Sunteți pe pagina 1din 16

Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol. 23, No. 6, pp.

555 570, 1998


1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved
Printed in Great Britain
0306-4530/98 $19.00 + .00
PII: S0306-4530(98)00036-5

INVITED REVIEW

THE PSYCHOLOGY IN PSYCHONEUROENDOCRINOLOGY

Holger Ursin
Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Aarstadveien 21, N-5009 Bergen,
Norway

(Recei6ed 8 July 1998)

SUMMARY
This paper reviews the psychology (P) of psychoneuroendocrinology (PNE). The relationship between the P
factors and the endocrine responses (E) is described within cognitive reformulations of stress theory and
activation theory. The formulations are valid across species, including man. The emphasis is on acquired
expectancies, which are assumed to determine the physiological response, including the E responses. The terms
coping, helplessness, and hopelessness are defined, and related to other labels covering the same or similar
phenomena. The implications of contemporary P theory for the typical PNE experimental situations are
discussed. Finally, the PNE positions outlined in 1968 by Mason is revisited. The 30 years of PNE research that
followed confirm and expand his conclusions. 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

KeywordsStress; Activation; Coping; Helplessness; Hopelessness.

INTRODUCTION
The technical possibilities of identifying and measuring biologically active molecules
increase, almost by the day. There is a corresponding increase in the number of reports
that demonstrate that these new molecules are under control of the central nervous system
(CNS). They are often measured in animals or man under some sort of stress, and then
published as new contributions to psychoneuroendocrinology (PNE). The position of this
paper is that too many of these reports ignore that there has been an increase in our
knowledge and understanding also of psychology. There is more to the psychology (P) bit
of PNE than some sort of stress. The aim of the paper is to furnish the P novice with
information on the P aspects of PNE, to avoid confusion and distress.
The first part of this paper is to present a cognitive approach to stress theory, and
describe a simple model for the relationship between the P factors and the endocrine
responses. The second part will examine the typical experimental situations in PNE, and
suggest nomenclature for identification of the psychological variables that determine the
PNE in the different types of experimental situations. Finally, this paper reviews 30 years
of progress in understanding the P part of PNE, by discussing PNE positions outlined by

Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Holger Ursin, Department of Biological and
Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Aarstadveien 21, N-5009 Bergen, Norway (Tel: 47 55
586227; Fax: + 47 55 589872; E-mail: holger.ursin@psych.uib.no).

555
556 H. Ursin

Mason (1968), one of the PNE pioneers. With this key to 30 years of PNE research, the
P novice should have possibilities to explain why some stimuli are regarded as negative
by some individuals, but positive by others, and why previous experience with the
stimulus, or similar stimuli, determines the endocrine response. In short, why do individu-
als with brains differ from each other? How do we change the noise from individual
variance to meaningful data and PNE music?
The relationship between psychological dimensions and physiological concomitants has
gone through an enormous development during the more than 60 years since Selye
formulated his stress theory. His formulations are historically important and interesting,
but do not cover the field today. The developments in cognitive learning theory, as well as
in neurophysiology (activation theory), psychophysiology (PP), psychoneuroimmunology
(PNI), and PNE have given us a dramatically new understanding of the relationship
between psychology and somatic responses. Old positions within psychosomatic medicine
must be revised and reformulated.
The PP field is a respectable and well developed part of psychology. Contemporary
developments in brain mapping and brain imaging have increased the importance of this
contribution. The PNI is still in its infancy as regards the serious contributions to
psychological theory. The PNE has not received the interest it deserves. Too few
psychologists understand endocrinology (and immunology), too few endocrinologists have
any ideas about what psychology is all about.

COGNITIVE MECHANISMS IN PNE


It seems fair to state that one of the areas that has developed most in psychology in
recent years is cognitive science, in particular cognitive neuroscience. In PNE research, and
in all stress research, the main cognitive factor to be considered is the existence of filters
that evaluate or appraise the stimuli. In a review of the stress concept, Levine and Ursin
(1991) formulated three consensus statements on the relationship between the PP, PNE,
and PNI responses to an external challenge or threat, the stress response. The first was that
if there was anything common to the stimuli that produced the physiological changes
described as stress, this was not the physical characteristics of the stimuli. The second
consensus statement was that psychological, emotional loads were the most frequently
reported stress stimuli. The third consensus statement was that all stimuli are evaluated or
filtered by the brain. These three factors produce the individual difference, and the P part
of PNE is to understand the principles for this variance.
The coping filter is the best understood and best developed concept in the stress
litterature, and, also, in many related disciplines (Carpenter, 1992). The concept is found
in PNE, PNI, and PP, in animals and in man (Toates, 1995), as well as in clinical
psychology (Ursin and Hytten, 1992) and developmental psychology (Gunnar, 1989;
Levine, 1969, 1983). It is a crucial concept in contemporary formulations of the relation-
ships between environmental factors and health, psychosomatic pathophysiology (Dantzer,
1989; Levine and Ursin, 1991; Murison and Overmier, 1988), and in psychiatric, psycho-
logical, and somatic therapy (Carpenter, 1992; Haug et al., 1994; Wilhelmsen et al., 1994).
The term, or related terms, are essential in the theories for healthy work (Karasek and
Theorell, 1990), in sports psychology (Pensgard and Ursin, 1998), and in educational
psychology (Skinner, 1996).
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 557

In spite of a considerable degree of consensus the terminology is not standardized.


