Sunteți pe pagina 1din 29

ReviewofGeneralPsychology Copyright1998bythe EducationalPublishingFoundation

1998, VoL 2, No. 3, 271-299 1089-2680/98/$3.00

The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review


J a m e s J. G r o s s
Stanford University

The emerging field of emotion regulation studies how individuals influence which
emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express them.
This review takes an evolutionary perspective and characterizes emotion in terms of
response tendencies. Emotion regulation is defined and distinguished from coping,
mood regulation, defense, and affect regulation. In the increasingly specialized disci-
pline of psychology, the field of emotion regulation cuts across traditional boundaries
and provides common ground. According to a process model of emotion regulation,
emotion may be regulated at five points in the emotion generative process: (a) selection
of the situation, (b) modification of the situation, (c) deployment of attention, (d)
change of cognitions, and (e) modulation of responses. The field of emotion regulation
promises new insights into age-old questions about how people manage their emotions.

Conquer your passions and you conquer the wodd. as Seneca (trans., 1963) and Ryle (1949), have
---Hindu proverb
seen emotions as troublesome deviations from
To yield to man's emotions will assuredly lead to strife proper functioning, and thus in need of severe
and disorderliness. . . . It is only under the influence of
teachers and l a w s . . , that courtesy will be observed,
correction. Others, such as Aristotle (trans.,
etiquette respected, and order restored. 1941) and Hume (1739/1969), have seen
- - H s u n Tzu (3rd C., B.C.E., DeBary, Chan, & Watson, emotions in a more positive light, and thus less
1960, p. 118) in need of strict regulation. This ambivalent
The principal use of prudence or self-control is that it Western legacy is reflected in conflicting say-
teaches us to be masters of our passions. ings such as "He who keeps a cool head
--Descartes (1649/1955, p. 427)
prevails" and "Let your feelings be your
How should we manage our emotions? guide."
Should we attend to them or disregard them? In the past 2 decades, an exciting new chapter
Esteem them or revile them? Encourage them or has opened in the age-old discussion of how we
suppress them? Each culture answers these should manage emotions: psychological re-
questions differently, but there is a common search has begun to focus explicitly on emotion
theme: we need to exert some measure of regulation. Research on emotion regulation
control over our emotions. Nowhere, perhaps, is originated in developmental psychology (Gaens-
this interventionist sentiment stronger than in bauer, 1982) and now is flourishing in the child
the West. One of the central tenets of Western and adult literatures alike (e.g., Campos, Cam-
philosophy is "the wisdom of reason against the pos, & Barrett, 1989; Gross, 1998). In this
treachery and temptations of the passions" article, I review the emerging field of emotion
(Solomon, 1976, p. 11). regulation. First, I orient the reader to an
Even within the Western tradition, however, evolutionary perspective that views emotions as
opinions differ as to just how much emotions response tendencies. Second, I define emotion
should be controlled. Some philosophers, such regulation and distinguish it from related
constructs. Third, I show how emotion regula-
tion cuts across traditional subdisciplinary
Work on this article was supported by Grant MH58147
from the National Institute of Mental Health. I would like to boundaries within psychology. Fourth, I propose
thank Lisa Feldman Barrett, Oliver John, Richard Lane, a process model of emotion regulation that
Randy Larsen, and James Pennebaker for helpful comments facilitates analysis of the potentially overwhelm-
on a draft of this article.
ing number of kinds of emotion regulation.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to James J. Gross, Department of Psychology, Fifth, I consider several important challenges
Stanford University, Stanford, California 94305-2130. that the field still needs to address. I conclude
Electronic mail may be sent to james @psych.stanford.edu. that we do not yet have complete answers to
271
272 GROSS

most of the questions about how emotions are Emotions as Response Tendencies
regulated. Nonetheless, I argue that psychologi-
cal research on emotion regulation shows every Other perspectives fairly cry out for an
promise of providing the theoretical models and analysis of emotion regulation. William James
the empirical findings needed to answer funda- (1884, 1894), for example, regarded emotions as
mental questions about how we can and should adaptive behavioral and physiological response
manage our emotions. tendencies that are called forth directly by
evolutionarily significant situations. Although
individuals often express these emotional re-
What Is Emotion? sponse tendencies, they do not always do so.
James's view of emotions as response tenden-
Any discussion of emotion regulation presup- cies allows that individuals may modulate their
poses an understanding of what emotion is. And emotional response tendencies, such as when
not just any definition will do. For example, they whistle instead of running away in fear.
Carver and Scheier (1990) view emotion as the Discrepancies between emotional response ten-
readout of a system that monitors the rate at dencies and manifest behavior prompt questions
which the discrepancy between a goal and about how, why, and when individuals might try
reality is being decreased (also see Hsee & to regulate their emotional response tendencies.
Abelson, 1991, for a similar position). Positive Researchers today continue to draw on
emotion signals a rate of discrepancy reduction James's response-tendency perspective. As
that is faster than expected; negative emotion shown in Figure 1, many contemporary research-
signals a rate that is slower than expected. ers conceive of emotions as flexible response
Although the individual may take actions that sequences (Buck, 1994; Frijda, 1986; Scherer,
lead to a decrease in negative emotion (e.g., 1984) that are called forth whenever an
allocating more resources to the task; Carver, individual evaluates a situation as offering
Lawrence, & Scheier, 1996), emotion regulation important challenges or opportunities (Tooby &
is viewed as an accidental by-product of such Cosmides, 1990). Emotional response tenden-
action, rather than an end in itself. For this cies are relatively short lived and involve
reason, Carver and Scheier's (1990) conception changes in the behavioral, experiential, auto-
of emotion provides relatively inhospitable nomic, and neuroendocrine systems (Lang,
ground for the study of emotion regulation. 1995). Importantly, emotional response tenden-

Emotional
Emotional Response Tendencies Emotional
Cues Responses
.Behavioral
.Experiential J-
I .Physiological

I
Evaluation Modulation
Figure 1. A consensual process model of emotion generation. Adapted from "Antecedent-
and Response-Focused Emotion Regulation," by J. J. Gross, 1998, Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74, p. 226. Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association.
Adapted with permission.
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTIONREGULATION 273

cies may be modulated, and it is this modulation news to a close friend, moods such as depression
that determines the final shape of the emotional and euphoria, dispositional states such as liking
response (Gross, 1998). t and hating, and traits such as cheerfulness and
Historically, emotions were seen as nonspe- irascibility (Chaplin, John, & Goldberg, 1988).
cific, disruptive activation states (Hebb, 1949; The most important distinctions among mem-
Young, 1943). More recent analyses emphasize bers of the affect family are those among
the functions emotions serve (Keltner & Gross, emotion, emotion episodes, and mood. Whereas
in press). Although emotions address different emotions unfold over a relatively short time
adaptive problems (Ekman, 1992), they gener- period, emotion episodes are more extended in
ally facilitate decision making (Oatley & both time and space (Frijda, 1993; Stein,
Johnson-Laird, 1987), prepare the individual for Trabasso, & Liwag, 1993). Emotion episodes,
rapid motor responses (Frijda, 1986), and also referred to as plots (Ekman, 1984), scripts
provide information regarding the ongoing (Tomkins, 1984), and adaptational encounters
match between organism and environment (Lazarus, 1991a), include each of the protago-
(Schwarz & Clore, 1983). In addition to their nists and all of the events in a given emotional
intraorganismic functions, emotions also serve scene (Forgas, 1982). For example, the emotion
social functions. They inform us about others' of anger involves acute changes in posture,
behavioral intentions (Fridlund, 1994), give us facial movements, tone of voice, verbal expres-
clues as to whether something is good or bad sion, experience, and autonomic responding.
(Walden, 1991), and script our social behavior The emotion episode of anger includes all of
(Averill, 1980; Keltner & Buswell, 1997). these things as well as the instigator, the social
Enthusiasm for functional analyses of emo- context, and the whole sequence of responses
tion should not blind us to James's observation and recriminations as they emerge in the
that emotional response tendencies often need to ongoing interaction (see Averill, 1982).
be modulated. Indeed, inherent in the notion of a Emotions also may be distinguished from
response tendency is the idea that a response moods (Parkinson, Totterdell, Briner, & Rey-
tendency is only one of many determinants of nolds, 1996). One distinguishing feature is
behavior. In the discussion of emotion regula- duration (Nowlis & Nowlis, 1956); mood is the
tion that follows, I draw on James's response- "pervasive and sustained 'emotional climate,'"
tendency perspective on emotion. First, however, and emotions are "fluctuating changes in
I clarify several important distinctions among emotional 'weather'" (APA, 1994, p. 763). A
terms that often are used interchangeably. second distinguishing feature is that emotions
typically have specific objects and give rise to
Relations With Related Constructs behavioral response tendencies relevant to these
objects (Frijda, 1993; Isen, 1984; Lazarus,
All manner of distinctions have been made in 1991a). By contrast, moods are more diffuse
an attempt to bring order to the "conceptual and (Morris, 1989), and although they may give rise
definitional chaos" that characterizes emotion to broad action tendencies such as approach or
research (Buck, 1990, p. 330). Many of these withdrawal (Lang, 1995), moods bias cognition
distinctions are idiosyncratic. However, a few more than they bias action (Davidson, 1994;
distinctions have broader currency, including Fiedler, 1988). Recently, several investigators
those made among affect, emotion, emotion have formulated a hierarchical view that inte-
episodes, and mood. grates emotions and moods (Diener, Smith, &
In some contexts, affect and emotion are used Fujita, 1995; Watson & Clark, 1992). This view
interchangeably. In others, affect is used to refer holds that specific emotions are lower order
to the experiential (Buck, 1993; MacLean, elements within higher order valenced mood
1990) or behavioral (American Psychiatric categories. In the context of emotion regulation,
Association [APA], 1994; Kaplan & Sadock,
1991) components of emotion. Following 1The process model of emotion generation presented in
Scherer (1984), I use affect as the superordinate Figure 1 is a distillation of major points of convergence
across emotionresearchersincludingArnold (1960), Ekman
category for valenced states, including emotions (1972), Izard (1977), Lazarus (1991a), Levenson (1994),
such as anger and sadness, emotion episodes Leventhal (1984), Plutchik (1980), Scherer (1984), and
such as a barroom brawl and delivering bad Tomkins(1962).
274 GROSS

however, differences in the response tendencies unconscious processes that increase or decrease
that are associated with moods and emotions the experience or expression of negative or
suggest the need for maintaining this distinction. positive emotions (Mayer & Salovey, 1995;
I therefore focus primarily on the regulation of Parrott, 1993). Methodologically, correlational
emotion rather than affect, emotion episodes, or and experimental approaches have taken the
mood. 2 place of the clinical method. Researchers still
view difficulties with emotion regulation as
What Is Emotion Regulation? being central to psychopathology (Cicchetti,
Ackerman, & Izard, 1995; Gross & Munoz,
From time immemorial, people have won- 1995); however, they now pay greater attention
dered how to manage their emotions. Only in the to normative emotion regulatory processes.
past 2 decades, however, has the field of emotion The stress and coping tradition is a second
regulation begun to emerge as a relatively important precursor to contemporary emotion-
independent research domain. Now that we have regulation research. The organizing principle in
a working definition of emotion, we can address this tradition is that organisms produce similar
the topic of emotion regulation. In the following psychophysiological responses to diverse chal-
sections, I consider two precursors to the lenges (Selye, 1956; see also Sapolsky, 1994).
contemporary study of emotion regulation. I Early researchers focused on responses to
then use a response-tendency perspective to physical challenges such as cold or crowding.
define emotion regulation. Later researchers expanded their focus to
include responses to psychological challenges
Precursors to the Contemporary Study such as public speaking or exams. Although
of Emotion Regulation psychological stress and coping research has its
roots in the psychoanalytic tradition, it is
The psychoanalytic tradition is one important distinguished by a concern with adaptive,
precursor to the contemporary study of emotion conscious coping processes, and by a focus on
regulation. This tradition emphasizes two types situational rather than person variables (Parker
of anxiety regulation (S. Freud, 1926/1959). The & Endler, 1996). Coping is defined as "cogni-
first concerns reality-based anxiety, which arises tive and behavioral efforts to manage specific
when situational demands overwhelm the ego. external and/or internal demands that are
Here, anxiety regulation consists of avoiding appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources
such situations in the future, even to the point of of the person" (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984,
excessive behavioral constriction. The second p. 141). Researchers have distinguished be-
type of anxiety regulation concerns id- and tween problem-focused coping, which aims to
superego-based anxiety, which arises when solve the problem, and emotion-focused coping,
strong impulses press for expression. Here, which aims to decrease negative emotion
anxiety regulation consists of curtailing the experience. Emotion regulation researchers have
expression of impulses that the ego judges will borrowed heavily from the stress and coping
create high levels of future anxiety. Ego defense tradition. However, by examining specific emo-
is the general term given to processes that tions, they have sought to make finer grained
regulate these two types of anxiety as well as distinctions among environment-organism inter-
other painful negative affects (Paulhus, Fridhand- actions than is possible using the broader rubric
ler, & Hayes, 1997). Typically, ego defenses of stress. Emotion regulation researchers also
operate outside of awareness (Erdelyi, 1993). have emphasized that both positive and negative
Individuals have characteristic defensive styles emotions may be regulated, and that both
that differ in reality distortion, impairment, emotion expression and experience may be
energy consumption, and unnecessary nongrati- targeted. Although traditional definitions of
fication of impulses (Fenichel, 1945; A. Freud,
1946; Haan, 1977; Vaillant, 1977). Emotion
regulation researchers remain concerned with 2Mayer,Salovey,Gomberg-Kaufman,and Blainey(1991)
reducing negative emotion experience through have proposed a conceptionof mood that subsumes mood
regulation.Thissubstantiallyenlargesthe traditionalconcep-
behavioral or mental control. However, the tion of mood. I believeit may be moreusefulto distinguish
focus has expanded to include conscious and regulatoryprocessesfromthe targetsof regulation.
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTIONREGULATION 275

