Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/263207348

A Longitudinal Moderated Mediation Model of


Nonsuicidal Self-injury among Adolescents

ARTICLE in JOURNAL OF ABNORMAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2014


Impact Factor: 3.09 DOI: 10.1007/s10802-014-9901-x Source: PubMed

CITATIONS DOWNLOADS VIEWS

2 55 196

3 AUTHORS:

Jianing You Min-Pei Lin


South China Normal University National Taiwan Normal University
14 PUBLICATIONS 71 CITATIONS 6 PUBLICATIONS 58 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Freedom Leung
The Chinese University of Hong Kong
35 PUBLICATIONS 398 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Available from: Jianing You


Retrieved on: 16 June 2015
J Abnorm Child Psychol
DOI 10.1007/s10802-014-9901-x

A Longitudinal Moderated Mediation Model of Nonsuicidal


Self-injury among Adolescents
Jianing You & Min-Pei Lin & Freedom Leung

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Abstract This study tested a longitudinal moderated media- impulsivity made additional contribution to the prediction of
tion model of the engagement in non suicidal self-injury future NSSI above and beyond the effects of other risk factors.
(NSSI) based on Nocks (2009) integrated theoretical model Findings of this study may help to elucidate the diverse roles
of the development of NSSI. We assessed general predispos- of different types of risk factors in the engagement in NSSI,
ing factors (i.e. borderline personality disorder features), pre- and may also shed new light on our understanding about the
cipitating factors (i.e. negative emotions), and NSSI-specific nature of this behavior.
vulnerability factors (i.e. behavioral impulsivity and self-
criticism) among 3,600 Chinese secondary school adolescents Keywords Nonsuicidal self-injury . Borderline personality
(56.6 % females, aged between 12 and 18 years). Assessments disorder . Negative emotions . Behavioral impulsivity .
were conducted for three times, 6 months apart. Results sup- Self-criticism . Moderated mediation
ported the longitudinal mediation model, such that negative
emotions mediated the relation of borderline personality dis- Nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) is the deliberate, direct, social-
order features to NSSI. The moderating effects of behavioral ly unacceptable destruction or alteration of body tissue that
impulsivity and self-criticism were both significant, indicating occurs in the absence of conscious suicidal intent (Nock and
that adolescents with higher levels of both variables were Favazza 2009). Common NSSI acts include cutting/carving
more likely to engage in NSSI. Moreover, behavioral skin, self-hitting, banging the head or other parts of the body
against the wall, burning skin and severely scratching
(Jacobson and Gould 2007). This behavior usually begins
This research was funded in part by National Natural Science Foundation
of China (Grant No. 31300874) and South China Normal University between 12 and 14 years of age and is prevalent among
Young Teacher Research Cultivation Grant (2012KJ013) awarded to adolescents (Jacobson and Gould 2007). In a review of studies
Dr. Jianing You. This study was also supported by Key Laboratory of with community adolescent samples across the globe, a mean
Mental Health and Cognitive Science of Guangdong Province and Re- lifetime prevalence rate of 17.9 %, a mean 12-month preva-
search Center for Crisis Intervention and Psychological Service of
Guangdong Province, South China Normal University. The funders had lence of 19.0 %, and a mean 6-month prevalence of 15.1 %
no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, were reported (Muehlenkamp et al. 2012). The relatively high
or preparation of the manuscript. prevalence rate of NSSI has elicited much attention to this
J. You (*) behavior. In fact, a new NSSI disorder has been included in the
Center for Studies of Psychological Application & School of fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association 2013)
e-mail: youjianing@gmail.com
as a condition requiring further study. The aim of the present
M.<P. Lin study was to address one of the major questions which are in
Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, National great need of further study: why adolescents engage in NSSI.
Taiwan Normal University, Taipei City, Taiwan, Republic of China To answer this question, Nock (2009) has proposed an
integrated theoretical model of the development of NSSI.
F. Leung
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, According to this model, NSSI is caused by the interplay of
Hong Kong, China multiple risk factors, including distal risk factors, general
J Abnorm Child Psychol

