Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

Neurocase

ISSN: 1355-4794 (Print) 1465-3656 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nncs20

Confabulation: Remembering another past,


planning another future

Gianfranco Dalla Barba , Jee Yun Cappelletti , Matteo Signorini & Gianfranco
Denes

To cite this article: Gianfranco Dalla Barba , Jee Yun Cappelletti , Matteo Signorini & Gianfranco
Denes (1997) Confabulation: Remembering another past, planning another future, Neurocase,
3:6, 425-436, DOI: 10.1080/13554799708405018

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554799708405018

Published online: 17 Jan 2008.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 96

View related articles

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=nncs20

Download by: [Universidad Nacional Colombia] Date: 31 May 2017, At: 09:55
Neuror.u.ve (1997) Vol. 3, pp, 4 2 5 4 3 6 8C:i O.\-Sortl Utiiversitjb Press 1997

Confabulation: Remembering Another Past, Planning


Another Future

Gianfranco Dalla Barball, Jee Yun Cappelletti3, Matteo Signorini3 and Gianfranco Denes3
U. 324 INSERM, Paris, 2Department of Neurology, H6pital Henri Mondor, Creteil, France and 3Department of Neurology,
University of Padua, Italy

Abstract

In this study we describe a patient, GA, who developed an amnesic-confabulatory syndrome, following a
subarachnoid haemorrhage and ischaemia due to rupture of the anterior communicating artery and
subsequent vasospasm. As far as the performance on memory tasks was concerned, GAs confabulation
was found to be restricted to the autobiographical aspect of episodic memory. Confabulation did not
manifest itself in episodic learning tasks nor on tasks tapping various kinds of semantic knowledge. In
contrast, GA confabulated in orientation in time and place tasks and also in tasks where she was required
to plan her personal future. GAs confabulation could not be accounted for in terms of an impairment of
strategic retrieval or of reality monitoring processes. It is suggested that GAS confabulation reflects a
pathological awareness of personal temporality.

Introduction
Confabulation is a symptom, observable in amnesic Johnson, 1991; Burgess and Shallice, 1996). There is
patients who are unaware of their memory deficit. It evidence, however, that considering confabulation as a
consists of actions and verbal statements that are un- disorder confined to long-term memory is probably restric-
intentionally incongruous to the patients history, tive. In fact, confabulators usually make confabulatory
background and present situation (Dalla Barba, 1993a). errors in time and space orientation tasks (e.g. Dalla Barba
This rather infrequent disorder is classically described et uI., 1990; Dalla Barba, 1993a,b). There is also some
in Korsakoffs syndrome (BonhoefTer, 1904; Wyke and clinical evidence that confabulation can also manifest itself
Warrington, 1960; Talland, 1961; Mercer et al., 1977; when the patient is required to plan his personal future.
Cermak rt a/., 1980; Dalla Barba et al., 1990; Benson rt al., For example, Dalla Barba (1993a) reported that on one
1996; Schnider et al., 1996), but also in patients suffering occasion the patient MB said that he was looking forward
from ruptured aneurysms of the anterior communicating to the end of the testing session because he had to go to the
artery, subarachnoid haemorrhage or encephalitis (Luria, general store to buy some new clothes . . . On this occasion
1976; Stuss er al., 1978; Kapur and Coughlan, 1980; the patient actually attempted to leave his hospital room,
Alexander and Freedman, 1984; Vilkki, 1985; Moscovitch, claiming that there was a taxi waiting for him downstairs
1989; Delbecq-Derouesne et ul., 1990; Irle et ul., 1992; (p. 4). Dalla Barbas tentative conclusion is that the sub-
Kopelman et i d , 1995; Moscovitch, 1995; Papagno and jectively experienced temporality, in its continuity and
Muggia, 1996; Schnider et al., 1996; Dalla Barba et al., unity, is a crucial factor in MBs confabulation.
1997), head injury (Weinstein and Lyerly, 1968; Baddeley This paper describes the study of a patient, GA, who
and Wilson. 1986; Dalla Barba, 1993b; Schnider et id., developed an amnesic-confabulatory syndrome, following
1996), Binswangers Encephalopathy (Dalla Barba, 1993a), a subarachnoid haemorrhage and ischaemia due to rupture
Alzheimers disease (Kern et a/., 1992) and aphasia of the anterior communicating artery and subsequent
(Sandson rt al., 1986). vasospasm. GAs confabulation involved the auto-
Confabulation has traditionally been considered a biographical aspect of episodic memory, as well as her
disorder of memory and, in particular, a disorder of the performance in orientation in time and place tasks and in
retrieval or post-retrieval stage from long-term memory tasks where she was required to plan her personal future.
(e.g. Baddeley and Wilson, 1986; Moscovitch, 1989, 1995; This is the first case in which confabulation extending to

Correspondence to: Gianfranco Dalla Barba, U. 324 INSERM, Centre Paul Broca, 2ter rue dAlesia, 75014 Paris, France. Tel: + 33 I 40 78 92 17;
Fax: + 33 I 45 89 68 48; e-mail: dallabarba@broca.inserm.fr
426 G. Dalla Barba tt (11.

