Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

Journal of Experimental Psychology

1965, Vol. 70, No. 1, 32-42

HEART RATE DURING CONDITIONING IN HUMANS:


EFFECTS OF UCS INTENSITY, VAGAL BLOCKADE,
AND ADRENERGIC BLOCK OF
VASOMOTOR ACTIVITY *
PAUL A. OBRIST, DONALD M. WOOD, AND MARIO PEREZ-REYES
School of Medicine, University of North Carolina

2 experiments concerned with the influence of the vagal and sym-


pathetic innervations of the heart and of peripheral reflex mechanisms
are reported on the deceleration of heart rate observed on test trials
during classical conditioning in human Ss. An increase in vagal re-
straint was observed to be the basis of the deceleratory response, which
in turn masks the manifestation of sympathetic acceleratory effects.
The latter were only observed when the vagus was pharmacologically
blocked and when a very intense UCS was used. When the conditioned
pressor responses were blocked pharmacologically, as evaluated by direct
recordings of arterial blood pressure, the deceleratory response was not
changed, indicating that the peripheral homeostatic reflex mechanisms
are not the basis for this response and that it is likely a conditioned
response.

The physiological basis of heart- biphasic response characterized by an


rate changes in human 5s during initial acceleration followed by a
classical conditioning was investigated usually more sustained deceleration
in order to determine the influence (Deane, 1964; Fuhrer, 1964; Geer,
of the vagal and sympathetic innerva- 1964; Lang & Hnatiow, 1962; Notter-
tions of the heart and of peripherally man, Schoenfeld, & Bersh, 1952;
initiated homeostatic reflexes. The Wilson, 1964; Wood & Obrist, 1964;
heart-rate changes regarded as condi- Zeaman, Deane, & Wegner, 1954;
tioned, i.e., those changes occurring Zeaman & Smith, 1964; Zeaman &
either between CS and UCS onset or Wegner, 1957). Recently, Wood and
for a more sustained period following Obrist (1964) have demonstrated
CS onset on nonreinforced trials, are that the initial acceleratory component
in certain respects paradoxical and of the biphasic response is a respira-
subject to the question of whether tory artifact, which leaves only the
they are conditioned responses or deceleration as a possible conditioned
only artifacts of the conditioning response.
process. When a noxious UCS is used, Two questions now arise. First,
such responses are reported to be why is there no sympathetic accelera-
either a deceleration of heart rate or a tory response since, under these con-
1 ditions, such a sympathetic effect
These studies were supported principally
by Research Grants M-6020-A and MH- would be expected and was observed
07995, National Institutes of Health, United as measured by GSR and vasomotor
States Public Health Service, and by faculty responses? There is evidence that
grants from the University of North Carolina. vagal inhibition, manifested as a
A portion of this paper was read at the deceleration of heart rate, is able to
Society for Psychophysiological Research
meetings, October 1964. The technical mask sympathetic activity (Rushmer,
assistance of James Howard and Norma 1958; Samaan, 1934-35a). There-
Thomas is gratefully acknowledged. fore, the purpose of the first of two
32
HUMAN HEART RATE DURING CONDITIONING 33

