Sunteți pe pagina 1din 15

Justin A.

Lavner University of Georgia


Benjamin R. Karney and Thomas N. Bradbury University of California Los Angeles

Does Couples Communication Predict Marital


Satisfaction, or Does Marital Satisfaction Predict
Communication?

The quality of communication between spouses predict communication and satisfaction and that
is widely assumed to affect their subsequent strengthen or moderate their association.
judgments of relationship satisfaction, yet this
assumption is rarely tested against the alterna-
tive prediction that communication is merely a Communication occupies a central role in
consequence of spouses prior levels of satisfac- models of relationship deterioration, as inti-
tion. To evaluate these perspectives, newlywed mate bonds are believed to remain strong to
couples positivity, negativity, and effectiveness the extent that partners respond with sensi-
were observed four times at 9-month intervals, tivity to one another (e.g., Reis & Patrick,
and these behaviors were examined in relation 1996). Nonetheless, evidence substantiating the
to corresponding self-reports of relationship sat- critical importance of communication comes
isfaction. Cross-sectionally, relatively satisfied almost exclusively from cross-sectional studies
couples engaged in more positive, less negative, (Woodin, 2011) and from longitudinal studies
and more effective communication. Longitudi- in which communication observed at one time
nally, reliable communication-to-satisfaction point is used to predict later marital satisfaction
and satisfaction-to-communication associations (Karney & Bradbury, 1995). If changes in com-
were identified, yet neither pathway was par- munication are truly the mechanism by which
ticularly robust. These findings raise important satisfaction changes, however, longitudinal data
doubts about theories and interventions that on communication behaviors are needed to
prioritize couple communication skills as the key show that communication consistently predicts
predictor of relationship satisfaction, while rais- changes in satisfaction over time. Moreover, in
ing new questions about other factors that might the absence of such data, cause and effect cannot
be disentangled: Actual effects of communica-
tion on later satisfaction might be overstated if
Department of Psychology, Psychology Building, earlier assessments of satisfaction are generating
University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602 variability in later communication. In the current
(lavner@uga.edu).
study we addressed this gap by using four waves
Department of Psychology, University of California, 1285
of observed communication and self-reported
Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 900951563. satisfaction data from a sample of newlywed
This article was edited by Rob Crosnoe. couples to examine whether communication
Key Words: communication, low-income families, marriage predicts changes in satisfaction and whether
and close relationships, satisfaction. satisfaction predicts changes in communication.
Journal of Marriage and Family (2016) 1
DOI:10.1111/jomf.12301
2 Journal of Marriage and Family

Brief Review of Research: Communication reliably distinguishes between distressed and


and Marital Satisfaction nondistressed couples in the cross section
(Woodin, 2011), how is it that poor communi-
Guided by social exchange theory, early
cation does not consistently predict relationship
approaches argued that happy marriages could
distress? One possibility is that communication
be distinguished from unhappy marriages by
and satisfaction are correlated concurrently not
the ratio of positive to negative behavior in
because communication predicts satisfaction but
the relationship (Jacobson & Margolin, 1979).
because satisfaction predicts communication.
Since then, cross-sectional studies have consis-
This idea is consistent with long-standing evi-
tently indicated that distressed couples display
dence from the social psychological literature
more negative communication behaviors and
that attitudes guide behavior (e.g., Ajzen &
fewer positive communication behaviors dur-
Fishbein, 1977) and would suggest that cou-
ing conflict resolution tasks than relatively
ples global evaluations of their relationship
satisfied couples (Bradbury & Karney, 2013).
should predict how partners behave toward one
Behavioral theory extended these findings to
another. As such, communication may be a
posit that marital distress is a consequence of
consequence of marital satisfaction rather than
poor communication, arguing that distress
a cause.
results from couples aversive and ineffectual
Support for this competing theoretical
response to conflict (Koerner & Jacobson,
perspective would have important applied
1994, p. 208).
implications. Because communication has been
Evidence for the notion that poor commu-
viewed as the key mechanism underlying rela-
nication predicts couple outcomes is mixed. tionship functioning, interventions designed
Consistent with the aforementioned pattern, to prevent or ameliorate couples distress have
low levels of positive affect and high levels of emphasized communication skills (e.g., Benson,
negative skills predict steeper declines in marital McGinn, & Christensen, 2012; Rogge, Cobb,
satisfaction over time (Johnson et al., 2005), Lawrence, Johnson, & Bradbury, 2013). In
negative behaviors observed at baseline distin- particular, this focus on decreasing negative
guish between satisfied and dissatisfied intact communication and increasing positive commu-
couples at 10-year follow-up (Kiecolt-Glaser, nication forms the core agenda in large-scale,
Bane, Glaser, & Malarkey, 2003), and cou- federally sponsored tests of leading couple
ples who express more negativity in the first 2 education programs (e.g., Hsueh et al., 2012;
years of marriage report greater unhappiness Wood, Moore, Clarkwest, & Killwald, 2014),
in their marriages after more than a decade following the assumption that improving cou-
compared to couples who are more positive ples communication will improve relationships
early on (Huston, Caughlin, Houts, Smith, and, ultimately, prevent relationship dissolution.
& George, 2001). However, other studies are This focus is appropriate if poor communication
inconsistent with this general pattern, revealing is the root of marital distress. If poor commu-
counterintuitive associations between negative nication is a symptom or correlate of distress,
communication and changes in satisfaction. however, prevention programs targeting com-
Husbands negativity has been shown to pre- munication may prove less useful than programs
dict a positive change in wives satisfaction targeting more proximal mechanisms generat-
1 year later, for example, and is unrelated ing distress. Thus, clarifying the relationship
to their own satisfaction (Heavey, Layne, & between communication and marital satisfaction
Christensen, 1993); more negative communi- may advance understanding of their association
cation predicts slower, not faster, declines in and inform intervention.
satisfaction (Karney & Bradbury, 1997); and few Understanding the antecedentconsequent
links are found between positive communica- associations involving communication and
tion and satisfaction trajectories (e.g., Markman, satisfaction therefore requires multiwave assess-
Rhoades, Stanley, Ragan, & Whitton, 2010). ments of both variables. Yet few studies to date
have assessed communication at multiple time
points, limiting our ability to directly test
Considering Bidirectional Linkages
these questions. Implementing a multiwave
These findings pose a critical challenge for design also allows for new questions about
behavioral theories: If poor communication whether the communication-to-satisfaction and
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 3

satisfaction-to-communication effects have new information about which is a stronger


