Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

School Psychology Review,

2012, Volume 41, No. 3, pp. 315-325

Commentary

Connecting Cognitive Theory and Assessment:


Measuring Individual Differences in Reading
Comprehens ion

Paul van den Broek and Christine A. Espin


Leiden University and University of Minnesota

The ability to read and comprehend of existing tools for assessing reading compre-
texts is an essential component of successful hension. In addition, we wish to illustrate
functioning in our world. A substantial more generally how advances in theories
amount of information comes to us through about psychological constructs (in this case,
written means, whether it is through regular reading comprehension) can contribute to the
print, Internet, or other media. Part of this development of assessment tools.
information is for our enjoyment, part of it is
vital for our basic functioningapplication The Integrated Model of Reading
forms and tax forms that need to be read and Comprehension (IMRED): Combining
filled out, instructions for operating a new car, Key Elements of Theoretical Models of
prescription instructions, food labels, and so Reading Comprehension
forth. The importance of reading is reflected in Reading and comprehending written
school settings, both as a primary means of language is a complex and uniquely human
conveying knowledge and as main target of activity. Extensive psychological research
instruction. Accordingly, the assessment of over the past 2 decades has resulted in detailed
children's (and adults') ability to read and and comprehensive theoretical models of read-
comprehend texts receives considerable atten- ing comprehension that allow us to understand
tion, in both school and research settings. what reading comprehension is and how indi-
In this article, we describe the complex viduals differ in their reading comprehension
nature of reading comprehension, review re- abilities. Although these models differ in their
cent insights from cognitive-psychological re- details, they agree to a considerable degree on
search into this complexity, and draw impli- the main components (e.g., Sabatini, Albro &
cations from this research for assessment. Our O'Reilly, 2012). The remainder of this section
purpose is to provide a theoretical foundation presents what we refer to as the Integrated
for the development of new and the adaptation Model of Reading Comprehension (IMREC),

Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Paul van den Broek, Department of Education
and Child Studies, Leiden University, Wassenaarseweg 52, 2333 AK Leiden, The Netherlands; e-mail:
broekpwvanden@fsw.leidenuniv.nl
Copyright 2012 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015

315
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 3

in which the main components of the various the meaning of the text, abstracted from the
models are combined into a single account. By textual information itself and supplemented by
providing a single account, the integrated background knowledge and semantic relations
model addresses a major challenge in translat- constructed by the reader. The interpretation
ing theory to assessmentnamely, that differ- of the text based on information within the text
ent theories can lead to different types of as- and background knowledge is commonly re-
sessments (Messick, 1989). ferred to as the reader's situation model of the
text (Kintsch, 1988a), to distinguish it from a
The Product of Reading Comprehension representation of the text information only (the
textbase) and from a visual-perceptual repre-
In considering reading comprehension sentation of the text (the surface model).
and its assessment, it is useful to distinguish The mental representation of a text cre-
between the product and the process of com- ated by the reader is the basis for subsequent
prehending a text (Pearson & Hamm, 2005; performance such as retelling, question an-
van den Broek, Bohn-Gettler, Kendeou, Carl-, swering, or applying the textual information.
son, & White, 2011). The product of compre- Thus, the coherence of a reader's mental rep-
hension is the mental representation of the resentation of a text is an indicator of his or
textual information in the reader's mind after her comprehension of that text. For example,
he or she has completed reading the text good readers consistently remember text ele-
(Kintsch, 1988a, 2012; Cain & Oakhill, 2012; ments with many connections to other ele-
van den Broek, White, Kendeou, & Carlson, ments better than text elements with few con-
2009). In successful comprehension, this rep- nections. Poor readers show less selective
resentation is coherentthat is, the text ele- memory for this structurally central informa-
ments (events, facts, and so on) are intercon- tion (van den Broek, Helder, & van Leijen-
nected through semantic relations and form an horst, in press). As a second example, system-
integrated whole. Different types of relations atic developmental differences in the ability to
may contribute to this coherence but in most identify various types of relations and to con-
cases referential and causal/logical relations stmct coherent representations of text have
are central. Referential relations establish co- been identified (e.g., Oakhill & Cain, 2011;
herence by capturing the identity of objects, van den Broek et al., 2009).
persons, and so on across text elements (e.g.,
the pronoun she in one sentence refers to a
The Process of Reading Comprehension
particular female character mentioned earlier
in the text), whereas causal and logical rela- The mental representation of a text is the
tions establish coherence by capturing depen- puteme of comprehension processes that take
dencies between text elements (e.g., that the place as the reader proceeds through the text.
action of a character is brought about by his or Success and failure in executing these pro-
her motivation to achieve a particular goal). cesses determine the quality of the outcome,
Many of the relations that contribute to thus consideration of these processes is of
the coherent mental representation of a text prime interest in the diagnosis and remediation
must be inferred; to make these inferences the of reading comprehension problems (Pearson
reader activates relevant background knowl- & Hamm, 2005; Perfetti & Adlof, 2012; van
edge. Together, the elements from the text, the den Broek, 2010).
elements activated from background knowl- Before considering the comprehension
edge, and the interconnections between these processes that take place during reading in
elements form a semantic network that repre- detail, three general observations are relevant.
sents the reader's comprehension of the text First, coherence-building processes during
(Graesser & Clark, 1985; Kintsch & van Dijk, reading reflect a balancing act between the
1978; Trabasso & van den Broek, 1985). This reader's attempt to create coherence on the
representation is an interpreted rendering of one hand, and his or her limited attentional or
316
Connecting Cognitive Theory and Assessment