Coping is used both for the strategies selected (Folkman and Lazarus, 1985; Lazarus and
Folkman 1984), and for the expected results of these strategies (Ursin, 1988). Only in the
latter case does the concept attain predictive power for PNE, PNI, and PP, and, therefore,
for health issues. Coping refers to states where this outcome expectancy is positive. When
the expectancy is that the outcome is negative, the state is referred to as hopelessness.
When the expectancy is that there are no relationships between available responses and
results, the condition is referred to as helplessness (Levine and Ursin, 1991; Overmier,
1988; Overmier and Seligman, 1967). All these expectancies have a tendency to generalize.
This affects mood, and health. The PNE, PNI, and PP consequences of these expectancies
or states have been studied extensively in man and animals, in acute laboratory stress
situations in man (Brosschot et al., 1992; Olff et al., 1995) and in animals (Dantzer, 1989;
Murison and Overmier, 1988; Toates, 1995). There is also an extensive literature from field
experiments like parachutist jumps (Schedlowski et al., 1993; Ursin et al., 1978) and free
fall lifeboats (Endresen et al., 1991), and from longlasting stress situations (expeditions,
submarines, space simulations (Ursin et al., 1991; Vrnes et al., 1993), hyperbaric
environments (Vrnes and Darragh, 1982), military stress situations (Sandal et al., 1998),
serious life events (Lindstrm, 1997)).
There are also several related concepts. The ability or possibility to cope with the
environment is often referred to as control (Heckhausen, 1991; Skinner, 1996). The most
important model in the work stress literature is the demands/control model of Karasek
and Theorell (1990). In their theory, control is used for the formal aspects of the work
situation, which may or may not lead to mastery, which, again, is determining whether
the work situation is healthy or not. The essential aspect seems to be the subjective or
perceived feeling of being able to control the situation (Skinner 1996), which develops into
what Levine and Ursin refer to as a positive response outcome expectancy. For both
terms, the generalization from one situation to all situations is an important aspect
(Skinner, 1996). Individuals feeling that they have control over their situation are said to
have an internal locus of control (Parkes, 1988; Rotter 1975). Other related concepts are
toughness (Dientsbier, 1989), hardiness (Kobasa et al., 1982), high self esteem, and
affective stability (Zorrilla et al., 1995).
The most consistent finding across species and experimental situations is that whenever
a subject has acquired control over a potentially dangerous situation, and is given clear
and salient feedback that this is the case, the PNE, PNI, and PP responses are reduced,
compared to what happens if the individual has not acquired this type of outcome
expectancy. Under these conditions humans report subjective feelings of being able to
control or cope with the situation. The cognitive formulations of these expectancies make
it possible to draw conclusions across species. It is not acceptable to state anything about
the feelings of a rat, but it is acceptable to state what kind of expectancies that are
established in the rat brain. The assumed consequences for the endocrine and immune
system have been confirmed in many different types of experiments (Dantzer, 1989;
Overmier, 1988; Toates, 1995).
Psychological defense is an additional filtering mechanism. Psychological defense is only
identified in humans. It covers cognitive mechanisms that distort, deny or explain away
threatening stimuli. Therefore, it may be said to be related to the stimulus expectancies
(Bolles, 1972), rather than to the response outcome expectancies. Psychological defense
reduces the endocrine response to a threatening stimulus, without the individual being
aware of this strategy. Defense is operationalized with questionnaires, tachistoscopic
558 H. Ursin

methods, or clinical interviews (Olff et al., 1991). At least in tachistoscopic experiments


defense seems to be associated with particular ways of handling new and threatening
information (Eriksen et al., 1996).

COGNITIVE REFORMULATIONS OF STRESS THEORY


The relationships between the external world and changes in the organism, be this
within PP, PNI or PNE, were covered traditionally by theories of emotion, later by stress
theories, with assumptions of somatic mechanisms that in some ill defined way had its own
regulating system. Vegetative nerves were autonomic, the stress response through the
pituitary corticoid axis was without any or beyond the regular brain control of the
organism. Later limbic theories and the understanding of hypothalamic regulation of the
endocrine processes were important milestones in a more rational approach to these
phenomena, although some of this mysticism still seems to linger in the field.
The link from cognitive mechanisms treating the information available to the brain to
the PNE, PNI, and PP phenomena requires a more sophisticated model than just referring
to stress. The easiest and least controversial model is still the neurophysiological arousal
or activation theory (Levine and Ursin, 1980, 1991). General activation theory has
developed from the initial neurophysiological observations of Moruzzi and Magoun (1949)
to a well developed concept (Hobson and Brazier, 1980; Vanderwolf and Robinson, 1981)
still useful in physiology, psychology, sports science, clinical disciplines, and for the
understanding of PNE. The theory was integrated with similar concepts from psychophys-
iology (Duffy, 1962; Malmo, 1966), and later empirical observations from PNE (Levine
and Ursin, 1991; Mason, 1968) and PNI (Dantzer, 1989; Murison and Overmier, 1993).
The response is characterized by increased wakefulness in the brain, increased metabolism
and turnover of transmitters (Anisman, 1978), increased muscle tone, specific behaviors,
and vegetative, endocrine, and immunological changes (Levine and Ursin, 1991).
Lately the most important aspects of activation theory have been reformulated in
cognitive terms. Briefly, a Cognitive Activation Theory of Stress (CATS) states that the
stress response is identical to activation, and should be regarded as a general alarm system
operating whenever the organism registers that there is a discrepancy between what is
expected (set value) and what really exists (actual value) (Sokolov, 1963; Ursin, 1988). This
means that the stress response or the alarmoccurs when something is missing, and
that the organism does not expect to be able to cope with the challenge (Levine and Ursin,
1991). Lack of control or lack of being able to cope with a situation elicits this alarm, and
the individual reports stress. The general, non-specific activation response yields a
general, non-specific PNE, PNI and PP response.

SPECIFIC ACTIVATION MODELS


The main remaining controversy is whether it is really true that there is one and only
one, general alarm or activation response, or that the response observed is specific to a
particular situation, a particular strategy, or a specific emotion. To some extent both
positions are possible, for some theories the existence of specific activation patterns is
crucial.
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 559

Each individual seems to develop a preference or style in their activation pattern


