Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

Creativity Research Journal Copyright 2006 by

2006, Vol. 18, No. 1, 314 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Can We Trust Creativity Tests?


A Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT)
Kyung Hee Kim
Eastern Michigan University

ABSTRACT: Dr. E. Paul Torrance, Father of Creativ- The TTCT does not entirely operationalize
ity, is best known for developing the Torrance Tests of Torrances definition of creativity (Chase, 1985);
Creative Thinking (TTCT). The TTCT was developed however, Torrance neither concluded that his tests as-
by Torrance in 1966. It has been renormed 4 times: in sess all dimensions of creativity, nor did he suggest
1974, 1984, 1990, and 1998. There are 2 forms (A and that they should be used alone as a basis for decisions
B) of the TTCT-Verbal and 2 forms (A and B) of the (Treffinger, 1985). Torrance (1974) stated that show-
TTCT-Figural. However, in the scope of this review, ing a high degree of these abilities on the TTCT does
only the TTCT-Figural was examined. The TTCT has not guarantee a persons chances of behaving cre-
been translated into more than 35 languages (Millar, atively. According to Torrance (Torrance, 1990, 1998;
2002). It has become highly recommended in the edu- Torrance & Ball, 1984), creative motivation and skills
cational field and is even used in the corporate world. as well as creative abilities are necessary for adult
It is the most widely used test of creativity (Davis, creative achievement to occur.
1997) and is the most referenced of all creativity tests The TTCT-Verbal and the TTCT-Figural are two
(Lissitz & Willhoft, 1985). Basic information is pre- versions of the TTCT. The TTCT-Verbal has two paral-
sented, including purposes, content area, norms, reli- lel forms, A and B, and consists of five activities:
ability, and validity. Strengths and weaknesses of the ask-and-guess, product improvement, unusual uses,
TTCT, including use of the TTCT in identifying gifted unusual questions, and just suppose. The stimulus for
learners and suggestions for further development and each task includes a picture to which people respond in
improvement, are provided and discussed. writing (Torrance, 1966, 1974). The TTCT-Figural has
two parallel forms, A and B, and consists of three activ-
ities: picture construction, picture completion, and re-
peated figures of lines or circles. For the purposes of
E. Paul Torrance was an international leader in cre- this article, only the TTCT-Figural will be discussed.
ativity research and was best known for developing the Ten minutes are required to complete each activity. In
Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT), which Activity I, the subject constructs a picture using a pear
are used in the business world and in education to as- or jellybean shape provided on the page as a stimulus.
sess individuals capacity for creativity (E. Paul The stimulus must be an integral part of the picture
Torrance, 87, 2003, p. B13). construction. Activity II requires the subject to use 10
Torrance (1966, p. 6) defined creativity as incomplete figures to make an object or picture. The

a process of becoming sensitive to problems, deficiencies,


gaps in knowledge, missing elements, disharmonies, and so This is a version of a paper presented at the 8th Asian-Pacific Confer-
on; identifying the difficulty; searching for solutions, making ence on Giftedness held in Daejeon, Korea, July 2630, 2004.
guesses, or formulating hypotheses about the deficiencies: Correspondence and requests for reprints should be sent to
testing and retesting these hypotheses and possibly modify- Kyung Hee Kim, 313 Porter College of Education Building, Depart-
ing and retesting them; and finally communicating the ment of Teacher Education, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti,
results. MI 48197. E-mail: kkim7@emich.edu

Creativity Research Journal 3


K. H. Kim

last activity, Activity III, is composed of three pages of 5. To be aware of latent potentialities.
lines or circles that the subject is to use as a part of his
or her picture (Torrance, 1966, 1974, 1990, 1998; In other words, although the tests have been used
Torrance & Ball, 1984). mostly for assessment in the identification of gifted
Torrance (1966) recommended the creation of a children, Torrance originally planned to use them as a
game-like, thinking, or problem-solving atmosphere, basis for individualizing instruction for different stu-
avoiding the threatening situation associated with test- dents based on the test scores (Torrance, 1966, 1974).
ing. His intent was to set the tone so that the expecta- The test may yield a composite score (the Creativity
tion that examinees would enjoy the activities was cre- Index[CI]), but Torrance discouraged interpretation of
ated. Examinees should be encouraged to have fun scores as a static measure of a persons ability and, in-
and should experience a psychological climate that is stead, argued for using the profile of strengths as a
as comfortable and stimulating as possible. Thus, ac- means to understand and nurture a persons creativity
cording to the administration of the TTCT in the man- (Hbert et al., 2002; Torrance, 1966, 1974, 1979).
ual (Ball & Torrance, 1984), administrators of the tests Thus, the purposes of the TTCT are for research and
should invite the examinees to enjoy the activities and experimentation, for general use, for instructional
view the tests as a series of fun activities, thereby re- planning, and for determining possible strengths of
ducing test anxiety. students.
The TTCT can be administered as an individual or
group test from the kindergarten level through the
graduate level and beyond. It requires 30 min of work- Content Areas
ing time, so speed is important, and artistic quality is
not required to receive credit (Chase, 1985). Scholastic Guilford (1956, 1959, 1960, 1986) considered cre-
Testing Service, Inc., holds the copyright for the TTCT ative thinking as involving divergent thinking, which
and has provided a 1998 norms manual for the test. emphasizes fluency, flexibility, originality, and elabo-
ration. Guilford, however, noted that creative thinking
is not the same as divergent thinking, because creativ-
Purpose
ity requires sensitivity to problems as well as redefini-
The TTCT was part of a long-term research pro- tion abilities, which include transformations of
gram emphasizing classroom experiences that stimu- thought, reinterpretations, and freedom from func-
late creativity (Swartz, 1988). Torrance is readily tional fixedness in driving unique solutions. Although
identified with his eponymous tests of creativity, but there have been several revisions of the TTCT-Figural
assessment of creativity was not one of Torrances manual, the test itself has remained unchanged. The
goals. Torrances main focus was in understanding first edition in 1966 measured fluency, flexibility, orig-
and nurturing qualities that help people express their inality, and elaboration, which were taken from the di-
creativity. The tests were not designed to simply mea- vergent-thinking factors found in Guilfords (1959)
sure creativity, but instead to serve as tools for its en- Structure of the Intellect Model (Baer, 1997; Torrance,
hancement (Hbert, Cramond, Neumeister, Millar, & 1966). The second edition measured the same four
Silvian, 2002). Torrance (1966, 1974) suggested the scoring variables as that of 1966 (Torrance, 1974).
following uses for the tests: The stimuli of the TTCT of 1984 are identical to
those of 1966 and 1974, but the scoring procedures
1. To understand the human mind and its func- were changed in the third edition, the TTCT of 1984.
tioning and development. Chase (1985) criticized earlier editions of the TTCT
2. To discover effective bases for individualizing because of a lack of empirical basis for the scoring de-
instruction. cisions caused by the subjectivity of scoring. However,
3. To provide clues for remedial and the scoring system has been improved since the 1984
psychotherapeutic programs. edition, because Torrance (Ball & Torrance, 1984) en-
4. To evaluate the effects of educational pro- hanced the scoring of the TTCT by designing a stream-
grams, materials, curricula, and teaching pro- lined scoring system. The TTCT-Figural manual has
cedures. presented a simplification of the scoring procedures

