Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

Counselling and Psychotherapy Research, 2013

Vol. 13, No. 3, 210219, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14733145.2012.739633

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Repurposing process measures to train psychotherapists:


Training outcomes using a new approach

ANTONIO PASCUAL-LEONE* & CRISTINA ANDREESCU

Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario, Canada

Abstract
Aims: First, this paper presents the rationale for a novel approach to training counsellors in which measures for
psychotherapy process research are taught to students before moving on to teaching basic empathic reflections and
interventions. The rationale for this is that client process measures can be re-purposed to help orient and sensitise trainees
to key in-session moments. Second, we present a training outcome study that assesses the effectiveness of this approach.
Method: Using an experiential-integrative therapy approach, a 13-week training program was used to teach psychotherapy
skills and process research measures to22 clinical graduate students taken from two cohorts. As part of the course, trainees
conducted several single sessions with volunteer clients on four separate occasions. Training outcomes were measured using
both trainee and client reports. Results: Compared to baseline, therapists reported significant and steady gains (all ps B.05)
in session management, reducing their anxious self-awareness, and in improved sense of self-efficacy, with the latter having
the largest effect (partial Eta Sq. .381). Discussion: While the findings provide some support for a new training strategy, a
dismantling design is needed next to more closely examine the process-measure approach to training.

Keywords: graduate student; measures; outcome; process; psychotherapy integration; training

Introduction This paper offers two main contributions. First,


the introduction of this paper describes new ideas in
Psychotherapy research has largely focused on
a research-informed practice for training; one which
developing and testing treatment programmes,
draws on what we now know about psychotherapy
while surprisingly little attention has been dedi-
process to inform the strategy we use in training
cated to teaching psychotherapy and examining
novice therapists. This notion is one that has recently
how to best train professionals. Even so, an
informed our teaching of basic counselling skills, and
increasing demand for psychotherapy points to
is also the approach we examine in this paper.
the need for more therapists who can provide
Second, we report on a pilot study examining the
effective services for a range of difficulties (van impact of our introductory course in psychotherapy
Deurzen-Smith, 1996). Furthermore, in a broader skills training, by measuring training effects over
context, the true effectiveness of psychological time.
treatments is always constrained in some way
by our ability to successfully train clinicians. It
follows that there is also an important need to Findings on psychotherapy training
develop and test psychotherapy training pro- In a recent meta-analysis, Hill and Lent (2006)
grammes and, indeed, this has started to become examined 14 studies on specific methods used in
an area of increased interest (Hilsenroth, Defife, counselling and psychotherapy skillstraining, which
Blagys, & Ackerman, 2006). were completed from 1967 to 2006 (surprisingly