working memory resources on the other hand. Automatic processes. The automatic
Thus, at any one point in the text, the reader processes occur at every cycle and are not
can only attend to a limited amount of poten- under the control of the reader. The most
tially relevant information and execute only a important of these is spread of activation:
subset of all processes in his or her cognitive The concepts in each new sentence trigger
"toolbox" (Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978; Oakhill activation of other, related concepts from
& Cain, 2011; van den Broek, 2012). It fol- the representation that the reader has con-
lows that if lower level processes such as letter structed during processing of the earlier text
recognition or word decoding are not fluent (O'Brien & Albrecht, 1992) and from the
and thus are consuming precious working- reader's background knowledge (Kintsch,
memory resources, then comprehension pro- 1988b; van den Broek, Young, Tzeng, &
cesses will be compromised (Perfetti, Yang, & Linderholm, 1998/2004). In addition, the
Schmalhofer, 2008; Whitehurst & Lonigan, reader may have information from recently
1998). Second, the comprehension processes processed sentences available in his or her
that take place during reading are partly auto- working memory. Together, information
matic and partly strategic (Rapp & van den from these four sources (current sentence,
Broek, 2005; van den Broek, Rapp, & Kend- both prior text and background knowledge
eou, 2005). Strategic processes must be activated through spread of activation, and
learned and therefore are of primary interest working memory) is available "for free" to
for instruction and intervention. With practice, the reader to use to comprehend the current
these processes can become routinized to the sentence.
point of being virtually automatic in many
situations (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974). As de- Strategic processes. A second set of
scribed below, the execution of strategic pro- processes concerns strategic processes, initi-
cesses depends on the efficiency of the auto- ated by the reader to construct coherence. Al-
matic processes. Third, there is considerable though strategic processes may become more
agreement that reading comprehension is not a and more automated with practice ahd ex-
singular activity but an activity that consists of perience, they demand effort even on the
multiple component processes that are applied part of skilled readers (as evidenced, for ex-
dynamically and in varying combinations ample, by slower reading and/or additional,
throughout the process of reading a particular eye movements). These strategic processes
text (Kintsch 2012; Oakhill & Cain, 2011; include looking back at preceding text for
Perfetti & Adlof, 2012; van den Broek, 2012; relevant information, searching one's repre-
van den Broek, Young, Tzeng, & Linderholm, sentation of earlier text, searching one's back-
1998/2004). This observation is supported by ground knowledge to explain the informa-
research that found that multiple brain regions tion in the current sentence, and so on (cf.
are involved in reading comprehension and van den Broek, 1994, 2010). Strategic pro-
that these regions are recruited to various de- cesses also include specific processes that
grees at different points in the text (e.g., Ferstl, the reader executes in response to text-struc-
2007; Ferstl, Neumann, Bogler, & von Cra- tural features such as text signals (italics,
mon, 2008; Perfetti & Frishkoff, 2008; Prat, headers; see Lemari, Lorch, Eyrolle, & Vir-
Mason, & Just, 2011). bel, 2008), integrative processes elicited by
Turning to the details of reading com- paragraph or section structures (e.g., Lorch,
prehension processes, the general consensus is Lemari, & Grant, 2011; Lorch & Lorch,
that the processing of a text is cyclical, with 1995), and so forth. Individuals differ in the
each new reading input (e.g., sentence) pro- toolbox of strategies at their disposal, both
viding new information that triggers a new in terms of which strategies they have mas-
combination of automatic and, possibly, stra- tered and of how proficient they are in ap-
tegic processes. plying those strategies.

