Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

Neuro-Ophthalmology, 32:93104, 2008

Copyright 
c Informa Healthcare USA, Inc.
ISSN: 0165-8107
DOI: 10.1080/01658100701501109

CASE REPORT

Six Adult Cases with a Pseudo-Vestibular


Syndrome Related to Vergence
Abnormalities
Q. Yang, F. Jurion,
and M. P. Bucci ABSTRACT Saccades and vergence were studied in adults with vertigo but
Laboratoire de Physiologie de la without abnormal vestibular function; vertigo was associated with headaches
Perception et de lAction and occurred mostly after prolonged computer work. Horizontal eye move-
(LPPA), IRIS Group, UMR 7152,
ments from both eyes were recorded simultaneously with the IRIS SKALAR
CNRS-College de France, 11,
place M. Berthelot, 75005, device. Several of the specific characteristics of eye movements described previ-
Paris, France ously in controls were not present in vertigo adults: latency of convergence was
not longer than that of divergence; components of combined saccade-vergence
A. Lucek movements did not have longer latency than pure saccades or vergence;
Service dORL, Hopital
vergence velocity was not accelerated by the saccade during combined
Europeen Georges Pompidou,
75015 Paris, France movements. Relative to control adults, vertigo subjects showed longer latencies
of saccades to distant targets, divergence and convergence (tendency), slower
S. Wiener-Vacher speed for convergence, divergence and vergence components of combined
and D. Bremond-Gignac movements. Eye movement abnormalities could be useful for the differential
Service dORL, Hopital Robert
diagnosis of pseudo-vestibular syndrome (without vestibular dysfunction).
Debre, 48 Bld. Serurier, 75019,
Paris, France Oculomotor training is suggested for such subjects with vertigo to improve
their abnormal eye movements and reduce symptoms.
C. Orssaud
Service dOphtalmologie, KEYWORDS Vertigo; saccades; vergence; latency; speed
Hopital Europeen Georges
Pompidou, 75015 Paris, France

Z. Kapoula INTRODUCTION
Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Vertigo is a common symptom due to either peripheral or central vestibu-
Perception et de lAction lar dysfunction.7 However, some patients may lack typical signs of vestibular
(LPPA), IRIS Group, UMR 7152, dysfunction but still manifest vertigo symptoms. Anoh-Tanon et al.1 reported
CNRS-College de France, 11,
that about 5% of children consulting the ENT service for vertigo and headaches
place M. Berthelot, 75005,
had normal vestibular function; they suggested that vertigo in such cases can be
Paris, France
a symptom of ocular disorders. De Haller et al.7 also diagnosed visual vertigo
based on the history and clinical examination in 11 from 242 vertigo or dizzi-
Address correspondence to Dr. Qing
Yang, IRIS Group, LPPA, UMR 7152, ness patients (4.5%). The symptoms immediately improved either on cessation
CNRSCollege de France, 11, place M. of the visual input or upon closure of the eyes. They suggested that the visual
Berthelot, 75005, Paris, France. Tel:
(331) 44.27.16.36; E-mail:
vertigo could result from a mismatch between vestibular, proprioceptive and
yangqing165@hotmail.fr visual inputs (neuro-ophthalmological examination was normal in all cases).