(Grinker, 1966; Lacey, 1950). Both classical and instrumental conditioning contribute to
this individual specificity. But to what extent is there any general patterns to the
specificity?
There is some data to support that individuals that prefer active problem solving
strategies have lower levels of activation than others in similar stress situations. This may
be explained by higher levels of positive response outcome expectancies (coping) (Olff et
al., 1995). Preference of passive strategies over active strategies is often associated with
cortisol-responses in animals (Bohus, 1988; Henry and Meehan, 1981). Cortisol reactivity
and regulation is related to depression and stress (Anisman and Zacharko, 1982), in
particular to long-lasting effects of trauma (post-traumatic stress disorder) (Yehuda et al.,
1996a)
There are several reports of an apparent independence of the reactivity within the
cortisol axis and the catecholamine-system (Frankenhaeuser et al., 1980; Toates, 1995),
even if both systems respond according to general activation theory. Factor analyses of
endocrine responses to an extreme situation, for instance parachutist training jumps,
confirm this independence, cortisol and catecholamines appear in two different, orthogonal
factors (Rose et al., 1967; Ursin et al., 1978). There are also psychological differences,
those that respond most within the cortisol axis tend to have higher defense mechanisms
than those that respond mostly with the catecholamine system (Ursin et al., 1978). High
defense mechanisms seem related to immune responses (Endresen et al., 1991; Olff et al.,
1995). The catecholamine responder is related to the Type A personality (Friedman and
Rosenman, 1959; Glass, 1977).
It has been suggested that these psychological and corresponding PNE expressions may
be dependent on different limbic structures. According to Henry and Meehan (1981),
amygdala may be more related to catecholamine reactivity, while hippocampus may be
more related to cortisol reactivity. This does not seem to be supported by later findings.
But there is accumulating evidence for the importance of amygdala, particularly its central
nucleus, for the reactivity and regulation of at least the cortisol axis (LeDoux, 1993). The
possibility of specific neurotoxic effects on the hippocampus by very high levels of cortisol
suggested by Sapolsky et al. (1987) does not seem to be related to this issue.
The possibility of specific relations between particular coping strategies and specific
personality traits should be investigated further. Recent data point to the possibility of
relationships between particular personality traits and characteristics, in particular expres-
sions of sensation seeking behavior, and specific neuroendocrine reactivity (Zuckerman,
1994). But in these experiments the general, non-specific activation must be accounted for.
It is also possible that apparent personality differences in endocrine response profiles are
due to differences in the time course of the general activation pattern (Eriksen et al., 1998).

CONSEQUENCES FOR PNE RESEARCH DESIGN AND INTERPRETATIONS

Single E6ents: Short-lasting


Most experimental situations involve a short-lasting stimulus. The simplest situation is
the presentation of a novel, short-lasting stimulus without any significance for the
individual. This is referred to as a non-signal stimulus. The first presentation produces an
orienting response, characterized by arrest of ongoing activity, general activation, and
560 H. Ursin

orienting behavior directed at the source of stimulation. Pavlov referred to this response
as a what is it response. The PP aspects of the response is well mapped, and is related to
a short-lasting arrest also of the heart, evident as an increment in the interbeat interval
(Graham, 1979). The stimulus and the response are too short-lasting to have any
impressive endocrine effects, but the effects of novelty in itself must be considered in any
PNE experiment.
Presentation of a short-lasting stimulus with significance has effects dependent on this
significance. The significance is based on previous experience with this stimulus, or, within
learning theory, by previous conditioning. In simpler terms, the significance depends on
the meaning of the stimulus. The stimulus carries signal value, it signals a positive or
negative reward (reinforcement). Presentation of this signal elicits conditioned responses
(or expectancies). If the stimulus is presented out of context, i.e. in a different setting,
additional effects may be produced by orienting responses.
The reinforcing properties of the stimuli that are signalled, the events that are expected
to follow, depends on the quality of these events. Noxious stimuli are often used in stress
experiments, varying from air puffs to electric shocks. Why stimuli are reinforcing is not
always a simple question, and may depend on previous experience.

Single E6ents: Long-lasting

Exploration. When an animal or a child is placed in a novel environment, the response


pattern seems to follow a standard pattern. After an initial stage of fear and inactivity
(freezing), the individual starts exploring the new environment. This is a fear-reducing
strategy, the exploration yields information about the new environment which reduces the
uncertainty, and, therefore, the anxiety or fear (Jellestad et al., 1991). The same mecha-
nisms seem involved in humans faced with uncertainty. The most challenging aspect of this
problem area is the relationship between these basic mechanisms and curiosity, creativity,
exploration and sensation seeking behavior in humans (Zuckerman, 1994).
Life E6ents, Real Life Stressors. Real life stress situations are often long-lasting and
related to important life events (Rahe and Arthur, 1978). One very frequent long-lasting
stimulus situation used is (medical) students preparing for an exam (Kiecolt-Glaser et al.,
1984). In this case, as in other long-lasting situations, the psychological situation is very
complex, with many factors producing individual differentiation. Learning factors change
the reinforcing properties of the stimulus. In addition to the coping, control, defense, and
expectancy factors we have described, we must account for motivational factors, achieve-
ment motivation, previous results, and other psychological dimensions specific for this
particular situation, for this particular individual or group of individuals. In humans most
dimensions are covered by standardized questionnaires. Whenever new questionnaires are
tried, these must be tested for reliability and validity. In animals long-lasting stress stimuli
(immobilization, isolation, crowding, labile social hierarchies, maternal separation) may be
very complex, and the dynamics of the learning may often be difficult to account for.
However, this should be attempted, to avoid seemingly meaningless attributions to
physical properties of the stimulus.
The time factor is also an important factor for the type of response to be observed.
There is a gradual change from the initial stages influenced by novelty and the orienting
response, to acquisition of the signal value of the situation, and the appreciation of what
coping and defense mechanisms can do to the situation, and what would happen if nothing
is done to the situation.
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 561

Long-lasting stress situations are valid experiments in animals, and valid situations for
humans, for the understanding of the development of psychosomatic consequences of
stress. Due to the many psychological and physiological mechanisms that act to dampen
the response, this is often a complex situation to disentangle. If the attempt of the
experiment is to illustrate any pathophysiological contribution or effect, the experimenter
should account for this mechanism. It is also important to discuss whether there is any
epidemiological reason to believe that this possible pathophysiological mechanism has any
real impact on the condition.

Repeated E6ents: Response Decrements


Habituation. Repeating a stimulus with no signal value with inter trial intervals of
seconds or minutes leads to a decrement in the orienting response after ten to 30 trials.
This gradual decrement in response is referred to as habituation (Thompson and Spencer,
1966). The term is also used for response decrements in simpler circuits (Rayport and
Kandel, 1986), but still habituation should be identified as a process distinctly different
from adaptation and fatigue. When the pattern or the setting is changed, the response
reappears. If the brain shows habituation to a sequence of tones, it is sufficient to reverse
the order of the same tones to reestablish the response. The response also reappears if
activation is produced by a new, different stimulus, the next presentation of the first
stimulus now again produces the orienting response (dishabituation). Repeated presenta-
tions produce habituation, this time more rapidly (rehabituation) (Kimmel et al., 1979).