predisposing factors for maladaptive behaviors, precipitating studies have shown the link between BPD and NSSI (e.g.,
factors, and NSSI-specific vulnerability factors. Distal risk Chapman et al. 2005; Nock et al. 2006; Shearer 1994), as well
factors include genetic predisposition for high emotional/ as the close relations of various BPD features, e.g., high
cognitive reactivity, childhood abuse/maltreatment, and famil- emotional reactivity, unstable relationship and unstable sense
ial hostility/criticism. General predisposing factors comprise of self, to NSSI (e.g., Hilt et al. 2008a; Nock et al. 2008; You
both intrapersonal and interpersonal factors, such as high et al. 2012a; You et al. 2012b). With regard to the precipitating
aversive emotion and poor communication skills. These fac- factors of negative emotions, a considerable number of studies
tors correspond to two of the core features of borderline have documented the correlation between depression, anxiety,
personality disorder (BPD), i.e. high emotional reactivity anger, hostility and NSSI (e.g., Hilt et al. 2008b; Laye-Gindhu
and unstable relationship (Leung and Leung 2009). In fact, and Schonert-Reichl 2005; Muehlenkamp and Gutierrez
NSSI is one of the diagnostic criteria of BPD in DSM-IV-TR 2004, 2007; Ross and Heath 2002).
(Association 2000). Finally, both of the NSSI-specific vulnerability factors,
In Nocks (2009) theoretical model, precipitating factors behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism, stemming from the
for NSSI are examined through functional analysis, which pragmatic and self-punishment hypotheses respectively have
focuses on the immediate events that precede and follow been relatively well studied. First, evidence supporting the
NSSI. Four functions of NSSI have been identified (Nock association between poor impulse control and NSSI came
and Prinstein 2004). They are intrapersonal positive reinforce- from two lines of research. On the one hand, past research
ment function (e.g., to feel something), intrapersonal negative showed that self-injurers scored significantly higher on vari-
reinforcement function (e.g., to relieve depression), interper- ous self-report measures of impulsivity than non-injurers
sonal positive reinforcement function (e.g., to get attention), (Claes et al. 2001; Herpertz et al. 1997; Janis and Nock
and interpersonal negative reinforcement function (e.g., to 2009). On the other hand, NSSI has been found to link with
avoid punishment). Previous studies demonstrated that NSSI many other impulsive behaviors, including frequent antisocial
primarily serves the intrapersonal negative reinforcement behaviors, alcohol consumption, smoking, and drug use (De
function, or the emotion regulation function (see Klonsky Leo and Heller 2004; Hawton et al. 2006; Hilt, Nock et al.
2007; Messer and Fremouw 2008; for reviews). Thus, nega- 2008; Patton et al. 1997). Non-significant relationship be-
tive emotions may be one of the precipitating factors for NSSI. tween NSSI and behavioral tasks of impulsivity (e.g., go/no
However, there are many non-injuring or less destructive go task) was also revealed in laboratory studies (e.g., Glenn
ways to regulate negative emotions. Why certain people spe- and Klonsky 2010; Janis and Nock 2009). This inconsistency
cifically select NSSI rather than other coping behaviors? To may be due to that the laboratory behavioral tasks could not
answer this question, Nock (2009) proposed several hypoth- capture self-injurers heightened impulsivity under emotional
eses, including the social learning hypothesis, self-punishment distress in real life situations. Second, some studies also
hypothesis, social signaling hypothesis, pragmatic hypothesis, found that those who engaged in NSSI reported significantly
pain analgesia hypothesis and implicit identification hypothe- higher levels of self-criticism than do non-injurers (Glassman
sis. This study focused on testing the self-punishment and the et al. 2007), and self-punishment was among the primary
pragmatic hypothesis. According to the self-punishment hy- reasons self-injurers gave for engaging in NSSI (Nock and
pothesis, individuals specifically select NSSI as a coping Prinstein 2004).
strategy because they simultaneously possess a sense of self- Although previous research provided preliminary support
criticism or self-hatred along with negative emotions, and by for Nocks (2009) theoretical model of NSSI, they were lim-
injuring the body, they could punish the self. The pragmatic ited in several areas. First, most of past studies examined the
hypothesis, on the other hand, proposes that people may bivariate associations between various risk factors and NSSI.
choose to engage in NSSI over other self-regulating strategies They seldom included different types of risk factors and NSSI
because it is a rapid, effective, and easily implemented method in a single model and assessed their relationships simulta-
of regulating ones affective/cognitive and social experiences. neously. Thus, the distinct roles played by these diverse types
Thus, individuals who are more impulsive and less tolerant of of risk factors in the engagement in NSSI are largely unexam-
distress are more likely to conduct NSSI. ined. Second, most of previous research examining the rela-
Previous studies have provided some initial support for tions of various risk factors and NSSI was cross-sectional. The
different types of risk factors in Nocks (2009) theoretical temporal associations thus could not be ascertained, which
model. Regarding the distal risk factors, Zoroglu et al. may undermine the significance of these results. Third, if
(2003) found that adolescents who had suffered childhood behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism are specific vulnera-
abuse or neglect had 2.7-fold higher NSSI behaviors than their bility factors for NSSI, they should make additional contribu-
counterparts with no history of abuse or neglect. Baral et al. tions to the prediction of NSSI above and beyond those of the
(1998) also revealed the strong association between sexual general and precipitating factors. Extant literatures, neverthe-
abuse and NSSI. For the general predisposing factors, many less, have not tested this hypothesis. Fourth, many previous
J Abnorm Child Psychol

studies on NSSI were conducted with clinical samples. With from school on the day of assessment. Comparisons among
the prevalence of NSSI in community adolescents participants who participated in Wave 1 testing only, those
(Muehlenkamp et al. 2012), more attention should be paid to who participated in Wave 1 and Wave 2 testing, and those who
NSSI among non-clinical youths. participated in all three testing revealed no significant differ-
To partly fill in the knowledge gaps, the present study ences on all studied variables. Descriptive analyses of NSSI at
examined the roles of multiple types of risk factors in the different waves were based on samples of the corresponding
engagement in NSSI among a large sample of Chinese com- waves. Other analyses were based on the three-wave
munity adolescents assessed over three waves, 6 months apart. followed-up sample.
We focused on the contributions of general predisposing
factors (i.e. BPD features), precipitating factors (i.e. negative Procedure
emotions), and NSSI-specific vulnerability factors (i.e. behav-
ioral impulsivity and self-criticism) to the engagement in We obtained written informed consent from participants par-
NSSI. Towards this end, we tested a longitudinal moderated ents before the testing. During each assessment wave, the
mediation model, in which negative emotions served as a same batch of questionnaires was group administered in class-
mediator of the relation between BPD features and NSSI, rooms of 3542 students under the supervision of school
and behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism exerted moder- personnel. At the beginning of each assessment, teachers were
ating effects on the mediation model. We also tested whether required to make it clear to students that participation was
behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism provided additional voluntary and students could stop anytime whenever they feel
predictive utility above and beyond those of the general pre- uncomfortable. A unique ID number for each student was
disposing factors and the precipitating factors in examining created for data-matching purpose. Participants were assured
future NSSI. Based on Nocks (2009) theoretical model, we strict confidentiality of the collected data and that only re-
hypothesized that adolescents with high levels of behavioral search personnel have access to the questionnaires. All testing
impulsivity and self-criticism would be more likely to engage materials and procedures were approved by the university
in NSSI than their counterparts with low levels of both vari- Institutional Review Board.
ables. Both of the NSSI-specific vulnerability factors were
also hypothesized to make additional contributions to the Measures
prediction of NSSI.
Nonsuicidal Self-Injury (NSSI) Seven NSSI behaviors, i.e.,
self-cutting, burning, biting to injury, punching to bruise,
Method severely scratching skin to injury, inserting sharp objects to
the nail or skin, and banging the head or other parts of the
Participants body against the wall that leads to bruise, were assessed in
this study. Participants were asked In the past 6 months,
Participants came from eight secondary schools in Hong have you engaged in the following behaviors to deliberately
Kong and were surveyed semiannually for three times. All harm yourself but without suicidal intent? All the seven
students from the participating schools were invited to partic- NSSI behavior items were rated on a 4-point scale, ranging
ipate in the study, yet participation was on a voluntary basis. from 1 never, 2 once or twice, 3 three to five times to 4
Because of the cooperation of school authorities and their six times or more.
strong encouragement, overall student participation rates were
close to 99 % in all schools. At the baseline assessment (Wave Emotional Reactivity (ER) The 21-item Emotion Reactivity
1), 5,423 students (2,857 females) participated. They aged Scale was used to measure emotional reactivity as character-
between 12 and 18 years (M=14.63 years, SD=1.25) and ized by emotional sensitivity, intensity and persistence (Nock
studied in Grades 710. All of them are Chinese. et al. 2008). Sample items included I tend to get very emo-
We re-assessed the Wave 1 sample after a 6-month and a tional very easily, When I experience emotions, I feel them
12-month interval, respectively. At the Wave 2 assessment, very strongly/intensely, and When I am angry/upset, it takes
6,911 adolescents (52.6 % females) participated and 3,999 me much longer than most people to calm down. Factor
(55.4 % females) of them were retained from the Wave 1 analysis revealed a unidimensional structure. The ERS
sample. At the Wave 3 assessment, 6,831 adolescents showed good convergent and divergent validity via relations
(52.6 % females) were included and 3,600 (56.6 % females) to behavioral inhibition/activation, and criterion-related valid-
of them were successfully followed for all three waves. The ity as measured by associations with specific types of psycho-
participant retention rates were 73.7 and 66.4 % for the first pathology and self-injurious thoughts and behaviors (Nock
two waves and all three waves, respectively. Attrition was et al. 2008). Responses were made on a 5-point Likert scale,
mainly due to students transferring to other schools or absent ranging from 1 not at all like me to 5 completely like me.
J Abnorm Child Psychol

