is still above pretraining levels for most at
612 months (30,31,33,69,72,7683). After initial training and early reinforcement, it is
helpful if repeat remedial mannequin practice is made available every 612 months
(31,33,69,72,7680).

FUTURE DIRECTIONS
One night, a friend of Dr. Wiks experienced a CA in an atypical patienthis mother
lying on the bathroom floor, looking dead. His emergency medical technician mind
was not with him that summer evening. He was too preoccupied, too nervous just like any
other lone layperson facing such a situation. He did not think about details. Instead, he
thought, call 911, bend the head back, grab the chin, pinch the nose, blow. Then hands
in the middle of the chest, and start pumping and blowing. No rates or numbers were in
his mind. No complex and time-consuming steps regarding where to place his hands
crossed his mind. He just thought pump and blow.

206

Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation

Dr. Wiks friend later realized that little of the real experience of people witnessing and
acting during a CA is represented in our CPR education programs. This observation also
was made by the lay bystanders surveyed regarding their thoughts, feelings, and motivations when attempting to resuscitate a stranger (84). He came to recognize from his
experience that CPR training has to be more readily accessible. It has to get to the right
people, the people most likely to use it. It must to be perceived as being so easy that the
actual learning of CPR is not even thought about or contemplated.
Technology and state of the art teaching methods are needed if we are going to achieve
these goals. CPR practice that is routinely scenario-based to include professionals such
as 911 dispatchers who will coach the performance will be more effective than traditional
approaches. There have to be easily accessible home adjuncts such as speaker phones, a
CPR prompt, and maybe someday, an even more user-friendly home AED as well as
content-on-demand video instruction.
To achieve this dream, we will need to influence policy so that the government regulations of yesteryear are amended to rid our first aid and emergency cardiac care programs
of superfluous content that is confusing and overwhelming to students. The data collected
to date suggest strongly that this content is diffusing our message to a point that CPR
learners cannot remember how to perform the most basic skills when needed.
To improve training and minimize human error, these authors believe that emergency
cardiac care education must use behavior and education theory. Computer and virtual
technologies like those being used in other life-essential skill domains such as pilot and
physician training could be used. With simulation, expert performance can be modeled
and immediate feedback obtained. With instant feedback and remediation from the simulator, CPR performance can only improve. There is technology available todayfrom
simple clickers that give feedback when one has compressed deep enough to electronic
feedbackthat can give immediate feedback to students as they are learning. Earlier
versions of these feedback devices were shown to improve learning, but were not used
because they were misunderstood by the instructors. We now know that accurate and
timely feedback is essential to learning and retention.
Technology soon to be available will provide not only immediate feedback but realtime verbal input as the student performs (85,86). Using virtual reality technology, the
research team of Dr. Wik of Norway has developed a system that uses a type of video selfinstruction synchronous practice in which almost the entire training and testing can be
supervised from central control sites thousands of miles away. Internet learning cannot
teach hands-on psychomotor skills and measure skill performance directly. But, as the
human eye is unable to evaluate the adequacy of ventilations and compressionsand
only instrumented manikins can do sothis technology will allow immediate feedback,
regardless of proximity. Additionally, simulator mannequins now available can be programmed with realistic attributes such as airways that swell, heads that are difficult to
bend backwards, real lung sounds, distal pulses, agonal breaths, and so on. This technology is being used to improve the effectiveness of self-training and has been shown to
increase learning and retention (85,86).
Perhaps in the future, as digital bandwidth expands, would-be rescuers might be able
to merely flick on their televisions to see a skills demonstration. Content-on-demand
technology could allow dispatchers to send the exact demonstration needed right to the
callers television, laptop computer, or personal digital assistant, rather than trying to
explain the sequence of steps verbally.

