Sunteți pe pagina 1din 9

Review

Special Issue: Cognition in Neuropsychiatric Disorders

The psychopath magnetized: insights


from brain imaging
Nathaniel E. Anderson and Kent A. Kiehl
Mind Research Network, 1101 Yale Blvd NE, Albuquerque, NM 87106, USA

Psychopaths commit a disproportionate amount of social, legal and philosophical implications stemming from
violent crime, and this places a substantial economic research suggesting that psychopathy has a neural basis
and emotional burden on society. Elucidation of the [10,11] with strong genetic heritability [12].
neural correlates of psychopathy may lead to improved Psychopathy is considered a personality disorder. It had
management and treatment of the condition. Although been specifically included in the Diagnostic and Statistical
some methodological issues remain, the neuroimaging Manual of Mental Disorders [13] until it was conceptually
literature is generally converging on a set of brain consolidated in version three and subsequent editions with
regions and circuits that are consistently implicated in antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) [14]; however, the
the condition: the orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, and ASPD designation, which the framers of the DSM-IV hoped
the anterior and posterior cingulate and adjacent would capture the essential components of psychopathy,
(para)limbic structures. We discuss these findings in has been criticized for its over-emphasis on behavioral
the context of extant theories of psychopathy and outcomes (such as criminality) and under-emphasis of
highlight the potential legal and policy implications of the core personality features such as affective deficits
this body of work. [15]. Thus, psychopathy and ASPD are not the same
condition. The most widely used and validated instrument
Psychopathy: the personality disorder for the assessment of psychopathy in clinical, research and
Psychopathy is a construct characterized by symptoms of forensic contexts is Hares Psychopathy Checklist-Revised
emotional detachment and a propensity for disinhibited, (PCL-R) [16,17]. The Hare PCL-R comprises the complete
impulsive behavior combined with a general callousness set of traits classically associated with psychopathy that
and lack of insight for the impact such behavior has on contribute to considerable disturbances in character, be-
others [1]. Psychopathy is among the most important havior and maladaptive social functioning, which are re-
psychological constructs in legal settings, having strong markably stable [18]. Thus, psychopathy is consistent with
predictive utility for recidivism (the tendency to re-offend), the widely accepted mental illness definition of a person-
institutional adjustment and treatment outcomes [2,3]. ality disorder, especially when recognized in its full clinical
Indeed, within 1 year of release, psychopaths are approxi- manifestation.
mately three times more likely to recidivate than non- An alternative view asserts that psychopathic traits
psychopaths, and four times more likely to violently reci- may be characterized as conferring a competitive advan-
divate [4]. Furthermore, this pattern is alarmingly stable tage in some circumstances [19]. For instance, an individ-
and persistent. Juveniles with identifiable psychopathic ual who is cunning and manipulative and maintains a
traits demonstrate similar recidivism rates [5], and tradi- general disregard for the wellbeing of others might do very
tional therapeutic intervention strategies with adult well for him/herself in a variety of contexts ranging from
psychopaths have often proven to be ineffective or even basic competition for resources to politics and business.
counterproductive [68]. Indeed, executive boardrooms are reportedly rife with
The symptoms of psychopathy, defined by 200 years of these characteristics [20]. Research focusing on such traits
clinical and forensic work, are well established. Psycho- outside the traditional criminal or forensic context has
metric analyses of these symptoms suggest theoretically sometimes referred to this brand of psychopath as adap-
relevant subcomponents representing features such as tive, subclinical, non-criminal or successful [21]. Specula-
affective deficits and impulsive antisocial tendencies tive notions about the value of such traits for survival and/
(Box 1). Forensic research indicates that the impulsive or reproductive advantages are intuitively plausible. For
facets of psychopathy are particularly important in pre- example, during stressful times in our evolutionary histo-
dicting criminal activity [3], and clinicians maintain that ry, a physiological make-up that is relatively immune to
the affective deficits are fundamental to the construct, stress and anxiety may prevent the development of post-
distinguishing psychopathy from other etiological paths traumatic stress disorder. Likewise, a biology that craves
to criminality [9]. An understanding of the nature of psy- novelty and a changing environment might have encour-
chopathy has become an increasingly urgent need among a aged migration and a wider search for resources.
diverse set of researchers owing to the mental health, History and literature have provided us with examples
Corresponding author: Kiehl, K.A. (kkiehl@unm.edu).
seemingly consistent with psychopathy across cultures, yet
52 1364-6613/$ see front matter ! 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.008 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