This scale had Cronbachs alphas of 0.96, 0.97 and 0.97 for good in this study (for the Wave 1 data, the Cronbachs alpha
the Wave 1, 2, and 3 data in this study, respectively. values were 0.86, 0.80 and 0.83 for DASS-D, DASS-A and
DASS-S, respectively; for the Wave 2 data, the Cronbachs
Unstable Relationship (UR) A 7-item scale extracted and alpha values were 0.87, 0.82 and 0.85, respectively; and for
modified from the Interpersonal Relationship Section of the the Wave 3 data, the Cronbachs alpha values were 0.86, 0.81
Revised Diagnostic Interview for Borderlines (DIB-R; and 0.86, respectively).
Zanarini et al. 1989) was used to assess unstable relationship
in this study. We extracted all three items in the Abandonment/ Behavioral Impulsivity (BI) Behavioral impulsivity was
Engulfment/Annihilation Concerns subsection, all three items assessed by ten impulsive behaviors. These items were de-
in the Unstable Close Relationship subsection, and one item in rived from the Impulse Action Patterns Section of the DIB-R
the Recurrent Problems in Close Relationships subsection. (Zanarini et al. 1989). From this section, we extracted both
Among all items in the Interpersonal Relationship Section, items in the Substance Abuse subsection (i.e. alcohol abuse
we believe these items best characterize unstable relationship and drug abuse), one item in the Sexual Deviance subsection
in adolescents. Adjustments were made to the language ex- (i.e. promiscuity), and seven items in the Other Impulsive
pression to make sure that these items could be understood by Behaviors subsection (i.e. binge eating, spending sprees, ver-
Chinese adolescents. This scale has good concurrent validity bal outbursts, physical fights, physical threats, physical as-
and has been validated and used in previous studies with saults, and property damage). Items measuring paraphilias,
Chinese community adolescents (Leung and Leung 2009; gambling sprees, reckless driving and antisocial actions were
You et al. 2012a, 2012b). Sample items of this scale included not selected because these behaviors are very rare among
I either love or hate other people in an extreme way and Chinese adolescents. Participants rated the frequency of en-
My relationships with other people are very unstable. gaging in these behaviors during the past 6 months using a 4-
Responses were made on a 4-point scale (1=strongly disagree point scale, ranging from 1 never, 2 once or twice, 3
and 4=strongly agree). This scale had Cronbachs alpha three to five times to 4 six times or more. This scale had
values of 0.85, 0.84 and 0.86 for the Wave 1, 2, and 3 data Cronbachs alphas of 0.69 for the Wave 1 data, 0.69 for the
in this study, respectively. Wave 2 data, and 0.73 for the Wave 3 data in this study.

Unstable Self-Image (USIM) We extracted four items from Self-Criticism (SC) The 9-item Self-Criticism Subscale of the
the Rosenbergs 5-item Stability of Self Scale (Alsaka and Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ; Blatt et al.
Olweus 1986) to assess the construct of unstable self-image. 1976) was used to measure participants self-critical thinking
The items included My self-evaluations are entirely different style. This scale has sufficient internal consistency and satis-
everyday, Compared to most people, my self-evaluation factory inter-correlations with the other two subscales, i.e.,
changes very quickly, Some days I have a very good opin- Dependency and Efficacy, of the DEQ (Zuroff et al. 1990).
ion about myself, other days I have a very poor opinion about Sample items of the SCS include I often find that I dont live
myself, and Sometimes I feel good 1 minute and then the up to my own standards or ideals and There is a consider-
next minute I feel terrible. The item I feel nothing, or almost able difference between how I am now and how I would like
nothing, can change the opinion I currently hold of myself. to be. Responses were made on a 7-point scale (1=not at all
was not included in this study because the meaning of this like me and 7=like me very much). This scale had Cronbachs
item was quite different from those of the others. Ratings were alpha values of 0.82, 0.86 and 0.87 for the Wave 1, 2, and 3
made on a 4-point scale (1=strongly disagree and 4=strongly data, respectively.
agree). This scale had Cronbachs alpha values of 0.90, 0.90
and 0.91 for the Wave 1, 2, and 3 data, respectively. Data Analysis

Negative Emotions (NE) The Chinese version of the short The longitudinal moderated mediation model was tested fol-
Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS21; Taouk et al. lowing a series of steps. First, we tested the longitudinal
2001) was used to assess three common negative emotions: factorial invariance of BPD features as indicated by ER, UR,
depression (DASS-D), anxiety (DASS-A) and tension/stress and USIM, and NE as indicated by DASS-D, DASS-A, and
(DASS-S). The DASS-21 has good convergent and DASS-S. Establishment of the longitudinal factorial invari-
discrimant validity (Antony et al. 1998). Sample items of ance ensures that the fundamental meanings of the latent
DASS-21 included I feel that life was meaningless, I find constructs of BPD features and NE do not change over time.
myself getting agitated, and I find it difficult to relax. Factorial invariance is traditionally established by fitting a
Responses were made on a 4-point scale (0=do not apply to sequence of models: a) the unconstrained model with the
me at all and 3=apply to me very much or most of the time). covariances among the residuals of corresponding indicators
Internal consistency for all three subscales of DASS-21 was across measurement occasions being freely estimated; b) the
J Abnorm Child Psychol