*Corresponding author. Email: apl@uwindsor.ca

# 2013 British Association for Counselling and Psychotherapy


Training psychotherapists 211

few). The authors found that the aggregated effect client.Current approaches to psychotherapy training
size of training as compared to a no-training control emphasise the former (i.e. intervention skills), while
was quite large (i.e. d .89), although there was the question of developing a trainees ability to
substantial variability among studies. They also recognise and orient to key client processes (what
concluded that modeling is more effective than we will call perceptual acuity) receives much less
instruction and feedback, and multi-method ap- attention (see Hill & Lent, 2006; Ronnestad &
proaches to training are more impactful than single Ladany, 2006). We believe this imbalance may be
methods. Since the Hill and Lent (2006) review, due to a scarcity of formal teaching strategies, which
only a handful of new studies have been conducted might be used to heighten the perceptiveness and
on training. There have been inconsistent findings sensitivity of trainees, helping them to recognise and
about whether training in specific approaches is interpret phenomena that are emerging in-session.
actually related to specific intervention use (Boswell, Client process measures, however, may turn out to be
Castonguay, & Wasserman, 2010; Hilsenroth et al., particularly useful in this regard.
2006). Yet, trainees do report reliable increases in For example, novice therapists often do not
their sense of self-efficacy (Hill et al., 2008). immediately notice when clients deflect away from
Despite the generally promising findings, Hill and evocative (hot) content; but narrative process
Lent (2006) argue that much of the existing research measures (i.e. Angus, Levitt, & Hartke, 1999) could
would benefit from more rigorous design and out- be used to help sensitise trainees in attending to their
come measurement. Boswell et al. (2010) have also clients spontaneous shifts in narrative style. Thus, a
made the case that more proximal changes, such as research measure such as this can help trainees to
session outcomes, might be more suitable when firstly: simply perceive, recognise, and track a clients
examining the impact of training. Finally, it seems ephemeral shifts from external narratives (i.e. plot-
that psychotherapy training would also benefit from and-characters) to more internal narratives (i.e.
developing newer formulations and more practical meaning-and-experiences). Secondly, this appercep-
strategies to training. tive framework provides a lens to help orient trainees
as they use interventions to actively facilitate a
clients elaboration of more promising processes
Training psychotherapists with client process measures:
(i.e. internal narratives, hot cognitions).
A new approach
In a similar example, certain client vocal qualities,
Although there have been important advances in the such as a highly external, rapid, lecturing voice vs. a
research on training, pedagogy of psychotherapy has softer, searching, stop-and-go and internally focused
not yet fully exploited the dramatic advances that voice have also been operationalised and then related
have also been made in process research (see to unproductive and productive processes, respec-
Pachankis & Goldfried, 2007). For example, there tively (Rice & Kerr, 1986). Descriptions of these
are a number of well known process measures vocal qualities can provide very intuitive and easy
(detailed below) that could be used concretely as cues to trainees about a clients internal process and
teaching tools. While this sometimes happens in- his or her need for moment-by-moment interven-
formally, particularly when research and clinical tions. In the same vein, while beginner therapists are
training are conducted under the same roof, it is often anxious about silences during therapy sessions,
uncommon that researchers suggest re-purposing familiarising them with research on the nature of
process measures for psychotherapy training. productive vs. obstructive client pauses (i.e. Levitt,
Hilsenroth (2007) is one exception, having proposed 2001) also provides trainees with practical and
that the Comparative Psychotherapy Process Scale immediately usable information on what is happen-
(CPPS) be used as a training tool to familiarise ing with their clients and how to respond in-session.
trainees with discriminating features of cognitive- To date, a few client process research instruments
behavioral vs. interpersonal-psychodynamic inter- have already been incipiently and informally inte-
ventions. But this idea can be further developed: grated into methods for teaching psychotherapy. The
While measures of therapist process or intervention client experiencing scale (Klein, Mathieu-Coughlan,
(i.e. the CPPS) could help trainees to identify and & Kiesler, 1986) has long been considered a gold
grasp, what to do as a therapist,  client process standard of therapeutic process because it has