experiments to be reported was to EXPERIMENT I


determine whether sympathetic effects Method
are present during conditioning but Subjects.The Ss were 60 healthy male
are not manifested due to the domi- undergraduate students randomly selected
nance of vagal restraint. For this from the introductory psychology class.
purpose, heart-rate changes during Apparatus.Methods for measuring heart
rate, respiration, and skin resistance have been
conditioning in 5s with an intact vagus previously described (Obrist, Hallman, &
are compared to those with vagal Wood, 1964; Wood & Obrist, 1964). Addi-
activity pharmacologically blocked. tionally, R-R interval time was measured to
Additionally, two intensities of the the nearest .001 sec. on a sequence event timer
UCS were used to determine whether and recorder (SETAR; Welford, 1952) and
recorded on punch tape for computer analysis.
sympathetic effects become more pro- Procedure.There were four experimental
nounced with a far more intense UCS conditions with 5s assigned randomly to each.
than was previously used (Wood & This provided a comparison of the effects of
Obrist, 1964). It was anticipated vagal and nonvagal blockade at two in-
tensities of the UCS. Vagal blockade was
that a more intense UCS would result achieved by an IV injection of atropine sulfate
in an observable sympathetic effect (.02 mg/kg of body weight) just prior to the
even with the vagus intact, due to the first conditioning trial. The dosage was
possibility of greater fear or anxiety- derived from pilot studies. The 5s not given
producing qualities of the stimulus atropine received IV 5 cc of saline but were
told that it was atropine. Eleven 5s were
(Martin, 1961). The conditioned eliminated from the experiment prior to data
GSR was also evaluated to demon- analysis either because they could not relate
strate the presence of sympathetic correctly which CS was followed by shock, or
activity. the atropine in part or whole was administered
subdermally. Therefore, the final N was:
A second question concerned whether saline, less intense UCS, N = 12; vagal block,
the deceleratory response is a condi- less intense UCS, N = 11; saline, more intense
tioned response or rather is arti- UCS, N = 13; and vagal block, more intense
factual, resulting from homeostatic UCS, N = 13.
reflex mechanisms initiated by a 1. UCS intensity. The intensity of the
UCS was adjusted for each S as follows. All
conditioned pressor response. The 5s were given a 9-point scale extending from
use of a noxious UCS as well as the "just felt" to "very painful" by which to
evidence of conditioned vasomotor judge a series of shocks. They were then
responses indicate that such pressor given an ascending series of 6-sec., 60-cycle
shocks spaced 30 sec. apart and administered
responses may occur. Further, the to the finger tips of the left hand through two
presence of such reflex influences on dry S-mm. diameter silver EEC electrodes.
heart rate is well documented (Neil At random intervals, the intensity was de-
& Heymans, 1962), but their signifi- creased one step to determine the reliability
of 5's judgments. At the point where the
cance during conditioning has not shock was first judged as "very painful," the
been determined. Therefore, the series was stopped. For 5s in the low-in-
second experiment reported was to tensity UCS groups, this was the level to be
used during conditioning and 5 was so in-
determine whether the deceleratory formed. This intensity approximated the
response was still observed once level used in the Wood and Obrist (1964)
pressor responses of vasomotor origin study. For 5s in the high-intensity UCS
groups, JB entered the 5 room following the
were pharmacologically blocked. In "very painful" judgment and read an appeal
order to evaluate the effectiveness of for 5 to take considerably more shock, which
the pharmacological agent, direct was defined to 5 as "as much as you can
humanly stand." Thus, an appeal was made
recordings of both diastolic and sys- to his masculinity in addition to offering him
tolic blood pressure were obtained. a substantial financial bonus. After the first
34 P. A. OBRIST, D. M. WOOD, AND M. PEREZ-REYES

live conditioning trials, these 5s were again yses were done for each experimental condi-
asked to take one further increase (.3 ma.), tion except adaptation. Here the saline and
but most refused. The average amount of vagal blockade groups were combined for each
current for the less and more intense condi- level of UCS intensity since atropine had not
tions was 1.88 and 3.86 ma., respectively yet been administered. This increased the N
(t = 10.61, p < .001). to 23 in one group and 25 in the other. As in
2. Respiratory control. Respiration was the previous study (Wood & Obrist, 1964),
controlled throughout conditioning by re- heart-rate changes, either acceleratory or
quiring 5 to maintain his normal resting deceleratory, were not related to base level.
frequency and depth so as to minimize the Therefore, no correction for base level was
influence of respiratory effects on heart-rate used.
changes in 5s not receiving vagal block. 2. Respiration. A mean change score was
These procedures are similar to those previ- calculated as a ratio of the pre-CS average
ously reported (Wood & Obrist, 1964), with respiratory amplitude to the single largest
one exception. Respiratory activity was inspiration during the first 6, next 9, and last
paced by an auditory signal which mimicked 10 sec. following CS onset. This was calcu-
human breathing sounds. This signal could lated separately for test, sensitization, and
be adjusted with respect to both inspiration UCS trials. This method of quantification
and expiration time as well as the pause be- is more completely described elsewhere (Wood
tween each so as to follow exactly any one 5's & Obrist, 1964).
resting pattern. 3. GSR. Conditioned GSR activity was
3. Conditioning procedures. A 10-min. quantified for test and sensitization control
rest period, training for respiratory control, trials by first obtaining the highest resistance
UCS intensity determination and a 10-trial level (ohms) up through 2.0 sec. following CS
adaptation series preceded, in that order, onset and the lowest resistance level occurring
differential, trace conditioning procedures. between 2 and 10 sec. after CS onset. In
During adaptation each CS was presented cases where there were two or more responses
five times in a fixed random order. Condi- having a 600-ohm or greater change and a
tioning consisted of a series of 36 trials, with recovery of at least 600 ohms, the lowest level
16 reinforced, 10 test, and 10 sensitization was obtained by totaling the magnitude of all
control trials, presented in a fixed random such responses and subtracting this from the
order. Short rest periods were given after highest level. These two levels were then
every 12 trials. Test trials were used so as averaged for each S separately for test and
to determine the heart-rate changes during sensitization control trials and then con-
the period when the UCS would normally verted to conductance units. The difference
occur, since the maximum deceleration occurs between these two-level scores was then
during this time (Wood & Obrist, 1964). The obtained as the measure of GSR magnitude
CS+ and CS were dim red and blue lights and then averaged for only the two experi-
with a duration of 2 sec. The 6-sec. UCS mental groups not receiving the vagal block-
started 7 sec. after CS onset. The intertrial ade, since atropine blocks GSR activity. This
interval averaged 75 sec. with a range of 60 to resulted in an N too small to correct for base-
90 sec. These intervals were varied in a fixed level effects by regression analysis (Lacey &
random order. Except during instructions, Laccy, 1962).
a low-level white masking noise was used.
Respiratory control was continuous excepl
during rest periods. The 5s were instructed Results
that only one of the lights would be followed
by shock and then only some of the time. No significant differences were ob-
Data quantification,The following are served in the second-by-second heart-
data quantification procedures. rate changes between the CS+ and
1. Heart rate. Second-by-second changes CS during adaptation. Therefore,
in R-R interval from a pre-CS base line for the sensitization control (CS ) was
each of the 25 sec., following CS onset were
averaged separately for the 5 CS+ and 5 used as a base line from which heart-
CS adaptation trials, 10 test, 10 sensitiza- rate changes on nonreinforced test
tion control, and 6 of the UCS trials, using trials and UCS trials could be evalu-
procedures previously described (Wood & ated. A direct comparison of the
Obrist, 1964). This period was used in order
to observe long latency sympathetic effects heart-rate changes between vagal
(see Dykman & Gantt, 1959). These anal- blockade and nonvagal blockade
HUMAN HEART RATE DURING CONDITIONING 35

