Sunteți pe pagina 1din 18

10.

1177/1524838003259320
TRAUMA,
Schneider, Wright
VIOLENCE,
/ DENIALAND ARTICLE
& ABUSE /ACCOUNTABILITY
January 2004

UNDERSTANDING DENIAL IN SEXUAL OFFENDERS


A Review of Cognitive and Motivational
Processes to Avoid Responsibility

SANDRA L. SCHNEIDER
ROBERT C. WRIGHT
University of South Florida

Treatment of sexual offenders is routinely complicated by the presence of denial.


This article examines how denial is related to the willingness to take responsibility
for offense-related thoughts and actions and how conceptualizations of denial have
developed and changed over time. Multiple facets of denial are described in detail,
along with an assessment of how different forms of denial undermine acceptance of
responsibility throughout treatment. Evidence is presented to show that resistance
and denial often hinge on cognitive and motivational processes that are commonly
accepted as fundamental treatment targets rather than treatment obstacles. The
authors propose that denial may be best understood as the acceptance of expla-
nations that reduce accountability and are reinforced by distorted beliefs and
self-deceptive thinking processes. The article concludes with a discussion of the
rich clinical information embedded in different expressions of denial and the bene-
fits of treatment strategies designed to assess and work through them.

Key words: denial, accountability, responsibility, cognitive distortions, explanations, refutation, minimi-
zation, depersonalization, FoSOD

AN OVERALL GOAL of sexual offender treat- The purpose of this article is to trace concep-
ment programs is to reduce the likelihood that tualizations and treatments of denial as well as
offenders will engage in future acts of sexually related cognitive and motivational processes in
abusive behavior. Reaching this goal is difficult child molesters. This article provides a frame-
both because the variables leading to sexual work to evaluate the relationship between vari-
abuse are not yet fully understood and because ous forms of denial and treatment progress, em-
there is often considerable resistance on the part phasizing the advantages of recognizing forms
of the offender to become engaged in the treat- of denial that go beyond complete disavowal of
ment process. This treatment resistance is an offense. A preview of important points is
largely the product of processes of denial. provided in Key Points of the Research Review.

TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE, Vol. 5, No. 1, January 2004 3-20


DOI: 10.1177/1524838003259320
2004 Sage Publications

3
4 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

ACCOUNTABILITY AND DENIAL KEY POINTS OF THE


AS TREATMENT GOALS RESEARCH REVIEW
Since their introduction in the late 1970s, Accountability and offender denial are inversely
cognitive-behavioral therapies have become related and should be approached as treatment
the dominant approach to the treatment of sex- targets rather than treatment obstacles.
A conceptualization of denial as a coherent con-
ual offenders (Marshall & Barbaree, 1990). The struct with multiple forms holds distinct clinical
overall aim of cognitive-behavioral treatment advantages over a view of denial as a dichoto-
programs is to equip sexual offenders with self- mous construct.
management skills that can be used to manage Denial is closely related to the construct of cogni-
or avoid situations that increase their risk of tive distortions; denial often results in distorted
and biased thinking stemming from explanations
reoffending. To accomplish this, offenders are
used to excuse the offenders behavior.
trained to alter their views in a prosocial direc-
tion, attend to negative consequences of their
actions both for themselves and others, estab-
lish a less distorted view of their deviant behav- goals, and more likely to fail to complete treat-
ior, develop more acceptable responses to meet ment (Brake & Shannon, 1997; Hunter &
their needs, and learn strategies to control devi-
Figueredo, 1999; ODonohue & Letourneau,
ant sexual arousal and manage risky situations
1993; Salter, 1988).
(Marshall, Laws, & Barbaree, 1990).
Given this, it is not surprising that account-
The effectiveness of these approaches rests to
ability is viewed as an essential component of
a large extent on the offenders cooperation and
treatment and that denial is considered a central
investment in treatment. However, as clinicians
obstacle that stands in the way of accepting re-
working with this population are well aware,
some offenders entering treatment totally deny sponsibility for the offense. In her now classic
any involvement in the sexual offense, and guide to treating sexual offenders, Anna Salter
many continue to deny critical aspects of their (1988) (see also Furniss, 1984) emphasized that
offense even after being convicted (Barbaree, offenders must take responsibility for child
1991; Denton, Konopasky, & Street, 1994; sexual abuse without minimizing, external-
Grossman & Cavanaugh, 1990; Happel & izing, or projecting blame onto others (p. 67).
Auffrey, 1995; Langevin, 1988; Marshall, 1994; The U.S. Department of Health and Human Ser-
Quinsey, 1986; Schlank & Shaw, 1996, 1997). It vices has also issued a report stating that offend-
has been argued that offenders cannot be ex- ers must be required to accept responsibility for
pected to fully participate the abusive acts, without relying on any excuses
Offenders who in treatment or to work for or minimizations of the behavior (Faller,
disavow the toward changing their 1993). Similarly, in its Practice Standards and
commission of an behavior without ac- Guidelines, the Association for the Treatment of
offense or deny knowledging their own Sexual Abusers (ATSA Professional Issues
accountability are responsibility for the of- Committee, 2001) advises that most clients pres-
likely to be fense and their problem ent with some degree of denial and that clients
noncompliant with with sexual behaviors acceptance of responsibility concerning their
treatment tasks, (ODonohue & Letour- sexually abusive behaviors should be a treat-
resistant to accepting neau, 1993; Salter, 1988; ment goal. Denial is defined in the manual as
ownership of Schlank & Shaw, 1996, the failure of sexual abusers to accept responsi-
treatment goals, and 1997). As a result, offend- bility for their offenses (p. 63) and is char-
more likely to fail to ers who disavow the com- acterized as an obstacle to treatment progress and
complete treatment. mission of an offense or to compliance with treatment requirements.
deny accountability are As these examples demonstrate, there is a
likely to be noncompliant with treatment tasks, clear consensus that accountability and denial
resistant to accepting ownership of treatment are fundamental issues in the treatment of sex-
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 5

ual offenders. Although this consensus includes By early in the 1980s, it was becoming in-
the implication that reducing denial and in- creasingly apparent that the interpersonal tac-
creasing accountability go hand-in-hand, there tics of child molesters tend to be strategic
have been subtleand not so subtlediffer- (deYoung, 1982; Frude, 1982; Justice & Justice,
ences in how the two constructs are delimited 1979). Such formulations implied the presence
within the literature. Denial is almost always of cognitive processes that allow offenders to si-
characterized as an obstacle to treatment prog- multaneously overcome their own inhibitions
ress, whereas acceptance of responsibility is and the resistance of the victim. Finkelhor
typically considered a treatment goal. Those (1984) was among the first to explicitly ac-
who focus on denial tend to view clients in knowledge the role of cognition in explaining
terms of readiness for treatment, exploring how sexual abuse. In his integrative theory,
to prepare clients to be sufficiently motivated to Finkelhor included a cognitive or strategic com-
engage in the treatment process (e.g., ponent within his four fundamental prerequi-
ODonohue & LeTourneau, 1993). In contrast, sites to sexual abuse. He argued that offenders
those who focus on responsibility bypass the is- must overcome both internal and external inhi-
sue of readiness for treatment and focus on bitions as well as the resistance of the victim in
treatment interventions that can modify dis- order for abuse to occur. In other words, offend-
torted attitudes and beliefs to improve ers must find a way to avoid taking responsibil-
motivation to control deviant behavior (Salter, ity for or to deny the harmfulness of behaviors
that they would otherwise recognize as
1988).
abusive.
Others also began to point to the need to in-
THE EVOLUTION OF INTEREST IN vestigate cognitive contributors to sexual of-
DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY fending and to include them as targets in treat-
ment interventions (see Ward, Hudson,
Clinical interest in issues of denial and ac-
Johnston, & Marshall, 1997 for a review). Conte
countability in sexual offenders can be traced as
(1985) paved the way for these efforts by sug-
far back as the 1960s and 1970s (Cowden &
gesting that the personality constructs com-
Morse, 1970; Hitchens, 1972; McCaghy, 1968;
monly being used to differentiate sexual offend-
Resnik & Peters, 1967). Nevertheless, there was
ers were less useful to clinicians than problem-
virtually no systematic theoretical or empirical
focused dimensions such as denial, sexual
attention to the larger role of cognitive pro- arousal, sexual fantasies, cognitive distortions,
cesses in the treatment of child molesters until social sexual deficits, and other psychological
the mid-1980s. Common approaches to research and social problems. At roughly the same time,
and treatment prior to that time focused on is- Abel and his colleagues argued that the expla-
sues such as psychodynamic motives (Fenichel, nations provided by child molesters are not
1945; Freud, 1918/1950; Hammer & Glueck, simply excuses and justifications but represent
1957; Justice & Justice, 1979), sources of deviant preexisting beliefs or cognitive distortions that
sexual preferences and arousal (Adams, evolve to legitimize sexual contact with chil-
Tollison, & Carson, 1981; Groth, 1979; Little & dren (Abel, Becker, & Cunningham-Rathner,
Curran, 1978; Marks, 1981), family dysfunction 1984). They conceptualized cognitive distor-
and related developmental deficits (Bell & Hall, tions to consist of rationalizations developed by
1976; deYoung, 1982; Gaddini, 1983; Herman & offenders prior to and during offending to
Hirshman, 1981; Howells, 1981; Kaufman, Peck, justify their continued abuse of children (Abel
& Tagiuri, 1954; Lustig, Dresser, Spellman, & et al., 1989; see also Murphy, 1990).
Murray, 1966; Storr, 1965; Weiner, 1962), and A major assumption underlying this work
predictive or predisposing personality vari- was that distortions promote and maintain of-
ables (Gebhard, Gagnon, & Pomeroy, 1967; fending behavior and need to be directly tar-
Kirkland & Bauer, 1982; Meiselman, 1978; geted in treatment (Abel et al., 1984; Murphy,
Panton, 1978/1979; Weinberg, 1955). 1990; Stermac & Segal, 1989). As a result, inves-
6 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