4 Creativity Research Journal


Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking

and has also provided a detailed scoring workbook To get a CI, the standard scores of each of five vari-
(Ball & Torrance, 1984) in addition to the ables are used according to the TTCT Norms-Techni-
Norms-Technical Manual. cal Manual (Torrance, 1998). Raw scores are converted
Two norm-referenced measures of creative poten- into standard scores with means of 100 and standard
tial, abstractness of titles and resistance to premature deviations of 20. The standard scores of each subscale
closure, were added to fluency, originality, and elabo- can be ranged as follows: fluency, 40154; originality,
ration; the measure of flexibility (scored by the variety 40160; elaboration, 40160; Abstractness of titles,
of categories of relevant responses) was eliminated be- 40160; resistance to premature closure, 40160. The
cause it correlated very highly with fluency (Hbert et standard scores for each of the five norm-referenced
al., 2002). The five subscales are listed as follows, with measures are averaged to produce an overall indicator
descriptions of each subscale and information about of creative potential. For the frequency of creative
scoring and the content measured (Torrance & Ball, strength, a + or ++ is awarded on the basis of the scor-
1984; Torrance, 1990): ing guide. The number of +s is added (range for Cre-
ative Strengths: 026) to the averaged standard scores
to yield a Creative Index (Torrance, 1998).
Fluency: The number of relevant ideas; shows an Torrance added these subscales based on informa-
ability to produce a number of figural images.
tion that was unavailable when he originally developed
Originality: The number of statistically infre- the TTCT in 1966, because of his concern that the
quent ideas; shows an ability to produce uncom-
norm-referenced score was not measuring the breadth
mon or unique responses. The scoring procedure
of creativity manifestations he had observed (Hbert et
counts the most common responses as 0 and all
al., 2002; Torrance, 1979). He used continued research
other legitimate responses as 1. The originality
to expand his test, including studies of the creative gi-
lists have been prepared for each item on the ba-
ants, personality studies of creative people, creativity
sis of normative data, which are readily memo-
training guides, his own studies, and other literature in
rized by scorers.
the field. Torrance (1979, 1988, 1994; Torrance & Ball,
Elaboration: The number of added ideas; demon- 1984) also provided evidence to show that the new
strates the subjects ability to develop and elabo-
subscales were valid predictors of creative achieve-
rate on ideas.
ment and that they improved the tests validity. Content
Abstractness of Titles: The degree beyond label- and construct validity of the scoring variables have
ing; based on the idea that creativity requires an
been studied in a factor-analytic study (Mourad, 1976),
abstraction of thought. It measures the degree a
a comparative study (Rungsinan, 1977), a develop-
title moves beyond concrete labeling of the pic-
mental study (Ali-el-din, 1978; 1982), and Torrances
tures drawn.
The Search for Satori and Creativity (1979). Accord-
Resistance to Premature Closure: The degree of ing to Johnson and Fishkin (1999), the TTCTs revised
psychological openness; based on the belief that
scoring system addresses essential constructs of cre-
creative behavior requires a person to consider a
ative behaviors reflective of Torrances (1988) defini-
variety of information when processing informa-
tion of creativity. Therefore, Torrance showed that the
tion and to keep an open mind.
TTCT is not only a divergent thinking test but also a
creativity test as of the 1984 revisions.
Thirteen criterion-referenced measures, which
Torrance called creative strengths, also were added to
the scoring (Torrance, 1990; Torrance & Ball, 1984). Norms
The creative strengths are emotional expressiveness,
storytelling articulateness, movement or action, ex- The 1998 TTCT manual provides norms generated
pressiveness of titles, synthesis of incomplete figures, in the summer of 1997 and includes both grade-related
synthesis of lines or circles, unusual visualization, in- (kindergarten through Grade 12) and age-related
ternal visualization, extending or breaking boundaries, norms (ages 6 through 19). Samples included 55,600
humor, richness of imagery, colorfulness of imagery, kindergarten through 12th-grade students from the
and fantasy. central (3.6%), northeast (11.4%), southeast (15.2%),

