Sunteți pe pagina 1din 5

Dysfunction in the Neural Circuitry of Emotion

Regulation--A Possible Prelude to Violence


Richard J. Davidson, et al.
Science 289, 591 (2000);
DOI: 10.1126/science.289.5479.591

The following resources related to this article are available online at


www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of November 23, 2006 ):

Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online
version of this article at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/289/5479/591
A list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites related to this article can be

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 23, 2006


found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/289/5479/591#related-content
This article cites 48 articles, 14 of which can be accessed for free:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/289/5479/591#otherarticles

This article has been cited by 242 article(s) on the ISI Web of Science.

This article has been cited by 56 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see:
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/289/5479/591#otherarticles

Information about obtaining reprints of this article or about obtaining permission to reproduce
this article in whole or in part can be found at:
http://www.sciencemag.org/help/about/permissions.dtl

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the
American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright
c 2005 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title SCIENCE is a
registered trademark of AAAS.
VIOLENCE
REVIEW

Dysfunction in the Neural Circuitry of Emotion


RegulationA Possible Prelude to Violence
Richard J. Davidson,* Katherine M. Putnam, Christine L. Larson

Emotion is normally regulated in the human brain by a complex circuit lence, the increase in OFC and ACC acti-
consisting of the orbital frontal cortex, amygdala, anterior cingulate vation usually observed in such conditions
cortex, and several other interconnected regions. There are both genetic would be attenuated.
and environmental contributions to the structure and function of this A large corpus of animal studies has ex-
circuitry. We posit that impulsive aggression and violence arise as a amined the anatomical and neurochemical
consequence of faulty emotion regulation. Indeed, the prefrontal cortex substrates of fear-potentiated startle reflexes
receives a major serotonergic projection, which is dysfunctional in indi- in which modulation of the magnitude of the
viduals who show impulsive violence. Individuals vulnerable to faulty startle response by concomitant emotion is
regulation of negative emotion are at risk for violence and aggression. measured (21). This research is now being
Research on the neural circuitry of emotion regulation suggests new extended to humans to probe the chronometry

