Sunteți pe pagina 1din 36

Psychological Bulletin

1980, Vol. 88, No. 2, 370-405

The Smoking Problem: A Review of the Research and Theory


in Behavioral Risk Modification
Howard Leventhal Paul D, Cleary
University of WisconsinMadison School of Social Work, Rutgers University

This article reviews findings and examines theoretical implications of studies


concerned with initiation, maintenance, and therapy of cigarette smoking in chil-
dren and adults. The following conclusions are offered: (a) Recent increases in
success rates may be due to changes in the social environment that multiply the
action of previously unsuccessful procedures, (b) current interventions are based
on communication and behavioral models that ignore features unique to smoking,
(c) methodological refinements of current interventions are unlikely to signifi-
cantly increase success rates, (d) prevention studies should be directed toward
critical steps in the developmental history of smoking (e.g., preventing initial
attempts, facilitating negative interpretations of initial attempts, and retarding
regular usage), and (e) studies of cessation and withdrawal must consider find-
ings and theories of nicotine dependence. A hypothesis is presented to account
for the development of craving conditioned to internal and external cues, and
suggestions about how to eliminate the craving state are made.

Recent epidemiological and biomedical re- evident from the response of Solomon Islanders
search has clearly shown that smoking in- and Saharan Africans: Though uninformed of
creases risks of diseases such as lung cancer the evidence on risks, they believe that smok-
(Cornfield et al., 19S9; Doll & Hill, 1964; ing is a health hazard and a practice unfit for
Hammond & Horn, 1954; Horn, 1968), coro- children (Damon, 1973). But they also stated
nary heart disease (Doll & Hill, 1964; Doll & that they could not go without cigarettes even
Peto, 1976; Friedman, Dales, & Ury, 1979; for a day.
Gordon, Kannel, McGee, & Dawber, 1974; The clear demonstration of risk and the
Hammond & Horn, 1954; Horn, 1968), and desire of so many smokers to quit establish
emphysema (Horn, 1968; U.S. Dept. of Health, the need for effective techniques to help people
Education and Welfare, 1964, 1973); King stop smoking and to reduce the number of
James's attacks on smoking over 3 centuries persons who start. Despite the intensive re-
ago (Eckholm, 1977) have been vindicated. search efforts of epidemiologists, health edu-
Despite the evidence, young people continue cators, physicians, physiologists, psycholo-
to be recruited into the ranks of smokers, and gists, and sociologists on the origins and na-
smokers continue to struggle with the problem ture of smoking and on ways of modifying it,
of quitting. This may seem all the more pe- the procedures for bringing about smoking
culiar, given that most smokers believe smok- reduction and cessation are still imperfect.
ing is hazardous; indeed, even in 1968 (Horn, Indeed, it is unclear what mixture of social,
Note 1), 53% of smokers reported that they psychological, and pharmacological factors ac-
would like to stop. That the antipathy does counts for the apparent strength of the smok-
not necessarily depend on scientific data is er's dependence on his or her cigarettes
(Borgatta & Evans, 1968; Dunn, 1973; Frith,
1971b; Fuller, 1973, 1975; Fuller & Forrest,
Requests for reprints should be sent to Howard 1973; Hunt, 1970; Jaffe, 1976; Knapp, Bliss,
Leventhal, Department of Psychology, W. J. Brogden
Psychology Building, University of Wisconsin, 1202 & Wells, 1963; McKennelL 1973; Peele, 1977;
West Johnson Street, Madison, Wisconsin S3706. Russell, 197la, 1971b; Schubert, 1965). What

Copyright 1980 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0033-2909/80/8802-0370500.75

370
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 371

is clear is that it is now far more difficult to research therapies tend to be unaware of the
control the behavioral risk factors that pre- literature on community programs. Because
dict chronic disease (e.g., smoking, excessive of such isolation, researchers have failed to
consumption of alcohol, or diets overly rich recognize the startling similarities between
in cholesterol) than it is to modify the risk findings from investigations of therapies,
factors for infectious disease (Ebert, 1978); small-scale communication programs, school
there is no smoking swamp analogous to the programs, and large community efforts. Thus
breeding ground of the anopheles mosquito the results from a diverse set of research per-
and no hypertension pump analogous to the spectives point to common issues in behavioral
Broad Street pump in London that helped risk factor intervention. We can profit from
identify contaminated water as the source of the almost endless stream of smoking research
cholera. Intervention aimed at risk factors only if it addresses these key issues.
linked to behavior requires careful attention
not only to the intervention process but to the Intervention Studies
processes involved in the targeted risk be-
havior (Leventhal, 1968, 1973; Leventhal & We review two classes of intervention study:
Cleary, 1979) and to the fit between the two. (a) those following a therapy or doctor-pa-
Lewin's statement that "there is nothing so tient model for intervention and (b) those
practical as a good theory!" (cited in Hovland, following a public health, community, or pub-
Janis, & Kelley, 1953) appears to be an apt lic opinion model. Therapy studies are time
summary of the need for a theoretical ap- and procedure intensive; thus it is often sup-
proach to the process of chronic disease risk posed that they set an upper limit for the ef-
factor modification. fectiveness of intervention. Public health stud-
The present article has three goals. The ies provide insight into the possibility of
first is to review what we now know about the effective, publicly directed intervention efforts.
intervention process. This entails briefly de- Both sets of studies need to be evaluated with
scribing the basic approaches to intervention, regard to both practical effectiveness (i.e., is
the assumptions they make about the smok- smoking reduced?) and theoretical significance
ing response, and their successes and failures. (i.e., do they provide insight into the inter-
The second goal is to review what we now vention process?). In our view the evidence
know about the origins and maintenance of suggests that all methods have failed to pro-
smoking behavior, with particular attention duce sustained change; neither public health
to the processes that underlie its initiation and programs nor face-to-face therapies have
maintenance. The third and final goal is to proven effective in achieving long-term reduc-
relate knowledge about the determinants of tions in smoking when compared with control
smoking to theories of intervention and when cases or with the rates of spontaneous quitting
possible to suggest ideas for developing new in the population at large. We also suggest
intervention procedures. The theoretical analy- that these studies have added little to our
sis and integration of these areas may provide knowledge as to what is needed for a success-
important clues for resolutions to what now ful intervention. The final section of our ar-
seems an intractable problem. A touch, if not ticle addresses this issue in detail.
a goodly dose, of chauvinism may well be re-
sponsible for the lack of theoretical develop-
ment in the area of smoking prevention and Therapy Model
cessation. People doing research on interven-
In therapy studies smoker and therapist are
tion are usually unaware of findings from re- usually in direct dyadic interaction, though
search on initiation and maintenance. Indeed, group and self-treatment programs are also
people doing research on one aspect of inter- used (Schwartz, 1969). In most therapy studies
vention, such as community health programs, the smoker assumes the role of a patient; he
are usually unaware of the findings from re- or she is clearly motivated (at some level) and
search on smoking therapies, and those who has made a decision to stop smoking. In addi-
372 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

tion, he or she anticipates some cost or un- havior therapies, appear to focus on both con-
pleasantness to get to, stay in, and benefit trolling the smoking response and eliminating
from treatment. Subjects in these samples the urge to smoke that precedes it. In sensi-
may be biased toward smokers who have tried tization therapies, a noxious stimulus, such as
but failed to stop smoking on their own, but an electric shock or the imagination of aver-
they are also likely to be more motivated to sive scenes (Barrett & Sachs, 1974; Conway,
stop and more aware of the difficulty of chang- 1977), immediately follows the act of smoking
ing their habit. There also is a strong passive or the thought of a situation that induces
component to the patient role that may vitiate smoking (Berecz, 1971, 1972a, 1972b; Best &
efforts at effective self-control (Carlson, 197S). Steffy, 1971; Carlin & Armstrong, 1968;
Thus in comparison with public health stud- Steffy, Meichenbaum, & Best, 1970; von
ies, therapy studies are likely to deal with a Hilsheimer ; 1968; Whitman, 1972).
more homogeneous audience. Finally, the A common stimulus in smoking cessation
"messages" used in many therapies involve programs is, not surprisingly, cigarette smoke.
direct experience, for example, shock, inhala- One technique is to induce aversion and/or
tion of warm smoke, or smoking on signal, as satiation by having a person smoke at two to
opposed to the verbal-symbolic material to three times the normal rate (Best, Owen, &
which public health studies are generally Trentadue, 1978; Dawley, 197S; Lando, 1976;
restricted. Lando & Davison, 1975; Resnick, 1968;
Schmahl, Lichtenstein, & Harris, 1972; Wilde,
1964). Other research used artificially pro-
Types of Therapies duced warm, stale, smoky air, sometimes in
combination with rapid smoking, to induce
Behavior therapies. The behavior therapies aversion (Dawley, 1975; Lichtenstein, Harris,
treat the smoking response, not the smoker; Birchler, Wahl, & Schmahl, 1973; Wilde,
smoking is a habit that is to be modified. 1964; see, also, the review by Pechacek, 1979).
Operand procedures are designed to detect the The aim of these studies was to produce an
environmental stimuli that control the smok-
aversive state and to associate that state with
ing response (Gutmann & Marston, 1967; the act of smoking in the hope that the state
Levinson, Shapiro, Schwartz, & Tursky, 1971;
will generalize to the environmental cues that
A. Miller & Gimpl, 1971; Ober, 1968) and stimulate smoking. The objective is identical
extinguish their control of the response. For
to that of programs that teach avoidance of
example, smokers are asked to carry a timer
poisonous foods (Garcia, Hankins, & Rusiniak,
that buzzes at random intervals and to smoke 1974) and control of alcohol intake (Baker &
whenever it signals. Smoking regularly to this
Cannon, 1979). Early studies using sensitiza-
cue is supposed to attach the response to the tion therapies showed high initial rates of
buzzer and detach it from environmental cues. success in quitting and smoking reduction
When the buzzer is discarded, so too is smok- (60%-90% reductions in relation to base-
ing. In one study, subjects reduced smoking by line) with reasonably good long-term effects
75% after 2 months of buzzer-controlled (e.g., 45%). More recent studies, however,
smoking, but the success rate had declined to are less promising with respect to both the
43 % 6 weeks later (Shapiro, Tursky, Schwartz, short- and the long-term effectiveness of the
& Shnidman, 1971); there was no change in procedure (Elliott Si Denney, 1978; Glasgow,
untreated controls. Other procedures, such as
1978; Lando, 1975, 1976; Sutherland, Amit,
earning points toward a course grade or pro- Golden, & Roseberger, 1975); for example,
viding verbal rewards that one is doing well,
rapid smoking is not superior to other controls
achieved roughly comparable effects31%
reduction after only 2 weeks (A. Miller & and shows only 20% reductions at 6-month
Gimpl, 1971), a level of success that is repre- follow-up.
sentative of other behavior therapies. Desensitization therapies attempt to elimi-
Sensitization and/or desensitization proce- nate the distress and anxiety that stimulate
dures, the most commonly used of the be- smoking by practiced relaxation in response
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 373