Creativity Research Journal 5


K. H. Kim

and western (57.6%) regions in the United States and mentary education majors (Torrance, Tan, & Allman,
other areas, including Canada (2.2%; Torrance, 1998). 1970), seventh-grade students (Cropley, 1971), and
These groupings are used by the National Assessment economically disadvantaged elementary school Black
of Educational Progress, the U.S. Department of Com- children (Witt, 1971), which increased the TTCTs
merce, and the National Education Association. The credibility as a predictor of creative productivity
central region was somewhat undersampled, and the (Hbert et al., 2002). However, four points of data
western region was oversampled. Although the TTCT that were collected from two elementary schools and
has been used in more than 35 countries for research a high school provide the major body of longitudinal
purposes, there have been fewer authorized versions research on the TTCT. Beginning in 1958 and contin-
for which the developers have developed (or been de- uing through 1964, all students enrolled in grades 16
veloping) country norms. Among those are Brazil, in two elementary schools took the TTCT each year.
France, Italy, Portugal, Saudi Arabia (and the Arabic Beginning in 1959, all students enrolled in grades
countries), Slovenia, Taiwan, Turkey, and Korea (un- 712 of the University of Minnesota High School
der development). In each of these countries, the norms also took the TTCT.
were developed by the local author (J. Kauffman, vice The results of the follow-up of 46 high school stu-
president of Scholastic Testing Service, Inc., personal dents at a 7-year interval are as follows (Torrance,
communication, January 25, 2005). In addition, norms 1969, 1972). Three of the TTCT subscales (fluency,
for many other countries either have been either or are flexibility, and originality) correlated significantly (at
being developed for research purposes. the .01 level) with both quantity and quality of creative
achievements, and the significant correlations ranged
from r = .39 to .48. IQ (Iowa Test of Basic Skills,
Reliability Lorge-Thorndike, or the Stanford-Binet Intelligence
Scale) correlated (r = .37) with quality of creative
According to the TTCT-Figural Manual of 1998, the achievements. The three TTCT subscale scores were
reliability estimates of the creative index from better predictors of creative achievement than IQ, high
KuderRichardson 21 using 99th percentile scores as school achievement, or peer nominations.
the estimates of the number of items ranged between .89 The results of the follow-up of 236 high school stu-
and .94. The TTCT-Figural Manual of 1990 states that dents at the 12-year interval indicated that all of the
the interrater reliability among the scorers for Scholas- creativity predictors (fluency, flexibility, inventive
tic Testing Service, Inc., was greater than .90. Samples level, elaboration, originality, and IQ [only for qual-
included 88,355 kindergarten through 12th-grade stu- ity]) were found to be significant (at the .01 level), ex-
dents in the United States from the south (41.4%), north- cept IQ for quantity of creative achievements (r = .06, p
east (28.5%), north central (5.1%), and west (5.1%), as > .05) and creativeness of aspiration (r = .18, p .05)
well as some students from Canada (Torrance, 1990). for girls (Torrance, 1972).
According to the TTCT manuals of 1966 and 1974, The results of the follow-up of 211 elementary
the testretest reliability coefficients have ranged from school students at the 22-year interval are as follows
.50 to .93, which is not so high. Torrance (1974) indi- (Torrance, 1980, 1981a, 1981b). The criteria used were
cated that motivational conditions affect the measure- number of high school creative achievements, number
ment of creative functioning, which could explain the of posthigh school creative achievements, number of
low testretest reliability. Treffinger (1985) concluded creative style of life achievements, quality of highest
that, given the complexity of creative thinking, the creative achievements, and creativeness of future ca-
TTCT can be seen as having reasonable reliability for reer image. The judges interrater reliabilities obtained
group and research applications. using Cronbachs alpha were .81 (Torrance, 1981b).
Pearson productmoment correlation coefficients cal-
culated between the CI from elementary school TTCT
Validity scores and each of the five criteria was significant (at
the .001 level). A multiple correlation coefficient of .63
The preliminary studies were conducted examin- was obtained for the five criteria entered into a step-
ing the predictive validity of the TTCT including ele- wise multiple regression equation (Torrance, 1981b).

6 Creativity Research Journal


Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking

The results of the follow-up of 99 elementary tween originality and scores on the Gordon test was .30
school students at the 40-year interval are as follows (p < .01). The correlation between resistance to prema-
(Torrance, 2002). The predictors included IQ, flu- ture closure and PMA was .33 (p < .001), and resis-
ency, flexibility, originality, elaboration, creative tance to premature closure and the Gordon test was .26
strengths, whether respondents had a mentor in 1980, (p < .01).
and whether respondents had a mentor in 1998. The In terms of construct validity, studies on the TTCT
criteria used were number of publicly recognized cre- have shown conflicting results regarding its
ative achievements and quality of public achieve- dimensionality (Chase, 1985; Clapham, 1998; Dixon,
ments. The judges interrater reliabilities obtained us- 1979; Heausler & Thompson, 1988; Hocevar, 1979a,
ing Cronbachs alpha ranged from .78 to .88. IQ was 1979b; Hocevar & Michael, 1979; Runco & Mraz,
a significant predictor of quantity (r = .44, p .01) 1992; Treffinger, 1985). Guilford (1959, 1962) con-
and quality (r = .46, p .01) of creative achievements ceptualized divergent thinking as multidimensional,
for females but not for males. Originality was a sig- and many researchers have hypothesized that creativity
nificant predictor of quality of creative achievements consists of several independent psychological factors.
for both males (r = .32, p .05) and females (r = .40, Torrance (1966, 1974) also encouraged the use of indi-
p .01). Creative strengths was a significant predic- vidual scale scores and warned that using a single score
tor of quality for males (r = .45, p .01), and both may be misleading.
quality (r = .41, p .01) and quantity (r = .29, p However, Hocevar (1979a, 1979b) argued that the
.05) were significant predictors of quality for fe- TTCT and Guilfords divergent thinking tests measure
males. Having had a mentor in 1980 was a significant only fluency. Dixon (1979) was concerned that origi-
predictor of quantity (r = .41, p .01) and quality (r nality scores of the TTCT depended heavily on fluency
= .50, p .01) of creative achievements for females scores. Abernathy Tannehill (1998) also considered
but not for males. Having had a mentor in 1998 was a the significant correlation between fluency and origi-
significant predictor for both males (r = .36, p .05) nality as the sign that the subscores of the TTCT may
and females (r = .40, p .01) for quality of creative not measure independent constructs.
achievements but not for quantity. Quantity and qual- Hocevar and Michael (1979) found that the
ity of creative achievements were highly correlated heterotraitmonomethod coefficients were too high
for both males (r = .90, p .01) and for females (r = compared with monotraitheteromethod coefficients
.81, p .01). This showed the link between quantity based on multitraitmultimethod analyses using the
of ideas and the production of quality of ideas TTCT and Guilford tests, which suggests that these
(Hbert et al., 2002). two tests lack discriminant validity. Runco and Mraz
Torrance and Wus (1981) study and Yamada and (1992) also criticized the lack of discriminant validity
Tams (1996) reanalysis and Pluckers (1999) of the TTCT in a study including several other diver-
reanalysis of Torrances data concluded that the Cre- gent thinking tests.
ative Index was the best predictor for adult creative Heausler and Thompson (1988) concluded that the
achievement. Plucker (1999) found that the standard- correlations among the subscales were so high that
ized path coefficient from the TTCT to adult creative each subscale could not provide meaningfully different
achievement was .60, whereas the standardized path information. Chase (1985) suggested that the correla-
coefficient from IQ scores (the Stanford-Binet Intelli- tion coefficients among fluency, flexibility, and origi-
gence Scale, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, nality were so high (.74 to .80) that one single score
or California Test of Mental Maturity) was .19. could be appropriate for the three subscores. Thus,
In terms of concurrent validity, Gonzales and Cam- Treffinger (1985) warned that interpretations of TTCT
pos (1997) studied the correlations between TTCT and subscores as if they were independent should be
the Spatial Test of Primary Mental Abilities (PMA) avoided. Hassan (1986) also concluded that there was
and the Gordon Test of Visual Imagery Control. The no justification for considering creativity as composed
results indicated that imagery was significantly corre- of the distinct traits recommended by Torrance.
lated with various aspects of creative thinking. Among There are not many studies that have analyzed fac-
those with IQ > 120, the correlation between original- tor structures of the TTCT. One study modeled two
ity and PMA was .36 (p < .001) and the correlation be- components through a principal component analysis