the entire personal temporality, i.e. past, present and be accounted for as the result of damage t~a different
ttiturc, is experimentally documented. components of control processes. These are considered
The study of GAs case also provides interesting infor- part of the Supervisory System (Norman and Shallice.
mation concerning the role of search and monitoring 1980/1986) which is held to be a function of the frontal
processes in confabulation. Most of the current ideas cortex.
about mechanisms underlying confabulation emphasize the These hypotheses about the origin of confabulation
role of ;I frontal executive dysfunction in this disorder are challenged by several types of observations: ( a ) two
(Baddeley and Wilson, 1986; Kopelman, 1987; Moscovitch, patients have been described with a confabulatory syn-
1989; Delbecq-Derouesne et ul., 1990; Johnson, 1991; drome where there were spared executive functions and no
Moscovitch, 1995; Burgess and Shallice, 1996: Moscovitch frontal pathology (Dalla Barba ct ul., 1990; Dalla Barba.
and Melo, 1997). These ideas are based on the common I993a); (b) the confabulating patient described by Delbccq-
assumption that frontal lobe neural structures are the Derouesne and co-workers (1990) had a documented fron-
iinatomical correlate of cognitive functions that are tal lobe lesion but performed normally on tasks supposed
involved in organizing and controlling the cognitive to be sensitive to frontal lesion; (c) the confabulating
material processed by post-rolandic brain structures. As patient recently described by Dalla Barba and co-workers
fitr as memory is concerned, current theories assume that ( 1997) showed impaired executive functions without any
the frontal lobe represents the neural substrate involved in evidence of structural or functional damage to the frontal
organizing a search in long-term memory and in evaluating lobe; (d) in some patients confabulation atTects the
the resull of this search, which is the retrieved memory. performance on episodic memory tasks but not on
According to this view, confabulation would be the result semantic memory tasks. Taken together, these findings
ot an impairment of these search and monitoring processes suggest that a retrieval control hypothesis is insufficient i n
based on frontal neural structures. accounting for confabulation. They also cast doubts on the
Moscovitch (1989, 1995; Moscovitch and Melo, 1997), hypothetical prominent role of a frontal dysfunction i n
for example, distinguishes between two components of confabulation, and on the anatomical basis of executive
retrieval. One, associative retrieval, is relatively automatic tasks.
and independent of frontal functions. The other, strategic
retrieval. is self-initiated, goal-directed, effortful and intel-
ligent. Within strategic retrieval processes. two further Case report
components are hypothesized. The first involves organizing
;I mcniory search that uses whatever knowledge is avail-
History
able, whether semantic or episodic. Once knowledge is GA is a 52-year-old right-handed woman with 5 years 01
recovered. a second strategic process is involved in moni- education. She is married and has two children. She has
toring the output of the memory search and checking always worked as a housewife. She has no history of
whether it is consistent with other information in semantic psychiatric or neurologic diseases.
and episodic memory. According to Moscovitch (1989, On 29 June 1995 she was admitted to the Neurosurgery
1995; Moscovitch and Melo, 1997) the disruption of the Department of the Treviso General Hospital suffering from
monitoring sub-component of strategic retrieval would be a severe headache, with no neurological signs. A CT brain
responsible for confabulation. scan showed a subarachnoid haeniorrhage. An arterio-
Following a similar perspective Johnson ( 1 991) argues graphy showed an aneurysm of the anterior csmmunicat-
that confabulation is a disorder resulting from a failure of ing artery. On 3 July she was operated on in order to clip
rcality monitoring (i.e. deciding whether a memory is a the aneurysm. A few hours after the operation the
trace of something that actually happened to you, or is a patient fell into a coma. An Echo-Doppler study showed it
memory of an imagined event), and suggests that reality severe bilateral vasospasm of the brain vessels. A CT scan
monitoring is a function of two kinds of judgement pro- performed on that occasion showed an ischaemic lesion
cesses. The first is based on the evaluation of the qualitative involving the frontal lobe bilaterally. An ( IxF)fluorodeoxy-
characteristics of the retrieved information (e.g. the type glucose PET study in the resting state performed I week
and amount of perceptual details); the second is based on a later showed an extensive cortico-subcortical hypo-
more deliberate evaluation of retrieved information in metabolism involving the anterior commissure, the niediul
the light of other supporting memories and knowledge. frontal cortex, the inferior part of the left frontal lobe and
Damage to frontal structures would result in an impair- the periventricular white matter. An MRI performed on 3 I
ment of judgement processes involved in reality monitoring July showed bilateral frontal degenerative areas extending
and so in confabulation. from the orbito-frontal regions to the dorsal areas, involv-
In a study based on the analysis of healthy volunteers ing the anterior cingulum and the anterior two-thirds of
autobiographical recollections of recent everyday events, the corpus callosum. The patient remained in a coni;i
Burgess and Shallice ( 1996) argue that errors in recall made for approximately 3 months. Nine months after the
by normal subjects and those made by confabulators can vascular accident the patient was referred t o the Neurology
Confabulation and temporality 427

Department of the University of Padova for neuropsycho- Table 1. GAs scores 011 the subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale
logical evaluation and was seen on various occasions
between March and November 1996. The neurological Mean for
GA her age SD
examination was normal. ~

Personal information 4 5.91 0.3 I


Orient at io t i 4 4.96 0.24
Neuropsychological examination Mental Control 5 6.63 3.04
Logical memory 4 10.15 3.52
The patients collaboration during the testing sessions Digits 8 10.36 2. I 4
was good. On direct questioning she was shown to be Visual reproduction 1 10.67 3.20
disoriented and confabulated in time and space. She Paired absociate learning 6.5 16.25 3.91
performed well on certain tasks which are in general MQ 70
Delayed logical memory 0
considered to be sensitive to intellectual deterioration, such
as the Ravens Coloured Progressive Matrices-PM 47
where she achieved a score of 25/36 (Equivalent Score = 3),
the Mini Mental State Examinations (Folstein r t d.. 1975) Test (Warrington, 1984) she performed at the chance level
(25/30). and proverbs interpretation. On bedside tests of both on the words (25/50) and on the faces (26/50) subtests.
oral expression, understanding of oral language, reading, In another task she was presented with 50 photographs of
writing. neglect, praxis, calculation and short-term memory unknown faces and then asked for a Yes-No recognition
the patient was judged to be normal. test in which the 50 target faces together with an equal
number of distracters were presented and she had to select
the items that she had seen before. Her performance on this
Tests considered sensitive to frontal lesions test was also at the chance level since she correctly recog-
As G A had a lesion extensively involving the frontal lobe nized 30/50 target items and made 20/50 false recognitions.
structures. her performance was examined on tasks that are A questionnaire concerning public events relevant to the
traditionally considered sensitive to frontal lobe lesions. Italian population for which normative data are available
Her performance on these tasks varied greatly. On the (Costa rf NI., 1989) was given to GA. The questionnaire is
Modified Card Sorting Test (Nelson, 1976) she performed given in a recognition form and includes six questions
normally: she achieved five categories and produced only per biennium, from 1965 to 1985, with four choices per
four perseverative errors (score = 36.1, cut-off for frontal question. On this test she was correct only five times out of
lobe lesion = 50% of perseverative errors). Her perform- 60, she made six false recognitions and refused to choose
ance was also normal on the Cognitive Estimates (Shallice any of the four alternatives 49 times. She was also very
and Evans, 1978) and on the Tower of London (Shallice, defective in learning tasks. She was unable to learn a
1982; Alamanno et ( I / . , 1987) where she achieved the supraspan spatial sequence of length corresponding to her
maximum score. On other tests, however, her performance spatial span + 2 (De Renzi et a/., 1977a) and to learn a
was markedly impaired. In Lurias sequencing tests (Luria. 10-word list after 20 presentations (De Renzi et ul., 1977b).
1966) she was unable to learn a sequence of gestures of
length 2. On tasks of Verbal Fluency she was also very
Semantic memory
defective. On Category Fluency, a test that requires gener-
ating as many words as possible in 1 minute from a given GAs performance in tasks involving various types of
semantic category (e.g. animals), she produced an average semantic knowledge was normal: definition of words,
of four words, and on Letter Fluency, involving generating pointing out the correctly coloured object, performing a
as many words as possible in 1 minute starting with the symbolic gesture on spoken request, the Boston Naming
letters F. A and S, she produced an average of three words. Test, detecting semantic anomalies (Baddeley, 1982), were
The Trial Making Test (Davies, 1968) was impossible. all carried out correctly.
To summarize, GAs performance on tests traditionally
considered sensitive to frontal lesions varied greatly: in
seven tests of this type, her performance was completely
Examination of confabulation
normal on three and markedly impaired on the others. G A produced most of her confabulations in informal
conversation, when answering specific questions and
spontaneously, mainly referring to autobiographical epi-
Clinical tests of memory sodic memory. Confabulation was always plausible and
On the Wechsler Memory Scale (see Table I ) GAs MQ semantically appropriate so that a hypothetical observer
was 70, which is a clearly pathological score. Individual not familiar with GAs history, background and present
subtest scores ranged from very defective on Visual Repro- situation could hardly tell whether GAs reports were
duction and Logical Memory to average on Digits and confabulatory or not. For example, she would claim that
Mental Control. On the Warrington Recognition Memory the day before, she went out shopping and then prepared a
428 G. Dalla Barba ct trl.

lable 2. GAs and normal controls percentage of correct responses and confabulations on the Confabulation Battery
-