93
In addition to vestibular eye movements, saccade in the control of eye movements have not been entirely
and vergence are important for exploration of 3D space matured and this conceals oculomotor abnormalities.
and for high quality 3D vision. Saccades are rapid However, for adults as cortical areas have been matured
eye movements during which both eyes rotate in the and the oculomotor plasticity has been accomplished,
same direction; vergence is the slow eye movement in their deficit of eye movements may be different from
opposite direction for the two eyes allowing to adjust children despite the similarity of vertigo symptoms.
the angle of visual axes according to the depth of the The main goal of this study is to explore whether
object of interest. Under natural conditions, most shifts similar deficits exist in adults with a complaint of ver-
of the line of sight are to objects that lie in different tigo but without vestibular abnormalities. First, latency,
direction and different depths. Such gaze shifts require accuracy and speed characteristics of different types of
a combination of a saccade and vergence movement. eye movements are compared within subjects to con-
The spatial and temporal characteristics of saccades and firm the differences known to exist in normal adults.
vergence movements are different, indicating different Secondly, for all the above mentioned parameters com-
control mechanisms albeit interacting. For example, parisons are made with values of the same types of
the latency of saccades is longer to near targets than to movements from control adult subjects of similar age
distant targets and the latency of convergence is longer studied in the same conditions by our group.25,26
than that of divergence; combined saccade-vergence
movements have longer latencies than pure saccades or METHODS
vergence movements such as required when the targets
are aligned along the median plane.19,24,26 Another im-
Patients
portant characteristic for combined saccade-vergence Six subjects, aged 2856 years old (means 40.5 11.8
movements is that the saccade component is slower years), 2 males and 4 females, were recruited presenting
(in speed) than the pure saccade while the vergence complaints of vertigo. All subjects were recruited in
component is faster than pure vergence.5,6,10,25,27 Stud- the hospital and underwent vestibular, ophthalmology
ies of the variety of naturally made eye movements and orthoptic examination. They were addressed to
in adults with vertigo symptoms are non-existent. In us because no vestibular dysfunction was found and
children with vertigo but normal vestibular function, the orthoptic examination revealed abnormalities
saccade and vergence eye movements have been (see Table 1). Vestibular testing was extensive for some
studied by Bucci et al.;3,4 they reported abnormally of the subjects including caloric test, canal and otolith
long latencies for saccades and vergence, particularly examination, off vertical axis rotation with a computer
for convergence, and for saccades combined with controlled rotating chair in the dark.2123 Conven-
convergence or divergence; reduced accuracy, long tional hearing tests (tonal and speech audiometric
duration and low speed for vergence relative to techniques) were also performed to examine the inner
normal children of matched age. Bucci et al.2 showed ear function. The common symptoms reported by
poor binocular coordination of saccades in children all these patients were severe headaches and vertigo,
with vertigo. often after prolonged computer use (5 of them were
Thus, there is evidence that pseudo-vestibular employed as secretaries). Subject S3 had orthoptic
syndrome, e.g. vertigo symptoms are associated with training several years ago, subject S2 had such training
abnormalities of saccades and mostly of vergence. recently and subject S5 started taking medication lectil
Bucci et al.3,4 proposed some mechanisms by which 16 (betahistine, against vertigo) a week before our
such abnormalities could interfere actually with the oculomotor testing. No other subject took medication.
optimal adjustment of VOR gain according to the All these subjects were regularly follow-up by the
viewing distance, thereby leading to incomplete visual ophthalmology service due to their persisting vergence
stabilization and thus to vertigo. Such hypothesis is in abnormalities. The investigation adhered to the tenets
line with electrophysiological and behavioral studies of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by
showing the modulation of VOR by viewing distance.16 the institutional human experimentation committee.
For children studied by Bucci et al.,3,4 cortical areas Informed consent was obtained from each subject after
such as the parietal cortex and the frontal lobe involved the nature of the procedure had been explained.

Q. Yang et al. 94
TABLE 1A Clinical characteristics of subjects
Subject Visual Stereoacuity NPC Heterophoria Divergence Convergence Main clinical
(years) acuity (TNO) (cm) (pD) (pD) pD materials

S1 LE:10/10 40 6 Far: 4E Far: 2 Far: 25 Vertigo, headache


(28) RE:10/10 Near: 4E Near: 6 Near: 40 Visual fatigue
S2 LE:10/10 40 6 Far: 6x Far: 2 Far: 16 Vertigo, headache
(31) RE:10/10 Near: 1x Near: 12 Near: 18 Visual fatigue

S3 LE:10/10 40 6 Far: ortho Far: 4 Far: 40 Vertigo


(37) RE:10/10 Near: ortho near: 14 Near: 40 Visual fatigue

S4 LE:10/10 40 4 Far: ortho Far: 4 Far: 40 Rotatory vertigo


(56) RE:10/10 Near: 6x Near: 12 Near: 40 Visual fatigue
S5 LE:10/10 120 6 Far: 8 Far: 12 Vertigo
(37) RE:10/10 Near: 12 Near: 40 Medication of antivertigo
S6 LE:10/10 120 20 Far: 3x Far: 4 Far: 6 Vertigo
(54) RE:10/10 Near: ortho Near: 6 Near: 25

Note: NPC - near point of convergence.