Creativity Research Journal 7


K. H. Kim

and concluded that the scores of the TTCT primarily TTCT showed more differences across grade-level
reflect one general factor (Heausler & Thompson, groups than across gender groups. These findings are
1988). Clapham (1998) also concluded that there is consistent with Torrances conclusion (1977) that the
only one general factor for the TTCT. Further, the re- TTCT-Figural was fair in terms of gender. These re-
sult of a principal component analysis indicated that sults also indicate that when TTCT scores are com-
resistance to premature closure explained the highest pared among different grade levels, more caution may
amount of the variance in the CI. be needed for interpretation.
Contrary to the research cited here, the results of
confirmatory analyses (Kim, in press; Kim &
Cramond, 2004) based on 500 sixth graders indicated Merits
that a two-factor model fits significantly better than a
single-factor model. This study examined the possi- Positive features of the TTCT include the wealth of
bility of a two-factor model based on Kirtons (1976, information available on it, the short time needed for
1978, 1989) AdaptorInnovator (AI) Theory. Ac- administration, and the ease of its administration
cording to Kirton (1978), innovators prefer to create (Swartz, 1988). It has fewer limitations and cautions to
change by threatening the paradigm, whereas adap- apply, and it is more researched and analyzed than any
tors prefer to create change by working within the ex- other creativity instrument (Johnson & Fishkin, 1999;
isting paradigm. Factor innovative is loaded by flu- Swartz, 1988; Treffinger, 1985). The TTCT-Figural
ency, originality, and resistance to premature closure, has had 25 years of extensive development and evalua-
whereas factor adaptive is loaded by elaboration, ab- tion (Millar, 2002). It has one of the largest norming
stractness of titles, and creative strengths. The factor samples, with valuable longitudinal validations (Davis,
models with and without creative strengths also were 1997) and high predictive validity over a very wide age
analyzed, because creative strengths had different range (Cropley, 2000). The standardized administra-
procedures from the other subscales in scoring. How- tion, scoring procedures and norms, and the develop-
ever, this subscale is too important to be excluded ment and evaluation (Davis & Rimm, 1994) have made
from full explanations of the scores of the TTCT (E. the TTCT especially useful for identifying gifted and
P. Torrance, personal communication, October 30, talented students. The TTCT-Figural can be fair in
2002). The factor models without creative strengths terms of gender, race, and community status, as well as
fit better than those with creative strengths. This indi- for persons with a different language background, so-
cates that creative strengths might represent a sepa- cioeconomic status, and culture (Cramond, 1993;
rate factor. However, more indicators of the TTCT Torrance, 1977). Torrance (1971; Torrance &
would be needed to test this model. Torrance, 1972) found that in most situations there are
The high (.844) correlation between fluency and no statistically significant differences in performance
originality also was noted in Kim and Cramonds study on the TTCT because of race or socioeconomic status;
(2004), as criticized by several researchers before. in some cases, the TTCT favors Black children and
However, Torrance and Safter (1999) argued that the children of low socioeconomic backgrounds.
person who produces a large number of alternatives is
more likely to produce original ones. Simonton (1990)
also concluded that a persons originality is a function Use in Identifying Gifted Learners
of the number of ideas formulated. Several longitudi-
nal studies of the TTCT also have shown a link be- The most extensive use of the TTCT-Figural is for
tween the quantity of ideas and the quality of ideas that identification of children for gifted programs. The
are produced (Hbert et al., 2002), as mentioned in the TTCT is a helpful addition to the assessment reper-
validity section. toire, because most measures for gifted identification
Other studies (Kim, 2004a; Kim, Cramond, & are heavily driven by verbal and quantitative content
Bandalos, 2004, in press) based on 3,000 kindergarten- (Torrance, 1977), largely measuring achievement and
ers, third graders, and sixth graders support a two-fac- aptitude in those specific areas. Even teacher recom-
tor structure. In addition, the results of the multiple mendation focuses more on the students classroom
group analyses indicated that the latent structure of the performance than other kinds of potential. For these