Adaptation. This is the most treacherous term. Used in the broadest sense, any observed
phenomenon in nature may be said to be adaptive, otherwise it would not exist. In
general physiology, it is used for changes that are due to exposure to a particular
environment, for instance low or high atmospheric pressure, bed rest, or cold. It takes time
to be established, it lasts for some time after the exposure. However, to refer to learning
phenomena as adaptation is too imprecise for meaningful communication. In the neuro-
sciences, the term is sometimes used for receptor up- or down-regulation, again, there are
better terms, up- or down-regulation is understood by us all. In neurophysiology, a clear,
non-controversial and consistent use is to reserve the term for processes in the sensory
organs themselves.

Fatigue. Fatigue is most often used for processes that are tied to the effector organ
itself, in particular for muscles. Fatigue is the gradual decrement in contraction power in
muscles due to repeated contractions. It should be kept separated from exhaustion
(Vllestad and Sejersted, 1988).

Learning. The most important, and most interesting, reason for response decrement is
that the individual learns something about the situation. Uncertainty is reduced, outcome
expectancies are established, the individual learns to cope, to control, to master the
situation. Even if there is no consensus on terminology, there is enough consistency in this
literature at the present time to plead for more consistency. There is no excuse for adding
to the confusion by neglecting the literature and inventing new terms, or use vague or
extremely general terms for phenomena that have been described and categorized.
Repeated exposures to novelty, or fear-inducing stimuli, may lead to extinction of the
fear responses. This happens under, for instance, handling of experimental animals
562 H. Ursin

(Dobrakovova et al., 1993) or extinction of fear responses. Fear reduction may also be due
to learning to avoid the fear source, and developing coping (Coover et al., 1973).

Repeated E6ents: Increased Responses


Sensitization. Repeating stimuli may lead to an increased response. A classical finding is
an increase in the orienting response during the first couple of stimulus presentations,
before habituation appears. The dual process theory of Groves and Thompson (1970)
ascribed this phenomenon to two opposing processes, an initially dominating sensitization,
i.e. an increase in response, which is then gradually taken over by habituation, i.e. a
response decrement.
The neurophysiological mechanisms for habituation and sensitization on the synaptic
level have been mapped in great detail (Rayport and Kandel, 1986). Sensitization processes
have also been suggested as being relevant for memory, as for instance in the long term
potentiation studies of the hippocampus (Bliss and Lmo, 1973). However, simple
sensitization is the most frequent source of error in conditioning experiment since this
really is pseudo conditioning rather than true associative conditioning. If you repeat a
stimulus and the response changes due to the repeating of the one and same stimulus, this
is pseudoconditioning, or sensitization. Conditioning means that one stimulus given in
combination with another stimulus changes the response to the first stimulus, as in
classical conditioning. If Pavlovs dog had started salivating just because the bell had been
presented many times, this would not be conditioning, it would be sensitization or
pseudoconditioning. The hot issue in learning research is whether the sensitization
produced by a single stimulus is a condition for conditioning to occur. The long term
potentiation may be related to learning if the sensitization given by the unconditioned
stimulus is enough to change the total reactivity of neurons, thereby changing the
reactivity also to the first, originally neutral stimulus. This remains a hypothetical
substrate for learning.
Sensitization may be an interesting phenomenon from a psychosomatic point of view.
Sensitization occurs in pain pathways, in this case it is not a non-stimulus. However,
repeating pain producing stimuli leads to an increased sensitivity, tenderness, and in-
creased responses. This may be an important mechanism for increased sensitivity to pain,
and to subjective health complaint states, i.e. muscle pain, fibromyalgia, and low back pain
(Ursin, 1997). The same substrate has been suggested for multiple chemical sensitivity
(Bell, 1994) and for sensitivity to noise (Nivison and Endresen, 1993).

Learning. When the increase in response strength is due to the association with other
events, learning has occurred. The new course of events is determined by the relationship
between stimuli, or the relationship between stimuli and available responses. These
relationships are described within learning theory. This is the bestsome say the only
true theory in psychology. There is no way to ignore this theoretical foundation, even if
it requires work to penetrate a field with its own nomenclature. Many of us have found it
easier, and more convenient, to stick to the simpler formulations in cognitive reformula-
tions of learning theory (Bolles, 1972; Dickinson, 1980). Within the cognitive reformula-
tions of stress theory, it is the expectancy to the situation which is the overall determining
factor. When one stimulus is presented, what does the individual now expect? What does
this stimulus signal? Which responses are available, and what is the result to be expected?
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 563

THE 30 YEARS ANNIVERSARY OF THE MASON PRINCIPLE


Psychoendocrine studies prior to 1955 had produced indirect and non-specific informa-
tion indicating that the adrenal cortical activity could be influenced by psychological
factors. At that time, the relationship was mapped with the help of biochemical measure-
ment of 17-hydroxy corticosteroids (17-OHCS). In his very influential review in 1968,
based on about 200 studies, Mason concluded with what is later referred to as the Mason
principle: Psychological stimuli are capable of influencing the level of pituitaryadrenal
cortical activity. He saw no longer any room for any reasonable doubt on this basic
conclusion, and emphasized that the burden of proof rested on those that did not realize
this. If they wanted to prove that any observed change was not due to psychological
factors, they would have to eliminate or minimize extraneous environmental and psycho-
logical stimuli to obtain the necessary control. Thirty years of research has proven him
right on most or all counts.
Mason formulated eight different conclusions. They will be reviewed in the following, all
seem to be confirmed by the research since 1968. The psychological principles he outlined
have been established and systematized in contemporary psychology, and are in consensus
with general theories and knowledge in psychology today.

Masons Principle no. 1


Psychological influences are among the most potent natural stimuli known to affect the
pituitary adrenal cortical activity.
This conclusion has been substantiated in a long series of empirical work, in animals
and in humans, and across a variety of situations. It should be admitted that there is still
some resistance in the acknowledgment of this basic fact, as pointed out by Mason in
1968. However, these factors must be taken into account in any measurement of cortisol,
or any other physiological process in an intact animal or human with an awake brain.

