Sunteți pe pagina 1din 9

Article

Countertransference Phenomena and Personality


Pathology in Clinical Practice: An Empirical Investigation

Ephi Betan, Ph.D. Objective: This study provides initial positive, 4) special/overinvolved, 5) sexu-
data on the reliability and factor structure alized, 6) disengaged, 7) parental/protec-
of a measure of countertransference pro- tive, and 8) criticized/mistreated. The
Amy Kegley Heim, Ph.D.
cesses in clinical practice and examines eight factors were associated in predict-
the relation between these processes and able ways with axis II pathology. An aggre-
Carolyn Zittel Conklin, Ph.D. patients’ personality pathology. gated portrait of countertransference re-
Method: A national random sample of sponses with narcissistic personality
Drew Westen, Ph.D. disorder patients provided a clinically
181 psychiatrists and clinical psycholo-
gists in North America each completed a rich, empirically based description that
battery of instruments on a randomly se- strongly resembled theoretical and clini-
lected patient in their care, including cal accounts.
measures of axis II symptoms and the Conclusions: Countertransference phe-
Countertransference Questionnaire, an nomena can be measured in clinically so-
instrument designed to assess clinicians’ phisticated and psychometrically sound
cognitive, affective, and behavioral re- ways that tap the complexity of clinicians’
sponses in interacting with a particular reactions toward their patients. Counter-
patient. transference patterns are systematically
Results: Factor analysis of the Counter- related to patients’ personality pathology
transference Questionnaire yielded eight across therapeutic approaches, suggest-
clinically and conceptually coherent fac- ing that clinicians, regardless of therapeu-
tors that were independent of clinicians’ tic orientation, can make diagnostic and
theoretical orientation: 1) overwhelmed/ therapeutic use of their own responses to
disorganized, 2) helpless/inadequate, 3) the patient.

(Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:890–898)

F reud first introduced the concept of countertransfer-


ence in 1910, noting that the patient’s influence on the an-
therapist acts in accordance with a role that is part of a re-
lationship paradigm the patient unconsciously re-creates
alyst’s unconscious feelings can interfere with treatment. with the therapist. Wachtel (11, 12) proposed the similar
This early and relatively narrow view of countertransfer- concept of cyclical psychodynamics, by which patients’
ence as an impediment to treatment prevailed in the psy- fears, wishes, expectations, and behaviors often create
choanalytic literature for several decades. Over time, how- self-fulfilling prophecies.
ever, theorists broadened the concept, recognizing that Although the clinical literature on countertransference
the clinician’s reactions to the patient (conscious and un- is rich and rapidly expanding, the corresponding empiri-
conscious, emotional and cognitive, intrapsychic and be- cal literature is limited (13–17). Research with largely non-
havioral) may have diagnostic and therapeutic relevance clinical samples has provided indirect support for some of
and can, if properly used, facilitate rather than inhibit these ideas, demonstrating that depressed people tend to
treatment (1–5). elicit criticism from significant others that matches their
According to this expanded view, just as the patient’s be- own self-criticism (18) and that people who are sensitive
haviors with the therapist could provide in vivo insight to rejection tend (through needy, angry, and otherwise dis-
into his or her repetitive interpersonal patterns and asso- tancing behavior) to elicit rejection and hence to confirm
ciated thoughts, feelings, and motives, so, too, could the and reinforce their internal working models of relation-
clinician’s responses to the patient provide insight into ships (19). Giesler and colleagues (20) demonstrated that
patterns the patient wittingly or unwittingly evokes from some of these processes occur in clinical settings as well. A
significant others. Klein (6) suggested that the patient may series of analogue studies (21–26) attempted to operation-
induce the clinician to experience the feelings that the pa- alize the concept of countertransference, defining coun-
tient is having trouble acknowledging (7) or may draw the tertransference responses as therapists’ reactions to pa-
clinician into enactments that reflect the patient’s endur- tients that are based on the therapists’ unresolved conflict
ing expectations of relationships (8, 9). Sandler (10) intro- and operationalizing countertransference in terms of
duced the concept of role responsiveness, in which the avoidant behaviors (e.g., disapproval, silence, ignoring,

890 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005


BETAN, HEIM, ZITTEL CONKLIN, ET AL.