Box 1. (Sub)types of psychopathy


There is a long history of dividing the symptoms of psychopathy into Measuring psychopathy and its subfactors
subtypes or classifying different types of psychopaths. Some of the Several tools have been used to operationalize psychopathy in
more prominent divisions are described below. experimental settings, but Hares Psychopathy Checklist-Revised
(PCL-R) [16,17] is the most widely validated and most popular. The
Primary versus secondary psychopathy PCL-R was developed for use in forensic settings, comprises 20 items
Karpman suggested that primary psychopathy is the consequence of (Table 1), each scored on a two-point scale, and has a recommended
an intrinsic idiopathic deficit and that secondary psychopathy is the cutoff score of 30 (out of 40) for designation of psychopathy in
result of indirect factors (i.e. trauma exposure) [84,85]. The distinction forensic or clinical settings. Unfortunately, neuroimaging studies
has evolved somewhat and it is often suggested that primary often use different group cutoffs, raising concerns about compar-
psychopaths are characterized by lower anxiety and poverty of ability across samples.
emotional expression, and tend to commit crimes that are fundamen- Several other tools have been developed for use in non-incarcer-
tally instrumental in nature; conversely, secondary psychopaths are ated samples, such as Hares Self-Report Psychopathy Scale [89],
more anxious, show more emotional volatility, and commit more Levensons Self-Report Psychopathy Scale [90] and the Psychopathic
impulsive, reactionary crimes [86]. The symptoms are more or less Personality Inventory [91], each of which has particular strengths and
the same for primary versus secondary psychopathy, with the groups weaknesses [92]. The psychopathic deviate scale of the Minnesota
only differing in anxiety. Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) [93] has also been used;
however, it does not correlate well with PCL-R scores [89]. Owing in
Successful and unsuccessful part to the modest correlations between expert rater devices such as
Criminal activity, although a common correlate of psychopathy, is the PCL-R and these self-report measures, considerable debate exists
not a necessary component for its definition [1,9]. The putative regarding whether these devices measure the same construct. Thus,
successful (or adaptive or non-criminal) psychopath, while posses- readers of this literature should pay careful attention to the
sing the core personality traits associated with psychopathy, either assessment procedure used.
refrains from traditional criminal activity or possesses resources that Nevertheless, factor analyses of these measures typically support at
allows him/her to avoid conviction [87]. It is unclear whether these least two underlying subcomponents of psychopathy, most com-
subtypes are primarily distinguished by fundamental differences in monly corresponding to affective deficits and impulsive/antisocial
the brain (or genetics) or by protective environmental factors such as traits [94,95]. More complex, alternative solutions have been sug-
socioeconomic status, IQ, education and parenting. It is also unclear gested [17,96]; even so, each of these alternative solutions has
whether successful is a useful term [88], because psychopathic components that distinguish impulsive and antisocial behavior from
traits, at least at the clinical level, are associated with impairment in emotional deficits. It is important to recognize that a DSM-IV
multiple domains of life, including interpersonal problems at home, diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) is most strongly
work and school and with extended family, and general impairments related to the impulsive/antisocial factor of the PCL-R, and ASPD has
in moral sensibility. Investigations that make use of this character- been highly criticized as being synonymous with criminality, all but
ization often rely on self-reported criminal convictions as a ignoring the affective traits that most consider central to the construct
distinguishing factor; however, this would not account for the full of psychopathy [15]. Thus, ASPD and psychopathy as assessed by the
range of impairments associated with psychopathy, nor would it PCL-R are not the same constructs and have little overlap. It is crucial
account for patterns of antisocial deviance not resulting in criminal that readers of the literature consider the assessment procedure for
conviction. psychopathy when reviewing and interpreting results.

Table 1. The 20 items listed in the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) [16,17]a


Item Two-factor model Three-factor model Four-factor model
1 Glibness or superficial charm 1 1 1
2 Grandiose sense of self-worth 1 1 1
3 Need for stimulation 2 3 3
4 Pathological lying 1 1 1
5 Conning or manipulative 1 1 1
6 Lack of remorse or guilt 1 2 2
7 Shallow affect 1 2 2
8 Callous or lack of empathy 1 2 2
9 Parasitic lifestyle 2 3 3
10 Poor behavioral controls 2 4
11 Promiscuous sexual behavior
12 Early behavioral problems 2 4
13 Lack of realistic long-term goals 2 3 3
14 Impulsivity 2 3 3
15 Irresponsibility 2 3 3
16 Failure to accept responsibility 1 2 2
17 Many marital relationships
18 Juvenile delinquency 2 4
19 Revocation of conditional release 2 4
20 Criminal versatility 4
a
Items corresponding to the early two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy [94,98], the subsequent three-factor model [96] and the current four-factor model are listed
[17]. The two-factor model labels are interpersonal/affective (factor 1) and social deviance (factor 2). The three-factor model labels are arrogant and deceitful interpersonal
style (factor 1), deficient affective experience (factor 2), and impulsive and irresponsible behavioral style (factor 3). The four-factor model labels are interpersonal (factor 1),
affective (factor 2), lifestyle (factor 3) and antisocial (factor 4). Items with did not load on any factor.

53
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

the full clinical manifestation of psychopathy occurs in less some important differences. Both theories implicate com-
than 1% of the general population [17]. Selective promotion ponents of the limbic system, a network of brain regions
of these traits may be limited because those recognized for supporting the utilization of emotional information in
these traits have consistently been ostracized, shunned behavioral regulation. Blairs model has primarily empha-
and punished within social systems that rely on coopera- sized dysfunction in the amygdala leading to the develop-
tion to thrive [19], a stigma pervasive across cultures [22]. ment of psychopathy. The amygdala is integral in forming
The discussion of what should appropriately be called a associations between environmental cues and affective
mental health disorder is a familiar one among other states and the activation of basic threat circuits. More
psychiatric designations that probably occur on a continu- recently, Blair emphasized the role of both the amygdala
um. It is important to recognize that, similar to other and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in ongoing moni-
psychiatric conditions, moderate expressions of these toring of behavior against established reinforcement ex-
traits may be advantageous in certain contexts; however, pectancies [30]. This expansion of the model accounts for
in its full clinical manifestation, psychopathy represents a distinguishable forms of antisocial deviance observed in
maladaptive condition promoting severe functional deficits psychopathy.
including impaired passive avoidance learning [23] and Kiehls paralimbic dysfunction model [11], relative to
aversive conditioning [24], combined with the significant Blairs model, describes more widely distributed abnormal-
consequences of impulsive risk-taking and probable dis- ities throughout the brains of psychopaths. This model is
advantages for social integration. A great deal of variation grounded in the study of cytoarchitectonics [31,32], which
exists in subclinical manifestations of psychopathic traits, has identified closely related regions of the brain based on
so careful consideration should be given to research in similarities in neuronal type, structure and density. The
which subjects do not meet full criteria for the disorder. anterior superior temporal gyrus (temporal pole), anterior
Without explicit consideration of this variability, subtle cingulate, posterior cingulate, orbitofrontal cortex, insula
differences in experimental outcomes are likely to contrib- and parahippocampal regions are intimately connected to
ute to inconsistencies and confusion. Despite some vari- primary limbic regions including the amygdala, septal
ability in such methodological issues, a general consistency region and substantia innominata. The paralimbic system
has begun to emerge in the outcomes of neuroimaging refers to primary limbic structures and this extended
investigations of psychopathy. Recent reports indicate network of brain regions that provide a transition from
dysfunction in limbic or paralimbic brain regions associat- subcortical structures to higher neocortical regions
ed with the integration of emotional information into (Figure 1). Kiehls model accounts for evidence that psy-
higher-order cognitive processes. The current review high- chopaths present with abnormalities in areas beyond pri-
lights the most recent of this literature and applies this to mary limbic structures such as the anterior and posterior
existing models of psychopathy. cingulate cortex, the temporal pole, insula and parahippo-
campal gyrus [11,33].
Neurobiological models of psychopathy A third model deserves attention here, although it may be
Determination of the physiological correlates of psychopa- better described as a cognitive model of psychopathy. New-
thy has been a concern of empirical research since at least mans response modulation hypothesis suggests that appar-
the 1950s, when it was recognized that psychopaths fail to ent deficits in the processing of emotional information by
show appropriate autonomic responses to aversive stimuli psychopaths may be attributable to problems in switching
[25]. Over the years, many models of psychopathy have attention to stimuli with emotional salience or peripheral
been suggested, which we summarize below. stimuli in general [34]. While the attention of healthy
A common thread among models of psychopathy has individuals is occupied automatically and involuntarily by
emphasized abnormalities in the integration of emotional stimuli that are relevant to species safety and survival, some
response into behavior, essentially recognizing aversive evidence suggests that psychopaths have deficiencies in this
situations and acting to avoid them. This is a prominent allocation of attention, which adversely impacts the inte-
feature of Lykkens low fear hypothesis [26], which sug- gration of this information into ongoing behavioral modifi-
gests that psychopaths (specifically primary psychopaths; cation. Primary support for this notion comes in the form of
Box 1) have a subdued fear response, something that diminished performance in passive avoidance learning
ordinarily promotes avoidance of dangerous or embarras- tasks [35], diminished interference in a primary task due
sing situations in healthy individuals. Likewise, using to peripherally presented distractors [36] and abnormal
Grays two-factor reinforcement sensitivity theory [27], modification of startle reflexes in psychopaths [37]. If their
Fowles suggested that psychopaths have a weak behavior- capacity to attend to emotional information is compromised,
al inhibition system, leaving a relatively unconstrained this may help to explain a fundamental reason behind the
behavioral activation system to run amok, promoting im- apparent failure of psychopaths to integrate emotional in-
pulsive behavior [28]. Damasios somatic marker hypothe- formation into higher cognitive function. Future neuroim-
sis [29] has also been invoked to suggest that psychopaths aging research in this area is much needed because
are inadequate in their ability to utilize somatic emotional functional anatomical units supporting different modes of
cues for the purposes of anticipating punishment. attention are poorly understood.
Modern neuroimaging investigations have contributed
to the development of two prominent neurobiological theo- Recent neuroimaging and psychopathy
ries of psychopathy put forth by Blair [10] and Kiehl [11]. Escalating attention to psychopathy has been accompanied
These models share a number of attributes but also have by a meteoric rise in neuroimaging research on this topic,
54
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1
[(Figure_1)TD$IG]
available models of psychopathy and conclude that both
3 1 2
Blairs and Kiehls models continue to be supported, yet
there remains a need for greater clarity in accounting for
5
specific features of psychopathy (Box 1), which may be
8 4
6 7 addressed through careful attention to certain methodo-
9
logical details outlined below (Box 2).