measures, in contrast, would help trainees to, been shown to be predictive of good treatment
perceive what is happening in-session with the outcomes in experiential, psychodynamic, and
212 A. Pascual-Leone & C. Andreescu

cognitive treatment approaches (Greenberg & students the evidence related to a given phenomen-
Pascual-Leone, 2006). Being competent in the use on, trainees could be asked to engage in a research-
of this observer rating scale is known (particularly in like exercise of coding or rating phenomenon from
the experiential tradition) to be helpful in orienting personal narratives or audio/video segments to pro-
therapists to a clients immediate process and then in vide the kind of perceptual training that is prerequi-
deliberately deepening a clients level of meaning site to the incisive and timely use of interventions.
exploration in-session, whether that be affective or
cognitive in nature. However, it has become com-
Starting a programme of research on perceptual acuity
mon that trainees studying emotion-focused therapy
training
are encouraged to practice using the experiencing
scale whenever such research training is available. We have proposed a new process research approach
Furthermore, the fact that the research scale is to training but empirically exploring this notion
reprinted in a number of treatment manuals (e.g. requires at least three steps. (1) One needs to
Paivio & Pascual-Leone, 2010) is particularly reveal- develop a training programme or course that ex-
ing as to its perceived usefulness in learning the plicitly focuses on perceptual acuity; particularly,
therapy itself. given that there is no coherent precedent for this
This re-integration of a research measure, into the approach and given that it contrasts with an estab-
training for a therapy from which the measure itself lished pedagogy which gives preference to interven-
originated, is even more dramatic in the psychody- tion as such. (2) Next, one should conduct a pilot
namic tradition. The core conflictual relationship study,verifying that the new course has initial pro-
theme (CCRT; Luborsky, Popp, Luborsky, & Mark, mise and can be delivered in a coherent and clinically
1994) method was first introduced as a new research relevant fashion.(3) Finally, a fully controlled dis-
tool for testing and verifying Freuds hypothesis of mantling study would be required to demonstrate
transference. Since then it has been increasingly used the benefit of the new training component.Thus, in
as a way of palpably introducing students to the this paper, we do not yet hope to directly test the
construct of transference and teaching them to make training strategy against a control but rather aim only
case formulations. This seamless shift in use of the to employ the new approach and then report within-
CCRT process measure (from research to training group training effects.
purposes) is not often acknowledged in treatment
manuals or training literature. Nevertheless, becom-
Our current training programme
ing proficient in using the CCRT by applying it to
transcripts can, again, sensitise trainees to the In the context of training for professional practice we
perceptual framework that is important to a psycho- have adopted the perceptual-acuity training as a key
dynamic therapy. A perceptual framework of this sort strategy for training novice therapists. In what
is central to the training strategy we are proposing. follows we describe the course to illustrate how
When it comes to observing key changes in a process measures are integrated into the larger
therapeutic alliance, research criteria used to identify context of skills training.
and study relationship ruptures (Safran, Samstag,
Muran, & Stevens, 2001), have now been largely Course structure. The course is given using an
assimilated into best practice of psychotherapy and integrative framework with a primary emphasis on
training. Thus, the original research criteria for experiential psychotherapy. The class follows a
recognising and managing ruptures have influenced 13-week curriculum and meets for one three-hour
a range of dynamic, experiential, and integrative- class per week. Students are first given weekly
cognitive approaches (e.g. see Castonguay et al., lectures (60 minutes per class), and participate in a
2004). Familiarising trainees with the original re- discussion of readings for the week, followed by
search criteria that have been used for detecting and video therapy demonstrations with commentary by
classifying different types of relationship ruptures the course director (40 minutes). Finally, they
between clients and their therapists, as well as the participate, on a weekly basis, in supervised informal
steps in repairing a therapeutic relationship, offers practice sessions with their peers (45 minutes).
them concrete instruction for identifying key rela-
tional features. However, as with all the process tools Supervised practice with peers. During the weekly
we have discussed, rather than simply teaching practice sessions, trainees work in groups of three:
Training psychotherapists 213