to internal or situational cues that are stress- many of the investigations reported failed to
ful (Gerson & Lanyon, 1972; Hoenig & Reed, include control groups. The success rates in
1966; Kazdin & Wilcoxon, 1976; Koenig & these single-group studies range from 17%
Masters, 1965; Sutherland et al, 197S; Wag- to 38% quitting for 6 months or longer, sug-
ner & Bragg, 1970). gesting effects equal to those found with other
Other methods of behavior therapy include interventions (Burns, 1969; Burnum, 1974;
contracting, in which the subject pays a sum Handel, 1973; Pincherle & Wright, 1970;
of money to the experimenter and has it re- H. 0. Williams, 1969). Studies using controls
turned if he or she succeeds in cutting down, also show varying rates, for example, 33%
and self-monitoring procedures, in which the versus 9% quitting, as measured at a 6-month
subject keeps records of varying degrees of follow-up, in experimental and control groups,
complexity for each cigarette smoked (Karoly respectively (J. S. Mausner, Mausner, & Rial,
& Doyle, 197S; Leventhal & Avis, 1976; Mc- 1968) and 10.3% to 19.1% quitting, as mea-
Fall, 1970; McFall & Hammen, 1971). sured at 1 year, in control and maximal impact
Psychological therapies. Psychotherapy and experimental condition in a careful random-
counseling claim to focus on the total patient ized trial conducted by Russell, Wilson, Tay-
and use verbal procedures rather than direct lor, and Baker (1979), These studies also
experience. Controlled studies of traditional reported substantial differences between physi-
therapies (Schwartz & Dubitzky, 1967) are cians (Pincherle & Wright, 1970; Russell et
relatively rare, though a therapy group is al,, 1979) and between the sexes, males quit-
sometimes included as a condition in studies ting and cutting down more than did females
of behavior therapy (Gerson & Lanyon, 1972; (Burnum, 1974; Handel, 1973; H. 0. Wil-
Jenks, Schwartz, & Dubitzky, 1969; Ober, liams, 1969), as well as more difficulty with
1968). withdrawal symptoms in smokers scoring high
A possible disadvantage to the psychothera- on scales of neuroticism (Burns, 1969).
peutic approach is that the therapist (source) Unfortunately, there is little theoretical
can have an unpredictable effect on the out- analysis and perhaps even less empirical data
come (Kiesler, 1966; Locke, 1971). Jenks et to suggest how to strengthen these effects.
al. (1969) reported that patients failed to There is some reason to believe that inter-
stop smoking if their life situation contained vention may be more effective at critical junc-
factors the therapist regarded as insurmount- tures, such as, when a smoker is pregnant
able barriers to successful quitting; this oc- (Barec, MacArthur, & Sherwood, 1976) or
curred even though these factors were unim- symptomatic with chest disease (Burns, 1969;
portant for patients of other therapists. Thus H. 0. Williams, 1969). At least two factors
a fair evaluation of traditional therapy pro- make study in this area of critical importance.
cedures requires that therapists hold accurate First, the number of smokers that can be
beliefs about smoking as well as be masters reached is large, and the cumulative effect
of the therapy procedures (Kiesler, 1966; could be substantial (Russell et al,, 1979),
Koenig & Masters, 1965; Marston & McFall, Second, the interventions so far used have
1971). This problem seems less serious for been relatively weak, consisting of serious ad-
behavior therapies in which the therapist's vice and pamphlets, and the time per patient
behavior is more closely circumscribed. is typically brief, from a suggestion to stop to
Physician intervention. White, Williams, 7 minutes. Both the theoretical and practical
and Greenberg (1961) estimated that 750 out payoffs appear to be potentially great.
of 1,000 adults are ill during an average month Clinics. Clinics (Schwartz, 1969) have been
and that 2SO of them will visit a physician. used to provide relationship and group ther-
Given the potential coverage, it is of obvious apy with (Ejrup, 1964) or without a drug
importance to determine whether the primary component. Examples range from the 5-day
care physician or internist can be an effective treatment plan of the Seventh-day Adventists
agent for smoking reduction. Little research (combining educational material, threat films,
has been done on this problem, however, and buddy support systems, etc,) to clinics run
374 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

in a wide variety of medical and community using data from different studies, as they dif-
settings (Bozzetti, 1972; Dubitzky & Schwartz, fer in procedures for subject selection, dropout
1968, 1969; Jenks et al, 1969; Marrone, rates, criteria for success, and so forth. Ther-
Merksamer, & Salzberg, 1970; B. Mausner, apies can be compared only in those studies in
1966; Schwartz & Dubitzky, 1967; Tamerin, which investigators randomly assigned sub-
1972; West, Graham, Swanson, & Wilkinson, jects to different treatments; there is high
1977). Early studies of group therapy and internal validity to comparisons of treatments
drug treatment reported success rates of 76% in these studies, even if the external validity
of the smokers quitting for the short run and (generalization to other populations and set-
31% for the long run. Later studies included tings) is low (Campbell & Stanley, 1963).
control conditions, and showed no advantage Despite the limitations, we feel that four
of drug over placebo, implying that the ef- findings stand out. First, whether the criterion
fect was at a behavioral level (e.g., Bartlett & be quitting or significant reduction, there is
Whitehead, 1957; Edwards, 1964). Recent an impressive decrease in smoking during
clinic studies promise no greater success than treatment. The reviews of the pre-1970 litera-
earlier ones. Initial cessation rates are high, ture (Bernstein, 1969, 1970; Keutzer, Lich-
but so is the rate of return to smoking (Jacobs, tenstein, & Mees, 1968; Lichtenstein &
1972; West et al., 1977). Keutzer, 1971; Schwartz, 1969), the report
Hypnosis and sensory deprivation. Finally, by Hunt and Bespalec (1974) based on a
there are numerous studies on procedures such summary of all studies listed in Psychological
as hypnosis (M. M. Miller, 197S; Pederson, Abstracts and Cumulated Index Medicus from
Scrimgeour, & Lefcoe, 1975; Perry & Mullen, 1968 to 1973, and the more recent summaries
1975). This technique has been used for smok- (Hunt & Matarazzo, 1973; Lichtenstein &
ing modification, often incorporating aversion Danaher, 1975; Zane, Kunze, & Kunze, 1974)
and desensitization treatments, though some all agree that initial results are impressive.
hypnotic approaches simply have the smoker Second, there is a relatively high dropout
repeat short health messages such as "My rate, often reaching 50% of those included in
body is a machine; I live in my body; I must the initial sample. Thus many outcome reports
take care of my body; I must not smoke" are inflated, as dropouts from therapy are ex-
(Perry & Mullen, 1975). Investigators report cluded from the denominator when success
high success rates with hypnosis, such as 90% rates are computed. For example, Schwartz
abstention rates 1 month and 68% 1 year (1969) reported that in the Institute-Perma-
after starting therapy (M. M. Miller, 1975). nente Smoking Control Research Project, the
Most of these studies are clinical reports, how- end-of-treatment success rate would increase
ever, and do not give data on the number of from 47.2% to 70% if only those subjects
successes relative to patients treated, criteria completing at least half of the treatment were
for success, and comparable results for un- counted. The success rate at the 1-year fol-
treated control groups (Johnston & Donoghue, low-up would change from 27.8% to 45%
1971). with similar adjustments.
In sensory deprivation studies (Suedfeld & Third, therapy works well in helping people
Best, 1977; Suedfeld & Heard, 1974; Suedfeld, to stop smoking but does less well in keeping
Landon, Pargament, & Epstein, 1972), the them stopped. Whereas most backsliding oc-
smoker may spend 24 hours in bed, in a dark, curs within 6 months, it continues with inex-
sound-reducing chamber, with no objects avail- orable force for 12 months till there is a resid-
able, and eat by sucking liquid through plastic ual of quitters and smoking reducers of 10%
tubes. Suedfeld and Ikarcl reported an 88% to 25% of the pretherapy base level although
reduction in smoking postdeprivation, and 1 figures of 35%. to 45% are occasionally
year later these subjects were smoking 48% reported.
less than they had before the session. Fourth, success rates for different therapies
Key findings. One must be careful, of are not markedly different; it is simply better
course, in making comparisons among therapies to do something than nothing. The similar
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 375

outcomes across therapies could occur because the effectiveness of antismoking treatments
each therapy affects a different group of smok- when they are followed by phone calls or
ers. On the other hand, the therapies could booster sessions to reinforce quitting (Best,
work about equally well with similar people in Bass, & Owen, 1977; Elliott & Denny, 1978;
each sample studied because they share com- Kapel, 1974).
mon components such as motivated volunteer- The varied outcome of combined therapies
ing, behavioral structuring, and self-monitor- may seem discouraging, but it should not be
ing (McFall & Hammen, 1971). At this point unexpected, as most of the studies compared
no particular therapy stands out as vastly su- and combined treatments on an empirical basis
perior to the others, and simple interventions (e.g., Elliott & Denny, 1978); the theories
by medical practitioners may well match the used to design these treatments are theories
effectiveness of more complex therapies. of intervention, not theories of smoking. Al-
Given the poor long-term results, one might though all of the techniques attempt to in-
ask whether the effects of different therapies fluence the person's motivation to quit and
in combination are superior to the effects of develop skills to break the habit, both moti-
each used alone. Recent data suggests that vation and skills training are conceptualized
combinations of therapeutic procedures con- in an overly simplistic way. For example, mo-
taining both motivational and skill-training tivation is usually seen as a combination of
components produce more favorable outcomes, undefined internal desires and various external
though the increments are not striking (Dela- commitments and contracts as well as the de-
hunt & Curran, 1976; Elliott & Denney, 1978; sire to control the impulse to smoke. Such
Lando, 1977; Pederson et al., 1975; Suther- impulses are usually defined in vague and gen-
land et al., 197S). In some instances, however, eral terms such as addiction, anxiety, or lack
combinations produce no positive gain over of health concern. Rarely is any attention paid
single therapies (Danaher, 1977) and may to the complex psychological and physiologi-
even decrease effectiveness (Lamontagne, Gag- cal processes underlying smoking and the tre-
non, & Gaudette, 1978). Best (197S) com- mendous individual differences in these pro-
bined aversion therapy with three other pro- cesses. Skill training usually focuses on tech-
cedures: (a) focusing treatment on internal niques such as step-by-step self-management
or external cues; (b) punishment, by smoking programs (e.g., Conway, 1977), how to make
at double the normal rate on a clay following smoking inconvenient, attaching smoking to
an infraction; and (c) presenting an attitude some new stimulus such as a buzzer, counting,
change communication either before or after and so forth and ignores or fails to identify
behavior change. None of the three procedures the multifaceted stimuli actually controlling
produced main effects or interacted with aver- the behavior. Unless the range, frequency,
sion therapy. Two of the factors (treatment periodicity, and strength of the various stim-
focus and timing of attitude change) inter- uli controlling a person's smoking are identi-
acted significantly with client characteristics fied and dealt with, the skills taught in any
in determining treatment outcome, but the intervention program will be only partially
matching of the intervention factors to sub-
ject personality dispositions did not augment relevant to the person's problems and may in
the impact of the aversive treatment; rather, fact lure the person into a false sense of se-
it prevented the added components from re- curity about his or her ability to control the
ducing the impact of treatment relative to an habit. It could be counterproductive to teach
aversive conditioning only control. Similarly, people skills to stop smoking if they learn
in two studies of sensory deprivation (Sued- that even with professionally taught tech-
feld & Ikard, 1974; Suedfeld et al., 1972), niques they are incapable of controlling their
there was no lasting increment in smoking re- urge to smoke. This need not happen if the
duction as a result of adding a procedure (ex- participant is appropriately prepared and so is
plicit persuasive messages) to the basic treat- led to recognize the trial-and-error or experi-
ment. Several studies here found decreases in mental aspects of the procedures.
376 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

Public Health Model compared with milder messages, generate


stronger attitudes and intentions favorable to
The public health model is clearly a mass stopping smoking. But although these atti-
communication model. The hope is to find a tudes and behavioral intentions may be strong
cost-effective means of reducing the smoking immediately after the message, they are not
epidemic. The basic assumption appears to be persistent; the attitudes and related inten-
that repetition of the antismoking message tions dissipate as the emotion associated with
over multiple media will attract attention, the message weakens (Leventhal & Niles,
lead to its learning, change attitudes, and 1965).
change behavior. If advertising does not work It was also found that strong fear messages
in this straightforward way, why then do might stimulate undesirable reactions such as
cigarette companies, auto manufacturers, avoidance of threatening situations; for ex-
pharmaceutical houses, and others spend bil- ample, not taking X rays (Leventhal & Watts,
lions on advertising? Unfortunately, this 1966) and not exposing oneself to health
simple compliance model (Kelman, 1958) of information.
media effects does little more than hold out Messages conveying information on per-
false hope. Our review of the literature from sonal vulnerability to damage, a variable
laboratory communication studies and school identified by the health belief model as a
and community studies shows how the model central mediator of health decisions (Hoch-
has misdirected media efforts, making these baum, 1965; Leventhal, Hochbaum, & Rosen-
intervention efforts even less effective than stock, 1960; Maiman & Becker, 1974; Rosen-
they should be. stock, 1974), appear to be successful in stim-
Types of field study. Studies of antismok- ulating feelings of personal vulnerability, thus
ing programs in this area fall into one of three strengthening antismoking attitudes and re-
categories: (a) small-scale communication ex- ducing smoking (Horn, 1960; Janis & Mann,
periments, (b) studies in schools, and (c) 1965; B. Mausner & Platt, 1971; Watts,
studies in communities. These studies vary 1968). But these messages can have unex-
greatly in the degree to which they provide pected effects when combined with informa-
outcome statistics, for example, figures on tion on threat; whereas the independent ef-
number of smokers quitting in experimental fects of each factor are often favorable, their
versus control conditions, and/or insight into joint effects may be less than that of either
the social and psychological processes under- alone (Leventhal, 1970; B. Mausner & Platt,
lying continuance and change in smoking 1971; Niles, 1964; Watts, 1968). Combining
behavior. these factors seems to stimulate the belief
Small-scale communication studies. The that protective action is impossible (Selig-
majority of the studies carried out in a mass man, 1975) by undercutting feelings of com-
communication framework on changing health petence.
attitudes and behavior have examined two Studies in our laboratory (Leventhal, 1974)
kinds of message content: (a) information also showed that although vivid threat mes-
that is fear arousing (scenes of lung surgery, sages aroused more fear and stronger inten-
death from tetanus, etc.) and (b) informa- tions to stop smoking, these changes did not
tion that provokes a sense of personal vul- lead to behavior change in the absence of
nerability to the illness threat. Findings from specific action plans. Action plans specified
these studies show that the process of influ- the details of the recommended response;
ence is far more complex than that pictured for example, when, where, and how to get a
by the compliance model. First, studies on chest X ray or tetanus shot, including exact
tetanus shot taking, X ray taking, and smok- streets, turns, and landmarks, how to control
ing cessation conducted by our group in the the urge and how to regulate external induce-
mid 1960s (Dabbs & Leventhal, 1966; Leven- ments to smoke. Subjects who received spe-
thal, Singer, & Jones, 196S; Leventhal & cific instructions on how to control the smok-
Watts, 1966; Leventhal, Watts, & Pagano, ing habit along with a high- or a low-fear
1967) indicate that vivid threat messages, message showed significantly reduced smoking
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 377