groups was prohibited by the very without vagal blockade showed a


large difference in variance between biphasic response. There was an
groups. Base-level heart rate is initial acceleration extending from
greatly elevated by vagal blockade, Sec. 2-6, the effect being significant
resulting in a reduction of responsive- on Sec. 3 and 4. This was followed
ness. However, since the principal by a more sustained deceleration
effects of vagal blockade were expected extending from Sec. 7-15, the effect
to be a shift in the direction of the being significant on Sec. 8 and 9.
response, that is, from deceleration to On the other hand, when the vagus
acceleration, an evaluation of the was blocked, only an acceleratory
reliability of heart-rate changes within response was observed on test trials
each experimental group makes direct with the exception of the first second
comparisons unnecessary. (see Fig. 1 and Table 1). The
The deceleration of heart rate acceleration was significant on Sec. 4-
during conditioning was found to be 10, peaking at the exact second that
the result of an increase in vagal the deceleration peaked when the
restraint which masked sympathetic vagus was not blocked, i.e., Sec. 9.
effects. This result was most obvious There was a second less pronounced
with the more intense UCS con- acceleration which began around Sec.
dition. The high-intensity UCS group 16 and peaked on Sec. 21, the ampli-

A. Without Vagal Blockade

less Intense UCS


more intense UCS

"I
"I -15-
-IO-
B. With Vagal Blockade

-5-
O
+5-
+10-
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 II 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25
Time From CS Onset (seconds)
ONSET

FIG. 1. Heart-rate changes expressed as the difference between test and sensitization
control trials with and without vagal blockade at two intensities of the UCS.
36 P. A. OBRIST, D. W. WOOD, AND M. PEREZ-REYES

TABLE 1
SECOND BY SECOND CHANGES IN R-R INTERVAL (MILLISECONDS) EXPRESSED AS
TEST MINUS SENSITIZATION CONTROL TRIAL MEAN DIFFERENCES

Low-Intensity UCS High-Intensity UCS

Sec. since No Vagal Blockade Vagal Blockade No Vagal Blockade Vagal Blockade
CS Onset N = 12 N = 11 N - 13 N 13

X t X i X / X t
1 0 <1 + 2 1.17 0 <1 + 4 3.49**
2 0 <1 + 2 <1 -10 1.34 -1 <1
3 -11 <1 + 4 1.07 -19 2.79* -1 <1
4 -22 1.47 1
<1 -24 2.54* -5 2.30*
S -20 1.25 - 1 <1 -21 1.70 7 2.45*
6 + 4 <1 - 1 <1 -11 1.25 -11 2.69*
7 + 8 <1 + 1 <1 + 17 1.76 -11 2.89*
8 +21 1.46 + 2 <1 +21 2.50* -13 2.53*
9 + 50 4.01** + 3 <1 +23 2.78* -IS 2.85*
10 +50 3.20** + 6 1.88 + 16 1.38 -12 2.40*
11 +39 2.87* + 9 2.72* +21 1.60 -11 2.00
12 +37 2.94* + 8 2.28* +20 1.51 -8 1.66
13 +28 2.31* + 10 3.55** +22 1.55 -8 1.92
14 +20 1.70 + 6 2.58* + 18 1.15 -5 1.32
15 -5 <1 + 5 2.15 + 5 <1 -5 1.41
16 -20 1.93 + 4 1.92 _ 4 <1 -6 1.50
17 -14 1.34 - 1 <1 - 5 <1 - 5 1.30
18 -4 <1 + 1 <1 + 10 1.12 -8 1.93
19 - 5 <1 - 2 <1 +9 1.41 -7 1.62
20 + 1 <1 0 <1 +4 <1 -10 2.02
21 + 12 <1 0 <1 -5 <1 -12 2.40*
22 + 11 <1 + 1 <1 -7 <1 -11 2.18*
23 + 14 <1 + 1 <1 -5 <1 -9 1.62
24 -13 1.16 + 3 <1 -6 <1 -11 1.83
25 -19 1.23 + 6 1.23 -4 <1 -8 1.33