Chapter 11 / Adult CPR Training

207

The real measure of success for CPR, AED, and other emergency cardiac care programs is learner performancebystander CPR and survivor rates. Building programs
and course content, methods, and administration around learner outcome is essential. We
now know enough about how to teach emergency cardiac care content. Until recently, we
have focused on courses that are too long and taught by instructors with lecture notes and
fixed minutes for manikin practice. These courses have focused more on the acquisition
of cognitive knowledge than performance skills. A new approach is needed. These authors
maintain that the traditions surrounding CPR training should be respected as our history but
it is time to move on and totally revise CPR training programs to emphasize simplicity,
essential skills, and the use of technology to broaden the population of those trained in
CPR and the use of bystander CPR in our communities.

REFERENCES
1. Eisenberg MS, Horwood BT, Cummins RO, Reynolds-Haertle R, Hearne TR. Cardiac arrest and resuscitation: a tale of 29 cities. Annals of Emergency Medicine 1990; 19:17986.
2. Eisenburger P, List M, Schorkhuber W, Walker R, Sterz F, Laggner AN. Long-term cardiac arrest
survivors of the Vienna emergency medical service. Resuscitation 1998; 38:13743.
3. Gaul GB, Gruska M, Titscher G, et al. Prediction of survival after out-of-hospital cardiac arrest: results
of a community-based study in Vienna. Resuscitation 1996; 32:16976.
4. Sefrin P, Eilmes H. [First-aid measures in 939 fatally injured victims (authors translation)]. Anaesthesist
1975; 24:53440.
5. Abrams JI, Pretto EA, Angus D, Safar P. Guidelines for rescue training of the lay public. Prehospital &
Disaster Medicine 1993; 8:151156.
6. Van Hoeyweghen RJ, Bossaert LL, Mullie A, et al. Quality and efficiency of bystander CPR. Belgian
Cerebral Resuscitation Study Group. Resuscitation 1993; 26:4752.
7. Mullie A, Van Hoeyweghen R, Quets A. Influence of pre-CPR conditions on EMS response times in
circulatory arrest. The Cerebral Resuscitation Study Group. Resuscitation 1989; 17(Suppl):S45S51;
discussion S199S206.
8. Lund I, Skulberg A. Cardiopulmonary resuscitation by lay people. Lancet 1976; 2:702704.
9. Guzy PM, Pearce ML, Greenfield S. The survival benefit of bystander cardiopulmonary resuscitation
in a paramedic served metropolitan area. American Journal of Public Health. 1983; 73:766769.
10. Gallagher EJ, Lombardi G, Genis P. Effectiveness of bystander cardiopulmonary resuscitation and
survival following out-of-hospital cardiac arrest. JAMA 1995; 274:19221925.
11. Ritter G, Wolfe RA, Goldstein S, et al. The effect of bystander CPR on survival of out-of-hospital cardiac
arrest victims. American Heart Journal 1985; 110:932937.
12. Troiano P, Masaryk J, Stueven HA, Olson D, Barthell E, Waite EM. The effect of bystander CPR on
neurologic outcome in survivors of prehospital cardiac arrests. Resuscitation 1989; 17:9198.
13. Becker LB, Ostrander MP, Barrett J, Kondos GT. Outcome of CPR in a large metropolitan areawhere
are the survivors? [see comments]. Annals of Emergency Medicine 1991; 20:355361.
14. Copley DP, Mantle JA, Rogers WJ, Russell RO, Jr., Rackley CE. Improved outcome for prehospital
cardiopulmonary collapse with resuscitation by bystanders. Circulation 1977; 56:901905.
15. Cummins RO, Eisenberg MS, Hallstrom AP, Litwin PE. Survival of out-of-hospital cardiac arrest with
early initiation of cardiopulmonary resuscitation. American Journal of Emergency Medicine 1985; 3:
114119.
16. Jackson RE, Swor RA. Who gets bystander cardiopulmonary resuscitation in a witnessed arrest? Academic Emergency Medicine 1997; 4:540544.
17. Wik L, Steen PA, Bircher NG. Quality of bystander cardiopulmonary resuscitation influences outcome
after prehospital cardiac arrest. Resuscitation 1994; 28:195203.
18. Sanders AB, Kern KB, Bragg S, Ewy GA. Neurologic benefits from the use of early cardiopulmonary
resuscitation. Annals of Emergency Medicine 1987; 16:142146.
19. Bossaert L, Van Hoeyweghen R. Bystander cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR) in out-of-hospital
cardiac arrest. The Cerebral Resuscitation Study Group. Resuscitation 1989; 17(Suppl):S55S69; discussion S199S206.