Visual Display called for a pure vergence eye movement, along the
median plane. When it was at the same circle it called
The visual display consisted of LEDs (each LED on
for a pure saccade (left or right), and when it was lateral
2.9 mm of diameter) placed at two isovergence circles:
and on the other circle the required eye movement was
one at 20 cm from the subject, and the other at 150 cm.
a combined saccade and vergence eye movement. Note
At each viewing distance three LEDs were used; one
that our use of saccades both to near and to distant
at the center and the other at 20 . At 20 cm, the
targets allowed us to keep target direction and depth
required mean vergence angle for fixating any of these
unpredictable for all trials. All target LEDs for saccades
three LEDs was 17 ; at 150 cm such angle was 2.3 .
were at 20 . All targets along the median plane required
a change in ocular vergence of 15 ; similarly, combined
Oculomotor Tasks movements required a saccade of 20 and a vergence of
In a dark room the subject was seated in an adapted- 15 . In each block, the three types of eye movements
chair with a head and chin support. He/she viewed were interleaved randomly. Each block contained 72
binocularly and faced the 3D visual display of the trials, i.e. 12 trials per type of movement, saccades to
LEDs. The distance between the subject and the close distant targets, saccades to near targets, convergence,
isovergence surface was at 20 cm. The visual display of divergence, combined convergent movements and
the LEDs was placed at eye level to avoid vertical eye combined divergent movements. For each subject, two
movements. blocks were run, separated by a rest of few minutes; cal-
Each trial started by lighting a fixation LED at the ibrations were repeated at the beginning of each block.
center of one of the circles (distant or close). After For calibration the subject made a sequence of sac-
a 1.5 s fixational period the central LED was turned cades to a LED target jumping from zero to 10 ,
off and a target-LED appeared for 1 s. When the 20 . During each of these trials, the target remained
target-LED was on the center of the other circle it at each location for 2 s; the subject was instructed to

TABLE 1B Normal values of near point of convergence (NPC), of heterophoria and of the range of divergence and convergence
amplitudes at far and close viewing distance measured in adults

Heterophoria
Stereoacuity NPC (cm) (Dp) Divergence (Dp) Convergence (Dp)