Normal controls

Correct Confab. Correct (SD) Conlhh

Pcrsonal semantic rneiiiory 8.5 5


I:pisodic incmory 46 33
01-ientation time place 60 40
(icncriil scm;intic memory 73 6
Linguistic semantic memory 53 0
I dont know episodic 0 0
I dont know semantic 0 0

menl for her family. In this case it is impossible to tell semantic memory. Accordingly, and in order to attempt to
whether this is a confabulatory report unless one knows equate the strategic retrieval demands of semantic and
that since her illness GA had never gone shopping nor had episodic memory, Moscovitch ( 1995; Moscovitch and
she ever prepared any meals. Melo, 1997) devised a semantic, historical ver:;ion of thc
word-cue test developed by Galton ( 1879) and revised by
Crovitz and SchiRman (1974) to complement the tradi-
Confabulation Battery tional episodic version of the test. Using thi.. techniquc
To further examine GAs confabulation, we submitted her Moscovitch (1995: Moscovitch and Melo, 1997) found that
t o the Confabulation Battery (Dalla Barba, 1993a), includ- patients with damage to the ventromedial portion of thc
ing questions probing personal, general and linguistic frontal lobe confabulated on both the episodic and thc
semantic memory. episodic memory, orientation for space semantic version of the test. We used Moscovitchs tech-
and time and questions to which the appropriate response nique i n order to ascertain whether the virtual absence 01
would be 1 dont know, both semantic (e.g. What did confabulation in semantic memory i n GA was ;in artetact
Marilyn Monroes father do?) and episodic (e.g. Do you due to the poor involvement of strategic retrieval i n
remember what you did on 13 March 1985?). Table 2 answering the semantic questions of the Confabulation
shows GAs and 12 normal control (NC)subjects perform- Battery. In the episodic version of the test, G A and sevcii
ance on the Confabulation Battery. As it clearly emerges control subjects were presented with eight cue-words and
from Table 2, GAs confabulations were found to be asked to produce a specific detailed account of ;in auto-
restricted mainly to episodic memory and orientation in biographical event related to that word. I n tlte semantic
time and place. GAs confabulation appears to be indepen- version of the test the procedure was identical to that used
dent of the availability of the correct answer since she never in the episodic version, except that this time i n response t o
confabulates on I dont know questions or on Linguistic the cue-word subjects had to describe i n detail a historical
Semantic Memory questions where she was able to provide event which occurred before they were born (see Appendix
the correct answer only eight times out of 15. 1). Responses were scored on a 0-2 scale with two points
for a detailed description of the event. one point for ;I less
detailed description and zero points for a general statement
Strategic retrieval and confabulation or if nothing was provided. All responses werc scored by
Moscovitchs ( 1989, 1995: Moscovitch and Melo, 1997) two independent raters. Agreement between them was
hypothesis of confabulation as a consequence of a deficit of 100%1.Memories for both semantic and personal cue-words
strategic retrieval is meant to be applied equally across all were verified for confabulations, using historical sourcch
domains of memory. In other words, confabulation should for the semantic test and questioning close relatives for the
equally affect episodic and semantic memory if both epi- episodic test. Table 3 shows GAs and normal controls
sodic and semantic memory tasks require the involvement performance on this test. As seen in the table. G A scored
of the strategic retrieval process. This hypothesis seems to poorly on both versions of the test when compared to
have ditliculties in accounting for the present case and for normal subjects. Confabulations were present only i n the
two previously described cases (Dalla Barba et a/., 1990; episodic version of the test. In addition, confabulations i n
Dalla Barba, 1993a,b) in which confabulations were the episodic test were much less frequent ( I3%,):IS com-
confined to the domain of episodic memory. However, pared to confabulations observed in episodic niemorj
Moscovitch (1995) argued that Dalla Barba and (33%) and orientation in time and place questions (40%) 01
co-workers patients showed confabulation in episodic the Confabulation Battery. This difference may reflect thc
memory but not in semantic memory because the tests fact that the Confabulation Battery included specific ques-
used to ;issess episodic memory made greater demands on tions that were probably more eflective as retrieval cuts
strategic retrieval processes than those used to assess than the cue-words included in the Crovitz test. As far ;IS
Confabulation and temporality 429

Table 3. GA'b and norniiil controls' performance on thc Crovitr teat

Episodic Semantic

I' 0of Mean score "I, of


c o n lit b ti l a t ion (SD) con fa h ti lii t i o n

GA 0.9 13 0.6 0
Normal controls I .9 ( 0 . 2 ) (1 I .8 ( 0 . 2 ) 0

Table 4. GA's and normal controh' performance ('XI) o n recognition niemor) and sotircc monitoring tasks

Recognit ion So ti rce nion ic o r ing

False
Correct rrcogn i t ion Corrcct 'Seen' 'Imagined'

GA 56 31 62 SO 50
Normal control5 I00 0 I00 50 50

the low baseline level of confabulation in the episodic were first presented with a new set of 16 coloured drawings
memory version of the Crovitz test is concerned. this niay of common objects. They were then read a list of 16 names
have prevented finding confabulation in the semantic of common objects and for each name they were asked to
version of the same task. Yet. however low the rate of imagine the corresponding object. Immediately afterwards
confabulation in the episodic version of the test, the results they were presented with a list of 32 printed words, where
of this test were dissociated from the results of the semantic half of the words were names of the objects seen and the
version of the same task where confabulation was never other half were names of the objects they had to imagine.
observed. Therefore, the present results, i.e. the greater For each word, subjects were asked to say whether that
amount of confabulation in the Confabulation Battery word was the name of an object they had seen or imagined
where strategic retrieval is likely to be less involved than in in the study phase. Table 4 shows the results of these
the Crovitz test and the absence of confabulation in the recognition memory and source monitoring tasks. On the
semantic version of the Crovitz test, cast serious doubts recognition memory task GA's performance was at the
on a strategic retrieval deficit hypothesis as the cause of level of chance. However, her poor performance on this
Confabulation. task cannot be attributed to a tendency to recognize as
already seen any presented item, since the rate of false
recognition was 3 1%. On the source monitoring task GA's
Source monitoring and confabulation perforniance was also very impaired, but there was no
It has been argued (Johnson, 1991) that confabulation evidence of a tendency to attribute imagined events to
might be the result of the disruption of mechanisms direct perception or vice versa. I n fact, regardless of
involved in monitoring the origin of information so that whether the response was correct or not, GA gave 50'%1
imagined events are mistaken for really experienced events. of 'seen' responses and 50%) of 'imagined' responses.
In order to test this hypothesis we administered a yes/no Although these results show that GA is suffering from a
recognition memory task to GA followed by a source source monitoring deficit, GA's confabulation cannot be
monitoring task. The first task was devised in order to have attributed to the incorrect acceptance of any item as a
a baseline control for the experimental material. I n the memory or to the tendency systematically to consider
study phase 16 coloured drawings of cominon objects were imagined events as really experienced events.
presented to GA and 10 control subjects. one after the It might be suspected, however, that the low rate of false
other, for approximately 3 seconds each. Immediately recognition, in the recognition memory task, and the lack
following the presentation of the 16 items. the subjects were of the tendency to attribute to visual perception what was
presented with a list of 32 printed words, where half of the imagined, in the monitoring task, are due to an etrect of the
words were names of the objects seen i n the study phase experimental material. In fact, the items that were used.
and the other half were distracters, i.e. names of common drawings and names of common objects, are 'neutral
objects not presented in the study phase. Subjects had to events', i n the sense that they are irrelevant to GA's daily
indicate the words corresponding to the objects seen in the life and biography. I t could be the case that GA shows
study phase. In the study phase of the second task, follow- ii tendency to recognize as true memories only events,
ing the same procedure as the previous one. the subjects experienced or imagined. that concern her daily life and
xh
13