at a 3-month follow-up; the uninstructed one must integrate information about a wide
groups had reverted to baseline levels. When variety of perceived outcomes of smoking to
presented without a fear message, the specific predict whether subjects will cut down or
action plans had no effects on attitudes or quit. His data showed that people make the
behavior. Both motivation and action plans decision to stop smoking not only because
are necessary for action. The specific action they have a heightened fear of the conse-
plans apparently serve to insure the carry-over quences of continuing to smoke but also
of attitudes to action, developing feelings of because they have an increased expectation of
competence. Other factors that might be im- the benefits of stopping.
portant in developing an action structure (D. Each type of appeal may have its own
Cartwright, 1949) include the act's relevance specific pattern of effects on attitudes, inten-
to the goal and reference group support for tions, and behaviors, and each may interact
the goal (Fishbein, 1967). in interesting and sometimes unexpected ways
Small-scale communication studies have also with other appeals, depending on the char-
demonstrated that the impact of persuasive acteristics of the source and audience. More
health messages differs with the population detailed and precise theoretical models are
studied. For example, educational level alters needed to deal with these complexities (Lev-
response to threat messages: Students in col- enthal, 1974; Peak, 19SS; Rosenberg & Abel-
lege-directed programs do not become more son, 1960).
persuaded of the need to take protective ac- School studies. The school studies are of
tion as messages become more fear provoking; special interest because of the often expressed
students in noncollege programs do (Haefner, hope that risk prevention will be more effec-
196S; Singer, 196S). Response to increasingly tive than risk reduction (Leventhal, 1973).
threatening communications is also moderated As early as 1968, a report by Merki, Creswell,
by self-esteem: Subjects low in esteem may Stone, Huffman, and Newman mentioned that
fail to act when made overly fearful (Korn- over SO studies of antismoking programs had
zweig, 1968) though efforts to enhance the in- been conducted in school systems. More re-
dividual's sense of competence before exposing cently Thompson (1978) reviewed twice that
him or her to threat can overcome this bar- number (see, also, Green, 1979), Some used
rier (T. J. Rosen, 1970). The esteem factor simple posttest designs, with questionnaires
may also be related to the helplessness phe- given to assess attitudes and behavior in
nomenon discussed earlier. Individual dif- youngsters after an antismoking program
ference variables may be more or less impor- (Morison & Medovy, 1961; Morison, Me-
tant at different stages of the processing dovy, & MacDonell, 1964; Swinehart, 1966).
sequence. Some individual differences may be Others used complex experimental designs,
more important with regard to stimulating with programs varying on several factors,
motivation and decisions to quit smoking, either with a single school assigned to each
others may be more important with respect to type of communication program and com-
planning and action, and still others may be pared with a control (nonprogram) school
more important with respect to establishing (Monk, Tayback, & Gordon, 196S) or with
sustained behavioral change. several schools or classes randomly assigned
Little research has been directed to other to both experimental and control conditions
types of information that might increase the (Evans, 1976; Holland & Elliott, 1968; Horn,
desire to stop smoking, such as economic fac- 1960; Merki et al, 1968; Piper, Jones, &
Matthews, 1970, 1971, 1974; Rabinowitz &
tors (cost of cigarettes, of maintaining health,
Zimmerli, 1974); the latter procedure per-
and of treating diseases resulting from smok- mitted an estimate of program effectiveness
ing), positive emotions or humor (Evans, independent of the particular classroom or
Rozelle, Lasater, Dembroski, & Allen, 1970), school.
and messages that introduce special effects on The programs are varied. The most limited
the self-image of the recipient (Evans, 1976). might include lectures by the school principal
B. Mausner (1973), for example, found that or a physician and posters displayed about the
378 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

school. More intensive programs add threat tion or higher cost. The issue is what type of
films, teacher participation (e.g., introducing information or change in the social context
material on smoking into science and hygiene is necessary to achieve impact, not how to
classes; Morison et al., 1964), and student package or present the information. The stud-
participation (e.g., planning communication ies discussed attempted to build antismoking
content, poster construction, antismoking motivation through an emphasis on health
essays, and group discussion). risks and attempted to shape behavior using
These programs are generally successful in models and role playing to develop action
teaching students about the hazards of smok- plans for dealing with the motivation to
ing; those participating in the programs smoke. For example, Evans's (1976) inno-
acquire more knowledge than do unexposed vative school program is based on concepts
students (Jefferys & Westaway, 1961; Monk such as role playing specific action plans to
et al., 1965; Morison et al.. 1964; Piper et al., innoculate oneself against later peer pressures
1974; Rabinowitz & Zimmerli, 1974). In (Leventhal et al., 1967; McGuire, 1964).
addition, antismoking attitudes are usually However, none of the programs reviewed deal
stronger in the schools with the program than extensively with the motivations to smoke,
in control schools. These differences, however, such as the need to be accepted (Hill, 1971),
are not always statistically reliable, and when achieve status and self-definition (Ferguson,
reliable they are often small. Freedman, & Ferguson, 1977; Newman,
Unfortunately, behavioral effects are usually 1970b) or control anxiety and distress (Ikard
nonexistent, and when they do appear they are & Tomkins, 1973). Only recently has anyone
usually of small magnitude. For example, attempted to make vivid the immediate im-
Horn (1960) reported roughly 10% less pact of smoking on the body (Mitchell, 1978;
smoking in a school given messages about the C. L. Williams, Arnold, & Wynder, 1977), and
potential future dangers of smoking. Piper et rigorous tests of these procedures are lacking.
al. (1971) reported 5% ( f o r females) to 1% What is more, the programs fail to provide
(for males) lower smoking rates in experi- ways of sustaining nonsmoking attitudes and
mental than in control schools, and Rabino- behaviors, by tying them either to more
witz and Zimmerli (1974) reported about an fundamental self-concepts or to social and
\lc/c drop in the experimental and a 1% in- group norms (Salber, Freeman, & Abelin,
crease in the control schools. In some studies 1968).
conducted with very young children, there are It appears that teacher and student involve-
indications of a drop in the rate of recruit- ment can enhance program effectiveness, but
ment to smoking (Evans, 1976; Merki et al., nowhere to the degree hoped for (Morison et
1968); the difference here is also roughly al., 1964). Also, although many efforts in
lO^o fewer students recruited to smoking. schools are motivated by the belief that pre-
Perry, McAlister, and Farquhar (Note 2) also ventive efforts with children will be more
reported that there were 5%&% fewer stu- effective than intervention efforts with adults,
dents who smoked in treatment groups than in it is conceivable that prevention programs
control groups. The Evans (1976), Rabino- merely delay the onset of smoking rather than
witz and Zimmerli (1974), and Perry et al. actually reduce the proportion of youngsters
(Note 2) studies are of special interest, as who become smokers. We have little reason to
their programs were initiated with very young believe that children are more easily influ-
children. enced than adults or that the influence will
The outcomes of these studies are clearly carry over to the later years (Leventhal,
small in magnitude; indeed, Thompson 1973). In fact, studies in Canada (Piper et
(1978) suggested that none of the programs al., 1970) and Portland, Oregon (Horn,
are effective. Is there reason to anticipate any 1960), suggested that the effect of programs
substantial increase in the effectiveness of in the Sth through 10th grades all but disap-
such programs in studies of higher quality? pear by the llth and 12th grades.
The answer to this question is likely to be no, One important benefit of these studies is
if quality is equated with technical sophistica- that they have provided us with a good deal
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 379

of descriptive information about the factors media-only community, and a 12% increase
associated with the initiation of smoking. in the control community. The difference be-
Examples of such variables are social class tween the intensive instruction and control
(Borland & Rudolph, 1975; A, Cartwright, community was statistically significant, but
Martin, & Thompson, 19S9; Leeder & Wool- the difference between the mass media and
cock, 1973), use of other drugs (Kandel, control community was not.
1973, 1975), the image of what a "smoker" Among high-risk participants (risk defined
is (Bland, Bewley, & Day, 1975), and pa- by a multiple logistic risk function), 20 out
rental smoking (Baer & Katkin, 1971). They of the 40 smokers quit for 2 years following
have also provided information about the exposure to both media and intensive instruc-
process of becoming a heavy smoker (e.g., tion, whereas only 15% of the smokers quit
Salber et al, 1968; Salber, Goldman, Buka, in the control community (Meyer et al.,
& Welsh, 1961; Salber, Reed, Harrison, & 1980). This 35% difference is large but not
Green, 1963). Unfortunately, the studies substantially greater than that reported in
lacked an adequate conceptual base, and thus other therapy studies (Leventhal, Safer,
their contribution to a truly effective science Cleary, & Gutrnann, 1980; Schwartz, 1969).
of prevention and intervention has been lim- A recent study in Belgium (Kornitzer, De-
ited (Evans, Henderson, Hill, & Raines, Backer, Dramaix, & Thilly, 1980) investi-
1979). gated the impact of a health program on
Community studies. Evaluations of a smoking, blood pressure, serum cholesterol,
number of community antismoking efforts in and so forth in randomly selected and high-
Scotland (McKennell, 1968) show essentially risk male factory workers. Thirty factories
no change from pre- to postcampaign assess- were randomly assigned to intervention and
ments of attitudes and smoking behavior in control conditions, and 16,222 male workers
the program community (A. Cartwright et were studiedthe men were about equally
al., 1959). distributed in the two groups. Among the
Perhaps the single largest community study workers of high risk, 18.7% stopped smoking
to date is the Stanford Heart Disease Preven- in the intervention group, and 12.2% re-
tion Program (Maccoby & Farquhar, 1975; ported stopping in the control group, a dif-
Meyer, Nash, McAlister, Maccoby, & Far- ference that is significant, given the large
quhar, 1980; Stern, Farquhar, Maccoby, & sample. There were no differences between
Russell, 1976) that studied three communities, experimental and control groups for the ran-
one of which served as a control, a second of dom-sample (non-high-risk) participants. The
which was exposed to a mass media campaign intervention material (TV, booklets, medical
on heart disease risk factors including smok- advice) does not seem to have been designed
ing, and a third of which received a mass to achieve maximum behavior change, and it
media campaign and face-to-face instruction is difficult to tell why this group achieved
for selected high-risk persons. Although the effects for reductions in serum cholesterol
study is classified as a community study, the similar to those of the Stanford program
behavioral therapies in the intensive treat- while doing so much more poorly for smoking.
ment condition were traditional behavior It is important to compare the results of
therapy interventions (Meyer & Henderson, community studies with prior expectations
1974), and the intensive treatment results are lest we too quickly regard these efforts as
better compared with those of therapy studies failures and overlook the lessons they teach.
than with those of studies in which commu- We know from past research that the effect of
nity programs (media, use of existent groups, media on anything except minor decisions
etc.) were the vehicles for intervention. among brands is likely to be small (Katz &
At a 2-year follow-up, there was a 17% Lazarsfeld, 1955; Klapper, 1961; O'Keefe,
reduction in the number of smokers (relative 1971; Weiss, 1969). For most topics studied,
to base levels) in the media plus face-to-face for example, changes in political attitudes
instruction community, a 5% reduction in the and behavior (Klapper, 1961), movie going,
380 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