tigators began to emphasize that offenders por- based on this view is typically concerned with
trayal of their offense was not simply inten- differences between deniers and admitters
tional deceit to avoid the consequences of their (e.g., Baldwin & Roys, 1998; Grossman &
actions. Instead, their explanations were likely Cavanaugh, 1990; Haywood & Grossman, 1994;
to reflect biased and distorted views stemming Haywood, Grossman, & Hardy, 1993; Nugent &
from preexisting beliefs (Barbaree, 1991; Mar- Kroner, 1996; Sefarbi, 1990). In addition, other
shall & Eccles, 1991). Soon clinicians began sys- types of denial are generally disregarded or re-
tematically reporting the prevalence and char- defined as minimization. The focus for these au-
acteristics of denial and distortions among their thors is placed on eliminating denial (i.e., con-
clients (Barbaree, 1991; Fowler, Burns, & Roehl, vincing offenders to admit that they did engage
1983; French, 1989; Geller, Devlin, Flynn, & in inappropriate sexual behavior) as a prerequi-
Kaliski, 1985; Maletsky, 1991; Marshall & site to beginning offense-specific treatment.
Barbaree, 1990; Pithers, 1990; Stevenson, From this perspective, denial has been viewed
Castillo, & Sefarbi, 1989). These reports made it as a problem that is of importance primarily in
clear that denial and cognitive distortions were the early stages of treatment (e.g., Schlank &
pervasive characteristics among offenders. Shaw, 1997).
However, little distinction was made between The impact of this narrower definition of de-
them, and both denial and cognitive distortions nial has been pivotal for several reasons. First,
were frequently used interchangeably to refer by focusing exclusively on complete deniers
to diminished accounts of offense behavior. and methods to eliminate complete denial in
advance of standard treatment, the perceived
relevance of denial has been largely discounted
THE FOCUS ON COMPLETE DENIAL
in all but the initial stage of treatment. Second,
During the 1990s, research and theorizing the work of those who have adopted a broader
concerning denial, accountability, and cognitive view of denial has been overshadowed by the
distortions grew at an impressive rate. Al- focus on eliminating complete denial on the one
though this helped bring needed attention to hand and characteristics ascribed to cognitive
the constructs, it also brought confusion to the distortions on the other. As a result, for many,
literature. There was not always agreement denial has come to be narrowly associated with
about what was meant by these constructs, and intentional deceit, despite empirical support
different authors often defined the terms differ- suggesting that denial is also likely to be
ently. As a result, a critical rift developed in grounded in distorted cognitions (Ward et al.,
which some authors used the construct of de- 1997; Wright & Schneider, 1999).
nial in a restricted sense to refer only to the dis- Third, many authors subscribing to an all-
avowal of having committed an offense, or-none view of denial have interpreted the
whereas others used denial to refer to a broader complete disavowal of an offense to indicate
range of explanations provided by offenders to poor amenability to treatment and, in response,
justify or minimize offense-related behavior. have excluded these offenders from programs
Those who have emphasized complete de- because they have been deemed untreatable
nial have variously referred to it as categorical (Frenken, 1994; McGrath, 1991). These prac-
denial (Marshall, Thornton, Marshall, titioners reason that if offenders deny their of-
Fernandez, & Mann, 2001), full denial (Brake fense, they are not motivated to learn self-
& Shannon, 1997), or absolute denial management skills because they do not view
(Barbaree, 1991; Schlank & Shaw, 1996). Despite themselves to have a problem or to have done
the use of different terms, each of these formula- something wrong (ODonohue & Letourneau,
tions shares similar core features. Each de- 1993; Schlank & Shaw, 1996, 1997). Moreover,
scribes the offender dichotomously as either some providers are concerned that if these of-
in or out of denial, often with the accompa- fenders are allowed to remain in programs
nying assumption that denial results from de- without admitting to their offense, that clini-
liberate attempts to avoid blame. Research cians are in effect reinforcing the illusion that of-
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 7

fenders can benefit from treatment without tak- Yet another concern generated by the lack of
ing responsibility for their offense. This latter agreement over how to conceptualize denial is
argument seems especially objectionable given the difficulty in determining the relationship
practice standards that specifically include ac- between denial and recidivism. The few exist-
ceptance of responsibility as a fundamental goal ing studies have focused exclusively on com-
of treatment (ATSA Professional Issues plete denial (Hanson & Bussiere, 1998; Kennedy
Committee, 2001). & Grubin, 1992). Al-
Those with a contrasting point of view argue though neither of these
studies revealed a clear Those with a
that complete denial cannot be a prerequisite for
link between denial and contrasting point
continuation in a program precisely because tak-
recidivism, the results are of view argue that
ing responsibility is a goal of treatment
likely to be misleading complete denial
(Maletzky, 1996; Wright & Schneider, in press). cannot be a
From this viewpoint, requiring that offenders and to potentially cause
scholars to dismiss the prerequisite for
must be out of denial before starting treatment continuation in a
is tantamount to requiring them to (at least par- importance of denial pre-
maturely. This is unfortu- program precisely
tially) cure themselves before they can receive because taking
treatment. Such contradictory views of the role nate given reliable find-
ings that those who are responsibility is a
of offender accountability (i.e., prerequisite or goal of treatment
treatment goal) have not been resolved and con- un s ucce s s f ully dis-
charged from treatment (Maletzky, 1996;
tinue to significantly affect how denial is Wright & Schneider,
conceptualized and treated. (typically because they
d e n y t h e ir o ff e n s e ) in press). From this
Nevertheless, there has been a significant re- view, requiring that
(Hunter & Figueredo,
duction in the practice of excluding offenders offenders must be
1 9 9 9 ; Sa lt e r, 1 9 8 8 ;
from treatment if they are denying the commis- out of denial before
Schlank & Shaw, 1997) do
sion of their offense. The most notable reason starting treatment is
have a reliably greater
for this stems from findings suggesting that tantamount to
risk of reoffending (Hall,
those who fail to complete treatment (for any requiring them to
1995; Marques, Day, Nel-
reason) evidence a higher rate of recidivism son, & West, 1994; Mar- (at least partially)
than those who complete treatment programs shall, 1994; Marshall & cure themselves
(Hall, 1995; Hanson & Bussiere, 1998; Marshall, Barbaree, 1990; McGrath, before they can
1994; Marshall, Anderson, & Fernandez, 1999). 1995). Resolving this con- receive treatment.
In addition, a series of court rulings, including flict in findings may re-
State v. Imlay (1991), concluded that an offender quire a more sophisti-
could not be punished on the basis of denial cated view of denial, along with improved
alone. Therefore, offenders could not have their measures of denial. The link to recidivism may
probation revoked if dismissed from a treat- not be discernible in previous studies because of
ment program due to their refusal to admit to an problems inherent in accepting a static, dichoto-
offense. Such rulings created an ethical di- mous conceptualization of denial (Wright &
lemma for clinicians: If they judged deniers to Schneider, in press) as well as methodological
be inappropriate for treatment, but the court weaknesses associated with operationalizing
system did not return them to prison, then these denial (Lund, 2000; see also Barbaree, 1997).
offenders would in effect be discharged to the
community with no further attempt to reduce
their risk of recidivism. In consequence, it has DENIAL AS A MULTIFACETED CONSTRUCT
been argued that the practice of dismissing de- Although many have focused on denial as a
niers from treatment increases the risk to the dichotomous construct, a large number of clini-
community by essentially preventing some of cians and scholars have acknowledged that de-
the most at-risk offenders from participating in nial is not an all-or-none phenomenon but
treatment programs (Marshall, 1994). rather a complex, multifaceted construct. Dur-
8 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