loading invariance or weak factorial invariance model with the were tested using EQS 6.1 (Bentler 2005). Missing data were
loadings of corresponding indicators being equated across handled using the Full Information Maximum Likelihood
time; c) the intercept invariance or strong factorial invariance method.
model with the intercepts of corresponding indicators being
equated across time; and d) the residual invariance or strict
factorial invariance model with the residual variances of cor- Results
responding indicators being equated across time. In this study,
we only tested the weak factorial invariance model as this is Prevalence of NSSI
the minimum requirement for examining covariance relations
(Little et al. 2007). Among the Wave 1, Wave 2 and Wave 3 samples, 10.3 %
Next, we tested the full hypothesized longitudinal media- (n=559 out of 5,423), 12.7 % (n=869 out of 6,911) and 9.2 %
tion model of NSSI with the whole sample. Model fit for the (n=621 out of 6,831) of the participants endorsed one or more
longitudinal factorial invariance and the mediation models of the seven NSSI behaviors in the preceding 6 months,
was assessed using a number of indexes. Because of the large respectively. For all three waves, females (13.9 %, n=397
sample size, the chi-square (2) statistic is likely to overesti- for Wave 1; 15.1 %, n=546 for Wave 2; 10.8 %, n=382 for
mate the lack of fit and will always suggest a lack of fit Wave 3) were significantly more likely than males (6.3 %, n=
(Bollen 1989). Therefore, multiple complementary fit indices 162 for Wave 1; 10.0 %, n=323 for Wave 2; 7.5 %, n=239 for
were used to evaluate individual model fit (Hu and Bentler Wave 3) to conduct NSSI [for Wave 1, 2 (1, N=5,423)=
1999), specifically the non-normed fit index (NNFI), compar- 84.04, p < 0.001; for Wave 2, 2 (1, N = 6,859) = 40.82,
ative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of p<0.001; for Wave 3, 2 (1, N=6,749)=22.23, p<0.001].
approximation (RMSEA) and its 90 % confidence interval Among participants engaging in NSSI, cutting was the most
(CI). Acceptable model fit was defined by the following prevalent method (5.2, 5.7, and 4.6 % in Wave 1, 2, and 3
criteria: RMSEA <0.08, NNFI >0.90, CFI >0.90. Good model respectively), followed by biting (4.8, 5.5, and 4.4 % in Wave
fit was defined by the following criteria: RMSEA <0.05, 1, 2, and 3 respectively), scratching (4.7, 4.6 and 4.0 % in
NNFI >0.95, CFI >0.95. Wave 1, 2, and 3 respectively), banging the head or other parts
Finally, we tested the moderating effects of BI and SC of the body against the wall (3.2, 3.6, and 3.0 % in Wave 1, 2,
separately on the longitudinal mediation model. To test the and 3 respectively), punching (2.5, 2.8, and 2.4 % in Wave 1,
moderating effect of BI, we used the residual centering 2, and 3 respectively), and inserting sharp objects to the nail or
approach proposed by Little et al. (2006). This approach skin (2.1, 1.7, and 1.5 % in Wave 1, 2, and 3 respectively).
includes two steps. In the first step, BI and the indicators of Burning (0.8, 0.7, and 0.8 % in Wave 1, 2, and 3 respectively)
the first-order latent variables (i.e. ER, UR, and USIM of was the least prevalent method of NSSI.1
BPD features, and DASS-D, DASS-A, and DASS-S of
NE) were multiplied and the resulting product is then The Longitudinal Factorial Invariance Models
regressed on BI and all first-order effect indicators. The
residuals of these regression analyses were saved in the BPD Features We first fitted the unconstrained model of the
data set. In the second step, the residuals were used as latent construct of BPD features. The data violated the as-
indicators of the product variable in the latent interaction sumption of multivariate normality, Mardias coefficient=
model. Significant coefficients of the paths from the prod- 24.69, p<0.01. Thus, analyses were rescaled by Satorra
uct latent variable to the outcome variable indicate signif- Bentler modification (Satorra et al. 1991). The unconstrained
icant moderating effect of BI. The moderating effect of SC model achieved a satisfactory fit, 2SB (15, N=3336)=26.12,
was tested using the same procedure. A detailed practical p<0.05, CFI=0.999, NFI=0.998, NNFI=0.998, RMSEA=
illustration of the residual centering approach can be found 0.015 (90 % CI: 0.0040.024). Next, we tested the loading
in Steinmetza et al. (2011). invariance model. This model also showed a satisfactory fit to
To examine whether BI and SC made additional contribu- the data, 2SB (19, N=3336)=37.82, p<0.01, CFI=0.999,
tions to the prediction of NSSI above and beyond those of NFI = 0.998, NNFI = 0.998, RMSEA = 0.017 (90 % CI:
BPD features and NE, we conducted a hierarchical multiple 0.0090.025). Thus, the weak factorial invariance was
regression analysis. The dependent variable was Wave 3 NSSI established for BPD features across the three waves of assess-
total frequency. Independent variables included Wave 1 NSSI ment. Standardized factor loadings and factor correlations are
frequency at Step 1, Wave 1 BPD features (i.e. ER, UR, and presented in Fig. 1.
USIM) and NE (i.e. DASS-D, DASS-A, and DASS-S) at Step
2, and Wave 1 BI and SC at Step 3. 1
Detailed information regarding the percentages of participants engaging
Descriptive and regression analyses were conducted using in each NSSI behaviors with different frequencies could be obtained from
SPSS 20.0. The longitudinal moderated mediation models the correspondence author upon request.
J Abnorm Child Psychol

UR1 DASS-D1
.77 .85
ER1 DASS-A1
.82 Wave 1 NE1 .88
Wave 1 BPD1
.91
.87 DASS-S1
USIM1

.61
.62
DASS-D2
UR2 .86
.76 .52 DASS-A2
.56 Wave 2 NE2 .89
ER2
Wave 2 BPD2 .82 .92
DASS-S2
.85
USIM2
.63
.65
DASS-D3
UR3 .87
.78 DASS-A3
Wave 3 NE3 .90
ER3 .89
Wave 3 BPD3 .83 DASS-S3
.87
USIM3 Fig. 2 Standardized factor loadings and factor correlations for the weak
factorial invariance model for negative emotions. N=5,423. NE negative
Fig. 1 Standardized factor loadings and factor correlations for the weak emotions; DASS-D the depression subscale for the Depression Anxiety
factorial invariance model for BPD features. N=5,423. BPD borderline Stress Scale; DASS-A the anxiety subscale for the depression anxiety
personality disorder features; UR unstable relationship; ER emotional stress scale; DASS-S the stress (tension) subscale for the depression
reactivity; USIM unstable self-image. All factor loadings and factor anxiety stress scale. All factor loadings and factor correlations are signif-
correlations are significant at 0.001 level icant at 0.001 level