or prescribing of medications by physicians 3.9% of the active female smokers and 14.27c
(Katz & Lazarsfeld, 1955), media techniques of the active male smokers were abiding by
are less effective in changing beliefs or be- their nonsmoking pledge. As Leventhal et al.
havior than is face-to-face communication in a (1980) noted, most community studies give
laboratory setting. There are at least four us little more than outcomes for smoking. It
reasons why this is the case. First, exposure is the rare study that tells us about the so-
to the message, assured in a face-to-face set- cial-psychological factors associated with the
ting, is uncontrollable in the field. Second, initiation and cessation of the habit (e.g., Mc-
media messages are usually symbolic and Kennell, 1973).
abstract and do not focus on the concrete Indeed, we know far more about the process
experiences that are salient to the audience. of media influence on individual opinion lead-
With few exceptions (e.g., Evans, 1976; Horn, ers and the interactions among persons and
1960; Meyer et al., 1980), researchers have groups in establishing the stages that led
failed to include specific action instructions to farmers to adopt new seed and forms of crop
ensure that motivational effects are carried rotation (Rogers & Shoemaker, 1971) than
forward into performance. Third, messages we know about the impact of these same
are often developed on the basis of hunches, processes on an individual's decision and abil-
with minimal prior experimental assessment ity to quit smoking.
or formative evaluation of actual interest in
and response to the message by the audience Overall Evaluation of Intervention Efforts
of interest (Scriven, 1967). Fourth, the con-
text of communication is different for media From the practical point of view, when the
studies than for therapy or laboratory com- goal was to demonstrate the effectiveness of
munication studies. In media studies the audi- techniques to discourage new smokers and
ence may be alone at home, among friends, encourage quitting, the studies reviewed dem-
or in school, and the context and groups may onstrated that specific, isolated programs must
interfere with or be supportive of message be regarded largely as failures. What is needed
acceptance. Social contexts, the individual's is innovative studies that provide more in-
reference, and face-to-face groups provide sight into processes involved in the initiation
important anchors for his or her beliefs and and cessation of smoking (see Leventhal et
action (Weiss, 1969). When those group al., 1980; Lichtenstein, 1971). Past studies
memberships and their associated norms are were conducted in a communication frame-
salient, influence by media is usually weak work, a communicator delivering a message
(Bauer & Bauer, 1960; Kelley, 1958; Kel- over a particular medium to a particular recip-
ley & Volkart, 1952). ient, and focused on outcome, that is, number
As could be expected, the community stud- of people quitting or cigarettes reduced. As a
ies show that comrnunitywide campaigns have whole the studies make clear the incorrect-
had only a minor impact on smoking. The ness of the common sense view of communica-
most dramatic example of the limitations of tion as a process in which a message is re-
community programs is Ryan's (1973) report peated by an authoritative source (TV star,
of the cold turkey project in Greenfield, Iowa. famous athlete, Nobel laureate, doctor) a
In association with United Artists's filming of sufficient number of times (a) to capture at-
Cold Tiirkey on location in Greenfield, the tention, (b) to produce learning of the con-
tent, (c) to change attitudes, and (d) to
city council voted August 8th, 1969, Cold
change behavior. This compliance model of
Turkey Day, declared it a holiday, had smok-
the communication process (Kelman, 1958)
ers sign pledges to quit, advertised the dangers sees the steps from attending to behavior
of smoking and virtues of quitting, installed change as an inexorable progression. It leaves
no smoking signs, removed tobacco from few degrees of freedom for complex processes
stores, and had a 30-day follow-up honoring in message receivers or their social context
the quitters. Seven months later Ryan inter- and does not accurately describe what we
viewed 1,385 respondents and found that only know about these processes. Because this
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 381

model focuses on the trappings of the act of gent in ignoring changes in community values
communication and not the process, it should for smoking reduction and that these changes
come as no surprise that major conceptual may be the primary levers for initiating and
and substantive problems concerning the maintaining smoking cessation. The first is a
process of intervention and the processes complex statistical analysis of cigarette con-
underlying smoking have, to a large extent, sumption over the years immediately preced-
been ignored. ing and following the surgeon general's report.
There are several ways in which the com- The second is national survey data collected
munication model is deficient in describing by the National Clearinghouse for Smoking
the intervention process. First, nearly all in- Research (U.S. Dept. of Health, Education
tervention efforts reviewed involved a single and Welfare, 1973). Warner (1977) showed
effort or a series of efforts over a relatively that although major events in the campaign
short span of time. Second, intervention ef- against smoking, such as the surgeon general's
forts have usually targeted the individual. report (U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and
This is true for school and community studies Welfare, 1964), may have caused only a 4%-
as well as for therapy studies. The involve- 5% transitory decrease in annual per capita
ment of peers, groups, family, and so forth is consumption, the cumulative effects are more
generally minimal, and little attention is paid dramatic. He estimated that in the absence
to the way these group and social factors are of the antisrnoking campaign, per capita con-
associated with individual change. It is usually sumption would have exceeded its actual 1975
assumed that intensive, individual therapy value by 20% or 30%. This is a conservative
can generate the most significant impact on estimate, as it ignores other behavior changes
smoking, but the opposite may in fact be true. such as the shift to low-tar and low-nicotine
Intensive efforts to alter societal, community, cigarettes. The National Clearinghouse for
and group values and norms might be more Smoking Research has reported findings from
effective in producing change in smoking be- a pair of U.S. national studies showing the
havior. Restricting smoking in public build- impact of the surgeon general's report on
ings, requiring nonsmoking sections in air- smokers. Of a representative sample of smok-
planes and restaurants and changing attitudes ers first interviewed in 1964, 80% were rein-
about trie rights of nonsmokers might do more terviewed about 20 months later. Of this
to change smoking habits than intensive pro- group, 52.9% considered quitting, 34.4%
cedures directed at individuals. The Stanford tried, 15.5% achieved short-term success, and
investigation has taken a step toward looking 7.1%) achieved long-term success (minimum
for interactions between media and intensive of 3 months). More recent data suggest short-
therapy procedures (Meyer et al., 1980). and long-term success rates roughly double
The available research tells us little about those reported earlier (Horn, Note 3; see,
the perception of community norms and val- also, Swinehart & Kirscht, 1966). Panel data
ues and how such perceptions affect the smok- and the national survey data provided by the
er's desire to smoke, motivation to stop, abil- National Clearinghouse for Smoking Research
ity to stop, and ability to remain a nonsmoker. suggest that the continuing antisrnoking ef-
The school and community studies should fort could have both cumulative effects and
have made a major contribution to our under- a number of unexpected indirect effects; for
standing of these normative and group pro- example, these campaigns stimulated smokers
cesses but they have not. Their failure to do to switch to filter cigarettes, led to the lower-
so reflects a focus on outcome as opposed to ing of tar and nicotine levels (Gori, 1976),
process. The practical demands for solving the
perhaps making it easier for more recently
problem build hopes for quick solutions
through media efforts and detracts from ef- recruited smokers to quit, and stimulated the
forts to trace the complex social processes militancy of nonsmokers for ordinances re-
involved. quiring nonsmoking sections in aircraft, res-
Two kinds of data support the contention taurants, and so forth. All of these factors may
that investigators have been seriously negli- intensify the pressures on smokers to quit or
382 HOWARD LEVENTRAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

reduce their level of smoking (see Eisinger, Investigators are acutely aware of the diffi-
1971). culty of developing methods to sustain reduc-
These historical changes may also have tion and cessation of smoking (Lichtenstein,
important effects on the outcomes of specific 1971). The situation contrasts with the suc-
intervention experiments, as it may be easier cess of behavior therapy in treating phobias
to induce and sustain smoking change now (Lang, Lazovik, & Reynolds, 1965; Lang,
than before. The usual experimental design, Melamed, & Hart, 1970). Results in this latter
for example, therapy versus control or experi- area have been more lasting (e.g., G. M.
mental versus control community, does not Rosen, Glasgow, & Barrera, 1976, 1977), and
allow the investigator to detect these effects, the durability of therapy has been enhanced
as they may interact with the treatment. by conducting therapy in real-life settings
Historical changes may be necessary but not (Sherman, 1972) or by using booster sessions
sufficient conditions for improved treatment and by various forms of modeling with guided
outcomes. The changes in attitude and knowl- participation (Bandura, Blanchard, & Ritter,
edge may also encourage smokers to experi- 1969). However, there is doubt that impres-
ment more with quitting, and participants in sive short-term results of the smoking thera-
experimental treatments may be more likely pies can be extended to the longer term simply
to try a variety of tactics when they experi- by adding on these extra methods. For ex-
ence difficulty in maintaining smoking reduc- ample, buddy systems and follow-up booster
tions. Experimenting with different procedures sessions have been recommended (Hunt &
over a period of time may achieve quitting Matarazzo, 1973), but controlled studies of
rates better than those reported in the studies booster sessions show no advantage over
reviewed. In summary, statements about the standard treatments, such as rapid smoking,
effectiveness of specific treatment interven- for long-term follow-ups of 6 months (Best
tions (of any kind) independent of the norms et al., 1977; Elliott & Denney, 1978; Kapel,
and values of the social context are meaning- 1974).
less. Media and other specific interventions To develop a more powerful therapy that
may operate differently within a context of can sustain long-term change, it is necessary
values and beliefs that are supportive of their to consider both the dependence process itself
messages than in the absence of such a con- and skills for self-regulation. As for depen-
text (see Hovland, 19S4; Weiss, 1969). dence, we need to better understand the
Intervention efforts with school children underlying process, the stimuli that trigger
have also been severely limited by ignoring the craving to smoke, the emotional desires
the processes underlying smoking and by generated by these stimuli (their nature and
treating smoking as a response like any other origin), and the links between these experi-
response. Because of this set, the goal of inter- ences and pharmacological factors such as
vention has been to place a barrier between nicotine (Schachter, Silverstein, & Perlick,
the youngster and his or her first cigarette, 1977, p. 40). One can readily collect system-
hence the focus on techniques for resisting atic data and incidental comments pointing
peer pressure. But as is demonstrated in the to a host of factors that make it difficult to
following section, smoking has a long and sustain quitting. These include a wide range
complex developmental history, beginning of physiological and subjective withdrawal
well before the first cigarette is smoked and symptoms, some of which appear soon after
extending sometimes over S or 10 years to smoking cessation (e.g., decreases in heart
rate and systolic blood pressure, declines in
dependence. This history can be divided into
body temperature, increase in low-frequency
stages and processes, and preventive methods alpha, drops in adrenaline and norepine-
should be designed to deal with the key phrine) and others of which appear over
points of this history and should incorporate longer periods of time (e.g., weight gain, im-
different procedures to deter smoking at each paired psychomotor performance, nausea,
step in the process of becoming a dependent headache, constipation, drowsiness, fatigue,
smoker. sleep disturbances, and increases in anxiety,
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 383