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 23, 2006


avenues of intervention for such at-risk populations. and regulation of emotion (2226).
The circuitry underlying this phenome-
The prevalence of violence in our society has cortex (PFC), the amygdala, hippocampus, non has been well characterized in rodents
stimulated both social and biological scientists hypothalamus, anterior cingulate cortex and includes a descending pathway from
to search for the predictors and causes of this (ACC) (8), insular cortex, ventral striatum, the central nucleus of the amygdala to the
often destructive human behavior. Here we fo- and other interconnected structures has nucleus pontine reticularis in the brain stem
cus on the nature of the affective style that been implicated in various aspects of emo- that serves as the proximal substrate of the
might predispose an individual to this aberrant tion, affective style, and emotion regulation reflex. In rodent studies, the accentuation
form of behavior. Affective style is a term that (Fig. 1). Emotion regulation includes pro- of the startle response magnitude during
refers to consistent individual differences in the cesses that amplify, attenuate, or maintain fear is produced by the amygdala amplify-
various parameters that govern emotional reac- an emotion. Here we will focus on the ing the brain stem startle circuit. Lesions of
tivity (1). We contend that the propensity for associated affective phenomena of anger, the central nucleus of the amygdala have no
impulsive aggression is associated with a low general negative affect, and impulsive ag- effect on baseline startle, but they abolish
threshold for activating negative affect (a mix- gression (3). the fear-potentiation of the startle response
ture of emotions and moods that include anger, A variety of evidence indicates that the (27 ).
distress, and agitation) and with a failure to amygdala is crucial for learning to associ- In analogous studies in humans, when
respond appropriately to the anticipated nega- ate stimuli with primary punishers and re- subjects view unpleasant pictures, there is an
tive consequences of behaving aggressively. wards (i.e., those events that are intrinsical- increase in the magnitude of the eyeblink
Social psychological research underscores ly punishing or rewarding) (10, 11). In reflex (measured from surface electromyo-
the relation between aggression and emotion. human neuroimaging studies, the amygdala graphic recording from the orbicularis oculi
Numerous studies show that negative affect can is activated in response to cues that connote muscle) in response to a brief burst of noise
precipitate and accentuate aggressive behavior threat (such as facial signs of fear) (12, 13), (28). Moreover, the magnitude of the eye-
(2, 3). In this article we feature those forms of as well as during induced fear (for example, blink reflex to the same stimulus during the
aggression that are relatively unplanned and fear conditioning) (14, 15) and generalized viewing of pleasant pictures is smaller than
spontaneous, termed impulsive aggression (4), negative affect (for example, negative af- that during the viewing of neutral pictures
which is different from premeditated aggression fect provoked by watching unpleasant pic- (28, 29).
(5). Although most neurobiological studies of tures) (16 ). Patients with selective bilateral By triggering a startle response at dif-
aggression and violence typically do not differ- damage to the amgydala have a specific ferent times during affective processing,
entiate between premeditated and impulsive ag- impairment in the recognition of fearful information about the time course of emo-
gression, this distinction is likely relevant in facial expressions (17 ). The amygdala is tion can be gleaned (2325, 30). In one
understanding their genetic, neurochemical, and more strongly activated by facial expres- experiment (25), normal subjects viewed
functional neuroanatomical bases, and conse- sions of fear than it is by other facial unpleasant or neutral pictures for 8 s. Four
quently, we have confined our discussion to expressions, including anger. For example, seconds after the picture appeared, a digi-
impulsive aggression, which often culminates increasing intensity of fearful facial expres- tized human voice instructed the subject to
in physical violence. sions is associated with activation of the regulate the emotion they were experienc-
amygdala. In contrast, increasing intensity ing in response to the picture. For unpleas-
Emotion Regulation: Core Neural of angry facial expressions is associated ant pictures, subjects were asked to sup-
Substrates with increased activation of the orbitofron- press, enhance, or maintain their emotional
As reviewed recently (6, 7 ), a circuit that tal cortex (OFC) and the ACC (18). In two response. Subjects were instructed to con-
includes several regions of the prefrontal neuroimaging studies that have attempted tinue voluntary regulation of their emotion-
to induce anger specifically (19, 20), nor- al response even after the picture disap-
mal subjects showed increased activation in peared. During and after the presentation of
Laboratory for Affective Neuroscience and W. M. Keck
Laboratory for Functional Brain Imaging and Behavior,
the OFC and ACC. These activations may the pictures, brief noise bursts were pre-
University of WisconsinMadison, Madison, WI 53706, normally be part of an automatic regulatory sented to probe the time course of the
USA. response that controls the intensity of ex- emotion before and after the instruction to
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E- pressed anger. We would expect that in regulate was presented. When subjects
mail: rjdavids@facstaff.wisc.edu. individuals prone to aggression and vio- were requested to suppress their negative

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 289 28 JULY 2000 591


VIOLENCE
affect, they showed significantly dimin- rewarded or punished stimulus (10)]. Sup- ing (f MRI) amygdala activation after sub-
ished startle magnitude during and after pressing negative affect in response to a jects were instructed to maintain a negative
unpleasant pictures compared with both the stimulus that previously aroused such emo- emotion as compared with the subjects sim-
maintain and enhance conditions (Fig. 2). tion can be conceptualized as a form of ple passive viewing of unpleasant pictures.
Most important was the fact that subjects reversal learning. Patients with lesions in We found greater activation of the amygdala
varied considerably in their skill at sup- the OFC and those vulnerable to impulsive after the picture was turned off in the group
pressing negative emotion. By inserting aggression should be particularly deficient asked to maintain that emotion as compared
startle stimuli before the instruction was in this task, although they would still show with the passive viewing condition, as the
presented, we confirmed that this variation the basic enhancement of startle magnitude model would predict (36 ).
in the ability to regulate emotions could not in response to negative stimuli. The implications of these findings are sev-
be accounted for by differences in the ini- We have proposed that the mechanism eral: First, individual differences in the ca-
tial reactivity to the negative stimuli. In underlying suppression of negative emotion pacity to regulate emotion are objectively
more recent work (26 ), we found that base- is via an inhibitory connection from regions measurable. Second, individual differences in
line levels of regional brain activation in- of the prefrontal cortex, probably the OFC, patterns of prefrontal activation predict abil-
ferred from high-density scalp-recorded to the amygdala (7 ). This proposal is based ity to perform this task and thus reflect dif-
brain electroencephalography (EEG) (31) on several lines of evidence. First, data in ferences in aspects of emotion regulation.
predicted the ability of subjects to suppress rodents show that lesions of the PFC inter- Third, individual differences in emotion reg-
emotions. Those subjects with greater rel- fere with extinction of a classically condi- ulation skills, particularly as they apply to