differential temporal sequencing, such that mar- predictor.
ital satisfaction initially predicts communication We considered two factors that may affect
early in marriage whereas communication these general patterns. First, we examined
predicts marital satisfaction as time passes. whether the relationship between communica-
Prior research on the association between tion and marital satisfaction varies depending
communication and satisfaction is also limited on what type of communication is being con-
by its focus on middle-class Caucasian cou- sidered. We can distinguish between several
ples, which narrows the range of experiences different types of communication behavior,
captured and limits the generalizability of find- including positive communication (warmth,
ings. Studying samples that are culturally and endearment), negative communication (hostil-
economically diverse is especially important in ity, contempt), and effective communication
light of the interventions described earlier, as (assertiveness, generating solutions); each of
recent federal initiatives have sought to develop these may operate differently. Kim, Capaldi,
and deliver communication-based interventions and Crosby (2007) found that positive emotion
to ethnically diverse low-income couples (Hsueh was more important than negative emotion
et al., 2012; Wood et al., 2014). The theoretical in predicting subsequent marital satisfaction,
assumption underlying these modelsthat bet- consistent with the view that positivity serves
ter communication yields stronger and more ful- a predictive role in promoting intimacy and
filling relationshipshas yet to be tested in these enhancing relationship functioning. However,
populations, however. other theoretical frameworksmost notably
Gottmans (1994) Four Horseman of the
Apocalypsepredict that negativity should
The Current Study prove especially destructive to relationship satis-
In this study we used four waves of data from faction. It is also possible that low levels of effec-
a sample of low-income, ethnically diverse tive communication may serve to undermine
newlywed couples studied over the first 3 years the relationship, whereas positivity may only
of marriage to examine the direction of the be the result of positive feelings about the rela-
relationship(s) between marital satisfaction tionship. Accordingly, we considered separate
and observed communication. The early years models for positivity, negativity, and effective-
of marriage are an ideal time to study these ness to allow for the possibility that the pattern
associations because they are a period of sig- of results may vary across communication
nificant risk and change for many couples (e.g., type.
Kreider & Ellis, 2011). Disentangling associa- Second, we examined reciprocal associations
tions between satisfaction and communication between spouses own satisfaction and commu-
also requires studying them before any linkages nication (e.g., husband satisfaction and husband
between them become too well-established, thus negativity) and between their satisfaction and
necessitating research early in couples marital their partners communication (e.g., husband
careers. satisfaction and wife negativity). Within the
The antecedentconsequent models yield two marital literature there has been a great deal of
sets of basic predictions: (1) communication at interest in partner effects in domains such as per-
one time point should lead to changes in satisfac- sonality (e.g., Luo, et al., 2008) and stress (e.g.,
tion at a subsequent time point, consistent with Neff & Karney, 2007), but there has been less
behavioral models, and (2) satisfaction at one attention to these processes within the context of
time point should lead to changes in communi- communication. Examining partner effects can
cation at a subsequent time point, consistent with provide a test of the robustness of the within-sex
attitudebehavior models. Bidirectional associ- effects and also allows for the possibility that
ations between satisfaction and communication within-spouse and cross-spouse effects will
may also be present, indicating that communi- take different forms. For example, satisfaction
cation and satisfaction mutually reinforce one might predict ones own future communication
another. In addition, simultaneously examining behaviors, consistent with attitudebehavior
communication-to-satisfaction and satisfaction- models, but communication might predict the
to-communication allowed us to compare the partners subsequent satisfaction. This study
relative magnitude of the pathways, providing examined these possibilities.
4 Journal of Marriage and Family

Method averaged 4.8 months in duration (SD = 2.5), and


Sampling 38.5% of couples had children. Mens mean age
was 27.9 (SD = 5.8), and womens mean age
The sampling procedure was designed to yield was 26.3 (SD = 5.0). Wives had a mean income
participants who were first-married newlywed of $28,672 (SD = $24,549), and husbands had
couples in which partners were of the same eth- a mean income of $34,153 (SD = $27,094).
nicity, living in low-income neighborhoods in Twelve percent of couples were African Amer-
Los Angeles County. Recently married couples ican, 12% were Caucasian, and 76% were
were identified through names and addresses Hispanic, which is comparable to the proportion
on marriage license applications in 2009 and of people living in the sampled neighborhoods in
2010. Addresses were matched with census Los Angeles County (12.9% African American,
data to identify applicants living in low-income 14.7% Caucasian, and 60.5% Hispanic; U.S.
communities, defined as census block groups Census Bureau, 2002). Of the Hispanic couples,
wherein the median household income was no 33% spoke Spanish during their interactions and
more than 160% of the 1999 federal poverty 67% spoke English. All African American and
level for a four-person family. Next, names on Caucasian couples spoke English during their
the licenses were weighted using data from interactions.
a Bayesian Census Surname Combination,
which integrates census and surname informa-
tion to produce a multinomial probability of Procedure
membership in each of four racial/ethnic cate- Couples were visited in their homes by
gories (Hispanic, African American, Asian, and two trained interviewers who described the
Caucasian/other). Couples were chosen using IRB-approved study and obtained written
probabilities proportionate to the ratio of tar- informed consent from each participant. The
get prevalences to the population prevalences, marital satisfaction measure was administered
weighted by the couples average estimated orally to participants by an interviewer who
probability of being Hispanic, African Ameri- entered their responses immediately via com-
can, or Caucasian, which are the three largest puter. After completing this and other self-report
racial/ethnic groups among people living in measures individually, partners were reunited
poverty in Los Angeles County (U.S. Census for three 8-minute videotaped discussions.
Bureau, 2002; see also Elliott, et al., 2013). For the first interaction, which was designed to
These couples were telephoned and screened assess problem-solving behaviors, partners were
to ensure that they had married, that neither asked to identify a topic of disagreement in their
partner had been previously married, and that relationship and then to devote 8 minutes to
both spouses identified as Hispanic, African working toward a mutually satisfying resolution
American, or Caucasian. A total of 3,793 cou- of that topic. For the second interaction, which
ples were contacted through addresses listed on was designed to assess social support behaviors,
their marriage licenses; of those, 2,049 could one randomly chosen spouse was asked to talk
not be reached and 1,522 (40%) responded about something you would like to change
to the mailing and agreed to be screened for about yourself while the partner was instructed
eligibility. Of those who responded and agreed to be involved in the discussion and respond
to be screened for eligibility, 824 couples were in whatever way you wish. Spouses were
screened as eligible, and 658 of those couples instructed to avoid selecting or discussing any
agreed to participate in the study, with 431 cou- topics that were sources of tension or difficulty
ples actually completing the study. The response within the relationship. After a short break, a
rate to the initial screening compares favorably third discussion was held that was identical to
to other studies of newlywed couples recruited the second discussion, with the roles reversed.
from marriage licenses (e.g., 17.8% in Johnson Couples were debriefed and paid $75 for
et al., 2005; 18% in Kurdek, 1991). participating.
These procedures were repeated three more
times at approximately 9-month intervals subse-
Participants
quent to the initial assessment (i.e., Wave 2 = 18
For the 431 couples who completed the study, months into marriage; Wave 3 = 27 months into
at the time of initial assessment, marriages marriage; Wave 4 = 36 months into marriage).
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 5