coping overlap with contemporary conceptions and cortical centers that regulate emotion is
of emotion regulation, coping and emotion important. It forces us to move beyond simple
regulation are by no means redundant. Coping models of top-down control (Head, 1921;
includes nonemotional actions taken to achieve Jackson, 1884) to models that emphasize
nonemotional goals (Scheier, Weinbtraub, & emotional tuning of higher brain centers (Derry-
Carver, 1986) as well as actions taken to berry & Tucker, 1992) as well as more
regulate emotions. Emotion regulation includes traditional top-down control.
processes that may or may not tax the individu- I focus on five aspects of this definition of
al's resources, as well as processes not tradition- emotion regulation. First, individuals increase,
ally considered in the coping literature, such as maintain, and decrease negative and positive
sustaining or augmenting positive emotions (but emotions (Parrott, 1993). All of these processes
see Folkman, 1997). are included in my definition of emotion
regulation. Second, neural emotion circuits do
Defining Emotion Regulation not appear to overlap completely (LeDoux,
1994; Panksepp, 1982; 1998). This suggests that
What, then, is emotion regulation? Emotion circuits involved in regulating these emotions
regulation refers to the processes by which also may not overlap completely, and that there
individuals influence which emotions they have, may be important differences in emotion regula-
when they have them, and how they experience tory processes across emotions. Third, this
and express these emotions. Emotion regulatory definition of emotion regulation emphasizes
processes may be automatic or controlled, regulation in self. Other definitions include
conscious or unconscious, and may have their attempts to influence others' emotions (e.g.,
effects at one or more points in the emotion Gross & Levenson, 1993; Masters, 1991;
generative process (which I describe in a later Thompson, 1994). I now believe this double
section). Because emotions are multicomponen- usage is unfortunate, as it mixes two potentially
tial processes that unfold over time, emotion quite different sets of motives, goals, and
regulation involves changes in "emotion dynam- processes. Fourth, prototypic examples of emo-
ics" (Thompson, 1990), or the latency, rise time, tion regulation are conscious, such as deciding
magnitude, duration, and offset of responses in to change an upsetting conversational topic or
behavioral, experiential, or physiological do- squelching laughter at a child's inappropriate
mains. Emotion regulation also involves changes antics. One can imagine, however, emotion
in how response components are interrelated as regulatory activity that occurs without con-
the emotion unfolds, such as when large scious awareness, such as hiding one's disap-
increases in physiological responding occur in pointment at an unattractive present (Cole,
the absence of overt behavior. 1986) or turning one's attention away from
This perspective on emotion regulation treats potentially upsetting material (Boden &
the nervous system as multiple, partially indepen- Baumeister, 1997). Previous discussions have
dent information processing subsystems (e.g., distinguished categorically between conscious
Fodor, 1983; Gazzaniga, 1985; LeDoux, 1989; and unconscious processes (Masters, 1991;
Maclean, 1975; Malmo, 1975; Panksepp, 1982). Mayer & Salovey, 1995). I prefer to think of a
Subsystems work with differing inputs, and continuum from conscious, effortful, and con-
often provide different outputs, even given the trolled regulation to unconscious, effortless, and
same input. Imagining a provocation can automatic regulation (Shiffrin & Schneider,
produce anger, even when we know that there is 1977). Fifth, I make no a priori assumptions as
no threat (Lang, 1979). Similarly, seeing a roach to whether emotion regulation is good or bad
in our soup can produce feelings of disgust and (Thompson & Calkins, 1996). This circumvents
fear of disease, even when we know the roach the confusion that was created in the stress and
has been sterilized (Rozin & Fallon, 1987). coping literature by predefining defenses as
Interconnected neural subsystems monitor one maladaptive and coping as adaptive (Parker &
another to varying degrees and are in continuous Endler, 1996). Thus, cognitive strategies that
bidirectional excitatory or inhibitory interaction. dampen negative emotions may permit medical
The notion that there are bidirectional links professionals to operate successfully (Lief &
between limbic centers that generate emotion Fox, 1963; Smith & Kleinman, 1989). The same
276 GROSS

strategies, however, may be used to dehumanize Emotion Regulation Across


an enemy and neutralize empathic distress that Psychological Subdisciplines
could interfere with state-sanctioned killing
(Bandura, 1977). Emotion regulation cuts across traditional
subdisciplinary boundaries. 3As shown in Figure
3, each of the major subfields of psychology
Relations With Related Constructs contributes to an understanding of emotion
regulation. The field of emotion regulation
Constructs related to emotion regulation therefore provides important common ground in
include coping, mood regulation, mood repair, an age of fractionation and specialization. In the
defense, and affect regulation. Coping is distin- following sections, I consider emotion regula-
guished from emotion regulation by its primary tion from the perspective of biological, cogni-
focus on decreasing negative emotion experi- tive, developmental, social, personality, clinical,
ence and by its use of the protracted organism- and health psychology. Recognizing that each
situation interaction----or emotion episode--as section could grow into a full-length review, I
the preferred unit of analysis. Moods are illustrate, rather than fully review, the contribu-
distinguished from emotions by their less well tions these seven subfields have made or could
defined behavioral response tendencies. Thus in make to the study of emotion regulation.
comparison with emotion regulation, mood
regulation and mood repair are more concerned Biological Psychology
with altering emotion experience than emotion
behavior (Forgas, 1995; Parkinson et al., 1996; One challenge that faces biological psychol-
Thayer, 1996). Like coping, defenses typically ogy in general, and the allied discipline of
have as their focus the regulation of negative affective neuroscience in particular (Davidson
emotion experience, particularly anxiety. De- & Sutton, 1995; Panksepp, 1991; 1998), is to
fenses usually are unconscious (Bond, Gardner, elucidate the neural substrate of emotion regula-
Christian, & Sigal, 1983) and are studied as tion. Researchers now think that bidirectional
stable individual differences rather than as pathways (Mega & Cummings, 1994) between
processes. The construct of affect regulation has prefrontal cortex and subcortical emotion-
been used in a variety of ways (e.g., Taylor, generative structures modulate subcortical activ-
Bagby, & Parker, 1997; Westen, 1994). In ity (LeDoux, 1987; MacLean, 1990; Ploog,
keeping with the broad conception of affect 1992) and suffuse cortical information process-
described above, Figure 2 depicts affect regula- ing with emotional meaning (Damasio, 1994).
tion as superordinate to coping, emotion regula- Evidence implicating the prefrontal cortex in
tion, mood regulation, and traditional ego- emotion regulation comes both from lesion and
defensive processes. These closely related developmental studies. Individuals who have
constructs have permeable boundaries, but I lesions involving the prefrontal region tend to be
conceptualize emotion regulation as one of emotionally impulsive and poorly affectively
several major forms of affect regulation. regulated (e.g., Kolb & Taylor, 1990; Rolls,
Hornak, Wade, & McGrath, 1994; Stuss &
Benson, 1986; Tucker, Luu, & Pribram, 1995).
Developmentally, too, a correlation has been
Affect Regulation reported between structural changes in the
prefrontal cortex that take place toward the end
of the first year and the emergence of rudimen-
tary forms of emotion regulation (Dawson,

3I focus on psychological research, although other


disciplines also have made important contributions to the
study of emotion regulation, including philosophy (e.g.,
Solomon, 1976), sociology (e.g., Hochschild, 1979), anthro-
pology (e.g., White, 1993), and economics (e.g., Loewen-
Figure 2. A hierarchical conception of affect regulation. stein, 1996).
SPECIALISSUE: EMOTIONREGULATION 277

sona,i
~ ocial~

J
I motionRe ,,1,,tion I
J oa,O
Figure3. Emotionregulationand sevensubfieldsof psychology.

Panagiotides, Klinger, & Hill, 1992; Diamond, 1997)? Answers to questions regarding the
1991). neural bases of emotion regulation will provide
There is much to learn, however, about the a much needed biological foundation for future
precise nature of central (e.g., Diamond, 1991) theoretical and empirical analyses.
and peripheral (e.g., Fabes & Eisenberg, 1997;
Porges, 1995) mechanisms that mediate emotion Cognitive Psychology
regulation. Are emotional impulses regulated by
the same mechanisms that provide control over Long cool to emotion, cognitive psychology
other prepotent impulses (APA, 1994; Lion, has begun to show an appreciation of the rich
1992)? Or are regulatory structures specific not interdigitation of cognitive and affective pro-
only to various classes of impulses (e.g., cesses or, as Lewis and colleagues put it, the
emotional, appetitive, aversive) but even to the "cognitive-emotional fugue" (Lewis, Sullivan,
individual emotions themselves? Hints of speci- & Michalson, 1984). Researchers have demon-
ficity have emerged, but researchers have yet to strated complex dependencies between affective
agree about even such basic issues as whether processes and cognitive processes such as
the left or right prefrontal cortex is preferentially problem solving (e.g., Clore, 1994; Isen, Daub-
charged with the regulation of negative versus man, & Nowicki, 1987), learning (e.g., Mineka,
positive emotion (e.g., Dawson et al., 1992; Fox, Davidson, Cook, & Keir, 1984; Ohman, 1986),
1994b; Tucker et al., 1995). Effective regulation and memory (e.g., Blaney, 1986; Cahill, Prins,
requires feedback from the monitored system, Weber, & McGaugh, 1994; Chdstianson, 1992).
and there are multiple internal and external Research directly relevant to emotion regula-
sources of information about emotional respond- tion has been limited. However, Wegner and
ing (Pennebaker & Roberts, 1992). However, colleagues recently have begun to clarify the
little is known about how emotional response cognitive underpinnings of one form of emotion
tendencies are represented at various levels of regulation (Wegner, Erber, & Zanakos, 1993).
the neuroaxis. How might these representations They have shown that attempts to regulate
influence emotion regulation? Is the capacity to negative emotions via thought suppression yield
verbally label emotional response tendencies paradoxical increases in negative mood if
necessary for certain forms of emotion regula- cognitive load is high. Wegner (1994) hypoth-
tion (Feldman Barrett, in press; Lane, Ahem, esizes that when cognitive resources are limited,
Schwartz, & Kaszniak, 1997; Taylor et al., the conscious operating system that seeks out
278 GROSS

desired mental contents is out-performed by a al., 1995; Rubin, Coplan, Fox, & Calkins,
less cognitively costly monitoring system that 1995). Developmental psychologists also have
flags undesirable mental contents. Several re- explored children's developing conceptions of
cent studies have assessed the cognitive conse- and capacities for emotion regulation (e.g.,
quences of emotion regulation more directly. In Meerum Terwogt & Stegge, 1995). This litera-
two studies, Richards and Gross (in press) found ture has focused on children's emerging under-
that emotion suppression impaired memory for standing that they can--and often should--
auditory information that had been presented control their emotions (e.g., Cole, Zahn-Waxler,
during an emotion-eliciting slide-viewing task. & Smith, 1994; Harris, 1989; Kopp, 1989;
In a complementary study, Baumeister (in press) Saarni, 1990). Developmental psychologists
found that both emotion suppression and have focused primarily on infancy and early
exaggeration impaired performance on subse- childhood. Recently, life-span theorists also
quent cognitive tasks such as anagram solving. have begun to study emotion regulatory pro-
Because emotion regulation involves both atten- cesses. For example, Carstensen and colleagues
tion allocation and cognitive processing, cogni- have found that the salience of emotion
tive psychology is uniquely equipped to probe regulatory goals increases with age (Carstensen,
these processes. 1995) and that emotional control may actually
increase with age (Gross et al., 1997). One
Developmental Psychology important challenge is to chart the developmen-
tal course of emotion regulation across the life
Recognizing that self-regulation is the founda- span.
tion of organized behavior (Maccoby, 1980),
developmental psychologists have paid consid- Social Psychology
erable attention to emotion regulation (e.g.,
Bridges & Grolnick, 1995; Eisenberg & Fabes, Social psychology might seem irrelevant to a
1992a; Fox, 1994a; Garber & Dodge, 1991). conception of emotion that prioritizes internal
One key discovery is temperamental differences action tendencies. However, response tenden-
in emotion and emotion regulation (Derryberry cies are inextricably linked to social context
& Rothbart, 1997). Some children have lower (Buck, 1984, 1994); classic investigations of
thresholds for negative or positive affect than do authority (Milgram, 1974) and deindividuation
others (e.g., Davidson, 1992; Derryberry & (Zimbardo, 1969) have revealed just how far
Rothbart, 1984; Fox, 1989). Likewise, some emotional responses can be shaped by social
children have better emotion regulatory capaci- context. More recently, social psychologists
ties, such as self-soothing, than others (Rothbart have explored the social foundations of emo-
& Derryberry, 1981). Under the rubric of social tional processes in studies of independent versus
referencing, researchers have considered how interdependent cultures (e.g., Markus & Kita-
adult emotion-expressive behavior alters chil- yama, 1991; Weisz, Rothbaum, & Blackburn,
dren's ongoing behavior (Bandura, 1992; Cam- 1984). By considering social context, research-
pos & Stenberg, 1981), and Block (1971) has ers will transcend inner or "push" models of
described the importance of parental investment emotion, and develop more sophisticated mod-
in ego control. From an early age, nonverbal els that emphasize both "push" and "pull"
guidance is richly supplemented by emotion talk factors (Ekman, 1972; Kappas, 1996).
(Dunn & Brown, 1991; Hooven, Gottman, & Such models are clearly necessary to under-
Katz, 1995). One important question is how stand the emotion regulatory bases of important
others' emotion regulatory suggestions interact social processes such as helping behavior,
with temperamental factors to shape what self-handicapping, marital interaction, and disso-
children say to themselves when they are on nance reduction. For example, Cialdini and
their own (Kopp, 1982; Luria, 1961). colleagues' negative-state relief model (Cialdini,
The interaction between response tendencies Darby, & Vincent, 1973) holds that helping
and regulatory capacities has been explored in behavior is motivated by the desire to decrease
the context of attachment (e.g., Belsky & one's own sadness (see Batson & Shaw, 1991;
Rovine, 1987; Dozier & Kobak, 1992; Thomp- Cialdini & Fulz, 1990). But when does an
son, 1990) and social competence (Eisenberg et individual alter a situation in order to decrease
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 279