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 23, 2006


ative left-sided activation in prefrontal tioned aversive response [(33), but see suppression of negative affect, may be espe-
scalp regions showed greater startle atten- (34 )], implying that the PFC normally in- cially important in determining vulnerability
uation in response to the suppression in- hibits the amygdala and that, when the PFC to aggression and violence.
struction. Although the EEG methods used is lesioned, it releases the amygdala from
in this study precluded definitive localiza- this inhibition and results in much slower The Neurobiology of Anger and
tion of the sources of these signals, other extinction of aversive responses. Second, Aggression
evidence implicates the OFC in this pro- data from a positron emission tomography Disruption of the serotonin (5-HT) system
cess. Research with humans who have se- (PET) study show reciprocal relations be- has been linked to aggression and violence by
lective damage to the OFC or ventromedial tween glucose metabolism in several areas a variety of methods. Serotonin has been
PFC (32), and with nonhuman primates, of the frontal cortex (including the OFC) hypothesized to exert inhibitory control over
supports the role of these prefrontal terri- and the amygdala (35). impulsive aggression (37). Cerebrospinal flu-
tories in reversal learning [changing emo- On the basis of this reasoning, we exam- id (CSF) level of the 5-HT metabolite 5-hy-
tional behavior in response to a previously ined by functional magnetic resonance imag- droxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) is believed
to reflect presynaptic serotonergic activity in
the brain. Reduced CSF 5-HIAA has been
found in aggressive psychiatric patients (38,
39); impulsive, violent men (40, 41); and
victims of suicide by violent means (42).
Furthermore, 5-HIAA concentration has been
found to predict aggression 2 to 3 years in the
future in conduct-disordered boys (43) and
recidivous adults (44 ). Lower CSF 5-HIAA
levels have been reported in impulsive vio-
lent offenders (45) and impulsive fire setters
(46 ) than in nonimpulsive violent offenders.
Pharmacological challenge studies pro-
vide another indirect method for studying
central serotonin function. The prolactin ele-
vation in response to a single dose of a 5-HT
agonist has been used to index central 5-HT
activity. Lower prolactin responses to a 5-HT
agonist have been associated with aggressiv-
ity (38), antisocial personality disorder (47),
and suicidal behavior (38). Traits of aggres-
sion in a large normative community-derived
sample were associated with lower prolactin
responses to the 5-HT agonist fenfluramine in
men (48). The synthesis of 5-HT depends on
the availability of the amino acid tryptophan.
By limiting dietary tryptophan, brain levels
of 5-HT can be reduced (49). Tryptophan
depletion induced by consuming a beverage
Fig. 1. Key structures in the circuitry underlying emotion regulation. (A) Orbital prefrontal cortex containing a group of amino acids without
in green and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in red. (B) Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. (C)
Amygdala. (D) Anterior cingulate cortex. Each of these interconnected structures plays a role in
tryptophan increased laboratory aggression in
different aspects of emotion regulation, and abnormalities in one or more of these regions and/or normal men (50, 51) and both spontaneous
in the interconnections among them are associated with failures of emotion regulation and also and competitive aggression in monkeys (52),
increased propensity for impulsive aggression and violence. [Adapted from (9)] as compared with increasing tryptophan with

592 28 JULY 2000 VOL 289 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org