NE Data for the indicators of NE also violated the assumption


of multivariate normality, Mardias coefficient = 60.82, regulation strategy. The whole model accounted for 48.3,
p<0.01. Satorra Bentler modification method was thus used 40.4 and 36.0 % of the total variance in Wave 3 BPD features,
again. The unconstrained model for the latent construct of NE NE, and NSSI, respectively. Standardized path coefficients for
demonstrated an excellent fit, 2SB (15, N=3288)=14.71, the full longitudinal mediation model are presented in Fig. 3.
p>0.05, CFI=1.000, NFI=0.999, NNFI=1.000, RMSEA=
0.000 (90 % CI: 0.0000.016). The loading invariance model The Moderated Mediation Models
also showed a good fit to the data, 2SB (15, N=3288)=55.02,
p<0.001, CFI=0.997, NFI=0.996, NNFI=0.995, RMSEA= BI as a Moderator We tested the moderating effect of BI on
0.024 (90 % CI: 0.0170.032). Therefore, the weak factorial the longitudinal mediation model using the residual centering
invariance was also established for negative emotions across approach as described in the data analysis section. The
the three waves of assessment. Standardized factor loadings resulting model had a good fit to the data, 2SB (231, N=
and factor correlations for this model are presented in Fig. 2. 2994)=1162.20, p<0.001, CFI=0.955, NFI=0.944, NNFI=
0.943, RMSEA=0.036 (90 % CI: 0.0340.038). The product
The Full Longitudinal Mediation Model latent variable BI Wave 1 BPD was significant in predicting
Wave 2 NE (B=0.28, SE=0.11, p<0.01), BI Wave 1 NE
After establishing factorial invariance for both the latent con-
structs of BPD features and NE, we proceeded to test the full Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3
hypothesized longitudinal mediation model of NSSI with the
whole sample. Satorra Bentler modification method was used BPD1 .67 BPD2 .70 BPD3
again for adjusting multivariate non-normality (Mardias co- .67 .26 .26
efficient=398.43, p<0.01). This full mediation model dem-
.30 NE1 .43 NE2 .50 NE3
onstrated an acceptable fit to the data, 2SB (169, N=3001)=
.33 .13 .06
1599.89, p<0.001, CFI=0.935, NFI=0.928, NNFI=0.919,
RMSEA=0.053 (90 % CI: 0.0510.055). This suggests that NSSI1 .44 NSSI2 .59 NSSI3
BPD features characterized by emotional reactivity, unstable
Fig. 3 Standardized path coefficients for the full longitudinal mediation
relationship and unstable sense of self-image may lead to model for NSSI. N=5,423. BPD borderline personality disorder features;
negative emotions, i.e., depression, anxiety and tension, which NE negative emotions; NSSI non suicidal self-injury. All coefficients are
may then lead to the performance of NSSI as an emotion significant at 0.01 level
J Abnorm Child Psychol

was significant in predicting Wave 2 NSSI (B=0.14, SE= Table 1 Summary of the hierarchical multiple regression analysis
predicting wave 3 NSSI frequency by wave 1 bpd features, negative
0.04, p<0.001), BI Wave 2 NE was significant in predicting
emotions, behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism after controlling for
Wave 3 NSSI (B=0.37, SE=0.17, p<0.01), BI Wave 1 NSSI wave 1 nssi frequency (N=3,167)
was significant in predicting Wave 2 NSSI (B=0.06, SE=
0.01, p<0.001), and BI Wave 2 NSSI was significant in Predictor B SE t R2
predicting Wave 3 NSSI (B=0.09, SE=0.01, p<0.001). These Step 1 0.243
results suggested that BI significantly moderated the longitu- Wave 1 NSSI 0.47 0.02 0.49 21.71*
dinal mediation model, such that a higher level of BI made
Step 2 0.005
adolescents more vulnerable to NE and subsequent NSSI.
Wave 1 UR 0.02 0.01 0.06 1.78
Wave 1 ER 0.00 0.00 0.02 0.47
SC as a Moderator The same residual centering approach was
Wave 1 USIM -0.00 0.01 -0.01 0.24
used in testing the moderating effect of SC. The resulting
Wave 1 DASS-D 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.31
model had a good fit to the data, 2SB (231, N=2990)=
Wave 1 DASS-A 0.01 0.01 0.04 0.98
1099.82, p<0.001, CFI=0.947, NFI=0.934, NNFI=0.933,
Wave 1 DASS-S -0.01 0.01 -0.05 1.01
RMSEA=0.051 (90 % CI: 0.0480.054). The product latent
Step 3 0.014
variable SCWave 2 BPD was significant in predicting Wave *
Wave 1 BI 0.02 0.01 0.14 4.70
3 NE (B=0.44, SE=0.13, p<0.01), SCWave 1 NE was
Wave 1 SC 0.00 0.00 0.04 1.20
significant in predicting Wave 2 NSSI (B=0.15, SE=0.04,
Total R2 =.258
p<0.001), SC Wave 2 NE was significant in predicting
Wave 3 NSSI (B=0.50, SE=0.15, p<0.001), SCWave 1 UR, unstable relationship; ER, emotional reactivity; USIM, unstable self-
NSSI was significant in predicting Wave 2 NSSI (B=0.15, image. DASS-D, the depression subscale for the depression anxiety stress
SE=0.03, p<0.001), and SCWave 2 NSSI was significant in scale; DASS-A, the anxiety subscale for the depression anxiety stress
scale; DASS-S, the stress (tension) subscale for the depression anxiety
predicting Wave 3 NSSI (B=0.23, SE=0.02, p<0.001). These
stress scale
results also suggested that SC exerted a significant moderating *
p<0.001
effect on the longitudinal mediation model.