Note. (- =longer R-R interval or heart-rate deceleration; = shorter R-R interval or heart-rate acceleration.
* i> < .05.
**t < .01.

tude being significant on both Sec. 21 tude of the acceleration is positively


and 22. correlated (rho = .62, p < .05) with
The acceleration on Sec. 2-6 after the largest single inspiration during
CS onset observed with the high- the period of acceleration, the latter
intensity UCS with the vagus intact showing an 8% increase (t = 2.02,
appears to be a momentary decrease p < .10). In contrast, during vagal
in vagal restraint rather than a blockade, there was no correlation
sympathetic effect. First, it has a between the magnitude of the ac-
shorter latency and peaks consider- celeration and respiratory amplitude.
ably sooner than the sympathetic Therefore, even with a very intense
effect observed with the vagus blocked. UCS, there is little evidence that
Such a latency difference has been sympathetic effects are manifested
previously proposed bySamaan (1934- when the vagal innervation is intact.
35b) and Dykman and Gantt (1959). A reliable deceleratory response was
Second, this acceleration appears to also found on test trials with the less
be significantly influenced by uncon- intense UCS without vagal blockade,
trolled respiratory effects. The ampli- extending from Sec. 6-14 (see Fig. 1
HUMAN HEART RATE DURING CONDITIONING 37

and Table 1) and peaking at Sec. 9, TABLE 2


as in the higher UCS intensity condi- SECOND-BY-SECOND DIFFERENCE IN R-R
tion. In contrast, little responsive- INTERVAL (MILLISECONDS) BETWEEN THE
ness was observed in the lower in- LOWER AND HIGHER UCS INTENSITIES
tensity UCS condition with vagal DETERMINED FROM TEST MINUS
SENSITIZATION CONTROL TRIAL
blockade. A small deceleration, sig- MEAN DIFERENCES
nificant on Sec. 11-14, suggests that
there is little sympathetic involvement Without Vagal With Vagal
at this UCS intensity, at least with Blockade Blockade
Sec.
respect to the heart. This latter
deceleration appears due to a reduc- * / * t
tion in the amount of vagal blockade 1 0 <! -2 <1
during the later test trials which 2 -10 <1 -3 <1
3 - 7 <1 -4 1.25
allows some vagal inhibition to be- 4 -2 <1 -4 1.36
come manifested. An analysis of S - 1 <1 -6 1.68
heart-rate changes was performed 6 -IS 1.07 -10 2.03
7 + 9 <1 -12 2.49*
using only the first two test and 8 0 <1 -IS 2.32*
sensitization control trials, when vagal 9 -27 1.83 -18 2.80**
10 -34 1.80 -18 2.91**
blockade is at its maximum as meas- 11 -18 <1 -19 3.00**
ured by basal levels of heart rate. 12 -17 <1 -16 2.65*
This revealed no reliable response of 13 -6 <1 -18 3.49**
14 -2 <1 -10 2.39*
any kind, although the mean second- IS +10 <1 - 9 2.22*
by-second effects are acceleratory 16 -16 1.26 - 9 2.09*
from the sixth second on. This is in 17 -9 <1 - 4 <1
18 -14 <1 - 8 1.74
contrast to a large deceleratory re- 19 -14 1.56 - 6 1.08
sponse, which was already significant 20 + 3 <1 -10 1.67
21 -16 1.14 -12 1.90
on 1 sec. in the low-UCS-intensity 22 -18 1.05 -12 1.77
group without vagal blockade during 23 -20 1.35 9 1.34
the same trials. 24 -8 <1 -14 1.83
25 -IS <1 -14 1.77
The lower and higher UCS intensity
conditions without vagal blockade Note. = higher intensity, shorter R-R interval
were not reliably differentiated on any or faster heart rate.
*P <.OS.
of the 25 sec. following CS onset **P <.01.
(see Table 2). Only during the
deceleratory phase of the response to decrease the duration of the un-
are any appreciable differences ob- conditioned response. In all condi-
served. But this is only a difference tions, a reliable acceleration of heart
in degree of vagal restraint. On the rate occurred to the UCS which was
other hand, when the vagus is blocked significantly greater for the more
the two conditions are sharply dif- intense UCS both with and without
ferentiated, with the higher intensity vagal blockade. This acceleration
condition having a significantly greater was significant (p < .05 or less) with
acceleratory response on Sec. 7-16 the vagus intact on 3 sec. for the less
(see Table 2). Therefore, differences intense UCS and on 8 sec. for the
in UCS intensity result in different more intense UCS. On the other
degrees of sympathetic activity, while hand, with the vagus blocked the
having a less pronounced effect on acceleration was significant on 8 and
vagal activity. 14 sec. for these respective conditions.
Vagal restraint was also observed Furthermore, reliable differences be-
38 P. A. OBRIST, D. M. WOOD, AND M. PEREZ-REYES