irritability, and hostility; Shiftman, Note 4). of generating new and more powerful anti-
Some of these signs, for example, images and smoking programs. It is deficient because it
dreams of smoking (Knapp et al, 1963) and fails to fully take into account the many
craving (Shiftman, Note 4) may appear 5 or complex social and individual processes medi-
more years after quitting. Many of these ating intervention efforts and the functions
symptoms appear to be more noticeable underlying smoking. It is naive to expect
among neurotic than nonneurotic quitters remote threats of illness to serve as sufficient
(Burns, 1969). Current intervention methods motives for smoking prevention, cessation, or
fail to take account of these effects and offer abstinance. The research reviewed suggests
no means of dealing with these phenomena. that the functions of smoking are extremely
The second issue, the longer term aspects important to the individual smoker, and if we
of developing skills for self-regulation, is are to be at all successful in our bid to reduce
receiving more attention from social behavior smoking, we must identify substitutes for
theorists (e.g., Bandura, 1977; Kanfer, 197S; these functions and stimulate motivation to
Meichenbaum, 1979) but needs much work in adopt those substitutes by vivid and salient
the specifics of efforts to self-regulate smok- illustrations of the actual impact of smoking
ing. We know little about the effects of re- on an individual's health. In addition, we must
peated attempts to quit, the consequences of develop better techniques to induce cessation
success followed by failure, or of overt success that are based on the mechanism(s) control-
(not smoking) and covert yearning, on belief ling the habit. In B. Mausner's (1973) terms,
in one's ability to self-regulate and willingness we must deal with the full range of factors
to exert effort to do so. We know less than we that determine the expected utility of
should about the events surrounding success smoking.
and failure in smoking withdrawal (e.g., In this section we present material basic to
Pechacek, 1979; Schwartz & Dubitzky, 1968; the conceptual and empirical aspects of the
Shiftman, Note 4; Russell, Note S). Lack of initiation, maintenance, and cessation of smok-
knowledge of the long-term aspects of the ing. We present our ideas in a developmental
dependence and coping process is clearly due framework, that is, in terms of stages leading
in part to the assumption that smoking is from anticipation and initial experimentation
treatable by methods applicable to any other with cigarettes to regular smoking and smok-
response. Ignoring the processes underlying ing cessation. We relate our theory of the
the behavior, its similarities and uniqueness stages of smoking to the intervention ap-
in the internal (affective and pharmacological) proaches reviewed and make suggestions for
and external domain (the stimuli provoking new interventions.
pharmacological and affective changes),
greatly limits our ability to develop an inter- Stages of Smoking
vention capable of maintaining long-term
smoking withdrawal. Smoking has a complex ontogeny (Hoch-
In the next section, we review and evaluate baum, 1965; Leventhal, 1968; B. Mausner &
several models of smoking behavior. Our goal Platt, 1971; McKennell, 1968; Tomkins,
is to discuss what is known about the origin 1968; Horn, Note 1). The developmental
and maintenance of smoking, to evaluate the history for the individual smoker seems to
relevance of this information to past inter- move in steps through the stages of prepara-
vention procedures, and to suggest new possi- tion, initiation, becoming a smoker, and main-
bilities for intervention. tenance 0} smoking (Dunn, 1973; Salber et
al., 1968) to stages of dissatisfaction, decision
Theories of Smoking to stop, and adoption and maintenance of the
self-image of ex- or nonsmoker (B. Mausner
We have found that the model separating & Platt, 1971; Sarbin & Nucci, 1973). Our
motivational and skill factors is an improve- discussion focuses on the first four of these
ment on the simple compliance model of inter- stages: preparation, initiation, becoming, and
vention, but it is still inadequate to the task maintenance.
384 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARV

Preparation, Initiation, and Becoming: there is a preparatory stage that includes


First Three Stages oj Smoking perceptions of what smoking involves and
what the potential functions of smoking are.
Social pressure is probably a prime initiator Grade school students assign different person-
of experimentation with cigarettes (Bergen & ality characteristics to smokers and nonsmok-
Olesen, 1963; Hill, 1971); at the urging of ers; they rate the smoker as foolish, careless,
peers or perhaps an elder sibling (Gorsuch & tough, easygoing, lazy, more often in trouble,
Butler, 1976), a youngster is encouraged to and so forth (Bland et al., 197S; Bynner,
try a cigarette. Smoking by family members, 1970). Interestingly, few youngsters who
parents in particular, seems to operate by smoke see themselves as possessing these
reducing the barriers to peer pressure (Bor- characteristics. Why would this image serve
land & Rudolph, 197S) and by increasing the as a motive to smoke? One possibility is that
opportunities for obtaining cigarettes for it suggests a toughness, an ability to express
further experimentation (Baer & Katkin, impulses, an independence from authority,
1971). and a state of being grown-up and successful
Initial experimentation is a critical step to in areas that, though "bad," are exciting and
becoming a smoker; data show that 85$- interesting. These inviting aspects of the
90$ of those who smoke four cigarettes image may encourage experimentation with
become regular smokers (Salber et al., 1968), smoking, and we believe they may form one
implying that adolescent experimentation with basis for becoming a smoker. Indeed, we
smoking led to adult smoking whether or not speculate that children who are less successful
the young people had originally intended to in school (Bewley & Bland, 1977; Borland &
become smokers. Rudolf, 197S; Horn, 1963; Jessor & Jessor,
One consequence of these findings has been 1977; Newman, 1970b), more rebellious, and
the inclusion in antismoking programs of doing less well in meeting expectations of
educational material on the social pressures parents and traditional authorities (Newman,
that promote smoking. Evans (1976) and his 1970a; Smith, 1970; Stewart & Livson, 1966)
associates (Evans et al., 1978) are teaching are more likely to be attracted to smoking at
grade school students about peer and media an early age and begin using cigarettes as a
pressure and using films of models demon- means of defining themselves as tough, cool,
strating self-assertive techniques to avoid ac- and independent of authority, as well as using
cepting cigarettes, and so on. This innovative other drugs later on (Seltzer, Friedman, &
approach to action planning seems likely to Siegelaub, 1974; Tennant & Detels, 1976).
succeed with those who do not wish to experi- It is unlikely that youngsters who define
ment with smoking, but it may be less effec- themselves as independent of authority try
tive with those who want to experiment or cigarettes to comply with peer pressures.
take on the air of maturity, rebelliousness, and Youngsters who first try cigarettes at the
sexual sophistication often attributed to the urging of friends may have a different pre-
smoker (Leventhal, 1973). paratory set. Some may feel anxious and in-
The limited successfulness of these well- adequate and smoke to achieve social accep-
designed programs suggests that there is more tance and be part of the gang. Others may
to the initiation of smoking than social pres- share a wide variety of experimental activities
sure. It is likely that "smoking" begins well with a close friend, and smoking may simply
before a child tries a first cigarette. Children be one of the experimental or risk-taking
develop attitudes about smoking and have actions. These two sets of factors, defining the
images of what smoking is like well before self as tough and seeking social approval, are
they try it. These attitudes seem to be im- not likely to exhaust the attitudes that make
portant in the development of the smoking up the preparatory stage. It is probable that
habit. In one study a statement of intention some people begin smoking to control emo-
to try cigarettes proved to be the best single tions such as work anxiety. Smoking may be
predictor of later smoking (Newman, Martin, seen as useful for enhancing performance on
& Petersen, Note 6). Indeed, it is clear that examinations and increasing one's chances of
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 385

achieving the rewards of high grades and good longitudinal data would reveal a variety
admission to top colleges. This orientation of patterns, but we also suspect that Salber
may be important for individuals starting et al. (1968) are correct in estimating that 2
smoking relatively late in adolescence or early years pass between initial experimentation
adulthood, the kind of smoking seen in college and becoming a regular smoker.
and medical students (Borgatta & Evans, What does this analysis suggest about the
1968; Smith, 1970; Thomas, 1960). intervention process? First, it is clear that dif-
Whatever the reasons, preparation for trying ferent information is needed to deal with the
the first cigarette is only a beginning step: different stages, that is, with preparation and
The next is actually smoking one's first cig- the different readiness or sets within it, with
arette. Smoking the first cigarette is but one initial experiences, and with becoming a smoker
additional step in the developmental process. through repeated tries and regular usage. Sec-
Although the Salber et al. (1968) data sug- ond, it is clear we need to understand more
gested that smoking four cigarettes was suf- about the mechanisms underlying these phe-
ficient to lead to adult smoking and depen- nomena to select content and to decide how
dence on cigarettes, it is also clear that many best to present this information. If prepara-
youngsters never get to their fourth cigarette, tory sets exist, that is, if young people do
Studies show that &0%-9Qfo of youngsters start smoking to define themselves, to achieve
have tried at least one cigarette (Grant & social acceptance, and to control affect, why
Weitman, 1968; Palmer, 1970; Wohlford & are these gains so clear and the losses so ob-
Giammona, 1969), yet the proportion of scure? Can we make salient the paradox that
smokers in grade, junior, and high school young smokers do not apply the negative im-
years rarely exceeds 50%'of those who try. age of "smoker" to themselves? In other
Something happens to move many of the ini- words, can we make young people see that
tial triers off the experimental track. when they try smoking they are adopting a
Indeed, the data seem to support the hy- habit they view negatively? Making explicit
pothesis that it takes upwards of two years the differences between the students' current
and possibly longer (A. Cartwright et al., self-images and their view of a "smoker" may
1959) from initial tries and occasional experi- lead some students to question their motiva-
mentation to arrive at heavy, consistent smok- tion to experiment. Also, it suggests that
ing: This is the stage of becoming a smoker. efforts be made to show that the exciting as-
There are gradual increases in the percentage pects of the image are illusory and that good
of students smoking (1% in 7th grade to health, attractive appearance, neat dress, and
46% in llth grade) and gradual increases in not smoking are linked to greater success in
the number of cigarettes smoked (1 per week sex, greater strength and daring, and so forth.
to 20 per day), with the big increase occurring Unfortunately, smoking is highly salient: It
as females enter the 5-9 per day category and presents a concretely visible action by an in-
as males enter the 10-19 per day category in dividual as he or she relates to others. It may
Grade 10 (Horn, Courts, Taylor, & Solomon, be difficult to make the behaviors leading to
1959; Salber et al., 1961). Unfortunately, economic and social success equally salient;
many of the data are cross-sectional, and those they may be less visible, and adults may be
that are not are retrospective (A. Cartwright less willing and less able to articulate the role
et al., 1959), so we do not know whether there they play in these achievements.
is a gradual change in the percentage of smok- It is also clear that the grade schooler's
ing and the amount smoked by individual picture of the smoker is incomplete. Young
smokers or if the average reflects an agglom- people in the preparatory phase seem to be
eration with some students rapidly moving unaware of the problem of addiction or de-
from experimentation to pack-a-day smoking, pendence. They believe they will be able to
others gradually making the transition, and stop smoking when they wish and seem un-
some staying at low levels of consumption aware of the degree to which adults become
that vary little over time. We believe that dependent on cigarettes (Brecher, 1972) and
386 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

struggle and humiliate themselves for their from the experience of adaptation. To counter
inability to control the habit. Elaborating the such beliefs, it may be necessary to alter the
presmoker's concept of the smoker as a per- school curriculum to increase understanding
son lacking self-control, using cigarettes to of the human body and its reaction to insult.
compensate for inadequate social skills, inabil- This is a general issue in biological educa-
ity to manage emotions, and inadequacy in tion; it is not specific to smoking. Our con-
heterosexual relationships, might add sub- ceptions of pain are undifferentiated and un-
stantially to the perceived unattractiveness of informed. Children can be taught about pain,
the habit. It might also conflict more directly symptoms, and their adaptation and that not
with motivation to define oneself as strong all serious illness and harm are preceded by
and "cool" and socially competent. The pain (see Leventhal & Everhart, 1979). The
thought of using smoking to regulate emotional initial cigarette might also seem much more
tension might even become aversive if it elicits dangerous if the various sensations have been
anxiety at loss of self-control. Thus informa- described and clearly labeled as cues to noxious
tion on dependence could be tuned to each of effects on sensory organs and tissues.
the preparatory views of smoking. Finally, it is important to confront the is-
The second stage, the initial tries at smok- sue of becoming a regular smoker. The grad-
ing, appears to be a time when many young- ual increase in frequency of smoking and the
sters decide not to continue with their experi- increasing variety of situations in which cig-
mentation. Although the initial harshness of arettes are used suggests that becoming a
smoking may be a critical factor in this de- smoker is not unlike the process involved in
cision, it seems unlikely that individual dif- concept formation, with the smoker going
ferences in physiological responsivity to smoke through a lengthy stage of "correct respond-
and heat can fully account for who does and ing," that is, smoking when it is appropriate
who does not stop at this point. The mere to do so, before he or she develops a schema
presence of pain is not sufficient to deter or or self-image as a smoker (Hull, 1920; Posner,
promote behavior; the way a pain is inter- 1973). Several nonverbal motivational sys-
preted, for example, as a sign of illness and tems, iconic, kinesthetic, autonomic, and so
danger to the self (Safer, Tharps, Jackson, & forth, may be involved in integrating smoking
Leventhal, 1979; Suchman, 1965; Zborowski, with a variety of coping responses before the
19S2) is critical for motivating seeking medi- individual fully develops this self-concept. Re-
cal advice and regarding oneself as at risk. stricting the situations in which smoking oc-
Interpreting noxious bodily sensations as be- curs and developing a narrow definition of the
nign may actually facilitate getting used to and functions of smoking (e.g., as a social gesture)
ignoring them (Leventhal, Brown, Shacham, may help prevent the development of heavy
& Engquist, 1979; Leventhal & Everhart, addictive smoking.
1979) and may play an important role in Unfortunately, we know very little about
adapting to smoking. the processess involved in the stages of be-
With time there is clearly a development of coming a heavy smoker. Longitudinal descrip-
tolerance to the physiological effects of smok- tive data on variables such as those mentioned
ing (Russell, Note 5 ) . The daily experience previously and on other aspects of the social
of smoking and being alert, relaxed, and re- processes involved in smoking (B. Mausner
freshed can then serve as powerful evidence & Platt, 1971) would provide better ground
for its usefulness. The fading of harsh cues for the formulation of process models that
that accompany initial smoking experiences, could guide educational programs.
for example, the burning, roughness, and bite
of the heat and smoke of the cigarette, may
Maintenance: Fourth Stage of Smoking
then be interpreted as evidence that smoking
will not be harmful to oneself. Children's be- Once smoking is an integral part of self-
lief that smoking is harmful for others and for regulation in a variety of situations, the indi-
older people but not for themselves may arise vidual is truly a smoker. Understanding the
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 387