mislabeling, and changing the topic). Najavits and col- pact on the representativeness of the sample (see also the discus-
leagues (27) developed the Ratings of Emotional Attitudes sion of limitations in the Discussion section).
to Clients by Treaters scale, a clinically subtle measure of Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
countertransference designed primarily to study thera- To obtain a cross-section of psychotherapy patients seen in
pists’ response to patients in treatment for substance clinical practice, we asked clinicians to describe a nonpsychotic
abuse. patient at least 18 years old whom they had treated for a mini-
mum of eight sessions (to maximize the likelihood that they
The present study provides initial data on the reliability
would know the patient well enough to provide a reasonably ac-
and factor structure of a clinician-report measure of coun- curate description of the patient). To minimize selection biases,
tertransference processes designed to assess counter- we directed clinicians to consult their calendar to select the last
transference, broadly defined to include the range of cog- patient they saw during the prior week who met study criteria.
nitive, affective, and behavioral responses therapists have Each clinician described only one patient in order to minimize
rater-dependent biases. Clinicians received a modest honorar-
to their patients. Although the concept of countertransfer-
ium ($85) for a procedure that took 3–4 hours to complete, with a
ence emerged from psychoanalytic theory and practice, response rate of approximately 10%.
our goal was to devise a measure that could be used by cli-
nicians of any theoretical orientation, so that we could as- Procedure
sess the extent to which particular countertransference re- Clinicians could participate either by pen-and-paper forms or
sponses are specific to certain forms of therapy and so that on an interactive web site (http://www.psychsystems.net). Web
versus paper participants did not differ on any variable studied
clinicians of any orientation who are trying to get a better here (e.g., countertransference factor scores; eight t tests). Clini-
diagnostic sense of the patient or a better understanding cians provided no identifying information about the patient
of what is happening in the therapeutic dyad can make (such as name, initials, or social security number) and were in-
use of the instrument by comparing their own responses structed to use only information already available to them from
their contacts with the patient so that data collection would not
to normed psychometric data. Our primary aims were 1)
compromise patient confidentiality or interfere in any way with
to describe the factor structure and reliability of a broad- ongoing clinical work.
band measure of countertransference phenomena and 2)
to examine associations between countertransference Measures
phenomena and patients’ personality pathology. Thus, We employed a number of measures in standardized sequence.
our goals were to provide both initial validity data for the We describe those of relevance to the present study here.
measure and a test of clinically derived hypotheses that Clinical Data Form. The Clinical Data Form (see references 28,
have never been put to empirical test. In addition, to illus- 29) assesses a range of variables relevant to demographics, diag-
nosis, and etiology. Clinicians first provide basic demographic
trate the potential clinical and empirical uses of the in- data on themselves, including discipline (psychiatry or psychol-
strument, we derived a prototype of the “average expect- ogy), theoretical orientation, employment sites (e.g., private prac-
able countertransference response” to patients with tice, inpatient unit, school), and sex, and then provide data on the
narcissistic personality disorder. patient’s age, sex, race, education level, socioeconomic status,
axis I diagnoses, etc. After completing basic demographic and di-
agnostic questions, clinicians complete ratings of the patient’s
Method adaptive functioning, developmental history, and family history
(which will not be described further here).
Participants Axis II diagnosis. To assess axis II disorders, we asked clinicians
Participants were 181 clinicians who constituted a random na- to rate as present or absent each criterion of each of the DSM-IV
tional sample of experienced psychiatrists and psychologists from axis II diagnoses, randomly ordered. This procedure provides
the membership registers of the American Psychiatric Association both a categorical diagnosis of each disorder (obtained by apply-
and American Psychological Association. We requested mailing ing DSM-IV cutoffs) and a dimensional measure (number of cri-
lists of clinicians with at least 3 years’ postlicensure or postresi- teria met for each disorder). Our research group and others have
dency experience who indicated that they performed at least 10 successfully used similar measures in a number of investigations
hours per week of direct patient care. As in prior research with this (29–31).
method, psychologists responded at a substantially higher rate Countertransference Questionnaire. The Countertransfer-
than did psychiatrists to the solicitation, allowing us to assess for ence Questionnaire (32) is a 79-item clinician-report question-
biases imposed by differential training or response rates. We naire designed to provide a normed, psychometrically valid
found no differences between patients described by psychologists instrument for assessing countertransference patterns in psycho-
and those described by psychiatrists on any variable of interest de- therapy for both clinical and research purposes. (The instrument
spite a roughly 3:1 response rate ratio. (Variables of interest in- can be downloaded at http://www.psychsystems.net/lab.) The
cluded age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, education level, treat- items measure a wide range of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors
ment length, and countertransference factor scores [14 t tests, not expressed by therapists toward their patients. We derived the 79
significant at p<0.01].) We also compared the patients of this sam- items by reviewing the clinical, theoretical, and empirical litera-
ple of clinicians to those of the first 181 clinicians in another sam- ture on countertransference and related variables and by solicit-
ple with whom we used a similar method but paid a substantially ing the advice of several experienced clinicians to review the ini-
higher honorarium and obtained a correspondingly higher re- tial item set for comprehensiveness and clarity. We wrote the
sponse rate (>30%) for the psychiatrists; we found no differences items in everyday language, without jargon, so that the instru-
between the samples of patients. Together, these data suggest that ment could be used comparably by clinicians of any theoretical
any potential biases in the tendency to respond had minimal im- orientation. Items assess a range of responses, from relatively

Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 891


COUNTERTRANSFERENCE

specific feelings (e.g., “I feel bored in sessions with him/her.”) to matory factor analyses). This solution accounted for 69%
complex constructs such as “projective identification” (e.g., of the variance and included factors well marked by at
“More than with most patients, I feel like I’ve been pulled into
least five items each, suggesting a stable factor structure
things that I didn’t realize until after the session was over.”).
unlikely to be substantially affected by sample size (36).
Table 1 presents the factor structure. To create factor-
Results based scores for use in this and subsequent studies, we in-
Sample Characteristics cluded items loading ≥0.50 for factors 1 and 2, ≥0.40 for
The clinician sample consisted of 141 (77.9%) psycholo- factor 3, and ≥0.375 for factors 4–8 to maximize reliability
gists and 40 (22.1%) psychiatrists; 58.6% (N=106) of the cli- (coefficient alpha). Intercorrelations among the eight fac-
nicians were male. The majority saw patients in private tors ranged from –0.16 to 0.58, with a median of 0.30.
practice (N=145, 80.1%), but they also worked in other set- Factor 1, overwhelmed/disorganized (coefficient alpha=
tings, including hospital (N=57, 31.5%), forensic (N=15, 0.90), was marked by items indicating a desire to avoid
8.3%), clinic (N=14, 7.7%), or school (N=9, 5.0%) settings. or flee the patient and strong negative feelings, includ-
(As might be expected, psychiatrists were more likely to ing dread, repulsion, and resentment. The items accord
have primary or secondary employment in hospital set- with clinical descriptions of countertransference reac-
tings.) The most common self-reported theoretical orien- tions to patients with axis II cluster B disorders, notably
tations included psychodynamic (N=73, 40.3%), eclectic borderline personality disorder and narcissistic person-
(N=55, 30.4%), and cognitive behavioral (N=37, 20.4%). ality disorder, and with research on disorganized and
Reflecting our efforts to obtain a patient sample strati- unresolved attachment patterns (e.g., references 37, 38).
fied by sex, about one-half of the patients were male and Factor 2, helpless/inadequate (coefficient alpha=0.88), in-
one-half were female, with an average age of 40.5 years cluded items describing feelings of inadequacy, incom-
(SD=13.4). The sample was predominantly Caucasian (N= petence, hopelessness, and anxiety.
168, 92.8%). Most were middle class (N=102, 56.4%), with Factor 3, positive (coefficient alpha=0.86), was marked by
2.8% (N=5) rated as poor, 24.3% (N=44) as working class, items indicating the experience of a positive working al-
and 16.6% (N=30) as upper class. The mean Global Assess- liance and close connection with the patient.
ment of Functioning Scale score was 58.0 (SD=12.9). Factor 4, special/overinvolved (coefficient alpha=0.75),
Length of treatment averaged 19 months (SD=30.0), with a was marked by items describing a sense of the patient as
median of 13 months, indicating that the clinicians knew special, relative to other patients, and by items describ-
the patients very well. The most common diagnoses re- ing “soft signs” of problems in maintaining boundaries,
ported by the clinicians were major depressive disorder including self-disclosure, ending sessions on time, and
(N=89, 49.2%), dysthymic disorder (N=68, 37.6%), general- feeling guilty, responsible, or overly concerned about
ized anxiety disorder (N=46, 25.4%), and adjustment dis- the patient.
order (N=45, 24.9%). Factor 5, sexualized (coefficient alpha=0.77), included
items describing sexual feelings toward the patient or
Factor Structure
experiences of sexual tension.
of the Countertransference Questionnaire
Factor 6, disengaged (coefficient alpha=0.83), included
To identify the factor structure of the Countertransfer- items describing feeling distracted, withdrawn, an-
ence Questionnaire, we first subjected the items to a prin- noyed, or bored in sessions.
cipal-component analysis using Kaiser’s criteria (eigenval- Factor 7, parental/protective (coefficient alpha=0.80), was
ues >1). We used the scree plot, percentage of variance marked by items describing a wish to protect and nur-
accounted for, and parallel analysis (33–35) to select the ture the patient in a parental way, above and beyond
number of factors to rotate. The scree plot indicated a normal positive feelings toward the patient.
break between eight and nine factors, and parallel analysis Factor 8, criticized/mistreated (coefficient alpha=0.83),
indicated eight factors with eigenvalues larger than would included items describing feelings of being unappreci-
be expected by chance (p<0.05 with 100 random data ated, dismissed, or devalued by the patient.
sets). We therefore conducted factor analyses with seven,
eight, and nine factors to maximize interpretability. Ruling Out Theoretical Bias
Several factors emerged across algorithms, rotations, as a Rival Hypothesis
and estimation procedures. We report here the most co- This factor structure is conceptually coherent and clini-
herent solution, the eight-factor promax (oblique) solu- cally recognizable. However, an important question is the
tion using maximum likelihood estimation, which we fa- extent to which its coherence simply reflects the theoreti-
vored a priori because of the characteristics of promax cal beliefs of participating clinicians, particularly given
rotations (notably the absence of the assumption of or- that 40% of clinicians in the sample reported a psychody-
thogonal factors and the tendency to maximize factor namic orientation. To evaluate this possibility, we con-
loadings within factors) and maximum likelihood estima- ducted a second factor analysis, this time eliminating all
tion (notably the advantages for use in subsequent confir- clinicians who reported a psychoanalytic or psychody-

892 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005


BETAN, HEIM, ZITTEL CONKLIN, ET AL.