<60 <10 Far: 02 XP Far: 59 Far: 1523


Close : 46 XP Close : 1523 Close: 1824

95 Vergence Abnormalities in a Pseudo-Vestibular Syndrome


fixate the LED as accurately as possible; the LED pre- of amplitude of eye movements (amplitude difference
sentation was sufficiently long to allow accurate and between i and e in degree) to target amplitude; the
stable fixation. From these recordings were extracted mean velocity, ratio of amplitude of eye movement to
the calibration factors. duration of eye movement (time difference between
i and e). To evaluate the difference between any two
Eye Movement Recording different types of eye movements the non-parametric
Wilcoxon test was used. The U-Mann-Whitney test
Horizontal movements from both eyes were
was used to compare the difference between adults
recorded simultaneously with the IRIS SKALAR
with vertigo and control adults.
device. Eye position signals were low-pass filtered with
a cut-off-frequency of 200 Hz and were digitized with
a 12-bit analogue-to-digital converter and each channel RESULTS
was sampled at 500 Hz. This system has an optimal res-
olution of 2 minutes of arc and linear range of 25
Latency
degrees. Data collection was directed by REX, software Differences According to Type
developed for real-time experiments and run on a PC. of Movement
Figure 2 presents individual and group mean laten-
Data Analysis cies together with standard errors for each type of eye
Calibration factors for each eye were extracted from movements, pure saccade or saccade components of
the saccades recorded in the calibration task; a linear combined movements (A), and pure vergence or ver-
function was used to fit the calibration data. From the gence components of combined movements (B) in
two individual calibrated eye position signals we derived adults with vertigo. The non-parametric Wilcoxon test
the vergence signal (left eye right eye) and the con- showed that, as in normals, the latency for saccades to
jugate signal ((left eye + right eye)/2). Figure 1 shows distant targets or saccade components of combined di-
an example of pure saccades (A), Pure convergence (B), vergent movements was significantly longer than that
combined convergent movements (C), combined diver- for saccades to near targets (both Z = 2.2, p < 0.05).
gent movements (D). For each type of these movements In contrast, there was no statistically significant differ-
are shown the saccadic or conjugate components and ence of latencies between convergence and divergence
the vergence or disconjugate components. The onset (Z = 0.73, p = 0.46), nor between pure vergence and
of a pure saccade or of the saccadic component of the vergence components of combined movements (both
combined movements was defined as the time when eye Z < 1.15, p > 0.05), nor between saccades to distant tar-
velocity exceeded 450 /s (mark i in figure A, C or D); gets and saccade components of combined convergent
the offset when eye velocity dropped below 10 /s (mark movements (Z = 0.10, p = 0.92).
e in figure A, C, D). The onset and the offset of the
vergence signals (for pure vergence movement and for Comparison with Normal Adults
the vergence component of the combined movements) The nonparametric U-Mann-Whitney test was used
were defined as the time point when the eye velocity ex- to compare mean latencies between adults with vertigo
ceeded or dropped below 5 /s (marks i and e in figures and control adults (15 subjects) without vertigo symp-
B, C, D, respectively). The placement of the markers by toms (horizontal lines in figure indicating mean values
the computer was verified by one of the investigators of control subjects) data extracted from the study of
scrutinizing saccade and vergence components on the Yang et al. [24]. Latencies of saccades to distant targets
screen. Eye movements in the wrong direction, move- were significantly longer for subjects with vertigo than
ments with latency shorter than 100 ms or longer than for control adults (U = 18, p < 0.05), but there was no
1000 ms or movements contaminated by blinks were significant difference for saccades to near targets (U =
rejected. These criteria are standard, also used by others 38, p = 0.59). Latency of divergence was significantly
[9, 17, 26]. About five percent of trials had to be rejected. longer for subjects with vertigo than for control adults
For all types of eye movements, we measured the (U = 18.5, p < 0.05). For convergence, the difference of
latency, the time difference between onset of eye latency between these two adult groups was marginally
movement (i) and onset of target; the gain, the ratio significant (U = 21, p < 0.06). There was no significant

Q. Yang et al. 96
FIGURE 1 Examples of eye movements. (A) saccades (to left or to right); (B) vergence ( convergence or divergence); (C) combined
convergent movements (with saccades to left or to right); (D) combined divergent movements (with saccades to left or to right). On the
right side of each figure shown the excursion of the target. The arrows i and e indicate the onset and the end of eye movements,
respectively. Time at 0 ms indicates the target onset.

97 Vergence Abnormalities in a Pseudo-Vestibular Syndrome


FIGURE 2 Individual mean latency with standard errors (SE) of saccades or saccade component of combined movements (A), and
of vergence or vergence components of combined movements (B). The right bar for group means based on 6 subjects, on which the
horizontal line is mean value form normal adults. * indicates the statistically significant difference in latency between normal adults and
subjects with vertigo.