Mason Principle no. 2


The central nervous system (CNS) exerts a constant tonic influence on this axis, much
in the same way as had been demonstrated for the autonomic and skeletal muscular
effector systems.
Mason referred to the psychological influence as tonic meaning that the influence was
not restricted to sudden and unusual events. Later research has confirmed that the
influence includes everyday events. Mason did not refer to general activation theory or any
brain system maintaining wakefulness and level of activation. However, within this theory
the influence is simply a function of the activation level of the brain. This influence rides
on top of the circadian rhythm, and may completely override the circadian rhythm under
particularly stressful circumstances.
The most difficult aspect of this position is whether there are long-term, tonic changes
in the hormonal regulation. Ursin (1980) postulated that psychosomatic pathology would
result as a response to sustained activation. This has been difficult to demonstrate, in
most experiments any such change is drowned in diurnal and episodic influences, and by
homeostatic self-regulatory principles. There are also problems with the model since
coping, defined as high self esteem, hardiness, and affective stability, is associated with
high rather than low basal levels of cortisol (Zorrilla et al., 1995). There are other
recent data on what seems to be permanent changes in some of the regulatory mechanisms
(Yehuda et al., 1996a).
564 H. Ursin

Mason Principle no. 3


The pituitary adrenal cortical activity is not related to any specific affective state, but
reflects a relatively undifferentiated state of emotional arousal, involvement, anticipation
of activity, and coping.
This is the strongest Mason statement in support of general activation theory, even if he
avoided using this term. The statement is also preceding the cognitive reformulations of
activation theory, in the emphasis on the importance of coping, involvement, and
anticipation of activity. Finally, the statement is also compatible with the essential
elements in stress theory, another term Mason avoided. Selye also underlined the non-spe-
cificity of his stress response. Cortisol elevations are produced even by amusement (Hubert
et al., 1993).

Mason Principle no. 4


Particularly potent influences on the system are novelty, uncertainty, and
unpredictability.
Later research has confirmed this principle. It is, in many ways, the reciprocal formula-
tion of the previous principle. Contemporary psychological theory holds that stress and
unhealthy work is related to uncertainty and unpredictability. Coping, or perceived
control, and restructuring the work situation, aim at establishing predictability and
certainty.

Mason Principle no. 5


Intense and disorganising emotional reactions with behavior breakdown are associated
with unusually marked 17-OHCS elevations.
This position is true for pituitaryadrenal activity under special circumstances. Present
research interest has moved to questions on whether the CNS regulation of this axis is
altered during prolonged exposures to stressors the individual is unable to cope with. The
states of helplessness and hopelessness are accompanied by CNS changes, but these go
beyond any specific regulation system, and areperhapsbest understood within non-
specific activation theory (Anisman and Sklar, 1979; Coover et al., 1984).
However, there has been a long lasting research interest for the specific CNS regulation
of the pituitary adrenal axis, particularly for depression, lately, also for other chronic
stress conditions. It is possible that the amygdala nuclei are involved, in particular the
central nucleus of amygdala, an important area for emotional activation, arousal, and
exploration (Jellestad et al., 1991). The neurons in the central nucleus produce corti-
cotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH), but respond with increased production to increased
levels of glucocorticoids, rather than the expected decrease (Schulkin et al., 1994). Similar
alterations in this axis have been reported for patients with chronic pain and posttraumatic
stress disorders (Yehuda et al., 1996a,b).

Mason Principle no. 6


Psychological factors may either raise or lower the HPA activity.
The apparent inconsistencies in PNE results are probably due to lack of control and
insights in the psychological factors involved. Mason pointed out that the quality of the
emotional response might be important, as well as the style and effectiveness of defense.
He also pointed out that it was important whether the threat was chronic or acute. He was
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 565

right on all accounts, but today these statements should be made more specific, and with
the proper operationalizations of the assumed psychological dimensions involved.

Mason Principle no. 7


There are marked individual differences in the response to any particular situation,
dependent on multiple psychological and other determinants. The response depends on
how that individual interprets this particular situation, what are his goals, and what are his
expectancies and defense mechanisms.
This is a very early formulation of what later has been referred to as interactive stress
models, the appraisal dimensions of Lazarus and Folkman (1984), and the third Levine
and Ursin (1991) consensus statement express the same. All challenging and threatening
stimuli are filtered by coping and defense mechanisms, acting on stimulus and response
outcome expectations.

Mason Principle no. 8


The measurement of pituitaryadrenal activity offers a particularly interesting, sensi-
tive, and objective index of the psychological or emotional state of the individual. This
makes it possible to measure the factors both in man and in animal that prevent, minimise
or increase arousal and distress.
Again, Mason was indeed right, and the HPA axis has remained one of the favoured
psychoendocrine indicators of the central state and the psychological state, in spite of the
increased laboratory possibilities to measure almost any physiological consequence of
activation. One reason for this is the relatively slow response, which makes it possible to
evaluate the situation before the biological sample was obtained. The molecule is also very
stable, and this makes it useful under field conditions. The sensitivity has been impressive,
there are, for instance, examples of the HPA axis reacting to the change from a fixed ratio
to a variable ratio of reinforcement (Coover et al., 1984).
In humans, the possibility to supply standard questionnaires by simply asking them
what happened is often ignored. It is permissible, and recommendable, to ask the subjects.
The human advantage is that they talk. But it should be remembered that interview data
are particularly sensitive to expectancy bias, from the experimenter, and from the subject.
Interviews are also a validation of the research questionnaires. Far too many researchers
rely on the label of their questionnaire. This label research should be avoided, and may
be a main source of inconsistency in this literature.