40 Amygdala
39
10 46 The amygdala is located in the medial temporal lobe and
44 43
52 41 19 has an important role in the acquisition of stimulus rein-
42 18 forcement learning and the recognition of emotionally
45 22
salient information, such as the detection of threat cues
11 21 37 [41] and salient auditory or visual stimuli [42]. Because of
38
47 17 its integral role in threat detection, the amygdala was
20 thought to be dysfunctional in psychopaths even before
recent support from neuroimaging research [43]. This
3 1 2 hypothesis has been confirmed in several fMRI studies,
which have revealed that psychopathic traits are
6 4 5 associated with abnormalities in hemodynamic activity
8
in the amygdala [33]. In recent investigations, relative
7 to controls, psychopaths demonstrated lower levels of
9 24 amygdala activation when viewing pictures depicting mor-
23 31 al violations [44] and fearful faces [45]. With the use of
19 scores for psychopathy measures as continuous variables,
10 32 33
18
higher levels of psychopathy have been associated with
29 26 30 reduced amygdala activity during aversive conditioning
27 17
[46], and while viewing pictures of aversive stimuli [47].
11 25
34 18 Youths with callous/unemotional traits and conduct disor-
28 37 19 der also show lower amygdala activity when engaged in
38 35
36 passive avoidance learning [48].
20 In terms of anatomical structure, scores for psychopathy
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences measures have been associated with gray-matter volume
Figure 1. A cytoarchitectonic map of the human brain [31]. This map divides reductions in the amygdala. In a large-scale assessment
regions of the brain according to similarity in the types and density of neurons. For involving nearly 300 incarcerated subjects, Ermer and
example, primary visual (17), auditory (41), and motor (4) regions have similar
colleagues reported reduced volumes in the amygdala,
neuronal organization. Prefrontal and parietal cortex are also similar in structure.
Paralimbic regions include the amygdala (34), orbital frontal cortex (25, 47), along with several other regions discussed further below
anterior (32, 33, 24) and posterior cingulate (23, 26, 29, 30, 31), temporal pole (38), [49]. This effect has also been demonstrated in those with
parahippocampal area (27, 28, 35, 37) and insula (not depicted).
criminal records compared to those without self-reported
criminal records (but who reported that they engaged in as
partly because of rapidly improving technology and meth- much criminal behavior as those with records) [50]. Still
odological advances. This trend reveals a pressing need for others have found this effect more reliably associated with
frequent review and synthesis of this information, along affective/interpersonal facets of psychopathy than with
with careful consideration of methodological variation. implusive/behavioral symptoms [51]. Comparing psycho-
Recent reviews indicate that the most common struc- pathic criminal offenders to controls, Boccardi et al. exam-
tural and functional abnormalities observed in the brains ined specific anatomical nuclei of the amygdaloid complex
of psychopaths are distributed in frontal and temporal and found significant reductions in the basolateral amyg-
cortical regions [3840]. Particularly relevant are core dalae of psychopaths [52]. The basolateral nucleus shares
limbic structures of the medial temporal lobe, such as reciprocal connections with the orbitofrontal cortex and is
the hippocampus and amygdala, along with specific por- thought to be important for updating reinforcement value
tions of the frontal lobe including the ventromedial/orbito- [53,54]. Boccardi et al. also found that central and lateral
frontal and frontopolar regions. Other functional units that nuclei were enlarged in psychopaths compared to controls.
are commonly implicated in psychopathy are the superior The central and lateral nuclei are integral components of
temporal cortex, cingulate cortex, striatum and insula. The the basic threat circuit and are strongly implicated in fear
most recent data generally support these previous find- conditioning [54,55]. It should be noted that in this partic-
ings. Furthermore, recent research has begun to identify ular sample, all the psychopaths also met criteria for
with greater specificity the subtypes and facets of psycho- substance abuse, but none of the controls did, leaving open
pathy distinctly related to these neural abnormalities. the possibility for substance use as an intervening factor.
Here we address the most recent data attributing differ- In by far the largest sample used to examine these fea-
ences in psychopaths to three broad functional units: the tures, Ermer and colleagues still found reductions in amyg-
amygdala, prefrontal cortex and extended paralimbic dala volumes after controlling for substance, brain size and
structures. We discuss these findings in the context of age [49].
55
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