After completing each phase, couples were paid for each code across the three discussion tasks,
for participating ($100 at Wave 2, $125 at Wave to investigate their latent structure. The scree
3, and $150 at Wave 4). plot suggested three factors (i.e., positivity,
negativity, effectiveness) for husbands and for
Behavioral Observation wives (Cattell, 1966), which explained 35.7%
Videotapes were scored by 16 trained coders of the total variance for husbands and 34.7%
using the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales of the total variance for wives. Adding a fourth
(IFIRS; Melby et al., 1998). Codersfive of factor accounted for only an additional 3.6% of
whom were native Spanish speakerscoded the variance for husbands and 5.1% for wives
only in their native language. Factor analysis and was not indicated by the scree plot (for
was used to reduce the IFIRS codes to three details, see Williamson, Bradbury, Trail, &
scales: positivity, negativity, and effective- Karney, 2011). The means, standard deviations,
ness. At Wave 1, principal axis factor analysis and intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs)
was applied to the IFIRS codes, which were for each of the behavioral scales are presented
formed by averaging each individuals scores in Table 1.

Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of Marital Satisfaction and Communication Over Time

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4


(n = 431) (n = 375) (n = 359) (n = 336)

Husbands
Marital satisfaction
Mean 33.90 33.43 33.44 33.02
SD 3.0 3.7 3.5 4.1
Communication
Positivity
Mean 2.38 2.30 2.24 2.33
SD 0.8 0.6 0.6 0.6
ICC 0.83 0.81 0.83 0.77
Negativity
Mean 1.91 1.76 1.82 1.81
SD 0.6 0.6 0.7 0.6
ICC 0.72 0.85 0.89 0.74
Effectiveness
Mean 4.18 3.86 3.77 3.85
SD 0.9 1.0 0.9 0.9
ICC 0.74 0.77 0.79 0.84
Wives
Marital satisfaction
Mean 33.15 32.83 32.38 32.30
SD 3.4 3.7 4.1 4.2
Communication
Positivity
Mean 2.35 2.29 2.24 2.35
SD 0.8 0.6 0.5 0.6
ICC 0.81 0.86 0.82 0.79
Negativity
Mean 1.94 1.84 1.92 1.93
SD 0.6 0.6 0.7 0.6
ICC 0.77 0.78 0.88 0.78
Effectiveness
Mean 4.29 3.98 3.88 4.00
SD 0.9 1.0 0.9 0.9
ICC 0.80 0.75 0.77 0.85
Note. ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient: SD = standard deviation. We report results for all available data.
6 Journal of Marriage and Family

A composite positivity behavioral scale 37, with higher scores indicating higher marital
was created by averaging an individuals satisfaction. Coefficient was acceptable at
scores on the group enjoyment, positive each time point (mean = .77 for husbands and
mood, warmth/support, physical affection, .75 for wives; range: 0.700.83). The means and
humor/laugh, endearment, and listener respon- standard deviations of marital satisfaction for
siveness codes. At each time point, a positivity husbands and wives at each wave are shown in
score was calculated for each of the three dis- Table 1.
cussion tasks, and the average of these three
scores was used in the analyses. A compos- Results
ite negativity behavioral scale was created by
averaging an individuals scores on the angry Cross-Sectional Correlations
coercion, contempt, denial, disruptive process, Before examining the longitudinal associations
dominance, hostility, interrogation, and verbal between marital satisfaction and communication
attack codes. At each time point, a negativity behaviors, we examined their cross-sectional
score was calculated for each of the three discus- associations (Table 2). For husbands, marital
sion tasks, and the average of these three scores satisfaction was positively associated with
was used in the analyses. Finally, a composite positivity at each time point (all ps < .01)
effectiveness scale was created by averaging an and negatively associated with negative com-
individuals scores on the assertiveness, com- munication at each time point (all ps < .05).
munication, effective process, solution quality, Effectiveness was not associated with marital
and solution quantity codes. At each time point, satisfaction at the first two time points for
an effectiveness score was calculated for each husbands, was marginal at the third time point
of the three discussion tasks, and the average of (p < .10), and positively associated at the fourth
these three scores was used in the analyses. time point (p < .05). For wives, marital satis-
faction was positively associated with positivity
Marital Satisfaction Questionnaire and effectiveness at each time point (all ps < .05)
and negatively associated with negativity at each
Marital satisfaction was assessed by summing time point (all ps < .01). Together, these findings
responses on an eight-item questionnaire. Five are consistent with the idea that more satisfied
items asked how satisfied the respondents were couples communicate in a more positive manner
with certain areas of their relationship (e.g., (more positive, less negative, and more effec-
satisfaction with the amount of time spent tive), with robust findings for positivity and
together), and were scored on a 5-point scale negativity across husbands and wives.
(ranging from 1 = very dissatisfied to 5 = very
satisfied). Three items asked to what degree the
participants agreed with a statement about their Analytic Plan
relationship (e.g., how much do you trust your We then used cross-lagged path models to exam-
partner) and were scored on a 4-point scale ine the bidirectional associations between com-
(1 = not at all, 2 = not that much, 3 = somewhat, munication and marital satisfaction over time
4 = completely). Scores could range from 8 to (see Figure 1 for sample model). These models

Table 2. Cross-Sectional Correlations Between Marital Satisfaction and Communication for Husbands and Wives

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4

Husbands
Marital satisfactionpositivity 0.16** 0.19** 0.24** 0.24**
Marital satisfactionnegativity 0.15** 0.13* 0.16** 0.17**
Marital satisfactioneffectiveness 0.03 0.08 0.10+ 0.12*
Wives
Marital satisfactionpositivity 0.25** 0.28** 0.32** 0.23**
Marital satisfactionnegativity 0.16** 0.17** 0.22** 0.15**
Marital satisfactioneffectiveness 0.13* 0.18** 0.20** 0.16**
+ p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01.
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 7

Figure 1. Cross-Lagged Panel Model Examining Bidirectional Associations Between Newlyweds Marital
Satisfaction and Communication Over Time.

