Sunteți pe pagina 1din 2

On the Road to Sweetness: A Clear-Cut Destination?

Margaretta Midura November 4, 2012 04:55 http://www.yalescientific.org/2012/11/on-the-

road-to-sweetness-a-clear-cut-destination/
Imagine putting a piece of candy on your tongue. To taste its full sweetness, you would most likely follow the
taste map you have seen in biology and psychology textbooks and place it at the tip of your tongue. After all, the
well-known taste map shows that the tip of the tongue is responsible for tasting sweetness, the surrounding area
for saltiness, the tongues edges for the sourness, and the back end of the tongue for bitterness. This map has
become so widespread that it is believed wine glasses are shaped to cater to it. Despite how popular this idea may
be, current research asserts it is nothing more than a myth.
The idea of a taste map dates back to a study by the German scientist D.P. Hanig during the early 1900s. After
tasting a food, volunteers were asked to determine which part of the tongue they tasted it, and this was repeated
for each of the four main taste groups (sweetness, saltiness, bitterness, and sourness). While Hanig showed that
certain areas of the tongue did in fact vary with regards to sensitivity for the four tastes, he was not implying that
these areas were insensitive to any of the tastes.

The original tongue map depicting which areas of the tongue sense the four
primary tastes. Photo courtesy of James Beard Foundation.

Edwin G. Boring, a Harvard psychologist, contributed to the misinterpretation


of the data. He quantified Hanigs original findings by substituting subjective
volunteer responses with numerical data, then used these numbers to create
the tongue map with which we are familiar today. Borings map was supposed
to document relative sensitivities. However, his unclear notation misled many
scientists of the era to believe that the areas he designated on the map could
only sense one particular taste, thus propagating the deceptive idea of a taste
map.
This taste map concept spread at a surprisingly fast pace. It could have been
easily disproved by a few simple taste tests, but people held on to the idea of a map. People like having discrete
classifications. Its a system that is easy to be locked into, explains Dr. Aidan Kiely, a postdoctoral fellow who is
currently researching taste and smell mechanisms in the Carlson Lab at Yale University. Another potential reason
is that certain animals do in fact have taste maps. Kiely noted that fruit flies have 32 taste hairs and each has
receptors that are sensitive to different tastes, but this does not necessarily mean such a trait exists in humans.
The real mechanism by which our taste buds pick up on individual tastes elucidates why the taste map is a myth.
Projections called papillae are spread all across the tongue. The papillae contain the majority of the taste buds.
These taste buds house a mixture of elongated taste cells that are capable of responding to salty, sweet, bitter,
sour or umami tastes. (Umami is a distinct fifth taste that was neglected in the original studies. It is the taste of
glutamate and gives meat its savory flavor.) The taste cells have approximately 50 to 150 receptors for each taste,
which allow specific molecules to bind and eventually translate the signal into the taste we perceive. A sweet

cell, for example, will have receptors that can bind to sugar molecules. Therefore, each region of the tongue can
sense each of the five primary tastes.

The process of sensing taste involves papillae that contain taste buds. The taste buds are made up of taste cells
with receptors that bind to specific molecules (such as the sugar molecule pictured). Photo courtesy of Scientific
American.
Virginia Collings, a researcher at the University of Pittsburgh, offered evidence for this reconstructed layout of the
tongue in 1974. She found that even though there was a slight difference in concentrations of certain taste
receptors in certain areas of the tongue, the overall effect this had on sensing taste was negligible. Her research
also showed that taste receptors are not confined to the tongue. They are also present in areas surrounding the
tongue, such as the soft palate of the mouth and on the epiglottis.
Even though the commercial aspect of taste is greater than that of hearing or smell, it has been greatly
understudied, says Kiely. The fact that so little is known about taste definitely contributed to the dominant taste
map myth. Fortunately, there has recently been a great deal of research done on taste. Studies done on mice
suggest that there may be unique maps of neurons in the mammalian brain (rather than on the tongue) that are
found in specific areas for each primary taste. Other studies are trying to determine whether there are receptors
for other tastes, such as for fat. Research in this field is quickly progressing and myths such as the taste map are
being brought to light.
So next time you put that piece of candy into your mouth, do not worry. You will taste it regardless of where it
lands.

S-ar putea să vă placă și