regulatory functions of the behavior is likely Tomkins (1966, 1968) advanced the addi-
to be critical for the development of interven- tional hypothesis that dependence (addiction)
tion techniques able to generate long-term develops by the sequential linking of emotional
smoking cessation and reduction. The fac- states: A negative emotional state (e.g., anx-
tors maintaining smoking have been investi- iety) stimulates smoking, smoking produces a
gated at both a psychological and biological positive affect or lift that reduces the nega-
level. Unfortunately, the two approaches are tive emotion, and when the individual stops
often presented as antagonistic, as though the smoking for a period of time, the negative af-
description of psychological functions has in- fect returns and becomes a powerful stimulus
volved nothing more than investigating the to smoking. Thus the absence of cigarettes re-
phenomenology of the smoking experience or instates smoking and the behavior becomes
smokers' reports of the satisfactions of smok- part of a self-reinforcing system.
ing, whereas biological research has elucidated The factors described by Tomkins appear
only the physiological mechanisms underlying to be reliable. Leventhal and Avis (1976) fac-
smoking. Actually, the two approaches are tor analyzed the responses of four samples of
complementary ways of describing the same college students on different questionnaires on
problem (Graham, 1Q72). It is unlikely that reasons for smoking and found virtually iden-
one can describe the mechanisms for smoking tical results from sample to sample, despite
at a biological level if one cannot first de- changes in the items included in the analysis.
scribe them at a psychological level: Biologi- McKennell (1968) also reported virtually
cal analysis often follows the contours of psy- identical factors from a set of independently
chological analysis. At the very least a psy- derived questions about the occasions on which
chological analysis can lead to a more refined individuals smoked. Thus the reported func-
view of the processes underlying the response tions of smoking appear to be similar whether
and assist in the defining of samples of per- smokers are asked about the satisfactions
sons and situations in which smoking is likely they obtain while smoking or the occasions on
to reflect particular processes for which one which they smoke (Kozlowski, 1979). A study
can locate particular biological mechanisms. by Best and Hakstian (1978) that collected
Psychological models. Psychological models reasons for smoking responses from com-
of smoking are based primarily on factor muters waiting for a ferry failed, however, to
analyses of self-report data. Tomkins's (1966) replicate these findings.
model, the basis for the self-appraisal kit de- Of course, consistency is not equivalent to
veloped by the National Clearinghouse for validity: Smokers may give similar descrip-
Smoking Research (Ikard, Green, & Horn, tions of their reasons for smoking when ques-
1969; Horn, Note 7), suggests that smoking tioned in different ways and still be wrong in
is used to regulate internal emotional states, the causal analysis of their behavior. Leventhal
producing positive emotional reactions and and Avis (1976) and Ikard and Tomkins
minimizing negative emotional reactions in a (1973) examined the validity of these judg-
ments by having subjects complete a question-
variety of situations. Factor analysis of smok-
naire on reasons for smoking, sorting them
ers' ratings of reasons for smoking generate into high and low scorers on particular scales,
factors such as smoking for stimulation, smok- and comparing the actual smoking behaviors
ing for pleasure and relaxation (taste), smok- of these groups in response to experimentally
ing to reduce anxiety or tension, smoking to manipulated variables. When smokers were
reduce social anxiety, smoking to reduce gen- given cigarettes adulterated with vinegar,
eral anxiety, smoking to reduce craving (psy- those high on the pleasure-taste factor showed
chological addiction), smoking simply to a sharp drop in the number of cigarettes
handle the cigarette, and smoking from habit. smoked; those low on the factor did not.
The questions used to measure these factors When smokers were assigned the task of moni-
ask directly about the subjects' experience toring their smoking (filling out a card for
with cigarettes. each cigarette smoked), habit smokers signift-
388 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

cantly reduced their smoking, pleasure-taste smoking types: Time and cost tend to militate
smokers did not (Leventhal & Avis, 1976). against this. The group most clearly targetable
There was more smoking following a fear film by current procedures, the anxiety-reducing
for smokers who used smoking for anxiety re- smoker, is the most intractable to treatment
duction (Ikard & Tomkins, 1973). Both of (e.g., Burns, 1969), and the closest interven-
the studies support earlier data (e.g., Schwartz tionists come to dealing with the addictive
& Dubitzky, 1968) in finding that psycho- smoker is examining the impact of their pro-
logical addicts suffered the most during pe- cedure for light and heavy smokers.
riods of deprivation, though Leventhal and It seems reasonable to conclude therefore
Avis could not document increased smoking by that intervention has proceeded by ignoring
addictive smokers following 18 hours of depri- what is known about the psychological func-
vation. A short deprivation (20 minutes) ap- tions of smoking. The possible exception is
pears to have led to increased smoking by the National Clearinghouse for Smoking Re-
addictive smokers in a study by Herman search manuals for quitting, which ask the
(1974). smoker to self-monitor and score himself or
In summary, the data suggest that the func- herself on the factors, but it is unclear how
tions of smoking are reliably reported and that this is to effect subsequent quitting efforts.
some of these self-reports, addiction being an Pharmacological models. Pharmacological
exception, identify the conditions that stimu- research is designed to locate specific chemi-
late smoking. It is also clear that the rela- cal agents, which are either in the cigarette or
tionship between report and behavior is not generated during combustion, that are re-
perfect. Many smokers have inaccurate per- sponsible both for dependency and the harm-
ceptions of the mechanisms that underly their fulness of smoking and to then develop anti-
smoking, and smokers are more likely to see dotes for them. The most likely candidate for
much of their behavior as habitual after a dependency is nicotine (Jarvik, 1970, 1973).
prolonged period of self-monitoring (Leven- It is not clear precisely how nicotine generates
thal & Avis, 1976). This latter finding sug- dependence. An examination of the literature
gests that systematic self-monitoring could be suggests, however, that three different models
an important precursor to smoking reduction are used to account for nicotine's effectiveness
or withdrawal (Engeln, 1969; Lichtenstcin & in maintaining smoking: (a) the fixed-effect
Keutzer, 1971; McFall & Hammen, 1971). model, (b) the nicotine regulation model,
It is also clear that only a few of the inter- and (c) the multiple regulation model. For
ventions reviewed are relevant to the particu- clarity of exposition we greatly oversimplify
lar functions identified by the reasons for each, recognizing that few if any investigators
smoking factors. Of the behavioral therapies, would adopt these positions when they are
the densensitization procedures appear to tar- reduced to their bare essentials and not com-
get anxiety-control smokers, and aversive pro- bined with other, more psychological processes.
cedures appear to target taste and positive- The nicotine fixed-effect model assumes that
affect smokers. There is little reason, however, smoking is reinforced because nicotine reliably
to argue that the investigators who used these stimulates specific reward-inducing centers of
procedures considered the dynamics of the the nervous system. For example, nicotine
underlying functions in any detail in designing changes the levels of neuroamines (Essman,
their intervention techniques, and virtually 1973), and alters nervous system activity
none of the studies reviewed even bothered to (Brown, 1973); for example, it lowers the.
examine the impact of the intervention on strength of evoked cortical potentials produced
particular smoking subgroups. One might ar- by external stimuli (Hall, Rappaport, Hop-
gue that sensory deprivation (removal of all kins, & Griffin, 1973). Nicotine also speeds
affect-arousing cues) was designed to deal with heart rate (Armitage, 1973) and acts on the
particular smoking subtypes, but it appears inhibitory (Renshaw) cells in the dorsal col-
this method was simply tried, and no effort umn of the spinal cord to produce skeletal
was made to look at the impact for separate muscle relaxation (Domino, 1973). Some of
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 389

these effects are produced indirectly by nico- reductions are statistically significant, they
tine's action on the level of circulating are far smaller than one would expect on the
catecholarnines. basis of a dose-dependent hypothesis (Jarvik,
It is still unknown how nicotine can simul- 1973). Russell and his associates (Russell,
taneously produce autonomic arousal and feel- 1977; Russell, Feyerabend, & Cole, 1976;
ings of mental alertness and relaxation (Gil- Russell, Wilson, Feyerabend, & Cole, 1976)
bert, 1979). For present purposes, it is more had smokers chew nicotine-laden gum, and
important to note that the described effects smoking reductions were slight even with the
are basically acute in nature; that is, they are higher of the two dosage levels used. Russell
induced by nicotine and disappear when the suggests the rate of oral nicotine absorption
drug is withdrawn. These short-term effects may be too slow to affect smoking.
may account for the initial repeated use of It may well be that the oral route is not
cigarettes, but they do not seem able to ac- the best for evaluating the nicotine regulation
count for the extraordinary persistence of hypothesis. Absorption through the lungs
smoking or the inability of most people to and/or other organs may be more effective
avoid returning to smoking after they methods of regulating plasma nicotine levels
have stopped (Russell, 1971a, 1971b). If and testing the nicotine regulation model. In
these effects are viewed as reinforcers, some a series of ingenious studies, Schachter (1977)
additional factors, such as conditioning, would and his associates have generated the most
have to be responsible for maintaining the be- compelling evidence to date for the nicotine
havior. The inability to produce sustained regulation model. In their first study they
smoking withdrawal with behavioral therapies lowered the level of nicotine in cigarettes and
based on operant and classical conditioning found an average increase in cigarettes smoked
models suggests, however, that a simplified of 25.3% for heavy smokers and 17.5% for
version of this model cannot fully describe light smokers. Assuming this change reflected
the mechanisms underlying and controlling the need to maintain an optimal plasma level
the behavior. of nicotine, Schachter, Kozlowski, and Silver-
Nicotine regulation models argue that smok- stein (1977) compared smoking levels in
ing serves to regulate or titrate the level of groups of subjects who took vitamin C, Acidu-
nicotine (or a related agent) in the internal lin, bicarbonate, or placebo pills on different
milieu; departures from the optimal level of weeks. (Subjects were blind as to pill content.)
the agent stimulates smoking. This model These pills varied the pH of the individual's
differs from a simple reinforcement model in urine, which in turn varied the rate of excre-
predicting that smoking is rewarded only when tion of nicotine; when urinary pH is low
the level of nicotine circulating in the body (acidic) nicotine (an alkaloid) is excreted,
is below a "set point" and suggests that sus- and smoking should increase to compensate
taining the level of nicotine is a sufficient for this loss. When urinary pH is high (alka-
mechanism for maintaining smoking. line), nicotine is retained and smoking should
Jarvik (1973) reviewed a considerable body be at relatively low levels. Subjects taking
of evidence consistent with this model. First, vitamin C or Acidulin increased an average of
individuals who switched from high- to low- about four cigarettes per day or roughly 15%-
nicotine cigarettes increased the number of 20% from base levels.
cigarettes smoked, smoked more of the ciga- The findings suggest that smokers, or at
rette, and inhaled more deeply (Frith, 1971a; least heavy smokers, smoke to regulate nico-
Jarvik, 1973), apparently compensating for tine and not to regulate stress, anxiety, or
the reduced nicotine levels of the new ciga- boredom. If so, why do both light and heavy
rettes (Ashton & Watson, 1970). Second, smokers smoke more at parties, during doc-
doses of nicotine led to drops in cigarette con- torate exams, colloquia, and stressful seminar
sumption; for example, 5 mg of nicotine tar- presentations (Schachter, Silverstein, Kozlow-
trate per day orally led to an average reduc- ski, Herman, & Liebling, 1977)? Schachter
tion of two cigarettes per day. Although these suggests they do because stress leads to a
390 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