VIOLENCE
a tryptophan-containing beverage. committed affective and impulsive murder. skin conductance responses to the anger and
A polymorphism in the gene that codes The affective, impulsive murderers showed disgust expressions compared with the com-
for tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH), the rate- reductions in lateral PFC metabolism com- parison groups. In response to threatening
limiting enzyme in serotonin biosynthesis, is pared with controls, whereas the predatory objects such as pointed weapons, J.S. was
correlated with indirect measures of central group did not. Subcortical regions were av- hyporesponsive compared with other groups.
serotonergic function, as well as individual eraged together in this report and included the Thus, J.S. exhibited a strikingly specific def-
differences in aggressive behavior (53, 54 ). hippocampus, amygdala, thalamus, and mid- icit in the recognition of anger signals and in
In a study (54) with 251 community volun- brain. An increased metabolic rate was ob- inferring the emotions of others in situations
teers, the association between the A218C served in these regions in the right hemi- where anger, disgust, and embarrassment are
polymorphism in intron 7 of the TPH gene sphere in the affective, impulsive murderers usual. Facial and other expressive signs of
and interview and self-report measures of as compared with the other groups. anger function to inhibit the behavior of oth-
aggression and anger-related personality Lesions in the OFC and adjacent PFC ers in situations where social rules or expec-
traits was examined. In addition, a fenflura- regions produce syndromes characterized by tations may be violated.
mine challenge was administered to a sub- impulsivity and aggression. Anderson et al. In other related work, patients with anti-
sample. Subjects having any TPH A218C U (64 ) reported on two individuals, tested in social personality disorder who exhibit a pro-
allele scored significantly higher on several their twenties, who suffered early damage pensity for impulsive aggression exhibit an
measures of aggression, including the tenden- (ages 3 and 15 months) to orbital and lateral 11% reduction of overall prefrontal gray mat-
cy to experience unprovoked anger, than in- sectors of the PFC. Both exhibited a remark- ter volume by MRI (66 ). Interictal episodes

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 23, 2006


dividuals homozygous for the alternate able deficit in moral reasoning; a history of of impulsive aggression are sometimes ob-
A218C L allele. Among the male subjects, verbal and physical abusiveness; and inter- served in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy
prolactin response to fenfluramine was also mittent, explosive bursts of anger. Blair and (TLE). TLE patients who display such epi-
blunted in those subjects having any U allele Cipolotti (65) reported that a 56-year-old man sodes of impulsive aggressive (diagnosed as
compared with the LL homozygotes. ( J.S.), who sustained damage to the OFC having intermittent explosive disorder) have
The PFC, an important component of a bilaterally and some damage to the left amyg- a highly significant reduction (approximately
circuit critical to emotion regulation that has dala, showed unpredictable, impulsive ag- 17%) in left prefrontal gray matter as com-
been implicated in aggressive and violent gression and violence. J.S. was described by pared with TLE patients with no history of
behavior, is a region with a high density of a relative as being premorbidly a quiet, rath- aggression or controls (67 ).
serotonin type 2 receptors (55). In a study er withdrawn person who was never aggres-
designed to evaluate the functional impact of sive ( p. 1124). In a series of tasks adminis- Emotion Regulation, Aggression, and
a serotonergic challenge on regional cerebral tered to J.S., a control patient with damage to Violence: Common Neural Substrates?
glucose metabolism, Mann and his col- other sectors of the PFC, and a small group of Impulsive, affective aggression may be the
leagues (56 ) administered either fenflura- psychopathic and nonpsychopathic individu- product of a failure of emotion regulation.
mine or a placebo to normal subjects on two als, J.S.s pattern of affective processing ab- Normal individuals are able to voluntarily
separate occasions while cerebral glucose normalities was identified. He had deficits in regulate their negative affect and can also
metabolism was measured with PET. Signif- recognition of facial expression, and he was profit from restraint-producing cues in their
icant drug-induced increases in glucose met- particularly impaired relative to all other environment, such as facial and vocal signs of
abolic rate were found in left hemisphere groups in the recognition of anger and dis- anger or fear, that also serve a regulatory role.
regions of the PFC, including the inferior and gust. J.S. also produced significantly lower We suggest that individuals predisposed to
middle frontal gyrus, ventromedial PFC, and
anterior cingulate. These regions include Fig. 2. Experimental paradigm for
those with activation inversely associated investigating emotion regulation
with activation in the amygdala (35). (25). Unpleasant or neutral pic-
The increase in glucose metabolism in tures are presented during the 8-s
picture presentation period illus-
PFC and ACC regions in normal subjects in trated in yellow. At 4 s into the
response to fenfluramine is blunted or entire- picture presentation, an instruc-
ly absent in patients with aggressive impul- tion (ENHANCE, MAINTAIN, OR
sive personality disorder (57, 58). These find- SUPPRESS) is presented. Subjects
ings imply that an important site of seroto- are instructed to continue to exe-
nergic abnormality in subjects with impulsive cute the regulatory strategy until
they view the word RELAX that is presented 8 s after
aggression is the prefrontal cortex, which the picture is removed from sight. Noise bursts (95
likely plays a role in emotion regulation, dB, 50 ms, with a near instantaneous rise time) are
particularly the regulation of negative affect. presented as indicated. Probe A is presented before
Studies of regional glucose metabolism the delivery of any regulation instruction to verify the
assessed with PET (unprovoked by pharma- presence of the intended emotional state and to rule
cological challenge) also reveal prefrontal ab- out differences in initial reactivity to the emotional
pictures in subjects who differ in their regulatory
normalities in individuals prone to impulsive skills. The bar graph displays data from 43 subjects
aggression (59 63). A study of 41 murderers aggregated across all postinstruction probes in re-
(62) found hypoactivation in prefrontal terri- sponse to negative pictures. When subjects are re-
tories including lateral and medial zones of quested to suppress negative emotion, they show
the PFC, as well as hyperactivation in the reliable decreases in startle magnitude compared with
right but not the left amygdala, compared the maintain instruction. Instructions to enhance
emotion produce increases in startle magnitude com-
with age- and sex-matched controls. In a pared with the other conditions. There were wide-
subsequent reanalysis of these data (63), mur- ranging differences among subjects in the magnitude
derers were classified as those who commit- of startle attenuation in response to the SUPRESS
ted planned, predatory murder or those who instruction.