TABLE 1. Factor Structure of the Countertransference Questionnairea


Factor and Itemb Loading
Factor 1: overwhelmed/disorganized
I feel resentful working with him/her 0.72
I wish I had never taken him/her on as a patient 0.71
When checking my phone messages, I feel anxiety or dread that there will be one from him/her 0.69
S/he frightens me 0.67
I feel used or manipulated by him/her 0.62
I return his/her phone calls less promptly than I do with my other patients 0.61
I call him/her between sessions more than my other patients 0.60
I think or fantasize about ending the treatment 0.59
I feel mistreated or abused by him/her 0.55
I feel pushed to set very firm limits with him/her 0.54
I feel angry at him/her 0.52
I feel repulsed by him/her 0.50
Factor 2: helpless/inadequate
I feel I am failing to help him/her or I worry that I won’t be able to help him/her 0.84
I feel incompetent or inadequate working with him/her 0.80
I feel hopeless working with him/her 0.78
I think s/he might do better with another therapist or in a different kind of therapy 0.67
I feel overwhelmed by his/her needs 0.62
I feel less successful helping him/her than other patients 0.62
I feel anxious working with him/her 0.61
I feel confused in sessions with him/her 0.52
Factor 3: positive
I look forward to sessions with him/her 0.69
S/he is one of my favorite patients 0.67
I like him/her very much 0.67
I find it exciting working with him/her 0.58
I am very hopeful about the gains s/he is making or will likely make in treatment 0.52
I have trouble relating to the feelings s/he expresses –0.48
If s/he were not my patient, I could imagine being friends with him/her 0.44
I feel like I understand him/her 0.43
I feel pleased or satisfied after sessions with him/her 0.43
Factor 4: special/overinvolved
I disclose my feelings with him/her more than with other patients 0.64
I self-disclose more about my personal life with him/her than with my other patients 0.64
I do things for him/her, or go the extra mile for him/her, in way that I don’t do for other patients 0.52
I feel guilty when s/he is distress or deteriorates, as if I must be somehow responsible 0.39
I end sessions overtime with him/her more than with my other patients 0.39
Factor 5: sexualized
I find myself being flirtatious with him/her 0.99
I feel sexually attracted to him/her 0.89
I feel sexual tension in the room 0.78
I tell him/her I love him/her 0.62
Factor 6: disengaged
I feel bored in sessions with him/her 0.82
My mind often wanders to things other than what s/he is talking about 0.72
I don’t feel fully engaged in sessions with him/her 0.53
I lose my temper with him/her 0.46
I watch the clock with him/her more than with my other patients 0.46
I feel annoyed in sessions with him/her 0.42
Factor 7: parental/protective
I feel like I want to protect him/her 0.69
I feel nurturant toward him/her 0.68
I have warm, almost parental feelings toward him/her 0.67
I wish I could give him/her what others never could 0.53
I feel angry at people in his/her life 0.45
Factor 8: criticized/mistreated
I feel unappreciated by him/her 0.75
I feel criticized by him/her 0.63
I feel dismissed or devalued 0.60
I feel am “walking on eggshells” around him/her, afraid that if I say the wrong thing s/he will explode, fall apart, or walk out 0.56
I have to stop myself from saying or doing something aggressive or critical 0.44
a Based on data provided by a national random sample of psychiatrists and clinical psychologists (N=181) who were asked to describe a ran-
domly selected nonpsychotic psychotherapy patient at least 18 years old whom they had treated for at least eight sessions.
b Items with high loadings on each factor are listed. As is standard in factor-analytic studies, a substantial number of items (N=25) did not load
strongly on any single factor and hence are not listed here. Whether the unlisted items will be deleted from the instrument or reworked in
the future will depend on 1) replication of the factor structure in a second sample by using confirmatory factor analysis and 2) whether those
items prove useful in composite portraits of different disorders.

Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 893


COUNTERTRANSFERENCE

TABLE 2. Partial Correlations Between Countertransfer- countertransference. The data supported the hypothesis
ence Questionnaire Factors and Dimensional Measures of
for cluster C.
DSM-IV Personality Disorder Cluster Symptoms Rated for a
Random Psychotherapy Patient Treated by a Random In secondary analyses, we followed up on some of these
Sample of Clinicians (N=181) patterns (particularly those that were contrary to our ex-
Countertransference Factor Cluster Aa Cluster Bb Cluster Cc pectations) with hypotheses specific to particular disor-
Overwhelmed/disorganized 0.13 0.43d*** –0.02
ders (rather than clusters) using partial correlation analy-
Helpless/inadequate 0.10 0.16d* 0.14
Positive –0.12 –0.22** 0.06 sis, holding constant the other nine disorders in each
Special/overinvolved 0.08 0.08d 0.13 analysis. We hypothesized that borderline personality dis-
Sexualized –0.02 0.24d*** –0.09
Disengaged 0.10d 0.24*** 0.14 order would show the expected association with the spe-
Parental/protective 0.07 0.03 0.24d*** cial/overinvolved factor. This hypothesis was supported
Criticized/mistreated 0.17d* 0.38d*** –0.01 (partial r=0.23, df=170, p=0.002). We also hypothesized
a Partial correlations controlling for cluster B and C scores.
b Partial correlations controlling for cluster A that narcissistic personality disorder would account for
and C scores.
c Partial correlations controlling for cluster A and B scores. the correlation between cluster B disorders and the disen-
d Correlation hypothesized to be significant.
gaged factor. In fact, whereas the other cluster B disorders
*p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.
showed no significant associations with therapist disen-
gagement, narcissistic personality disorder was associated
namic orientation (remaining N=108). Using the same ro- with this factor (partial r=0.30, df=170, p<0.001).
tation and estimation procedures, the factor analysis re-
produced the same factor structure as in the complete Countertransference Responses
sample, except that the factors appeared in a slightly dif- to Narcissistic Personality Disorder Patients
ferent order. Thus, the factor structure does not appear to To illustrate the uses of the Countertransference Ques-
be an artifact of clinicians’ theoretical preconceptions. tionnaire in clinical practice and to examine the extent to
which it can be used to create empirical prototypes of
Countertransference and Personality Pathology
common countertransference patterns in specific types of
As a first test of the validity and clinical applicability of pathology, we created a composite description of counter-
the Countertransference Questionnaire, we examined the transference patterns in the treatments of patients who
relationship between each of the eight factors and dimen- met the DSM-IV criteria for narcissistic personality disor-
sional measures of the DSM-IV personality disorders. Be- der. We standardized (z-scored) the items across patients
cause of the extensive comorbidity of the axis II disorders, and then averaged the item scores from patients meeting
we analyzed the data at the personality disorder cluster the DSM-IV criteria for narcissistic personality disorder
level (clusters A, B, and C) by summing the number of assessed from the axis II checklist. By standardizing items
symptoms endorsed for each of the personality disorders (setting means to 0) before aggregating, we reduced the
in each cluster. To control for comorbidity across clusters salience of items that were descriptive of all patients in the
(and for general severity of personality disturbance), we sample (e.g., positive feelings) but not specific to patients
used partial correlations, controlling for pathology associ- with narcissistic personality disorder.
ated with the other two clusters in all analyses. Table 3 presents the items most and least descriptive of
Based on the item content of the factors, we made the therapists’ descriptions of countertransference responses
following a priori predictions: 1) that the cluster A (odd/ to patients with narcissistic personality disorder (N=13).
eccentric) disorders would be associated with the disen- The composite description is remarkably similar to theo-
gaged factor and secondarily with the criticized/mis- retical and clinical accounts (e.g., references 39–42). Clini-
treated factor; 2) that the cluster B (dramatic/erratic) dis- cians reported feeling anger, resentment, and dread in
orders would be associated with the overwhelmed/ working with narcissistic personality disorder patients;
disorganized, helpless/inadequate, special/overinvolved, feeling devalued and criticized by the patient; and finding
and sexualized factors; and 3) that the cluster C (anxious) themselves distracted, avoidant, and wishing to terminate
disorders would be associated with the parental/protec- the treatment.
tive factor. As with the factor analysis, to see whether this portrait
The primary findings are reported in Table 2. As pre- of countertransference responses to narcissistic person-
dicted, cluster A showed a significant association with the ality disorder patients could be accounted for by clini-
criticized/mistreated factor, although it was not correlated cians’ theoretical preconceptions, we created a second
with the disengaged factor. The data strongly supported composite description excluding the data for the three
the associations for cluster B, except for the special/over- clinicians reporting a psychoanalytic or psychodynamic
involved factor. The cluster B (dramatic/erratic) disorders orientation (N=10). The items and means were virtually
showed an additional (unpredicted) association with the identical, suggesting once again that the data were not
disengaged factor and a negative correlation with positive theory dependent.