one offering his or her own personal material as client practice skills by providing eight single-sessions each
(i.e. real and unscripted), another serving in the role with a different volunteer client distributed across
of counsellor, and a third acting as observer or on-call several occasions during the course. Unlike the
counsellor, who makes process observations and supervised informal practice done with peers, volun-
provides support when requested by the counsellor. teer clients are unknown to the trainee and they
While these informal practice sessions are conducted present with real and unscripted mild-to-moderate
in semi-private settings, the course supervisor peri- personal difficulties. These sessions provide trainees
odically drops in and discretely attends to the with the uninterrupted opportunity to independently
moment-by-moment process, offering live supervi- practice their skills in a realistic context.
sion and modeling as needed.
Client-level feedback to trainees. Following the
Assignments. There are eight assignments distrib- 45-minute sessions, clients and trainees each com-
uted throughout the semester. The purpose of these plete a set of session outcome questionnaires (see
assignments is to increase students awareness of measure in Method section, below). Similar to the
productive therapeutic processing. To that end, in work of Lambert et al. (2005), this feedback is later
addition to learning intervention techniques, stu- summarised and made available to students as part
dents receive instruction and rate therapy-like ma- of their training (these measures were also the source
terial using a number of established psychotherapy of research data that is presented later in this paper).
process measures in the hopes of sensitising them to
relevant client markers of therapeutic process. As- Current study and hypotheses
signments are as follows:
Following the articulation of a new strategy for skills
(1) Keep an emotion diary and use the levels of training, the current research aim was to pilot the
emotional awareness scale (Lane, 1991). training approach and provide initial support. It was
(2) Write a personal narrative about a traumatic hypothesised that (1) client ratings of the sessions
experience (c.f. Pennebaker, 1997). they participated in, as well as (2) trainee/therapist
(3) With the help of a peer, create an audio-taped rating of their own sense of competence, would each
session describing a personal relationship epi- reflect the improvement of trainees skills over time.
sode, and then transcribe it.
(4) Apply the narrative process coding system (An- Methods
gus et al., 1999) to both the personal narrative
and to the transcribed relationship account Participants
(i.e. assignments 2 and 3). Graduate trainees. Graduate counsellors in training
(5) Apply the vocal quality scale (Rice & Kerr, were 22 first year Masters-level students in a clinical
1986) to the audio file one helped ones peer psychology programme and enrolled in an introduc-
make for assignment 3. tory psychotherapy skills course. The course was run
(6) Apply the client experiencing scale (Klein et al., for two consecutive years so that participants volun-
1986) to both narrative and relationship teering for this study came from one of two cohorts
accounts (i.e. assignments 2 and 3). (each with 11 students). Thus, 22 graduate trainees
(7) Describe a past relationship rupture from ones (16 female, four male) participated in the study, with
personal life and retrospectively identify the ages ranging from 2248, (M 25.4).Two trainees
markers of that rupture. Then invent a ficti- were dropped from the study on account of their
tious new ending to the story, one which each having both missed classes and extreme out-
deliberately follows the steps in Safran et al.s lying data, making the final sample N 18.
(2001) model for rupture repairs.
(8) Apply the core conflictual relationship themes Volunteer clients. The clients that volunteered to
method (Luborsky et al., 1994) to narrative participate in single sessions were undergraduates
and relationship accounts (i.e. assignments 2, with personal problems and were solicited through a
3, and 7). research participant pool. These volunteer clients
responded to an internal online advertisement seeking
Formal practice with volunteer clients. Finally, as an individuals to receive single session interventions
extension of the practice with peers, trainees formally conducted by counsellors-in-training as part of a
214 A. Pascual-Leone & C. Andreescu