317
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 3

Balancing automatic and strategic described, each new input automatically trig-
processes: Standards of coherence. gers activation of semantically related infor-
Whether a sentence triggers strategic pro- mation from earlier parts of the text and from
cesses and, if so, what combination of pro- background knowledge. Furthermore, a small
cesses is triggered depends on the information buffer of immediately preceding information
available to the reader via automatie processes is available in working memory. The informa-
at that sentence and on the reader's standards tion that becomes available through spread of
of coherence in reading that particular text. activation depends on the depth and breadth of
Standards of eoherenee refer to the type and the reader' background knowledge (McNamara
degree of semantic coherence that the reader & Kintsch, 1996; McNamara, Kintsch,
aims to maintain while reading (van den Broek Songer, & Kintsch, 1996; Schneider, Krkel,
et al., 2011; van den Broek, Risden, & Huse- & Weinert, 1989; Wolfe, 2005) and on the
bye-Hartman, 1995). These standards vary quality of the representation that the reader has
among readers and among text types (Linder- constructed of the text to that point (which
holm & van den Broek, 2002; Narvaez, van itself is the outcome of the automatic and
den Broek, Barron-Ruiz, 1999; Wolfe, 2005), strategic processes applied to the preceding
but they ean also vary for a particular reader text, reflecting the eyclical nature of text pro-
depending on, for example, reading instruc- cessing; van den Broek et al., 1998/2004).
tions and reading goals (e.g., van den Broek, Differences also occur in what information is
Lorch, Linderholm, & Gustafson, 2001) or as available from the immediately preceding
a result of general cognitive functioning (e.g., textfor example, as a function of the read-
fatigue, effects of distractors). If the automatic er's working memory capacity (Linderholm &
processes meet the reader's standards, then van den Broek, 2002) and the extent to which
few or no additional strategic processes will this capacity is consumed by lower level pro-
take plaee. Standards may be met on the basis cessing such as lexical decoding (e.g., Perfetti
of automatic processes alone, for example, & Hart, 2002).
when the text at hand is simple or when the Individual and developmental differ-
reader has little motivation or reason to engage ences also emerge as a function of the readers'
in deep processing. If the automatic processes ability to employ strategic processes during
do not meet the reader's standards, then the reading of a text. For example, readers differ
reader may engage in strategic processes. in the standards of coherence that they employ
To summarize which, if any, strategic and in their ability to adjust their standards to
processes occur at each particular point in the different reading goals (van den Broek et al.,
reading of a text depends on the information 2011; Kendeou, Bohn-Gettler, & Fulton,
available from the current sentence, on the 2011; McCrudden, Magliano, & Schraw,
automatic processes that take place while 2010; McCrudden, Schraw, & Kambe, 2005).
reading that sentence, on the reader's stan- These standards are a major factor in deter-
dards of coherence, and on the quality of his or mining whether a reader will engage in stra-
her toolbox of strategies. Thus, as a reader tegic processing beyond the processing that
proceeds through a text he or she engages in a takes place automatically. As a second exam-
dynamically changing mix of automatic and ple, there are substantial differences between
strategic processes. individual readers and between different age
groups in the range and efficiency of the strat-
Individual and Developmental egies available for comprehension (e.g., Oak-
Differences hill & Cain, 2011; van den Broek et al., 2009;
van den Broek et al., in press). These strate-
Significant individual and developmen- gies are in part general to comprehension and
tal differences exist in the execution of these reasoning in any domain, in part specific to
processes and, hence, in the quality of the reading and even to specific text genres
resulting mental representation of the text. As (Oakhill & Cain, 2011). The effectiveness of
318
Connecting Cognitive Theory and Assessment

all of these strategies is strongly influenced by Snowling, 1997; Keenan, 2012). The ques-
practice and instruction. Thus, facility in exe- tions originate from research that reveals,
cuting inferential processes and strategies, among other things, a lack of consistency in
knowledge about text genres and about text the skill sets sampled by various reading com-
schmas, knowledge and application of stan- prehension tests (e.g., some tests rely more on
dards of coherence, and the ability to allocate word-level skills than others), the fact that
attention to stmcturally central aspects of the comprehension questions in these tests often
text are among the major factors that deter- can be answered without reading the text, and
mine a reader's ability to engage in strategic a lack of consistency in the rank ordering of
processes, and that allow him or her to identify students' scores among different tests (e.g..
important relations while reading. Cutting & Scarborough, 2006; Keenan &
Together, individual and developmen- Betjemann, 2006; Keenan, Betjemann, &
tal differences in automatic and strategic Olson, 2008; Keenan & Meenan, in press;
processing result in the identification of dif- Nation & Snowling, 1997).
ferent sets of relations during reading and, Given recent increases in our under-
as a consequence, in different representa- standing of reading comprehension and its
tions. In so doing, they form the basis for components, and the questions raised about
systematic variations in the quality and type existing measurement tools, it is timely to
of comprehension. consider whether theoretical advances could
be used to improve existing assessments or to
Toward Assessment develop new assessments. However, the trans-
The insights into the process and prod- lation of theory to assessment may not be as
uct of reading comprehension, as summarized straightforward as it seems.
in the IMREC, may inform the development
of new tests or the adaptations of existing tests Connecting Theory and Assessment
for assessing reading comprehension. Specif-
The question of how theory and assess-
ically, the IMREC model emphasizes the mul-
ment interrelate is not a new one. In 1955,
tidimensionality of reading comprehension
Cronbach and Meehl discussed the relation
and provides a framework for more precisely
between theory and assessment in their formu-
capturing individual differences and tracking
lation of the concept of construct validity.
development in reading comprehension skills.
Construct validity was presented as a method
The IMREC model (and theoretical models in
for validating assessments of constructs for
general) lay out components skills and pro-
which no operational definition existed, for
cesses that contribute to reading comprehen-
example, intelligence. The then existing ap-
sion. Such models can potentially lead to im-
provements in the diagnostic power of tests proaches for validationpredictive, concur-
and hence inform intervention and instruction. rent, and content validitywere insufficient
Furthermore, as interventions increasingly are because no clear criteria or definable content
aimed at fostering deep comprehension, it be- existed for such constructs. In Cronbach and
comes important to have instruments with Meehl's (1955) formulation, construct valid-
which that comprehension can be measured ity, and theory development were nearly syn-
and with which the effectiveness of the inter- onymous. Building an assessment would be
ventions can be evaluated (Kintsch, 2012). building a theory:
The assessment of reading comprehen- We do notfirst"prove" the theory, and then
sion has received considerable attention in re- validate the test, nor conversely. In any prob-
cent years, as reading comprehension re- able inductive type of inference from a pat-
tern of observations, we examine the relation
searchers have raised questions about existing between the total network of theory and ob-
methods for assessing reading comprehension servations. The system involves propositions
(e.g.. Cutting & Scarborough, 2006; Nation & relating test to construct, construct to other
319
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 3