894 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005


BETAN, HEIM, ZITTEL CONKLIN, ET AL.

Discussion TABLE 3. Standardized Countertransference Questionnaire


Items Most and Least Descriptive of Therapists’ Response
to Patients Meeting the DSM-IV Criteria for Narcissistic
The results point to several conclusions. First, we identi- Personality Disorder (N=13)
fied eight countertransference dimensions that were robust
Countertransference Item Meana
across extraction methods and rotations: 1) overwhelmed/ Most descriptive
disorganized, 2) helpless/inadequate, 3) positive, 4) spe- I feel annoyed in sessions with him/her 1.63
cial/overinvolved, 5) sexualized, 6) disengaged, 7) parental/ I feel used or manipulated by him/her 1.42
protective, and 8) criticized/mistreated. These dimensions I lose my temper with him/her 1.38
I feel mistreated or abused by him/her 1.29
are clinically and theoretically coherent, representing di- I feel resentful working with him/her 1.24
verse reactions clinicians may have toward patients that I talk about him/her with my spouse or significant other
likely reflect a combination of the therapist’s own dynamics, more than my other patients 1.17
I feel I am “walking on eggshells” around him/her, afraid
responses evoked by the patient, and the interaction of pa- that if I say the wrong thing s/he will explode, fall apart,
tient and therapist. or walk out 1.10
When checking my phone messages, I feel anxiety or
The factor structure offers a complex portrait of coun- dread that there will be one from him/her 1.07
tertransference processes that is substantially more nu- I feel unappreciated by him/her 1.04
anced than global distinctions between positive and nega- At times I dislike him/her 1.04
I have to stop myself from saying or doing something ag-
tive countertransference. For example, factor analysis gressive or critical 1.04
identified an overwhelmed/disorganized pattern of coun- My mind often wanders to things other than what s/he is
tertransference response, characteristic of clinicians’ re- talking about 0.99
I feel criticized by him/her 0.98
sponse to primarily axis II cluster B patients, which bears I feel angry at him/her 0.98
substantial similarities to descriptions of disorganized at- I watch the clock with him/her more than with my other
tachment in young children and “unresolved” attachment patients 0.97
I get enraged at him/her 0.97
patterns in adults (37, 38). This factor was distinct from I dread sessions with him/her 0.93
other forms of negative countertransference, notably feel- I feel dismissed or devalued 0.91
ing helpless and inadequate, disengaged, and mistreated I feel like I’m being mean or cruel to him/her 0.91
I feel hopeless working with him/her 0.88
by the patient. Similarly, we identified three forms of con- I feel sexual tension in the room 0.88
nection with the patient that have elements of closeness— I think or fantasize about ending the treatment 0.87
sexualized, special/overinvolved, and parental/protec- I feel bored in sessions with him/her 0.86
I wish I had never taken him/her on as a patient 0.85
tive—that represent both positive feelings as well as po- I feel interchangeable—that I could be anyone to him/
tential countertransference snares. This complexity is her 0.84
consistent with clinical observation. What this study sug- I don’t feel fully engaged in sessions with him/her 0.83
I return his/her phone calls less promptly than I do with
gests, however, is a way of transcending some of the limi- my other patients 0.82
tations inherent in clinical theories derived from case I feel like my hands have been tied or that I have been
studies, in which a single clinician attempts to classify put in an impossible bind 0.80
I feel envious of, or competitive with him/her 0.78
countertransference experiences or constellations based I feel frustrated in sessions with him/her 0.76
on his or her own experience with a limited number of pa- Least descriptive
tients. By using an instrument that provides a “common I like him/her very much –0.32
I feel compassion for him/her –0.42
language” (see reference 43) for describing a subtle clinical
I am very hopeful about the gains s/he is making or will
phenomenon, we can essentially pool the knowledge of likely make in treatment –0.47
dozens of clinical observers, identifying latent constructs I look forward to sessions with him/her –0.47
S/he is one of my favorite patients –0.52
(varieties of countertransference experience) that reflect
a Means are in standard deviation units and describe the number of
patterns that individual observers themselves may not
standard deviations the average narcissistic personality disorder pa-
have recognized. tient in the sample is rated above the sample mean on each item.
Second, although every clinician and every therapeutic
dyad is distinct, the significant correlations between the view of countertransference reactions as useful in the di-
countertransference factors and personality disorder agnostic understanding of the patient’s dynamics, partic-
symptoms suggest that countertransference responses oc- ularly those involving repetitive interpersonal patterns. To
cur in coherent and predictable patterns. To put it another
the extent that patients sharing diagnostic features on axis
way, patients not only elicit idiosyncratic responses from
II have similar ways of thinking, feeling, and behaving in-
particular clinicians (based on the clinician’s history and
terpersonally, one would expect them to evoke similar re-
the interaction of the patient’s and the clinician’s dynam-
actions from others, including therapists, and this appears
ics) but also elicit what we might call average expectable
countertransference responses, which likely resemble re- to be the case.
sponses by other significant people in the patient’s life. Third, data from clinicians of different theoretical orien-
The associations between countertransference patterns tations showed similar patterns vis-à-vis patients with
and personality disorder characteristics support the broad particular kinds of pathology, suggesting that the results

Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 895


COUNTERTRANSFERENCE

are not artifacts of clinicians’ theoretical preconceptions. A related concern is that clinicians provided all the data
What is striking about this finding is that coherent pat- and hence that their responses on one questionnaire may
terns of countertransference response emerge in treat- not be independent of their responses on others (e.g., that
ments regardless of whether the clinician even “believes” their diagnosis of narcissistic personality disorder may not
in the concept of countertransference responses or has have been independent of their observations of the pa-
been trained to attend to them. Put another way, although tient’s behavior in the room with them). This limitation is
the concept of countertransference emerged from psy- common to virtually all studies in psychiatry, in which a
choanalytic observation, the data suggest that clinicians single observer (usually the patient) provides data on one
of all theoretical persuasions should attend to and, where measure that are then correlated with data from another
possible, make use of information provided in the context measure completed by the same informant (either by self-
of the therapeutic relationship, including their own re- report questionnaire or structured interview). It would
sponses to the patient. have been preferable to collect diagnostic data indepen-
dently of clinicians’ reports of their countertransference
Finally, the empirical portrait of countertransference re-
responses, and future research should clearly do so.
sponses toward patients with narcissistic personality dis-
order points to the way researchers can use this measure Several factors, however, mitigate the concern that the
to create empirical prototypes of subtle countertransfer- results primarily reflect clinicians’ biases or preconcep-
ence constellations with patients presenting with specific tions. First, as noted earlier, clinicians of widely different
types of personality disturbance. In principle, with a large theoretical perspectives and with widely different training
(M.D. training versus Ph.D. training) produced highly sim-
enough sample, one could empirically map the terrain of
ilar data. If, for example, cognitive behavior clinicians
countertransference patterns in response to multiple
share a theory of countertransference with psychoanalytic
forms of personality pathology. One could also identify
therapists, we are unaware of such a theory. Indeed, the
distinct constellations within diagnoses (e.g., different
similarity across theoretical orientations is one of the
kinds of narcissistic patients) or to patients who share
most interesting findings of this study, suggesting that pa-
certain experiences (e.g., survivors of childhood sexual
tients’ interpersonal patterns are quite robust in the face
trauma) that may occur across treatments, at different
of different technical styles. Second, because we used di-
points in therapy, or at different points in a single therapy
mensional rather than categorical measures of axis II pa-
hour. In working with survivors of childhood sexual abuse,
thology, clinicians’ responses on the countertransference
for example, clinicians often face the opposite danger of
measure were not likely to be influenced by their beliefs
pushing too much or too early for the patient to remem-
about whether the patient had one personality disorder or
ber—and potentially recapitulating the patient’s subjec-
another. Third, previous research suggests that clinicians
tive experience of unwanted penetration, abuse, or lack of
tend to make highly reliable and valid judgments if their
boundaries—versus avoiding discussion of traumatic observations and inferences are quantified using psycho-
events in intimate detail for fear of traumatizing the pa- metric instruments such as the ones used in this study. For
tient—and potentially recapitulating the patient’s experi- example, correlations between treating clinicians’ and in-
ence of unacknowledged but shared secrets or the inabil- dependent interviewers’ assessments of a range of vari-
ity or unwillingness of a caregiver who knew about the ables, such as measures of personality pathology and
abuse to talk about it. Identification of such patterns as adaptive functioning, tend to be large, typically >0.50 (44–
common constellations in the treatment of abuse survi- 46). Empirically, clinicians’ theoretical orientation pre-
vors could be very useful in teaching clinicians about po- dicts little variance in descriptions of clinical phenomena
tential countertransference dangers inherent in working when clinicians were asked to describe a specific patient
with abuse survivors in a way that is both clinically sensi- rather than their beliefs or theories of psychopathology
tive and empirically grounded. (47, 48). Nevertheless, future research using this measure
should assess the association between countertransfer-
Limitations
ence phenomena and patients’ personality pathology by
This study has three primary limitations. First, although using data about the patient provided by other observers.
clinicians are the most obvious informants to report on The second limitation was clinicians’ response rate to
their own countertransference responses, the counter- our request for participation (approximately 10%, for a
transference measure we used shares the inherent limits of study described as requiring 3–4 hours of work for a token
self-report measures, such as defensive biases and failure honorarium). Three factors, however, limit the likelihood
to recognize processes that an outside observer might that the results reflect response rate biases. First and fore-
identify. Thus, it would have been useful to have ratings of most, it is hard to imagine a response rate hypothesis that
therapy process by an independent observer (perhaps could explain the pattern of results. By virtue of their will-
based on audiotaped sessions) to identify patterns of clini- ingness to donate 3–4 hours of their time, the clinicians
cians’ behavior that would likely converge with clinicians’ who participated in the study may have been character-
self-reports in some ways and diverge from them in others. ized by greater interest in research, altruism, financial dis-

896 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005


BETAN, HEIM, ZITTEL CONKLIN, ET AL.