Q. Yang et al. 98
difference of latency for any components of combined divergent) and they are regrouped. The Wilcoxon test
eye movements between the two adult groups (all U > showed significantly higher mean velocity for conver-
25, p > 0.05). At the individual level, inspection of gence than for divergence (Z = 1.99, p < 0.05). To test
the data in Figure 2, shows that the above conclusion the saccade-vergence interaction, i.e. the well-known
applies for the majority of subjects except subject S5 acceleration of vergence by saccades in combined
who was taken a drug against vertigo. movements [24], we will compare pure vergence with
In summary, vertigo subjects have longer latencies vergence components of combined movements, pure
of the majority of eye movements than for controls. saccades with saccade components of combined move-
Because of such slowing the normal differences between ments. Contrary to what is known in controls [24],
types of movements appear less for the group of vertigo there was no significant difference of mean velocities
subjects. between vergence and vergence components of com-
Accuracy bined movements (Z = 0.31, p = 0.75 for convergence
and Z = 0.94, p = 0.35 for divergence). Similar to
Differences According to Type controls, adults with vertigo still showed significantly
of Movement higher mean velocities for pure saccades than saccade
Figure 3 presents individual and group mean gains components of combined movements (Z = 2.2, p <
(eye-movement amplitude/target amplitude) together 0.05). Thus, in subjects with vertigo the saccade did
with standard errors for pure movements (saccade or not accelerate vergence but it was itself slowed.
vergence, A) and combined movements (B) in adults
with vertigo. As there was no significant difference in Comparison with Normal Adults
the accuracy between saccades to distant targets and to The nonparametric U-Mann-Whitney test was used
near targets, nor between the two saccade components to compare mean velocities between adults with vertigo
of combined movements (convergent and divergent) and control adults (mean values indicated by horizontal
they were grouped. The Wilcoxon test showed that lines in Fig. 4). Relative to controls, adults with vertigo
the mean gain (saccade amplitude/target amplitude) of showed significantly lower mean velocities for conver-
saccade components of combined movements was sig- gence (U = 0, p < 0.001), for divergence (U = 14,
nificantly reduced relative to pure saccades (Z = 2.2, p < p < 0.05), and for convergence components (U =11,
0.05), but there was no difference between convergence p < 0.01) or divergence components (U = 0, p < 0.001)
and divergence (Z = 0.73, p = 0.46), nor between pure of combined movements. There was no statistically sig-
vergence and corresponding vergence components of nificant difference of mean velocity for pure saccades
combined eye movements (both Z < 0.73, p > 0.05). (U = 27, p = 0.16), for saccade components of com-
bined movements (U = 32, p = 0.31).
Comparison with Normal Adults
The nonparametric U-Mann-Whitney test showed
DISCUSSION
no significant difference of gains of any type of eye
movements between adults with vertigo and control To summarize, the data show that adults with vertigo
adults (All U > 27, p > 0.05). without abnormal vestibular function or other pathol-
ogy present abnormalities in their eye movements,
Mean Velocity mainly in their temporal-dynamics parameters (laten-
cies, speed). Two types of abnormalities are seen. Firstly,
Differences According to Type one does not observe several of the specific characteris-
of Movement tics of eye movements described previously in controls.
Figure 4 presents individual and group mean veloc- Vertigo adults show no longer latency for convergence
ities together with standard errors for pure movements than for divergence nor longer latency for correspond-
(saccades or vergence, A) and combined movements ing components of combined movements than for
(B) in adults with vertigo. There was no significant dif- pure movements (saccades or vergence); they show no
ference of mean velocities between saccades to distant higher speed for vergence combined with saccades than
targets and to near targets, nor between two saccade for vergence alone. Secondly, relative to values from
components of combined movements (convergent and control adults, they show longer latencies of saccades

99 Vergence Abnormalities in a Pseudo-Vestibular Syndrome


FIGURE 3 Individual mean gain with standard errors (SE) of pure movements (saccades, convergence or divergence, A), and combined
movements (B). The right bar for group means based on 6 subjects, on which the horizontal line is mean value form normal adults. Dotted
horizontal lines indicate a gain of 1, which corresponds to a perfectly accurate eye movement (eye movement amplitude = target excursion
amplitude).

to distant targets, for divergence and convergence vertigo a similar difference exists, and is even more ac-
(tendency), slower speed for convergence, divergence centuated than in control adults (63 vs 17 ms). Yang
and vergence components of combined movements. et al.24 considered that this difference between viewing
These findings will be discussed further next. distances could be due to facilitation of oculomotor
or attention disengagement at close. The same factors
Latency could explain the prolongation of latency for saccades
to distant targets in adults with vertigo. Relative to con-
Saccades to Distant-Near Targets trol adults, adults with vertigo showed longer latency
Latency of saccades is longer to distant targets than for saccades to distant targets but not for saccades to
to near targets in normal adults.24,26 For adults with near targets. It seems that initiating saccades to distant

Q. Yang et al. 100


FIGURE 4 Individual mean speed (mean velocity) with standard errors (SE) of pure movements (saccades, convergence or divergence,
A), and combined movements (B). The right bar for group means based on 6 subjects, on which the horizontal line is mean value form
normal adults. indicates the statistically significant difference in speed between normal adults and subjects with vertigo.