ing the late 1980s and throughout the 1990s, ing harmful happened to the alleged victim and
several descriptive typologies of denial were that the offender himself (or herself) is the one
created primarily based on clinical observations who has been wronged.
of different types and degrees of denial Virtually every author who has addressed is-
(Barbaree, 1991; Barrett, Sykes, & Byrnes, 1986; sues of denial among sexual offenders acknowl-
Brake & Shannon, 1997; Happel & Auffrey, edges that complete denial of the offense is a se-
1995; Laflen & Sturm, 1994; Langevin, 1988; Or- rious issue in treatment. For some authors
lando, 1998; Salter, 1988; Trepper & Barrett, (described in the previous section), this is the
1989; Winn, 1996/1997). The findings from de- only type of denial that is acknowledged, but
scriptive empirical studies (e.g., Kennedy & for many others this is the extreme of a contin-
Grubin, 1992; Pollock & Hashmall, 1991) have uum of denial. Within typologies of denial, this
resulted in categories similar to those produced has also been referred to as denial of the offense,
on the basis of clinical experience alone. Table 1 denial of the facts, denial of the behavior, or de-
provides a summary of the many types of denial nial of the crime. As shown in Table 1, all of the
that have been described in these efforts. typologies distinguish complete denial (e.g., I
Perhaps the most striking finding across all of never touched her) from other types of denial.
the typologies shown in Table 1 is the similarity The typical description is that the offender to-
in the identified categories of sexual offender tally denies any involvement in a sexual of-
denial. In a recent study, we attempted to em- fense. Despite the fact that this suggests a clear
pirically verify the existence of these various all-or-none assertion that I didnt do it, there
forms of denial through the creation of a self- may be some subtle variations in what offend-
report measure (Schneider & Wright, 2001). Us-
ers mean by this claim. In some cases, the of-
ing factor analytic techniques, empirical sup-
fender may be willing to concede that some-
port was found for virtually all of these different
thing happened, but they may insist that the
components of denial.
event was not sexual or harmful in any way so
In what follows, we discuss these compo-
that it should not be construed as an offense
nents within a framework that is graphically
(see, e.g., Pollock & Hashmall, 1991).
represented in Figure 1 and that is designed to
distinguish denial on the basis of three levels of Complete denial of the offense is highly re-
accountability (see also Wright & Schneider, in lated to another aspect of denial that contributes
press). The framework expands on Barbarees directly to refutation and the desire to avoid all
(1991) differentiation of two levels of account- responsibility. The assertion that nothing hap-
ability represented by absolute denial, de- pened implies that the alleged victim was not
scribed here as refutation, and various forms of harmed. In fact, several authors have specifi-
minimization. In addition to these, a third level cally identified denial of victim impact as a dis-
of accountability associated with depersonal- tinct form of denial. Denial of victim harm is
ization is also included to account for more also apparent in the large literature focused on
deeply entrenched forms of denial. empathy deficits in offenders (e.g., Hilton, 1993;
Hudson & Ward, 2000; Marshall, Hamilton, &
Refutation F e rn a n d e z , 2 0 0 1 ; McG ra t h , C a n n , &
Konopasky, 1998; Pithers, 1994). Indeed, empa-
Refutation provides a mechanism for com- thy training is an important component in many
pletely alleviating the offender from having to cognitive-behavioral treatment interventions.
take any responsibility for the offense. If the of- In our empirical evaluation, denial of victim im-
fenders explanation is that there was no of- pact accompanied complete denial in virtually
fense or that there was a harmless interaction all cases (Schneider & Wright, 2001). In fact,
that should not be thought of as an offense, then complete denial and denial of victim harm com-
there is nothing for which to take responsibility. bined to form a single factor, along with items
Refutation involves complete denial that an of- suggesting that the offender was adopting a vic-
fense occurred, coupled with claims that noth- tim stance. These items included alleged
TABLE 1: A Comparison of Typologies of Sexual Offender Denial

Types of Denial

Denial of Denial of Denial of Denial of Denial of


Author Offense Victim Impact Denial of Extent Responsibility Denial of Planning Sexual Deviancy Relapse Potential Other

Barbaree (1991) Denial of the Minimization of Minimization of Minimization of


facts victim harm extent of behavior responsibility
Barrett, Sykes, & Denial of facts Denial of impact Denial of facts Denial of
Byrnes (1986) responsibility,
denial of
awareness
Brake & Shannon Full denial, Partial denial- History-specific Partial denial- Denial of arousal Denial of arousal Denial of future
(1997) plausible minimizations denial, denial justifications, behavior
denial, (of harm) screen, current false dissociation
pathological incident-specific
denial denial
Happel & Auffrey Denial of the Denial of injury Denial of Denial of Denial of intent, Denial of deviant Denial of relapse
(1995) crime and impact on intrusiveness responsibility, planning, and arousal and potential and possible
the victim or extent of the denial of intent, premeditation; denial fantasies; denial recidivism; denial of
behavior, denial planning, and of deviant arousal of gratification and risk management
of frequency of premeditation and fantasies; denial sexual pleasure activities; denial of
deviant acts of physical, mental, difficulty to change
and environmental and need for help
grooming
Kennedy & Denial of Denial of effect Denial of effect Denial of Denial of deviant Denial of
Grubin (1992) offense responsibility, sexual preference need for social
denial of internal sanction
attribution,
assertion of
external attribution
Laflen & Sturm Denial of Minimization of Denial of Denial of planning Denial of risk of
(1994) behavior seriousness of responsibility (Stage 3) relapse (Stage 4)
(Stage 1) behavior (Stage 2) (Stage 3)

9
10
TABLE 1 (continued)

Types of Denial

Denial of Denial of Denial of Denial of Denial of


Author Offense Victim Impact Denial of Extent Responsibility Denial of Planning Sexual Deviancy Relapse Potential Other

Langevin (1988) Denying Claiming special Denial of anoma-


everything circumstances lous sexual
preferences
Orlando (1998) Denial of the Denial of harm Denial of extent or Denial of Denial of planning Denial of sexual Denial of likelihood
offense magnitude of the responsibility, gratification, de- of reoccurrence
abuse denial of sexual nial of sexual
intent arousal
Pollock & Denial of fact Denial of Denial of sexual Denial of
Hashmall (1991) wrongfulness intent, denial of responsibility,
wrongfulness denial of self-
determination,
denial of sexual
intent
Salter (1988) Denial of Denial of internal Denial of Denial of Denial of fantasy Denial of fantasy Denial of difficulty
the acts guilt for behavior, seriousness of responsibility for and planning and planning in changing abusive
themselves denial of behavior the acts, denial patterns
seriousness of of internal guilt
behavior for the behavior
Trepper & Denial of facts Denial of impact Denial of facts Denial of Denial of grooming Denial of deviant Denial of
Barrett (1989) responsibility, sexual arousal denial
denial of
awareness
Winn (1996) Denial of facts Denial of impact Denial of facts Denial of Denial of grooming Denial of deviant Denial of deviant Denial of
responsibility, oneself and the sexual arousal sexual arousal and denial
denial of environment and inappropriate inappropriate
awareness sexualization of sexualization of
nonsexual nonsexual problems
problems
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 11

Refutation Minimization Depersonalization commit an offense (either because of a stressful


Denial of situation or a mistake), and (c) denial of respon-
sibility based on the assertion of victim desire.
EXTENT