Satisfaction Satisfaction Satisfaction Satisfaction


Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4

Communication Communication Communication Communication


Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3 Wave 4

are commonly used in longitudinal research to represent rank-order stability within the sample
test the direction of influence between two vari- (e.g., whether someone high on satisfaction at
ables (e.g., Johnson & Anderson, 2015; Shaffer, one time point continues to be high on satisfac-
Lindhiem, Kolko, & Trentacosta, 2013). This tion at the subsequent time point; Shaffer et al.,
design examines both pathways of interest (e.g., 2013) rather than an estimate of within-person
early communication to later marital satisfaction change (e.g., whether someones satisfaction
and early marital satisfaction to later commu- changes over time) like in growth curve analysis.
nication) simultaneously, while controlling for All results presented here and in the tables are
all potential relationships among the variables standardized model results (STDYX standard-
(e.g., Martens & Haase, 2006). It is more con- ization). We examined the significance of the sta-
servative than a regression analysis because both bility and cross-lagged paths and compared their
dependent variables are entered into the model relative magnitude using Wald tests. In all mod-
and allowed to correlate, thereby accounting for els, stability paths for satisfaction and communi-
the multicollinearity between the two dependent cation were significant (p < .01), and Wald tests
variables and leaving less variance in the depen- indicated that the satisfaction-to-satisfaction
dent variables to be explained by the indepen- paths were stronger than the communication-to-
dent variables. communication paths (results are shown in
Analyses were conducted in MPlus (Muthn Tables 36). We focus now on the cross-lagged
& Muthn, 2002). This procedure accom- effects.
modates missing data using full information
maximum likelihood (FIML), so models were
estimated using all available observations (i.e., Cross-Lagged Models: Four-Wave Analyses
N = 431 for each of the models). Predictor We analyzed 12 four-wave models, one for
variables included communication and marital each of the communication behaviors of interest
satisfaction from the preceding time point (e.g., (positivity, negativity, and effectiveness), run
when dependent variables were negativity and separately for husbands within-sex effects (e.g.,
marital satisfaction at Wave 2, predictor vari- husbands positivity and husbands satisfaction),
ables were negativity and marital satisfaction wives within-sex effects (e.g., wives positivity
at Wave 1). Because the stability paths are and wives satisfaction), husbands cross-spouse
included in the model (e.g., negativity at Wave effects (e.g., husbands positivity and wives sat-
1 to negativity at Wave 2), each of the effects isfaction), and wives cross-spouse effects (e.g.,
should be conceptualized as examining change wives positivity and husbands satisfaction).
over time (e.g., negativity at Wave 1 predicts
marital satisfaction at Wave 2, controlling for Positivity. Results for positivity are shown in
marital satisfaction at Wave 1). Of note, in Table 3. Satisfaction was a significant predic-
cross-lagged path models, the stability paths tor of positivity at 6 of the 12 lags (median
8 Journal of Marriage and Family

Table 3. Stability and Cross-Lagged Effects for Positivity and Marital Satisfaction

Stability effects Cross-lagged effects


Satisfaction Positivity Wald Satisfaction to Positivity to Wald
() () test positivity () satisfaction () test

Husbands positivity and husbands satisfaction


Wave 12 0.60** 0.43** 42.09** 0.10* 0.09* 4.05*
Wave 23 0.68 ** 0.42** 18.14** 0.11* 0.02 0.25
Wave 34 0.69** 0.46** 15.49** 0.05 0.00 0.00
Husbands positivity and wives satisfaction
Wave 12 0.61** 0.42** 35.10** 0.15** 0.05 0.99
Wave 23 0.69** 0.40** 33.45** 0.18** 0.08* 3.97*
Wave 34 0.65** 0.44** 5.44* 0.10+ 0.02 0.08
Wives positivity and wives satisfaction
Wave 12 0.62** 0.37** 38.43** 0.14* 0.00 0.02
Wave 23 0.69 ** 0.40** 35.98** 0.14* 0.07+ 2.61
Wave 34 0.66** 0.47** 5.44* 0.09+ 0.02 0.33
Wives positivity and husbands satisfaction
Wave 12 0.60** 0.38** 43.73** 0.09+ 0.06 1.92
Wave 23 0.68** 0.42** 21.92** 0.08 0.01 0.04
Wave 34 0.69** 0.48** 15.95** 0.10+ 0.00 0.00
Note. Wald tests compare the relative strength of the paths (all df = 1). All results are standardized coefficients.
+ p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01.

Table 4. Stability and Cross-Lagged Effects for Negativity and Marital Satisfaction

Stability effects Cross-lagged effects


Satisfaction Negativity Wald Satisfaction to Negativity to Wald
() () test negativity () satisfaction () test

Husbands negativity and husbands satisfaction


Wave 12 0.60** 0.44** 22.16** 0.10* 0.09* 3.95*
Wave 23 0.68** 0.45** 2.72+ 0.10+ 0.03 0.52
Wave 34 0.70** 0.55** 24.57** 0.06 0.01 0.04
Husbands negativity and wives satisfaction
Wave 12 0.62** 0.44** 17.37** 0.07 0.03 0.55
Wave 23 0.70** 0.45** 9.96** 0.15** 0.09* 5.24*
Wave 34 0.67 ** 0.57** 7.24** 0.03 0.06 1.45
Wives negativity and wives satisfaction
Wave 12 0.61** 0.47** 7.28** 0.08 0.05 1.47
Wave 23 0.69 ** 0.45** 13.72** 0.13* 0.13** 9.57**
Wave 34 0.66** 0.47** 9.72** 0.04 0.02 0.13
Wives negativity and husbands satisfaction
Wave 12 0.60** 0.46** 10.31** 0.11* 0.10* 4.85*
Wave 23 0.67** 0.44** 5.13* 0.11* 0.06 1.60
Wave 34 0.70 ** 0.44** 30.23** 0.08 0.02 0.28
Note. Wald tests compare the relative strength of the paths (all df = 1). All results are standardized coefficients.
+ p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01.

|| across all lags = .10). Effects were found significant predictor of communication at 2 of
across all three lags and on a within- (e.g., the 12 lags (median || across all lags = .02):
husbands positivity to husbands satisfaction) Husbands positivity predicted their satisfaction
and cross-spouse (e.g., wives satisfaction to over the first lag (Wave 12) and their wives
husbands positivity) basis. Positivity was a satisfaction over the second lag (Wave 23).
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 9

Table 5. Stability and Cross-Lagged Effects for Effectiveness and Marital Satisfaction

Stability effects Cross-lagged effects


Satisfaction Effectiveness Wald Satisfaction to Effectiveness to Wald
() () test effectiveness () satisfaction () test