Box 2. Methodological concerns


Inherent in the rapid growth of both neuroimaging and psychopathy Categorical and dimensional analysis Psychopathy may be viewed as
research is a great deal of variability in methodological practices, existing on a continuum, that is, some individuals are more
which can drastically influence outcomes and adversely affect psychopathic than others. Historically, most investigations of
coherence between studies. We summarize below a list of methodo- psychopathy have been carried out in a categorical fashion;
logical issues that require attention. For a more detailed review, see however, there has been an increasing trend to implement contin-
Koenigs et al. [97]. uous analyses. Our preference is to report both continuous and
Assessment tools Several measures have been developed to assess grouped analyses.
psychopathy, ranging from structured interviews designed to be Comorbidity and other intervening variables Psychopathy very often
carried out in forensic settings by trained experts to self-report occurs together with substance abuse, and care should be taken to
measures designed to be implemented in normal-range community control for this as a mediating factor, particularly in physiological
samples. Debate persists over the consistency of the construct across outcomes. Other common subject variables that may need to be
measures. Readers of the literature must pay attention to the controlled for are intelligence, level of education, socioeconomic
psychopathy assessment procedure used because the different status, gender, and age.
measures may not agree. Multiple comparisons Neuroimaging investigations are particularly
Cutoffs Some assessment tools have recommended cutoffs for prone to type I errors if care is not taken to control for the large
designating psychopathy; for instance, the PCL-R recommends a number of multiple comparisons inherent in this field. This has
cutoff of 30 for incarcerated samples. These thresholds are often not become an important standard in empirical reporting of these data,
adhered to in attempts to increase the number of purported but one that has not always been adhered to.
psychopaths in a particular sample. This raises concerns regarding Redundant samples Neuroimaging provides rich data sets that often
computation of effect sizes and comparisons across studies. allow for investigation and reporting of several distinct hypotheses
Factors and subtypes The construct of psychopathy may be divided using the same subjects; however, investigators do not always make
into several relevant subcomponents including, but not limited to, it clear that a given report utilizes the same subjects as a previous
comparisons of successful and unsuccessful psychopaths, primary report. This can artificially reinforce effects investigated across
and secondary psychopaths, and factor elements such as affective multiple studies, particularly in large-scale reviews and in meta-
traits and antisociality. Careful attention should be given to the analyses.
precise comparisons being used in reports of a given effect. Small sample size Neuroimaging investigations require considerable
Supression effects Factor 1 (affective) and factor 2 (impulsive/ resources and thus reports have often been published for very small
antisocial) elements of psychopathy are correlated with one another; sample sizes. Generalization of effects is virtually impossible for very
however, evidence suggests that these factors may have opposing small sample sizes because the idiosyncratic characteristics of small
effects on some physiological outcomes. Failure to account for groups are less likely to average out across a larger number of
these components separately may, in some cases, dilute valuable subjects. The consequences of this are often apparent in their lack of
effects of interest. replicability.

The amygdala remains a prominent fixture in the in- The orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal cortex
vestigation of neural underpinnings of psychopathy. Most remains the most common prefrontal region implicated
contemporary models of psychopathy, including the Kiehl in recent neuroimaging investigations of psychopathy.
and Blair models, acknowledge the involvement of the Reductions in orbitofrontal gray matter have been consis-
amygdala in the development of psychopathic traits, and tently observed in comparing psychopaths to non-psycho-
the most recent neuroimaging data continue to support paths [52,60,61], along with reductions in the most
existing models. What remain to be shown are the reasons anterior frontopolar regions of the prefrontal cortex
why the amygdala may fail to show appropriate activation [60,61]. It has also been reported that cortical thickness
in psychopaths. It could be a physical deficiency in the in the orbitofrontal region of psychopaths is inversely
ability of the amygdala to encode certain forms of salient related to response perseveration, a classic behavioral
information, but it remains possible that it is a failure in correlate of psychopathy [62]. The orbitofrontal cortex of
other networks governing automated attention to salient those with high psychopathic traits and criminal convic-
information that are impaired. Further investigations into tions shows reduced gray-matter volume and thickness
these possibilities should be pursued. compared to those without self-reported criminal convic-
tions [50].
Prefrontal cortex Functional imaging studies examining the orbitofrontal
Areas of the prefrontal cortex are important for monitoring cortex are consistent with structural findings. Using a
ongoing behavior, estimating consequences and incorpo- prisoners dilemma task, which involves complex decisions
rating emotional learning into decision-making [56,57]. about social cooperation, psychopaths evince lower orbito-
Like the amygdala, the prefrontal cortex has featured frontal activity when choosing to cooperate and lower
prominently in theories of psychopathy. Before recent dorsolateral prefrontal activity when choosing to defect
neuroimaging evidence emerged, the prefrontal cortex than do non-psychopaths [46]. Reduced orbitofrontal activ-
was implicated in psychopathy because of indications that ity has also been observed in adolescents with callous-
damage here is capable of instigating patterns of poor unemotional traits and conduct disorder during reinforce-
moral judgment and impulsivity similar to those observed ment stages of a passive avoidance learning task [48]. In an
in psychopathy [58]. Recent evidence suggests that damage emotional Simon paradigm, which depends on the integra-
to the ventromedial portion of the prefrontal cortex is also tion of emotional information into ongoing behavioral out-
associated with specific types of utilitarian moral judg- comes, adult criminal psychopaths demonstrated no
ments (e.g. sacrificing one life to save two) [59], which prefrontal cortical activation in scenarios of emotional
exemplifies the type of non-empathic rationality that char- integration [63]. Similarly, contrary to controls, medial
acterizes decision-making by psychopaths. prefrontal areas in psychopaths were inactive when
56
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