VICTIM
Denial of
Denial of
These forms of denial all share a common
goal. Offenders admit that something about
Responsibility:
STANCE VICTIM DESIRE PLANNING

their offense-related behavior was problematic


FULL Denial of
DENIAL RISK of

or potentially harmful but then try to discount


Denial of Denial of Denial of RELAPSE
VICTIM Responsibility: SEXUAL

their responsibility through explanations fo-


IMPACT STRESS DEVIANCY

Denial of
Responsibility:
cused on external circumstances and other ex-
MISTAKE cuses. Interestingly, we also found that these
forms of denial were systematically related to
Figure 1: Framework for Organizing Types of Denial Accord- measures of cognitive distortions (Schneider &
ing to Level of Accountability Wright, 2001). Nevertheless, the three denial
factorsbut not the cognitive distortions
measurescould distinguish offenders as a
mistreatment by the system, the lack of function of treatment progress. This provided
credibility of the victim, and a focus on self- evidence in support of distinguishing denial
harm. concerning responsibility for the offense from
According to Winn (1996), denial of victim cognitive distortions concerning general
impact derives largely from the offenders ten- beliefs.
dency to be self-focused. Often, offenders report
that they are the ones who have been harmed Depersonalization
and who are most negatively affected by the ac-
cusation of sexual violence. This theme of self- Even after offenders acknowledge their re-
focus represents a third characteristic of refuta- sponsibility for an of-
tion. By suggesting that nothing happened, the fense, they may not be Even after offenders
offender also maintains that he or she has been prepared to admit that acknowledge their
wronged. they are the type of per- responsibility for an
son who is vulnerable to offense, they may
Minimization committing sexual of- not be prepared to
fenses. Several authors admit that they are
Once offenders admit that something inap- have identified these the type of person
propriate happened, they frequently attempt to more deeply ingrained who is vulnerable to
deny their active participation in and responsi- forms of denial, which in- committing sexual
bility for the offense. Although some (e.g., clude denial of (a) plan- offenses. Several
Barbaree, 1991; Marshall, 1994; Rogers & n in g t h e o ff e n s e , ( b) authors have
Dickey, 1990) have used the term minimiza- grooming, (c) deviant identified these more
tion to distinguish these processes from com- arousal, (d) fantasizing, deeply ingrained
plete denial, several other authors have (e) sexual gratification, (f) forms of denial, which
recognized these explicitly as alternative need for help, and (g) fu- include denial of
forms of denial (e.g., Salter, 1988; Trepper & ture risk or relapse poten- (a) planning the
Barrett, 1989). Denial in these cases typically in- tial. These issues were offense,
volves blaming and justifications that have at empirically discernable in (b) grooming,
different times been categorized as partial de- two factors of the Facets (c) deviant arousal,
nial, denial of responsibility, or denial of extent of Sexual Offender Denial (d) fantasizing,
(see Table 1) (e.g., It was an accident, I was Scale (FoSOD): denial of (e) sexual
drunk, I didnt do as much as people think I planning and denial of re- gratification, (f) need
did). Three of the factors we identified empiri- lapse potential (which in- for help, and
cally fall into this group. They include (a) denial cluded items concerning (g) future risk or
of the extent of the offense, (b) denial of intent to deviant sexual interests). relapse potential.
12 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

Attempts to deny these abuse-related tenden- changes on the specific constructs described
cies often exist even after offenders acknowl- (Maletzky, 1991).
edge considerable responsibility for an isolated More recently, investigators have reported
offense (e.g., Laflen & Sturm, 1994). Deperson- the development of separate group treatment
alizing explanations reduce accountability by protocols targeting what they refer to as cate-
preventing the offender from coming to terms gorical deniers (Marshall et al., 2001). This type
with predispositions that contribute to the of program is designed to reduce the risk of
likelihood of future deviant thoughts and reoffending for those offenders who completely
behavior. deny the commission of an offense. The treat-
ment strategy consists of eliminating the re-
quirement of personal accountability for the
PERSPECTIVES ON DENIAL AND commission of an offense and instead targeting
APPROACHES TO TREATMENT ancillary criminogenic variables associated
It should come as no surprise that practitio- with sexual offending, such as unstable lifestyle
ners perspectives on denial are likely to have a and social skills deficits. This approach appears
substantial impact on approaches to treatment. to rest on the assumption that it is not necessary
For those who concentrate on issues surround- for offenders to take responsibility for their de-
ing complete denial, the focus has shifted from viant sexual behavior at any time. Instead, the
excluding deniers from treatment to issues con- authors hypothesize that the modification of
cerning (a) how long to allow deniers to stay in deficits associated with ancillary criminogenic
treatment without admitting and (b) how best factors is sufficient to reduce the likelihood of
to eliminate (complete) denial so real treatment recidivism for this population. This represents a
can begin. In this context, several authors have decisive departure from established treatment
developed pretreatment interventions to dimin- goals and implies that motivational issues (i.e.,
accepting oneself as a sexual offender) are not
ish denial before entering an offense-specific
essential targets of offense-specific sexual of-
sexual offender program (Brake & Shannon,
fender treatmentat least for categorical de-
1997; Burditt, 1995; Murphy & Berry, 1995;
niers. It is not clear on what basis the authors
ODonohue & LeTourneau, 1993; Schlank &
separate treatment goals for categorical deniers
Shaw, 1996).
versus admitters and what implications their
Other interventions have been designed to
approach has for larger motivational issues that
modify the offenders denial in the early stages permeate treatment programs. The rationale for
of treatment (Barbaree, 1991; Barnard, Fuller, the effectiveness of the criminogenic approach
Robbins, & Shaw, 1989; Maletzky, 1991; Mar- in the absence of accountability needs to be
shall, 1994; Murphy, 1990; Schwartz, 1995). made clearer, although the validity of the
Tools adopted to assist in reducing denial have approach is ultimately an empirical issue. To
included phallometry (e.g., Launay, 1994), date, the authors have not yet evaluated the
polygraphy (e.g., Hagler, 1995; Wilcox, 2000; effectiveness of their program for achieving
Williams, 1995), survivor reports (Valente & within-treatment goals.
Borthwick, 1995), and directed group work Consistent with other treatments designed to
(Crighton, 1995; cf. Barker & Beech, 1993). In reduce denial (Barrett et al., 1986; Brake & Shan-
general, these types of programs have reported non, 1997; Happel & Auffrey, 1995; Schlank &
moderate success at reducing denial in some of- Shaw, 1996; Winn, 1996/1997), the Marshall
fenders. Although such reports have supported et al. (2001) approach does suggest the potential
the use of incorporating interventions to mod- use of indirect strategies to reduce resistance to
ify denial in child molesters, many are based on treatment rather than relying on confronta-
small numbers, are anecdotal or descriptive in tional techniques that may provoke additional
nature, are designed as program evaluations opposition or yield only compliant behavior. If
rather than controlled studies, or have not em- the Marshall et al. approach were coupled with
ployed standardized measures to assess follow-up treatment that returns to the issue of
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 13

TABLE 2: Relationship of Types of Denial to Salters (1988) TABLE 3: Relationship of Denial to Greens (1995)
Recommended Goals of a Sexual Offender Recommended Goals of a Sexual Offender
Treatment Program Treatment Program

Salters (1988) Treatment Goals Greens (1995) Treatment Goals


Recommended Treatment Pro- Rephrased in Terms of Type of Recommended Treatment Rephrased in Terms of Type
gram Goals Denial to Be Reduced Program Goals of Denial to Be Reduced

Physical participation Denial of offense 1. Admitting guilt Denial of offense, extent


2. Accepting responsibility Denial of responsibility
Accepting responsibility for Denial of extent, responsibility 3. Understanding dynamics Denial of planning
offense 4. Identifying deviant cycle Denial of planning, sexual
Intellectual understanding of Denial of planning, sexual deviancy, relapse potential
offense chain and therapeutic deviancy, relapse potential 5. Making restitution Denial of victim impact
techniques
Emotional understanding of Denial of victim impact
impact of offenses
Attempts to change behavior Denial of responsibility, tions are not taken to disrupt problematic
planning, sexual deviancy, thoughts and behaviors. Getting and staying
relapse potential out of denial (of several types) are likely to be
Assertiveness and willingness continuing goals that the offender must adopt
to help other group members
prevent relapse
to motivate and maintain lasting changes.
In support of this view, we have recently
found empirical evidence that various forms of
denial are critically linked to treatment progress
accountability, it might reveal that offenders be-
come gradually more likely to accept responsi- in both early and advanced stages of treatment
bility as they learn to make improvements on (Wright & Schneider, in press). Specifically, we
other problematic issues within their lives. monitored offenders over the course of 18
Perspectives that acknowledge the larger months. Those who progressed during that
continuum of denial may be more likely to pro- time from an early to an advanced level in treat-
vide this kind of comprehensive approach to is- ment showed significant reductions in com-
sues associated with accepting responsibility plete denial (i.e., refutation) and minimization
throughout the entire course of treatment. This but remained relatively high in other forms of
seems especially true in cases wherein denial is denial. Those who did not graduate to an ad-
characterized as a treatment target rather than vanced level started and stayed with relatively
an obstacle to treatment. Tables 2 and 3 provide high scores on all forms of denial.
a summary of the goals of treatment itemized by Of perhaps greater interest are the reductions
Salter (1988) and Green (1995), respectively. in denial that were observed among offenders
These tables demonstrate how reductions in the who were out of complete denial at the start
different types of denial can be characterized as of the study. Offenders who began the study in
fulfilling or contributing to almost all of these an advanced level of treatment and progressed
treatment goals. The goal of accepting responsi- to an even higher level remained low in refuta-
bility for a particular offense, for instance, re- tion throughout the study period. Nevertheless,
quires reductions in denial of extent, denial of their continuing progress in treatment was tied
intent, and denial based on the assertion of vic- to reductions in denial of responsibility for their
tim desire. To diminish these forms of denial, of- offense (i.e., minimization) and in denial of
fenders must recognize that their offense behav- planning, sexual deviancy, and relapse risk (i.e.,
ior represents a serious intrusion over which depersonalization). The latter forms of denial
they had control. Similarly, understanding the remained high for all but offenders in the most
offense cycle ultimately requires that offenders advanced levels of treatment. These findings
admit that the offense was planned, that their suggest that changes in various types of denial
deviant sexual interests played a role, and that are strongly and systematically related to mak-
the cycle is likely to continue in the future if ac- ing progress across all stages of treatment, even
14 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