constructs, andfinallyrelating some of these detail the proposed interpretations and uses of
constructs to observables, (p. 294) test scores, and then provide evidence support-
If we were to adopt Cronbach and ing the plausibility of such interpretations and
Meehl's (1955) conceptualization of construct uses. The approach is also reflected in Rupp's
validity, the building of a well-developed, (2012) and Mislevy & Sabatini's (2012) de-
well-supported theory would be the building scriptions of building reading comprehension
of a well-developed, well-supported assess- assessments. In the concluding sections, we
ment. Thus, the task of creating a valid assess- outline examples of hypotheses that might be
ment for a given construct would require only generated from the IMREC model for the de-
that the theory be translated into a reasonable velopment of new or adapted reading compre-
set of measurement tasks. Specific to our dis- hension assessments.
cussion here, we might then argue that, be-
cause theories of reading comprehension are Implications of Cognitive Theory for
fairly well developed and well supported, all Assessing Reading Comprehension
that remains is to translate the information
The conceptual framework of inference
from these theories into a set of measurement
making and coherence building during reading
tasks, and our work on the improvement of
summarized in the IMREC model has poten-
assessments for reading comprehension is
tial implications for the design of assessment
complete. However, such a direct translation
tools. In this section, we elaborate on several
would likely not be effective and would not
implications that appear particularly promis-
reflect more recent theories of validity.
ing and worth testing.
In recent theories of validity, construct
A general implication of the model is
validity is seen as a unifying factor in validi-
that reading comprehension is a multidimen-
tythat is to say, all validity is construct
sional, multicomponential construct (Sabatini
validity (Messick, 1989, 1994, 1995). In this
et al., 2012). In comprehension, multiple pro-
conceptualization, validity is established via
cesses come together in combinations that
the generation of hypotheses about the mean-
shift constantly during reading of the text as a
ing of test scores for particular uses. These
function of properties of the text and the goals
hypotheses are then examined empirically.
and skills of the text and reader. The various
Messick (1989) describes validity as "an inte-
components of reading comprehension are
grated evaluative judgment of the degree to
likely to interact. They may support, compete
which empirical evidence and theoretical ra-
with, or compensate each other. As a conse-
tionales support the adequacy and appropriate-
quence of its multidimensional and multicom-
ness of inferences and actions based on test
ponential character, the development of a sin-
scores or other modes of assessment" (p. 13).
gle, unidimensional indicator of reading com-
It is not tests or measures, then, that are vali-
prehension that would be apphcable across all
dated, but the interpretation of the scores gen-
testing purposes is unlikely and potentially
erated by those tests or measures for a partic-
misleading. Decisions about test development
ular use that are validated.
should be made with the complex nature of
This view of validity emphasizes that
reading comprehension and the specific pur-
theories lead to hypotheses about the improve-
pose for testing in mind.
ment of existing measures or the development
of new measures. Such hypotheses must then Screening
be empirically tested; that is, studies must be
conducted to examine whether the scores gen- One important purpose for testing is
erated from these new or revised tests improve screening, which involves comparing individ-
our ability to make the decisions we wish to uals on their overall reading comprehension
make. This approach is captured in Kane's ability or to determine whether individuals
(1992, 2001, 2011) argument-based approach surpass or fall below a benchmark. Sensitivity
to validation, in which researchers must first to Structural Centrality (SSC) could be a guid-
320
Connecting Cognitive Theory and Assessment