another person's negative emotion-expressive 1993). The study of emotion regulatory pro-
behavior that is upsetting (Hoffman, 1981) cesses forms a natural part of this inquiry.
rather than deflecting attention (Shaw, Batson, But personality psychology is more than the
& Todd, 1994)? Social handicapping, or actions study of basic personality processes. It also is
taken to excuse failure or magnify credit for concerned with individual differences. Under-
success (Berglas & Jones, 1978), may be framed standing emotion regulatory processes requires
as a trade-off between optimally effective action a working conception of what is being regulated
and anticipatory emotion regulation (Baumeis- in a particular person at a particular time.
ter & Scher, 1988). In order to feel proud, or to Research on individual differences in emotion
avoid feeling shame, individuals may introduce has focused on emotional expression (e.g.,
obstacles or withdraw effort, thereby diminish- Gross & John, 1997, 1998; Kring, Smith, &
ing the objective probability of success. Emo- Neale, 1994), experience (e.g., Feldman, 1995;
tion regulatory processes also figure promi- Lane & Schwartz, 1987; Larsen & Ketelaar,
nently in marital interaction: Gottman (1993) 1991), and physiological responding (e.g., Fox,
suggested that marital interaction styles are 1989; Goldsmith, 1993; Porges, 1995), as well
crafted so as to achieve a certain ratio of positive as on the interrelations among response systems
to negative emotion. The motivating power of (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 1992). Unfortunately, a
negative-emotion reduction also is relevant to consideration of individual differences in emo-
cognitive dissonance processes, originally de- tion regulation is complicated by the large
scribed as efforts undertaken to reconcile two number of terms that have been used (John,
logically inconsistent cognitions (Festinger, 1990). These include emotional control (Roger
1957). Although typically framed in cognitive & Najarian, 1989), negative-mood regulation
terms, dissonance reduction can be conceptual- (Catanzaro & Mearns, 1990), repression (Wein-
ized in terms of individuals' attempts to berger, 1990), and rumination-distraction
diminish negative emotions engendered by (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1993). Related constructs
self-relevant discrepancies (see Abselson, 1983; include monitoring-blunting (Miller, 1987),
J. Cooper & Fazio, 1984; Steele, 1988; Swann, sensation seeking (Zuckerman, 1979), construc-
1987; Tesser & Cornell, 1991). Because emo- tive thinking (Epstein & Meier, 1989), optimism
tion regulation is almost always a social affair, (Scheier & Carver, 1985), impulsivity (Eysenck
social psychology will play a vital role in & Eysenck, 1969), behavioral inhibition (Ka-
emotion regulation research. gan, Reznick, & Snidman, 1988), constraint
(Tellegen, 1985), ambivalence over emotional
expressivity (King & Emmons, 1990), delay of
Personality Psychology gratification (Mischel, 1974), alexithymia (G. J.
Taylor et al., 1997), levels of emotional
Personality psychology is centrally concerned awareness (Lane, Quinlan, Schwartz, Walker, &
with agency (Bandura, 1982). An agentic Zeitlin, 1990), coping style (Carver, Scheier, &
perspective acknowledges the role of social Weintraub, 1989), ego control (Block & Block,
factors, such as those described in the previous 1980), and emotional intelligence (Salovey,
section, but emphasizes the active role that Hsee, & Mayer, 1993). Some constructs, such as
individuals play in shaping their own behavior rumination, focus on specific regulatory pro-
and the world around them. Much of the cesses. Other constructs, such as emotional
research inspired by this perspective has focused intelligence, include diverse processes such as
on perceptions of environmental control (Roth- "the verbal and nonverbal appraisal and expres-
baum et al., 1982; Seligman, 1975); perceived sion of emotion, the regulation of emotion in the
coping efficacy now is known to affect a wide self and others, and the utilization of emotional
range of outcomes, including anxiety and context in problem solving" (Mayer & Salovey,
depression (for a review, see Bandura, 1997). 1993, p. 433). This wealth of constructs testifies
Recently, analyses of control over external to the importance of individual differences in
events have been complemented by analyses of emotion regulation. However, one important
control over internal psychological processes contribution will be to further specify the
such as thoughts and emotions (Bandura, 1997; emotion regulatory processes that underlie each
Logan & Cowan, 1984; Wegner & Pennebaker, of these individual-difference constructs.
280 GROSS

Clinical Psychology must be re-experienced in therapy (Cicchetti et


al., 1991; Greenberg & Safran, 1987) where the
Emotion regulatory processes are central to therapist can help the patient develop the
mental health; they can either support or disrupt capacity to regulate emotions in new ways
the capacity to work, relate to others, and enjoy (Averill & Nunley, 1992; Folkman & Lazarus,
oneself (Gross & Munoz, 1995). Indeed, emo- 1988). Establishing what constitutes "appropri-
tion dysregulation is implicated in over half of ate" emotion regulation is an important chal-
the DSM-1V Axis I disorders and in all of the lenge for clinicians who must help patients
Axis II disorders (APA, 1994; Gross & Leven- examine what their implicit emotion regulatory
son, 1997; Thoits, 1985). In adults, emotion goals are, what they would like them to be, and
dysregulation is associated with clinical prob- how they may best achieve these goals.
lems including binge eating (e.g., Lingswiler,
Crowther, & Stephens, 1989), alcohol abuse Health Psychology
(e.g., M. L. Cooper, Frone, Russell, & Mudar,
1995; Marlatt, 1985; Sayette, 1993), and of Health psychologists long have maintained
course anxiety and the mood disorders (e.g., that mismanaging negative emotions can cause
Barlow, 1986; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emory, illness (Alexander & French, 1946; Dunbar,
1979). In children, difficulties regulating depres- 1954; Friedman, 1990). Some evidence now
sion and anxiety are related to internalizing supports this claim. Chronic hostility and anger
disorders, whereas difficulties regulating anger inhibition are associated with hypertension and
are related to externalizing disorders (e.g., coronary heart disease (e.g., Dembroski, Mac-
Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt, & Silva, 1995; Dougal, Williams, Haney, & Blumenthal, 1985;
Rubin et al., 1995). Within the normal range of Jorgensen, Johnson, Kolodziej, & Schreer,
functioning, poor emotion regulation in conjunc- 1996; Julkunen, Salonen, Kaplan, Chesney, &
tion with high levels of negative emotion Salonen, 1994; T. W. Smith, 1992; but see Suls,
predicts lesser social competence and decreased Wan, & Costa, 1995). Emotion inhibition also
peer acceptance and liking (Eisenberg & Fabes, may exacerbate minor ailments (Pennebaker,
1992b). A shared focus on emotion regulation 1990; Pennebaker, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser,
unites developmental and clinical psychology in 1988) and may even accelerate cancer progres-
the study of attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisor- sion (Fawzy et al., 1993; Gross, 1989; Spiegel,
der (Barkley, 1997; Hinshaw, Simmer, & Heller, Bloom, Kraemer, & Gottheil, 1989). The theme
1995), Down's syndrome (Thompson, 1991), that unites these findings is that tight control of
and child maltreatment (Cicchetti et al., 1991). negative emotions may adversely affect physical
Emotion dysregulation--particularly high lev- health.
els of poorly regulated hostility--has effects that How might emotional control affect physical
extend beyond the dysregulated individual. health? One possible mechanism is sustained
Vulnerable family members, such as those physiological responding that exceeds meta-
recently hospitalized for schizophrenia (Ka- bolic demands (Folkow, 1987; Steptoe, 1981;
vanagh, 1992) or depression (Coiro & Gottes- R. B. Williams, 1986). Relative to natural
man, 1996), are at elevated risk for relapse if emotional expression, suppression leads to
their family environment is characterized by increased sympathetic activation despite a con-
high levels of negative-emotion expression. comitant decrease in somatic activity (Gross &
Interventions to help individuals, couples, and Levenson, 1993; 1997). However, there is an
families modify ineffective patterns of emotion immense difference between acute changes in
regulation are the staple of psychotherapy. Such sympathetic tone and clinically significant
interventions target emotion regulatory patterns pathology. This gap needs to be bridged by
ranging from those that influence the situation studies that link the short- and long-term
(e.g., Lewinsohn, Munoz, Youngren, & Zeiss, consequences of emotion regulation. A second
1986) or the way the situation is construed (e.g., possible mechanism is immune suppression.
Dodge, 1991; Ellis, 1962) to those that alter the Increased sympathetic nervous system activa-
emotional response itself (e.g., Deffenbacher, tion appears to selectively inhibit certain aspects
1994; Novaco, 1975). Many schools of therapy of the immune response (e.g., Maier, Watkins, &
teach that difficulties with emotion regulation Fleshner, 1994). In principle, selective down-
SPECIALISSUE: EMOTIONREGULATION 281

regulation of immune parameters might, over infinite number of behavioral acts that might
the longer term, lead to greater incidence of qualify as affect regulatory. Although descrip-
illness. Here, too, however, much more needs to tion of behavior is an important first step, it
be done before researchers can draw any firm eventually may prove to be too low a level of
conclusions. Given the myriad forms of emotion analysis.
regulation, the important role of incompletely A second approach is to categorize emotion
defined individual differences in emotion genera- regulatory efforts on the basis of the emotion
tion and regulation, and the multiple pathways component targeted for regulation, such as
to good or poor health, documenting the experience, expression, or physiology (e.g.,
long-term health consequences of various forms Walden & Smith, 1997). This approach has the
of emotion regulation is a task as daunting as it advantage of parsimony. However, it has the
is important. disadvantage of lumping diverse ways of
achieving change in each domain. For example,
Emotion Regulatory Processes inhibiting emotion-expressive behavior may be
accomplished by changing the way one thinks
Emotions encode situation-response depen- about a situation or by relaxing one's facial
dencies that have proven valuable over the muscles (Gross, 1998). Grouping these pro-
sweep of millennia (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). cesses together obscures important differences
As the preceding review amply demonstrates, in causes, consequences, and underlying mecha-
however, emotional response tendencies aren't nisms of action. This approach also has the
always appropriate to the situations we now liability that individuals often try to change
face. Contemporary physical and social environ- multiple aspects of the emotion at once rather
ments differ dramatically from those that shaped than just one aspect as, for example, when they
our emotions. Responses that served our ances- want to make the emotion go away altogether.
tors well often are suited poorly to modem Although specification of the target system is
exigencies. For example, technological ad- important, it too may not be quite the right level
vances have magnified the consequences that of analysis.
our emotional responses have for ourselves and A third approach is to undertake a conceptual
others: an angry impulse that once would have analysis of the processes underlying diverse
bruised now kills, thanks to the ready availabil- emotion regulatory acts (see Frijda, 1986, for a
ity of handguns. These considerations suggest more detailed discussion along these lines). In
the importance of emotion regulation. But how the response-tendency conception of emotion
should we conceptualize the potentially over- depicted in Figure 1, emotional response tenden-
whelming number of processes involved in cies are generated once stimuli have been evaluated
regulating emotional response tendencies? as important. Then, once emotional response
One approach is to spell out exactly what tendencies have been generated, they may be
people do when they try to regulate a particular modulated in various ways. Within this frame-
emotion or mood. For example, Rippere (1977) work, emotion regulatory acts may be seen as
asked participants what they thought a person having their primary impact at different points in
should do if that person felt depressed. Thayer the emotion generative process. Of course, what
and colleagues (Thayer, Newman, & McClain, individuals do to regulate their emotions--such
1994) asked participants in more general terms as going out to a bar with friends in order to get
what they do to change their moods. In a similar their mind off an upsetting fight with a
vein, Parkinson and colleagues (Parkinson et al., coworker--often involves multiple regulatory
1996) identified over 200 mood regulatory processes. However, a process-oriented ap-
strategies using both interview and question- proach may bring us closer to understanding the
naire methods. The descriptive approach used causes, consequences, and underlying mecha-
by these researchers elicits reports of behaviors nisms than the other two approaches.
ranging from exercise to drinking to seeking I distinguish five sets of emotion regulatory
social support. It has the advantage of staying processes: situation selection, situation modifica-
close to the phenomenon of interest. However, tion, attention deployment, cognitive change,
when the focus is broad--on affective processes and response modulation. This is an elaboration
including mood and emotion--there is an of the two-way distinction I have offered previ-
282 GROSS