Husbands effectiveness and husbands satisfaction


Wave 12 0.61** 0.40** 14.61** 0.07 0.04 0.88
Wave 23 0.69 ** 0.41** 25.81** 0.04 0.01 0.11
Wave 34 0.69** 0.43** 25.46** 0.04 0.03 0.38
Husbands effectiveness and wives satisfaction
Wave 12 0.61** 0.40** 9.96** 0.07 0.10* 5.55*
Wave 23 0.70 ** 0.40** 43.25** 0.11* 0.06 1.62
Wave 34 0.66** 0.40** 12.70** 0.17** 0.00 0.04
Wives effectiveness and wives satisfaction
Wave 12 0.61** 0.40** 8.69** 0.05 0.06 2.16
Wave 23 0.70 ** 0.41** 36.62** 0.02 0.05 1.65
Wave 34 0.66** 0.44** 12.30** 0.11+ 0.01 0.16
Wives effectiveness and husbands satisfaction
Wave 12 0.61** 0.38** 14.21** 0.18** 0.05 0.82
Wave 23 0.67 ** 0.39** 21.51** 0.08 0.06 2.05
Wave 34 0.69** 0.45** 28.45** 0.10+ 0.01 0.00
Note. Wald tests compare the relative strength of the paths (all df = 1). All results are standardized coefficients.
+ p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01.

Table 6. Stability and Cross-Lagged Effects Using the First and Last Waves

Stability effects Cross-lagged effects


Satisfaction Communication Wald Satisfaction to Communication to Wald
() () test communication () satisfaction () test

Husbands positivity
Husbands satisfaction 0.57** 0.35** 42.08** 0.10+ 0.01 0.03
Wives satisfaction 0.57** 0.32** 35.92** 0.25** 0.02 0.03
Wives positivity
Husbands satisfaction 0.57** 0.36** 41.03** 0.09+ 0.01 0.00
Wives satisfaction 0.57** 0.32** 36.31** 0.24** 0.00 0.00
Husbands negativity
Husbands satisfaction 0.57** 0.48** 13.64** 0.10* 0.03 0.43
Wives satisfaction 0.57** 0.48** 10.35** 0.10+ 0.01 0.01
Wives negativity
Husbands satisfaction 0.57** 0.39** 15.71** 0.09+ 0.01 0.06
Wives satisfaction 0.57** 0.40** 11.43** 0.04 0.01 0.04
Husbands effectiveness
Husbands satisfaction 0.57** 0.36** 22.27** 0.06 0.04 0.66
Wives satisfaction 0.56** 0.34** 17.94** 0.16** 0.06 1.38
Wives effectiveness
Husbands satisfaction 0.57** 0.36** 21.55** 0.12* 0.02 0.07
Wives satisfaction 0.57** 0.36** 16.67** 0.12* 0.00 0.04
Note. Wald tests compare the relative strength of the paths (all df = 1). All results are standardized coefficients.
+ p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01.
10 Journal of Marriage and Family

We compared the relative magnitude of Wave 1 to Wave 4 Analyses


the satisfaction-to-positivity effect and the Finally, we used only the first and last waves
positivity-to-satisfaction effect using Wald tests of data to examine the associations between
(Table 3). The satisfaction-to-positivity effect satisfaction and communication over a longer
was stronger than the positivity-to-satisfaction period of time (approximately 2.5 years between
effect at two lags (husbands positivity and hus- waves). Doing so allowed us to examine whether
bands satisfaction [Wave 12] and husbands the length of the lags affected the results and is
positivity and wives satisfaction [Wave 23]). more consistent with previous studies that have
The relative magnitude of the cross-lagged examined couples initial communication as a
effects did not differ significantly at the other predictor of subsequent satisfaction.
lags (all p > .10). First, as shown in Table 6, we analyzed the
cross-lagged models described earlier using the
Negativity. Results for negativity are shown in first and last waves of data. Initial satisfaction
Table 4. Satisfaction was a significant predic- reliably predicted subsequent communication
tor of negativity at 5 of the 12 lags (median || in 6 of 12 possible effects (median || = .10),
across all lags = .09). Effects were found at the with significant results found for all three com-
first two lags and on a within- and cross-spouse munication behaviors and on a within- and
basis. Negativity was a significant predictor of cross-spouse basis. In contrast, communica-
satisfaction at 4 of the 12 lags (median || across tion did not predict subsequent satisfaction in
all lags = .06), with effects found at the first any of the 12 possible tests (median || = .01).
two lags and on a within- and cross-spouse However, the relative magnitude of these
basis. cross-lagged effects did not differ significantly
Wald tests comparing the satisfaction-to- for any of the effects (all p > .10).
negativity effect and the negativity-to-satisfaction Given that stability effects were consistently
effect indicated that the satisfaction-to-negativity stronger for satisfaction than for communication
effects were stronger than the negativity-to- (see Table 6, left side), it is possible that the non-
satisfaction effects at four of the lags: hus- significant behavior-to-satisfaction effects are a
bands negativity and husbands satisfaction statistical artifact, as controlling for baseline sat-
(Wave 12), husbands negativity and wives isfaction removes more explainable variance in
satisfaction (Wave 23), wives negativity and satisfaction than is the case when controlling for
wives satisfaction (Wave 23), and wives neg- baseline communication. To evaluate this pos-
ativity and husbands marital satisfaction (Wave sibility, we examined whether Wave 1 behavior
12). The relative magnitude of the cross-lagged scores covaried with Wave 4 satisfaction scores
effects did not differ significantly at the other before controlling for Wave 1 satisfaction
lags (all p > .10). scores. We therefore calculated the zero-order
correlations between (a) Wave 1 satisfaction
Effectiveness. Results for effectiveness are scores and Wave 4 communication behaviors
shown in Table 5. Satisfaction was a significant and (b) Wave 1 communication behaviors and
or marginal predictor of effectiveness at 3 of Wave 4 satisfaction scores. As shown in Table 7,
the 12 lags (median || across all lags = .08), 10 of the 12 satisfaction-to-behavior corre-
with effects across all three waves and on a lations were statistically significant, whereas
cross-spouse basis. Effectiveness was a sig- only 3 of the 12 behavior-to-satisfaction cor-
nificant predictor of satisfaction only once relations were significant. When we directly
(median || across all lags = .05): husbands compared the magnitude of the correlations
effectiveness was a significant predictor of using a macro developed by Lee and Preacher
wives marital satisfaction from Wave 1 to 2. (2013), there was no instance where the
Wald tests comparing the relative magnitude of communication-to-satisfaction correlation was
the lags indicated that husbands effectiveness stronger than the satisfaction-to-communication
was a stronger predictor of wives satisfaction correlation, but there were two instances where
from Wave 1 to 2 than wives satisfaction was of the satisfaction-to-communication correla-
husbands effectiveness during that period. The tion was stronger than the communication-
relative magnitude of the cross-lagged effects to-satisfaction correlation (wives satisfaction
did not differ significantly at the other lags and husbands positivity and wives satisfaction
(all p > .10). and wives positivity). These results indicate that
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 11