208

Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation

20. Knowles MS. The Modern Practice of Adult Education: Andragogy Versus Pedagogy. New York: New
York Association Press, 1970.
21. Rogers CR. Freedom to learn. Columbus, OH: Merrill, 1969.
22. Bilgera MC, Giesena BC, Wollanb PC, White RD. Improved retention of the EMS activation component
(EMSAC) in adult CPR education. Resuscitation 1997; 35:219224.
23. Braslow A, Brennan RT, Newman MM, Bircher NG, Batcheller AM, Kaye W. CPR training without an
instructor: development and evaluation of a video self-instructional system for effective performance
of cardiopulmonary resuscitation. Resuscitation 1997; 34:207220.
24. Eisenberg M, Damon S, Mandel L, et al. CPR instruction by videotape: results of a community project.
Annals of Emergency Medicine 1995; 25:198202.
25. Kaye W, Rallis SF, Mancini ME, et al. The problem of poor retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation
skills may lie with the instructor, not the learner or the curriculum. Resuscitation 1991; 21:6787.
26. Braslow A. An Evaluation of the Knowledge and Practices of Basic Cardiac Life Support Instructors.
Urbana-Champaign, IL: University of Illinois, 1985.
27. Thell R. CPR training for drivers license applicants in Austria (in German). Rettungsdienst, in press.
28. Brennan RT. A Question of Life and Death: An Investigation of CPR Instruction Using Hierarchical
Linear Modeling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, 1989.
29. Brennan RT. Student, instructor, and course factors predicting achievement in CPR training classes.
American Journal of Emergency Medicine 1991; 9:220224.
30. Berkebile P, Benson D, Ersoz C, Barnhill B, Safar P. Public education in heart-lung resuscitation.
Evaluation of three self-training methods in teenagers. Proceedings of the National Conference on
Standards for Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation and Emergency Cardiac Care. Dallas, TX: American
Heart Association 1975, pp. 1323.
31. Berkebile P. Education in cardiopulmonary-cerebral resuscitation (CPCR) using self-training methods.
Prehospital & Disaster Medicine 1985; 1(Suppl 1).
32. Gordon AS. CPR training films. (a) Breath of Life (Steps A and B); (b) Pulse of Life (Steps A, B and
C); (c) Prescription for Life (Steps A-D); and (d) Life in the Balance (Cardiac Care Unit). Dallas, TX:
American Heart Association 1960s and 1980s; 1986.
33. Kaye W, Mancini ME. Retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills by physicians, registered
nurses, and the general public. Critical Care Medicine 1986; 14:620622.
34. Braslow A, Brennan, RT, Newman NM, Kaye W. A self-instructional system for one-rescuer cardiopulmonary resuscitation. Circulation 1995; 92(Suppl):834.
35. Breivik H, Ulvik NM, Blikra G, Lind B. Life-supporting first aid self-training. Critical Care Medicine
1980; 8:654658.
36. Esposito G, Safar P, Medsger A, Nesbitt J. Life supporting first aid self training for the lay public.
Prehospital & Disaster Medicine 1985; 1(Suppl 1):9193.
37. Safar P, Berkebile P, Scott MA, et al. Education research on life-supporting first aid (LSFA) and CPR
self-training systems (STS). Critical Care Medicine 1981; 9:403404.
38. Bierhoff H-W. Just world, social responsibility, and helping behavior. Ross, Michael 2002.
39. Bandura A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist 1982; 37:122147.
40. Wik L, Brennan RT, Braslow A. A peer-training model for instruction of basic cardiac life support.
Resuscitation 1995; 29:119128.
41. Bircher N, Otto C, Babbs C, et al. Future directions for resuscitation research. II. External cardiopulmonary resuscitation basic life support. Resuscitation 1996; 32:6375.
42. Kaye W, Mancini ME. Teaching adult resuscitation in the United Statestime for a rethink. Resuscitation. 1998;37:17787.
43. Gudmundsen A. Teaching psychomotor skills. Journal of Nursing Education 1975; 14:2327.
44. Heimlich HJ. A life-saving maneuver to prevent food-choking. Journal of the American Medical Association 1975; 234:398401.
45. Safar P, & Bircher, N.G. Cardiopulmonary Cerebral Resuscitation. An Introduction to Resuscitation
Medicine. Guidelines by the World Federation of Societies of Anaesthesiologists (WFSA), 3rd Ed.
London: Saunders, 1988.
46. Gordon AS, Belton MK, Ridolpho PF. Emergency management of foreign body airway obstruction:
comparison of artificial cough techniques, manual extraction maneuvers, and simple mechanical devices.
In: Advances in Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation. Safar P, Elam, JO, eds. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag,
1977, pp. 3950.
47. Safar P. Ventilatory efficacy of mouth-to-mouth artificial respiration. Airway obstruction during manual
and mouth-to-mouth artificial respiration. JAMA 1958;167:335341.