retaliating against an opponent during a competitive rein- Contemporary techniques for whole-brain structural
forcement task; however, psychopaths demonstrated rela- analysis have revealed abnormalities in many of these
tively increased activity in this region when observing an key regions. For instance, Boccardi and colleagues used
opponent being punished, but this was specifically associ- cortical distance mapping and radial distance mapping to
ated with impulsivity and antisocial behavior on Hares measure precise regions of interest in psychopaths [52]. In
measure [64]. Reduced activity in the medial prefrontal addition to differences in the orbitofrontal cortex and
cortex has also been observed while psychopaths were amygdala, they found volume reductions in the anterior
engaged in moral reasoning [65] and viewing pictures cingulate, parahippocampal gyrus and superior frontal
depicting moral violations [44,47]. Furthermore, complex gyrus, along with smaller volumes in the primary visual
relationships between specific facets of psychopathy and cortex and precuneus, which plays a role in episodic mem-
activation patterns in the orbitofrontal cortex and amyg- ory and self-referential experiences [68]. In related work,
dala while viewing pictures of facial affect have been this group found that abnormal morphological features of
suggested [45]. the hippocampus were correlated with psychopathy [69].
Not all tasks induce reduced prefrontal activity in psy- Others have reported relative tissue reductions in the
chopaths; for instance, it has been reported that healthy temporal pole [70]. In a large-scale assessment involving
individuals engage the mirror neuron system (supramar- nearly 300 incarcerated subjects, Ermer and colleagues
ginal and superior frontal gyrus) while attributing emo- reported that associations between psychopathy and de-
tional states to others, whereas psychopaths conversely creased gray matter in the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex,
engage the orbitofrontal and medial frontal cortex and the posterior cingulate, parahippocampal region and the tem-
temporoparietal junction [66]. This may indicate that poral pole [49], all components of the paralimbic system.
structures commonly labeled as dysfunctional in psycho- This study importantly quantified and controlled for mod-
paths are not necessarily inoperative, but may simply be erating variables including substance abuse, age and rela-
utilized differently, processing information under abnor- tive brain size, and included over 40 individuals who scored
mal contexts. This also highlights the importance of in the clinical range of psychopathy (PCL-R score !30 out
attending to task requirements in the evaluation of func- of 40), and thus stands as a good example of the methodo-
tional imaging reports, because ostensible abnormalities logical rigor needed to advance the field. It represents a
are only interpretable with regard to the cognitive function significant leap forward in our understanding of the neural
a particular task is accessing. systems underlying the full manifestation of psychopathy
In sum, the most recent data support longstanding claims and highlights the need to control for potentially moderat-
that psychopaths present with irregular structural features ing or confounding variables.
of the prefrontal cortex accompanied by atypical functional A number of studies have examined relationships be-
activity in prefrontal regions during various tasks, often tween widespread structural features of the brain and the
marked by hypofunctioning in the ventromedial/orbitofron- individual factor elements of psychopathy. For instance,
tal cortex. Again here, contemporary models of psychopathy the primary affective deficits of psychopathy have been
including Blairs and Kiehls generally acknowledge these linked to expansive reductions in gray-matter thickness in
abnormalities, implicating these regions in the apparent the anterior and medial temporal lobe [71], as well as
deficits in updating reinforcement values, appraising emo- reductions in the superior temporal regions and the insula
tional states for behavioral regulation and engaging in [61]. Relatively larger striatum volumes have also been
moral evaluations by psychopaths. A recent meta-analysis linked to component factors of psychopathy, because cau-
by Yang and Raine [67] emphasizes a specific link between date body volume was reportedly associated with affective
the antisocial facets of psychopathy and prefrontal abnor- facets and caudate head volume was more consistently
malities, and the most recent research continues to report associated with impulsivity and stimulation-seeking
rather complex relationships in this regard. In the future, [72]. The striatum as a whole plays an important role in
more rigorous attention to these elements may help to reinforcement learning [73], which is a potential neuroan-
specify such relationships with greater clarity. atomical basis for the increased reward sensitivity ob-
served in psychopaths [23].
Extended paralimbic and additional structures Abnormal task-specific functional activation patterns in
The amygdala and areas of the prefrontal cortex have well- psychopaths are also commonly found in widespread
understood relationships with emotional learning and the regions of the brain, echoing some of these structural
estimation of rewards and consequences, but several other findings. While engaging in moral dilemmas, psychopaths
brain regions with subtler supportive roles in these pro- exhibited reduced activation in the hippocampal formation
cesses have also been shown to be abnormal in psychopaths and posterior cingulate, in addition to the medial prefron-
[11]. The parahippocampal gyrus, temporal pole, insula tal cortex and amygdala [65]. Other reports indicate that
and cingulate cortex (both anterior and posterior) have psychopathic traits are associated with reduced cingulate
been emphasized by Kiehl as particularly relevant to cortex activity during defection in the prisoners dilemma
psychopathy, in addition to the amygdala and orbitofrontal task [46] and exaggerated activity in the mesolimbic re-
cortex. The most recent data have continued to support the ward system (ventral striatum) in response to a psychos-
indication of dysfunction beyond primary limbic structures timulant [74]. Reduced anterior temporal cortex activity
in psychopaths, which may suggest impairments carried was also observed in criminal psychopaths while they were
up in a hierarchical fashion from primary limbic regions or looking at pictures depicting moral violations [44]. Fur-
broader cognitive impairments in general. thermore, several investigations examining youths with
57
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