the most advanced (Wright & Schneider, in beliefs and social information-processing defi-
press). cits. In part, this is because denial typically
involves a dispute about seemingly specific, ob-
jectively verifiable events, whereas distortions
INTERRELATIONSHIPS
more often involve disagreements about more
BETWEEN DENIAL, ACCOUNTABILITY,
subjective interpretations of the general signifi-
AND COGNITIVE DISTORTIONS
cance of events. For example, categorical denial
Even with awareness of the continuum of is assumed to focus on factual events that can be
types of denial and the close association be- answered through a direct access of memory
tween denial and accountability, there is still the (e.g., I didnt do it) and thus it has been pre-
question of how cognitive distortions can best sumed to entail intentional deceit. On closer in-
be understood in this context. A variety of in- spection, what appear to be assertions of fact
vestigators have noted a close relationship be- may also involve beliefs, opinions, or subjective
tween these constructs, although the nature of interpretations. A claim that one did not commit
these relationships has never been made ex- a deviant act may include definitional distinc-
plicit (Conte, 1985; Gudjonsson, 1990; Johnston tions over what is meant by a term such as de-
& Ward, 1996; Ward et al., 1997; Ward, Hudson, viant or disputes over the presence of deliber-
& Marshall, 1995; Wright & Schneider, 1999). A ate intentions implied by the term commit. As
closer examination of the literature suggests a result, there are any number of apparent
both similarities and differences between denial facts that must be considered and interpreted
and cognitive distortions. In general, cognitive to determine the truth or falsity of the aforemen-
distortions have been described as biased ac- tioned assertion (see also Jenkins-Hall &
counts stemming from preexisting beliefs, Marlatt, 1989). As a result, even disputes over
whereas denial has typically referred to deliber- apparently objective facts are likely to involve
ate excuses and justifications intended to de- any number of cognitive processes that allow
ceive. Both have been described to reduce the offender to believe what he (or she) wants
offenders accountability for their offenses. without explicit awareness. Hence, denial is
Conte (1985) was one of the first to suggest likely to include both more and less intentional
that there may be a relationship between distor- attempts to deceive.
tions and denial, arguing that cognitive distor- In addition, it is not clear that biases and dis-
tions are likely to make it easier for offenders to tortions stem from preexisting beliefs or
misconstrue their behavior and its conse- whether they occur during the commission of
quences. This emphasis converged with the fo- an offense (Abel et al., 1984; Ward et al., 1997).
cus on justifications and excuses typically asso- Some investigators have suggested that the ac-
ciated with denial and provided a framework counts provided by offenders may simply rep-
that suggested a relationship between them resent rationalizations after the fact and suggest
(Barbaree, 1991; Marshall, 1994; Murphy, 1990; that there is no empirical evidence to support
Pollock & Hashmall, 1991). Within this perspec- the position that they represent preexisting be-
tive, denial has come to refer to explanations liefs (Pollock & Hashmall, 1991; Quinsey, 1986;
made with reference to a specific offense, Stermac & Segal, 1989). Hence, it is unclear
whereas distortions have been construed as whether it is even necessary to change preexist-
broader preexisting beliefs that must be true for ing beliefs to produce positive therapeutic out-
denial-based explanations to be plausible. For comes. It may be more valuable to focus on well-
example, the cognitive distortion that little girls learned strategies that justify or excuse deviant
enjoy sex may serve as the basis to deny victim behavior (Ward et al., 1997).
harm by claiming that a particular victim was a Although the origin of cognitive distortions
willing participant. has not been empirically validated, the exis-
Nevertheless, denial has come to be associ- tence of biased and distorted cognitions are
ated more with deliberate acts of deception, and well-documented in offenders accounts of their
cognitive distortions have been linked to faulty offense (e.g., Bumby, 1996; Hartley, 1998;
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 15

Stermac & Segal, 1989). Explanations generated This focus on explanations to excuse or jus-
by offenders to excuse or deny their behaviors tify deviant behavior is not unique to sex of-
are themselves likely to produce biased and dis- fenders. Researchers have previously evaluated
torted thinking (Wright & Schneider, in press). the significance of at-
Over time, offenders are likely to become in- tempts by non-sexual of- Whether the
creasingly confident in the accuracy of reasons fenders to reinterpret offenders portrayal
that they generate to explain their behavior (An- their actions in ways that of the offense stems
derson, 1982; Anderson & Sechler, 1986; diminish their culpability from preexisting
Koehler, 1991; Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, 1975). or neutralize its conse- beliefs, is grounded in
For this reason, some have suggested that of- quences (Saxe, 1991; Scott evidence accrued
fenders beliefs about their own behavior may & Lyman, 1968), and oth- through biased data
be better predictors of future offense risk than ers have specified verbal collection strategies,
more general attitudes toward offense-related tactics used by non-sexual or reflects a
offenders to justify or le- deliberate attempt to
issues (Hogue, 1994; Kennedy & Grubin, 1992).
gitimize deviant behaviors avoid perceived
Both denial and distortions, then, are likely to be
products of some combination of intentional de- (Bandura, 1973; Stokes & consequences, it still
Hewitt, 1976). Regardless represents an
ceit and biased reasoning processes that serve to
of their source of origin, absence of personal
protect offenders from facing their responsi-
changes in explanations accountability for
bility for committing sexual offenses.
generated by offenders to ones actions.
Although the distinctions between distor-
tions and denial are not yet entirely clear, both deny their actions are
likely to be critical to progress in treatment. Ex-
constructs serve to emphasize the importance of
planations represent a dynamic factor main-
(a) the offenders current view of his or her of-
tained by ongoing cognitive processes, both in-
fense, (b) the cognitions that fortify that view,
tentional and implicit. Changes in explanations
and (c) the extent to which the offender is cogni-
are likely to have clinical use because they re-
zant of the validity of his or her view. Whether
flect reductions of both intentional deceit and
the offenders portrayal of the offense stems
cognitive distortions that prevent offenders from
from preexisting beliefs, is grounded in evi-
taking responsibility for their offenses.
dence accrued through biased data collection
strategies, or reflects a deliberate attempt to
avoid perceived consequences, it still represents DENIAL AND THE MOTIVATION
an absence of personal accountability for ones TO TAKE RESPONSIBILITY
actions. It is for this reason that authors have Deceptive explanations in the clinical setting
emphasized the importance of changing the are not new phenomena. They have been regu-
way that offenders think about their offending larly observed in association with a wide vari-
behaviors (Ward et al., 1997). ety of clinical disorders (Rogers, 1997; Rogers,
The primary vehicle for assessing and modi- Sewell, & Goldstein, 1994). Dishonest and bi-
fying offenders cognitions is likely to be found ased accounts of offending behavior appear to
in the explanations provided by offenders to ac- be endemic within the sexual offender treat-
count for their offenses (Hogue, 1994; Pollock & ment setting. This is hardly unexpected given
Hashmall, 1991; Wright & Schneider, in press). that most offenders do not enter treatment vol-
Pollock and Hashmall (1991) pointed out that untarily. Instead, they are mandated to partici-
the basic units of analysis that are accessible to pate as a condition of their probation or parole.
clinicians are the explanatory statements pro- Given the threat to the offenders social status,
vided by offenders with regard to their offense. integrity, and family stability, there is tremen-
These explanations serve as a window into un- dous pressure to deny and distort information
derstanding the offenders point of view as well about having committed a sexual offensenot
as the network of ideas that need to be trans- just to others but also to the offender himself (or
formed to motivate positive behavior change. herself).
16 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