8 Creativity Research Journal


Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking

reasons, the TTCT-Figural is valuable in that it allows about the childs lack of the expected characteristics.
another perspective on the students ability that is Reasons included immature social and emotional be-
vastly different from other aptitude and achievement havior, lack of striving to achieve, and less productivity
tests. It also may be less biased for those who speak than other classmates.
English as a second language (Torrance, 1977), be- Identification based on IQ scores may appear like
cause the test is not based on a students ability to use an obvious way to identify gifted students in an educa-
the English language. tional system that favors conformity, but such a limited
As an alternative to standardized testing, expert selection criteria risks allowing creatively gifted chil-
opinions are highly recommended for identifying dren to go unnoticed, thus leaving their needs unmet.
gifted students by Baer (1993, 1994). Cramond Torrance (1962) was concerned that a great deal of cre-
(1994b), however, pointed out that experts might have ative talent goes unrecognized. Torrance (1960, 1962,
failed to identify Van Gogh, Einstein, and Edison as 2002) concluded that if we identify gifted children
gifted when they were children either because of a only on the basis of IQ and scholastic aptitude tests, we
vested interest in the status quo or because of the chil- are eliminating approximately 70% of the top 20% of
drens lack of production in the field of their future suc- creative students from consideration. According to
cess. Experts may find the child who is already suc- Barron and Mackinnons (Barron, 1961; Macckinnon,
ceeding in a field but are hard pressed to discover latent 1961, 1978) threshold theory, creativity and IQ are
potential. Furthermore, such opinions are costly, and moderately related, but the relationship disappears for
true experts are rare, especially in rural areas. people with an IQ above 120. Walberg and Herbig
In practical situations, teacher nomination is one of (1991; Walberg, 1988) concluded that the very bright-
the most common methods for identifying gifted stu- est, as identified by estimated IQ, are not necessarily
dents. However, teachers tend to prefer gifted children the most creative.
who are low in creativity to those who are highly cre- To balance the risk of missing creative students that
ative (Anderson, 1961). Research has shown that comes from identifying students by IQ only, an addi-
teachers are apt to identify students who are achievers tional form of selection may be called for. The
and teacher pleasers as gifted rather than creative stu- TTCT-Figural may be a less biased and more efficient
dents who may be disruptive or unconventional (Davis method than expert opinions, which may be inaccessi-
& Rimm, 1994; Oliphant, 1986; Rimm & Davis, 1976; ble, subjective, and expensive. It may also be less sub-
Ritchie, 1980; Robinson, 1980). Even worse, energetic jective and biased than teacher recommendations. Fur-
and unconventional students can be seen as having At- thermore, the TTCT should be used to add highly
tention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) by creative students who may remain unnoticed in other
their teachers (Cramond, 1994a). As a result of scho- ways, rather than to eliminate students who otherwise
lastic expectations and the needs of creatively gifted qualify for gifted education services (Torrance, 2002).
children, the potential of creatively gifted students may The TTCT is not usually used by itself to make
be overlooked by teachers who view them as trouble- high-stakes decisions on admission to gifted programs.
makers rather than successful young scholars. As an example, according to the Georgia Department
Getzels and Jackson (1958) found that highly cre- of Education (1998; Krisel & Cowan, 1997), to be eli-
ative adolescents are estranged from their teachers and gible for gifted programs (a) a student must score ei-
peers. Children with high IQs were rated by their ther at the 99th percentile (for kindergarteners2nd
teachers as more desirable, better understood, more graders) or the 96th percentile (for 3rd 12th graders)
ambitious, and more studious than children with high on the composite or full-scale score of a standardized
creativity (Torrance, 1962). Drews (1961) found that intelligence test and meet the achievement criteria or
the studious achievers attained the highest teacher (b) qualify through a multiple-criteria assessment pro-
grades, whereas the creative intellectuals attained the cess by meeting the criteria in three of the following
lowest among three types of gifted high school stu- four areas: intelligence, achievement, creativity, and
dents: social leaders, studious achievers, and creative motivation. This indicates that creativity is onenot
intellectuals. In Whitmores study (1980), when in- the onlycriterion used to identify a gifted student.
formed that children they had not recommended might Because Torrances purposes for developing the TTCT
be gifted, teachers usually volunteered information were for inclusion of students rather than exclusion of

Creativity Research Journal 9


K. H. Kim

students for individualizing instructional programs pears likely that the frequency of different responses
(Treffinger, 1985), the TTCT should be used for in- would have changed between 1984 and the present. In
cluding students for gifted programs. An example of addition, these responses may differ across cultures.
the merits of an inclusive system can be found in Geor- The fact that one of Torrances associates, Richard
gia, where many school systems use the TTCT for their Johnson, found that originality is culture specific
creativity measure in the selection of students for (Millar, 1995) supports this opinion. Saeki, Fan, and
gifted programs (a student must score at the 90th per- Van Dusen (2001) also noticed this difference among
centile on the TTCT). Since Georgia adopted the mul- cultural groups when comparing Japanese and Ameri-
tiple criteria selection process, more minority, disad- can responses. The experiences of the TTCT scorers
vantaged, and other at-risk students have been placed also have confirmed that there might be cultural differ-
in gifted programs than when Georgia merely used IQ ences in the originality lists (Kim, 2004b). These stud-
scores (Williams, 2000). ies suggested the creation and use of independent crite-
ria for each group. The statistical frequency of various
responses will vary among people from different cul-
Suggestions for Further Development tures, which would bring misleading results if the
TTCT-Figural were used in different cultures without
The TTCT-Figural appears to display adequate re- adequate norm groups for each population and, subse-
liability and validity (Treffinger, 1985; Cooper, 1991) quently, their own originality lists.
for the purposes of the test. However, reliability and Some studies (Kwon, 1997; Kwon, Gotez, &
validity information for the latest addition of Zellner, 1998) explored the development of a com-
TTCT-Figural (Torrance, 1998) have not been pro- puter-based TTCT-Figural. Goff (2001) developed a
vided. This information is needed, because the new brief version of the TTCT on compact disk, which is
norm group may provide different reliability and va- encouraging further development of the TTCT. Use of
lidity than the 1990 norm group. This is an issue, be- computer scoring would provide efficient and
cause without reliability and validity, use of a test vi- easy-to-understand interpretations of test results, espe-
olates American Psychological Association standards cially when one considers the amount of time required
of practice, particularly when a test is used as an in- for scoring the TTCT. It appears wise to carefully ex-
strument for making a decision for admission to a periment with appropriate applications of the com-
program. puter and Internet for future testing. In the com-
Besides a lack of reliability and validity information puter-based TTCT studies (Kwon, 1997; Kwon, Gotez,
in the latest version of the TTCT for the norm groups, & Zellner, 1998), the TTCT was interpreted in terms of
demographic characteristics such as gender, race, com- time of response so that time-related information use-
munity status, and speakers of English as second lan- ful for understanding the creative thinking processes
guage were not outlined (e.g., Torrance, 1990, 1998). could be gathered; however, computer-based scoring
This omission could stem from the conclusion that the was not conducted. Using a mouse to respond to the
TTCT is fair in terms of race, socioeconomic status, stimuli was reported to be more difficult and time-con-
and culture (Cramond, 1993; Torrance, 1971, 1977; suming compared with the paper-and-pencil TTCT.
Torrance & Torrance, 1972). However, such demo- Further developments of the TTCT could include the
graphic information should be included at different use of a drawing pad instead of a mouse (Kwon, 1997;
time periods for each version of the TTCT, because la- Kwon et al., 1998). In addition, computer-based ver-
tent structures could vary across different groups for sions of the TTCT should be studied to determine
each CI. This information could lead to a greater un- whether they yield equivalent scores as well as equal
derstanding of the TTCT, other creativity tests, the na- levels of reliability and validity as the original.
ture of creativity, and, ultimately, how to encourage Torrance (1974) demonstrated that a variety of
creativity. motivational procedures affect creative functioning,
It is probable that the originality scores also would furthering the measurement of the abilities involved
change among various demographics over time. The in creative thinking. For instance, rewarding creative
author questions the credibility of originality scores behavior (Torrance, 1965a), stress and mental health
from 1998, which used the same lists as in 1984. It ap- (Torrance, 1965b), and teaching procedures and envi-