biography. It' this is tlie case. GA should recognize iis


memories both really experienced episodes as well as her
conl'itbulatiotis. I n order t o check this possibility we adniin- that a s far ;IS questions about the personal future arc
istercd ;I yes/no recognition memory task to G A i n which concerned, any possible answer is ;I confabulaticln. since 13)
she \v;is presented orally with sentences referring either to definition the future is only 'probublc' and there is iii
iruc episodes I'rom her recent past. collected from her principle no 'correct' mswer to questions iibotit the t'u~urc.
husband. o r to contabulations she had produced i n pre- Yet, answers concerning the personal future c..in be dcti-
\,ious testing sessions. Sentences representing true episodes, nitely confabtilatory when they s h o w ;I m:irked tliscrcpa ncy
c.g. 'todny for lunch you had boiled meat', and sentences or ii real con t rndict io t i with what ;I prccl ic tcd fit t LI re act io I 1
representing confabtilatory cpisodes, e.g. 'today for lunch might be, in view of the present situation. For cwniple. to
y o u cooked spaghetti'. were selected and presented to her the question 'What iire you going to d o in ii tbw miniitcs'?'.
in ii ranclom order. Table 5 gives tlie results of this task. G A answered 'I will go home to cook the suppei'. Since 1ic.i.
< i A recognized its true memories 86'% of her confabu- disease, she actually never cooked and she wiix living o w
lations. whereas she recognized a s true memories only 43% and I I half hours from tlie hospital. To the quc5,tion 'Whiii
ot'episodes she had really experienced. From these results i t are you going t o do tomorrow?' she replied * I will go out
clearly emerges that GA does not show tlie tendency to shopping alone by car'. She never did that since Iicr discaw
accept ;is true any candidate autobiographical memory and she would certainly not do it the following d a y . As
because 01' ii delicit of monitoring processes. She actually shown in Table 6, GA's confabulations extended equally t o
seems to xihere much more to her confahulatory past than questions concerning episodic memory iitid licr pcrson;il
t o lier real piist. GA's tendency to recognize her confabu- future.
lation. rather tlim episodes she had really experienced. as The second task was ii 'future' version of tlic (.'rovit/ test.
true may bc cxplained ;IS follows: GA's confabulation I n this task tlie same eight cue-words used in the c1;issic
I-etlects ;I tendency to consider her general habits or per- episode version of the test were given to G A m l t o control
soniil semantic information, which are presumably more subjects. For each word. subjects were asked to produce ;I
strongly represented in long-term memory. 11s true autobio- specific, detailed account of ii personal project relutcd to
graphical episodes. This will be discussed more extensively t h a t word. Subjects responses were scored on ;I 0 2 poinis
1iitcr. scale following tlie procedure employcd for the episodic
and semantic versions of the test. ('r~nliibit1;itions wcrc
detected according to the same criteria used in the previous
Confabulation and temporality task. The results of this task iire presented #.)tiTublc 7
Accord i ng to the o bserva t i on that con fa b u la tors ofte ti together with tlic results of the episodic and scm;tntic
confabuliite i n orientation i n time and place tasks as well as versions of the test for comparison. The rewlts cleat-I!
when they iirc questioned about their personal future, it has show that G A also confabulated when i n response IO ii
becn proposed (Dalla Burba. I W 3 a ) that confabulation is cue-word she hod to provide ii personal project and indeccl
not only ;I disturbance of memory but also involves all even more than when she was asked to provi~lc;ti1 a u t o -
subjectively experienced tempornlity extending to tlie per- biographical past event. GA's performance on these tiisks.
sonal present and t o the personal future. In order to test together with her confabulntions on orientation i n timc ;Itid
this hypothesis we administered two further tasks to G A space tasks. provides evidence in support of tlic liypotlicsi:,
and seven control subjects. I n the first task we selected 10 that confabulation involves all the subjectively cxpericnccd
episodic memory cl uestions concet-ning her personal past temporality i ti its co ti t in ui t y and ti ni t y .
and I0 questions concerning her personal future (see
Appendix 2). Episodic memory questions and questions
concerning the I'uture were matched for proximity to the
Discussion
moment when tlie questions were asked. Answers were The patient we have described sutl'crs from it rclntivcl!~purc
scored ;IS 'correct', 'wrong' or 'confabulation', according to amnesic syndrome. in tlie sense t h a t tlie menlory impair-
wliethcr they were consistent with information (collected ment is not associated with any other cognitive impiiirment
fi-om her liusband) concerning the patient's past, present likely to interfere with memory pertormnnce. CiA pertimiis
situation and probable future activities. I t might be argued well on tests of intellectual efficiency siicli iis Progrcssi\ c
Confabulation and temporality 431

Table 7. G A . 5 a n d iioriiiiil controls' performance on the ('1-ovitr test

GA Normal coii t rol s

Mean score ":, of


(SD) con fa b ula t ion

Episodic 0.9 I3 1.9 ( 0 . 2 )


Seinantic 0.6 0 1.8 ( 0 . 2 )
F tit ti re 0.5 50 I .h (0.3)

Matrices and the MMSE a s well as on tests of short-term episodic and the semantic version of the Crovitz test. In the
memory. She has an extensive lesion involving the i'rontal present study we were not able to replicate Moscovitch's
lobe from the orbital to the dorsal areas, yet her perform- results. In fact, the study of GA's case shows that she does
ance on tests considered sensitive to frontal lobe lesions not confabulate on seniantic memory tasks even when a
varied greatly. ranging from normal on the Nelson's considerable amount of strategic retrieval is required, a s in
version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Cognitive the semantic version of the Crovitz test. Further evidence
Estimates and the Tower of London. to massively impaired against ;I strategic retrieval deficit account of confabulation
on Verbal Fluency, Luria's sequencing tests and the Trail conies from GA's performance on the Cognitive Estimates
Making Test. Her amnesia involves memory for both test. This test requires that the subject answer questions
personal and public episodes, a s well as learning of new which d o not have a readily available correct answer (e.g.
information as reflected by her poor performance on the 'How many camels are there i n Holland'?'). and the subject
Wechsler Memory Scale and on tests requiring the retrieval in order to provide a plausible answer. is constrained to
or recognition of personal or impersonal episodes. In make ii judgement involving the activation. co-ordination
contrast. dthough her performance on tasks tapping and control of several kinds of semantic information. Thus,
various types of general knowledge is poor. but not strategic retrieval is expected to be prominently involved i n
dramatically impaired. her confahulatory syndrome does the prodiiction of a plausible answer on this test. However,
not extend to tasks involving the retrieval of information GA not only does not confabulate on this test. but her
frotii general knowledge. In addition, she never confabu- performance is perfectly normal. So. according to the
lates when she is required to answer questions. semantic present results, a strategic retrieval deficit hypothesis as the
and episodic, to which the appropriate response would be 'I origin of confabulation is not contirmed. Consequently we
don't know'. Taken together these findings suggest that argue that confabulation confined to episodic memory. as
GA's confabulation does not reflect ii tendency to fill a gap in GA's case and. possibly, i n other studies (Dalla Barba
in memory (Barbizet, 1963; Talland. 1965; Sandson rt NI., c't trl., 1990; Delbecq-Derouesne e / rrl.. 1990; Dalla Barba.
1986: Kopeliiian. 1987) in order to avoid embarrassment. 1993a.b: Dalla Barba r'/ ol., 1997) is an empirically proved
since it ii ppears to be tot ;i 11y independent of the a v;i i la b i I i t y phenomenon and not the result of an experimental artefact
of the correct answer. In other words, GA does not due to poor involvement of strategic retrieval.
confabulate in response to those questions to which she
does not have an answer.
Confabulation and monitoring processes
A second issue we wanted to address in this study concerns
Strategic retrieval and confabulation the relationship between confabulation and processes
One of the iiiiiis of this study was to assess the validity of involved in iiionitoring the origin of information. Johnson
Moscovitch's hypothesis ( 1989, 1995; Moscovitch and ( 1991) argues that confabulation reflects a disruption of
Melo. 1997) of confabulation a s the result of an impair- processes involved in 'reality monitoring' (i.e. deciding
ment of strategic retrieval. According to Moscovitch, coii- whether a memory represents something that actually
fa hula t i oti sh o Lild ma ti i fest itself eq Lial I y i t i episodic a iid happened to you. or is a memory of an imagined event). A
semantic mcmory if episodic and semantic memory tasks disruption of this type of process would result in mistaking
require the involvement of strategic retrieval. Moscovitch imagined events for really experienced events. thus deter-
(1995) argued that a 'reason for the greater prevalence of iii i ti i ng con fa b ul a t i o n . This kind of per spect i ve , however,
cont'abulation about episodic memory is that retrieval of does not account for the fact that confabulation. as in the
episodic memories in the laboratory and i n real life are present case and other studies (Dalla Barba P / ( I / . , 1990:
likely to make greater demands on strategic processes than Delbecq-Derouesne e/ t i / . , 1990; Dalla Barba. 199321,b:
retrieval of semantic memories' (p. 16). In support of the Dalla Barba ct a/.. 1997). can be confined to the autobio-
idea that confabulation is the result of ii strategic retrieval graphical aspect of episodic memory while it is absent in
deficit. Moscovitch (1995) Moscovitch and Melo (1997) episodic learning tasks and in tasks that require retrieval of
provides evidence of patients who confabulate both in the non-episodic information. I n fact, if ii disruption of reality
131 G. Dalla Barba ~t rrl.