ing principle to develop an overall test that ness of instructional programs for those stu-
reflects the complex nature of reading compre- dents (Deno, 1985). One issue of concern in
hension processes and the resulting represen- CBM progress monitoring is the unreliability
tations. SSC refers to the extent to which a associated with growth rates generated by
reader processes and represents information CBM measures (e.g., Christ, 2006; Christ &
that is central to the semantic structure of the Ardoin, 2009a, 2009b; Hintz & Christ, 2004;
text (van den Broek et al., in press). Tolar, Barth, Francis, Fletcher, Stuebing, &
SSC can be measured after the reader Vaughn, 2012; Wayman, Wallace, Wiley,
has finished reading a text (off-line) as well as Ticha, & Espin, 2007). The "bounce" or
during reading (online). With respect to off- amount of error associated with these growth
line measurement, one possible indicator of rates is of major concern if one wishes to
SSC is the degree to which a reader selectively reliably and validly track students' progress
recalls or summarizes elements with many over time. A likely contributor to unstable
connections. Selectivity can be expressed, for growth rates is the difficulty in creating paral-
example, by the correlation between the ele- lel forms of the reading measures. For a stu-
ments' number of connections and frequency dent, scores may vary from passage to pas-
of their being recalled/summarized by the sage. Various methods have been proposed for
reader or by the ratio of recall/summary of the improvihg form equivalence, and proposals
highly connected versus less connected ele- have been made for methods that equate
ments. Such measures have been found to scores "after the fact" (e.g., Ardoin, Suido,
capture individual and developmental differ- Witt, Aldrich, & McDonald, 2005; Poney,
ences (Rapp et al., 2007; McMaster et al., Skinner, & Axtell, 2005; Tolar et al., 2012;
2012; for review, see van den Broek et al., in Wayman et al., 2007). The presented models
press). Other off-line measures of comprehen- of reading comprehension could be used to
sion, such as question answering, likewise can create parallel texts that are similar in form
be adapted to gauge SSC. and relational complexity. Such an approach
With respect to online measures, one might serve to improve the equivalence of
possible indicator is the degree of selective passages within students.
attention during reading to text elements that
are central in the text's structure. Examples Identifying Strengths and Weaknesses
are the ratio of reading times for central com-
pared to peripheral elements, the ratio of fre- A third purpose for assessment is to
quency and duration of eye fixations on central obtain a profile of an individual's strength and
elements relative to peripheral elements, or weaknesses in the component skills and pro-
focus on central elements in think-aloud tasks cesses that contribute to his or her reading
(van den Broek et al., in press). Of course, comprehension. Such profiles may be used, for
such online measures may be difficult to im- example, to diagnose the source of a reader's
plement in practical settings, such as the class- comprehension problems and to guide instruc-
room, but they can be used in educational tional interventions. In preceding sections, we
research studies. have described several skills and processes
that are major factors in determining individ-
Monitoring Progress ual and developmental differences in reading
comprehension. These factors roughly fall into
A second purpose for testing is the mea- three categories (van den Broek, 2012), per-
surement of student progress over time with taining to (a) general cognitive characteristics
approaches such as curriculum-based mea- of the reader, (b) the efficacy and efficiency of
surement (CBM). CBM is an assessment general comprehension-related processes, and
method designed to monitor progress of stu- (c) language and text-specific skills and pro-
dents over time and to help educators make cesses, respectively. Table 1 lists the factors
instructional decisions regarding the effective- discussed in this article, grouped by their pri-

321
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 3

Table 1 their outcomes, the mental representation of


Factors underlying individual and texts. We have sketched a general model of
developmental differences in reading reading comprehension, IMREC, in which
comprehension, grouped by their major components across current theoretical
models are combined. With current theories
primary category.
of assessment in mind, we proposed several
General cognitive reader eharaeteristics ways in which cognitive theories might lead to
Verbal working-memory capacity the generation of empirically testable implica-
Background knowledge relevant to text topic tions for constructing measures of reading
Efficiency/efficacy of comprehension-related comprehension.
processes Most cognitive theories of reading com-
Inferential skills prehension focus on the construction of a co-
Knowledge and application of appropriate herent mental representation of a text. How-
standards of coherence ever, representations are not the end product in
Attention allocation to central information
terms of using texts in our interactions with
Language- and text-related skills and processes
the world. Children may comprehend what
Basic language and reading skills (e.g.,
decoding) they read in a textbook in school, but be un-
Breadth and depth of vocabulary able to apply what they have comprehended.
On-line and off-line sensitivity to structural Likewise, an adult may comprehend the in-
centrality structions of tax forms, but may not be able to
Knowledge about text genres and text schmas take the appropriate actions based on that un-
Text-specific coherence-building and inference- derstanding. Thus, in addition to comprehen-
generation skills sion of texts, it may be useful to also consider
Motivation for reading assessing the range of uses to which readers
can put their representations of text. Cognitive
theories are in their infancy on this topic.
In this commentary, we have concen-
mary category. Note that category boundaries trated on the potential implications of cogni-
are not strict: some factors may fall into mul- tive theories of reading comprehension for
tiple categories. For example, general compre- assessment development. In doing so, we have
hension skills and text-specific comprehension ignored potential implications of assessment
skills are likely to overlap. on theory development. Such implications un-
Based on cognitive models, these factors doubtedly exist. One can imagine, for exam-
are prime candidates for inclusion in a com- ple, that the need to specify a certain process
prehensive test of reading comprehension. It targeted by a test under development (e.g.,
would be worthwhile to explore how the ex- inference making) brings to light weaknesses
perimental paradigms of research on these or imprcisions in the theoretical accounts of
cognitive processes could be adapted to update that process. Similar application-to-theory im-
current tests of reading comprehension or to plications have been observed for computa-
develop new tests. tional simulations of the reading process
Conclusions (Goldman, Golden, & van den Broek, 2007).
We hope we have illustrated how theo-
The aim of this commentary is to pro- retical grounding can support the development
vide a theoretical foundation for the develop- of valid reading comprehension tests. Of
ment of new and the adaptation of existing course, many hurdles would need to be over-
tools for assessing reading comprehension. come, including the generation of items, bal-
This foundation is based on recent advances in ancing the need for long texts to accommodate
our understanding of what it means to com- the complexity of reading comprehension with
prehend a text and of what factors influence practical constraints on test length, and so on.
the quality of comprehension processes and Moreover, combining the various models into
322
Connecting Cognitive Theory and Assessment