CONCLUSIONS
To all of us that have been involved in this type of research the Mason principles are
amazingly representative of what I believe to be consensus. The most important addition
is what he predicted in his conclusion. He suggested that the evolution of psychoendocrine
research on the pituitary adrenal cortical system provided a valuable guide to the lines to
similar research on other endocrine systems which should follow. He suggested that similar
conclusions would be reached also for other endocrine systems when evidence was
collected along these lines. As he pointed out in his principal number no. 2, there is now
overwhelming evidence supporting that the endocrine system responds in much the same
fashion as autonomic and skeletal effector systems, and the last twenty years of psychoim-
566 H. Ursin

mune research has included also this system in a generalised type of response to
psychological stimuli and psychological states.
One of the most fascinating aspects of PNE is that the data and principles derive
partly from animal experiments, partly from humans. Laboratory experiments, often in
the animal laboratory, have contributed to the development of general theories in
psychology. On the other hand, the development in general psychology has con-
tributed to the understanding of PNE. The relationship between results and concepts
from animal research and research on human individuals is sometimes so close as to
warrant caution against too simple generalisations. There are some differences left,
particularly those related to the variance and individual differences found in humans.
This is not noise, this is psychology. It should also be noticed that there is more
consensus, conventions, and clarity in the terminology used in animal research, partic-
ularly in experiments related to learning theory. The terms are operationalized and
tied to well defined and standardised experimental situations. In ethology, the termi-
nology is looser and more difficult to standardise. In the human literature the opera-
tionalizations are most often tied to particular instruments. The P novice should exert
caution in accepting the names on the dimensions measured. Instruments claiming to
measure defense, for instance, do not necessarily measure the same phenomenon (Olff
et al., 1990, 1991).
It has taken some time to accept the existence of the networks within the brain,
and the feedback loops between the brain and peripheral somatic processes. It has
taken almost 100 years to get rid of the dichotomy between the James Lange position
and the Cannon Bard position for the physiology of emotions. Psychology students
through the ages have been taught that these were two different theoretical positions,
and they have even been taught that one is wrong, and one is right. Cannon Bard
held that emotions were primarily a brain event with physiological concomitants:
James Lange pointed out that an essential part of emotions was the feedback from
the peripheral physiological activation. Within an activation and psychobiological con-
cept, realizing the importance of feedback circuits for activation and emotional
arousal, these two positions are just describing two legs of a feedback circle. Cannon
Bard describes the efferent link, James Lange describes the afferent link.
However, the stumbling block for the P in PNE has, in my opinion, very often
proven to be the traditional dualistic way of thinking about body and soul. This
position is deeply rooted in religious, philosophical and even political points of view.
For the psychobiological scientist this position is untenable. Private beliefs must be
adjusted to the basic findings on the reciprocal relationship between brain and body.
PNE is, or should be, an interdisciplinary field. This is what makes it interesting,
and this is the condition for meaningful new contributions. It is also the foundation
for our contributions to science in general, and to our understanding of man. We live
in exciting times, and have the privilege of being able to reveal secrets of how man
functions. Finding a new molecule and a new gene location is great. However, to be
able to explain the relationship between our mind and our body is even greater, and
this gives us a unique role in the history of mankind. It is close to sacrilegious to
spoil this opportunity by ignoring the literature, and playing it safe by sticking to our
favorite molecule, favorite stimulus contingency, or favorite questionnaire. We simply
have to try a little harder, all of us.
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 567

REFERENCES
Anisman, H. (1978) Neurochemical changes elicited by stress, Behavioral correlates. In: Anisman, H.
and Bignami, G. (Eds.). Psychopharmacology of A6ersi6ely Moti6ated Beha6ior. Plenum Press,
New York, pp. 119172.
Anisman, H. and Sklar, L. S. (1979) Catecholamine depletion in mice upon reexposure to stress:
mediation of the escape deficits produced by inescapable shock. Journal of Comparati6e and
Physiological Psychology 93, 610 625.
Anisman, H. and Zacharko, R. M. (1982) Depression: The predisposing influence of stress. The
Beha6ioral and Brain Sciences 5, 89 137.
Bell, I. R. (1994) White paper: Neuropsychiatric aspects of sensitivity to low-level chemicals: A
neural sensitization model. Toxicology and Industrial Health 10, 277 312.
Bliss, T. V. P. and Lmo, T. (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate
area of the anesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. Journal of Physiology
232, 331356.
Bohus, B. (1988) Limbic-midbrain mechanisms and behavioral physiology of interactions with CRF,
ACTH and adrenal hormones. In: Hellhammer, D., Florin, I. and Weiner, H. (Eds.). Neurobio-
logical Approaches to Human Disease. Hans Huber, Toronto, pp. 267 285.
Bolles, R. C. (1972) Reinforcement, expectancy and learning. Psychological Re6iew 79, 391 409.
Brosschot, J. F., Benschop, R. J., Godaert, G. L. R., De Smet, M. B. M., Olff, M., Heijnen, C. J.
and Ballieux, R. E. (1992) Effects of experimental psychological stress on distribution and
function of peripheral blood cells. Psychosomatic Medicine 54, 394 406.
Carpenter, B. N. (1992) Personal Coping: Theory, Research and Applications. Praeger, Westport, CT.
Coover, G. D., Ursin, H. and Levine, S. (1973) Plasma corticosterone levels during active-avoidance
learning in rats. Journal of Comparati6e and Physiological Psychology 82, 170 174.
Coover, C. D., Murison, R., Jellestad, F. K., Sundberg, H. and Ursin, H. (1984) Plasma corticos-
terone and meal expectancy in rats: Effects of low probability cues. Physiology and Beha6ior 33,
179184.
Dantzer, R. (1989) Lillusion Psychosomatique. Editions Odile Jacob, Paris.
Dientsbier, R. A. (1989) Arousal and physiological toughness. Implications for mental and physical
health. Psychological Re6iew 96, 84 100.
Dickinson, A. (1980) Contemporary Learning Theory. Cambridge University Press, New York.
Dobrakovova, M., Kvetnansky, R., Oprsalova, Z. and Jezova, D. (1993) Specificity of the effect of
repeated handling on sympathetic-adrenomedullary and pituitary adrenocortical activity in rats.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 18, 163 174.
Duffy, E. (1962) Acti6ation and Beha6ior. Wiley, New York.
Endresen, I. M., Relling, G. B., Tnder, O., Myking, O., Walther, B. T. and Ursin, H. (1991) Brief
uncontrollable stress and psychological parameters influence human plasma concentrations of
IgM and complement component C3. Beha6ioral Medicine 17, 167 176.
Eriksen, H. R., Olff, M., Mann, C., Sterman, M. B. and Ursin, H. (1996) Psychological defense
mechanisms and electroencephalographic arousal. Scandina6ian Journal of Psychology 37, 351
361.
Eriksen, H.R., Murison, R., Olff, M., and Ursin, H. (1998) The time dimension in the stress
response. Psychiatry Research (accepted).
Folkman, S. and Lazarus, R. S. (1985) If it changes it must be a process: Study of emotion and
coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
48, 150170.
Frankenhaeuser, M., Lundberg, U. and Forsman, L. (1980) Dissociation between sympathetic-
adrenal and pituitaryadrenal responses to an achievement situation characterized by high
controllability: comparison between Type A and Type B males and females. Biological Psychology
10, 7991.
Friedman, M. and Rosenman, R. H. (1959) Association of specific overt behaviour pattern with
blood and cardiovascular findings. Journal of American Medical Association 169, 1286 1296.
Glass, D. C. (1977) Stress, behavior patterns, and coronary disease. American Scientist 65, 177 187.
Graham, F. (1979) Distinguishing between orienting, defense and startle reflexes. In: Kimmel, H. D.,
van Holst, E. H. and Orlebeke, J. F. (Eds.). The Orienting Reflex in Humans. Lawrence Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, pp. 137167.
568 H. Ursin