psychopathic traits revealed abnormal function in the Several neurobiological models of psychopathy agree
superior temporal gyrus [48,7577], superior frontal gyrus on the core functional elements involved, but differ mainly
[75,77], cingulate [76] and the parahippocampal gyrus in the scope of their focus. Blair currently favors a model
[48,76]. proposing dysfunction of the amygdala and orbital frontal
Some neurobiological models of psychopathy, such as cortex [30], whereas Kiehls model also includes other
Blairs, have limited the scope of emphasis to the amygdala closely related paralimbic structures (Figure 1) [11]. As
and orbitofrontal cortex; however, accumulating evidence more neuroimaging evidence accumulates, it seems less
indicates that functional abnormalities also occur in other likely that deficits in psychopaths are confined to the
paralimbic structures and extended regions that share amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. The extant variety
close connections with the limbic system and that contrib- in brain regions implicated also leaves open the possibility
ute to a broader range of cognitive functions. Owing to the of multiple neurodevelopmental pathways leading to sim-
intimate connections between these structures, abnormal ilar behavioral outcomes. A limitation of current models
functional activity in the extended paralimbic network may be that they primarily rely on personality traits and
may be partially attributable to primary dysfunction in behavioral styles to operationalize a construct for which
core limbic regions. However, considerable evidence sug- we are trying to describe a specific neurobiological
gests that damage to these regions can produce some origin, when physical problems in a number of brain
symptoms congruent with psychopathy, such as impair- regions which may occur independently could poten-
ments in affective recognition following lesions of the tially manifest as symptoms consistent with familiar
anterior cingulate [78], disruptions in social exchange psychopathic traits. Although the Blair and Kiehl models
reasoning following lesions to the temporal pole [79], are both supported by the data reviewed here, these still
and abnormal risky decision-making after damage to the ultimately describe correlational relationships between
insula [80]. As this evidence accumulates, it seems clear behavior, neural structure and functional activity. Deter-
that continued attention should be paid to these functional mination of a causal directional relationship between
units, and future investigations of psychopathy should biology and behavior or personality is more elusive, but
devise tasks to parse out the variable activity in these emerging research in determining path models of connec-
regions. What may be necessary to clarify the roles of these tivity and genetic analyses is a promising enterprise in
anatomical components in psychopathy is the development this regard.
of detailed models of connectivity and path models empha- With respect to the heritability of psychopathy [12], a
sizing the specific influence that these regions exert on one promising resource for future investigations will be neu-
another. rogenetic analyses that seek out allelic (and perhaps epi-
genetic) variations that determine structural and
Concluding remarks neurochemical abnormalities, which in turn contribute
The neuroscience of psychopathy is a field undergoing to divergent brain activity already reported in neuroimag-
rapid growth and, like any field in its nascent stages, is ing studies. This would certainly provide resources for
vulnerable to methodological inconsistencies and subse- devising more focused treatment and intervention strate-
quent interpretive variation. Particularly relevant are gies. Along these lines, Meyer-Lindenberg and colleagues
issues surrounding the operationalization of psychopathy, found evidence that a low-expression variant polymor-
such as the assessment tools implemented and the cutoffs phism influencing monoamine oxidase (MAOA-L) was
used to designate psychopathy. Other issues include vari- linked to structural volume reductions in the amygdala,
ation in control groups, proper statistical control over cingulate cortex, insula, hypothalamus and orbitofrontal
potentially confounding variables including substance cortex [81]. Furthermore, these structural abnormalities
abuse, and small sample sizes. Although standards are were associated with impulsive reactive aggression, which
still being established, we briefly summarize these issues often characterizes secondary psychopathy and the impul-
and offer a number of recommendations in Box 2. Never- sive/ antisocial facets of the construct. As evidence suggest-
theless, despite these growing pains, notable consistency is ing that psychopathy is a persistent neurocognitive
emerging in the field. disorder strongly influenced by genetic factors accumu-
The structures most commonly implicated in psychop- lates, it has become a pressing concern in forensic settings
athy are those that serve functions in the evaluation of to decide whether a designation of psychopathy is a miti-
emotional information, including basic processing of gating factor in determining responsibility and sentencing,
threat and reward contingencies, utilization of this infor- and neuroscientists are now joining the debate [82,83].
mation in the modification of ongoing behavior, and ex- In the future, two issues will be instrumental in main-
tension of these contingencies into the realm of higher taining the momentum of progress we are currently wit-
cognitive processing. Converging evidence from a number nessing in neuroimaging investigations of psychopathy
of imaging modalities and across a diverse range of popu- and in refining this knowledge to inform practical issues
lations supports the hypothesis that psychopathic traits such as treatment, intervention strategies and legal re-
are associated with abnormalities in the amygdala and sponsibility. Attention to methodological issues that con-
orbitofrontal cortex. In addition, accumulating research tribute to variability in the literature will be paramount, as
supports the implication of additional brain regions in well as application of current knowledge to more diverse
psychopathy, prominently the anterior cingulate, posteri- interdisciplinary fields including neurogenetics and legal
or cingulate, hippocampus and temporal pole (superior ethics. This knowledge will serve to inform our under-
temporal gyrus). standing of antisocial behavior, moral decision-making,
58
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