a single conceptualization such as the IMREC analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehen-
omits important details of the various pro- sion. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 581-593.
Goldman, S. R., Golden, R., SL van den Broek, P. (2007).
cesses, including interactions and boundary Why are computational models of text comprehension
conditions. We strongly recommend consulta- useful? In F. Schmalhofer & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher-
tion of the original, more detailed models level language processes in the brain (pp. 27-52).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
when developing an instrument to measure
Graesser, A. C , & Clark, L. F. (1985). Structures and
any of the indicated skills and processes. The procedures of implicit knowledge. Norwood, NJ:
development of sophisticated item-response Ablex.
techniques is likely to be helpful in elearing Hintze, J. M., & Christ, T. J. (2004). An examination of
variability as a function of passage variance in CBM
these hurdles (e.g.. De Boeck & Wilson, progress monitoring. School Psychology Review, 33,
2004). Together with such techniques, in- 204-217.
sights from cognitive-theoretical models Kane, M. T. (1992). An argument-based approach to
will lead to the next generation of tests of validity. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 527-535.
Kane, M. T. (2001). Concerns in validity theory. Journal
reading comprehension. of Educational Measurement, 38, 319-342.
Kane, M. T. (2011). Validating score interpretations and
uses: Messick Lecture, Language Testing Research
References Colloquium, Cambridge, 2010. Language Testing, 29,
3-17. doi: 10.1177/0265532211417210
Ardoin, S. P., Suido, S. M., Witt, J., Aldrich, S., &
Keenan, J. M. (2012). Measure for measure: Challenges in
McDonald, E. (2005). Aecuracy of readability esti-
assessing reading comprehension. In J. P. Sabatini,
mates' predictions of CBM performance. School Psy-
E. R. Albro, & T. O'Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up:
chology Review, 20, 1-22.
Advances in how we assess reading ability (pp. 77-
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2012). Reading comprehension
87). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Education.
development from seven to fourteen years: Implica-
Keenan, J. M., & Betjemann, R. S. (2006). Comprehend-
tions for assessment. In J. P. Sabatini, E. R. Albro, &
ing the Gray Oral Reading Test without reading it:
T. O'Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up: Advances in how we
assess reading ability (pp. 59-76). Lanham, MD: Why comprehension tests should not include passage-
Rowman & Littlefield Education. independent items. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10,
363-380.
Christ, T. J. (2006). Short-term estimates of growth using
curriculum-based measurement of oral reading fluen- Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., & Olson, R. K. (2008).
cy: Estimates of standard error of slope to construct Reading comprehension tests vary in the skills they
confidence intervals. School Psychology Review, 35, assess: Differential dependence on decoding and oral
128-133. comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 281-
Christ, T. J., & Ardoin, S. P. (2009a). Curriculum-based 300.
measurement of oral reading: Standard errors associ- Keenan, J. M., & Meenan C. E. (in press). Test differences
ated with progress monitoring outcomes from DI- in diagnosing comprehension deficits. Journal of
BELS, AIMSweb, and an experimental passage set. Leaming Disabilities.
School Psychology Review, 38, 266-283. Kendeou, P., Bohn-Geter, C , & Fulton, S. (2011). What
Christ, T. J., & Ardoin, S. P. (2009b). Curriculum-based we have been missing: The role of goals in reading
measurement of reading: Passage equivalence and se- comprehension. In M. T. McCrudden, J. Magliano, &
lection. Journal of School Psychology, 47, 55-75. G. Schraw (Eds.), Relevance instructions and goal-
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity focusing in text leaming (pp. 375-394). Charlotte, NC:
in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, Infonnation Age Publishing.
281-302. Kintsch, W. (1988a). Comprehension: A paradigm for
Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University
reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word Press.
recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive Kintsch, W. (1988b). The use of knowledge in discourse
skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. processing: A construction-integration model. Psycho-
Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277-299. logical Review, 95, 163-182.
De Boeck, P., & Wilson, M. (Eds.). (2004). Explanatory Kintsch, W. (2012). Psychological models of reading
item response models. A generalized linear and non- comprehension and their implications for assessment.
linear approach. New York, NY: Springer. In J. P. Sabatini, E. R. Albro, & T. O'Reilly (Eds.),
Deno, S. L. (1985). Curriculum-based measurement: The Measuring up: Advances in how we assess reading
emerging alternative. Exceptional Children, 52, 219- ability (pp. 21-38). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Little-
232. field Education.
Ferstl, E. C. (2007). The functional neuroanatomy of text Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of
comprehension: What's the story so far? In F. Schmal- text comprehension and production. Psychological Re-
hofer SL C. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher level language pro- view, 85, 363-394.
cesses (53-102). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. LaBerge, D., & Samuels. S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of
Ferstl, E. C , Neumann, J., Bogler, C , & von Cramon, automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive
D. Y. (2008). The extended language network: A meta- Psychology, 6, 293-323.