targets is more difficult in adults with vertigo and the fa- vergence and divergence relative to control adults hide
cilitation of disengagement of oculomotor fixation and the difference; convergence tended to have longer la-
visual attention to near targets allow them to maintain tency than divergence but did not reach statistical sig-
normal behavior. nificance (252 vs 240 ms, Z = 0.73, p = 0.46). The
examination of individual data in this study showed
Convergence-Divergence that two vertigo subjects showed longer latency for di-
Control adults studied in same conditions showed vergence. The more solid result of clinical relevance
longer latency of convergence than of divergence.24 is the increase of latencies for both convergence and
Adults with vertigo showed no difference of latency divergence for adults with vertigo compared to con-
between convergence and divergence. A possible rea- trols. These results are compatible with the study in
son was that the increase of latencies for both con- children with vertigo,4 which also showed increased

101 Vergence Abnormalities in a Pseudo-Vestibular Syndrome


latency of convergence and divergence by 145 ms and localize the target in depth correctly. However, adults
50 ms longer than of control children; in adults with with vertigo maintained a good accuracy for vergence
vertigo latencies are longer by 33 ms and 42 ms relative although they have vergence problems in the clinical
to control adults. tests. The dual-mode model of vergence control in-
cludes a fast, open-loop movement and a slower move-
Pure Versus Combined ment under visual feedback control; such control would
allow us to obtain accurate vergence movements.11 The
In controls (adults or children), latencies of both
dual-mode model could explain why despite the latency
saccade and vergence components of combined
prolongation of vergence the accuracy was maintained
movements are longer than corresponding pure eye
normal as they could adjust their accuracy on the
movements [19, 24]; this is believed to be due to the
basis of visual feedback (the slow-control mechanism).
higher complexity of preparation of two components.
But for pure saccades and saccade components, our
Combined movements were not longer to prepare for
vertigo subjects showed as good accuracy as controls,
vertigo adults except saccade components of combine
similar to vertigo children studied by Bucci et al.3 Note
divergent movements relative to pure saccades to
that these saccades are rapid movements and visual
near targets. However, the absence of such difference
feedback during their execution cannot modify their
is due to the fact that pure movements are already
accuracy. Therefore, one should conclude that all eye
longer to initiate than in controls. Saccades to distant
movements, fast or slow, remain accurate in vertigo
targets, convergence and divergence were initiated with
adults.
longer latencies in adults with vertigo than in controls.
Takagi et al. [19] proposed the conceptual model for
controlling combined movements. Once a common Speed
decision to generate an eye movement to a new target
The abnormality of speed of eye movements con-
in direction and in depth is made for saccades and
cerns the interaction between saccades and vergence
vergence at the same time, the activities are passed to
during their combination. In controls, saccades com-
parallel saccade and vergence trigger mechanisms. This
bined with vergence are slowed down relative to pure
model could explain the different initiation of saccade
saccades while vergence combined with saccade are
and vergence during combined movements. Moreover,
speeded relative to pure vergence.6,27 Zee et al.27 pro-
it also suggested that the latency prolongation during
posed three different models to attempt to explain the
combined movements was not simple addition based
interaction between saccade and vergence, i.e. a model
on pure movements but there existed an interaction
based on the existence of saccade-related vergence
between saccade and vergence. For adults with vertigo,
burst neurons (SVBN), a model based on separated
the latency of pure eye movements (saccades or
right and left eye burst neurons and a model based on
vergence) was increased to the level of combined
premotor vergence velocity neurons. Their simulation
movements while combined saccade-vergence move-
results favored the first and third model. The SVBN
ments maintained their initiation time like in control
model was modified recently by Kumar et al.,15 which
adults.
is a Hering-type model that assumes separate saccadic
In general, latency initiation problems are believed
and vergence systems but common commands to the
to be cortical dysfunction of parietal and frontal
two eyes. A different view is presented in a Helmholtz-
areas.12,1718,2527 The abnormalities observed here
type model, which hypothesizes that monocular burst
indicate problems of this level particularly for iso-
neurons could determine the dynamic properties of
lated convergence, divergence and saccades to distant
both conjugate (saccadic) and disconjugate (vergence)
targets.
components.14 Electrophysiological evidence exists to
support both Hering- and Helmholtz-type models, but
Accuracy the interpretation of these data remains controversial.15
Bucci et al.3 reported that children with vertigo Despite theoretical controversies behavioral data show
showed low accuracy for both convergence and diver- convincingly that vergence is accelerated by the
gence. They suggested that such poor vergence accuracy saccade. Yet, in our subjects vergence speed was not
could be caused by a reduced ability of their subjects to increased by the saccade. Most likely, the central