monitoring were responsible for confabulation. confusion lation. Also. she is very good on the Tower of London.
between re:ally experienced and imagined events and infor- which is a planning task. What all these ta
motion should not be limited to autobiographical episodic common, and at variance with tasks where GA contabu-
rclrievul but should extend across all memory domains. lates, is that they do not involve subjective temporality, in
Further doubts on the relationship between a deficit of the sense that they do not require the involvenient 01 the
reality monitoring and confabulation are cast by GAs personal history, present situation and probable future 01
pcrlormance on recognition and source monitoring tasks. the subject who is performing them. I n contrast. <;A
G A shows source amnesia as reflected by her performance .
cot i Fdbula t es when subjective tempo r;i I it y is i n vo I vcd i .c .
ar chance on the source monitoring task. However, she when she has to address her personal past, present and
does not systematically attribute to visual perception what future. Recently, Conway and Tacchi (Conway ;I nd Tacchi.
M ~ I Simagined or vice versa. In addition, on recognition 1996) described a patient. OP. who cotlf>tbtlliitt:d in auto-
memory tasks she does not show a tendency to recognize as biographical memory. They interpret 01s cot.tfiibuliitiotl
~ l l r c i i d yseen any item she is presented with, as reflected by within the framework of Conways theory of autobio-
rhe lo\+ rate of false recognition on the recognition memory graphical memory (Conway, 1992; Conway, 1990; Conwiil
task. Thcreforc. dthough G A shows a source monitoring and Rubin. 1993). suggesting that their patients confabu-
deficit. it scenis restrictive to trace back GAs confabulation lation is the result of a disruption of the self due t o thc
to a general deficit of processes involved in reality moni- damage of frontally based control processes of memory
toring. or to H systematic bias to recognize its part of her (Burgess and Shallice, 1996; Johnson. IWl). In their cieu
past experience any present information. One possibility, as OPs confabulation is motivated by the purpose of protect-
suggested elsewhere (Dalla Barba, 1993a), is that distinct ing the self in a time of stress and isolation. Thc problem
reality monitoring processes iire specific to different types with this interpretation is that none of the elements invoked
o l ~retrieval. so that confabulation confined to episodic to explain confabulation is conscious: the st:lf. control
memory would retlect the disruption of reality monitoring processes and the purpose to protect the self are all
processes operating in episodic memory retrieval, while unconscious. We have shown elsewhere (Dalla Harba.
reality monitoring processes operating in other types of 1993; Dalla Barba, 1997) that considering control processc\
retricval ;ire intact. This type of account, however, is of memory as operating outside consciousness poses tii:ijor
compromised by GAs performance on tlie task in which theoretical problems as this would necessitate the attribu-
she was asked to recognize sentences referring to true tion of intentionality to the unconscious. The sittnc argu-
cpisodes from her recent past or to confabulation she had ment applies to the hypothesis of an unconscious sell :in11
produced in previous testing sessions. The results of this of an unconscious purpose to protect this self. 10 iittributc
task show that GA recognizes her confabulations as true intentionality to an unconscious process ineatis to proc ide
memories, whereas she is at the level of chance in recogniz- it with the property of being . s i i h j w / , that is, 1.0 attributc
ing really experienced episodes. I f a deficit of processes consciousness to the unconscious. Like i n ,.I game 01
involved in reality monitoring were present in G A and Chinese boxes, the subject would contain another subjcci.
responsible for her confabulation, one would expect the an honri~ncu/ir.r,provided with a shady and inaccessible
siinie pattcrn of yes/no responses both for sentences refer- consciousness, that is busy solving problems such iis reject-
ring to confabulations and for those referring to true ing false memories and providing conscious conscioiisnehs
in cniot-i es . with the result of its accurate selection. According to these
criteria, can the homunculus distinguish betwci-n true and
false information? Does not the hoinunculus ;iIso need an
Confabulation and temporality
unconscious 11om u ncu 1us that he1ps it to Ji s t i ngu i sli
The most interesting aspect of GAs case study is that between true and false memories? Like this the Chinese
her coiiliibulation does not only involve the retrieval of boxes game continues endlessly, and what we arc Iclt with
personal past episodes and present orientation in time and is just a r d i r c t i o r i d itzfiuimi.
place, which is a known characteristic of some confabu- When GA confabulates, she acts like somebody who is
lators, but nianifests itself also when she is asked to plan aware of her past, present and future. The problem.
her personal future. The extension of GAs conpdbulation however, is that she is aware of a past, a present. and ;I
to a11 tlie three dimensions of temporality-past, present future that are false, in the sense that they do n o i
and future-seems to be something more than a fortuitous represent ii past, present and future coherent with her
coincidence. GA confabulates when she has to answer actual situation. I t has been suggested that conliibulation
questions like What did you do yesterday? Where are you reflects a disturbance of chronology or of time-order
now? What are you going to d o tomorrow? but she never memory, so that real memories are confused iii tcmpor;rl
confiibulates answering questions concerning historical context (Korsakoff, 1889; Schnider ct ( I / . , 1996; Talland.
events o r in traditional laboratory learning tasks. In this 1961; Van der Horst, 1956; Victor o f r i / , , 1971). T h i h
sort oftask her poor performance is in fact due to errors of interpretation, however, is challenged by rhe f ~ c ttlivi
omission and not to intrusions or even fleeting confabu- inany amnesic and non-amnesic patients have disordered
Confabulation and temporality 433

Consciousness Long-Term present or the future, nor are they organized according to
Storage System the order of succession, i.e. there is nothing in Y, for
I
- 1 I example, that tells that Y comes before Z and after X. They
are aspecific in the sense that they do not contain any
information specifying that they are representing episodes,
meanings, rules, procedures, algorithms, etc.