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 289 28 JULY 2000 593


VIOLENCE
aggression and violence have an abnormality ronmental influences (73). Moreover, the Psychophysiology, J. T. Cacioppo, L. Tassinary, G.
in the central circuitry responsible for these very circuitry identified here as playing a Bernston, Eds. (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York,
2000), pp. 2752.
adaptive behavioral strategies. Four key re- crucial role in emotion regulation is dra- 32. A. Bechara, H. Damasio, D. Tranel, A. R. Damasio,
gions that are components of a circuit under- matically shaped by early social influences Science 275, 1293 (1997).
lying emotion regulation are illustrated in (7 ). Interventions that combine pharmaco- 33. M. A. Morgan, L. M. Romanski, J. E. LeDoux, Neurosci.
Fig. 1. Functional or structural abnormalities logical (74 ) and psychosocial strategies Lett. 163, 109 (1993).
34. J. C. Gewirtz, W. A. Falls, M. Davis, Behav. Neurosci.
in one or more of these regions or in the will likely both operate on a common final 111, 712 (1997).
interconnections among them can increase pathway in the emotion regulatory circuitry 35. H. C. Abercrombie et al., Psychophysiology 33, 17
the propensity for impulsive aggression. The of the brain. Novel training procedures (1996).
evidence reviewed above suggests that abnor- based upon experimental paradigms now 36. S. M. Schaefer et al., Neurosci. Abstr., in press.
37. J. Volavka, J. Clin. Psychiatry 60 (suppl. 12), 43
malities in serotonin function in regions of being used to study the neural substrates of (1999).
the PFC may be especially important. Other emotion regulation (e.g., suppressing neg- 38. E. F. Coccaro, Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. 52 (1989).
neurotransmitters, neuromodulators, and hor- ative emotion) need to be developed and 39. M. Virkkunen et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 20
mones are also likely involved, including tes- then rigorously evaluated. Nevertheless, (1994).
40. M. Linnoila, J. DeJong, M. Virkkunen, Psychopharma-
tosterone (68), norepinepherine, dopamine the first important step is to recognize that col. Bull. 25, 404 (1989).
and cortiocotropin-releasing hormone (42), impulsive aggression and violence, irre- 41. A. Roy, B. Adinoff, M. Linnoila, Psychiatry Res. 24, 187
and cholesterol (69), although none of these spective of the distal cause, reflect abnor- (1988).
42. J. J. Mann, Nature Med. 4, 25 (1998).
has received much attention in human studies malities in the emotion regulation circuitry 43. M. J. Kruesi et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 429