ously between antecedent-focused emotion regu- the many possible meanings (ml, m2, m3) will
lation, which occurs before the emotion is be attached to a situation. It is this meaning (m2)
generated, and response-focused emotion regu- that gives rise to emotional response tendencies,
lation, which occurs after the emotion is including behavioral, experiential, and physi-
generated (Gross, 1998; Gross & Munoz, 1995). ological tendencies. Response modulation refers
To orient the reader, I briefly describe these five to influencing these response tendencies once
forms of emotion regulation before going into they have been elicited, illustrated in Figure 4 by
more detail below. decreased behavioral response tendencies (B-
The first is situation selection, shown in rather than B).
Figure 4 by the solid line toward Situation 2 ($2) The process model of emotion regulation
rather than Situation 1 (S1). Once selected, a shown in Figure 4 has clear limitations. First,
situation may be tailored so as to modify its situations, aspects, and meanings are terms that
emotional impact. This constitutes situation have proven notoriously resistant to definition.
modification Importantly, situations differ in What is offered is a set of working distinctions
terms of how much they may be modified, among regulatory processes that I have found
ranging from a hypothetical limiting case of a useful in navigating the tangled literatures
situation with no room for change (denoted by relevant to emotion regulation. Second, re-
Slx) to a situation with modest potential for sponse tendencies may be modulated in more
change (S2x, S2y, S2z). Situations also vary in subtle ways than increasing and decreasing
complexity, ranging from a hypothetical limit- them; Figure 4 illustrates rather than exhausts
ing case of a situation with one aspect (al) to a the possibilities. Third, there may be important
situation with multiple aspects (al, a2, a3, a4, asymmetries in control across response do-
a5). Attentional deployment may be used to mains. For example, it may be easier to directly
select which aspect of a situation a person modulate emotion-expressive behavior than
focuses on. Even after a situation has been emotion experience. Fourth, this scheme calls to
selected, modified, and selectively attended to, it mind external situations. However, I mean to
still is possible to alter its emotional impact. include internal "situations" also, in which case
Cognitive change refers to selecting which of attentional deployment may be used to select

Situations Aspects Meanings Responses

.R
SI . . . . . . . . . S i x . . . . . . . . . al . . . . . . . . .

.) al
ml
* Behavioral ~ B+
B
B-
.. "* S2x . . -""
" . . - ") a2
$2 "~. ) S2y'~'"') a3 ..... ) ml Emotional .--~ E+
. . . . S2z ' - . ' : 7 " ~ a4 ,"" ) m2 Response * Experiential "==~E
"l a5 .... m3 Tendencies E-

...~, p+
* Physiological
...
"-~p.

Situation Situation Attentional Cognitive Response


Selection Modification Deployment Change Modulation

I I I
Antecedent-Focused Response-Focused
Emotion Regulation Emotion Regulation

Figure 4. A process model o f emotion regulation. (See text above for explanation of
abbreviations.)
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 283

and modify imagined situations. Fifth, emo- 1986). Likewise, a sensation seeker's thrill
tional responses often modify situations, particu- seeking may lead to injury (Zuckerman, 1979).
laxly interpersonal situations. Emotions also Because of the complexity of these tradeoffs,
may give rise to attentional and cognitive effective situation selection may require the help
changes. Emotional responses thus may be seen of caring others, ranging from parents to
as the starting point for the next iteration of the partners to therapists. Indeed, one potent class of
S-O--R sequence depicted in Figure 4. Feedback cognitive-behavioral interventions involves
pathways certainly could be introduced. Sixth, coaching regarding the situations to seek out or
emotion regulation can occur in parallel at avoid, such as when a therapist helps a
multiple points in the emotion generative depressed patient plan pleasant activities (Lewin-
process, and regulatory processes are likely to sohn et al., 1986). Another important class of
be adjusted dynamically. Seventh, reflecting cognitive-behavioral interventions involves help-
current limitations in our knowledge, this
ing with stimulus control, which refers to the
scheme is silent regarding important topics such
regulation of behavior by anticipatory stimulus
as (a) the mechanisms subserving different
selection (Kanfer & Gaelick, 1986), such as
emotion regulatory processes, (b) the relations
when an individual avoids food cues in order not
among emotion regulatory processes, (c) the
to eat unwanted items (Schachter, 1968).
relative efficacy of different forms of emotion
regulation, and (d) the interdependencies among
behavioral, experiential, and physiological re-
sponse tendencies. With these provisos on the
Situation Modification
table, I turn to a discussion of the five forms of A potentially emotion-eliciting situation--
emotion regulation shown in Figure 4.
whether a flat tire on the way to an important
appointment or loud music next door at 3:00
Situation Selection a.m.--does not ineluctably call forth emotion.
One may convert a meeting into a phone
Situation selection refers to approaching or conference, or convince a neighbor to tone down
avoiding certain people, places, or objects in a raucous party. Active efforts to directly modify
order to regulate emotions. Examples include the situation so as to alter its emotional impact
taking a different route to the store to avoid a constitute an important form of emotion regula-
neighbor who tells offensive jokes or seeking
tion. Such efforts have been referred to in the
out a friend with whom one can have a good cry
stress and coping literature as problem-focused
(see Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997). To understand
coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and by
situation selection, one must appreciate the
Rothbaum et al. (1982) as primary control.
features of situations that typically make people
emotional (Scherer, Wallbott, & Summerfield, Boundaries with situation selection are not
1986). One also must appreciate individuals' always clear, as robust efforts to modify a
preferences regarding entertainment (Zillmann, situation may effectively call a new situation
1988), self-gift-giving (Luomala & Laaksonen, into being. Another interesting boundary issue is
1997), and various aggregations of good and bad distinguishing the direct consequences of emo-
news (Linville & Fischer, 1991). Situation tion expression from those of emotion regula-
selection assumes knowledge of likely features tion. Emotional expressions have important
of remote situations and of expectable emotional social consequences and can dramatically alter
responses to these features. However, situations ongoing interactions (Keltner & Kring, 1998). If
are complex, and often have multiple layers of one's partner suddenly looks sad, this can shift
emotional meaning. Self-knowledge is required the trajectory of an angry interaction as one
to make sound decisions about which situations pauses to express concern or even backpeddle.
to seek out and which to avoid, particularly Thus emotion expressions are a potent means of
when short-term benefits of emotion regulation changing ongoing social interaction, and emo-
are pitted against longer term costs. A shy tion regulatory efforts that target situation
person's efforts to decrease anxiety by avoiding modification must be distinguished from the
social situations may provide short-term relief at direct consequences of unregulated emotion
the cost of longer term social isolation (Leary, expression (see Gross, in press).
284 GROSS

Attentional Deployment One intriguing issue is how best to represent


ety. 4
the complex tradeoffs between short-term and
Attentional deployment is one of the first long-term costs and benefits of various attentional
emotion regulatory processes to appear (Rothbart, forms of emotion regulation.
Ziaie, & O'Boyle, 1992). Strategies for changing
attentional focus may be grouped loosely under the Cognitive Change
headings of distraction, concentration, and rumina-
tion. Distraction focuses attention on nonemotional Even after emotion-eliciting features of the
aspects of the situation (Nix, Watson, Pyszczynski, situation have been attended to, an emotional
& Greenberg, 1995) or moves attention away from response is by no means a foregone conclusion.
the immediate situation altogether (Derryberry & Emotion requires that percepts be embued with
Rothbart, 1988), such as when an infant shifts its meaning and that individuals evaluate their
gaze from the emotion-eliciting stimulus to de- capacity to manage the situation. Cognitively
crease stimulation (Stem, 1977; Stifter & Moyer, oriented emotion researchers have described the
1991). Dislraction also may involve changing cognitive steps needed to transform a percept
internal focus, such as when individuals disengage into something that elicits emotion (e.g., Scherer,
from elusive goals by shifting attention to more 1988; C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). Personal-
tractable ones (Mclntosh, 1996). Undesirable asso- ity psychologists have described the evaluations
ciative Wains may be disrupted by saying "stop!" individuals make regarding their capacity to
(Meichenbaum, 1985) or by invoking thoughts manage the perceived situation (e.g., Bandura,
(Fraley & Shaver, 1997) or memories (Joseph- 1997; Folkman & Lazarus, 1988). Cognitive
son, Singer, & Salovey, 1996) that are inconsis- change consists of modifying these cognitive
tent with the undesirable emotional state. For steps or evaluations (Frijda, 1986). Classical
example, repressive coping may involve quickly psychological defenses such as denial, isolation,
deflecting attention away from potentially threat- and intellectualization fall under this broad
ening stimuli (Boden & Baumeister, 1997; Krohne, heading. The tendency to interpret events more
1996). In such cases, attentional deployment positively than warranted also may be included
may effectively be used to select new (internal) (S. E. Taylor & Armor, 1996; S. E. Taylor &
situations, illustrating the permeable boundaries Brown, 1988). Another form of cognitive
among emotion regulation strategies. Concentra- change that has received attention is downward
tion-whether on work, gardening, rock climb- social comparison, which involves comparing
ing, or art has the capacity to absorb cognitive one's situation with that of a less fortunate
resources (Erber & Tesser, 1992); a well-chosen person, thereby altering one's construal and
task can create a self-sustaining transcendent decreasing negative emotion (Taylor & Lobel,
state that Csikszentmihalyi (1975) calls flow. 1989; Wills, 1981). Cognitive reframing may be
Concentration also may be used to draw attention to a particularly potent form of cognitive change,
emotion Iriggers. This is the heart of method acting, such as when one experiences failure with
in which an actor calls to mind an emotional respect to one goal but reframes this as a
incident in order to portray that emotion convinc- success--or at least a nonevent--with respect to
ingly (Stanislavski, 1965). Wegner and Bargh another goal (see Carver et al., 1996). One form
(1998) have termed this controlled starting of of cognitive change that has received particular
emotion. Like the distraction examples above, this attention is reappraisal. This involves cogni-
use of concentration might be considered an internal tively transforming the situation so as to alter its
version of situation selection. Rumination also emotional impact. Leading participants to reap-
involves directed attention, but here attention is praise negatively valenced films has been
directed to feelings and their consequences. Rumi-
nating on the negative emotions characteristic of 4 Worry directs attention toward threatening material,
depression leads to longer and more severe leading to chronic low-grade anxiety. However, worry also
depressive symptoms (Just & Alloy, 1997; Nolen- has a re-evaluative aspect, in that it limits how elaborately
Hoeksema, 1993). Similarly, Borkovec and col- emotional information is processed (Lang, 1979). This
cognitive re-evaluation negatively reinforces worry by
leagues (Borkovec, Roemer, & Kinyon, 1995) have detoxifying negative situations. Worry also may serve to
shown that worrying---or focusing attention on distract from other, more troubling concerns (Borkovek et
possible future threats--leads to long-lasting anxi- al., 1995).
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 285