Examining the Additional Predictive Utility of Behavioral a small effect size, behavioral impulsivity also made an addi-
Impulsivity and Self-Criticism for NSSI tional contribution to future engagement in NSSI above and
beyond those of BPD features, negative emotions, and previ-
As described in the data analysis section, we conducted a ous NSSI. This study advances our knowledge about the
hierarchical multiple regression analysis to examine the addi- nature of NSSI as an impulsive emotion regulation strategy.
tional predictive utility of BI and SC for NSSI. Results are Below, we discuss the main findings of the present study.
summarized in Table 1. The full model with all Wave 1 First, we found in this study that possessing a high level of
predictors were significant in predicting Wave 3 NSSI fre- BPD features, i.e. emotional reactivity, unstable relationship
quency, F(9, 1,457)=57.53, p<0.001. Step 1 with Wave 1 and unstable self-image significantly increased the likelihood
NSSI frequency was significant, F(1, 1,465) = 471.34, of engaging in NSSI as an emotion regulation strategy over
p<0.001. After controlling for Wave 1 NSSI frequency, all time. BPD features, according to Nock (2009), are regarded as
Wave 1 BPD features and NE variables were non-significant general predisposing factors for NSSI. These factors are those
at Step 2, F(6, 1,459)=1.66, p>0.05. Step 3 with Wave 1 BI that influence both the type and the amount of resources that
and SC were significant, F(2, 1,457)=13.50, p<0.001. the individual can elicit to cope with stress and/or distress. The
However, only BI acted as a significant individual predictor significant paths from BPD features to negative emotions are
at Step 3. in line with previous research revealing the associations be-
tween BPD and depression, anxiety, and tension (e.g., Gratz
et al. 2008; Levy et al. 2007; Stiglmayr et al. 2008). Apart
from its induction of negative emotions, BPD features may
Discussion also link with NSSI through the compromise of ones coping
ability. For example, since increased emotional reactivity con-
Using a longitudinal design, this study examined the roles of tributes to intense and prolonged negative emotional experi-
general predisposing factors, precipitating factors, and NSSI- ence, adolescents with high emotional reactivity usually could
specific vulnerability factors in the engagement in NSSI. not tolerate the extreme emotional distress and prefer emotion-
Findings suggest that negative emotions mediate the relation focused coping strategies (e.g., binge eating and NSSI) to
of BPD features to NSSI, and behavioral impulsivity and self- problem-focused ones, because the former ones could quickly
criticism moderated the mediating relationship. Although with distract them from aversive experience. Additionally, having
J Abnorm Child Psychol

unstable relationship and unstable self-image restricts ones impulse to hurt oneself. However, some previous studies
opportunity to learn effective interpersonal and self-regulation failed to find a significant association between impulsivity
skills, which may subsequently increase the likelihood that and NSSI among young adults (e.g., Chapman et al. 2009;
adolescents use extreme and maladaptive coping behaviors. Glenn and Klonsky 2011). These null findings are consistent
These possibilities may be examined in future studies. with the small effect size of the prospective association be-
Another significant contribution of the present study is that tween impulsivity and NSSI in the present study. Despite
we tested the role of two NSSI-specific vulnerability factors: these non-significant findings in some of the past studies
behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism deriving from the and the small effect size in this study, we believe that impul-
pragmatic and the self-punishment hypotheses in Nocks sivity should still be considered as an important risk factor for
(2009) model respectively, in the engagement in NSSI. Both NSSI, at least among community adolescents. This may be
of the variables significantly moderated the longitudinal me- due to four possible reasons. First, the null findings in previ-
diation model originating from BPD features to negative ous studies were revealed among young adults and the nature
emotions and to NSSI. Adolescents with a high level of of NSSI may differ across adolescents and adults. Second, the
behavioral impulsivity were more likely to engage in NSSI significant prospective relation of impulsivity to NSSI in this
than those with a low level of behavioral impulsivity. study was emerged above and beyond other NSSI risk factors.
Furthermore, behavioral impulsivity made an additional con- If we consider only the bivariate association between impul-
tribution to the prediction of future NSSI above and beyond sivity and future NSSI, the effect would be larger. Third, NSSI
the effects of previous NSSI, BPD features, and negative may have significant consequences (e.g., NSSI may be asso-
emotions. This result echoes previous findings showing that ciated with future suicide attempts; Guan et al. 2012) and thus
youths who self-injured scored significantly higher than non- even risk factors for NSSI with a small effect size should be
injurers on self-reported measures of impulsivity (Claes et al. taken seriously. Fourth, the impact of impulsivity on NSSI
2001; Glenn and Klonsky 2010; Herpertz et al. 1997; Janis may have a cumulative effect. This effect may be more obvi-
and Nock 2009). This maybe because NSSI is easily execut- ous over an extensive period. Spanning only a 1-year interval,
able and can be performed effortlessly in virtually any context the present study may not be able to reveal a large effect size.
as compared to other coping behaviors (Nock 2009). Given these considerations, we encourage future studies ex-
Moreover, since NSSI might cause severe physical pain and amining risk factors for NSSI to still regard impulsivity as a
even permanent scar, adolescents with low impulsivity may potential candidate. Moreover, interventions of NSSI may
prefer other less destructive behaviors in dealing with distress. target not only teaching adaptive emotion regulation skills,
Result regarding the moderating effect of self-criticism is but also increasing self-injurers distress tolerance and im-
also in line with previous research showing that low self- pulse control abilities.
esteem and a sense of self-hatred are related to NSSI in
adolescents (Glassman et al. 2007; Lundh et al. 2007), and Strengths and Limitations
self-hatred and anger towards the self are common
thoughts/feelings precipitating NSSI (Nock et al. 2009). In The present study has several strengths. First and foremost, we
the face of emotional distress and/or stressful events, adoles- examined the roles of different types of risk factors in adoles-
cents who have developed a more self-critical cognitive style cents engagement in NSSI. This advances our understanding
may be more likely to engage in NSSI for self-punishment. on why adolescents engage in NSSI. In particular, we revealed
This study also examined the additional predictive utility of the unique and additional contribution of behavioral impul-
behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism to future NSSI above sivity to NSSI above and beyond other risk factors. This may
and beyond the effects of previous NSSI, BPD features and shed light on the impulsive nature of NSSI among adoles-
negative emotions. Only behavioral impulsivity emerged as a cents. Other strengths include the longitudinal design, the
significant predictor, although the effect size may be small. large sample size, and the relatively low attrition rate.
This finding highlights the impulsive nature of NSSI and may Despite these strengths, results of this study should also be
inform the clinical conceptualization of this behavior. Many interpreted with some limitations in mind. First, despite the
previous researchers have put much emphasis on the large sample size, all participants in this study were Chinese
impulsivity facet of NSSI. For example, Lacey and Evans high school students. It is unclear to what extent one can
(1986) argued that NSSI, together with substance abuse, binge generalize our findings to adolescents in other countries or
eating, etc., are all behaviors of impulse dyscontrol. They cultures, to populations of older ages, or to clinical samples.
suggested individuals repetitively engaging in these behaviors However, the consistency between our findings and those
form a unitary multi-impulsive personality group. obtained from western samples reviewed above may give
Muehlenkamp (2005) also argued for a separate deliberate some confidence to the generalizability of our results.
self-injury syndrome, and one of the proposed diagnostic Second, all measures used in this study were self-report ques-
criteria of this syndrome pertains to the inability to resist the tionnaires. Shared method variance may thus influence the
J Abnorm Child Psychol