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on November 23, 2006


of aggression and violence. of the brain. (1992).
The role of the amygdala in impulsive 44. M. Virkkunen et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 46, 600
(1989).
aggression is complex (70). Individual be- 45. M. Linnoila et al., Life Sci. 33, 2609 (1983).
References and Notes
haviors that connote threat (e.g., staring 1. R. J. Davidson, in The Cognitive Neurosciences, M. S. 46. M. Virkkunen, A. Nuutila, F. K. Goodwin, M. Linnoila,
eyes, threatening vocalization, lunging pos- Gazzaniga, Ed. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000), Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 241 (1987).
pp.1149 1159. 47. H. B. Moss, J. K. Yao, G. L. Panzak, Biol. Psychiatry 28,
ture) are conveyed to the lateral nucleus of 325 (1990).
2. L. Berkowitz, in Handbook of Cognition and Emotion,
the amygdala, which then projects to the T. Dagleish and M. Power, Eds. (Wiley, Chichester, UK, 48. S. B. Manuck et al., Neuropsychopharmacology 19,
basal nuclei, and it is there that information 287 (1998).
about the social context derived from OFC 3.
1999), pp. 411 428.
, in Handbook of Affective Sciences, R. J. Da- 49. R. J. Wurtman, F. Hefti, E. Melamed, Pharmacol. Rev.
32, 315 (1980).
vidson, K. Scherer, H. H. Goldsmith, Eds. (Oxford
projections is integrated with the perceptual Univ. Press, New York, in press). 50. J. M. Bjork et al., Psychopharmacology 142, 24
information (71, 72). Behavioral responses 4. M. Linnoila, D. S. Charney, in The Neurobiology of (1999).
can then be initiated via projections from Mental Illness, D. S. Charney, E. J. Nestler, B. S. 51. A. J. Cleare and A. J. Bond, Psychopharmacology 118,
Bunney, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1999), 72 (1995).
the basal nuclei to various cortical zones, 52. B. Chamberlain, F. R. Ervin, R. O. Pihl, S. N. Young,
pp. 855 871.
and physiological responses can be pro- 5. E. S. Barratt et al., Psychiatry Res. 86, 163 (1999). Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 28, 503 (1987).
duced via projections from the basal nuclei 6. R. J. Davidson and W. Irwin, Trends Cognit. Sci. 3, 11 53. D. A. Nielsen et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 34
(1999). (1994).
to the central nucleus and then to the hy- 54. S. B. Manuck et al., Biol. Psychiatry 45, 603 (1999).
7. R. J. Davidson, D. C. Jackson, N. H. Kalin, Psychol.
pothalamus and brainstem. Too much or Bull., in press. 55. F. Biver et al., Neurosci. Lett. 204, 25 (1996).
too little activation of the amygdala may 8. G. Bush, P. Luu, M. I. Posner, Trends Cognit. Sci. 4, 215 56. J. J. Mann et al., J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 16, 418
(2000). (1996).
give rise to either excessive negative affect 57. L. J. Siever et al., Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 413
9. S. J. DeArrnond, M. M. Fusco, M. M. Dewey, Structure
or decreased sensitivity to social cues that of the Human Brain: A Photographic Atlas (Oxford (1999).
regulate emotion, respectively. Univ. Press, New York, 3rd ed., 1989). 58. P. H. Soloff et al., Biol. Psychiatry 47, 540 (2000).
10. E. T. Rolls, The Brain and Emotion (Oxford Univ. Press, 59. N. D. Volkow et al., Psychiatry Res. 61, 243 (1995).
The evidence we have reviewed indi- 60. P. F. Goyer et al., Neuropsychopharmacology 10, 21
New York, 1999).
cates that the OFC and the structures with 11. P. C. Holland and M. Gallagher, Trends Cognit. Sci. 3, (1994).
which it is interconnected (including other 65 (1999). 61. A. Raine et al., Biol. Psychiatry 36, 365 (1994).
prefrontal territories, the ACC, and the 12. J. S. Morris et al., Nature 383, 812 (1996). 62. A. Raine, M. Buchsbaum, L. LaCasse, Biol. Psychiatry
13. P. J. Whalen et al., J. Neurosci. 18, 411 (1998). 42, 495 (1997).