shown to result in decreased negative emotion ior slightly increases the feeling of that emotion
experience (Cantor & Wilson, 1984; Dandoy & (Izard, 1990; Matsumoto, 1987). Fewer studies
Goldstein, 1990; Gross, 1998; Langer, Janis, & have examined the suppression of ongoing
Wolfer, 1975; Lazarus & Alfert, 1964). How- emotional behavior. However, results to date
ever, in the context of extremely disgusting suggest that inhibiting expressive behavior
films, decreases in physiological responding are decreases self-reported experience of some
not always evident (Gross, 1998; Steptoe & emotions (e.g., pride, amusement) but not others
Vogele, 1986), perhaps because so little cogni- (e.g., disgust, sadness) while increasing sympa-
tive processing is needed in order to translate thetic nervous system responding (for a review,
these images into emotional responses (Lang, see Gross & Levenson, 1997). By seeing what
1979; LeDoux, 1994). This explanation leads to happens as response tendencies are disrupted,
the prediction that in more complex situations, researchers will obtain a better understanding of
reappraisals should have a greater impact on the basic emotional processes (Rinn, 1984). Re-
emotional response. And so they do: Stemmler search on response modulation thus provides an
(1997) has shown that reappraisal decreases important tool for better understanding the
physiological responding in an interpersonal response coherence presumed by the response-
situation in which participants are verbally tendency perspective.
harassed by an experimenter. These studies
suggest the potential for manipulating cognitive-
change strategies in order to probe the emotion Remaining Challenges
generative process.
This review demonstrates the integrative
power of the construct of emotion regulation.
Response Modulation Apparently disparate concerns are brought
Response modulation refers to directly influ- under one conceptual framework, sharpening
encing physiological, experiential, or behavioral questions about points of convergence and
responding. In contrast with the emotion regula- divergence among seemingly unrelated pro-
tory processes described above, response modu- cesses. This review also demonstrates the
lation occurs late in the emotion generative generative power of the construct of emotion
process, after response tendencies have been regulation. In the preceding sections, I touched
initiated. Attempts at regulating the physiologi- on a number of empirical questions. In the
cal and experiential aspects of emotion are following, I consider three challenges that the
common. Drugs may be used to target physi- field still needs to address. Can emotion
ological responses such as muscle tension regulation really be distinguished from emotion
(anti-anxiolytics) or sympathetic hyperreactiv- generation? What are individuals' emotion
ity (beta blockers). Exercise (Thayer et al., regulatory goals? How does emotion regulation
1994), biofeedback (Schwartz, 1975), and relax- relate to other forms of self-regulation?
ation (Suinn & Richardson, 1971; Wolpe, 1958)
also can be used to decrease physiological and
Emotion and Emotion Regulation
experiential aspects of negative emotions such
as anxiety, and Jacobsen's (1938) progressive As is shown in Figure 4, emotion regulatory
relaxation therapy is now an integral component processes are tightly intertwined with emotion
of cognitive-behavioral therapies (Barlow, generative processes. Indeed, some theorists
1986). As is widely appreciated, alcohol (e.g.,
Hull & Bond, 1986), cigarettes (e.g., Brandon,
1994; Gilbert, 1979), cocaine (e.g., Khantzian, 5 Alcohol has both direct and indirect emotion regulatory
1985), and even food (e.g., Lingswiler et al., effects. I have pointed to its direct effects on somatic tension.
1989) also may be used to modify emotion Alcohol also may have indirect emotion regulatory effects
experience. 5 The most common form of emotion via attentional deployment (Hull, 1981). For example, Steele
regulation, however, may be regulating emotion- and Josephs (1990) have argued that alcohol restricts
attention to immediate, salient cues, thereby decreasing
expressive behavior (Gross, Feldman Barrett, & negative emotions if one is engaged by stimuli unrelated to
Richards, 1998). By and large, studies have one's troubles, but increasing negative emotions if one has
shown that initiating emotion-expressive behav- little else to focus on other than one's misery.
286 GROSS

argue that emotion regulation is best seen as part (Ekman, 1972). Importantly, emotion regulatory
and parcel of emotion (Frijda, 1986). After all, goals often are context specific, such as
adult emotions are almost always regulated matching the emotional state of an anticipated
(Tomkins, 1984), and emotion-generative brain interaction partner (Erber, Wegner, & Therriault,
centers appear to be tonically restrained by the 1996). Goals also may vary by emotion. As a
prefrontal cortex (Stuss & Benson, 1986). On culture, Americans have a long history of
the other hand, both common sense and the concern with the management of anger (Steams
response-tendency conception of emotion sug- & Steams, 1986; Tavris, 1982), but other
gest the necessity of distinguishing between emotions such as surprise are less highly
emotion and emotion regulation. regulated. One additional complexity is that the
Disentangling emotion generative and emo- relative standing of emotion regulatory goals
tion regulatory processes represents a tremen- versus other important goals changes over the
dous challenge (Mayer & Salovey, 1995). life span. Carstensen (1995) has argued, for
Emotion regulation must be inferred when an example, that emotion regulatory goals increase
emotional response would have proceeded in in importance relative to other social goals as
one fashion but instead is observed to proceed in individuals perceive time limitations (e.g., as
another. Inferences such as these are fraught they approach death).
with difficulty. For example, an expressionless What are typical emotion regulatory goals?
face in someone who typically expresses and Individuals often seek to decrease negative
gesticulates wildly at the slightest excuse may emotions and increase positive emotions. For
be rich with meaning, but the same lack of example, flight attendants limit hostile feelings
expression in someone who rarely shows any toward unpleasant passengers (Hochschild,
sign of emotion is less strongly suggestive of 1983), and college students enhance positive
emotion regulation. Emotion regulation thus feelings by social sharing (Clark & Isen, 1982;
requires a formidable level of certainty about Langston, 1994). These goals are readily under-
emotion in its unregulated state. Without this, stood in hedonistic terms: people are motivated
discussions of emotion regulation devolve into to avoid pain and seek pleasure. But emotion
postdictions that can explain any pattern of regulation is broader than a simple hedonistic
findings (Kappas, 1996). This is particularly true account suggests. Emotion regulation also in-
when emotion regulation is invoked to explain a volves increasing or initiating negative emo-
lack of emotion where emotion might be tions and decreasing or stopping positive
expected--that is, when two hidden processes, emotions (Masters, 1991; Parrott, 1993; Wegner
emotion and emotion regulation, are invoked to & Bargh, 1998). For example, bill collectors
explain a nonresponse (Frijda, 1986). Because may increase their anger to help collect
certainty about when and how emotions have delinquent accounts (Sutton, 1991), and the
been regulated is low, thresholds for inferring bearers of bad news may limit positive emotions
emotion regulation should be high. as they deliver their unwelcome news (Tesser,
Rosen, & Waranch, 1973). An empirical account
Emotion Regulatory Goals of individuals' emotion regulatory goals is
sorely needed. Such an account would permit a
Like other goals, emotion regulatory goals more complete analysis of the costs and benefits
can be framed at varying levels of abstraction. 6 of different forms of emotion regulation as they
Goals can range from the specific, such as not are used in the service of various goals. This
letting friends see one's amusement at their analysis also would help shed light on how
bawdy jokes, to the general, such as behaving in factors such as fatigue (Baumeister & Heather-
a loving way toward others. These goals may be ton, 1996) and mental load (Wegner, 1994)
conscious and highly salient, such as not differentially compromise an individual's ability
showing nervousness when meeting one's fian- to achieve specific regulatory goals.
cee's parents for the first time. These goals also
may be less focal, such as when they are 6 As notedby Carveret al. (1996),the notionof goals has
embedded in the higher level goal of succeeding enjoyed increased prominence in psychology recently.
Related terms includecurrent concerns (Klinger, 1977)and
at a job that requires compliance with specific personal strivings (Emmons, 1986). I do not mean to
feeling rules (Hochschild, 1979) or display rules distinguish amongthesehere.
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTIONREGULATION 287

Emotion Regulation and Other Forms stronger empirical links across diverse forms of
of Self-Regulation self-regulation.

Emotional impulses are by no means the only Conclusions


impulses with which we must contend. How
does emotion regulation relate to the regulation Wisdom has been defined as "the harmony of
of moods (Carver & Scheier, 1990; Morris & reason and the passions" (Solomon, 1976, p.
Reilly, 1987; Thayer, 1996), appetitive impulses 412). But what is this elusive harmony, and how
(Mischel, 1974; Polivy, 1990), and other prepo- can it be achieved? Over the centuries, some
tent responses (Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996)? have argued that wisdom is only possible when
Are impulses to respond--and the processes by the emotions are silenced and when reason does
which they are modulated--crucially similar, as all the talking (e.g., Seneca, 40-50/1963).
suggested by Block and Block's (1980) concep- Others have taken the opposite view, as did
tion of ego control and by recent discussions of Hume, who asserted that "reason is, and ought
impulsivity (Newman et al., 1993)? Or must only to be, the slave of the passions" (1739/
distinctions among various forms of self- 1969, p. 462). In the past few decades, emotion
regulation be maintained? researchers using William James's (1884) re-
In the domain of affect, models of emotion sponse-tendency conception of emotion gener-
regulation and mood regulation eventually may ally have favored the view that emotions not
take their place in superordinate models of affect only served our ancestors well but serve us well
regulation. However, given that all behavior is too. Emotions thus are said to represent the
arguably affect regulatory in some broad sense, I accumulated "wisdom of the ages" (Lazarus,
believe it may prove useful to retain a somewhat 1991b, p. 820), preparing us to respond to
narrower focus, such as that provided by challenges and opportunities (Frijda, 1986) and
emotion regulation. Distinctions also may be providing us with information about what is
necessary between processes that regulate emo- important and how we are faring with respect to
tion and those that regulate learned responses our goals (Clore, 1994; Damasio, 1994).
such as keypresses (Logan, Schachar, & Tan- But the events that grip us emotionally are not
nock, 1997), reading (J.M.G. Williams, Mathews, always the ones that we should focus our
& MacLeod, 1996), or stereotypes (Devine, energies on, and the behavioral responses that
1989). Presumably, the coordinated set of are primed by emotions are not always the ones
response tendencies postulated in emotion is that are needed to achieve longer term goals
absent, or present in a simplified form, in the (Gross, in pres~). In the past few decades, an
context of these learned responses. It is less exciting new chapter has opened in the ongoing
clear, however, whether processes that regulate debate as to how to achieve just the right sort of
emotional impulses should be distinguished harmony between reason and the emotions.
from those that regulate impulses associated Psychological science has begun to offer empiri-
with hunger, thirst, aggression, and sexual cal insights into individuals' attempts to influ-
arousal (Buck, 1985; Loewenstein, 1996). Misch- ence which emotions they have, when they have
el's (1996) famous "marshmallow studies" of them, and how they experience and express
young children's ability to delay gratification these emotions. To date, researchers typically
highlight the role of attentional processes such have examined emotion regulatory strategies in
as distraction and reframing that also are isolation from one another, often without
relevant to emotion regulation. Similarly, Shift- offering an explicit definition of emotion. In this
man (1984) described cognitive strategies used review, I have outlined an evolutionary perspec-
for smoking abstinence that could just as well be tive that views emotions as response tendencies
used in emotion regulation. What are the and suggested several points in the emotion
differences among regulatory processes across generative process at which emotion might be
families of impulses, ranging from appetitive to regulated. This perspective suggests that well-
aggressive to emotional? This issue is certain to being may be most likely when we (a) regulate
attract continued interest as researchers from a emotion antecedents so that we are emotionally
variety of domains (e.g., weight loss, substance engaged by those pursuits that have enduring
abuse, aggression, emotion regulation) fashion value, (b) attend to and to experience our
288 GROSS

emotions in a richly differentiated fashion so Averill, J. R., & Nunley, E. P. (1992). Voyages of the
that we notice subtle changes in response heart. New York: Free Press.
tendencies, and (c) cultivate the capacity to Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying
modulate emotional response components in a theory of behavioral change. Psychological Re-
variety of ways with a full appreciation of the view, 84, 191-215.
immediate and longer term consequences (Fri- Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in
jda, 1988). human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-
147.
This opens a middle course between silencing
Bandura, A. (1992). Social cognitive theory of social
the emotions and listening to them and to them referencing. In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referenc-
alone. Cooperation between reason and emotion ing and the social construction of reality in infancy
brings our actions into line with our enduring (pp. 175-208). New York: Plenum Press.
concerns, motivating and sustaining action Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of
directed toward longer-term projects in the face control. New York: Freeman.
of temporary setbacks, helping us decide which Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sus-
battles are worth taking up and which to avoid tained attention, and executive functions: Construct-
(Batson, Shaw, & Oleson, 1992). At this early ing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological
stage, our theoretical and empirical grasp of Bulletin, 121, 65-94.
emotion regulation is still quite uncertain, and Barlow, D. H. (1986). Anxiety and its disorders: The
the details of how such an integration of reason nature and treatment of anxiety and panic. New
York: Guilford.
and emotion might be achieved remain obscure.
Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. (1991). Evidence for
Nonetheless, this review of the emerging field of altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives.
emotion regulation suggests that an investment Psychological Inquiry, 2, 107-122.
in this domain will be repaid amply as we Batson, C. D., Shaw, L. L., & Oleson, K. C. (1992).
develop the theoretical models and the empirical Differentiating affect, mood, and emotion. In M. S.
findings needed to provide better answers than Clark (Ed.), Emotion: Review of personality and
have ever before been possible to age-old social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 294-326). New-
questions about how emotions c a n - - a n d bury Park, CA: Sage.
should--be managed in order to optimize Baumeister, R. F. (in press). Ego depletion: Is the self
human functioning. a limited resource? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology.
Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. E (1996).
References Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychologi-
callnquiry, 7, 1-15.
Abelson, R. E (1983). Whatever became of consis- Baumeister, R. F., & Scher, S. J. (1988). Self-
tency theory? Personality and Social Psychology defeating behavior patterns among normal individu-
Bulletin, 9, 37-54. als: Review and analysis of common self-
Alexander, E, & French, T. M. (1946). Psychoana- destructive tendencies. Psychological Bulletin,
lytic therapy: Principles and applications. New 104, 3-22.
York: Ronald Press. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnos- (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New
tic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th York: Guiford.
ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1987). Temperament and
Aristotle. (1941). The basic works of Aristotle (R. attachment security in the Strange Situation: An
McKeon, Ed.). New York: Random House. empirical rapprochement. Child Development, 58,
Arnold, M. (1960). Emotion and personality. New 787-795.
York: Columbia University Press. Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a
Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in self-handicapping strategy in response to non-
time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psycho- contingent success. Journal of Personality and
logical Bulletin, 121, 417-436. Social Psychology, 36, 405-417.
Averill, J. R. (1980). A constructivist view of Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review.
emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229-246.
Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA:
305-339). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Bancroft Books.
Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of
on emotion. New York: Springer. ego-control and ego-resiliency in the organization
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 289

of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Development of referencing. In M. E. Lamb & L. R. Sherrod (Eds.),