drop in urinary pH, which increases smoking. are other findings that pose serious threats
To show that stress-induced changes in smok- to the nicotine regulation model.
ing were dependent on urinary pH change, A consideration of the nicotine regulation
Schachter, Silverstein, and Perlick (1977) ex- model's shortcomings leads us to the need
posed smokers to either a high- (painful elec- for multiple regulation model. The nicotine
tric shock) or a low-stress stimulus (barely regulation model suggests that "the brain is in
perceptible shock) and gave half the sub- the bladder"; a phrase Schachter has used
jects in each group three bicarbonate capsules for his colloquia. This statement has the ring
and the other half three placebo capsules. of truth, given that socially induced stress
High stress was expected to lead to more affects urinary pH levels that, in turn, affect
smoking than low stress when subjects were the rate at which nicotine is excreted or taken
given placebo pills; high stress was not ex- up in the blood stream. Unfortunately, neither
pected to increase smoking when subjects the parsimonj' of the model nor the brilliance
were given bicarbonate pills, which block pH of the experiments compensates for the failure
drop and nicotine excretion. The results were to address several important problems. Per-
as expected and are consistent with the nico- haps the most important of the problems ig-
tine regulation model rather than with a nored by the model is how the nicotine set
stress regulation model. point develops and how deviations from the
The nicotine regulation model paints a set point come to generate a craving for cig-
pessimistic picture of intervention efforts. In- arettes. Schachter's model and studies appear
deed, Schachter (1977) suggested that smokers to assume a direct and automatic step from
be left alone lest they be done harm by pro- changes in plasma nicotine level to craving and
cedures such as lowering cigarette nicotine smoking and say nothing about the mecha-
levels, which will increase the number of nisms and experience that give rise to either.
cigarettes smoked and expose the smokers to We believe that this omission makes it
more dangerous combustion products while impossible for the model to account for find-
they are working to obtain their requisite level ings such as the following: (a) High relapse
of plasma nicotine. This warning seems reason- rates are observed in the 3 months to 1 year
able if the plasma regulation model was suffi- following smoking cessation. Plasma nicotine
cient to explain all smoking behavior. But we levels and some of the withdrawal symptoms
must remember that regulation is not as tight are at zero levels a few days after smoking
as Schachter would hope. Indeed, it is clear cessation (Jacobs, Knapp, Rosenthal, & Has-
that some smokers follow an addicted pattern kell, 1970); other factors are needed to ac-
count for the motivation to resume smoking.
(e.g., difficulty quitting, withdrawal symp-
In addition, many smokers report a rapid
toms, etc.), even though they have zero return to heavy smoking (Jacobs et al., 1970;
plasma nicotine levels (Russell, Note S ) . Evi- Schwartz & Dubitzky, 1968). (b) Smokers
dence also suggests that the availability of will smoke lettuce cigarettes that contain no
low-tar, low-nicotine cigarettes has led to nicotine at all. (c) Smokers do not appear to
substantial reductions in tar intake: Schach- regulate their nicotine intake enough to com-
ter's fears do not seem to have been realized pensate for experimentally induced changes
in the population at large (Garfinkel, 1979). in the nicotine content of cigarettes (Jarvik,
The plasma regulation model also suggests 1970, 1973). Schachter (1977), for example,
that work to discover a nicotine antagonist is found that a 77% reduction in nicotine level
produced only a 11%-25% increase in cig-
the most important avenue for achieving smok-
arette consumption, (d) The oral ingestion of
ing reduction. This direction would be no dif- nicotine fails to produce substantial drops in
ferent from that unsuccessfully pursued to smoking, (e) Smoking is responsive to swiftly
overcome heroin addiction (Brecher, 1972). changing emotional states (Nesbitt, 1973) and
Still, if we compare this diagnosis and pre- minor changes in the salience of environ-
scription with other interventions, we see it mental cues (Herman, 1974). (f) Individuals
is at least part of the picture. However, there have substantially more difficulty quitting
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 391

smoking when under stress. White collar and man (1974) found light smokers to be more
executive workers still seeking advancement responsive to external cues than were heavy
or workers facing job loss are much less likely smokers. This is an unsatisfactory resolution
to quit than those'in less stressful job circum- to our question; The division between light
stances. By contrast, blue collar workers in and heavy smokers reflects an arbitrary cutoff
stable job situations often spontaneously re- point on a distribution of frequency of smoking,
duce or stop smoking during middle age, when and there is no clear evidence that the differ-
life stresses appear to be at a minimum (Cap- ences in level are associated with different
Ian, Cobb, & French, 1975). (g) Protected underlying mechanisms. Both Schachter and
environments such as sensory deprivation Herman found light smokers responsive to
chambers seem to make nonsmoking (not nicotine levels. In addition, many so-called
reduced smoking) relatively effortless (Sued- light smokers have enormous difficulty in
feld&Ikard, 1974). (h) Factor analyses show quitting. Besides, all smokers begin as light
that people smoke for different reasons, for smokers, implying some degree of continuity
example, to regulate stress, for stimulation, and overlap in function rather than discrete
pleasure, and so forth (Ikard et al., 1969; categories of underlying process.
Tomkins, 1966). (i) It appears that smoking We think that the answer to these problems
has a long developmental history; in one study lies not in refining or elaborating Schachter's
half of the adult smokers reported taking 6 model of pharmacological regulation but
months to 2 years to develop the habit, the rather in adopting a type of model which
other half still longer (A. Cartwright et al., assumes that the smoker is regulating emo-
1959). The rapidity of development may tional states and that nicotine levels are
depend on the reasons for initiation. It has being regulated because certain emotional
been speculated that emotionally motivated states have become conditioned to them in a
smokers who need to control negative emo- variety of settings. We refer to this model,
tion, distress due to failure, and so forth, which is an elaboration of similar models by
may become dependent more rapidly (Jacobs Tomkins (1968) and Solomon and Corbit
et al., 1970). (j) Finally, smokers who are (1973), as the multiple regulation model.
nicotine deprived do worse in tracking tasks The core of the model is that emotional
such as the driving simulator (Heimstra, Ban- regulation is the key to smoking and that
croft, & DeKock, 1967) and show decreased departures from emotional or hedonic home-
tolerance to shock when smoking low-nicotine ostasis stimulate smoking behavior. Heavy,
cigarettes, a finding that holds only for ex- dependent smokers act to regulate nicotine
perienced smokers (Nesbitt, 1973). Nicotine level because drops in plasma nicotine level
also reduces aggressive biting reactions in stimulate the dysphoric hedonic state of crav-
monkeys (Hutchinson & Emley, 1973). Al- ing, but other external stimuli can also gen-
though these last findings are not really incon- erate this affective reaction. The distress in-
sistent with Schachter's model, nowhere does duced by nicotine level drops can also combine
he give an explanation of the mechanisms that with the distress induced by various tasks
link nicotine deprivation to them. Yet they all (e.g., shock tolerance, driving), and by smok-
suggest that some kind of emotional state is ing to return nicotine to base levels, the
active in connecting changes in plasma nico- smoker can tolerate the same level of shock
tine level to behavior. (Nesbitt, 1973) and perform at the same
level of tracking (Heimstra, 1973; Heimstra
Schachter (1977) and his colleagues appear
et al., 1967) as nonsmokers. Thus externally
to deal with these problems by arguing that
and internally generated cues gain control
individual differences account for many of over fluctuation in emotional states.
these phenomena, They suggest that nicotine As pointed out earlier, an adequate model
regulation applies to heavy smokers, whereas must explain how craving becomes linked to
the use of smoking for the regulation of emo- drops in nicotine levels. We think that there
tional states induced by multiple sources are two possible ways in which such a linkage
applies to light smokers. For example, Her- develops and is maintained. One possible
392 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

source of craving is from the nicotine itself. outlined earlier. We need to address the
Following the assumptions of the opponent- question of why changes in life stress (in-
process theory of emotional motivation pre- creases or decreases) can substantially affect
sented by Solomon and Corbit (1973, 1974), craving, why the presence of craving should
it can be posited that nicotine gives rise to an exaggerate inaccuracies in driving (Heimstra
initial, positive, affective reaction, which in et al., 1967) and changes in response to
turn automatically gives rise to a weaker, shock (Nesbitt, 1973) and why craving can
slave, opponent (or homeostatic), negative, be so rapidly stimulated by external cues.
hedonic response. The opponent, slave state A few simple assumptions expand the range
becomes stronger with the repeated activa- of the model. First, it is assumed that several
tion of its initiating emotion; the more one emotional processes may operate simultane-
uses smoking to elicit positive affect, the more ously. These may be from multiple exogenous
one strengthens the opponent, negative state sources (thus one can feel threat from work
(craving?). Second, the negative, opponent or social situations and positive affect from
state can be eliminated by reinstating the smoking) or multiple opponent processes. One
initial positive emotion; that is, as Tomkins can also feel threat from work stress followed
(1968) argued, smoking will do away with its by a positive slave affect and a positive affect
opponent dysphoria. The catch, of course, is from smoking followed by a negative slave
that repeated smoking strengthens the nega- affect.
tive state it is used to control or builds its Second, we assume that emotional or he-
own craving. donic states from different sources combine
External stimulation provides an alterna- algebraically at any given point in time. For
tive source for craving. The smoker probably example, the negative, exogenously induced
begins smoking as an experiment. Smoking is affect of social stressors can lead to a posi-
initially seen as mature and exciting. Having tive, slave response that can be strengthened
learned the techniques of smoking (e.g., how by the positive affective response induced by
to inhale, habituation to physiological effects), smoking if the two overlap in time. The nega-
the new smoker will likely begin to use smok- tive slave response that follows the positive
ing in various stressful circumstances (i.e., in affect induced by smoking will then add to
the presence of social or work threats), and any negative affect induced by the continuing
these situations may themselves provide the presence of stressors. Thus the negative af-
negative emotion that becomes conditioned to fective craving for a cigarette will be intensi-
declines in plasma nicotine level. Thus if a fied by the summation of the slave response
youngster is socially anxious, high in need for to smoking and the negative affect induced
affiliation, and low in autonomy or self-confi- by social stress.
dence (Hill, 1971), smoking can instill a The third important assumption made in
sense of security, temporarily screening social the model is that changes in plasma nicotine
anxiety. The anxiety will reappear when the level generate a variety of bodily sensations
cigarette is withdrawn, and, at the very point (such as heart rate deceleration) that can be
that plasma nicotine levels begin to decline, it conditioned to these emotional states (Knapp
will be conditioned to or combined with the et al., 1963; Lucchesi, Schuster, & Emley,
bodily sensations generated by drops in nico- 1967; Shiftman & Jarvik, 1976; Shiffman,
tine level. As is discussed shortly, this model Note 4). We emphasize the term sensations
does not treat as craving the sensations gen- to distinguish the experiences generated by
erated by drops in nicotine level. These sen- increases or decreases in nicotine from well-
sations become craving only after they are formed emotional reactions. The various direct
conditioned to environmentally stimulated effects of nicotine mentioned earlier and simi-
negative affects. lar effects produced by nicotine decline do
Either of the above proposals provides a not comprise a fully structured craving reac-
source of negative emotion for the develop- tion, and it is hypothesized that conditioning
ment of the craving reaction, but they are of externally induced or opponent negative
not sufficient to account for the phenomena emotional reactions to these sensations is the
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 393