Assiriiiption 2
The modifications in the LTSS can be more or less stable
and more or less vulnerable depending on a number of
variables. These variables act at the encoding and storage
level and include, among others, attention at encoding,
emotional value of encoded and stored event, depth of
Fig. 1. A hypothesis concerning the relation between memory. conscious- encoding, rehearsal and repeated experience of the same
ness and temporality. TC = temporal consciousness; KC = knowledge
consciousness: X. Y and Z = aspecific modifications of the Long-Term event or similar events.
Storage System. varying from less ( X ) to more stable (Z).
Assuniption 3
chronology. yet do not confabulate (Milner and Petrides, Consciousness means to be conscious q/ sorilething in LI
1984; Nichelli, 1993; Stuss r t ul., 1978; Vriezen and .spec.ijzc N U J . That means that consciousness is not an
Moscovitch, 1990). Accordingly, it does not seem that aspecific dimension that passively receives and becomes
GAs pathological awareness of her personal temporality aware of different types of already specified information,
can be simply traced back to a disorder of time-order but rather that different types of consciousness exist, each
memory. Yet it remains to be explained of what GA is representing an original and irreducible way of addressing
aware, if not of real memories in a wrong temporal context, the world. Different types of consciousness include, among
when she is confabulating about her past, her present and others, temporal consciousness (TC) and knowing con-
her future. GAs confabulation is typically plausible, or sciousness (KC). TC means to become aware of something
semantically appropriate (Dalla Barba, 1993a) in the sense as part of a personal past, present or future. KC means to
that a hypothetical observer not familiar with GAs history, become aware of something as a meaning or as an element
background and present situation could hardly tell whether of impersonal knowledge or information.
GAs reports are confabulatory or not. She would claim,
for example, that the day before she went shopping or that Assuniption 4
after the testing session she will prepare a meal for her TC can, and usually does, interact with the less stable and
family-which are acts consistent with her life before her more vulnerable modifications of the LTSS in order to
illness. So this pattern of behaviour suggests that, at remember the past, be oriented in the present and plan the
variance with classic amnesic patients that are lost in the future.
present, in the sense that they are no longer aware of a
personal past and of a personal future (see for example According to this model, confabulation in episodic memory
Tulving (1 ul., 1988; Tulving, 1989). GA is perfectly aware (e.g. Dalla Barba, 1993a; Dalla Barba rt ul., 1990). in
of her personal temporality. but she mistakes her habits or orientation in time and place and in personal future
personal semantics for her real past, present and future. In planning tasks, as in GAs case, is the result of a condition
this sense G A is remembering another past and planning in which TC is still intact but cannot use less stable
knot her future. modifications of the LTSS but only more stable ones, in
What GAs case shows is that confabulation involves all order to remember the personal past, be oriented in the
the subjectively experienced temporality in its continuity present and plan personal future. In other terms, this is a
and unity extending from the past through the present to condition in which TC is still working but is no longer able
the future. If this is the case, we need to explain what to accomplish its usual task, i.e. to operate a sort of fine
subjectively experienced temporality is. One way to attempt grain search in the LTSS and use less stable modifications
to answer this question is to make a set of assumptions that in order to set up a personal temporal workspace. What
are shown schematically in the model represented in Fig. 1. T C does instead in this condition is to use more stable
modifications of the LTSS, with the result that habits or
Assuinption I personal semantic information are considered in a personal
Events produce atemporal and aspecific modifications i n temporal framework. When asked what they have done the
the Long-Term Storage System (LTSS). These modifi- previous day or what they are going to d o the following
cations, represented in Fig. 1 as X, Y, and Z, are atemporal day, confabulating patients of this type typically answer
in the sense that they do not contain any information with memories and plans that they usually have in their
concerning time. They do not represent the past, the daily life. Although admitted to the hospital, they will say,
434 C. Dalla Barba ct cd