Cognition, Affect, and Social Relations: The Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical
Minnesota symposia on chiM psychology (Vol. 13, considerations (pp. 273-314). Hillsdale, NJ:
pp. 39-51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Erlbaum.
Boden, J. M., & Baumeister, R. E (1997). Repressive Cantor, J., & Wilson, B. J. (1984). Modifying fear
coping: Distraction using pleasant thoughts and responses to mass media in preschool and elemen-
memories. Journal of Personality and Social tary school children. Journal of Broadcasting, 28,
Psychology, 73, 45-62. 431-433.
Bond, M., Gardner, S. T., Christian, J., & Sigal, J. J. Carstensen, L. (1995). Evidence for a life-span theory
(1983). Empirical study of self-rated defense of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in
styles. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 333- Psychological Science, 4, 151-156.
338. Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. E
Borkovec, T. D., Roemer, L., & Kinyon, J. (1995). (1996). A control-process perspective on the
Disclosure and worry: Opposite sides of the
origins of affect. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.),
emotional processing coin. In J. W. Pennebaker
Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals,
(Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 47-70).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
affect, and self-regulation (pp. 11-52). Mahwah,
tion. NJ: Erlbaum.
Brandon, T. H. (1994). Negative affect as motivation Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. E (1990). Origins and
to smoke. Current Directions in Psychological functions of positive and negative affect: A
Science, 3, 33-37. control-process view. Psychological Review, 97,
Bridges, L. J., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The 19-35.
development of emotional self-regulation in in- Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. E, & Weintraub, J. K.
fancy and early childhood. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically
Social development (Vol. 15, pp. 185-211). Thou- based approach. Journal of Personality and Social
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. Psychology, 56, 267-283.
Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., &
New York: Guilford Press. Silva, E A. (1995). Temperamental origins of child
Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of and adolescent behavior problems: From age three
motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, to age fifteen. Child Development, 66, 55-68.
389-413. Catanzaro, S. J., & Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring
Buck, R. (1990). Mood and emotion: A comparison of generalized expectancies for negative mood regula-
five contemporary views. Psychological Inquiry, 1, tion: Initial scale development and implications.
330-336. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 546-563.
Buck, R. (1993). What is this thing called subjective Chaplin, W. E, John, O. P., & Goldberg, L. R. (1988).
experience? Reflections on the neuropsychology of Conceptions of states and traits: Dimensional
qualia. Neuropsychology, 7, 490--499. attributes with ideals as prototypes. Journal of
Buck, R. (1994). Social and emotional functions in Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 541-557.
facial expression and communication: The readout Christianson, S.'(1992). Emotional stress and eyewit-
hypothesis. Biological Psychology, 38, 95-115. ness memory: A critical review. Psychological
Cacioppo, J. T., Uchino, B. N., Crites, S. L., Bulletin, 112, 284-309.
Snydersmith, M. A., Smith, G., Berntson, G. G., & Cialdini, R. B., Darby, I~(, & Vincent, J. (1973).
Lang, P. J. (1992). Relationship between facial
Transgression and altruism: A case for hedonism.
expressiveness and sympathetic activation in emo-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9,
tion: A critical review, with emphasis on modeling
underlying mechanisms and individual differences. 502-516.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, Cialdini, R. B., & Fultz, J. (1990). Interpreting the
110-128. negative mood-helping literature via "mega"-
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., & McGaugh, J. L. analysis: A contrary view. Psychological Bulletin,
(1994). B-Adrenergic activation and memory for 107, 210-214.
emotional events. Nature, 371, 702-704. Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P.,'& Izard, C. E. (1995).
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental
Emergent themes in the study of emotional psychopathology. Development and Psychopathol-
development and emotion regulation. Developmen- ogy, 7, 1-10.
tal Psychology, 25, 394--402. Cicchetti, D., Ganiban, J., & Barnett, D. (1991)
Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, Contributions from the study of high-risk popula-
appraisal, and emotion: The. onset of social tions t O understanding the development of emotion
290 GROSS

regulation. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The DeBary, W. T., Chart, W. T., & Watson, B. (1960).
development of emotion regulation and dysregula- Sources of Chinese tradition. New York: Columbia
tion (pp. 15-48). New York: Cambridge University University Press.
Press. Deffenbacher, J. L. (1994). Anger reduction: Issues,
Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward assessment, and intervention strategies. In A. W.
understanding the relationship between feeling Siegman & T. W. Smith (Eds.), Anger, hostility, and
states and social behavior. In A. H. Hastorf & A. M. the heart (pp. 239-269). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. Erlbaum Associates.
73-108). New York: Elsevier/North-Holland. Dembroski, T. M., MacDougall, J. M., Williams,
Clore, G. C. (1994). Why emotions are felt. In P. R. B., Haney, T. L., & Blumenthal, J. A. (1985).
Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of Components of type A, hostility, and anger-in:
emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 103-111). Relationship to angiographic findings. Psychoso-
New York: Oxford University Press. matic Medicine, 47, 219-233.
Coiro, M. J., & Gottesman, I. I. (1996). The diathesis Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Emotion,
and/or stressor role of expressed emotion in attention, and temperament. In C. E. Izard, J.
affective illness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cogni-
Practice, 3, 310-22. tions, and behavior (pp. 132-166). Cambridge,
Cole, P. M. (1986). Children's spontaneous control of England: Cambridge University Press.
facial expression. Child Development, 57, 1309- Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Arousal,
1321. affect, and attention as components of tempera-
Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Smith, K. D. (1994). ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Expressive control during a disappointment: Varia- ogy, 55, 958-966.
tions related to preschoolers' behavior problems. Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive
Developmental Psychology, 30, 835-846. and effortful processes in the organization of
Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at temperament. Development and Psychopathology,
dissonance theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances 9, 633-652.
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1992). Neural
229-262). New York: Academic Press. mechanisms of emotion. Journal of Consulting and
Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P. Clinical Psychology, 60, 329-338.
(1995). Drinking to regulate positive and negative Descartes, R. (1955). The philosophical works of
emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Descartes (Trans. E. S. Haldane & G. R. T. Ross).
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, New York: Dover. (Original work published 1649)
990-1005. Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice:
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and Their automatic and controlled components. Jour-
anxiety: The experience of play in work and games. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. 5-18.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, Diamond, A. (1991). Guidelines for the study of
reason, and the human brain. New York: Grossett/ brain-behavior relationships during development.
Putnam. In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton
Dandoy, A. C., & Goldstein, A. G. (1990). The use of (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp.
cognitive appraisal to reduce stress reactions: A 339-378). New York: Oxford University Press.
replication. Journal of Social Behavior and Person- Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, E (1995). The
ality, 5, 275-285. personality structure of affect. Journal of Personal-
Davidson, R. J. (1992). Prolegomenon to the structure ity and Social Psychology, 69, 130-141.
of emotion: Gleanings from neuropsychology. Dodge, K. A. (1991). Emotion and social information
Cognition and Emotion, 6, 245-268. processing. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The
Davidson, R. J. (1994). On emotion, mood, and development of emotion regulation and dysregula-
related affective constructs. In P. Ekman & R. J. tion (pp. 159-182). New York: Cambridge Univer-
Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamen- sity Press.
tal questions (pp. 51-55). New York: Oxford Dozier, M., & Kobak, R. R. (1992). Psychophysiol-
University Press. ogy in attachment interviews: Converging evi-
Davidson, R. J., & Sutton, S. K. (1995). Affective dence for deactivating strategies. Child Develop-
neuroscience: The emergence of a discipline. ment, 63, 1473-1480.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 217-224. Dunbar, E (1954). Emotions and bodily changes.
Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., Klinger, L., & Hill, D. New York: Columbia University Press.
(1992). The role of frontal lobe functioning in Dunn, J., & Brown, J. (1991). Relationships, talk
infant self-regulatory behavior. Brain and Cogni- about feelings, and the development of affect
tion, 20, 152-175. regulation in early childhood. In J. Garber & K.
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 291

Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regula- ric intervention, coping, and affective state on
tion and dysregulatian (pp. 89-108). Cambridge, recurrence and survival 6 years later. Archives of
England: Cambridge University Press. General Psychiatry, 50, 681-689.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (Ed.). (1992a). Emotion Feldman Barrett, L. (in press). Discrete emotions or
and its regulation in early development. San dimensions? The role of valence and arousal focus.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Cognition and Emotion.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992b). Emotion, Feldman, L. A. (1995). Valence focus and arousal
regulation, and the development of social compe- focus: Individual differences in the structure of
tence. Review of Personality and Social Psychol- affective experience. Journal of Personality and
ogy (pp. 119-150). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Social Psychology, 69, 153-166.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Maszk, E, Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of
Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of neurosis. New York: Norton.
emotionality and regulation in children's social Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive disso-
functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Develop- nance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
ment, 66, 1360-1384. Fiedler, K. (1988). Emotional mood, cognitive style,
Ekman, E (1972). Universals and cultural differences and behavior regulation. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas
in facial expression of emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behavior: New
Nebraska symposium of motivation (pp. 207-283). evidence and integrative attempts (pp. 100-119).
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Toronto, Canada: C. J. Hogrefe.
Ekman, E (1984). Expression and the nature of Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind: An essay on
emotion. In K. Scherer & E Ekman (Eds.), faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Approaches to emotion (pp. 319-344). Hillsdale, Folkman, S. (1997). Positive psychological states and
NJ: Erlbaum. coping with severe stress. Social Science and
Ekman, E (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Medicine, 45, 1207-1221.
Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169-200. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988). Coping as a
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psycho- mediator of emotion. Journal of Personality and
therapy. New York: Stuart. Social Psychology, 54, 466-475.
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An Folkow, B. (1987). Psychosocial and central nervous
approach to personality and subjective well-being. influences in primary hypertension. Circulation, 76
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, (Suppl. 1), 10-19.
1058-1068. Forgas, J. E (1982). Episode cognition: Internal
Epstein, S., & Meier, E (1989). Constructive representations of interaction routines. In L.
thinking: A broad coping variable with specific Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
components. Journal of Personality and Social psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Psychology, 57, 332-350. Forgas, J. E (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect
Erber, R., & Tesser, A. (1992). Task effort and the infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin,
regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis. 117, 39-66.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, Fox, N. A. (1989). Psychophysiological correlates of
339-359. emotional reactivity during the first year of life.
Erber, R., Wegner, D. M., & Therriault, N. (1996). On Developmental Psychology, 25, 364-372.
being cool and collected: Mood regulation in Fox, N. A. (Ed.). (1994a). The development of
anticipation of social interaction. Journal of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 757-766. considerations. Monographs of the Society for
Erdelyi, M. H. (1993). Repression: The mechanism Research in Child Development, 59 (Serial No.
and the defense. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. 240).
Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control Fox, N. A. (19946). Dynamic processes underlying
(pp. 126-148). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), The
Hall. development of emotion regulation: Biological and
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1969). behavioral considerations. Monographs of the
Personality structure and measurement. San Diego, Society for Research in Child Development, 59
CA: Knapp. (Serial No. 240), 152-166.
Fabes, R. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Regulatory Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, E R. (1997). Adult
control and adults' stress-related responses to dally attachment and the suppression of unwanted
life events. Journal of Personality and Social thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 1107-1117. Psychology, 73, 1080-1091.
Fawzy, E, Fawzy, N., Hyun, C., Elashoff, R., Guthrie, Freud, A. (1946). The ego and the mechanisms of
D., Fahey, J., & Morton, D. (1993). Malignant defense. New York: International Universities
melanoma: Effects of an early structured psychiat- Press.
292 GROSS