amygdala) constitute the core elements of a 14. C. Buchel, J. S. Morris, R. J. Dolan, K. J. Friston, Neuron 63. A. Raine et al., Behav. Sci. Law 16, 319 (1998).
circuit that underlies emotion regulation 20, 947 (1998). 64. S. W. Anderson et al., Nature Neurosci. 2, 1032
(Fig. 1). The OFC, through its connections 15. K. S. LaBar et al., Neuron 20, 937 (1998). (1999).
with other zones of the PFC and with the 16. W. Irwin et al., Neuroreport 7, 1765 (1996). 65. R. J. Blair and L. Cipolotti, Brain 123, 1122 (2000).
17. R. Adolphs, D. Tranel, H. Damasio, A. Damasio, Nature 66. A. Raine et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 119 (2000).
amygdala, plays a crucial role in constrain- 372, 613 (1994). 67. F. G. Woermann et al., J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychi-
ing impulsive outbursts, and the ACC re- 18. R. J. Blair et al., Brain 122, 883 (1999). atry 68, 162 (2000).
cruits other neural systems, including the 19. D. D. Dougherty et al., Biol. Psychiatry 46, 466 68. J. D. Higley et al. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 1067 (1996).
(1999). 69. J. R. Kaplan et al., Psychosoma. Med. 56, 479 (1994).
PFC, in response to conflict (8). In normal 20. T. A. Kimbrell et al., Biol. Psychiatry 46, 454 (1999). 70. N. J. Emery and D. G. Amaral, in The Cognitive Neu-
individuals, activations in these brain re- 21. M. Davis, in The Amygdala, J. P. Aggleton, Ed. (Wiley- roscience of Emotion, R. D. Lane and L. Nadal, Eds.
gions that occur during anger arousal and Liss, New York, 1992), pp. 255306. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 2000), pp. 156 191.
22. S. K. Sutton et al., Psychophysiology 34, 217 (1997). 71. D. O ngur and J. L. Price, Cereb. Cortex 10, 206 (2000).
other negative emotions constrain the im- 23. J. Globisch, A. O. Hamm, F. Esteves, A. O hman, Psy- 72. C. Cavada et al., Cereb. Cortex 10, 220 (2000).
pulsive expression of emotional behavior. chophysiology 36, 66 (1999). 73. A. Raine, P. Brennan, S. A. Mednick, Am. J. Psychiatry
Deficits in this circuit are hypothesized to 24. C. L. Larson, D. Ruffalo, J. Y. Nietert, R. J. Davidson, 154, 1265 (1997).
Psychophysiology 37, 92 (2000). 74. E. F. Coccaro and R. J. Kavoussi, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
increase a persons vulnerability to impul- 25. D. C. Jackson, J. R. Malmstadt, C. L. Larson, R. J. 54, 1081 (1997).
sive aggression. Davidson, Psychophysiology, in press. 75. Supported by NIMH grants MH43454, MH40747, P50-
There are many factors that influence 26. D. C. Jackson, C. Burghy, A. Hanna, C. L. Larson, R. J. MH52354, and P50-MH61083, by Research Scientist
Davidson, unpublished data. Award K05-MH00875, by a grant from the Research
the structure and function of this circuitry. 27. M. Davis, J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9, 382 Network on Mind-Body Interaction of the John D. and
Genetic factors clearly play a role as re- (1997). Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation (R.J.D.), by a Na-
vealed by the association of the polymor- 28. S. R. Vrana, E. L. Spence, P. J. Lang, J. Abnorm. Psychol. tional Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and De-
phism of the TPH gene with traits of anger 97, 487 (1988). pression (NARSAD) Young Investigator Award (K.M.P.),
29. P. J. Lang, Am. Psychol. 50, 372 (1995). and by NRSA F31-MH12085 (C.L.L.). We thank W. Irwin,
and aggressivity (54 ). However, these fac-
tors undoubtedly interact with early envi- 31.
30. R. J. Davidson, Cognit. Emotion 12, 307 (1998).
, D. C. Jackson, C. L. Larson, in Handbook of
D. Jackson, T. Oakes, A. Jahn, J. Lindwall, C. Koepsel, and
J. Topolovich for their help with manuscript preparation.

594 28 JULY 2000 VOL 289 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org

S-ar putea să vă placă și