tween the acceleratory responses to similar to those reported in Exp. I. The UCS
the two UCS intensities were observed was set at the less intense level. Thirteen test
trials were used. Because pressor responses
on only 3 sec. with the vagus intact, can also be initiated by respiratory-induced
but on 12 sec. with the vagus blocked. cardiac acceleration, special efforts were made
Sympathetic activity was mani- to keep respiratory activity controlled in-both
fested at both UCS intensities as conditioning sessions. During the second
measured by GSR activity. A greater session, sensitization control trials were
omitted for five 5s prior to drug administra-
response on test than on sensitization tion and for eight 5s following drug adminis-
control trials was observed in 11 of 12 tration in order to expedite proceedings
5s with the less intense UCS and in necessitated by the experiment. Therefore,
all 5s with the more intense UCS. heart-rate changes were determined for all 5s
There was some evidence of greater without correction for sensitization effects.
In order to evaluate the effectiveness of the
responsiveness with the more intense adrenergic blockade, direct readings of
UCS, though no reliable differences arterial blood pressure were attempted in the
could be demonstrated. The average last eight 5s and successfully obtained in five.
Pressure was measured from the radial artery
number of measurable responses (i.e., near the wrist of the right arm. The puncture
those greater than 600 ohms) on the was done under local anesthesia (Xylocaine)
10 test trials was 7.8 for the less using a No. 18 cordon needle. The needle
intense UCS and 9.6 for the more was attached to a strain guage by a catheter
intense UCS. Similarly, the mean filled with heparin saline solution for flushing.
response amplitude was greater with fiedData quantifications.Data were quanti-
as in the previous study. Both diastolic
with the more intense UCS, but the and systolic blood pressure were quantified on
difference was not reliable (Mann- a second by second basis, as with heart rate.
Whitney U > .10). Therefore, the Heart rate and blood pressure changes on the
failure of sympathetic effects to be first three test trials prior to drug administra-
tion were not quantified because 5s were
manifested by heart rate with the usually less responsive on these trials. This
less intense UCS cannot be attributed left five test trials before, and five after drug
to the absence of such effects. administration to be quantified.

EXPERIMENT II Results
Method There was no evidence to indicate
Subjects.The 5s were 11 healthy young- that the deceleration of heart rate was
adult male student volunteers preselected on the result of reflexes initiated by a
the basis of having shown an appreciable
deceleratory response during a previous condi- pressor response. There was initially
tioning session, a condition considered neces- observed in all 5 5s on whom measure-
sary to detect clearly the influence of homeo- ments were obtained a small pressor
static effects. response which had a mean peak
Equipment.The same equipment was response of 1.9 mm. Hg systolic and
used as in Exp. I in addition to a Statham
075 cm. Hg strain gauge for the direct 1.2 mm. Hg diastolic (see Table 3).
recording of arterial blood pressure. Adrenergic blockade reduced the re-
Procedure.Two conditioning sessions were sponse in all 5s, with the mean peak
required and were separated by at least 1 wk. response being 0.3 mm. Hg, systolic
The first was used only to preselect 5s. Dur-
ing the second, pressor responses of vasomotor and diastolic. On the other hand, a
origin were blocked pharmacologically after reliably large sustained deceleration
the eighth test trial by an IV injection of 0.6 of heart rate was observed both be-
mg. of Hydergine-Sandoz, the maximum rec- fore and after adrenergic blockade
ommended clinical dosage. (This agent has (see Table 4). Based on all 11 5s,
alpha-receptor adrenergic blocking character-
istics and is a combination of three ergot there were clearly no reliable differ-
alkaloids.) Conditioning procedures were ences between the deceleratory re-
HUMAN HEART RATE DURING CONDITIONING 39