10 Creativity Research Journal


Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking

ronmental conditions (1966, 1974) can influence Swartz, 1988), because variations in testing procedures
TTCT scores. Other researchers also concluded that can affect scores (Swartz, 1988). To ensure qualified
motivation (Bamber, 1973; Halpin & Halpin, 1973), scorers, there are ongoing TTCT Scoring Trainings by
fatigue (Johnson & Fishkin, 1999), and testing condi- the Torrance Center for Creativity and Talent Develop-
tions (Bamber, 1973; Callahan, 1991; Halpin & ment at The University of Georgia and Scholastic
Halpin, 1973), as well as exposure to diverse infor- Testing Service, Inc.
mation (Clapham, 20002001), influence TTCT In conclusion, the TTCT appears to be a good mea-
scores. At the same time, however, these effects may sure, not only for identifying and educating the gifted
be related to test reliability, depending on administra- but also for discovering and encouraging everyday life
tion differences. Some studies (Iscoe & Pierce-Jones, creativity in the general population. When used appro-
1964; Wallach & Kogan, 1965) indicated that creativ- priately, the TTCT is an important part of Torrances
ity measures are influenced when creativity tests are legacy and dream: to nurture and enhance creativity
administered as serious tests rather than as fun activi- among students.
ties, especially for children in kindergarten or in the
early grades. To address these issues, Torrance (1987)
suggested using a warm-up activity before adminis- References
tration of the TTCT to arouse the incubation pro-
cesses and increase motivation, thereby enabling cre- Abernathy Tannehill, R. L. (1998). An analysis of selected creativity
ative energy. The use of a standardized warm-up tests administered to students affiliated with the Cherokee tribe.
activity that meets Torrances criteria could enhance Dissertation Abstracts International, 58(7-a), 2526. (UMI No.
9801472)
the test and minimize fluctuations caused by motiva-
Ali-el-din, M. T. (1978). Torrance indicators of creative thinking: a
tional factors. Guidelines for a specific activity in- developmental study. Dissertation Abstracts International,
cluded in the manual and treated as a part of the test 1979, 39(7-a), 4129. (UMI No. 7901613)
would increase the likelihood that administrators Ali-el-din, M. T. (1982). Humor production of highly creative chil-
would follow Torrances suggestions. dren. Gifted International, 1(1), 129134.
Anderson, K. E. (1961). Research on the academically talented stu-
dent. Washington, DC: National Education Association Project
on the Academically Talented Student.
Conclusion Baer, J. (1993). Why you shouldnt trust creativity tests. Educational
Leadership, 51(4), 8083.
Both Torrance (Treffinger, 1985) and Cropley Baer, J. (1994). Why you still shouldnt trust creativity tests. Educa-
tional Leadership, 52(2), 7273.
(2000) suggested that, considering the multidimen-
Baer, J. (1997). Creative teachers, creative students. Needham
sional nature of the creativity concept, assessments Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
should be based on several tests, rather than relying on Ball, O. E., & Torrance, E. P. (1984). Streamlines scoring workbook:
a single score. A minimum of two measures to assess Figural A. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
childrens potential for creative behavior was recom- Bamber, R. T. (1973). Play, interest, domestication, and creativity.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 35, 1013B1014B. (UMI
mended by Johnson and Fishkin (1999). I also recom-
No. 7418463)
mend using at least two measures; that is, the TTCT Barron, F. (1961). Creative vision and expression in writing and
and another indicator (e.g., products, performance, rat- painting. In D. W. MacKinnon (Ed.), The creative person (pp.
ing scales, or recommendations). 237251). Berkeley: University of California, Institute of Per-
Torrances research into creativity as a measure of sonality Assessment Research.
Callahan, C. M. (1991). The assessment of creativity. In N.
intelligence shattered the theory that IQ tests alone can
Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of gifted education
measure real intelligence (Powell, 2003; Shearer, (pp. 219235). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
2003). The TTCT provided a physical measure and Chase, C. I. (1985). Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative
groundwork for the idea that creative levels can be Thinking. In J. V. Mitchell Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measure-
scaled and then increased through practicea premise ments yearbook (pp. 16311632). Lincoln: University of Ne-
braska, Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
that was previously only conceptual (Childs, 2003).
Childs, M. (2003, July 14). Father of creativity E. Paul Torrance
The TTCT can provide useful insights into creativity as dead at 87. The College of Education Online News. Retrieved
long as the tests are used with sensitivity and good February 2, 2006, from http://www.coe.uga.edu/coenews/2003/
judgment by qualified professionals (Treffinger, 1985; EPTorranceObit.html

