Sunteți pe pagina 1din 3

Language and Diversity

Differences among Languages


We are all aware that different languages have different words for the
same concept, as when English 'dog' shows up in Spanish as perro or in
Japanese as inu. And we are all aware that different languages are
pronounced in different ways, so that the strongly trilled 'r' of Spanish
perro is alien to most varieties of English. But equally important is the
fact that languages differ from one another in grammar.

A straightforward illustration of this can be seen by comparing the way


in which different languages order the various parts of a sentence. In
an English sentence, the usual order is for the subject to come first,
then the verb, then the object. In Japanese, by contrast, the usual
order is first subject, then object, then verb:

gakusei ga hon o katta.


student subj. book obj. bought
The student bought the book.

In Welsh, the usual order is for the verb to come first, followed by the
subject, followed in turn by the object:

prynodd y myfyriwr y llyfr


bought the student the book
The student bought the book.

A few languages have the order object-verb-subject, exactly the


opposite to English, e.g. Hixkaryana, spoken by some 400 people on a
tributary of the Amazon River in Brazil:

toto yahosIye kamara


man grabbed jaguar
The jaguar grabbed the man.

These differences would surely be soon noticed by anyone involved to


any depth with the languages in question. But there are other
differences in grammar between languages that are much more subtle.
Let us take the English sentence I saw you, and came here. The first
part of the sentence (before the 'and') is a complete sentence in its
own rightthe subject of the verb 'saw' is overt, appearing as the word
'I'. But the second part of the sentence is not complete in itself; its
subject is missing. However, as speakers of English we have no
hesitation in interpreting the second part to mean 'I came here', and
not to mean 'you came here', although there is no logical reason, other
than the requirements of English grammar, that this second
interpretation should be excluded.

Do all languages behave like English in this respect? No. In Dyirbal, an


almost extinct Australian Aboriginal language of northeast Queensland,
the sentence Ngadya nginuna buran, baninyu looks very much like the
English sentence. Indeed, the first part of the sentence, before the
comma, does mean 'I saw you'. However, the second part is
interpreted to mean 'you came here', not 'I came here':

ngadya nginuna buran, baninyu


I you saw came here
I saw you and you came here.

Dyirbal is just as strict in insisting on this interpretation as English is in


insisting on the other interpretation: Both languages have strict
conventions that are followed by speakers of the language; it just
happens that the conventions are different in each of these two
languages.

Language Universals

If languages can differ from one another in these ways, one might ask:
Are there any restrictions on the ways in which they can differ from
one another? Are there some general properties that are common to all
human languages? There are. For instance, many languages use
differences in the order of elements to carry differences in meaning. In
English, one difference between the statement The green parrot can
speak Hixkaryana and the question Can the green parrot speak
Hixkaryana? is a difference in the order of elements of this kind, more
specifically inversion of the subject and the auxiliary verb. But no
language is known to relate sentences by inverting the order of
sentences of indefinite length (so that the question would appear as
Hixkaryana speak can parrot green the?). Linguists believe that such a
relation would violate constraints on humans' linguistic ability. In other
words, while languages can be astonishingly different from one another
and this is why it is important for linguists to study languages of as
many different types as possiblethere are nonetheless features that
unite all languages as different manifestations of the human language
ability.

S-ar putea să vă placă și