Grinker, R. R. (1966) The psychosomatic aspects of anxiety. In: Spielberger, C.D. (Ed.). Anxiety and
Beha6ior. Academic Press, New York, pp. 129 142.
Groves, P. M. and Thompson, R. F. (1970) Habituation: A dual process theory. Psychological
Re6iew 77, 419450.
Gunnar, M. G. (1989) Studies of the human infants adrenocortical response to potentially stressful
events. New Directions for Child De6elopment 45, 3 18.
Haug, T. T., Wilhelmsen, I., Svebak, S., Berstad, A. and Ursin, H. (1994) Psychotherapy in
functional dyspepsia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 38, 735 744.
Heckhausen, H. (1991) Moti6ation and Action. Springer, Berlin.
Henry, J. P. and Meehan, J. P. (1981) Psychosocial stimuli, physiological specificity, and cardiovas-
cular disease. In: Weiner, H., Hofer, M.A. and Stunkard, A.J. (Eds.). Brain, Beha6iour and Bodily
Disease. Raven Press, New York, pp. 305 335.
Hobson, J. A. and Brazier, M. A. B. (1980) The Reticular Formation Re6isited: Specifying Function
for a Nonspecific System. Raven Press, New York.
Hubert, W., Moller, M.and and de-Jong-Meyer, R. (1993) Film-induced amusement changes in
saliva cortisol levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology 18, 265 272.
Jellestad, F. K., Folles, G. S. and Ursin, H. (1991) The Neurobiological Foundation of Explo-
ration. In: Keller, H., Schneider, K. and Henderson, B. (Eds.). Curiosity and Exploration.
Springer, Berlin, pp. 4363.
Karasek, R. A. and Theorell, T. (1990) Healthy Work. Stress, Producti6ity, and the Reconstruction of
Working Life. Basic Books, New York.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Garner, W. K., Speicher, C., Penn, G. M., Holliday, J. and Glaser, R. (1984)
Psychosocial modifiers of immunocompetence in medical students. Psychosomatic Medicine 46,
714.
Kimmel, H. D., van Holst, E. H. and Orlebeke, J. F. (1979) The Orienting Reflex in Humans.
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.
Kobasa, S.C., Maddi, S.R. and Kahn, S. (1982) Hardiness and health: a prospective study. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 42, 168 177.
Lacey, J. I. (1950) Individual differences in somatic response patterns. Journal of Comparati6e and
Physiological Psychology 43, 338 350.
Lazarus, R. S. and Folkman, S. (1984) Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. Springer, New York.
LeDoux, J. E. (1993) Emotional memory systems in the brain. Beha6ioral Brain Research 58, 69 79.
Levine, S. (1969) An endocrine theory of infantile stimulation. In: Ambrose, A. (Ed.). Stimulation in
Early Infancy. Academic Press, London, pp. 45 63.
Levine, S. (1983) A psychobiological approach to the study of coping. In: Garmezy, N. and Rutter,
M. (Eds.). Stress, Coping and De6elopment in Children. Plenum, New York, pp. 107 131.
Levine, S. and Ursin, H. (1980) Coping and Health. Plenum Press, New York.
Levine, S. and Ursin, H. (1991) What is stress? In: Brown, M. R., Rivier, C. and Koob, G. (Eds.).
Stress. Neurobiology and Neuroendocrinology. Marcel Decker, New York, pp. 3 21.
Lindstrm, T. C. (1997) Immunity and health after bereavement in relation to coping. Scandina6ian
Journal of Psychology 38, 253259.
Malmo, R. B. (1966) Studies of anxiety. Some clinical origins of the activation concept. In:
Spielberger, C.D. (Ed.). Anxiety and Beha6ior. Academic Press, New York, pp. 157 177.
Mason, J. W. (1968) A review of psychoneuroendocrine research on the pituitary adrenal cortical
system. Psychosomatic Medicine 30, 576 607.
Moruzzi, G. and Magoun, H. W. (1949) Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1, 455 473.
Murison, R and Overmier, J. B. (1988) Adrenocortical activity and disease, with references to gastric
pathology in animals. In: Hellhammer, D. H., Florin, I. and Weiner, H. (Eds.). Neurobiological
Approaches to Human Disease. Neuronal Control of Bodily Function: Basic and Clinical Aspects,
Vol. 2. Hans Huber, Toronto, pp. 335 343.
Murison, R and Overmier, J.B. (1993) Parallelism among stress effects on ulcer, immunosuppression
and analgesia: commonality of mechanisms? Journal de Physiologie (Paris) 87, 253 259.
Nivison, M. E. and Endresen, I. M. (1993) An analysis of relationships among environmental noise,
annoyance and sensitivity to noise, and the consequences for health and sleep. Journal of
Beha6ioral Medicine 16, 257276.
The psychology in psychoneuroendocrinology 569