and the routine integration of emotion into rational 25 Lykken, D.T. (1957) A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. J.
Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 55, 610
thought and behavior; therefore, continued progress in
26 Lykken, D.T. (1995) The Antisocial Personalities, Lawrence Earlbaum
this field is extremely valuable. However, further progress Associates
will not be achieved through simple accumulation of data, 27 Gray, J.A. (1981) A critique of Eysencks theory of personality. In A
but will require persistence accompanied by adaptive Model for Personality (Eysenck, H.J., ed.), pp. 246276, Springer-
strategies of investigation and a commitment to the most Verlag
28 Fowles, D.C. (1980) The three arousal model: implications of Grays
rigorous experimental methods available.
two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and
psychopathy. Psychophysiology 17, 87104
References 29 Damasio, A.R. (1994) Descartes Error: Emotion, Reason, and the
1 Cleckley, H. (1941) The Mask of Sanity: An Attempt to Reinterpret the Human Brain, Avon
So-Called Psychopathic Personality, Mosby 30 Blair, R.J.R. (2007) The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex
2 Leistico, A.R. et al. (2008) A large-scale meta-analysis relating the Hare in morality and psychopathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 387392
measures of psychopathy to antisocial conduct. Law Hum. Behav. 32, 31 Brodmann, K. (1909) Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex, Smith-
2845 Gordon
3 Walters, G.D. (2003) Predicting institutional adjustment and 32 Mesulam, M.M. (2000) Behavioral neuroanatomy: large-scale
recidivism with the psychopathy checklist factor scores: a meta- networks, association cortex, frontal lobe syndromes, the limbic
analysis. Law Hum. Behav. 27, 541558 system, and hemispheric specializations, In Principles of Behavioral
4 Hemphill, J.F. et al. (1998) Psychopathy and recidivism: a review. Legal and Cognitive Neurology (2nd edn) (Mesulam, M.M., ed.), pp. 1120,
Criminol. Psychol. 3, 139170 Oxford University Press
5 Gretton, H. et al. (2001) Psychopathy and recidivism in adolescent sex 33 Kiehl, K.A. et al. (2001) Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by
offenders. Crim. Justice Behav. 28, 427449 criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance
6 Garrido, V. et al. (1995) The effectiveness in the treatment of imaging. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 677684
psychopathy: a meta-analysis. Issues Crim. Legal Psychol. 24, 5759 34 Newman, J.P. and Lorenz, A.R. (2003) Response modulation and
7 Rice, M.E. et al. (1992) An evaluation of a maximum security emotion processing: implications for psychopathy and other
therapeutic community for psychopaths and other mentally dysregulatory psychopathology. In Handbook of Affective Sciences
disordered offenders. Law Hum. Behav. 16, 399412 (Davidson, R.J. and et, al., eds), pp. 904929, Oxford University Press
8 Seto, M.C. and Barbaree, H.E. (1999) Psychopathy, treatment behavior, 35 Newman, J.P. et al. (1990) Passive avoidance in psychopaths: the
and sex offender recidivism. J. Interpers. Violence 14, 12351248 effects of reward. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 11, 11011114
9 Skeem, J.L. and Cooke, D.J. (2010) Is criminal behavior a central 36 Newman, J.P. et al. (1997) The impact of motivationally neutral cues on
component of psychopathy? Conceptual directions for resolving the psychopathic individuals: assessing the generality of the response
debate. Psychol. Assess. 22, 433445 modulation hypothesis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 106, 563575
10 Blair, R.J.R. (2006) The emergence of psychopathy: implications for the 37 Newman, J.P. et al. (2010) Attention moderates the fearlessness of
neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition psychopathic offenders. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 6670
101, 414442 38 Blair, R.J.R. (2010) Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial
11 Kiehl, K.A. (2006) A cognitive neuroscience perspective on behavior: a targeted review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 12, 7682
psychopathy: evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry 39 Harenski, C. et al. (2010) Neurodevelopmental bases of psychopathy: a
Res. 142, 107128 review of brain imaging studies. In Responsibility and Psychopathy:
12 Larsson, H. et al. (2006) A genetic factor explains most of the variation Interfacing Law, Psychiatry and Philosophy (Malatesti, L. and
in the psychopathic personality. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 221230 McMillan, J., eds), pp. 125154, Oxford University Press
13 American Psychiatric Association (1968) Diagnostic and Statistical 40 Muller, J.L. (2010) Psychopathy an approach to neuroscientific
Manual of Mental Disorders, (2nd edn), American Psychiatric research in forensic psychiatry. Behav. Sci. Law 28, 129147
Association 41 Davis, M. and Whalen, P.J. (2001) The amygdala: vigilance and
14 American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical emotion. Mol. Psychiatry 6, 1334
Manual of Mental Disorders, (4th edn, text revision), American 42 Kiehl, K.A. et al. (2005) An adaptive reflexive processing model of
Psychiatric Association neurocognitive function supporting evidence from a large scale (n =
15 Cunningham, M.D. and Reidy, T.J. (1998) Antisocial personality 100) fMRI study of an auditory oddball task. Neuroimage 25, 899915
disorder and psychopathy: diagnostic dilemmas in classifying 43 Patrick, C.J. et al. (1993) Emotion in the criminal psychopath: startle
patterns of antisocial behavior in sentencing evaluations. Behav. reflex modulation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 102, 8292
Sci. Law 16, 333351 44 Harenski, C.L. et al. (2010) Aberrant neural processing of moral
16 Hare, R.D. (1991) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, Multi- violations in criminal psychopaths. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 863874
Health Systems 45 Dolan, M.C. and Fullam, R.S. (2009) Psychopathy and functional
17 Hare, R.D. (2003) The Hare Psychopathy Checklist, (2nd edn revised), magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level-dependent
Multi-Health Systems responses to emotional faces in violent patients with schizophrenia.
18 Viding, E. et al. (2005) Evidence for substantial genetic risk for Biol. Psychiatry 66, 570577
psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 592597 46 Rilling, J.K. et al. (2007) Neural correlates of social cooperation and
19 Glenn, A.L. et al. (2011) Evolutionary theory and psychopathy. Aggress. non-cooperation as a function of psychopathy. Biol. Psychiatry 61,
Violent Behav. 16, 371380 12601271
20 Babiak, P. et al. (2010) Corporate psychopathy: talking the walk. 47 Harenski, C.L. et al. (2009) Neuroticism and psychopathy predict brain
Behav. Sci. Law 28, 174193 activation during moral and nonmoral emotion regulation. Cogn.
21 Hall, J.R. and Benning, S.D. (2006) The successful psychopath: Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 115
adaptive and subclinical manifestations of psychopathy in the 48 Finger, E.C. et al. (2011) Disrupted reinforcement signaling in the
general population. In Handbook of Psychopathy (Patrick, C.J., orbitofrontal cortex and caudate in youths with conduct disorder or
ed.), pp. 459478, Guilford Press oppositional defiant disorder and a high level of psychopathic traits.
22 Murphy, J.M. (1976) Psychiatric labeling in cross-cultural perspective. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 152162
Science 191, 10191028 49 Ermer, E. et al. Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in criminal
23 Blair, R.J.R. et al. (2004) Passive avoidance learning in individuals psychopathy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. (in press)
with psychopathy: modulation by reward but not by punishment. Pers. 50 Yang, Y. et al. (2010) Morphological alterations in the prefrontal cortex
Indiv. Differ. 37, 11791192 and the amygdala in unsuccessful psychopaths. J. Abnorm. Psychol.
24 Birbaumer, N. et al. (2005) Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy: 119, 546554
a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51 Yang, Y. et al. (2009) Localization of deformations within the amygdala
62, 799805 in individuals with psychopathy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 986994

59
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences January 2012, Vol. 16, No. 1