results of the associations among variables. Third, this study among patients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 23(2), 122140. doi:10.1521/pedi.2009.23.
collected data for three times, with a 6-month interval between
2.122.
each time. Nevertheless, to study the effects of precipitating Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., & Vertommen, H. (2001). Self-injurious
factors on NSSI, future studies may benefit from using eco- behaviors in eating-disordered patients. Eating Behaviors, 2(3),
logical momentary assessment (EMA) method, asking partic- 263272. doi:10.1016/S1471-0153(01)00033-2.
De Leo, D., & Heller, T. S. (2004). Who are the kids who self-harm? An
ipants to report the antecedents, consequences, and affects
Australian self-report school survey. Medical Journal of Australia,
associated with each NSSI episode immediately after the 181(3), 140144.
episode. Together with the information about the general Glassman, L. H., Weierich, M. R., Hooley, J. M., Deliberto, T. L., &
vulnerability factors, such as adolescents BPD features, and Nock, M. K. (2007). Child maltreatment, non-suicidal self-injury,
and the mediating role of self-criticism. Behaviour Research and
the NSSI-specific vulnerability factors, such as their levels of
Therapy, 45(10), 24832490. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.04.002.
behavioral impulsivity and self-criticism, the EMA method Glenn, C. R., & Klonsky, E. D. (2010). A multimethod analysis of
may result in more thorough understanding on how the gen- impulsivity in nonsuicidal self-injury. Personality Disorders:
eral vulnerability factors influence adolescents reactivity to Theory, Research, and Treatment, 1(1), 6775. doi:10.1037/
a0017427.
stress, as well as how these factors work together to influence
Glenn, C. R., & Klonsky, E. D. (2011). Prospective prediction of
adolescents final decision of engaging in NSSI. nonsuicidal self-injury: a 1-year longitudinal study in young adults.
Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 751762. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.04.005.
Gratz, K. L., Tull, M. T., & Gunderson, J. G. (2008). Preliminary data on
Acknowledgments This research was funded in part by National Nat- the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and borderline person-
ural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 31300874) and South China ality disorder: the role of experiential avoidance. Journal of
Normal University Young Teacher Research Cultivation Grant Psychiatric Research, 42, 550559. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.
(2012KJ013) awarded to Dr. Jianing You. This study was also supported 05.011.
by Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Cognitive Science of Guang- Guan, K., Fox, K. R., & Prinstein, M. J. (2012). Nonsuicidal self-injury as
dong Province and Research Center for Crisis Intervention and Psycho- a time-invariant predictor of adolescent suicide ideation and at-
logical Service of Guangdong Province, South China Normal University. tempts in a diverse community sample. Journal of Consulting and
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, Clinical Psychology, 80(5), 842849. doi:10.1037/a0029429.
decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Hawton, K., Bale, L., Casey, D., Shepherd, A., Simkin, S., & Harriss, L.
(2006). Monitoring deliberate self-harm presentations to general
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of hospitals. Crisis: The Journal of Crisis Intervention and Suicide
interest. Prevention, 27(4), 157163. doi:10.1027/0227-5910.27.4.157.
Herpertz, S., Sass, H., & Favazza, A. (1997). Impulsivity in self-
mutilative behavior: psychometric and biological findings. Journal
of Psychiatric Research, 31(4), 451465.
References Hilt, L. M., Cha, C. B., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2008a). Nonsuicidal self-
injury in young adolescent girls: moderators of the distress-function
relationship. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(1),
Alsaka, F., & Olweus, D. (1986). Assessment of global negative self- 6371. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.76.1.63.
evaluations and perceived stability of self in Norwegian preadoles- Hilt, L. M., Nock, M. K., Lloyd-Richardson, E. E., & Prinstein, M. J.
cents and adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescents, 6, 269278. (2008b). Longitudinal study of nonsuicidal self-injury among young
doi:10.1177/0272431686063005. adolescents: rates, correlates, and preliminary test of an interpersonal
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical model. Journal of Early Adolescence, 28(3), 455469. doi:10.1177/
manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: American 0272431608316604.
Psychiatric Publishing. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in
Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new al-
(1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions ternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155. doi:10.1080/
of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a 10705519909540118.
community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10, 176181. Jacobson, C. M., & Gould, M. (2007). The epidemiology and phenom-
Association, A. P. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental enology of non-suicidal self-injurious behavior among adolescents:
disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington: Author. a critical review of the literature. Archives of Suicide Research,
Baral, I., Kora, K., Yuksel, S., & Sezgin, U. (1998). Self-mutilating 11(2), 129147. doi:10.1080/13811110701247602.
behavior of sexually abused female adults in Turkey. Journal of Janis, I. B., & Nock, M. K. (2009). Are self-injurers impulsive? Results
Interpersonal Violence, 13(4), 427437. from two behavioral laboratory studies. Psychiatry Research,
Bentler, P. (2005). EQS 6.1. Encino: Multivariate software. 169(3), 261267. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.041.
Blatt, S. J., D'Afflitti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976). The depressive Klonsky, E. D. (2007). The functions of deliberate self-injury: a review of
experiences quesionnaire. New Haven: Yale University. the evidence. Clinical Psychology Review, 27(2), 226239. doi:10.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New 1016/j.cpr.2006.08.002.
York: Wiley. Lacey, J. H., & Evans, C. D. H. (1986). The impulsivist: a multi-
Chapman, A. L., Specht, M. W., & Cellucci, T. (2005). Borderline impulsive personality disorder. British Journal of Addiction, 81,
personality disorder and deliberate self-harm: does experiential 641649. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1986.tb00382.x.
avoidance play a role? Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, Laye-Gindhu, A., & Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2005). Nonsuicidal self-
35(4), 388399. harm among community adolescents: understanding the whats
Chapman, A. L., Derbidge, C. M., Cooney, E., Hong, P. Y., & Linehan, and whys of self-harm. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34,
M. M. (2009). Temperament as a prospective predictor of self-injury 447457. doi:10.1007/s10964-005-7262-z.
J Abnorm Child Psychol