Olff, M., Godaert, G. L. R., Brosschot, J. F., Weiss, K. E. and Ursin, H. (1990) Tachistoscopic and
questionnaire methods for measurement of psychological defences. Scandina6ian Journal of
Psychology 31, 8998.
Olff, M, Godaert, G. and Ursin, H. (1991) Quantification of Human Defence Mechanisms. Springer,
Berlin.
Olff, M., Brosschot, J. F., Godaert, G., Benschop, R. J., Ballieux, R. E., Heijnen, C. J., de Smet, M.
B. M. and Ursin, H. (1995) Modulatory effects of defense and coping on stress-induced changes
in endocrine and immune parameters. International Journal of Beha6ioral Medicine 2, 85 103.
Overmier, J. B. (1988) Psychological determinants of when stressors stress. In: Hellhammer, D. H.,
Florin, I. and Weiner, H. (Eds.). Neurobiological Approaches to Human Disease. Neuronal Control
of Bodily Function: Basic and Clinical Aspects, Vol. 2. Hans Huber, Toronto, pp. 236 259.
Overmier, J. B. and Seligman, M. E. P. (1967) Effect of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape
and avoidance responding. Journal of Comparati6e and Physiological Psychology 63, 28 33.
Parkes, K. R. (1988) Locus of control in three behavioral domains: factor structure and correlates
of the Spheres Of Control scale. Personality and Indi6idual Differences 9, 631 643.
Pensgard, A. M., and Ursin, H. (1998) Expectations, perceptions of stress and coping at an elite level
in sport. Scandina6ian Journal of Medicine and Sciences in Sports (accepted).
Rahe, R. H. and Arthur, R. J. (1978) Life change and illness studies: past history and future
directions. Journal of Human Stress 4, 3 15.
Rayport, S. G. and Kandel, E. R. (1986) Development of plastic mechanisms related to learning at
identified chemical synaptic connections in Aplysia. Neuroscience 17, 283 294.
Rose, R. M., Poe, R. O. and Mason, J. W. (1967) Observations on the relationship between
psychological state, 17-OHCS excretion, and epinephrine, norepinephrine, insulin, BEI, estrogen
and androgen levels during basic training. Psychosomatic Medicine 29, 544.
Rotter, J. B. (1975) Some problems and misconceptions related to the construct of internal control
of reinforcement. Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, 56 67.
Sandal, G. M., Grnningster, H., Eriksen, H. R., Gravraakmo, A., Birkeland, K. and Ursin, H.
(1998) Personality and endocrine activation in military stress situations. Military Psychology 10,
4561.
Sapolsky, R., River, C., Yamamoto, G., Plotsky, P. and Vale, W. (1987) Interleukin-1 stimulates the
secretion of hypothalamic corticotropin releasing factor. Science 238, 522 524.
Schedlowski, M., Jacobs, R., Stratman, G., Richter, S., Hadicke, A., Tewes, U., Wagner, T. O. F.
and Schmidt, R. E. (1993) Changes of natural killer cells during acute psychological stress.
Journal of Clinical Immunology 13, 119 126.
Schulkin, J., McEwen, B. S. and Gold, P. W. (1994) Allostasis, amygdala, and anticipatory angst.
Neuroscience and Biobeha6ioral Re6iews 18, 385 396.
Skinner, E. A. (1996) Percei6ed Control, Moti6ation, and Coping. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Sokolov, Y. N. (1963) Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. Pergamon Press, Oxford.
Thompson, R. F. and Spencer, W. A. (1966) Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of
neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Re6iew 73, 16 43.
Toates, F. (1995) Stress. Conceptual and Biological Aspects. Wiley, Chichester.
Ursin, H. (1980) Personality, activation, and somatic health. A new psychosomatic theory. In:
Levine, S. and Ursin, H. (Eds.). Coping and Health. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 259 279.
Ursin, H. (1988) Expectancy and activation: An attempt to systematize stress theory. In: Hellham-
mer, D., Florin, I. and Weiner, H. (Eds.). Neurobiological Approaches to Human Disease. Hans
Huber, Toronto, pp. 313334.
Ursin, H. (1997) Sensitization: A mechanism for somatization and subjective health complaints.
Commentary to Coderre and Katz: Peripheral and central hyperexcitability: Differential signs and
symptoms in persistent pain. The Beha6ioral and Brain Sciences 20, 469.
Ursin, H., Baade, E. and Levine, S. (1978) Psychobiology of Stress. A Study of Coping Men.
Academic Press, New York.
Ursin, H. and Hytten, K. (1992) Outcome expectancies and psychosomatic consequences. In:
Carpenter, B.N. (Ed.). Personal Coping: Theory, Research and Applications. Praeger, Westport,
CT, pp. 171184.
Ursin, H., Bergan, T., Collet, J., Endresen, I. M., Lugg, D. J., Maki, P., Matre, R., Molvr, O.,
Muller, H. K., Olff, M., Pettersen, R., Sandal, G. M., Vaernes, R. and Warncke., M. (1991)
Psychobiological studies of individuals in small, isolated groups in the Antarctic and in space
analogues. En6ironment and Beha6ior 23, 766 781.
570 H. Ursin

Vanderwolf, C. H. and Robinson, T. E. (1981) Reticulo-cortical activity and behavior. A critique of


the arousal theory and a new synthesis. The Beha6ioral and Brain Sciences 4, 459 514.
Vrnes, R. J. and Darragh, A. (1982) Endocrine reactions and cognitive performance at 60 metres
hyperbaric pressure. Scandina6ian Journal of Psychology 23, 193 199.
Vrnes, R. J., Bergan, T., Warncke, M., Ursin, H., Aakvaag, A. and Hockey, R. (1993) Workload
and stress: Effects on psychosomatic and psychobiological reaction patterns. Ad6ances in Space
Biology and Medicine 3, 95120.
Vllestad, N. K. and Sejersted, O. M. (1988) Biochemical correlates of fatigue. A brief review.
European Journal of Applied Physiology 57, 336 347.
Wilhelmsen, I., Haug, T. T., Ursin, H. and Berstad, A. (1994) Effect of short-term cognitive
psychotherapy on recurrence of duodenal ulcer: A prospective randomized trial. Psychosomatic
Medicine 56, 440448.
Yehuda, R., Boisoneau, D., Lowy, M. T. and Giller, E. L. (1996a) Dose-response changes in plasma
cortisol and lymphocyte glucocorticoid receptors following dexamethasone administration in
combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Archi6es of General Psychiatry
52, 583593.
Yehuda, R., Levengood, R. A., Schmeidler, J., Wilson, S., et al. (1996b) Increased pituitary
activation following metyrapone administration in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology 21, 116.
Zorrilla, E. P., DeRubeis, R. J. and Redei, E. (1995) High self-esteem, hardiness and affective
stability are associated with higher basal pituitary adrenal hormone levels. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology 20, 591601.
Zuckerman, M. (1994) Beha6ioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking. Cambridge
University Press, New York.

S-ar putea să vă placă și