Sexual offender denial is also complicated by huge cost in their self-view and perceived social
the fact that being honest about ones culpabil- status. Determining whether offenders biased
ity is not necessarily a explanations stem from preexisting beliefs, self-
Determining whether straightforward matter. serving information-gathering strategies, or
offenders biased As described in the previ- justifications after the fact may be of less conse-
explanations stem ous section, significant quence than recognizing that effective interven-
from preexisting evidence exists that the tions require a therapeutic approach to evaluat-
beliefs, self-serving deviant actions of offend- ing these explanations in order to find ways to
information-gathering ers are embedded in an change them. Even if we know that an offender
strategies, or elaborate network of dis- is intentionally misrepresenting certain facts,
justifications after the torted ideas, grounded in understanding the lie from the perspective of
fact may be of less evidence accrued through the offender may be essential to developing
consequence than biased processes (Ward strategies to modify it.
recognizing that et al., 1997; Wright & Adopting more sophisticated views of de-
effective Schneider, 1997, 1999). It nial, and the related motivations to avoid versus
interventions require is not at all surprising, es- accept responsibility, will allow us to better un-
a therapeutic pecially when the penal- derstand not only the factors that make deviant
approach to ties are severe, that indi- behaviors acceptable to offenders but also the
evaluating these viduals can develop cog- changes that are likely to be required to render
explanations in order nitions that allow them to those same behaviors unacceptable to offend-
to find ways to deceive others and them- ers. Explorations of offenders denial provide a
change them. selves. rich source of clinical information about how of-
Clearly, there is an en- fenders view the world and what ideas are criti-
during need to reduce denial and distortion in cal in their thinking. Interventions designed to
sexual offenders and to promote honest self- assess rather than eliminate denial are likely to
assessments. Although it would be a mistake to produce information that reveals the varying
reinforce biased views or to excuse dishonesty, contexts within which offenders feel justified to
it may be just as harmful to attack these excuses avoid responsibility for their deviant behaviors.
and explanations without appreciating their Such information can then become the target of
meaning to the offender. Our understanding of therapeutic efforts, with an individualized basis
denial needs to encompass the fact that, to a for redirecting thinking processes to help of-
large extent, offenders are likely to believe the fenders more accurately assess and develop
assertions and arguments they make and that their capacities to take responsibility for and
abandoning their position is likely to exact a control their deviant thoughts and behaviors.

IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, POLICY, AND RESEARCH


Denial is best viewed as a source of rich clinical in- The recent development of a psychometric- ally
formation about the offenders view of the world sound measure of denial, the Facets of Sexual Of-
rather than as an obstacle that interferes with treat- fender Denial Scale (Schneider & Wright, 2001),
ment. holds promise as a measure of within-treatment
Because denial stems from a combination of inten- change and should improve methods of examining
tional deceit and distorted thinking, indirect ap- the relationship between denial and recidivism.
proaches designed to analyze denial, rather than
immediately eliminate it, are likely to be more effec-
tive.
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 17

REFERENCES tion of the MOLEST and RAPE scales. Sexual Abuse: A


Journal of Research and Treatment, 8, 37-54.
Abel, G. G., Becker, J. V., & Cunningham-Rathner, J. (1984). Burditt, T. (1995, October). Treating sex offenders who deny
Complications, consent and cognitions in sex between their guilt: The application of motivational interviewing to
children and adults. International Journal of Law and Psy- the deniers pilot study. Paper presented at the Annual
chiatry, 7, 89-103. Research and Treatment Conference of the Association
Abel, G. G., Gore, D. K., Holland, C. L., Camp. N., Becker, for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, New Orleans, LA.
J. V., & Rathner, J. (1989). The measurement of the cogni- Conte, J. R. (1985). Clinical dimensions of adult sexual
tive distortions of child molesters. Annals of Sex abuse of children. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 3, 341-
Research, 2, 135-152. 354.
Adams, H. E., Tollison, C. D., & Carson, T. P. (1981). Behav- Cowden, J. E., & Morse, E. L. (1970). The relationship of
ior therapy with sexual deviations. In S. M. Turner, K. S. defensiveness to responses on the Sex Inventory. Journal
Calhoun, & H. E. Adams (Eds.), Handbook of clinical of Clinical Psychology, 26(4), 505-509.
behavior therapy (pp. 318-346). New York: John Wiley. Crighton, D. (1995). Sex offender groupwork. Issues in
Anderson, C. A. (1982). Inoculation and counter-explana- Criminological & Legal Psychology, 23, 15-21.
tion: Debiasing techniques in the perseverance of social Denton, K., Konopasky, R. J., & Street, P. A. (1994, July). Sex-
theories. Social Cognition, 1, 126-139. ual harassment: Expansion of the likelihood of sexual harass-
Anderson, C. A., & Sechler, E. S. (1986). Effects of explana- ing and the positive relationship between sexual harassment
tion and counter-explanation on the development and and sexual aggression. Poster session presented at the
use of social theories. Journal of Personality and Social annual convention of the Canadian Psychological
Psychology, 50, 24-34. Association, Penticton, British Columbia, Canada.
ATSA Professional Issues Committee. (2001). Practice stan- deYoung, M. (1982). The sexual victimization of children. Jef-
dards and guidelines for members of the Association for the ferson, NC: McFarland.
Treatment of Sexual Abusers. Beaverton, OR: ATSA. Faller, K. C. (1993). Child sexual abuse: Intervention and treat-
Baldwin, K., & Roys, D. T. (1998). Factors associated with ment issues. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
denial in a sample of alleged adult sexual offenders. Health and Human Services.
Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 10(3), Fenichel, D. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis.
211-226. New York: Norton.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. research. New York: Free Press.
Barbaree, H. E. (1991). Denial and minimization among sex Fowler, C., Burns, S. R., & Roehl, J. E. (1983). Counseling the
offenders: Assessment and treatment outcome. Forum incest offender. International Journal of Family Therapy,
on Corrections Research, 3, 30-33. 5(2), 92-97.
Barbaree, H. E. (1997). Evaluating treatment efficacy with French, D. D. (1989). Distortion and lying as defense pro-
sexual offenders: The insensitivity of recidivism studies cesses in the adolescent child molester. Journal of
to treatment effects. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research Offender Counseling, Services & Rehabilitation, 14(2), 161-
and Treatment, 9(2). New York: Plenum. 167.
Barker, M., & Beech, T. (1993). Sex offender treatment pro- Frenken, J. (1994). Treatment of incest perpetrators: A five-
grammes: A critical look at the cognitive-behavioural phase model. Child Abuse & Neglect, 18, 357-365.
approach. Issues in Criminological & Legal Psychology, 19, Freud, S. (1950). Totem and taboo. London: Routledge Kegan
37-42. Paul. (Original work published 1918)
Barnard, G. W., Fuller, A. K., Robbins, L., & Shaw, T. (1989). Frude, N. (1982). The sexual nature of sexual abuse: A
The child molester: An integrated approach to evaluation and review of the literature. Child Abuse & Neglect, 6, 211-
treatment. New York: Brunner/Mazel. 223.
Barrett, M. J., Sykes, C., & Byrnes, W. (1986). A systemic Furniss, T. (1984). Organizing a therapeutic approach to
model for the treatment of intra-family child sexual intra-familial child sexual abuse. Journal of Adolescence,
abuse. In T. Trepper & M. J. Barrett (Eds.), Treating incest: 7(4), 309-317.
A multiple systems perspective (pp. 67-82). New York: Gaddini, R. (1983). Incest as a developmental failure. Child
Haworth. Abuse & Neglect, 7, 357-358.
Bell, A. D., & Hall, C. S. (1976). The personality of a child Gebhard, P. H., Gagnon, J. H., & Pomeroy, W. B. (1967). Sex
molester. In M. X. Weinberg (Ed.), Sex research: Studies offenders. New York: Bantam.
from the Kinsey Institute. Oxford, UK: Oxford University. Geller, M., Devlin, M., Flynn, T., & Kaliski, J. (1985). Con-
Brake, S. C., & Shannon, D. (1997). Using pretreatment to frontation of denial in a fathers incest group. Interna-
increase admission in sex offenders. In B. K. Schwartz & tional Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 35(4), 545-567.
H. R. Cellini (Eds.), The sex offender: New insights, treat- Green, R. (1995). Comprehensive treatment planning for
ment innovations and legal developments, Volume 2 (pp. 5- sex offenders. In B. K. Schwartz & H. R. Cellini (Eds.),
15-16). Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute. The sexual offender: Corrections, treatment, and legal prac-
Bumby, K. M. (1996). Assessing the cognitive distortions of tice (pp. 10-110-9). Kingston, NJ: Civic Research
child molesters and rapists: Development and valida- Institute.
18 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