TABLE 3 pressure were obtained. However,


SECOND-BY-SECOND MEAN BLOOD PRESSURE because of the small N, the reliability
LEVELS (MM. HG) ON TEST TRIALS BEFORE of this effect was not evaluated.
AND FOLLOWING ADRENERGIC
BLOCKADE (N = 5) The independence of pressor re-
sponses and heart-rate changes is
Systolic Diastolic indicated by still other aspects of the
Sec. data. First, there were trials even
Preblock Postblock Preblock Postblock prior to adrenergic blockade when
2 Pre a
121.9 114.6 73.8 70.1
heart rate was observed to decelerate
1 121.5 114.5 73.7 69.9 even when no pressor response oc-
2 121.4 114.3 73.7 70.4 curred. Second, in one S, the pressor
3 121.9 114.3 74.5 70.2 response was completely suppressed
4 123.0 114.8 74.9 70.4
5 123.2 114.9 75.0 70.4 following blockade but the decelera-
6 123.3 114.9 74.9 69.9 tory response was unaltered. Third,
7 123.6 114.8 75.0 70.3
8 123.8 114.8 74.3 69.7 no correlation could be found between
9 122.8 114.6 73.4 69.3 the amplitude of the heart-rate de-
10 121.9 112.4 73.1 69.1 celeration and the amplitude of either
11 122.0 112.8 72.6 69.3
12 120.9 113.1 72.4 68.8 the systolic or the diastolic response,
13 120.8 113.4 72.2 68.8 when these correlations were per-
14 119.6 113.2 72.1 69.0
IS 120.9 112.9 72.7 69.6 formed within 5s using responses on
test trials. There was a reduction
!
x Pre Mean of all readings in the 15 sec. before in the deceleratory response in one 5.
CS onset.
However, this appears attributable
sponses pre- and postblockade. A to the effects of 5's knowing when he
comparable effect was found with the received the adrenergic blocking agent
5 5s on whom measurements of blood (all 5s had been informed of the pos-
TABLE 4
SECOND-BY-SECOND CHANGES IN R-R INTERVAL (MILLISECONDS) ON TEST TRIALS
BEFORE AND FOLLOWING ADRENERGIC BLOCKADE EXPRESSED AS DIFFERENCE
FROM PRE-CS BASE LEVEL (N = 11)

Preblockade Postblockade Difference Post Pre


Sec.
X t X t "x (
1 + 6 <1 + 7 1.37 + 1 <1
2 + 12 <1 -4 <1 -16 <1
3 -10 <1 + 3 <1 + 13 1.20
4 + 2 <1 + 16 <1 + 14 <1
5 + 9 <1 + 9 <1 0 <1
6 + 12 1.08 + 19 1.31 + 7 <1
7 +33 3.30** +40 2.39* + 7 <1
8 +41 4.24** +59 4.00** + 18 <1
9 +45 4.43** +65 4.84** +20 1.24
10 +52 4.95** +46 3.83** -6 <1
11 +47 4.08** +42 2.81* - 5 <1
12 +60 4.29** +49 4.63** -11 1.09
13 +43 4.66** ' +38 3.91** - 5 <1
14 +31 3.11** +26 2.17* - 5 <1
15 +29 3.03** +24 2.37* - 5 <1

Note. 1- =longer R-R interval or heart-rate deceleration: =shorter R-R interval or heart-rate acceleration,
* p < .05.
40 P. A. OBRIST, D. M. WOOD, AND M. PEREZ-REYES