Chapter 11 / Adult CPR Training

209

48. Safar P, Lind B. Triple airway maneuver, artificial ventilation and oxygen inhalation by mouth-to-mask
and bag-valve-mask techniques. Proceedings of the National Conference on Standards for Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation and Emergency Cardiac Care, 1973. Dallas, TX: American Heart Association
1975, pp. 4958.
49. Morikawa S, Safar P, DeCarlo J. Influence of head position upon upper airway patency. Anesthesiology
1961; 22:265270.
50. Safar P, Paradis NA. Asphyxial cardiac arrest. In: Cardiac Arrest. The Science and Practice of Resuscitation Medicine. Paradis NA, Halperin HR, Nowak RM. eds. Philadelphia, PA: Williams and Wilkins;
1996, pp. 702726.
51. Redding J, Voigt C, Safar P. Drowning treated with IPPB! Journal of Applied Physiology 1960; 15:
849854.
52. Kristoffersen MB, Rattenborg CC, Holaday DA. Asphyxial death: the roles of acute anoxia, hypercarbia
and acidosis. Anesthesiology 1967; 28:48897.
53. Lind B, Stovner J. Mouth-to-mouth resuscitation in Norway. JAMA 1963; 185:933935.
54. Elam JO, Greene DG. Mission accomplished: successful mouth-to-mouth resuscitation. Anesthesia
Analgesia 1961; 40:57880.
55. Cole SC, Four minute limit for cardiac resuscitation. JAMA 1956; 161:14541458.
56. Radovsky A, Safar P, Sterz F, Leonov Y, Reich H, Kuboyama K. Regional prevalence and distribution
of ischemic neurons in dog brains 96 hours after cardiac arrest of 0 to 20 minutes. Stroke 1995; 26:
21272133; discussion 3334.
57. Safar P, Abramson NS, Angelos M, et al. Emergency cardiopulmonary bypass for resuscitation from
prolonged cardiac arrest. American Journal of Emergency Medicine 1990; 8:5567.
58. Reich H, Angelos M, Safar P, Sterz F, Leonov Y. Cardiac resuscitability with cardiopulmonary bypass
after increasing ventricular fibrillation times in dogs. Annals of Emergency Medicine 1990; 19:887890.
59. Boyle AJ, Wilson AM, Connelly K, McGuigan L, Wilson J, Whitbourn R. Improvement in timing and
effectiveness of external cardiac compressions with a new non-invasive device: the CPR-Ezy. Resuscitation. 2002; 54:6367.
60. Latane B, Darley JM. Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality
& Social Psychology 1968; 10:215221.
61. Latane B, Darley JM. Bystanders apathy. American Scientist 1969; 57:244268.
62. Latane B, Nida S. Ten years of research on group size and helping. Psychological Bulletin 1981; 89:
308324.
63. Shotland RL, Stebbins CA. Emergency and cost as determinants of helping behavior and the slow
accumulation of social psychological knowledge. Sociology & Psychological Quarterly 1983;
46:3646.
64. Piliavin IM, Rodin J. Good samaritanism: an underground phenomenon? Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology 1969; 13:289299.
65. Piliavin JAP, IM. Effect of blood on reactions to a victim. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology
1972; 23:353361.
66. Darley JM, Latane B. Bystander intervention in emergencies: diffusion of responsibility. Journal of
Personality & Social Psychology 1968; 8:377383.
67. Mogielnicki RP, Stevenson KA, Willemain TR. Patient and bystander response to medical emergencies.
Medical Care 1975; 13:753762.
68. Poulsen H. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Emergency Resuscitation. Acta
Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 1961; Suppl 9.
69. Tweed WA, Wilson E, Isfeld B. Retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills after initial overtraining. Critical Care Medicine 1980; 8:651653.
70. Oxendine JB. Effect of mental and physical practice on the learning of three motor skills. Research
Quarterly 1969; 40:755763.
71. Melnick MJ. Effects of overlearning on the retention of a gross motor skill. Research Quarterly 1971;
42:6069.
72. Vanderschmidt H, Burnap TK, Thwaites JK. Evaluation of a cardiopulmonary resuscitation course for
secondary schools retention study. Medical Care 1976; 14:181184.
73. Lovel RB. Adult Learning. New York, NY: Wiley, 1980.
74. Robb M. Feedback and skill learning. Research Quarterly 1968; 39:175184.
75. Lind B. Teaching mouth-to-mouth resuscitation in primary schools. Acta Anaesthesiologica
Scandinavica 1961; (Suppl 9):6369.