Grossman, L. S., & Cavanaugh, J. L. (1990). Psychopath- sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and
ology and denial in alleged sex offenders. Journal of Ner- Treatment, 11, 49-67.
vous & Mental Disease, 178(12), 739-744. Jenkins-Hall, K. D., & Marlatt, G. A. (1989). Apparently
Groth, N. A. (1979). Sexual trauma in the life histories of irrelevant decisions in the relapse process. In D. R. Laws
rapists and child molesters. Victimology: An Interna- (Ed.), Relapse prevention with sex offenders (pp. 47-55).
tional Journal, 4, 10-16. New York: Guilford.
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1990). Cognitive distortions and blame Johnston, L., & Ward, T. (1996). Social cognition and sexual
attribution among paedophiles. Sexual & Marital Ther- offending: A theoretical framework. Sexual Abuse: A
apy, 5(2), 183-185. Journal of Research and Treatment, 8(1), 55-80.
Hagler, H. L. (1995). Polygraph as a measure of progress in Justice, B., & Justice, R. (1979). The broken taboo. New York:
the assessment, treatment, and surveillance of sex Human Science.
offenders. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 2(2), 98-111 Kaufman, I., Peck, A. L., & Tagiuri, C. K. (1954). The family
Hall, G. C. N. (1995). Sexual offender recidivism revisited: constellation and overt incestuous relations between
A meta-analysis of recent treatment studies. Journal of father and daughter. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 802-809. 24, 266-277.
Hammer, R. F., & Glueck, B. C., Jr. (1957). Psychodynamic Kennedy, H. G., & Grubin, D. H. (1992). Patterns of denial
patterns in sex offenders: A four factor theory. Psychiat- in sex offenders. Psychological Medicine, 22, 191-196.
ric Quarterly, 31, 325-345. Kirkland, K., & Bauer, C. (1982). MMPI traits of incestuous
Hanson, R. K., & Bussiere, M. T. (1998). Predicting relapse: fathers. Journal of Criminal Psychology, 38, 645-649.
A meta-analysis of sexual offender recidivism studies. Koehler, D. J. (1991). Explanation, imagination, and confi-
Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 66, 348-362. dence in judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 499-519.
Happel, R. M., & Auffrey, J. J. (1995). Sex offender assess- Laflen, B., & Sturm, W. R., Jr. (1994). Understanding and
ment: Interrupting the dance of denial. American Journal working with denial in sexual offenders. Journal of Child
of Forensic Psychology, 13(2), 5-22. Sexual Abuse, 3(4), 19-36.
Hartley, C. C. (1998). How incest offenders overcome inter- Langevin, R. (1988). Defensiveness in sex offenders. In
nal inhibitions through the use of cognitions and cogni- R. Rogers (Ed.), Clinical assessment of malingering and
tive distortions. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13(1), deception (pp. 269-290). New York: Guilford.
25-39. Launay, G. (1994). The phallometric assessment of sex
Haywood, T. W., & Grossman, L. S. (1994). Denial of devi- offenders: Some professional and research issues. Crim-
ant sexual arousal and psychopathology in child inal Behaviour & Mental Health, 4(1), 48-70.
molesters. Behavior Therapy, 25(2), 327-340. Little, L. M., & Curran, J. P. (1978). Covert sensitization: A
Haywood, T. W., Grossman, L. S., & Hardy, D. W. (1993). clinical procedure in need of some explanations. Psycho-
Denial and social desirability in clinical examinations logical Bulletin, 85, 513-531.
of alleged sex offenders. Journal of Nervous & Mental Dis- Lund, C. A. (2000). Predictors of sexual recidivism: Did
ease, 181(3), 183-188. meta-analysis clarify the role and relevance of denial?
Herman, J., & Hirshman, L. (1981). Father-daughter incest. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 12, 275-
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. 287.
Hilton, N. Z. (1993). Childhood sexual victimization and Lustig, N., Dresser, J. W., Spellman, S. W., & Murray, T. B.
lack of empathy in child molesters: Explanation or (1966). Incest. Archives of General Psychology, 13, 31-40.
excuse? International Journal of Offender Therapy & Com- Maletzky, B. M. (1991). Treating the sexual offender. Newbury
parative Criminology, 37(4), 287-296. Park, CA: Sage.
Hitchens, E. W. (1972). Denial: An identified theme in mar- Maletsky, B. M. (1996). Denial of treatment or treatment of
ital relationships of sex offenders. Perspectives in Psychi- denial? Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment,
atric Care, 10(4), 152-159. 8(1), 1-5.
Hogue, T. E. (1994). Sex offence information questionnaire: Marks, I. M. (1981). Review of behavior psychotherapy 2:
Assessment of sexual offenders perceptions of respon- Sexual disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138,
sibility, empathy, and control. In N. K. Clark & G. M. 754-756.
Stephenson (Eds.), Rights and risks: The application of Marques, J. K., Day, D. M., Nelson, C., & West, M. A. (1994).
forensic psychology (pp. 68-75). London: British Psycho- Effects of cognitive-behavioral treatment on sex
logical Society. offender recidivism: Preliminary results of a longitudi-
Howells, K. (1981). Adult sexual interest in children: Con- nal study. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 21, 28-54.
siderations relevant to theories of etiology. In M. Cook Marshall, W. L. (1994). Treatment effects on denial and
& G. Wilson (Eds.), Adult sexual interest in children minimization in incarcerated sex offenders. Behavior
(pp. 55-94). New York: Academic Press. Research and Therapy, 32(5), 559-564.
Hudson, S. M., & Ward, T. (2000). Interpersonal compe- Marshall, W. L., Anderson, D., & Fernandez, Y. M. (1999).
tency in sex offenders. Behavior Modification, 24(4), 494- Cognitive behavioural treatment of sexual offenders. Lon-
527. don: Wiley.
Hunter, J. A., & Figueredo, A. J. (1999). Factors associated Marshall, W. L., & Barbaree, H. E. (1990). Outcome of com-
with treatment compliance in a population of juvenile prehensive cognitive-behavioral treatment programs.
Schneider, Wright / DENIAL AND ACCOUNTABILITY 19