Creativity Research Journal 11


K. H. Kim

Clapham, M. M. (1998). Structure of figural forms A and B of the Hassan, M. A. (1986). Construct validity of Torrance Tests of Cre-
Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Educational & Psycho- ative Thinking: A confirmatory factor-analytic study. Disserta-
logical Measurement, 58, 275283. tion Abstracts International, 46(8-A), 2233. (UMI No.
Clapham, M. M. (20002001). The effects of affect manipulation 8523000).
and information exposure on divergent thinking. Creativity Re- Heausler, N. L., & Thompson, B. (1988). Structure of the Torrance
search Journal, 13, 335350. Tests of creative thinking. Educational and Psychological Mea-
Cooper, E. (1991). A critique of six measures for assessing creativ- surement, 48, 463468.
ity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 25, 194204. Hbert, T. P., Cramond, B., Neumeister, K. L. S., Millar, G., &
Cramond, B. (1993). The Torrance tests of creative thinking: From Silvian, A. F. (2002). E. Paul Torrance: His life, accomplish-
design through establishment of predictive validity. In R. F. ments, and legacy. Storrs: The University of Connecticut, The
Subotnik & K. D. Arnold (Eds.), Beyond Terman: Contempo- National Research Center on the Gifted and Talented
rary longitudinal studies of giftedness and talent (pp. 229254). (NRC/GT).
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Hocevar, D. (1979a). Ideational fluency as a confounding factor in
Cramond, B. (1994a). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the measurement of originality. Journal of Educational Psy-
creativityWhat is the connection? Journal of Creative Behav- chology, 71, 191196.
ior, 28, 193210. Hocevar, D. (1979b). The unidimensional nature of creative thinking
Cramond, B. (1994b). We can trust creativity tests. Educational in fifth grade children. Child Study Journal, 9, 273278.
Leadership, 52(2), 7071. Hocevar, D., & Michael, W. (1979). The effects of scoring formulas
Cropley, A. J. (1971). Some Canadian creativity research. Journal of on the discriminant validity of tests of divergent thinking. Edu-
Research and Development in Education, 4(3), 113115. cational and Psychological Measurement, 39, 917921.
Cropley, A. J. (2000). Defining and measuring creativity: Are cre- Iscoe, I., & Pierce-Jones, J. (1964). Divergent thinking, age, and in-
ativity tests worth using? Roeper Review, 23(2), 7279. telligence in white and Negro children. Child Development, 35,
Davis, G. A. (1997). Identifying creative students and measuring 785798.
creativity. In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of Johnson, A. S., & Fishkin, A. S. (1999). Assessment of cognitive and
gifted education (pp. 269281). Needham Heights, MA: affective behaviors related to creativity. In A. S. Fishkin, B.
Viacom.
Cramond, & P. Olszewski-Kubilius (Eds.), Investigating cre-
Davis, G. A., & Rimm, S. B. (1994). Education of the gifted and tal-
ativity in youth: Research and methods (pp. 265306).
ented (3rd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Simon & Schuster.
Cresskill, NJ: Hampton.
Dixon, J. (1979). Quality versus quantity: The need to control for the
Kim, K. H. (2004a, April). Confirmatory factor analyses and multi-
fluency factor in originality scores from the Torrance Tests.
ple group analyses on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking.
Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 2, 7079.
Poster presented at the Research Symposium: The Preparation
Drews, E. M. (1961). Creative intellectual style in gifted adoles-
of Educators, Athens, GA. Sponsored by Georgia Systemic
cents. East Lansing: Michigan State University.
Teacher Education Program.
E. Paul Torrance, 87: developed series of creativity tests. (2003, July
Kim, K. H. (2004b). Cultural influence on creativity: The relation-
18). Los Angeles Times, p. B13.
Georgia Department of Education. (1998). 16042.38 Education ship between creativity and Confucianism. Unpublished doc-
program for gifted students. Retrieved February 2, 2006, from toral dissertation, The University of Georgia, Athens.
http://www.gadoe.org/_documents/doe/legalservices/1604 Kim, K. H. (in press). Is creativity unidemensional or multidimen-
2.38.pdf sional? Analyses of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking.
Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1958). The meaning of Creativity Research Journal.
Giftednessan examination of an expanding concept. Phi Kim. K. H., & Cramond, B. (2004, November). Confirmatory factor
Delta Kappan, 40, 7577. analyses of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Paper pre-
Goff, K. (2001). Creativity for success: Unleash the power of cre- sented at the 51st Annual Convention of the National Associa-
ativity. Stillwater, OK: Little Ox Books. tion for Gifted Children in Salt Lake City, UT.
Gonzales, M. A., & Campos, A. (1997). Mental imagery and creative Kim, K. H., Cramond, B., & Bandalos, D. L. (2004, July). The latent
thinking. Journal of Psychology, 131, 357364. structure of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking-Figural.
Guilford, J. P. (1956). Structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, Poster presented at the 112th Convention of the American Psy-
53, 267293. chological Association, Honolulu, HI.
Guilford, J. P. (1959). Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill. Kim, K. H., Cramond, B., & Bandalos, D. L. (in press). Latent struc-
Guilford, J. P. (1960). Basic conceptual problems of the psychology ture and measurement invariance of scores on the
of thinking. Proceedings of the New York Academy of Sciences, TTCTFigureal. Educational and Psychological Measurement.
91, 621. Kirton, M. J. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: A description and
Guilford, J. P. (1962). Factors that aid and hinder creativity. Teachers measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 622629.
College Record, 63, 380392. Kirton, M. J. (1978). Have adaptors and innovators equal levels of
Guilford, J. P. (1986). Creative talents: Their nature, uses and devel- creativity? Psychological Reports, 42, 695698.
opment. Buffalo, NY: Bearly Ltd. Kirton, M. J. (1989). Adaptors and innovators at work. In M. J.
Halpin, G., & Halpin, G. (1973). The effect of motivation on creative Kirton (Ed.), Adaptors and innovators: Styles of creativity and
thinking abilities. Journal of Creative Behavior, 7, 5153. problem-solving (pp. 5678). London: Routledge.