training evaluation. In particular, the notice called Ensing, 1996), while convergent validity was also
for volunteer clients to, discuss personal and real satisfactory, when compared to the Session Evalua-
difficulties that are currently troublesome and of tion Questionnaire (Stiles, 1980) and other indices
mild to moderate concern. Volunteer clients re- of client reaction (Reeker et al., 1996). In short, the
ceived a small course credit through the participant RSRS is a measure for clients perception of the
pool for their one-time involvement in this study. A usefulness of the therapy.
total of 71 participants were recruited (61 females
and 10 males) with a mean age of 21.8 (ranging
from1846). Trainee self-report measures

Counsellor Activity Self-Efficacy Scales (CASES).


Measures
The CASES (Lent, Hill, & Hoffman, 2003) is a
At the end of each counselling session, trainees and 44-item self-report measure aimed at assessing skills
clients each completed a separate set of self-reports, in overall sense of confidence on helping and
multiple measures were used to capture both trainee responding, session management, and counselling
and client evaluation. challenges efficacy. At the end of individual counsel-
ling sessions items are rated by counsellors on a
Likert scale from 0 (no confidence) to 9 (complete
Client self-report measures
confidence).The measure has been shown to have
good convergent and discriminant validity when
Working Alliance Inventory-Short Revised Version
compared with other counsellor self-reports such as
(WAI-SR). WAI-SR (Hatcher & Gillaspy, 2006)
Counsellor Self-Estimate Inventory and Social De-
consists of 12 Likert-scale items, ranging from 1
sirability scales. Test-retest reliability after two weeks
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), designed
was also adequate, ranging from .42 to .91 (Lent,
to gauge the depth of the therapeutic relationship.
Hill, & Hoffman, 2003).
The original Working Alliance Inventory (WAI) was
designed by Horvath and Greenberg (1989) to assess
the dyads ability to build a bond, and to collabora- Self-Awareness and Management Strategies Scales
tively set up goals, and tasks for the session. for Therapists (SAMS). The SAMS (Williams,
Correlations between the WAI-SR and the original Hurley, OBrien, & De Gregorio, 2003) is a self-
WAI suggest the new version is comparable (Hatcher report measure designed to assess the prepon-
& Gillaspy, 2006). Reliability coefficients reached derance of disruptive internal experiences or obs-
satisfactory levels (between .88 and .92). The WAI- tacles that intrude while conducting a therapy
SR also correlates strongly with the Confident session. The measure is composed of 33 Likert-scale
Collaboration Scale (Hatcher & Barends, 1996) items ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always), divided
lending further validity to the scale. into two subscales: hindering self-awareness (i.e.
anxious awareness, distracting self-awareness) and
Revised Session Reaction Scale (RSRS). The management of these interferences (i.e. self-care,
RSRS (Elliott, 1993), a revision of the Session relaxation, re-focusing on the client, suppression
Impact Scale (Elliott &Wexler, 1994), is a 24-item of intrusive thoughts). The self-awareness and
questionnaire assessing a clients session experience. management subscales are internally consistent
Items are on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) (.76 and .73, respectively). The measure has good
to 5 (very much) and comprise four sub-scales: the internal consistency (.73 to .76) and demonstrated
Helpful Reactions (e.g. progress towards knowing good convergent/discriminant validity when com-
what to do about problems), Task Reactions (work pared with the Self-Monitoring Scale (Snyder,
toward certain goals, e.g. gaining insight into others 1974) as well as a satisfactory reliability coefficient
behaviour), Relationship Reactions (i.e. feeling un- (Williams et al., 2003). In short, SAMS was
derstood by the counsellor), and finally, Hindering designed to assess the degree to which counsellors
Reactions, which assesses negative feelings about the in training feel compromised by the distracting self-
therapy and/or therapist (e.g. feeling stuck or a lack awareness of their own thoughts, posture, anxiety
of progress.).The scale has demonstrated excellent and any other elements that might interfere with
reliability for all scales (.83 to .92; Reeker, Elliott, & conducting a session.
Training psychotherapists 215