Freud, S. (1959). Inhibitions, symptoms, anxiety (J. peer ratings, and behavior. Journal of Personality
Strachey, Ed., & A. Strachey, Trans.). New York: and Social Psychology, 72, 435--448.
Norton. (Original work published 1926) Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1998). Mapping the
Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expression. San domain of emotional expressivity: Multi-method
Diego, CA: Academic Press. evidence for a hierarchical model. Journal of
Friedman, H. S. (Ed.). (1990). Personality and Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 170--191.
disease. New York: Wiley. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expres-
England: Cambridge University Press. sive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychology, 64, 970-986.
Psychologist, 43, 349-358. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Mood, emotion episodes, and
feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting positive
emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Ed.),
and negative emotions. Journal of Abnormal
Handbook of emotions (pp. 381--403). New York:
Psychology, 106, 95-103.
Guilford Press.
Gross, J. J., & Munoz, R. E (1995). Emotion
Gaensbauer, T. J. (1982). Regulation of emotional
expression in infants from two contrasting caretak- regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology:
ing environments. Journal of the American Acad- Science and Practice, 2, 151-164.
emy of ChiM Psychiatry, 21, 163-170. Haan, N. (1977). Coping and defending. New York:
Garber, J., & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.). (1991). The Academic Press.
development of emotion regulation and dysregula- Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion. New
tion. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University York: Blackwell.
Press. Head, H. (1921). Release function in the nervous
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain: Discover- system. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
ing the networks of the mind. New York: Basic London: Biology, 92B, 184-209.
Books. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A
Gilbert, D. G. (1979). Paradoxical tranquilizing and neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.
emotion-reducing effects of nicotine. Psychologi- Hinshaw, S. P., Simmel, C., & Heller, T. L. (1995).
cal Bulletin, 4, 643-661. Multimethod assessment of covert antisocial behav-
Goldsmith, H. H. (1993). Temperament: Variability in ior in children: Laboratory observations, adult
developing emotion systems. In M. Lewis & J. M. ratings, and child self-report. Psychological Assess-
Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. ment, 7, 209-219.
353-364). New York: Guilford Press. Hochschild, A. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules,
Gottman, J. M. (1993). The roles of conflict and social structure. American Journal of Sociol-
engagement, escalation, and avoidance in marital ogy, 85, 551-575.
interaction: A longitudinal view of five types of Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart.
couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Psychology, 61, 6--15. Hoffman, M. L. (1981). The development of empathy.
Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotions in In J. E Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),
psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press.
Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality,
Gross, J. (1989). Emotional expression in cancer
and developmental perspectives (pp. 41-63).
onset and progression. Social Science and Medi-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
cine, 28, 1239-1248.
Hooven, C., Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. E (1995).
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused
Parental meta-emotion structure predicts family
emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for
experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of and child outcomes. Cognition and Emotion, 9,
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224-237. 229-264.
Gross, J. J. (in press). Emotion regulation: Past, Hsee, C. K., & Abelson, R. E (1991). Velocity
present, future. Cognition and Emotion. relation: Satisfaction as a function of the first
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. C., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., derivative of outcome over time. Journal of
Gottestam, K., & Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 341-347.
and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Hull, J. G. (1981). A self-awareness model of the
Psychology and Aging, 12, 590-599. causes and effects of alcohol consumption. Journal
Gross, J. J., Feldman Barrett, L., & Richards, J. M. of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 586-600.
(1998). Emotion regulation in everyday life. Hull, J. G., & Bond, C. E, Jr. (1986). Social and
Manuscript in preparation. behavioral consequences of alcohol consumption
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1997). Revealing feelings: and expectancy: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Facets of emotional expressivity in self-reports, Bulletin, 99, 347-360.
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 293

Hume, D. (1969). A treatise of human nature. Kappas, A. (1996). The sociality of appraisals: Impact
Harmondsworth, England: Penguin Books. (Origi- of social situations on the evaluation of emotion
nal work published 1739) antecedent events and physiological and expressive
Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of reactions. In N. H. Frijda (Ed.), ISRE 1996:
affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull Proceedings of the IXth conference of the Interna-
(Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. tional Society for Research on Emotions (pp.
179-236), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 116-120). Toronto, Canada: International Society
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. E for Research on Emotions.
(1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem Kavanagh, D. J. (1992). Recent developments in
solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- expressed emotion and schizophrenia. British
ogy, 52, 1122-1131. Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 601-620.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Keltuer, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrass-
Plenum. ment: Its distinct form and appeasement functions.
Izard, C. E. (1990). Facial expressions and the Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250-270.
regulation of emotions. Journal of Personality and Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (in press). Functionalist
Social Psychology, 58, 487-498. accounts of emotions. Cognition and Emotion.
Jackson, J. H. (1884). The Croonian lectures on Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social
evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. function, and psychopathology. Review of General
British Medical Journal, 1, 591-593, 600-603, Psychology, 2, 320-342.
703-707. Khantzian, E. J. (1985). The self-medication hypoth-
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, esis of addictive disorders: Focus on heroin and
188-205. cocaine dependence. The American Journal of
James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion.
Psychiatry, 142, 1259-1264.
King, L. A., & Emmons, R. A. (1990). Conflict over
Psychological Review, 101, 205-210.
emotional expression: Psychological and physical
Jacobsen, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation. Chicago:
correlates. Journal of Personality and Social
University of Chicago Press.
Psychology, 58, 864-877.
John, O. P. (1990). The "big five" factor taxonomy:
Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experi-
Dimensions of personality in the natural language
ence and the incentives in people's lives. Minneapo-
and in questionnaires. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.),
lis: University of Minnesota Press.
Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. Kolb, B., & Taylor, L. (1990). Neocortical substrates
66-100). New York: Guilford Press. of emotional behavior. In N. Stein & B. Leventhal
Jorgensen, R. S., Johnson, B. T., Kolodziej, M. E., & (Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to
Schreer, G. E. (1996). Elevated blood pressure and emotion (pp. 115-144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
personality: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A
Bulletin, 120, 293-320. developmental perspective. Developmental Psychol-
Josephson, B. R., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). ogy, 18, 199-214.
Mood regulation and memory: Repairing sad Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and
moods with happy memories. Cognition and negative emotions: A developmental view. Develop-
Emotion, 10, 437 A.'!.'!.. mental Psychology, 25, 343-354.
Julkunen, J., Salonen, R., Kaplan, G. A., Chesney, Kring, A. M., Smith, D. A., & Neale, J. M. (1994).
M. A., & Salonen, J. T. (1994). Hostility and the Individual differences in dispositional expressive-
progression of carotid atherosclerosis. Psychoso- ness: Development and validation of the Emotional
matic Medicine, 56, 519-525. Expressivity Scale. Journal of Personality and
Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles Social Psychology, 66, 934-949.
theory of depression: Tests and an extension of the Krohne, H. W. (1996). Individual differences in
theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, coping. In M. Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.),
221-229. Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applica-
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1988). tions (pp. 381-409). New York: Wiley.
Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science, Lane, R. D., Ahem, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., &
240, 167-171. Kaszniak, A. W. (1997). Is alexithymia the
Kanfer, E H., & Gaelick, L. (1986). Self-management emotional equivalent of blindsight? Biological
methods. In E H. Kanfer & A. P. Goldstein (Eds.), Psychiatry, 42, 834-844.
Helping people change: A textbook of methods (3rd Lane, R. D., Quinlan, D. M., Schwartz, G. E., Walker,
ed., pp. 283-345). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press. E A., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1990). The Levels of
Kaplan, H. I., & Sadock, B. J. (1991). Synopsis of Emotional Awareness Scale: A cognitive-develop-
psychiatry (6th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & mental measure of emotion. Journal of Personality
Wilkins. Assessment, 55, 124-134.
294 GROSS

Lane, R. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of Lewinsohn, E M., Munoz, R. E, Youngren, M. A., &
emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental Zeiss, A. M. (1986). Control your depression (Rev.
theory and its application to psychopathology. ed.). New York: Prentice-Hall.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 133-143. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., & Michalson, L. (1984).
Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory on The cognitive-emotional fugue. In C. E. Izard, J.
emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495- Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, cognition,
512. and behavior (pp. 264-288). Cambridge, England:
Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of Cambridge University Press.
motivation and attention. American Psychologist, Lief, H. I., & Fox, R. C. (1963). Training for
50, 372-385. "detached concern" in medical students. In H. I.
Langer, E. J., Janis, I. L., & Wolfer, J. A. (1975). Lief, V. E Lief, & N. R. Lief (Eds.), The
Reduction of psychological stress in surgical psychological basis of medical practice (pp.
patients. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- 12-35). New York: Harper & Row.
ogy, 11, 155-165. Lingswiler, V. M., Crowther, J. H., & Stephens, M. A.
Langston, C. A. (1994). Capitalizing on and coping E (1989). Affective and cognitive antecedents to
with daily-life events: Expressive responses to eating episodes in bulimia and binge eating.
positive events. Journal of Personality and Social International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8,
Psychology, 67, 1112-1125. 533-539.
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and Linville, E W., & Fischer, G. W. (1991). Preferences
susceptibility to positive and negative emotional for separating or combining events. Journal of
states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 5-23.
ogy, 61, 132-140. Lion, J. R. (1992). The intermittent explosive
Lazarus, R. S. (1991a). Emotion and adaptation. disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 22, 64-66.
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral
Lazarus, R. S. (1991b). Progress on a cognitive- influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272-292.
motivational-relational theory of emotion. Ameri-
Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability
can Psychologist, 46, 819-834.
to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of
Lazarus, R. S., & Alfert, E. (1964). Short-circuiting of
control. Psychological Review, 91, 295-327.
threat by experimentally altering cognitive ap-
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997).
praisal. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological
ogy, 69, 195-205. Science, 8, 60--64.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Luomala, H. T., & Laaksonen, M. (1997). Mood-
appraisal and coping. New York: Springer. regulatory self-gifts: Development of a conceptual
Leafy, M. R. (1986). Affective and behavioral framework. Journal of Economic Psychology, 18,
consequences of shyness: Implications for theory, 407-434.
measurement, and research. In W. H. Jones, J. M. Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the
Cheek, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Shyness: Perspec- regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New
tives on research and treatment (pp. 27-38). New York: Liveright.
York: Plenum. Maccoby, E. E. (1980). Social development. New
LeDoux, J. E. (1987). Emotion. In E Plum (Ed.), York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovitch.
Handbook of physiology. Section 1. The nervous MacLean, P. D. (1975). Sensory and perceptual
system: Higher functions of the brain (Vol. 5, pp. factors in emotional functions of the triune brain.
419-459). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological In L. Levi (Ed.), Emotions--Theirparameters and
Society. measurement (pp. 71-92). New York: Raven Press.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interac- MacLean, P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution:
tions in the brain. Cognition and Emotion, 3, Role in paleocerebral functions. New York:
267-289. Plenum.
LeDoux, J. E. (1994). Emotion, memory and the Maier, S. F., Watldns, L. R., & Fleshner, M. (1994).
brain. Scientific American, 270, 50-57. Psychoneuroimmunology: The interface between
Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotions: A behavior, brain, and immunity. American Psycholo-
functional view. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson gist, 49, 1004-1017.
(Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental Malmo, R. B. (1975). On emotions, needs, and our
questions (pp. 123-126). New York: Oxford archaic brain. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
University Press. Winston.
Leventhal, H. (1984). A perceptual-motor theory of Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and
emotion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychol- the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and
ogy, 17, 117-182. motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 295

Marlatt, G. A. (1985). Cognitive factors in the relapse Mischel, W. (1996). From good intentions to
process. In G. A. Marlatt & J. R. Gordon (Eds.), willpower. In P. Gollwitzer & J. Bargh (Eds.), The
Relapse prevention (pp. 128-200). New York: psychology of action (pp. 197-218). New York:
Guilford Press. Guilford Press.
Masters, J. C. (1991). Strategies and mechanisms for Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The frame of mind. New
the personal and social control of emotion. In J. York: Springer.
Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of Morris, W. N., & Reilly, N. E (1987). Toward the
emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 182- self-regulation of mood: Theory and research.
207). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Motivation and Emotion, 11, 215-249.
Press. Newman, J. E, Wallace, J. E, Strauman, T. J.,
Matsumoto, D. (1987). The role of facial response in Skolaski, R. L., Oreland, K. M., Mattek, E W.,
the experience of emotion: More methodological Elder, K. A., & McNeely, J. (1993). Effects of
problems and a meta-analysis. Journal of Personal- motivationally significant stimuli on the regulation
ity and Social Psychology, 52, 769-774. of dominant responses. Journal of Personality and
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, E (1993). The intelligence of Social Psychology, I, 165-175.
emotional intelligence. Intelligence, 17, 433-442. Nix, G., Watson, C., Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, E (1995). Emotional (1995). Reducing depressive affect through exter-
intelligence and the construction and regulation of nal focus of attention. Journal of Social and
feelings. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 4, Clinical Psychology, 14, 36-52.
197-208. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Sex differences in
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, E, Gomberg-Kaufman, S., & control of depression. In D. M. Wegner & J. W.
Blainey, K. (1991). A broader conception of mood Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control
experience. Journal of Personality and Social (pp. 306-324). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Psychology, 60, 100-111. Hall.
McIntosh, W. D. (1996). When does goal nonattain-
Novaco, R. W. (1975). Anger control: The develop-
ment lead to negative emotional reactions, and
ment and evaluation of an experimental treatment.
when doesn't it? The role of linking and rumina-
Lexington, MA: Heath.
tion. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving
Nowlis, V., & Nowlis, H. H. (1956). The description
and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and
and analysis of mood. Annals of the New York
self-regulation (pp. 53-77). Mahwah, N J: Erlbaum.
Academy of Sciences, 65, 345-355.
Meerum Terwogt, M., & Stegge, H. (1995). Chil-
dren's understanding of the strategic control of Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a
negative emotions. In J. A. Russell, J. Fernandez- cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and
Dols, A. S. R. Manstead, & J. C. Wellenkamp Emotion, 1, 29-50.
(Eds.), Everyday conceptions of emotion: An Ohman, A. (1986). Face the beast and fear the face:
introduction to the psychology, anthropology and Animal and social fears as prototypes for evolution-
linguistics of emotion (pp. 373-390). Dordrecht, ary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology, 23,
The Netherlands: Kluwer. 123-145.
Mega, M. S., & Cummings, J. L. (1994). Frontal- Panksepp, J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiologi-
subeortical circuits and neuropsychiatdc disorders. cal theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosci- Sciences, 5, 407-467.
ences, 6, 358-370. Panksepp, J. (1991). Affective neuroscience: A
Meichenbaum, D. A. (1985). Stress inoculation conceptual framework for the neurobiological
training. New York: Pergamon Press. study of emotions. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.),
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New International review of studies of emotion (pp.
York: Harper & Row. 53-99). Chichester, England: Wiley.
Miller, S. M. (1987). Monitoring and blunting: Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The
Validation of a questionnaire to assess styles of foundations of human and animal emotions.
information seeking under threat. Journal of Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 345-353. Parker, J. D. A., & Endler, N. S. (1996). Coping and
Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. defense: A historical overview. In M. Zeidner & N.
(1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory,
rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, research, applications (pp. 3-23). New York:
93, 355-372. Wiley.
Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratifica- Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P., Briner, R. B., &
tion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimen- Reynolds, S. (1996). Changing moods: The psychol-
tal Social Psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 249-291). New ogy of mood and mood regulation. London:
York: Academic Press. Longman.
296 GROSS

Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for Developmental Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 37-86).
inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
moods. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & O'Boyle, C. G. (1992).
(Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 278-308). Self-regulation and emotion in infancy. In N.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Eisenberg & R. A. Fabes (Eds.), Emotion and its
Paulhus, D. L., Fridhandler, B., & Hayes, S. (1997). regulation in early development (pp. 7-23). San
Psychological defense: Contemporary theory and Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
research. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs Rothbaum, E, Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982).
(Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. Changing the world and changing the self: A
543-579). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. two-process model of perceived control. Journal of
Pennebaker, J. W. (1990). Opening up: The healing Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5-37.
powers of confiding in others. New York: Morrow. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on
Pennebaker, J. W., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23-41.
(1988). Disclosure of traumas and immune func- Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins,
tion: Health implications for psychotherapy. Jour- S. D. (1995). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, preschoolers' social adaptation. Development and
239-245. Psychopathology, 7, 49-62.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Roberts, T. (1992). Toward a his Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London:
and hers theory of emotion: Gender differences in Hutchinson.
visceral perception. Journal of Social and Clinical Saarni, C. (1990). Emotional competence: How
Psychology, 11, 199-212.
emotions and relationships become integrated. In
Ploog, D. (1992). Neuroethological foundations of
R. A. Thompson & R. Dienstbier (Eds.), Socioemo-
biological psychiatry. In H. M. Emrich & M.
tional Development: Nebraska Symposium on
Wiegand (Eds.), Integrative biological psychiatry
Motivation (pp. 115-182). Lincoln: University of
(pp. 3-35). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Nebraska Press.
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary
Salovey, P., Hsee, C. K., & Mayer, J. D. (1993).
synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.
Polivy, J. (1990). Inhibition of internally cued Emotional intelligence and self-regulation. In
behavior. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker, Handbook of
(Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. mental control (pp. 258-277). Englewood Cliffs,
2, pp. 131-147). New York: Guilford Press. NJ: Prentice Hall.
Porges, S. E (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Sapolsky, R. M. (1994). Why zebras don't get ulcers:
Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary A guide to stress, stress-related diseases, and
heritage: A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology, coping. New York: Freeman.
32, 301-318. Sayette, M. A. (1993). An appraisal-disruption model
Richards, J., & Gross, J. J. (in press). Composure at of alcohol's effects on stress responses in social
any cost? The cognitive consequences of emotion drinkers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 459-476.
suppression. Personality and Social Psychology Schachter, S. (1968). Obesity and eating. Science,
Bulletin. 161, 751-756.
Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism,
expression: A review of the neurological and coping, and health: Assessment and implications of
psychological mechanisms for producing facial generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol-
expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 52-77. ogy, 4, 219-247.
Rippere, V. (1977). "What's the thing to do when Scheier, M. F., Weintraub, J. K., & Carver, C. S.
you're feeling depressed?"---A pilot study. Behav- (1986). Coping with stress: Divergent strategies of
ior Research and Therapy, 15, 185-191. optimists and pessimists. Journal of Personality
Roger, D., & Najarian, B. (1989). The construction and Social Psychology, 51, 1257-1264.
and validation of a new scale for measuring Scherer, K. (1984). On the nature and function of
emotion control. Personality and Individual Differ- emotion: A component process approach. In K. R.
ences, 10, 845-853. Scherer & P. E. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to
Roils, E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D., & McGrath, J. emotion (pp. 293-317). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.
(1994). Emotion-related learning in patients with Scherer, K. R. (1988). Cognitive antecedents of
social and emotional changes associated with emotion. In V. Hamilton, G. H. Bower, & N. H.
frontal lobe damage. Journal of Neurology, Neuro- Frijda (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on emotion
surgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 1518-1524. and motivation (pp. 89-126). Dordrecht, The
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Develop- Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.
ment of individual differences in temperament. In Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G., & Summerfield,
M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in A. B. E. (1986). Experiencing emotion: A cross-
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 297

cultural study. Cambridge, England: Cambridge mypoia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American
University Press. Psychologist, 45, 921-933.
Schwartz, G. E. (1975). Biofeedback, self-regulation, Stein, N. L., Trabasso, T., & Liwag, M. (1993). The
and the patterning of physiological processes. representation and organization of emotional expe-
American Scientist, 63, 314-324. rience: Unfolding the emotion episode. In M.
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of
misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Infor- emotions (pp. 279-300). New York: Guilford Press.
mative and directive functions of affective states. Stemmler, G. (1997). Selective activation of traits:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, Boundary conditions for the activation of anger.
513-523. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 213-
Seligman, M. E. E (1975). Helplessness: On 233.
depression, development and death. San Francisco: Steptoe, A. (1981 ). Psychological factors in cardiovas-
Freeman. cular disease. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York: Steptoe, A., & Vogele, C. (1986). Are stress responses
McGraw-Hill. influenced by cognitive appraisal? An experimental
Seneca. (1963). On anger (J. W. Basore, Trans.). In comparison of coping strategies. British Journal of
Moral essays. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- Psychology, 77, 243-255.
sity Press. Stern, D. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and
Shaw, L. L., Batson, C. D., & Todd, R. M. (1994). mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Empathy avoidance: Forestalling feeling for an- Press.
other in order to escape the motivational conse- Stifter, C. A., & Moyer, D. (1991). The regulation of
quences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- positive affect: Gaze aversion activity during
ogy, 67, 879-887. mother-infant interaction. Infant Behaviors and
Shiffman, S. (1984). Coping with temptations to Development, 14, 111-123.
smoke. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- Stuss, D., & Benson, D. (1986). The frontal lobes.
ogy, 52, 261-267. New York: Raven Press.
Shiffrin, R., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and Suinn, R. M., & Richardson, R. (1971). Anxiety
automatic human information processing: II. Per- management training: A non-specific behavior
ceptual learning, automatic attending, and a therapy for anxiety control. Behavior Therapy, 2,
general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127- 498-510.
190. Suls, J., Wan, C. K., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1995).
Smith, A. C., & Kleinman, S. (1989). Managing Relationship of trait anger to resting blood
emotions in medical school: Students' contacts pressure: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 14,
with the living and the dead. Social Psychology A44 456.
Quarterly, 52, 56--69. Sutton, R. I. (1991). Maintaining norms about
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of expressed emotions: The case of bill collectors.
cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Person- Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 245-268.
ality and Social Psychology, 48, 813-838. Swann, W. B. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two
Smith, T. W. (1992). Hostility and health: Current roads meet. Journal of Personality and Social
status of a psychosomatic hypothesis. Health Psychology, 53, 1038-1051.
Psychology, 11, 139-150. Tavris, C. (1982). Anger: The misunderstood emo-
Solomon, R. C. (1976). The passions. New York: tion. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Anchor/Doubleday. Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. A. (1997).
Spiegel, D., Bloom, J. R., Kraemer, H. C., & Gottheil, Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in
E. (1989). Effect of psychosocial treatment on medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge, En-
survival of patients with metastatic breast cancer. gland: Cambridge University Press.
The Lancet, 2, 888-891. Taylor, S. E., & Armor, D. A. (1996). Positive
Stanislavski, C. (1965). An actor prepares. New illusions and coping with adversity. Journal of
York: Theater Arts Books. (Original work pub- Personality, 64, 873-898.
lished 1948) Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and
Steams, C. Z., & Steams, P. N. (1986). Anger: The well-being: A social psychological perspective on
struggle for emotional control in America's history. mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 210.
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self- Taylor, S. E., & Lobel, M. (1989). Social comparison
affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. activity under threat: Downward evaluation and
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21, upward contacts. Psychological Review, 96, 569-
261-302. 575.
Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personal-
298 GROSS

ity and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 69-88).
emphasis on self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders Walden, T. A., & Smith, M. C. (1997). Emotion
(pp. 681-706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 7-25.
Tesser, A., & Cornell, D. E (1991). On the confluence Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). Affects separable
of self processes. Journal of Experimental Social and inseparable: On the hierarchical arrangement
Psychology, 27, 501-526. of the negative affects. Journal of Personality and
Tesser, A., Rosen, S., & Waranch, E. (1973). Social Psychology, 62, 489-505.
Communicator mood and the reluctance to transmit Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental
undesirable messages (the mum effect). The control. Psychological Review, 101, 34--52.
Journal of Communication, 23, 266-283. Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and
Thayer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods: automaticity in social life. In D. Gilbert, S. T.
Managing energy, tension, and stress. New York: Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social
Oxford University Press. psychology (4th ed., pp. 446--496). New York:
Thayer, R. E., Newman, J. R., & McClain, T. M. McGraw-Hill.
(1994). Self-regulation of mood: Strategies for Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993).
changing a bad mood, raising energy, and reducing Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and
tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and
ogy, 67, 910-925. Social Psychology, 65, 1093-1104.
Thoits, P. A. (1985). Self-labeling processes in mental Wegner, D. M., & Pennebaker, J. W. (Eds.). (1993).
illness: The role of emotional deviance. American Handbook of mental control. Englewood Cliffs,
Journal of Sociology, 91, 221-249. NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self- Weinberger, D. A. (1990). The construct validity of
regulation. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Socioemo- the repressive coping style. In J. L. Singer (Ed.),
tional Development. Nebraska symposium on Repression and dissociation: Implications for
motivation (Vol. 36, pp. 367-467). Lincoln: personality theory, psychopathology, and health
University of Nebraska Press. (pp. 337-386). Chicago: University of Chicago
Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and Press.
emotional development. Educational Psychology Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, E M., & Blackburn, T. C.
Review, 3, 269-307. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The psychol-
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A ogy of control in American and Japan. Journal of
theme in search of definition. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 955-969.
The development of emotion regulation: Biological Westen, D. (1994). Toward an integrative model of
and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the affect regulation: Applications to social-psychologi-
Society for Research in Child Development, 59, cal research. Journal of Personality, 62, 641--667.
(Serial No. 240), 25-52. White, G. M. (1993). Emotions inside out: The
Thompson, R. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1996). The anthropology of affect. In M. Lewis & J. M.
double-edged sword: Emotional regulation for Haviland (Ed.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 41-
children at risk. Development and Psychopathol- 51). New York: Guilford Press.
ogy, 8, 163-182. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C.
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, conscious- (1996). The emotional stroop task and psychopa-
ness: The positive affects (Vol. 1). New York: thology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.
Springer. Williams, R. B. (1986). Patterns of reactivity and
Tomkins, S. S. (1984). Affect theory. In E Ekman stress. In K. A. Mattews, S. M. Weiss, T. Detre,
(Ed.), Emotion in the human face (2nd ed., pp. T. M. Dembroski, B. Falkner, S. B. Manuck, &
353-395). New York: Cambridge University Press. R. B. J. Williams (Eds.), Handbook of stress,
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains reactivity, and cardiovascular disease (pp. 109-
the present: Emotional adaptations and the struc- 125). New York: Wiley.
ture of ancestral environments. Ethology and Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward social comparison
Sociobiology, 11, 375-424. principles in social psychology. Psychological
Tucker, D. M., Luu, E, & Pfibram, K. H. (1995). Bulletin, 90, 245-271.
Social and emotional self-regulation. Structure and Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal
functions of the human prefrontal cortex. Annals of inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 213-239. Press.
Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. Cambridge, Young, P. T. (1943). Emotion in man and animal: Its
MA: Harvard University Press. nature and relation to attitude and motive. New
Walden, T. A. (1991). Infant social referencing. In J. York: Wiley.
Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management: Using
SPECIAL ISSUE: EMOTION REGULATION 299

entertainment to full advantage. In L. Donohew, Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond


H. E. Sypher, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Communica- the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ:
tion, social cognition, and affect (pp. 147-171). Erlbaum.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zimbardo, P. G. (1969). The human choice: Individu-
ation, reason, and order versus deindividuation, Received November 3, 1997
impulse, and chaos. Nebraska Symposium on Revision received March 22, 1998
Motivation, 17, 237-307. Accepted April 22, 1998

S-ar putea să vă placă și