12 Creativity Research Journal


Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking

Krisel, S. C., & Cowan, R. S. (1997). Georgias journey toward mul- Saeki, N., Fan, X., & Van Dusen, L. V. (2001). A comparative study
tiple-criteria identification of gifted students. Roeper Review, of creative thinking of American and Japanese college students.
20(2), A1A3. Journal of Creative Behavior, 35(1), 2438.
Kwon, M. C. (1997). An exploratory study of a computerized cre- Shearer, L. (2003, July 14). Former UGA professor Paul Torrance
ativity test: Comparing paper-pencil and computer-based ver- dies. Athens Banner-Herald. Retrieved February 2, 2006, from
sions of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Dissertation http://www.coe.uga.edu/coenews/2003/TorranceOA.html
Abstracts International, 58(1-A), 0141, (UMI No. 9718390) Simonton, D. K. (1990) Creativity and wisdom in aging. In J. E.
Kwon, M., Goetz, E. T., & Zellner, R. D. (1998). Developing a com- Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of
puter-based TTCT: Promises and problems. Journal of Creative aging (3rd ed., pp. 320329). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Behavior, 32, 96106. Swartz, J. D. (1988). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. In D. J.
Lissitz, R. W., & Willhoft, J. L. (1985). A methodological study of Keyser & R. C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test Critique Vol. 7 (pp.
the Torrance Tests of Creativity. Journal of Educational Mea- 619622). Kansas, MS: Test Corporation of America.
surement, 22, 1111. Torrance, E. P. (1960). Eight partial replications of the Getzels-Jack-
MacKinnon, D. W. (1961). Creativity in architects. In D. W. son study (Research memo BER6018). Minneapolis: Univer-
MacKinnon (Ed.), The creative person (pp. 237251). Berke- sity of Minnesota, Bureau of Educational Research.
ley: University of California, Institute of Personality Assess- Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent. Englewood Cliffs,
ment Research. NJ: Prentice Hall.
MacKinnon, D. W. (1978). In search of human effectiveness: Iden- Torrance, E. P. (1965a). Rewarding creative behavior. Englewood
tifying and developing creativity. Buffalo, NY: Creative Educa- Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
tion Foundation. Torrance, E. P. (1965b). Mental health and constructive behavior.
Millar, G. W. (1995). E. Paul Torrance: The creativity man. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Torrance, E. P. (1966). The Torrance Tests of Creative
Millar, G. W. (2002). The Torrance kids at mid-life. Westport, CT: Thinking-Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition-Verbal
Ablex. Tests, Forms A and B-Figural Tests, Forms A and B. Princeton,
Mourad, S. A. (1976). A factor-analytic study of the streamlined NJ: Personnel Press.
scoring of Figural Form A of the Torrance Tests of Creative Torrance, E. P. (1969). Prediction of adult creative achievement
Thinking. Unpublished masters thesis, University of Georgia, among high school seniors. Gifted Child Quarterly, 13,
Athens. 223229.
Oliphant, C. C. (1986). A descriptive study of factors associated with Torrance, E. P. (1971). Are the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking
teacher identification of gifted students. Dissertation Abstracts biased against or in favor of disadvantaged groups? Gifted
International, 47, 1691. (UMI No. 8611909) Child Quarterly, 15, 7580.
Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof in the pudding? Reanalyses of Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance Tests of
Torrances (1958 to present) longitudinal data. Creativity Re- Creative Thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6(4),
search Journal, 12, 103114. 236252.
Plucker, J. A., & Runco, M. A. (1998). The death of creativity mea- Torrance, E. P. (1974). The Torrance Tests of Creative
surement has been greatly exaggerated. Roeper Review, 21(1), Thinking-Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition-Verbal
3639. Tests, Forms A and B- Figural Tests, Forms A and B. Princeton,
Powell, K. (2003, July 16). Paul Torrance, writer, innovative educa- NJ: Personnel Press.
tor. Atlanta Journal-Constitution. Retrieved February 2, 2006, Torrance, E. P. (1977). Discovery and nurturance of giftedness in the
from http://www.uga.edu/newsbureau/releases/2003releases/ culturally different. Reston, VA: Council on Exceptional
0307/030714torrance.html Children.
Rimm, S. B., & Davis, G. A. (1976). GIFT: An instrument for the Torrance, E. P. (1979). The search for satori and creativity. Buffalo,
identification of creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 10, NY: Bearly Limited.
178182. Torrance, E. P. (1980). Growing up creatively gifted: A 22-year lon-
Ritchie, S. P. (1980). Creativity and risk-taking in young children. Dis- gitudinal study. Creative Child and Adult Quarterly, 5,
sertation Abstracts International, 42, 539. (UMI No. 8114593) 148158, 170.
Robinson, A. W. (1980). A study of the effectiveness of a special Torrance, E. P. (1981a). Empirical validation of criterion-referenced
class for academically gifted elementary students on the en- indicators of creative ability through a longitudinal study. Cre-
hancement of the characteristics of giftedness. Dissertation Ab- ative Child and Adult Quarterly, 6, 136140.
stracts International, 41, 1380. (UMI No. 8021164) Torrance, E. P. (1981b). Predicting the creativity of elementary
Runco, M. A., & Mraz, W. (1992). Scoring divergent thinking tests school children (195880) and the teacher who made a differ-
using total ideational output and a creativity index. Educational ence. Gifted Child Quarterly, 25, 5562.
and Psychological Measurement, 52, 213221. Torrance, E. P. (1987). Guidelines for administration and scor-
Rungsinan, A. (1977). Originality, elaboration, resistance to quick ing/comments on using the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking.
closure, unusual visual perspective, and movement among sec- Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc.
ond grade children in Thailand and the United States. Disserta- Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its test-
tion Abstracts International, 1977, 38A, 1309. (UMI No. ing. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp.
7718939) 4373). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Creativity Research Journal 13


K. H. Kim

Torrance, E. P. (1990). The Torrance tests of creative thinking Treffinger, D. J. (1985). Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative
normstechnical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. Thinking. In J. V. Mitchell Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measure-
Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc. ments yearbook (pp. 16321634). Lincoln: University of Ne-
Torrance, E. P. (1994). Creativity: Just wanting to know. Pretoria, braska, Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
Republic of South Africa: Benedic Books. Walberg, H. A. (1988). Creativity and talent as learning. In R. J.
Torrance, E. P. (1998). The Torrance tests of creative thinking Sternberg (Eds.), The nature of creativity (pp. 340361). New
normstechnical manual figural (streamlined) forms A & B. York: Cambridge University Press.
Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc. Walberg, H. J., & Herbig, M. P. (1991). Developing talent, creativity,
Torrance, E. P. (2002). The manifesto: A guide to developing a cre- and eminence. In N. Colangelo & G. A. Davis (Eds.), Hand-
ative career. West Westport, CT: Ablex. book of gifted education (pp. 245255). Boston: Allyn & Ba-
Torrance, E. P., & Ball, O. E. (1984). The Torrance Tests of Creative con.
Thinking Streamlined (revised) manual, Figural A and B. Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young
Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service, Inc. children. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Torrance, E. P., & Safter, H. T. (1999). Making the creative leap be- Whitmore, J. R. (1980). Giftedness, conflict, and underachievement.
yond. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education Foundation Press. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Torrance, E. P., Tan, C. A., & Allman, T. (1970). Verbal originality Willams, E. (2000). The history of the evolution of gifted identifica-
and teacher behavior: A predictive validity study. Journal of tion procedures in Georgia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Teacher Education, 21, 335341. The University of Georgia, Athens.
Torrance, E. P., & Torrance, P. (1972). Combining creative prob- Witt, G. (1971). The Life Enrichment Activity Program, Inc.: A con-
lem-solving with creative expressive activities in the education tinuing program for creative, disadvantaged children. Journal
of disadvantaged young people. Journal of Creative Behavior, of Research and Development in Education, 4(3), 1422.
6(1), 110. Yamada, H., & Tam, A. Y. (1996). Prediction study of adult creative
Torrance, E. P., & Wu, T. (1981). A comparative longitudinal study achievement: Torrances longitudinal study of creativity revis-
of the adult creative achievements of elementary school chil- ited. Journal of Creative Behavior, 30, 144149.
dren identified as highly intelligent and as highly creative. Cre-
ative Child and Adult Quarterly, 6, 7176.

14 Creativity Research Journal

S-ar putea să vă placă și