sible postexperimental side effects of the pressor response to the shock while
this drug, which can include hypo- the UCS would involve CNS activity
tensively induced nausea and faint- which slows heart rate. The activity
ing). In this S, a similar reduction of the latter is only manifested on test
in the deceleratory response was trials when respiratory and other effects
of the UCS are minimal. This would
observed to a saline injection on a mean that in the absence of pressor
third conditioning session. However, responses, as following adrenergic block-
no reduction in the response was ade, the CS is able to initiate anticipatory
observed to the adrenergic blocking restraining activity. That such a peri-
agent when it was then administered pheral response might be an effective
following the saline and when the UCS is suggested by data reported by
deceleration had returned to its Bykov (1957) where pharmacological
original amplitude. agents were used to initiate the response.
It is also reported that conditioning of
DISCUSSION this type requires from 20 to 100 rein-
forcements. However, in the present
These data indicate that the heart- study, a reliable deceleratory response
rate changes observed on test trials dur- was observed after only four reinforce-
ing classical conditioning under these ments, a fact which argues against this
experimental conditions are primarily position.
manifestations of variations in vagal A second explanation has been sug-
activity and that sympathetic effects gested by Zeaman and Smith (1964),
are only clearly manifested when a very who have observed heart-rate decelera-
intense UCS is used and then only when tion in anticipation of shock and both
the vagal innervation is blocked. Fur- pleasant and unpleasant auditory stimuli,
ther, the more dominant response is an using a variation on standard condition-
increase in vagal restraint (i.e., heart- ing procedures. It is proposed that this
rate deceleration), especially with a less deceleration is mediated first by an
intense UCS, and this response appears attention process initiated by the CS
to be conditioned. Considering the which then initiates respiratory changes
decelerative response as indicative of, resulting in a heart-rate deceleration.
or mediated by, some affective response However, the studies reported by Zea-
such as "experimental anxiety" (Deane, man and Smith do not appear to be
1964; Notterman et al., 1952) is con- definitive with respect to the influence
traindicated by the fact that this is a of respiration and are contrary to evi-
vagal response. Traditionally, sympa- dence from the Wood and Obrist (1964)
thetic activity has been at least implicitly study, as well as data recently reported
assumed to be involved in such affective by Deane (1964). Data from the present
responses. This assumption is supported experiments also contraindicate any in-
by the greater sympathetic involvement fluence of respiration on the decelerative
in the high-shock condition when the response. For example, in both experi-
vagus is blocked, a condition which should ments respiration amplitude was un-
result in a far greater affective response. correlated with the decelerative response
On the other hand, the deceleratory re- and, with the exception of the more
sponse is greater under the low-shock con- intense UCS conditions of Exp. I,
dition, or when the least affective re- remained perfectly controlled. There-
sponse might be found. fore, respiration does not appear to
There are three other ways the mediate whatever influence attention
decelerative heart-rate change might be might have on cardiovascular processes.
viewed. First, it reflects the conditioning A third possibility is suggested by some
of homeostatic processes and as such is recent work of Lacey (Lacey, 1959;
a type of exteroceptive-interoceptive Lacey, Kagan, Lacey, & Moss, 1963;
conditioning. The UCS in this case is Lacey & Lacey, 1958) where it has been
HUMAN HEART RATE DURING CONDITIONING 41

proposed that cardiovascular activity Wood and Obrist (1964) have ques-
influences environmental interaction via tioned the suitability of differential con-
visceral afferent control of central activ- ditioning to assess sensitization effects
ity. In part, this hypothesis is based on and have suggested that the CS might
the observation that various exterocep- be significantly influenced by stimulus
tive stimuli, some with clear affective generalization from the CS + . Data
value, result in both sympathetic dis- from the present study also support this
charge, as measured galvanically, and in possibility. For example, there was no
cardiac deceleration (Davis & Buchwald, heart-rate change to the CS during
1957; Lacey et al, 1963; Obrist, 1963). conditioning for the higher intensity
This deceleration of heart rate is thought UCS saline condition, even though a
to facilitate sensory intake. Therefore, reliable deceleratory response was ob-
the deceleration observed during condi- served in the same condition during
tioning could involve similar processes adaptation and to the CS during
and have a like function. As such, it conditioning for the less intense UCS
would be a conditioned response in saline condition. Respiration did not
anticipation of sensory intake (i.e., the appear to be a significant factor. Also,
UCS). However, there is one aspect of based on the adaptation data, the mag-
the conditioning data which is not nitude of the sensitization effect (i.e.,
consistent with this view and which, influence of UCS intensity) appears to
for that matter, is yet another unresolved be directly related to the magnitude of
paradox of heart-rate conditioning. This the deceleration. Stimulus generaliza-
is, the deceleratory conditioned response tion appears to be a possible basis for
is in the opposite direction from the this lack of response to the CS during
UCR, which is always reported as conditioning since it would be expected
acceleratory, a situation seemingly with- to be greater with the more intense UCS.
out parallel in the conditioning of striate In part, the failure of the initial accelera-
and other visceral responses. Therefore, tory response to differentiate on test
the significance of the deceleratory trials between the less and more intense
response is still not understood. None- UCS when the vagus was intact could
theless, the positions of Lacey and of be attributed to the failure to assess
Zeaman and Smith appear at this time sensitization effects.
to merit further evaluation. Among
other things, the peripheral and central REFERENCES
processes involved with the UCR would BYKOV, K. M. The cerebral cortex and the
appear necessary to study. internal organs. (Trans, by W. H. Gantt)
Recently, Geer (1964) has suggested New York: Chemical Publishing, 1957.
that the deceleratory heart-rate change DAVIS, R. C., & BUCHWALD, A. M. An
observed during conditioning is nothing exploration of somatic response patterns:
more than the orienting response to the Stimulus and sex differences. /. comp.
physiol. Psychol., 1957, SO, 44-52.
CS. This position is not supported by DEANE, G. E. Human heart rate responses
the present study. An assessment of during experimentally induced anxiety:
such effects, based on the heart-rate A follow-up with controlled respiration.
response both to the first adaptation /. exp. Psychol., 1964, 67, 193-195.
trial and to the sensitization control DYKMAN, R. A., & GANTT, W. H. The
for the less intense UCS, reveals a parasympathetic component of unlearned
significant deceleratory response which and acquired cardiac responses. /. comp.
peaks at the fifth second and which has physiol. Psychol., 1959, 52, 163-167.
become a small nonsignificant accelera- FUHRER, M. J. Differential verbal condition-
ing of heart rate with minimization of
tory response by the eighth second. In changes in respiratory rate. /. comp.
contrast, the deceleratory response on physiol. Psychol., 1964, 58, 283-289.
test trials has a longer latency, a larger GEER, J. H. Measurement of the conditioned
amplitude, and a longer duration with cardiac response. /. comp. physiol. Psy-
both intensities of the UCS. chol., 1964, 57, 426-433.
42 P. A. OBRIST, D. M. WOOD, AND M. PEREZ-REYES