for example, that the previous day they went out shopping Acknowledgements
and that the following day they will be visiting some friends. This article is a modified version of part of iin invited paper
acts that presumably were part of their routine life. presented at the XV European Workshop on Cognittvc
I t could be argued that patients who confabulate in Neuropsychology, Bressanone, Italy. January 1097. and at
episodic memory, orientation and planning tasks are not the Cognitive Neuroscience and Memory C'onferencc.
necessarily conscious of a confabulatory past, present and Stockholm, June 1997. We are grateful to manly people 111
future but rather they simply produce the more plausible the audience at those conferences and to Martin Conwit!
iitlswer without having a subjective experience of remem- and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comment:, on i i n
bering, of being in that place at that time or of planning earlier draft. This work was partially supported by thc
their actions. If this were the case our account of confabu- Institut National de la Sante et de la Rechercht: M ~ d i c a l .
lation in the past, present and future would be dismissed
because TC would not play any role at all in confabulation.
References
Yet there is evidence that patients who confabulate actually
do become aware of their confabulatory past, present and Alamanno N. Della Sala S. Loiacona M. Pasetti C. Spinnlcr H. Prohlcni
future. For example [Dalla Barba (1993a) following the solving ability in aging and dementia: noriii;itiw data or ii non-\erbal
test. Italian Journal of Neurologic;tl Scicncc 19x7: 8: I I 10.
procedure described by Tulving (1985)], the patient MB Alexander MP. Freedman M. Amnesia after :interior coiiiiiitiiiic;itiii~
was asked to attribute a 'remember' or a 'know' judgement artery aneurysm rupture. Neurology 1984: 34: 752 7.
to his confabulation in episodic memory task; he system- Baddeley A. Your memory. A user's guide. London: M ultinicdi;i
Publications, 1982.
atically gave 'remember' judgements. Also, the same Baddeley A. Wilson B. Amnesia, autobiographical iiieniory and c o i i I ' ~ i h ~ ~ -
patient showed he was ready to carry out his confabulatory lation. In: Rubin. DC, editor. Autobiographical niemor:y. Canihritlgc.
plans (see also Baddeley and Wilson, 1986; Moscovitch, Cambridge University Press, 1986: 225-52.
Barbizet J . Defect o f memorizing o E hippocampal maminill;iry origin
1989). I n addition, from a clinical point of view, confabu- Journal of Neurology. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1963; 26: 127 3 5 .
lating patients do not look like subjects who produce their Benson D F , Djenderedjirin A, Miller BL ( ' I trl. Neural b
'best guess' in answering questions, but rather they seem to lation. Neurology 1996; 46: 1239-43.
Berlyne N. Confabulation. British Journal o f Psychiatr) 1972; 120. 31 'J
adhere completely to their confabulatory reports. There- Bonhoeffer, K. Der Korsakowschc Syrnptonicnkoinplcu in w n c n 1 % ~ -
fore. although more experimental evidence is suitable, it ziehungen zu den verschiedenen Kr~tnkhcitsfornien.Allgemeinc Zcit t i n ?
does not seem to be misleading to consider confabulation Psychiatrie 1904; 61: 744-52.
Burgess PW. Shallice T. Conlitbulntion and the control 1.11' rccolltcti~ii~
i n episodic memory, orientation and future planning tasks Memory 1996; 4: 3 5 9 4 1 I
a s a condition in which TC can interact only with more Cermak LS. Uhly B. Reale L. Encoding specificity in the iilcoht)I~c
stable modifications of the LTSS. Korsakoff patient. Brain and Cognition 19x0: I I : I I 9 27.
Conway MA. A structural model ol' autobiographical mtmoi-!
I t is well known that patients sometimes also confabulate In: Conway MA, Rubiii DC, Spinnler H. Wagenaai- W A . editor\
i n some semantic memory tasks (e.g. Moscovitch, 1995; Theoretical perspectives on autobiographic;il memory. Ihrdi-ech13'flit
Moscovitch and Melo, 1997). According to our hypothesis, Netherlands: Kluwer Aciidemic Publishers. 1991: I67 94.
Conway MA. Autobiographical memories mid aut~)bi1,gr;ipliie~il
confabulation in semantic memory reflects a condition in knowledge. In: Rubin DC. editor. Remembering o u r past: studit\ i n
which KC is still there but is unable to use more stable autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge Clnivcrbity h e > \ .
modifications of the LTSS. When this condition occurs 1996: 67-93.
Conway MA. Rubin DC. The structure o f ati[obiogra[illic;II meinor) 111
patients confabulate i n tasks tapping general semantic Collins AE, Gathercole SE, Conway MA. Morris PEM. etlitoir
knowledge, such as knowledge of public and historical facts Theories of memory. Hove. LJK: Lawrence Erlbauni Associ;itca. 1 0 c j 7
and knowledge of famous people (e.g. Moscovitch, 1995; 103 37.
Conway MA, Tacchi PC. Motivated confithulation. Ncui ocitsc IWO. 2 .
Moscovitch and Melo, 1997), but they do not confabulate 32539.
i n semantic tasks that require the use of more stable Costa M, De Renzi E. Fnglioni P. U n questionario ilaliano per 1 ~ 1
modifications of the LTSS, such as word definition tasks. studio della memoria retrograda. Archivio di Psicologia. Ncurologia I'
Psichiatria 1989: 5 0 : 735-55.
Sonie patients, however, also confabulate in word defi- Crovitz HF. Schiffman H. Frequency o f episodic nieiiioric~;is l'iiiiction I ) I
nition tasks (Dalla Barba, 1993b). They also usually their ages. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1974: 4: S17 I X .
produce confabulation in episodic memory tasks that Dalla Barba G. Confabulation: knowledge and recollectioii espcricncc.
Cognitive Neuropsychology 1993a: 10: I 20.
are described as fantastic (Berlyne, 1972), implausible Dalla Barba G. Difercnt patterns of confabulation. Cortex l993b. 2 9
(Baddeley and Wilson, 1986), incoherent (Kopelman, 1987) 567- 8 I .
or semantically anomalous (Dalla Barba, 1993a). Confabu- Dalla Barba G. Confabulation and consciousness. In: X V Europc;iii
Workshop on Cognitive Neuropsychology, Bressanonc. lt;rIk. 1097.
lation in word definition tasks and fantastic confabulation Dalla Barba G. Cipolotti L. Denes G. Autobiogruphic;il ni8:mory l o s s ;ind
in episodic memory tasks reflect a condition in which the confabulation in Korsakotrs syndrome: ;I case report. Cortcx 1900; 10
modifications in the LTSS are degraded so that the material 525-34.
Dalla Barba G. Boise M-F, Bartolomeo P. Bachoud-LOLi A-C' C'oiil'aliii-
used by TC and KC is per s o incoherent. The result is that lation following rupture of posterior communicating arter) . C ' o r i ~ > \
the semantic content of confabulation in episodic memory 1997; 33: 563-70.
is anomalous so that confabulation takes a fantastic and Davies A. The influence or age oii Trail Making Tcsi pcr-li)rrii;iric.~~
Journal of Clinical Psychology 1968: 24: 96 8.
bizarre form and so does confabulation in word definition De Renzi E. Faglioni P, Previdi P. Spatial ineniory and hemispheric IOCLI\I
and other semantic tasks. of lesion. Cortex 1977a: 13: 424 33.
Con fa b ula t i on and tempo ra 1it y 4 35

De Renri E. Faglioni P. Ruggerini C. Provc di iiieiiioriii \crb;ile Tul\ing L:. Schacter DL. McLacIiliin DR. Moscovitch M Priming
di inipiego clinico per lii diagnosi clinica di :iiiinesiit. Archivio (11 of seniitntic ailtobiographical knowledge: H ciibe study o f retrograde
Psicologiii. Neurologia e Psichiatria 1997h: 38: 303 18. amnesia. Brain and Cognition 1988: 8: 3 2 0 .
Delbecq-Derouesne J. Beauvois MF. Shallice T . P r e s c r ~ ~reciill
l \erstis V;in clcr H o i - s t L. Lc sens de la teinpor;ilisatioii pour la niL;moirc ct pour
impaired recognition. Brain 1990: 113: 1045 153. I'orientation. Ecolution Psychintrique 1956: 1: 18') 205.
Folstein MF. Folstein SE. McHtigh PR. 'Mini Mental State': a practical Victor M. Adams RD. Collins G H . The Weriiicke Korsakolf syndrome.
method for grading t h e cogniti\e state of patients foi- t h e clinician. Philadelphiu. PA: Davis. 1971,
Journal 01' Psychiatric Research 1975: 12: 189-98. Vilkki J . Amnesic syndrome after surgery for antcrioi- coiiimiinic;rting
Galton F. Psychometric cxperirnents. Brain 1879: 2: IJC)6 2 . artery ;tneurysm. Cortes 1985: 21: 431 44.
Irk E. Wowra B. Kunert HJ. Kunze S. Memorq disturbances fnllowing Vriexn ER. Moscovitch M. Memory f o r temporal order and con-
anterior communicating artery rupture. Annals of Neurology I 997: 3 I ' ditional ;issociative-lcarning i n paticnts w i t h Parkinson's disease.
473 80. Netii-opsycliologiit 1990: 28: I283 93.
Johnson MK. Reality monitoring: evidence from confahulation i n organic Warrington EK. Recognition memory test. Windsor. LIK: NFER-Nelson.
brain disease patients. In: Prigatano GP. Schitcter DL..editors. Aware- 19x4.
ness o f dclicit after brain in.jury. New York-Oxfnrd: Oxl'nrd Llniversit!, Wcinstcin E. Lyerly 0. Conl"ibul;ttion folloning hr;iin injury. Archive5 of
Press. 1991 General Psychiatry I96X: 18: 348-54.
Kaptir N. Coughlan AK. Confahulation after frontal lobe dysftinction. Wyke M. Warrington E. An experimental analysis of conl'abulation i n il
Journal 01' Neurology. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1980: 43: 461 - 3 . case of KorsakolTa syndrome using ii t;ichistoscopic method. Journal of
Kern R. Van Grop W. CLininiings J. Brown W. Osato S. Confabulation i n Neurology. Neurosurgery and Psychiutry 1960: 23: 327 - 3 3 .
Alzheimer's Disease. Brain a n d Cognition 1997: 19: I72 82.
Kopelnian M D. Two types of confabulation. Journ:il of Neurnlogy. R i ~ c ~ c , i ~0' 1c1d 13 J////c,. 1997; rc.sr/h/rri/tc~rlO / I 20 ,-lu,yl/.s/, 1997:
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1987: 3 1 : 1482-87. clc.c.r'ptt'rl 0 1 1 3 .s"f"lllhc~r. I997
Kopelinan MD. Guinan EM. Lewis PDR. Delusional memory. confabu-
lation, a n d frontal lobe dysfunction: ii case study in De CIPranlhault's
syndrome. Neurocase 1995: I : 71 7.
Korsakotf SS. Etude iiic;dico-psychologiquesur tine foi-me dcs ni;hrlirs de
la meninire. Revue Philosophique 1889; 28: 501 -30.
Luria AR. Higher cortical fiinctions in man. London: T ~istock.L 1060.
Luria AR. The neuropsychology of iiiemory. New Y o r k : John Wilq .
1976. Appendix I . The list of w ~ r d stised t o cue episodic (;iiitohiogr:ipliical) and
Mercer B. Wapner W. Gardner H. B e n m i F. A 5t~1d)of conf~thulation. wniintic (historical) memorie\ i n the CrovitL test
Archives of Neurology 1977: 34: 479 3 3 .
Milner. B. Petrides. M. Behavioural eHects on frontal lobe lesions in nxtn. Episodic/' 1. tit urc' Semantic
Trends i n Neuroscience 1984: 7: 403-7.
Moscovitch M. Confabulation and the frontal system: strategic \ c r s t ~ s
associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memor). In: Train Assassination
Roedinger HL. Craik F1. editors. Varieties of memory and conscious- M ountaln Attack
ness: Essaq i n honor of Endel Tulving. Hillsdale. NJ. Lawrence Letter Discovery
Erlbaum. 1989. Mone) Train
Moscovitch M. Confithulation. I n : Schacter DL. editor. Memory Cinema War
distortion. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press. 1095: 2 2 6 5 1 Resta tirant President
Moscovitch M. Melo B. Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: evidence H ospi t ;II Kevolutinn
from conktbulation and amnesia. Ncuropsycliologia 1997; 35: I(1I7 34. MiirkcI King
Nelson HE. A modified card sorting test s e n s i t n e lo frontal defects.
Cortex 1976: 12: 313 23.
Nichclli P. Human temporal information processing. In: BolleI F. Appendix 2. Questions concerning personal past ;tiid future (bold type
Grafnian G . editors. Handbook of ncLIropsycliolog!. Ani.;terd;lln: s l i o ~ squestions that elicited confabulation)
Elsevier. 1993.
Norman DA. Shallice T. Attention t o action: willed and autnniittic control
-
of behavior. I n : Center for Human Information Processing (Techiiic;iI Po\/
report No 99). 1980. Reprinted i n revised form in: Davidson RJ. I . What did you do a few minutes ago?
Schwarz GE. Shapiro D. editors. Consciousness and self-regtilntion. 2. Who did you see this morning?
Advances in research and theor). Vol. 4. New York and London: 3. What did you eat for dinner yesterday?
Plenum Press. 1986: 1 18. 4. What did you do yesterday?
Papagno C. Muggin S. Conlibulation: dissociation between e v e r y h y life _5 . What did y o t i d o the d a y befnrc yesterday'!
and neuropsychological performance. Neurocase 1996: 7. I I I I X . 6 H o w did y o u spend last Christmas'?
Sandson J. Albert ML. Alexander M . Confabulation i n aphusia. Cortcs I. Do you remember the last time you went to see a doctor?
1986; 2': 6 2 1-6. X. Do you rememher the last time ) n i l went to visit a friend o r i~
Schnider A. von Diniken C. Gutbrod K. The mechanisms o f spontaneous relative'!
and provoked confabulations. Brain 1996: I 19: 1365-75. 9 Do you reinember the last time you went to it restaurant'!
Shallice T. Specific impairments of pliinning. Philosophical Transactions 10. Do you remember the last time you took vacation?
of the Royal Society of London B Biological Science 1982: 29X:
199 209. Fti/i/,
Yl'