Q. Yang et al. 102


interaction between saccadic and vergence systems is Conclusion
dysfunctioning in such subjects at the brainstem level.
The present study reports abnormalities in saccades
and vergence for adults with vertigo but without ab-
Possible Dysfunction of Neural
normal vestibular functions. Relative to control adults,
Structures Involved in Vertigo these subjects had latency prolongation and speed slow
Many cortical and subcortical structures are in- for some types of eye movements, particularly for ver-
volved in the control of saccades and vergence. For gence, pure or combined. These results could be use-
example, posterior parietal cortex (PPC), frontal eye ful for the differential diagnosis of pseudo-vestibular
field (FEF) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) are involved syndrome (without vestibular dysfunction). Oculomo-
more in the control of initiation of both saccades tor training is suggested for such subjects with vertigo
and vergence.12,13,16,18 At the brainstem level distinct to improve their abnormal eye movements and reduce
circuits are known to control saccades and vregence: symptoms.
the paramedian pontine reticular formation (PPRF)
for saccades, the mesencephalic reticular formation ACKNOWLEDGMENT
(MRF) for vergence.16 Our results show in adults with
vertigo the existence of a specific problem initiating Q. Yang was supported by European Union (QLK6-
saccades to distant targets only but a general problem CT-2002-00151: EUROKINESIS) and CNRS/CTI,
for vergence, e.g. latency prolongation and low speed Handicap contract.
for both convergence and divergence, and no accel-
eration of vergence by the saccade during combined REFERENCES
movements. Vergence problems could be related to [1] Anoh-Tanon MJ, Bremond-Gignac D, Wiener-Vacher SR. Vertigo is
cortical-subcortical mechanisms that are specific to an underestimated symptom of ocular disorders: dizzy children do
vergence and its interaction with saccades. not always need MRI. Pediatr Neurol. 2000;23:4953.
[2] Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Bremond-Gignac D, Wiener-Vacher S. Binoc-
ular coordination of saccades in children with vertigo: dependency
Clinical Consideration on the vergence state. Vision Res. 2006;46:35943602.
[3] Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Bremond-Gignac D, Wiener-Vacher
Vertigo without abnormal vestibular function is S. Speed-accuracy of saccades, vergence and combined eye move-
ments in children with vertigo. Exp Brain Res. 2004;157:286295.
often difficult to diagnose in clinic. Our results showing [4] Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Wiener-Vacher S, Bremond-Gignac D.
that adults with vertigo but without abnormal vestibu- Abnormality of vergence latency in children with vertigo. J Neurol.
lar function had both saccades and vergence problems, 2004;251:204213.
[5] Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM. Trajectories of the human
particularly for vergence, could provide supplementary binocular fixation point during conjugate and non-conjugate gaze-
tools for helping to diagnose such functional problems. shifts. Vision Res. 1997;37:10491069.
[6] Collewijn H, Erkelens CJ, Steinman RM. Voluntary binocular gaze-
Orthoptic vergence training is widely used by clinicians shifts in the plane of regard: dynamics of version and vergence.
to treat vergence abnormalities.20 Our group3,4 demon- Vision Res. 1995;35:33353358.
strated that the orthoptic vergence training in children [7] de Haller R, Maire R, Borruat FX. Visual vertigo: an observa-
tional case series of eleven patients. Klin Monatsbl Augenheilkd.
with vertigo but without abnormal vestibular function 2004;221:383385.
could eliminate vertigo symptoms, improve vergence [8] Fischer B. The preparation of visually guided saccades. Rev Physiol
Biochem Pharmacol. 1987;106:135.
capabilities clinically assessed, shorten latency of eye [9] Fukushima J, Hatta T, Fukushima K. Development of voluntary con-
movements and improve the accuracy of different eye trol of saccadic eye movements. I. Age-related changes in normal
movements. Perhaps the orthoptic vergence training children. Brain Dev. 2000;22:173180.
[10] Hung GK. Saccade-vergence trajectories under free- and
is also effective for adult subjects to improve their eye instrument-space environments. Curr Eye Res. 1998;17:159
movements, but this needs further investigation. We 164.
[11] Hung GK, Ciuffreda KJ. Models of saccade-vergence interaction. In:
suggest that the oculomotor training, i.e. making a Models of visual system. Kluwer Acadelic/Penum Publishers, 2002.
number of saccades, vergence and their combination [12] Kapoula Z, Isotalo E, Muri RM, Bucci MP, Rivaud-Pechoux S. Ef-
everyday or periodically could be used as the im- fects of transcranial magnetic stimulation of the posterior pari-
etal cortex on saccades and vergence. Neuroreport 2001;12:4041
provement training for subjects with vertigo. Fischer8 4046.
reported that even for normal subjects, saccade latency [13] Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Coubard O, Daunys G, Orssaud C. Transcra-
nial magnetic stimulation of the posterior parietal cortex delays
after the training of a repetition of 200 saccades per the latency of both isolated and combined vergence-saccade move-
day for a period of 10 days could be decreased. ments in humans. Neurosci Lett. 2004;360:9599.