Table 7. Zero-Order Correlations Between Communication and Satisfaction Using the First and Last Waves

Wave 1 satisfaction to Wave 1 communication to


Wave 4 communication (r) Wave 4 satisfaction (r) z test

Husbands positivity
Husbands satisfaction 0.14* 0.07 1.00
Wives satisfaction 0.28** 0.11* 2.45*
Wives positivity
Husbands satisfaction 0.15** 0.10+ 0.71
Wives satisfaction 0.30** 0.14* 2.33*
Husbands negativity
Husbands satisfaction 0.16** 0.11+ 0.75
Wives satisfaction 0.18** 0.10+ 1.18
Wives negativity
Husbands satisfaction 0.14* 0.10+ 0.58
Wives satisfaction 0.10+ 0.08 0.28
Husbands effectiveness
Husbands satisfaction 0.08 0.07 0.14
Wives satisfaction 0.19** 0.13* 0.85
Wives effectiveness
Husbands satisfaction 0.17** 0.10+ 0.99
Wives satisfaction 0.15** 0.07 1.12
Note. Z-tests compare the relative strength of the correlations (all N = 313).
+ p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01.

behavior-to-satisfaction effects were not reli- the hypothesis that communication predicted
able or stronger than the satisfaction-to-behavior satisfaction was limited. Of the 36 cross-lagged
effects even prior to controlling for baseline sat- effects using the 9-month lags, only 7 were sig-
isfaction and indicate that the reported results nificant for communication-to-satisfaction, and
are not an artifact of differential stabilities for communication did not predict subsequent sat-
satisfaction and communication. isfaction using only the first and fourth waves of
data. More support emerged for the reverse path-
way examining satisfaction-to-communication
Discussion effects. For the 9-month lags, satisfaction was a
Communication has long been viewed as a key significant predictor of communication in twice
element in partners judgments of relationship as many cases, and there was some evidence
satisfaction, but questions remain regarding that satisfaction was a reliable predictor of sub-
cause and effect in these associations. Using sequent communication using only the first and
four waves of data from a diverse sample of last lags. However, in the majority of cases, there
low-income newlywed couples, we assessed was not significant cross-lagged prediction.
concurrent and longitudinal links between rela- Directly comparing the magnitude of the
tionship satisfaction and spouses observed pos- communication-to-satisfaction effects and the
itivity, negativity, and effectiveness. Consistent satisfaction-to-communication effects indicated
with the idea that higher levels of satisfaction that the effects did not differ significantly in
are associated with better communication, 85% of cases. Of the seven lags that did dif-
cross-sectional correlations at each of the four fer in magnitude, satisfaction was a stronger
assessments were significant, such that more predictor of communication than communi-
satisfied spouses showed more positive, less cation was of satisfaction in six cases. Taken
negative, and more effective communication. together, these results indicate that satisfaction
Cross-lagged analyses examining the recipro- is a more consistent and stronger predictor of
cal predictive relationships between satisfaction communication than the reverse, but overall
and communication shed light on the direction- both effects are fairly inconsistent and similar in
ality of these cross-sectional effects. Support for magnitude.
12 Journal of Marriage and Family

Before discussing the implications of these were particularly robust or stronger than the
results, we first outline several caveats. First, the satisfaction-to-communication effects (Table 7).
study used a sample of low-income, ethnically Thus, the differential stability effects did not dis-
diverse, first-married, newlywed couples. This proportionately drive the effects reported here.
sampling strategy was a notable strength of the Notwithstanding these limitations, the current
study, as it captured the experiences of an under- study advances understanding of the association
studied population and likely allowed for a larger between couples communication and mar-
range of communication behavior and marital ital satisfaction during the newlywed years.
satisfaction than would be seen in a sample of Although poor communication (more negative,
middle-class White couples. At the same time, less positive, less effective) was associated with
the results may not generalize to other popula- lower levels of satisfaction cross-sectionally,
tions, such as more established couples, remar- communication was an inconsistent predictor
ried couples, same-sex couples, and low-income, of spouses own satisfaction or their partners
ethnically diverse couples who choose not to satisfaction over time. Thus, although communi-
marry. Further research is needed to determine cation predicted satisfaction in some instances,
whether the predictive power of communication in general these exchanges did not have lasting
on relationship satisfaction varies across sample effects on relationship satisfaction. These results
types. We note also that these associations were indicating that the causal influence of com-
examined over the first 3 years of marriage. munication on satisfaction may be more limited
This sampling method had the advantage of than previously thoughtchallenge leading
teasing apart these associations early in couples behavioral models of relationship change that
marital trajectories before they became well argue that relationship satisfaction changes as
established, but it is possible that different asso- a function of couples communication. This
ciations could emerge later in couples marital work suggests that more specificity is needed to
trajectories. Third, our assessment of commu- clarify the circumstances under which commu-
nication behavior was limited to the positivity, nication does and does not predict satisfaction.
negativity, and effectiveness dimensions coded For example, it could be the case that only
during couples interactions. Although the use of more severe forms of negative exchanges such
observational ratings of communication behav- as aggressive behavior undermine relationship
iors in couples homes is a significant strength of quality (e.g., Lawrence & Bradbury, 2007).
the study, these structured interactions may not Alternatively, couples subjective interpre-
fully capture the ways that couples interact in tations of their behavior may prove critical,
their everyday lives. Observational ratings from even if the observable behavior itself does
the IFIRS do correlate with couples own reports not, consistent with attributional models (e.g.,
of their behavior (Lorenz, Melby, Conger, & Bradbury & Fincham, 1990). More attention is
Surjadi, 2012), but it is nonetheless possible also needed to clarify whether factors other than
that couples subjective ratings of their com- communication serve as the drivers of change
munication quality or other behavioral patterns in satisfaction. For example, external stressors
(e.g., demandwithdraw behavior; Christensen and the broader environmental context have
& Heavey, 1990) may show different patterns of been shown to undermine couples relationship
association with satisfaction over time. Finally, satisfaction, particularly among low-income
the stability paths for satisfaction were sig- populations such as we have examined here
nificantly stronger than the stability paths for (e.g., Conger et al., 1990; Cutrona et al., 2003).
communication. This pattern of results indicates Future empirical work examining factors that
that the between-person, rank-order stability for do consistently predict satisfaction over time
satisfaction was greater than that for communi- during the early years of marriage will do much
cation, resulting in less variability in satisfaction to enhance our theoretical understandings of
to be explained relative to communication why relationships change.
scores. Nonetheless, after we removed this Satisfaction was a more consistent predictor
constraint by computing zero-order correlations of husbands and wives communication behav-
between Wave 1 communication and Wave 4 iors, and when the cross-lagged effects differed
satisfaction (and Wave 1 satisfaction and Wave 4 in magnitude they favored the satisfaction-to-
communication), there was no evidence communication paths in all but one instance.
that communication-to-satisfaction effects At the same time, the results for satisfaction-to-
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 13