In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), and enhancing external supervision. In W. L. Marshall,
Handbook of sexual assault: Issues, theories and treatment of D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of sexual
the offender (pp. 363-385). New York: Plenum. assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender (pp.
Marshall, W. L., & Eccles, A. (1991). Issues in clinical prac- 343-361). New York: Plenum.
tice with sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, Pithers, W. D. (1994). Process evaluation of a group therapy
6(1), 68-93. component designed to enhance sex offenders empa-
Marshall, W. L., Hamilton, K., & Fernandez, Y. (2001). thy for sexual abuse survivors. Behavior Research and
Empathy deficits and cognitive distortions in child Therapy, 32, 565-570.
molesters. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treat- Pollock, N. L., & Hashmall, J. M. (1991). The excuses of
ment, 13(2), 123-130. child molesters. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 9, 53-59.
Marshall, W. L., Laws, R. D., & Barbaree, H. E. (Eds.). Quinsey, V. L. (1986). Men who have sex with children. In
(1990). Handbook of sexual assault. New York: Plenum. D. N. Weisstub (Ed.), Law and mental health: International
Marshall, W. L., Thornton, D., Marshall, L. E., Fernandez, perspectives (Vol. 2, pp. 140-172). New York: Pergamon.
Y. M., & Mann, R. (2001). Treatment of sexual offenders Resnik, H. L., & Peters, J. J. (1967). Outpatient group ther-
who are in categorical denial: A pilot project. Sexual apy with convicted pedophiles. International Journal of
Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 13(3), 205-215. Group Psychotherapy, 17(2), 151-158.
McCaghy, C. H (1968). Drinking and deviance disavowal: Rogers, R. (Ed.). (1997). Clinical assessment of malingering
The case of child molesters. Social Problems, 16, 43-49. and deception (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
McGrath, R. J. (1991). Sex-offender risk assessment and Rogers, R., & Dickey, R. (1990). Denial and minimization
disposition planning: A review of empirical and clinical among sex offenders: A review of competing models of
findings. International Journal of Offender Therapy & Com- deception. Annals of Sex Research, 4, 49-63.
parative Criminology, 35(4), 328-350. Rogers, R., Sewell, K. W., & Goldstein, A. M. (1994).
McGrath, R. J. (1995, Winter). Sex offender treatment: Does Explanatory models of malingering: A prototypical
it work? Perspectives, 24-26. analysis. Law & Human Behavior, 18(5), 543-552.
McGrath, M., Cann, S., & Konopasky, R. (1998). New mea-
Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Persever-
sures of defensiveness, empathy, and cognitive distor-
ance in self-perception and social perception: Biased
tions for sexual offenders against children. Sexual Abuse:
attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm.
A Journal of Research and Treatment, 10(1), 25-36.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 880-892.
Meiselman, K. C. (1978). Incest. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Salter, A. C. (1988). Treating child sex offenders and victims.
Murphy, W. D. (1990). Assessment and modification of
London: Sage.
cognitive distortions in sex offenders. In W. L. Marshall,
Saxe, L. (1991). Lying: Thoughts of an applied social psy-
D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of sexual
chologist. American Psychologist, 46(4), 409-415.
assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender (pp.
331-342). New York: Plenum. Schlank, A. M., & Shaw, T. (1996). Treating sexual offenders
Murphy, J. J., & Berry, D. J. (1995, October). Treating sex who deny their guilt: A pilot study. Sexual Abuse: A Jour-
offenders who deny their guilt: A six-month adapted version nal of Research and Treatment, 8(1), 17-23.
of the deniers pilot study. Paper presented at the Annual Schlank, A. M., & Shaw, T. (1997). Treating sexual offenders
Research and Treatment Conference of the Association who denyA review. In B. K. Schwartz & H. R. Cellini
for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, New Orleans, LA. (Eds.), The sex offender: New insights, treatment innova-
Nugent, P. M., & Kroner, D. G. (1996). Denial, response tions and legal developments (Vol. 2, pp. 6-16-7).
styles, and admittance of offenses among child molest- Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute.
ers and rapists. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11(4), Schneider, S. L., & Wright, R. C. (2001). The FoSOD: A mea-
475-486. surement tool for re-conceptualizing the role of denial
ODonohue, W., & Letourneau, E. (1993). A brief group in child molesters. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16,
treatment for the modification of denial in child sexual 545-564.
abusers: Outcome and follow-up. Child Abuse & Neglect, Schwartz, B. K. (1995). Group therapy. In B. K. Schwartz &
17, 299-304. H. R. Cellini (Eds.), The sexual offender: Corrections, treat-
Orlando, D. (1998, September). Sex offenders. Special Needs ment, and legal practice (pp. 14-114-15). Kingston, NJ:
Offenders Bulletin, No. 3. Washington, DC: Federal Judi- Civic Research Institute.
cial Center. Scott, M. B., & Lyman, S. M. (1968). Accounts. American
Panton, J. H. (1978). Personality differences appearing Sociological Review, 33(1), 46-62.
between rapists of adults, rapists of children, and non- Sefarbi, R. (1990). Admitters and deniers among adoles-
violent sexual molesters of children. Residential Commu- cent sex offenders and their families: A preliminary
nity Psychology and Psychiatry Behaviors, 3, 385-393. study. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60(3), 460-
Panton, J. H. (1979). MMPI profile configurations associ- 465.
ated with incestuous and non-incestuous child molest- State v. Imlay. (1991). 249 Mont. 82, 813 P. 2d 979.
ing. Psychological Representative, 45, 335-338. Stermac, L., & Segal, Z. V. (1989). Adult sexual contact with
Pithers, W. D. (1990). Relapse prevention with sexual children: An examination of cognitive factors. Behavior
aggressors: A method for maintaining therapeutic gain Therapy, 20, 573-584.
20 TRAUMA, VIOLENCE, & ABUSE / January 2004

Stevenson, H. C., Castillo, E., & Sefarbi, R. (1989). Treat- SUGGESTED FUTURE READINGS
ment of denial in adolescent sex offenders and their
families. Journal of Offender Counseling, Services & Reha- Koehler, D. J. (1991). Explanation, imagination, and confi-
bilitation, 14(1), 37-50. dence in judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 499-519.
Pollock, N. L., & Hashmall, J. M. (1991). The excuses of
Stokes, R., & Hewitt, J. P. (1976). Aligning actions. American
child molesters. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 9, 53-59.
Sociological Review, 41, 838-849.
Saxe, L. (1991). Lying: Thoughts of an applied social psy-
Storr, A. (1965). Sexual deviation. London: Heineman.
chologist. American Psychologist, 46(4), 409-415.
Trepper, T., & Barrett, M. J. (1989). Systemic treatment of
Schneider, S. L., & Wright, R. C. (2001). The FoSOD: A mea-
incest: A therapeutic handbook. New York: Brunner/
surement tool for re-conceptualizing the role of denial
Mazel.
in child molesters. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16,
Valente, M., & Borthwick, I. (1995). Sexual abuse: Using 545-564.
survivors experience to confront denial. Child Abuse Wright, R. C., & Schneider, S. L. (in press). Mapping child
Review, 4(1), 57-62. molester treatment progress with the FoSOD: Denial,
Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., Johnston, L., & Marshall, W. L. explanations, and accountability. Sexual Abuse: A Journal
(1997). Cognitive distortions in sex offenders: An inte- of Research and Treatment.
grative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 17(5), 479- .
507.
Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., & Marshall, W. L. (1995). Cogni-
tive distortions and affective deficits in sex offenders: A
Sandra L. Schneider, Ph.D., earned her
cognitive deconstructionist interpretation. Sexual doctorate in experimental psychology from
Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 7(1), 67-83. the University of WisconsinMadison.
Weinberg, S. K. (1955). Incest behavior. New York: Citadel. She is currently associate dean of the Col-
Weiner, I. B. (1962). Father-daughter incest: A clinical lege of Arts and Sciences and professor of
report. Psychiatric Quarterly, 36, 607-632. cognitive and neural sciences in the
Wilcox, D. T. (2000). Application of the clinical polygraph Department of Psychology at the Univer-
examination to the assessment, treatment and monitor- sity of South Florida. She recently completed a leave at the
ing of sex offenders. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 5(2), National Science Foundation where she served as director
134-152. of the Decision, Risk, and Management Science Program.
Williams, V. L. (1995). Response to Cross and Saxes A cri- She has published numerous peer-reviewed articles, edited
tique of the validity of polygraph testing in child sexual a volume on decision-making processes, directed theses
abuse cases. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 4(3), 55-71.
and dissertations, and taught courses in psychological sci-
Winn, M. E. (1996). The strategic and systemic manage-
ence and research methodology at undergraduate and
ment of denial in the cognitive/behavioral treatment of
sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and
graduate levels. Her research on cognitive and decision
Treatment, 8, 25-36. processes has been funded both by the National Science
Winn, M. E. (1997). Using strategic, systemic, and linguistic Foundation and the National Institute of Mental Health.
principles to enhance treatment compliance. In B. K.
Schwartz & H. R. Cellini (Eds.), The sex offender: New Robert C. Wright, Ph.D., earned his
insights, treatment innovations and legal developments (Vol. doctorate in counseling psychology from
2, pp. 7-17-15). Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute. the University of WisconsinMadison. He
Wright, R. C., & Schneider, S. L. (1997). Deviant sexual fan- is a licensed psychologist and executive
tasies as motivated self-deception. In B. K. Schwartz & director of Psychological Management
H. R. Cellini (Eds.), The sex offender: New insights, treat- Groups Batterer Intervention and Sexual
ment innovations and legal developments. (Chapter 8, Vol.
Offender Treatment Programs in Tampa,
2, pp. 8-18-14). Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute.
Florida. He is also in private practice and is a clinical pro-
Wright, R. C., & Schneider, S. L. (1999). Motivated self-
deception in child molesters. Journal of Child Sexual fessor in the Department of Psychiatry at the University of
Abuse, 8, 89-111. South Florida. He has provided treatment to both batterers
Wright, R. C., & Schneider, S. L. (in press). Mapping child and sexual offenders for more than 18 years and has pub-
molester treatment progress with the FoSOD: Denial, lished several articles on cognitive factors associated with
explanations, and accountability. Sexual Abuse: A Jour- offending behavior. His research has been supported by the
nal of Research and Treatment. National Institute of Mental Health.

S-ar putea să vă placă și