52 Boccardi, M. et al. (2011) Cortex and amygdala morphology in 75 Finger, E.C. et al. (2008) Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex
psychopathy. Psychiatry Res. 193, 8592 function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning.
53 Schoenbaum, G. et al. (2003) Encoding predicted outcome and acquired Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65, 586594
value during cue sampling depends on input from basolateral 76 Marsh, A.A. (2008) Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions
amygdala. Neuron 39, 855867 in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and
54 Phelps, E.A. and LeDoux, J.E. (2005) Contributions of the amygdala to disruptive behavior disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 712720
emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 77 Jones, A.P. et al. (2009) Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys
48, 175187 with conduct problems and callous unemotional traits. Am. J.
55 LeDoux, J.E. et al. (1988) Different projections of the central Psychiatry 166, 95102
amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of 78 Hornak, J. et al. (2003) Changes in emotion after circumscribed
conditioned fear. J. Neurosci. 8, 25172529 surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain
56 Duncan, J. and Owen, A.M. (2000) Common regions of the human 126, 16911712
frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 79 Stone, V.E. et al. (2002) Reasoning about social exchange in patient
23, 475483 with bilateral limbic system damage. Pro. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99,
57 Salzman, D.C. and Fusi, S. (2010) Emotion, cognition, and mental state 1153111536
representation in amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Annu. Rev. 80 Weller, J.A. et al. (2009) The effects of insula damage on decision-
Neurosci. 33, 173202 making for risky gains and losses. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 347358
58 Anderson, S.W. et al. (2000) Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex 81 Meyer-Lindenberg, A. et al. (2006) Neural mechanisms of genetic risk
damage acquired in early childhood. Dev. Neuropsychol. 18, 281296 for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
59 Koenigs, M. et al. (2007) Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases 103, 62696274
utilitarian moral judgments. Nature 446, 908911 82 Glenn, A.L. et al. (2011) Is it wrong to criminalize and punish
60 Tiihonen, J. et al. (2008) Brain anatomy of persistent violent offenders: psychopaths? Emot. Rev. 3, 302304
more rather than less. Psychiatry Res. 163, 201212 83 Harenski, C. et al. (2010) Neuroimaging, genetics, and psychopathy:
61 de Oliveira-Souza, R. et al. (2008) Psychopathy as a disorder of the implications for the legal system. In Responsibility and Psychopathy:
moral brain: fronto temporo-limbic grey matter reductions Interfacing Law, Psychiatry & Philosophy (Malatesti, L. and McMillan,
demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage 40, J., eds), pp. 125154, Oxford University Press
12021213 84 Karpman, B. (1941) On the need for separating psychopathy in to two
62 Yang, Y. et al. (2011) Abnormal structural correlates of response distinct clinical subtypes: symptomatic and idiopathic. J. Criminol.
perseveration in individuals with psychopathy. J. Neuropsychiatry Psychopathol. 3, 112137
Clin. Neurosci. 23, 107110 85 Lykken, D.T. (1995) The Antisocial Personalities, Lawrence Erlbaum
63 Muller, J.L. et al. (2008) Disturbed prefrontal and temporal brain 86 Skeem, J. et al. (2007) Two subtypes of psychopathic violent offenders
function during emotion and cognition interaction in criminal that parallel primary and secondary variants. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116,
psychopathy. Behav. Sci. Law 26, 131150 395409
64 Veit, R. et al. (2010) Aberrant social and cerebral responding in a 87 Gao, Y. and Raine, A.D. (2010) Successful and unsuccessful
competitive reaction time paradigm in criminal psychopaths. psychopaths: a neurobiological model. Behav. Sci. Law 28, 194210
NeuroImage 49, 33653372 88 Ullrich, S. et al. (2008) Psychopathic personality traits and life-success.
65 Pujol, J. et al. (2011) Breakdown in the brain network subserving Pers. Indiv. Differ. 44, 11621171
moral judgment in criminal psychopathy. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 89 Hare, R.D. (1985) Comparison of procedures for the assessment of
DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsr075 psychopathy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 53, 716
66 Sommer, M. et al. (2010) In psychopathic patients emotion attribution 90 Levenson, M. et al. (1995) Assessing psychopathic attributes in a
modulates activity in outcome-related brain areas. Psychiatry Res. 182, noninstitutionalized population. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 151158
8895 91 Lilienfeld, S.O. and Widows, M. (2005) Professional Manual for the
67 Yang, Y. and Raine, A. (2009) Prefrontal structural and functional Psychopathic Personality Inventory Revised, Psychological
brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic Assessment Resources
individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 174, 8188 92 Lilienfeld, S.O. and Fowler, K.A. (2006) The self-report assessment of
68 Cavanna, A.E. and Trimble, M.R. (2006) The precuneus: a review of its psychopathy: problems, pitfalls, and promises. In Handbook of
functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564583 Psychopathy (Patrick, C.J., ed.), pp. 107132, Guilford Press
69 Boccardi, M. et al. (2010) Abnormal hippocampal shape in offenders 93 Butcher, J.N. et al. (1989) Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
with psychopathy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 438447 Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) Manual for Administration and Scoring,
70 Muller, J.L. et al. (2008) Gray matter changes in right superior University of Minnesota Press
temporal gyrus in criminal psychopaths: evidence from voxel based 94 Hare, R.D. et al. (1990) The revised psychopathy checklist: reliability
morphometry. Psychiatry Res. 163, 213222 and factor structure. Psychol. Assess. 2, 338341
71 Yang, Y. et al. (2009) Abnormal temporal and prefrontal cortical gray 95 Benning, S.D. et al. (2003) Factor structure of the psychopathic
matter thinning in psychopaths. Mol. Psychiatry 14, 561562 personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical
72 Glenn, A.L. et al. (2010) Increased volume of the striatum of assessment. Psychol. Assess. 15, 340350
psychopathic individuals. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 5258 96 Cooke, D.J. and Michie, C. (2001) Refining the construct
73 Haruno, M. et al. (1994) A neural correlate of reward-based behavioral of psychopathy: towards a hierarchical model. Psychol. Assess. 13,
learning in caudate nucleus: a functional magnetic resonance imaging 171188
study of a stochastic decision task. J. Neurosci. 24, 16601665 97 Koenigs, M. et al. (2011) Investigating the neural correlates of
74 Buckholtz, J.W. et al. (2010) Mesolimbic dopamine reward system psychopathy: a critical review. Mol. Psychiatry 16, 792799
hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nat. 98 Harpur, T.J. et al. (1988) Factor structure of the psychopathy checklist.
Neurosci. 13, 419421 J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 56, 741747

60

S-ar putea să vă placă și