communication were not altogether consistent The present findings also point to the importance
across time or across all domains of functioning. of not assuming that prediction of shorter term
Across all behaviors, for example, we identified follow-up will generalize to prediction at longer
no instances in which satisfaction predicted term follow-up, given that the few significant
that behavior across all three of the lags that results for communication did not replicate
we studied (see Tables 3, 4, and 5). Moreover, across the short and longer lags. Greater clarity
although it was true that the magnitude of the in the marital literature about the definition of
satisfaction-to-satisfaction lags were in some and meaning that can be inferred from different
cases stronger than the communication-to- follow-up periods would be valuable.
satisfaction lags, in the vast majority of cases Several applied implications also follow from
the relative magnitude of the lags did not differ. these results. Poor communication is the most
Thus, while satisfaction was a more consistent commonly cited reason why couples seek ther-
predictor of communication than communica- apy (Doss, Simpson, & Christensen, 2004) and
tion was of satisfaction, the effect of satisfaction is estimated by therapists to have the most dam-
on communication was not particularly robust aging impact on relationships (Geiss & OLeary,
either, suggesting that other potent forces are 1981). Improving communication has thus been
at work in affecting change in marriage. As the the primary goal in leading models of prevention
vulnerabilitystressadaptation model asserts (e.g., Wood et al., 2014) and intervention (e.g.,
(Karney & Bradbury, 1995), core functions in Benson et al., 2012), driven by this perceived
relationships are likely governed by personality need and by the assumption that communication
characteristics (e.g., neuroticism, self-esteem), is a key predictor of relationship satisfac-
dyadic processes (e.g., time spent together, sex- tion. Our results indicate that a more nuanced
ual intimacy), and external factors (e.g., chronic assumption is needed: Poor communication does
and acute stress), any of which may explain the in some cases lead to changes in satisfaction, but
cross-sectional associations between satisfac- assuming that poor communication consistently
tion and communication and perhaps even serve leads couples to be less satisfied is not supported
as more robust predictors of these constructs. by these data. One consequence of this insight
As spouses communication is unlikely to be is that improving communication may be a
simply a downstream manifestation of earlier valuable first step so that couples can engage
satisfaction, exploring other potential explana- more readily in treatment, but it is unlikely to
tions for how dyadic processes change over time be a sufficient ingredient for lasting change
would be particularly illuminating. in relationship satisfaction. Interventions that
More broadly, this study highlights the ben- also help couples understand and process their
efits of repeated assessments of independent other difficulties and that teach them to navigate
variables like communication for understanding these problems more effectively are likely to be
relationship development. Prior studies have beneficial (e.g., Jacobson & Christensen, 1996).
typically relied on data from a single initial These interventions may foster the development
assessment to predict longitudinal change in of higher-order dyadic capacities such as help-
satisfaction (e.g., Johnson et al., 2005), under ing couples learn when to raise concerns or why
the assumption that this information captures an certain problems are particularly difficult. Such
unfolding process (e.g., increasingly negative skills are distinct from helping them learn how
interactions) that remains robust over time. In to discuss their difficulties and could have more
contrast, this study indicates these processes robust and long-lasting effects on satisfaction.
may not remain consistent over time; longitudi- In sum, these results indicate that commu-
nal linkages between communication and satis- nication does in some cases foreshadow later
faction were generally less robust as time passed, judgments of relationship satisfaction and that
despite consistent cross-sectional associations at higher levels of initial satisfaction can eventu-
each assessment and significant prediction early ate into unions that are more interpersonally har-
in marriage. Fully understanding the nature of monious. On the whole, however, these effects
the linkages between satisfaction and indepen- are not particularly strong or consistent over
dent variables like communication thus requires time, leaving open important questions about the
assessing these variables repeatedly over time in interpersonal processes that enable couples to
tandem with satisfaction in order to adequately sustain high levels of satisfaction and adaptive
test theoretical models of relationship change. communication during the newlywed years.
14 Journal of Marriage and Family

Note of Marital and Family Therapy, 7, 515520.