Sh;illice T. Evans M. The involvement of the 1.rontal lobe i n cognitive I . What are you going to do in a few minutes?
estimation. Cortex 1978: 14: 294 303. 2. Who are you going to see this evening?
Stuss DT. Alexander MP. Liherman A. Levine H. An extraordinary l n r m 3. What are you going to eat this evening?
of confabulation. Neurology 1978: 28: I 160-72. 4. What are you going to do tomorrow?
Talland GA. Confabulation in the Wcrnickc Korsakolf syndronie. 3. What ;ire y o t i going to do the day after t o m o r r o w ?
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 1961: 137: 361 -81. 6. Htm will yoti spend next Christnias?
Talland G A . Deranged memory. New York: Academic Press. 1965. I. When will he the nest time you visit a doctor?
Tulving, E. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology 1985: 26: 8 . W h t n will be the next time you visit ;I friend?
1-12, 9. When will he the nest time y o u go to ii restaui-ant?
Tulving E. Remembering and knowing the past. American Scientist I9X9: 10. When will be the nest time you go on Facation?
361-6.
Confabulation: Remembering 'another'
past, planning 'another' future

G . Dalla Barba, J. Y. Cappelletti,


M. Signorini and G . Denes
Abstract
111 t l i i h s t u d y \\c desci-ihc ;I p;rticnt. GA. ulio developed iui amnesic-
L.l,iit'~ihiil;itory s) ndi.ome. following 21 suharachnoid haemorrhage due to
I tipturc ( 1 1 the anterior c(iiiii1itiiiic;itiiig artery. As far ;is tlie performance
1111 mcnioi-y t a s k s h i i s concerned. GA's conlihulation WLIS found to be

rc\iricted to the ;ititohiograpliic.al aspect of episodic memory. Confabu-


l;ition did not manilest itsell' in episodic learning tasks nor on tasks
t;iiiping \ iiriotis kinds 01' sem;intic knouledgc. I n contrast. GA confabu-
I;itcd in orientation in time ;ind place tasks and a l s o in tiisks where she was
I . ~ L I L I I I . ~ ~ t o pliiii Iicr pcrsoniil ftittirc. GA's conlhhulatiori could not he
;i~cuuntcdIOr in term5 o f :in impairment ol'str;itegic retrieval or o f reality
iiionitorinp 1iroccsscs. I t is htrggested that GA's confahulation reflects ii
pathologiciil iiw;ireiicss of pcrsoii;iI temporalit y.

Journal
wclll c1ciise 1097: 3 : 425 36

Neurocase Reference Number:


00.3

Primary diagnosis of interest


Siih;ir;iclinoiil haemorrhagr. I-:rontal strake

Author's designation of case


(i .I

Key theoretical issue


~'oiiflihul;itionrellecth ii pathological iiwi~reiiessof persoii;il temporal-
It!

Ac.1. I I . I W ~ \ c~iiif~i1~iil;ition:
: iiiiinesia: consciousness: temporality

Scan, EEG and related measures


c'r. hiiti. wr
Standardized assessment
I'M-47. Wcchsler Memory Scale. Mini-Mental State Examination. Modi-
lied C'arJ Sorting Test. Cognitive Estimates. Tower of London. Luria's
Sequencing ' l c s t s . Letter Fluency. Category Fluency. Trail Making Test,
Digit Span. C'orsi Test. Warrington Recognition Memory Test. Public
l . \ c i i t a Qurstionn;iire, 10-hord Lisl Learning. Boston Naming Test.
Seni;intic Anomalies Detection. Confabulation Battery

Other assessment
C ' I m itc Test (moililied). Personal PastlFuttire Interview. Source Monitor-
1ng l C \ l

Lesion location
MIII: 1iIatcr;il frontal degenerative areas extending from the orhito-
l'rontiil regions to tlie dors;il iirciis. involving the anterior cingulum and
t h e :interior two-thirds of t h e corpus collosum

Lesion type
L';l\clll~ll

Language
I:nglish

S-ar putea să vă placă și