323
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 3

Lemari, J., Lorch, R. F., Jr., Eyrolle, H., & Virbel, J. longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16,
(2008). A text-based and reader-based theory of sig- 91-121.
naling. Educational Psychologist, 43, 27-48. O'Brien, E. J., & Albrecht, J. E. (1992). Comprehension
Linderholm, T., & van den Broek, P. (2002). The effects strategies in the development of a mental model. Jour-
of reading purpose and working memory capacity on nal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
the processing of expository text. Journal of Educa- and Cognition, 18, Ill-IM.
tional Psychology, 94, 778-784. Pearson P., & Hamm, D. (2005). The assessment of read-
Lorch, R. F., Jr., Lemari, J., & Grant, R. A. (2011). ing comprehension: A review of practices-past, pres-
Signaling hierarchical and sequential organization in ent, and future. In S. G. Paris & S. A. Stahl (Eds.),
expository text. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 267- Children's reading comprehension and assessment
284. (pp. 13-69). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
Lorch, R. F., Jr., & Lorch, E. P. (1995). Effects of orga- ates.
nizational signals on text processing strategies. Journal Perfetti, C. A., & Adlof, S. M. (2012). Reading compre-
of Educational Psychology, 87, 537-544. hension: A conceptual framework from word meaning
McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (2010). to text meaning. In J. P. Sabatini, E. R. Albro, & T.
Exploring how relevancy instructions affect personal O'Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up: Advances in how we
reading intentions, reading goals, and text processing: assess reading ability (pp. 3-20). Lanham, MD: Row-
A mixed-methods study. Contemporary Educational man & Littlefleld Education.
Psychology, 35, 229-241. Perfetti, C. A., & Frishkoff, G. A. (2008). The neural
McGrudden, M. T., Schraw, G., & Kambe, G. (2005). The bases of text and discourse processing. In B. Stemmer
effect of relevance instructions on reading time and & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of the neurosci-
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 8 8 - ence of language (pp. 165-174). Cambridge, MA:
102. Elsevier.
McMaster, K. L., van den Broek, P. W., Espin, C. A., Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality
White, M. J., Rapp, D. N., Kendeou, P., Garlson, S.
hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma
(2012). Making the right connections: Differential ef-
(Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-
fects of reading intervention for subgroups of compre-
214). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
henders. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1),
Perfetti, C. A., Yang, C.-L., & Schmalhofer, F. (2008).
100-111.
Comprehension skill and word-to-text processes. Ap-
McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from
plied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 303-318.
text: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence.
Poney, B. C , Skinner, C. H., & Axtell, P. K. (2005). An
Discourse Processes, 22, 247-287.
investigation of the reliability and standard error of
McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch,
measurement of words read correctly per minute using
W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions
of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels curriculum based measurement. Journal of Psychoedu-
of understanding in leaming from text. Cognition and cational Assessment, 23, 326-338.
Instruction, 14, 1-43. Prat, C. S., Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2011). Individual
Messick, S. (1989). Validity. In R. L. Linn (Ed.), Educa- differences in the neural basis of causal inferencing.
tional measurement (3rd ed., pp. 13-103). National Brain and Language, 116, 1-13.
Council on Measurement in Education, American Rapp. D. N., & van den Broek, P. (2005). Dynamic text
Council on Education. MacMillan: New York. comprehension: An integrative view of reading. Cur-
Messick, S. (1994). The interplay of evidence and conse- rent Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 276-
quences in the validation of performance assessments. 279.
Educational Researcher, 23, 13-23. Rapp, D. N., van den Broek, P., McMaster, K. L., Ken-
Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: deou, P., & Espin, C. A. (2007). Higher-order compre-
Validation of inferences from persons' responses and hension processes in struggling readers: A perspective
performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning. for research and intervention. Scientific Studies of
American Psychologist, 50, 741-749. Reading, 11, 289-312.
Mislevy, R. J., & Sabatini, J. P. (2012). How research on Rupp, A. A. (2012). Psychological versus psychometric
reading and research on assessment are transforming dimensionality in reading assessment. In J. P. Saba-
reading assessment (or if they aren't, how they should). tinin, E. R. Albro, & T. O'Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up:
In J. P. Sabatini, E. R. Albro, & T. O'Reilly (Eds.), Advances in how to assess reading ability (pp. 135-
Measuring up: Advances in how we assess reading 151). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Litdefield.
ability (pp. 119-134). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Sabatini, J. P., Albro, E. R., & O'Reilly, T. (Eds.). (2012).
Littlefield Education. Measuring up: Advances in how we assess reading
Narvaez, D., van den Broek, P., & Barron-Ruiz, A. ability. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littleeld Educa-
(1999). The influence of reading purpose on inference tion.
generation and comprehension in reading. Journal of Schneider, W., Krkel, J., & Weinert, F. E. (1989). Do-
Educational Psychology, 91, 488-496. main-specific knowledge and memory performance: A
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1997). Assessing reading comparison of high- and low-aptitude children. Jour-
difculties: The validity and utility of current measures nal of Educational Psychology, 81, 306-312.
of reading skill. British Journal of Educational Psy- Tolar, T. D., Barth, A. E., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M.,
chology, 67, 359-370. Stuebing, K. K., & Vaughn, S. (2012). Psychometric
Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (2011). The precursors of reading properties of maze tasks in middle school students.
ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year Assessment for Effective Intervention, 37, 131-146.