103 Vergence Abnormalities in a Pseudo-Vestibular Syndrome


[14] King WM, Zhou W. Neural basis of disjunctive eye movements. Ann [21] Wiener-Vacher SR, Amanou L, Denise P, Narcy P, Manach Y. Vestibu-
N Y Acad Sci. 2002;956:273283. lar function in children with the CHARGE association. Arch Oto-
[15] Kumar AN, Han YH, Kirsch RF, DellOsso LF, King WM, Leigh RJ. Tests laryngol Head Neck Surg. 1999;125:342347.
of models for saccade-vergence interaction using novel stimulus [22] Wiener-Vacher SR, Mazda K. Asymmetric otolith vestibulo-
conditions. Biol Cybern. 2006;95:143157. ocular responses in children with idiopathic scoliosis. J Pediatr.
[16] Leigh RJ, Zee DS. The neurology of eye movement. Oxford University 1998;132:10281032.
Press, New York, 2006. [23] Wiener-Vacher SR, Toupet F, Narcy P. Canal and otolith vestibulo-
[17] Munoz DP, Broughton JR, Goldring JE, Armstrong IT. Age-related ocular reflexes to vertical and off vertical axis rotations in children
performance of human subjects on saccadic eye movement tasks. learning to walk. Acta Otolaryngol 1996;116:657665.
Exp Brain Res. 1998;121:391400. [24] Yang Q, Bucci MP, Kapoula Z. The latency of saccades, vergence, and
[18] Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Muri R, Vermer- combined eye movements in children and in adults. Invest Ophthal-
sch AI. Cortical control of saccades. Ann Neurol. 1995;37:557 mol Vis Sci. 2002;43:29392949.
567. [25] Yang Q, Kapoula Z. Saccade-vergence dynamics and interaction in
[19] Takagi M, Frohman EM, Zee DS. Gap-overlap effects on latencies of children and in adults. Exp Brain Res. 2003;156:212223.
saccades, vergence and combined vergence-saccades in humans. [26] Yang Q, Kapoula Z, Debay E, Coubard O, Orssaud C, Samson M.
Vision Res. 1995;35:33733388. Prolongation of latency of horizontal saccades in elderly is distance
[20] Von Noorden G, Campos E. Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility and task specific. Vision Res. 2006;46:751759.
theory and management of strabismus. The C.V. Mosby Company, [27] Zee DS, Fitzgibbon EJ, Optican LM. Saccade-vergence interactions
St. Louis, 2002. in humans. J Neurophysiol 1992;68:16241641.

Q. Yang et al. 104

S-ar putea să vă placă și