Leung, S., & Leung, F. (2009). Construct validity and prevalence rate of to self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 39(2),
borderline personality disorder among Chinese adolescents. 107116.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 23, 494531. doi:10.1521/pedi. Nock, M. K., Prinstein, M. J., & Sterba, S. K. (2009). Revealing the form
2009.23.5.494. and function of self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: a real-time
Levy, K. N., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (2007). Depressive ecological assessment study among adolescents and young adults.
experiences in inpatients with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(4), 816827. doi:10.1037/
Psychiatric Quarterly, 78, 129143. doi:10.1007/s11126-006- a0016948.
9033-8. Patton, G. C., Harris, R., Carlin, J. B., Hibbert, M. E., Coffey, C.,
Little, T. D., Bovaird, J. A., & Widaman, K. F. (2006). On the merits of Schwartz, M., & Bowes, G. (1997). Adolescent suicidal behaviours:
orthogonalizing powered and product terms: implications for model- a population-based study of risk. Psychological Medicine, 27(3),
ing interactions among latent variables. Structural Equation 715724.
Modeling, 13, 497519. doi:10.1207/s15328007sem1304_1. Ross, S., & Heath, N. (2002). A study of the frequency of self-mutilation
Little, T. D., Preacher, K. J., Selig, J. P., & Card, N. A. (2007). New in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of Youth and
developments in latent variable panel analyses of longitudinal data. Adolescence, 31(1), 6777.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 357365. Satorra, A., Chou, C., & Bentler, P. M. (1991). Scaled test statistics and
doi:10.1177/0165025407077757. robust standard errors for nonnormal data in covariance structure
Lundh, L. G., Karim, J., & Quilisch, E. (2007). Deliberate self-harm in analysis: a Monte Carlo study. British Journal of Mathematical and
15-year-old adolescents: a pilot study with a modified version of the Statistical Psychology, 44, 347357.
deliberate self-harm inventory. Scandinavian Journal of Shearer, S. L. (1994). Phenomenology of self-injury among inpatient
Psychology, 48(1), 3341. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00567.x. women with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous
Messer, J. M., & Fremouw, W. J. (2008). A critical review of explanatory and Mental Disease, 182(9), 524526.
models for self-mutilating behaviors in adolescents. Clinical Steinmetza, H., Davidovb, E., & Schmidt, P. (2011). Three approaches to
Psychology Review, 28(1), 162178. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.04.006. estimate latent interaction effects: intention and perceived behavior-
Muehlenkamp, J. J. (2005). Self-injurious behavior as a separate clinical al control in the theory of planned behavior. Methodological
syndrome. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 75, 324333. doi: Innovations Online, 6, 95110. doi:10.4256/mio.2010.0030.
10.1037/0002-9432.75.2.324. Stiglmayr, C. E., Bischkopf, J., Albrecht, V., Porzig, N., Scheuer, S.,
Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Gutierrez, P. M. (2004). An investigation of Lammers, C. H., & Auckenthaler, A. (2008). The experience of
differences between self-injurious behavior and suicide attempts in tension in patients with borderline personality disorder compared to
a sample of adolescents. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, other patient groups and healthy controls. Journal of Social and
34(1), 1223. Clinical Psychology, 27, 425446. doi:10.1521/jscp.2008.27.5.425.
Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Gutierrez, P. M. (2007). Risk for suicide attempts Taouk, M., Lovibond, P. F., & Laube, R. (2001). Psychometric properties
among adolescents who engage in non-suicidal self-injury. Archives of a Chinese version of the short depression anxiety stress scales
o f S u i c i d e R e s e a r c h , 11 ( 1 ) , 6 9 8 2 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 / (DASS21). Sydney: New South Wales Transcultural Mental Health
13811110600992902. Centre, Cumberland Hospital.
Muehlenkamp, J. J., Claes, L., Havertape, L., & Plener, P. (2012). You, J., Leung, F., & Fu, K. (2012a). Exploring the reciprocal relations
International prevalence of adolescent non-suicidal self-injury and between nonsuicidal self-injury, negative emotions and relationship
deliberate self-harm. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental problems in Chinese adolescents: a longitudinal cross-lag study.
Health, 6(10), 19. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 829836. doi:10.
Nock, M. K. (2009). Why do people hurt themselves? New insights into 1007/s10802-011-9597-0.
the nature and functions of self-injury. Current Directions in You, J., Leung, F., Lai, C. M., & Fu, K. (2012b). The associations
Psychological Science, 18(2), 7883. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721. between non-suicidal self-injury and borderline personality disorder
2009.01613.x. features among Chinese adolescents. Journal of Personality
Nock, M. K., & Favazza, A. R. (2009). Nonsuicidal self-injury: Disorders, 26, 226237. doi:10.1521/pedi.2012.26.2.226.
Definition and classification. In M. K. Nock (Ed.), Understanding Zanarini, M. C., Gunderson, J. G., Frankenburg, F. R., & Chauncey, D. L.
nonsuicidal self-injury: Origins, assessment, and treatment. (1989). The revised diagnostic interview for borderlines: discrimi-
Washington: American Psychological Association. nating BPD from other axis II disorders. Journal of Personality
Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional approach to the Disorders, 3, 1018. doi:10.1521/pedi.1989.3.1.10.
assessment of self-mutilative behavior. Journal of Consulting Zoroglu, S. S., Tuzun, U., Sar, V., Tutkun, H., Savas, H. A., Ozturk,
and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 885890. doi:10.1037/0022- M., & Kora, M. E. (2003). Suicide attempt and self-mutilation
006x.72.5.885. among Turkish high school students in relation with abuse, neglect
Nock, M. K., Joiner, T. E., Gordon, K. H., Lloyd-Richardson, E., & and dissociation. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 57(1),
Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Non-suicidal self-injury among adolescents: 119126.
diagnostic correlates and relation to suicide attempts. Psychiatry Zuroff, D. C., Quinlan, D. M., & Blatt, S. J. (1990). Psychometric
Research, 144(1), 6572. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.010. properties of the depressive experiences questionnaire in a college
Nock, M. K., Wedig, M. M., Holmberg, E. B., & Hooley, J. M. (2008). population. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 6572. doi:10.
The emotion reactivity scale: development, evaluation, and relation 1207/s15327752jpa5501&2_7.

S-ar putea să vă placă și