Procedures suitable precautions in care, respect, and informed


consent for both trainees and clients. The trainer/
The psychotherapy skills training course was run as
researcher was kept blind to the identity of trainees
described. Formal sessions, when clients were treated
who voluntarily participated and this was done with
(as contrasted with the weekly practice sessions con-
the help of a teaching assistant who acted as the
ducted among peers), were distributed over the
keeper of data. Volunteer clients, presenting with
course to allow the observation of change over
personal difficulties, attended an orientation just
time. Thus, sessions conducted with clients simul-
prior to study participation where they were re-
taneously served as practice and testing times in
minded of the aim and context of their counselling
this study. During the progression of the 13-week
sessions with trainees. Moreover, a licensed clinical
course, trainees met with clients at four points in the
psychologist was on-site for all sessions, and a
course: week 2 (time1), week 5 (time2), week 9
resource sheet was given to clients following their
(time3), and week 12 (time4).
participation.
Sessions at each of these time points were used to
address, as thoroughly as possible, the personal
difficulties presented by clients, while working within Results
the limits of a single session. Sessions were 45 minutes
Preliminary analyses
long and after each testing time/session both clients
and trainees each completed a set of measures. To Missing therapist data was rare and did not occur
protect against a social desirability effect (on the part systematically. At the first time point, however, client
of clients) or self-serving basis (on the part of data was only available for one of the two cohorts.
trainees), completed measures were placed in sealed All repeated analyses were conducted on both the
envelopes and both trainees and clients were in- complete data set (N11 clients over all four time
formed that ratings would remain anonymous. points) and the larger, truncated data set (N 18
Measurement at the four points in time provided clients over three times points; times 2, 3, 4) and
change indices of trainees skills acquisition in the both methods produced similar and consistently
form of a learning curve. However, as explained in non-significant findings. Thus, for simplicity, ana-
Hill and Lents (2006) recommendations for the lyses of client data are presented for the complete
evaluation of training, when single clients are used data set; while Figure 1 is based on all available data.
one risks that the same trainee might receive, for Additional analyses confirmed that there was no
example, a very psychologically-minded client at the indication of any known differences between the two
beginning of the training course and a very reluctant cohorts of graduate students.
client at the end of the course. Such discrepancies in
client-based evaluations would not accurately reflect Main analyses
the effects of training. Thus, in the aim of giving
further stability to the single session measurements
Testing hypothesis 1: Do clients reports demonstrate
and to protect against any atypical/extreme ratings,
change? Clients did not rate the counselling sessions
counsellors-in-training conducted two (separate)
as increasing in quality, which did not support the
single session interventions at each testing time.
first hypothesis. To that end, repeated-measures
Thus, at each of the four points in time, trainees
ANOVAs revealed the following levels of non-sig-
conducted two 45 minutes sessions back-to-back,
nificance: WAI total (F[3, 30] .205, p .892) and
each with a different client, for a total of eight single
on RSRS subscales (all Fs[3, 30]B.167, ps .835).
session cases. (Trainees never worked with the same
client twice). Finally, scores of every measure were Testing hypothesis 2: Do trainees self-reports de-
then averaged across the two clients treated by a monstrate change? Trainee self-reports indicated some
given trainee at each time point; this was done for skills improvement on each of the two measures,
both client and trainee measures. supporting the second hypothesis (see Figure 1;
Black markers denote trainee self-report measures).

Ethical considerations
Counsellor Activity Self-Efficacy Scales. The total
The design was approved by a university research CASES score indicated a significant improvement
ethics board, and was evaluated as having taken of trainees over time: F(3, 51) 10.470, pB.001,
216 A. Pascual-Leone & C. Andreescu

In addition, trainees showed a change on the


session management subscale, which refers to the
range of tools a therapist uses in order to minimise
self-awareness, anxiety and any other personal fac-
tors that might interfere with the flow of therapy. In
particular, results from this scale indicated a sig-
nificant improvement during the course: F(3, 51)
5.675, p B.002. The pattern of improvement ap-
pears to be linear, and analyses indicated a trend of
improvement between baseline (time 1) and final
session (time 4; p .065). In other words, trainees
reportedly use more self-care/management over the
course of the training programme.