324
Connecting Cognitive Theory and Assessment

Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. W. (1985). Causal van den Broek, P., Risden, K., & Husebye-Hartman, E.
thinking and the representation of narrative events. (1995). The role of readers' standards for coherence in
Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612-630. the generation of inferences during reading. In R. F.
van den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory of Lorch, Jr., & E. J. O'Brien (Eds.), Sources of coher-
narrative texts: Inferences and coherence. In M. A. ence in text comprehension (pp. 353-373). Hillsdale,
Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. NJ: Erlbaum.
539-588). New York, NY: Academic Press. van den Broek, P., White, M. J., Kendeou, P., & Carlson,
van den Broek, P. (2010). Using texts in science educa- S. (2009). Reading between the lines: Developmental
tion: Cognitive processes and knowledge representa- and individual differences in cognitive processes in
tion. Science, 328, 453-456. reading comprehension. In R. K. Wagner, C. Schatsch-
van den Broek, P. W. (2012). Individual and developmen- neider, & C. Phythian-Sence (Eds.), Beyond decoding:
The behavioral and biological foundations of reading
tal differences in reading comprehension: Assessing
comprehension (pp. 107-123). New York, NY: Guil-
cognitive processes and outcomes. In J. P. Sabatini,
ford Press.
E. R. Albro, & T. O'Reilly (Eds.), Measuring up:
van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm,
Advances in how we assess reading ability (pp. 3 9 -
T. (1998/2004). The landscape model of reading: In-
58). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Education.
ferences and the on-line construction of a memory
van den Broek, P., Bohn-Gettler, C , Kendeou, P., Carl- representation. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman
son, S., & White, M. J. (2011). When a reader meets a. (Eds.), The construction of mental representations dur-
text: The role of standards of coherence in reading ing reading (pp. 71-98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (Re-
comprehension. In M. McCrudden, J. Magliano, & G. printed in R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau [Eds.], Theo-
Schraw (Eds.), Text relevance and learning from text retical models and processes of reading [1244-1269].
(pp. 123-140). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Pub- Newark, NJ: International Reading Association)
lishing. Wayman, M., Wallace, T., Wiley, H. I., Ticha, R., &
van den Broek, P. W., Helder, A., & Van Leijenhorst, L. Espin, C. A. (2007). Literature synthesis on curricu-
(in press). Sensitivity to Structural Centrality: Devel- lum-based measurement in reading. Journal of Special
opmental and individual differences in reading com- Education, 41, 85-120.
prehension skills. In M. A. Britt, S. R. Goldman, & Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child devel-
J.-F. Rouet (Eds.), Reading: Erom words to multiple opment and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69,
texts. New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor & Francis 848-872.
Group. Wolfe, M. B. W. (2005). Memory for narrative and ex-
van den Broek, P., Lorch, R. F., Jr., Linderholm, T., & pository text: Independent influences of semantic as-
Gustafson, M. (2001). The effects of readers' goals on sociations and text organization. Journal of E.xperi-
inference generation and memory for texts. Memory & mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
Cognition, 29, 1081-1087. tion, 31, 359-364.
van den Broek, P., Rapp, D. N., & Kendeou, P. (2005).
Integrating memory-based and constructionist pro- Date Received: August 4, 2012
cesses in accounts of reading comprehension. Dis- Date Accepted: August 4, 2012
course Processes, 39, 299-316. Action Editor: Matthew K. Bums

Paul van den Broek is a professor in education and child studies and director of the Brain
and Education laboratory at the University of Leiden (The Netherlands), and adjunct
professor in cognitive sciences at the University of Minnesota. His expertise is on
cognitive processes in reading comprehension, on the development of these processes,
and on the application to reading comprehension interventions.

Christine Espin is a professor at Leiden University in the Department of Education and


Child Studies, Special Education and an adjunct professor in cognitive sciences at the
University of Minnesota. At Leiden, she holds the Leiden University Fund Chair for
Teachers' Assessment of Learning and Behavioral Problems in Adolescents. Her research
has focused on the development of CBM progress-monitoring procedures for secondary
school students in reading, written expression, language learning, and content-area learn-
ing. Most recently, her research has focused on teachers' interpretation and use of data in
educational decision making.

325
Copyright of School Psychology Review is the property of National Association of School Psychologists and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

S-ar putea să vă placă și