LACEY, J.I. Psychophysiological approaches OBRIST, P. A., HALLMAN, S. I., & WOOD,
to the evaluation of psychotherapeutic D. M. Autonomic levels and lability, and
process and outcome. In F. A. Rubenstein performance time on a perceptual task and
& M. G. Parloff (Eds.), Research in a sensory motor task. Percept, mot. Skills,
psychotherapy. Washington, D, C.: Ameri- 1964, 18, 753-762.
can Psychological Association, 1959. Pp. RUSHMER, R. F. Autonomic balance in
160-208. cardiac control. Amer. J. Physiol., 1958,
LACEY, J. I., KAGAN, J., LACEY, B. C., & 192, 631-634.
Moss, H. A. Situational determinants and SAMAAN, A. The antagonistic cardiac nerves
behavioral correlates of autonomic re- and heart rate. /. Physiol., 1934-35, 83,
sponse patterns. In P. J. Knapp (Ed.), 332-340. (a)
Expression of the emotions in man. New SAMAAN, A. Muscular work in dogs sub-
York: International Univer. Press, 1963. mitted to different conditions of cardiac and
Pp. 161-196. splanchnec innervations. /. Physiol.,
LACEY, J. I., & LACEY, B. C. The relationship 1934-35, 85, 313-331. (b)
of resting autonomic activity to motor WELFORD, N. T. An electronic digital record-
impulsivity. Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. ing machine: The SETAR. J. scient.
Dis., 1958, 36, 144-209. Instrum., 1952, 29, 1-4.
LACEY, J. I., & LACEY, B. C. The law of WILSON, R. S. Autonomic changes produced
initial value in the longitudinal study of by noxious and innocuous stimulation.
autonomic constitution : Reproducibility of J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1964, 58, 290-
autonomic responses and response patterns 295.
over a four year interval. Ann. N. Y. WOOD, D. M., & OBRIST, P. A. Effects of
Acad. Sci., 1962, 98, 1257-1290, 1322-1326. controlled and uncontrolled respiration on
LANG, P. J., & HNATIOW, M. Stimulus repeti- the conditioned heart rate response in
tion and heart rate response. /. comp. humans. 7. exp. Psychol., 1964, 68, 221-
physiol. Psychol., 1962, 55, 781-785. 229.
ZEAMAN, D., DEANE, G., & WEGNER, N.,
MARTIN, B. The assessment of anxiety by
Amplitude and latency characteristic of the
physiological-behavioral measures. Psychol.
conditioned heart response. /. Psychol.,
Bull., 1961, 58, 234-255.
1954, 38, 234-250.
NEIL, E., & HEYMANS, C. Cardiovascular ZEAMAN, D., & SMITH, R. W. Review and
and pulmonary reflexes. In A. A. Luisada analysis of some recent findings in human
(Ed.), Cardiovascular functions. New cardiac conditioning. In W. F. Prokasy
York: McGraw-Hill, 1962. Pp. 103-123. (Ed.), Classical conditioning: A symposium.
NOTTERMAN, J., SCHOENFELD, W., & BERSH, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1964.
P. Conditioned heart rate responses in ZEAMAN, D., & WEGNER, N. A further test
human beings during experimental anxiety. of the role of drive reduction in human
/. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1952, 45, 1-8. cardiac conditioning. /. Psychol., 1957, 43,
OBRIST, P. A. Cardiovascular differentiation 125-133.
of sensory stimuli. Psychosom. Med., 1963,
25, 450-458. (Early publication received February 23,1965)

S-ar putea să vă placă și