Discussion
In the introduction of this paper, we have conveyed
the rationale for exploring a new strategy in psy-
chotherapy skills training and illustrated how this
might be feasibly integrated into a training pro-
Note: Graduate (N20) training effects: A learning curve over gramme. Another aim of this paper has been to pilot
13 weeks.
this new approach and demonstrate initial support
Figure 1. WAI, RSRS, SAMS measurements range from 0 to 5;
CASES ranges from 0 to 9; White markers client report for a course that re-purposes process measures as
measures; Black markers trainee self-report measures; * p B psychotherapy training tools for perceptual-acuity.
0.05, and ** p B0.005 significant difference from baseline Thus, the study examines the feasibility of this
(time 1); There were no significant pairwise comparisons between approach; it is neither a process-outcome study nor
contiguous testing time. All therapist data (CASES, SAMS) is
a dismantling study and, as such, our finding cannot
based on N 18; Client data (WAI, RSRS) at time 1 is based on
N 11, and at all other times (2, 3, 4) based on N 18.
support claims regarding separate components of the
programme. Nevertheless, trainees indicated a stea-
dy improvement in their confidence and sense of
with an effect size (Partial Eta Sq. .381). The self-efficacy, which was consistent with previous
improvement appeared to be linear, and post-hoc research (Hill et al., 2008). They also reported
analyses indicated the only significant difference of consistent gains in their ability to manage sessions
improvement was between baseline (time 1) and and in reducing anxiousness self-awareness (e.g. by
final session (time 4; p B.005). Thus, although the way of relaxation, re-focusing on the client, and
pairwise changes between contiguous testing times suppression of intrusive thoughts). Furthermore, the
learning curve of graduate training showed a steady
were not statistically significant, this increase ap-
pattern ofchange.
peared to be consistent, suggesting that over the
Despite the positive changes in graduate trainees
13-week course trainees gradually felt more confi-
self-perception, post-session client reports did not
dent in their skills.
echo these effects; a non-effect that has also been
reported in other research on graduate therapists
Self-awareness and Management Strategies Scales (Boswell et al., 2010). Even so, the lack of recorded
for Therapists. Trainees reports indicated a signifi- change in this study may be due (at least in part) to a
cant improvement (i.e. a decrease) in the hindering ceiling effect. Indeed, clients rated the therapeutic
self-awareness subscale of the SAMS, F(3, 51) alliance as slightly over 4 on a 5-point scale, leaving
6.464, p .001, indicating that they changed in their little room for measurable improvement. This inter-
ability to manage in-session anxiety and intrusive pretation is supported by Goodyear and Guzzardo
self-focused thoughts. Pairwise comparisons indicate (2000), who describe a ceiling effect as possibly
a significant improvement from baseline to the masking graduate training improvements as, say,
subsequent testing point (p B.046) suggesting im- compared to those of undergraduate trainees. Hill
provement in this skill. and Lent (2006) also support this position, observing
Training psychotherapists 217

that students who are accepted into graduate training concern of randomly assigning volunteer clients to
programmes tend to already have good communica- working with a nave, not-in-training group.
tion skills. Furthermore, it was logistically impossible at our
Alternatively, from a developmental view, the university to find a course that could serve as a
inconsistency between measured perspectives (trai- control or suitable comparison group, given that
nees reported self-perceptions vs. their clients these specialised courses are only available to
reported benefits), might actually reflect different students at determined times in their degrees.
stages of the training process. In other words, Even so, we are encouraged by the triangulation
perhaps when novice therapists feel better oriented, of our findings through other means (i.e. Pascual-
less anxious, and have a clearer grasp of the Leone et al.s qualitative study). Finally, the impact
therapeutic process it may first lead to feeling more of therapist training on client session reports may
confident, but not until later does it translate into have been better appraised following multi-session
observable intervention changes; and not during a interventions. Yet, while this could be seen as a
single session in an introductory course. limitation of the current study, it is interesting to
note that Boswell et al. (2010) also found no effects
in client session reports, even when evaluations
Strengths, limitations, and future directions were taken around session 10 of an ongoing
Given the quasi-experimental design used in this treatment.
paper we were unable to directly test the use of As a quasi-experimental study of training effects,
process measures as a strategy for sensitising trainees our design also had a number of strengths. First, this
to key in-session phenomena. However, the study study set up several formal sessions with volunteer
does offer some preliminary findings regarding the clients seeking help for real life difficulties of a mild-
delivery of such training in a coherent and clinically to-moderate nature. This provided a realistic clinical
relevant fashion. It is also interesting to note that, experience for trainees and allowed for a more
despite a quasi-experimental design, many of these ecologically valid test of training effects as compared
findings on professional development have received to the many studies that use classmates-as-clients in
some corroboration through other research methods mock counselling sessions or teach directly to the
using the same training sample. In a qualitative study test (for a review of these problems see Hill & Lent,
(Pascual-Leone, Wolfe, & OConnor, 2012; Pascual- 2006). Second, data was collected from multiple
Leone, Rubio-Rodriguez, & Metler, under review), clients (two per time point), from a multiple
participants were asked to write open-ended narra- perspective (i.e. both counsellors and clients eva-
tives about what they perceived as the personal luations), and at multiple (four) time points. This
impact of this course. Results of that inquiry were multiplicity is relatively unique among studies on
strikingly consistent with our current findings and training (Hill et al., 2008, is a notable exception).
trainees attributed their changes to the training Plotting a learning curve using multiple perspectives
course (rather than historical or maturational fac- is also important because, as we have seen, trainees
tors). Nevertheless, the current study does not offer confidence and self-efficacy do not always reflect
a controlled comparison group. better skills performance from the clients perspec-
While it should be encouraging that our training tive. Moving forward, future research will require:
and study was conducted in the framework of a first a design that links process (of perceptual acuity)
classroom setting, suggesting that it is easily trans- to outcome (of training), and second a dismantling
portable, the primary purpose of the setting was study, to evaluate the benefit of adding this as a
educational rather than research. This is the training component.
principal reason why it was not possible to have
the same sorts of controls one might expect from a
Implications for practice
laboratory setting. The reasons for not having a
randomised no-training control group are similarly The most central implication of this work is that it
documented by Hill et al. (2008) in their own introduces a new and feasible strategy for training
research. Namely, it was not possible to find psychotherapists, one which capitalises on existing
comparable classes in which students would agree research tools by repurposing them for a broader
to complete measures and participate in formal aim. Until recently, being well versed in psychother-
counselling sessions; not to mention the ethical apy process measures has been somewhat of a
218 A. Pascual-Leone & C. Andreescu