210

Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation

75a.Nafziger SD, Riegel B, Sehra R, et al. Are CPR and AED skills retained over time?: results from the
Public Access Defibrillation (PAD) Trial. Submitted.
76. Wilson E, Brooks B, Tweed WA. CPR skills retention of lay basic rescuers. Annals of Emergency
Medicine 1983; 12:482484.
77. Mandel LP, Cobb LA. Reinforcing CPR skills without mannequin practice. Annals of Emergency
Medicine 1987; 16:11171120.
78. Weaver FJ, Ramirez AG, Dorfman SB, Raizner AE. Trainees retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation. How quickly they forget. Journal of the American Medical Association 1979; 241:901903.
79. Berden HJ, Willems FF, Hendrick JM, Pijls NH, Knape JT. How frequently should basic cardiopulmonary resuscitation training be repeated to maintain adequate skills? [see comments.] [erratum appears
in BMJ 1993 Sep 18;307:706.]. British Medical Journal 1993; 306:1576,1577.
80. Fossel M, Kiskaddon RT, Sternbach GL. Retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills by medical
students. Journal of Medical Education 1983; 58:568575.
81. Grogono AW, Jastremski MS, Johnson MM, Russell RF. Education graffiti: better use of the lavatory
wall. Lancet 1982; 1:1175,1176.
82. Wenzel V, Lehmkuhl P, Kubilis PS, Idris AH, Pichlmayr I. Poor correlation of mouth-to-mouth ventilation skills after basic life support training and 6 months later. Resuscitation 1997; 35:129134.
83. Huhnigk P, Sefrin P, Paulus T. Skills and self-assessment in cardiopulmonary resuscitation of the
hospital nursing staff. European Journal of Emergency Medicine 1994; 1:193198.
84. Skora J, Riegel B. Thoughts, feelings, and motivations of bystanders who attempt to resuscitate a
stranger: A pilot study. American Journal of Critical Care 2001; 10:408416.
85. Wik L, Myklebust H, Auestad BH, Steen PA. Retention of basic life support skills 6 months after training
with an automated voice advisory manikin system without instructor involvement. Resuscitation 2002;
52:2732739.
86. Thowsen J, Steen PA. An automated voice advisory manikin system for training in basic life support
without an instructor. A novel approach to CPR training. Resuscitation 2001; 50:167172.

S-ar putea să vă placă și