tress, or a host of other factors, compared with colleagues turn clinicians’ experiences into quantifiable dimensions
who did not participate, but it is difficult to see how any of that capture interpersonal patterns that emerge in ses-
these variables could have produced the obtained find- sions, allowing clinicians who normally attend to counter-
ings. Second, because we solicited data from clinicians in transference phenomena to hone and systematize their
multiple practice settings across North America and pro- self-reflections and providing clinicians whose theoreti-
vided them with a method of randomly selecting a patient cal orientations do not emphasize such processes a lan-
within their practice, we were able to obtain a broad cross- guage and method with which to capture information
section of patients. (Clinicians who agreed to participate about the patient and the treatment process that may be
were unaware that countertransference was one of the diagnostically and therapeutically significant.
constructs we intended to study, so we were not selecting
clinicians with a particular interest in or knowledge of this Received March 17, 2004; revision received July 13, 2004; accepted
Aug. 18, 2004. From Georgia School of Professional Psychology; Em-
domain.) Third, as noted earlier, psychologists’ response
ory University Psychoanalytic Institute, Atlanta; Hoover & Associates,
rate was almost three times the rate of psychiatrists, yet Orland Park, Ill.; Cambridge Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Cam-
the two sets of informants provided similar data, suggest- bridge, Mass.; Harvard Medical School, Boston; and the Departments
of Psychology and Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory Univer-
ing that neither training nor response rate was responsible
sity. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Betan, Geor-
for the findings. Finally, all studies have selection biases, gia School of Professional Psychology, 980 Hammond Dr., Suite 100,
some avoidable and others less so. An alternative method Atlanta, GA 30328; ebetan@argosyu.edu (e-mail); or Dr. Westen,
Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
would have been to sample clinicians at a single hospital
Emory University, 532 Kilgo Circle, Atlanta, GA 30322; dwesten@
or institution, but this strategy would likely have resulted emory.edu (e-mail).
in less generalizable results. It seems unlikely that the Supported in part by NIMH grants MH-62377 and MH-62378 to Dr.
Westen and by a grant from the Fund for Psychoanalytic Research of
sample in our study was less representative of the popula-
the American Psychoanalytic Association to Dr. Heim.
tion of clinicians or patients in psychotherapy than sam- The authors thank the 181 clinicians who contributed data to this
ples in typical studies of psychotherapy or psychopathol- study.
ogy, which tend to rely on patient populations from a
single site. Nevertheless, the data clearly require replica-
References
tion and extension with new groups of patients, and we
hope this article will generate interest in the measure for 1. Heimann P: On countertransference. Int J Psychoanal 1950;
future research. 31:81–84
2. Kernberg O: Notes on countertransference. J Am Psychoanal
A third potential objection is sample size, given the pos- Assoc 1965; 13:38–56
sibility of some instability of factor structure with a ratio of 3. Racker E: The meanings and use of countertransference. Psy-
cases to items of <3:1. However, recent thinking about fac- choanal Q 1957; 26:303–357
tor analysis, based on data from Monte Carlo simulations 4. Sandler J: Basic psychoanalytic concepts, IV: counter-transfer-
ence. Br J Psychiatry 1970; 117:83–88
and other studies, suggests that factor solutions stabilize
5. Winnicott D: Hate in the countertransference. Int J Psychoanal
with far fewer cases than previously believed (typically by 1949; 30:69–75
100 cases), as long as the factors are well marked by a suf- 6. Klein M: Notes on some schizoid mechanisms. Int J Psychoanal
ficient number of items with loadings above 0.40 or 0.50 1946; 27:99–110
(as they were here), and that conventional case-to-item 7. Bion W: Learning From Experience. London, Heinemann, 1962
8. Gabbard G: A contemporary psychoanalytic model of counter-
ratios do not take into consideration a range of variables
transference. J Clin Psychol 2001; 57:983–991
that qualifies them in one direction or the other (see, e.g., 9. Ogden TH: Projective Identification and Psychotherapeutic
references 36, 49). Clearly the next step in this research, Technique. New York, Jason Aronson, 1982
however, is a larger-N replication study using confirma- 10. Sandler J: Countertransference and role-responsiveness. Int
tory factor analysis and external ratings of variables such Rev Psychoanal 1976; 3:43–47
as personality disorder diagnosis and treatment outcome 11. Wachtel P: Psychoanalysis and Behavior Therapy. New York,
Basic Books, 1977
independent of the clinician’s reports.
12. Wachtel PL: Resistance as a problem for practice and theory. J
Psychotherapy Integration 1999; 9:103–117
Implications 13. Brody F, Farber B: The effects of therapist experience and pa-
The Countertransference Questionnaire represents an tient diagnosis on countertransference. Psychotherapy 1996;
33:372–380
effort to develop a readily administered measure that re-
14. Colson DB, Allen J, Coyne L, Dexter N, Jehl N, Mayer CA, Spohn
flects shared clinical wisdom in its item content and sta- H: An anatomy of countertransference: staff reactions to diffi-
tistical “wisdom” in its factor structure. This measure is cult psychiatric patients. Hosp Community Psychiatry 1986; 37:
germane to future research on countertransference phe- 923–928
nomena, as well as to practice, allowing clinicians to clar- 15. Holmqvist R, Armelius B: The patient’s contribution to the
ify the diagnostic relevance and utility of their reactions therapist’s countertransference feelings. J Nerv Ment Dis 1996;
184:660–666
by comparing their own responses to normed psycho- 16. McIntyre S, Schwartz R: Therapists’ differential countertransfer-
metric data. A broadband measure of countertransfer- ence reactions toward clients with major depression or border-
ence processes with known correlates such as this can line personality disorder. J Clin Psychol 1998; 54:923–931

Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org 897


COUNTERTRANSFERENCE

17. Dube J, Normandin L: The mental activities of trainee thera- 33. Horn JL: An empirical comparison of methods for estimating
pists of children and adolescents: the impact of personal psy- factor scores. Educational and Psychological Measurement
chotherapy on the listening process. Psychotherapy 1999; 33: 1965; 25:313–322
216–228 34. Horn JL: A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor
18. Swann WB: The trouble with change: self-verification and alle- analysis. Psychometrika 1965; 30:179–185
giance to the self. Psychol Sci 1997; 8:177–180 35. O’Connor BP: SPSS and SAS programs for determining the
19. Downey G, Freitas AL, Michaelis B, Khouri H: The self-fulfilling number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s
prophecy in close relationships: rejection sensitivity and rejec- MAP test. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 2000; 32:396–
tion by romantic partners. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998; 75:545–560 402
20. Giesler R, Josephs RA, Swann WB: Self-verification in clinical de- 36. Fabregar LR, Wegener DT, MacCallum RC, Strahan EJ: Evaluat-
pression: the desire for negative evaluation. J Abnorm Psychol ing the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological re-
1996; 105:358–368 search. Psychol Methods 1999; 4:272–299
21. Hayes JA, Gelso CJ: Effects of therapist-trainees’ anxiety and 37. Cassidy J, Mohr JJ: Unsolvable fear, trauma, and psychopathol-
empathy on countertransference behavior. J Clin Psychol 1991; ogy: theory, research, and clinical considerations related to dis-
47:284–290 organized attachment across the life span. Clin Psychol Sci
22. Hayes JA, Gelso CJ: Male counselors’ discomfort with gay and Pract 2002; 8:275–298
HIV-infected clients. J Couns Psychol 1993; 40:86–93 38. Main M, Kaplan N, Cassidy J: Security in infancy, childhood, and
23. Rosenberger EW, Hayes JA: Origins, consequences, and man- adulthood: a move to the level of representation. Monogr Soc
agement of countertransference: a case study. J Couns Psychol Res Child Dev 1985; 50:66–104
2002; 49:221–232 39. Altshul VA: The so-called boring client. Am J Psychother 1977;
24. Sharkin BS, Gelso CJ: The influence of counselor trainee anger- 31:533–545
proneness and anger discomfort on reactions to an angry cli- 40. Blieberg E: Stages in the treatment of narcissistic children and
ent. J Couns Dev 1993; 71:483–487 adolescents. Bull Menninger Clin 1987; 51:296–313
25. Yulis S, Kiesler DJ: Countertransference response as a function 41. Kernberg O: Further contributions to the treatment of narcis-
of therapist anxiety and content of patient talk. J Consult Clin sistic personalities. Int J Psychoanal 1974; 55:215–240
Psychol 1968; 32:413–419 42. Segel NP: Narcissism and adaptation to indignity. Int J Psycho-
26. Robbins SB, Jolkovski MP: Managing countertransference feel- anal 1981; 62:465–476
ings: an interactional model using awareness of feeling and 43. Block J: The Q-Sort Method in Personality Assessment and Psy-
theoretical framework. J Couns Psychol 1987; 34:276–282 chiatric Research. Palo Alto, Calif, Consulting Psychologists
27. Najavits LM, Griffin ML, Luborsky L, Frank A, Weiss RD, Liese BL, Press, 1978
Thompson H, Nakayama E, Siqueland L, Daley D, Onken LS: 44. Hilsenroth MJ, Ackerman SJ, Blagys MD, Baumann BD, Baity
Therapists’ emotional reactions to substance abusers: a new MR, Smith SR, Price JL, Smith CL, Heindselman TL, Mount MK,
questionnaire and initial findings. Psychotherapy: Theory, Re- Holdwick DJ Jr: Reliability and validity of DSM-IV axis V. Am J
search, Practice 1995; 32:669–677 Psychiatry 2000; 157:1858–1863
28. Westen D, Shedler J: Revising and assessing axis II, part I: devel- 45. Westen D, Muderrisoglu S, Fowler C, Shedler J, Koren D: Affect
oping a clinically and empirically valid assessment method. regulation and affective experience: individual differences,
Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:258–272 group differences, and measurement using a Q-sort proce-
29. Westen D, Shedler J, Durrett C, Glass S, Martens A: Personality dure. J Consult Clin Psychol 1997; 65:429–439
diagnoses in adolescence: DSM-IV axis II diagnoses and an em- 46. Westen D, Muderrisoglu S: Reliability and validity of personality
pirically derived alternative. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:952– disorder assessment using a systematic clinical interview: eval-
966 uating an alternative to structured interviews. J Personal Dis-
30. Blais M, Norman D: A psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV ord 2003; 17:350–368
personality disorder criteria. J Personal Disord 1997; 11:168– 47. Shedler J, Westen D: Refining the measurement of axis II: a Q-
176 sort procedure for assessing personality pathology. Assessment
31. Morey LC: The Personality Assessment Inventory: Professional 1998; 5:333–353
Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 48. Shedler J, Westen D: Dimensions of personality pathology: an
1991 alternative to the five-factor model. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:
32. Zittel C, Westen D: The Countertransference Questionnaire. At- 1743–1754
lanta, Emory University, Departments of Psychology and Psy- 49. Russell DW: In search of underlying dimensions: the use (and
chiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 2003. http://www.psychsys- abuse) of factor analysis in Personality and Social Psychology
tems.net/lab Bulletin. Pers Social Psychol Bull 2002; 28:1629–1646

898 http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org Am J Psychiatry 162:5, May 2005

S-ar putea să vă placă și