doi:10.1111/j.1752-0606.1981.tb01407.x
Preparation of this report was supported by Research Grants
HD053825 and HD061366 from the National Institute of Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Hills-
Child Health and Human Development awarded to Benjamin dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
R. Karney. We thank Andrew Christensen, Rashmita Mistry, Heavey, C. L., Layne, C., & Christensen, A. (1993).
and Letitia Anne Peplau for helpful feedback on earlier Gender and conflict structure in marital inter-
versions of this manuscript and Cameron Neece and Hannah action: A replication and extension. Journal of
Williamson for statistical consultation. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 1627.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.61.1.16
Hsueh, J., Alderson, D. P., Lundquist, E., Michalopou-
References los, C., Gubits, D., Fein, D., & Knox, V. (2012).
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior The supporting healthy marriage evaluation:
relations: A theoretical analysis and review of Early impacts on low-income families. OPRE
empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84, Report 201211. Washington, DC: Administration
888918. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888 for Children and Families, U.S. Department of
Benson, L. A., McGinn, M. M., & Christensen, A. Health and Human Services.
(2012). Common principles of couple therapy. Huston, T. L., Caughlin, J. P., Houts, R. M., Smith,
Behavior Therapy, 43, 2535. doi:10.1016/j.beth. S. E., & George, L. J. (2001). The connubial
2010.12.009 crucible: Newlywed years as predictors of marital
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attri- delight, distress, and divorce. Journal of Per-
butions in marriage: Review and critique. Psy- sonality and Social Psychology, 80, 237252.
chology Bulletin, 107, 333. doi:10.1037/0033- doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.237
2909.107.1.3 Jacobson, N. S., & Christensen, A. (1996). Accep-
Bradbury, T. N. & Karney, B. R. (2013). Intimate tance and change in couple therapy: A therapists
relationships (2nd ed.). New York: Norton. guide to transforming relationships. New York:
Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number Norton.
of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1, Jacobson, N. S., & Margolin, G. (1979). Marital
245276. doi:10.1207/ s15327906mbr0102_10 therapy: Strategies based on social learning and
Christensen, A., & Heavey, C. L. (1990). Gender behavior exchange principles. New York: Brunner/
and social structure in the demand/withdraw pat- Mazel.
tern of marital conflict. Journal of Personality Johnson, M. D., & Anderson, J. R. (2015). Tem-
and Social Psychology, 59, 7381. doi:10.1037/ poral ordering of intimate relationship efficacy
0022-3514.59.1.73 and conflict. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77,
Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, 968981. doi:10.1111/jomf.12198
K. J., Simons, R. L., Whitbeck, L. B., Melby, Johnson, M. D., Cohan, C. L., Davila, J., Lawrence,
J. N. (1990). Linking economic hardship to marital E., Rogge, R. D., Karney, B. R., Bradbury,
quality and instability. Journal of Marriage and the T. N. (2005). Problem-solving skills and affec-
Family, 52, 643656. doi:10.2307/352931 tive expressions as predictors of change in mari-
Cutrona, C. E., Russell, D. W., Abraham, W. T., tal satisfaction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Gardner, K. A., Melby, J. M., Bryant, C., & Psychology, 73, 1527. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.
Conger, R. D. (2003). Neighborhood context and 73.1.15
financial strain as predictors of marital interac- Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longi-
tion and marital quality in African American tudinal course of marital quality and stability: A
couples. Personal Relationships, 10, 389409. review of theory, method, and research. Psycholog-
doi:10.1111/1475-6811.00056 ical Bulletin, 118, 334. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.
Doss, B. D., Simpson, L. E., & Christensen, A. (2004). 118.1.3
Why do couples seek marital therapy? Professional Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroti-
Psychology: Research & Practice, 35, 608614. cism, marital interaction, and the trajectory of
doi:10.1037/0735-7028.35.6.608 marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and
Elliott, M. N., Becker, K., Beckett, M. K., Hambar- Social Psychology, 72, 10751092. doi:10.1037/
soomian, K., Pantoja, P., & Karney, B. R. (2013). 0022-3514.72.5.1075
Using indirect estimates based on name and cen- Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Bane, C., Glaser, R., &
sus tract to improve the efficiency of sampling Malarkey, W. B. (2003). Love, marriage, and
matched ethnic couples from marriage license divorce: Newlyweds stress hormones foreshadow
data. Public Opinion Quarterly, 77, 375384. doi: relationship changes. Journal of Consulting and
10.1093/poq/nft007 Clinical Psychology, 71, 176188. doi:10.1037/
Geiss, S. K., & OLeary, K. D. (1981). Therapist 0022-006X.71.1.176
ratings of frequency and severity of marital Kim, H. K., Capaldi, D. M., & Crosby, L. (2007).
problems: Implications for research. Journal Generalizability of Gottman and colleagues
Couple Communication and Marital Satisfaction 15

affective process models of couples relationship Melby, J., Conger, R., Book, R., Rueter, M., Lucy,
outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, L., Repinski, D., Scaramella, L. (1998). The
5572. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00343.x Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales (5th ed).
Koerner, K., & Jacobson, N. J. (1994). Emotion and Unpublished manuscript, Iowa State University,
behavior in couple therapy. In S. M. Johnson & Institute for Social and Behavioral Research.
L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), The heart of the matter: Muthn, B., & Muthn, L. (2002). MPlus: The
Perspectives on emotion in marital therapy (pp. comprehensive modeling program for applied
207226). New York: Brunner/Mazel. researchers. Los Angeles: Muthn & Muthn.
Kreider, R. M., & Ellis, R. (2011). Number, timing, Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2007). Stress crossover
and duration of marriages and divorce: 2009. Cur- in newlywed marriage: A longitudinal and dyadic
rent Populations Reports. Washington, DC: U.S. perspective. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69,
Census Bureau. 594607. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00394.x
Kurdek, L. A. (1991). Predictors of increases in Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and
marital distress in newlywed couples: A 3-year intimacy: Component processes. In E. T. Higgins
prospective longitudinal study. Developmental & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology:
Psychology, 27, 627636. doi:10.1037/0012-1649. Handbook of basic principles (pp. 523563). New
27.4.627 York: Guilford.
Lawrence, E., & Bradbury, T. N. (2007). Trajecto- Rogge, R. D., Cobb, R. J., Lawrence, E., John-
ries of change in physical aggression and mari- son, M. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (2013). Is skills
tal satisfaction. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, training necessary for the primary prevention of
236247. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.236 marital distress and dissolution? A 3-year exper-
Lee, I. A., & Preacher, K. J. (2013). Calcula- imental study of three interventions. Journal of
tion for the test of the difference between two Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 949961.
dependent correlations with no variable in doi:10.1037/a0034209
common [Computer software]. Available from Shaffer, A., Lindhiem, O., Kolko, D. J., & Trenta-
http://quantpsy.org/corrtest/corrtest3.htm costa, C. J. (2013). Bidirectional effects between
Lorenz, F. O., Melby, J. N., Conger, R. D., & Sur- parenting practices and child externalizing behav-
jadi, F. F. (2012). Linking questionnaire reports ior: A cross-lagged panel analysis in the context
and observer ratings of young couples hostility of a psychosocial treatment and 3-year follow-up.
and support. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41,
316327. doi:10.1037/a0028319 199210. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9670-3
Luo, S., Chen, H., Yue, G., Zhang, G., Zhaoyang, R., U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). Summary population
& Xu, D. (2008). Predicting marital satisfaction and housing characteristics: California. Washing-
from self, partner, and couple characteristics: ton, DC: Government Printing Office.
Is it me, you, or us? Journal of Personality, Williamson, H. C., Bradbury, T. N., Trail, T. E., & Kar-
76, 12311266. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008. ney, B. R. (2011). Factor analysis of the Iowa Fam-
00520.x ily Interaction Rating Scales. Journal of Family
Markman, H. J., Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., Psychology, 25, 993999. doi:10.1037/a0025903
Ragan, E. P., & Whitton, S. W. (2010). The premar- Wood, R. G., Moore, Q., Clarkwest, A., & Kille-
ital communication roots of marital distress and wald, A. (2014). The long-term effects of Building
divorce: The first five years of marriage. Journal Strong Families: A program for unmarried parents.
of Family Psychology, 24, 289298. doi:10.1037/ Journal of Marriage and Family, 76, 446463.
a0019481 doi:10.1111/jomf.12094
Martens, M. P., & Haase, R. F. (2006). Advanced Woodin, E. M. (2011). A two-dimensional approach
applications of structural equation modeling in to relationship conflict: Meta-analytic findings.
counseling psychology research. The Counseling Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 325335.
Psychologist, 34, 878911. doi:10.1177/001100 doi:10.1037/a0023791
0005283395

S-ar putea să vă placă și