narrow specialisation. However, applying process Hatcher, R.L., & Gillaspy, J.A. (2006). Development and valida-
measures to transcript, audio, or video records of tion of a revised short version of the Working Alliance
Inventory. Psychotherapy Research, 16 (1), 1225.
therapy is coming to be increasingly viewed as a Hill, C.E., Roffman, M., Stahl, J., Friedman, S., Hummel, A., &
potentially fruitful strategy for training psychothera- Wallace, C. (2008). Helping skills training for undergraduates:
pists (i.e. Hilsenroth, 2007). While most of the Outcomes and prediction of outcomes. Journal of Counseling
measures we have recommended were designed Psychology, 55 (3), 359370.
with the original intention of exploring specific Hill, C.E., & Lent, R.W. (2006). A narrative and meta-analytic
review of helping skills training: Time to revive a dormant area
research questions, they also represent a relatively
of inquiry. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
jargon-free operationalisation of key constructs, 43 (2), 154172.
often offering intuitive descriptions that are highly Hilsenroth, M.J., Defife, J.A., Blagys, M.D., & Ackerman, S.J.
accessible to novice therapists. Even more impor- (2006). Effects of training in short-term psychodynamic
tantly, this is an efficient way of overcoming a psychotherapy: Changes in graduate clinical technique.
Psychotherapy Research, 16 (3), 293305.
trainees limitations in perceptual acuity with respect
Hilsenroth, M.J. (2007). A programmatic study of short-term
to productive therapeutic process, which is a largely psychodynamic psychotherapy: Assessment, process, outcome,
unaddressed obstacle for fledgling therapists learn- and training. Psychotherapy Research, 17 (1), 3145.
ing to use interventions. Thus, rather than relying Horvath, A.O., & Greenberg, L.S. (1989). Development and
on the gradual accumulation of clinical exposure validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of
over many years, trainees can directly harvest from Counseling Psychology, 36, 223233.
Klein, M.H., Mathieu-Coughlan, P., & Kiesler, D.J. (1986). The
the clinical clarity developed by psychotherapy
experiencing scales. In L.S. Greenberg & W.M. Pinsof (Eds.),
researchers through process research tools that are The psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook (pp. 2171).
readily available. Finally, as we have demonstrated, New York: Guilford Press.
this approach could be easily integrated into Lambert, M.J., Harmon, C., Slade, K., Whipple, J.L., &
common training courses of various therapeutic Hawkins, E.J. (2005). Providing feedback to psychotherapists
on their patients progress: Clinical results and practice
approaches.
suggestions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61 (2), 165174.
Lane, R.D. (1991). LEAS scoring manual & glossary. Tucson, AZ:
University of Arizona Health Sciences Center.
References Lent, R.W., Hill, C.E., & Hoffman, M.A. (2003). Development
Angus, L., Levitt, H., & Hartke, K. (1999). The narrative and validation of the counselor activity self-efficacy scales.
processes coding system: Research applications and implica- Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50 (1), 97108.
tions for psychotherapy practice. Journal of Clinical Psychology, Levitt, H.M. (2001). Sounds of silence in psychotherapy: The
55 (10), 12551270. categorization of clients pauses. Psychotherapy Research, 11,
Boswell, J.F., Castonguay, G.L., & Wasserman, R.H. (2010). 295309.
Effects of psychotherapy training and intervention use on Luborsky, L., Popp, C., Luborsky, E., & Mark, D. (1994). The
session outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, core conflictual relationship theme. Psychotherapy Research, 4
78 (5), 717723. (3&4), 172183.
Castonguay, G.L., Schut, J.A., Aikins, E.D., Constantino, J.M., Paivio, S.C., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). Emotion focused therapy
Laurenceau, P.J., Bologh, L., & Burns, D.D. (2004). Integra- for complex trauma: An integrative approach. Washington, DC:
tive cognitive therapy for depression: A preliminary investiga- American Psychological Association.
tion. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 14 (1), 420. Pachankis, J.E., & Goldfried, M.R. (2007). On the next genera-
Elliott, R. (1993). Revised Session Reaction Scale. Retrieved tion of process research. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 760
February 5, 2010, from http://www.experiential-researchers. 768.
org/instruments/elliott/rsrs.html Pascual-Leone, A., Wolfe, B., & OConnor, D. (2012). The
Elliott, R., & Wexler, M.M. (1994). Measuring the impact of reported impact of psychotherapy training: Undergraduate
sessions in process-experiential therapy of depression: The disclosures after a course in experiential psychotherapy.
session impacts scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41 (2), Journal of Person Centered and Experiential Psychotherapy, 11,
166174. 152168.
Goodyear, R.K., & Guzzardo, C.R. (2000). Psychotherapy super- Pascual-Leone, A., Rubio-Rodriguez, B., & Metler, S. (under
vision and training. In S. Brown & R.W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook review). Graduate students experiences in psychotherapy training.
of counseling psychology (3rd ed., pp. 83108). New York: Wiley. Unpublished manuscript. University of Windsor.
Greenberg, L.S., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Emotion in Pennebaker, J.W. (1997). Writing about emotional experiences as
psychotherapy: A practice-friendly research review. Journal of a therapeutic process. Psychological Science, 8, 162166.
Clinical Psychology: In Session, 62, 611630. Reeker, J., Elliott, R., & Ensing, D. (1996). Measuring session
Hatcher, R.L., & Barends, A.W. (1996). Patients view of the effects in process-experiential and cognitive-behavioral therapies of
alliance in psychotherapy: Exploratory factor analysis of three PTSD: The Revised Session Reaction Scale. Paper presented at
alliance measures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, the Society for Psychotherapy Research, Amelia Island, FL,
64, 13261336. USA.
Training psychotherapists 219
Rice, L.N., & Kerr, G.P. (1986). Measures of client and therapist Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 40 (4),
vocal quality. In L.S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsoff (Eds.), The 278288.
psychotherapeutic process: A research handbook (pp. 73105).
New York: Guilford Press. Biographies
Ronnestad, M.H., & Ladany, N. (2006). The impact of psy-
chotherapy training: Introduction to the special section. Antonio Pascual-Leone, PhD, is a psychologist
Psychotherapy Research, 16 (3), 261267. and professor. He has published on psychotherapy
Safran, D.J., Samstag, W.L., Muran, C.J., & Stevens, C. (2001). process and outcomes, with a special focus on
Repairing alliance ruptures. Psychotherapy Research, 28 (4), emotional processing. He recently co-authored a
406412.
book on Emotion Focused Therapy for Complex Trauma
Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526537.
(APA). Dr Pascual-Leone runs a private practice in
Stiles, W.B. (1980). Measurement of the impact of psychotherapy Windsor seeing individuals and couples.
sessions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 176185. Cristina Andreescu, MA, is a doctoral candidate
van Deurzen-Smith, E. (1996). The future of psychotherapy in in clinical psychology. Her areas of specialisation
Europe. International Journal of Psychotherapy, 1 (1), 1521. include clinical skills training and the treatment of
Williams, E.N., Hurley, K., OBrien, K., & De Gregorio, A.
eating disorders. She has also done work in occupa-
(2003). Development and validation of the self-awareness and
management strategies (SAMS) scales for therapists.
tional rehabilitation.
Copyright of Counselling & Psychotherapy Research is the property of Routledge and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

S-ar putea să vă placă și