basis of such a response. This is clearly an This proposal of an emotional memory, our
area in need of study. fifth and final assumption, does more than
A fourth assumption is that smoking does restate the conditioning assumptions made by
far more than generate a positive hedonic Solomon and Corbit (1974). What it does is
state that can cancel the negative affect of provide a mechanism for integrating and sus-
stress or help to sum with the positive slave taining the combination of external stimulus
reactions elicited by such hedonically negative cues (social events, work, nothing to do,
situations. It also facilitates reactions that taste, etc.), internal stimulus cues (sensa-
can directly enhance adaptation to the specific tions from drops in plasma nicotine levels),
situation that is negatively hedonic. For ex- and a variety of reactions including sub-
ample, because smoking increases neural acti- jective emotional experience and expressive
vation, it can increase alertness, useful for motor and autonomic reactions associated with
coping with intellectually stressful situations. the hedonic experience and with smoking.
As a social gesture, it can reduce feelings of The elicitation of any of the components can
insecurity and the perceived threat in social provoke the remaining components of this
settings. By accelerating heart rate, it may memory schema. Hence, it is the schema that
help control anxiety when the arousal gen- makes possible the reexperiencing of craving
erated by external threats is misattributed to when one sees someone else smoke, and it is
the cigarette. Because smoking induces muscu- the schema through which nicotine loss stimu-
lar inhibition, it may enhance relaxation and lates craving because the sensory features of
help to control fear, anger, and level of dis- nicotine loss elicit the other affective com-
tress in a variety of threat situations (Gil- ponents of the schema.
bert, 1979; Malmo, 1975). In this model, differences between smokers,
If smoking can regulate the internal mileau that is, whether they label themselves as so-
by dampening or augmenting arousal induced cial, stimulation, anxiety reducing, addictive,
by external events and at the same time pro- or habitual, will depend on the elements
vide direct help in coping with these events, linked into the schema and on their relative
it is likely that the smoking experience will vividness. One strong possibility is that ex-
be strongly associated with memories of ternal cues such as taste or social settings are
these episodes, and so form emotional mem- more available or noticeable, and people are
ory schemata (Leventhal, 1979, 1980; Leven- likely to use them to label their "reason for
thal & Everhart, 1979). Schematic emotional smoking." The labeling is not totally spu-
memories should combine imagelike repre- rious: Light, social smokers are nicotine sensi-
sentations of situations with impressions of tive (see Herman, 1974), but their nicotine
motoric and physiological reactions in these sensations are linked to craving along with
situations, for example, the subjective emo- imagery of social situations. Anxious smokers
tions experienced in the situation and the are also nicotine sensitive, but their nicotine
autonomic, expressive motor and instrumental sensations are linked to craving along with
behaviors made in that setting. Schemata of imagery of threatening situations. Both re-
this sort will sustain the conditioning of a port the more salient component of the con-
range of memories experienced as craving to crete memory, both show some degree of se-
nicotine levels in a variety of situations. lective sensitivity, with craving elicited by
Leventhal and Everhart give examples of different external cues, and both show nico-
schematic memories such as phantom pain: a tine regulation.
long-lasting memory combining the sensations Heavy smokers are likely to "protect" their
of an amputated body part with the au- plasma levels of nicotine because declines in
tonomic, expressive, and subjective pain ex- nicotine level have been conditioned to emo-
perience associated with it. These phantom tional schemata and now elicit negative emo-
experiences show many of the characteristics tional states, This internal conditioning pro-
of smoking, such as a strong tendency to cess could take some time because the cues
reappear during times of life stress (see are less noticeable or because it takes a high
Melzack, 1973). rate of smoking across many situations before
394 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

internal cues dominate the onset of craving. have been conditioned to external cues (O'-
This could account for the time lag in becom- Brien, Testa, O'Brien, Brady, & Wells, 1977).
ing a heavy smoker and would also predict Finally, the model is similar to that devel-
that individuals who smoke to regulate strong oped by Marlatt (1976) to account for the
negative affects will experience internal crav- effects of alcohol and expectations on response
ings more quickly and more strongly and have to drinking intoxicating beverages.
more difficulty stopping, a common finding in In summary, this interpretation suggests
the literature (e.g., Coan, 1967). One impli- that the mechanism underlying the change
cation of this model is that an abrupt altera- from light to heavy smoking produces a shift
tion of the emotional schema associated with in the target of regulation; initially, smoking
a drug would help break the pattern of regulates emotional responses elicited by en-
addiction. vironmentally induced stress, then regulates
Robins, Davis, and Goodwin (1974) re- craving conditioned to external cues, and
ported some fascinating results that are con- finally regulates craving and distress caused
sistent with this prediction. In studying the by changes in the level of nicotine itself. In
archetype of pharmacological addiction all instances, however, emotional regulation
heroin dependencethey found that when is central to the dependence process; that is,
addicted U.S. soldiers returned from Vietnam, the individual is not smoking just to sustain
they were surprisingly successful in giving up plasma levels of nicotine but to suppress the
their habit. The strongest predictor of con- emotional distress induced by drops in plasma
tinued addiction was preservice use. Those nicotine levels. The analysis suggests the con-
who had integrated their drug-taking experi- nection between drops in plasma nicotine level
ences into everyday experiences continued to and craving is not automatic. It depends on a
be dependent. Those who had developed a special history of smoking experience, and it
pattern of use in a different environment may be altered by techniques that can dis-
(Vietnam) were able to quit when the environ- sociate or change the link between affective
ment was changed. (See also Peele, 1977, for states and discrepancies in plasma nicotine
a detailed discussion of the inadequacies of levels. It is likely that the dissociation occurs
the pharmacological model of addiction.) The naturally in cases in which there are major
use of nicotine across many life situations reductions in life stress and there is no longer
prior to efforts at smoking withdrawal may a negative emotional background to summate
develop a broad emotional smoking schema with the more temporary induced shifts of
and may account for the difficulty in sustain- decline in plasma nicotine levels.
ing quitting. Indeed, it has been claimed that Implications for intervention. The regula-
it is more difficult to quit nicotine than heroin tion models illuminate the reasons for the
(Brecher, 1972, p. 2 1 7 ) . limited success of the last 20 years of inter-
If this model sounds more complicated and vention research. As we have already made
therefore less plausible than a more physio- clear, intervention models to date are based
logically oriented model of addiction, we on the development of a motive to quit
should point out that even the physiological (usually an antismoking attitude related to
responses to certain drugs must be explained health concerns) and a management program
with a learning model. For example, Siegel's developed either through specific plans, mea-
(1975, 1977) data convincingly demonstrate sures to enhance self-esteem or what Bandura
that drug responses such as morphine toler- (1977) calls "self-effectance," or one or an-
ance are conditioned reactions. Repeated other form of behavioral therapy (e.g., con-
injections of morphine are less successful in tracting, self-monitoring, or reinforcement of
pain reduction if the injections are given in nonsmoking). None of these techniques
the same setting in which pain thresholds are clearly focus on changing motivational factors
tested. If the injections are given in a very such as the conditioned emotional schema,
different setting from that in which thresh- with its sensory and opponent affective com-
olds are tested, there is no drug tolerance. In ponents, that may underlie the maintenance of
addition, methadone withdrawal symptoms smoking. The evolution of these structures
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 395

from their origins in individual motivation or pulsion. Such a process may account for the
social pressures to try smoking a cigarette relatively high rate of successful abstention
has led some theorists to conclude these inde- when imagery is used with heavy smokers
pendent motivational systems must be pre- (Berecz, 1972a). When we use a stimulus such
vented, as they cannot be "cured" (Bejerot, as thinking noxious thoughts or an electric
1972). shock to avoid the pitfalls of reward associ-
The nature of the mechanism in the multi- ated with rapid smoking, we have to face the
ple regulatory model does indeed suggest that risk that the distress generated by the punish-
it will be extremely difficult to eliminate ment, whatever its nature, may add to the
craving (see Solomon & Corbit, 1973). The distress generated by being below plasma
key to the difficulty lies in breaking up the nicotine set point and so strengthen craving
emotional memory schema. In conditioning (see Solomon & Corbit, 1974).
terms, it is extremely difficult to produce the This brings us to the second set of alterna-
external and internal sensory (cue) com- tives, techniques for generating and attaching
ponents of the schema without recreating the positive emotion to the cues produced by
emotional reactions (subjective, expressive, drops in plasma nicotine levels. One possible
and autonomic) and thereby strengthening way of doing this is to expose subjects to sham
rather than disassembling the structure. This smoking and have them handle and light cig-
can be seen more clearly from two perspec- arettes but not smoke them. This procedure
tives: (a) the use of rapid smoking to elimi- would be analogous to that used by Siegel
nate craving and (b) the difficulty of produc- (1975, 1977) to extinguish morphine toler-
ing a positive affect to substitute for craving ance. How can strong positive affects be gen-
at the same time that declines in plasma nico- erated when nicotine plasma levels are de-
tine level generate their internal cues. clining? This may happen naturally when
In rapid smoking the smoker is told to puff there are major changes in life situations and
in time to a metronome and continue till he it may also occur in group support procedures
or she feels ill, in the expectation that the used to deal with alcoholism and drug addic-
feeling of illness will attach itself to the tion (Brecher, 1972; Sarbin & Nucci, 1973).
action. However, if therapy begins when the Devising a therapy to condition positive affect
plasma level of nicotine is low (e.g., in the to cues from drops in plasma nicotine level
morning or when the person is anxious), the would be abetted first by procedures designed
initial puffs will be reinforced. Thus the to more clearly identify the sensory com-
therapy provides a reward for smoking at the ponents associated with these drops and sec-
point at which the desire to smoke is highest ond by the generation of these cues by pre-
and does not generate disgust or aversion for cisely controlled alternative methods. Such
smoking until the desire to smoke is gone, methods could aid in pairing the cues with
that is, when the regulatory mechanism no other more positive expressive acts and emo-
longer signals a deficit. It is not at all remark- tional feelings. Agents that block nicotine
able that an individual will feel disgust if he receptor sites, such as pentolinium (Stolerman,
or she overeats or oversmokes, and it is not Goldfarb, Fink, & Jarvik, 1973), might serve
remarkable that aversive conditioning will this function. Positive imagery could be gen-
strengthen avoidance of smoking to cues of erated during these experiences, perhaps using
nicotine excess. imagery emphasizing feelings of health, vigor,
The analysis of aversive conditioning might and self-control. The therapy would also strive
suggest that we need to develop procedures to separate situational imagery (e.g., images
for generating negative affective reactions, of self smoking in social setting, during exam-
such as disgust and aversion, when nicotine inations, or when work loads are high) from
plasma levels are below set point. One might the affective craving and link-these images to
do this by using imagery to induce aversive positive affect clue to self-control.
feelings while one is experiencing the need to The result would be a new schematic struc-
smoke; that is, an image of self or of cig- ture linking situational imagery and body
arettes that converts an appetite into a re- cues to the positive emotion of self-control
396 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

and to expressive actions and gestures other number of cigarettes smoked, and to avoid
than those associated with craving and smok- smoking before retiring (which increases car-
ing. Removing the schematic structures for diovascular risk). But we may be reaching the
craving might well produce the cure needed limit of effectiveness of current intervention
to maintain long-term abstinence from smok- methods, and it is clear that new approaches
ing and reduce the risk that a single cigarette are needed to discourage young people from
will reestablish the complete pattern of crav- experimenting with cigarettes and developing
ing-based compulsive behavior. Major changes a dependence on them. The goal of preven-
in societal views of smoking and smokers tion will not be met simply with new tech-
could help sustain these new structures, just niques of delivering information; we need
as they now establish an environment sug- new kinds of information, information that
gesting connections between smoking and the builds a more valid, concrete understanding of
regulation of emotional experience in specific the body, its strengths and weaknesses, and
situations. These sociocultural sets orient the inadequacies and inaccuracies that exist in
smokers and help them find the associations our systems for self-monitoring and deciding
needed to establish addictive schemata. when we are ill or at risk. We need a new
approach to the perception of benefits and
Looking Ahead costs of smoking to enhance self-esteem and
social adjustment. Indeed, it may well be
We do not wish to give a negative view of necessary to alter our conception of preven-
the research on intervention, for it has helped tion, to expand the current view of preventing
us to define the essential elements in the inter- young people from ever smoking their first
vention process, including (a) the develop- cigarette (a one-chance-last-chance view of
ment of a motive to quit and (b) the learning prevention) to a more developmental view.
of techniques for behavioral management when The developmental view would use knowledge
quitting. The national survey data also sug- of the developmental history of smoking to
gests a cumulative impact of antismoking seek a series of specific junctures at which to
campaigns on community norms about smok- direct different antismoking efforts.
ing that is reflected in the surprisingly large We also need to study smoking as a socio-
percentage of smokers who regard smoking as logical phenomena. The largest changes in
dangerous and have succeeded in quitting smoking behavior seem to be a result of his-
smoking for short periods of time. The data torical factors. Changing norms and values,
also suggest that antismoking information and evolving definitions of the role of the indi-
activity has had a variety of unexpected ef- vidual in society, and shifting perceptions of
fects ranging from the switch to filter cig- the importance of self-regulation as opposed
arettes to the passage of legislation protecting to technological (i.e., drug) intervention are
the rights of nonsmokers. In addition, it is all important in determining the extent and
clear that current methods for dealing with meaning of smoking in our society. The socio-
smoking are probably no more effective than logical or cultural view of smoking must also
those devised by smokers trying to manage be connected to the individual psychology of
the problem on their own. the smoker. The individual's preparation, ex-
Our reading of the data suggests that anti- perimentation, becoming, regular use or main-
smoking efforts directed at school children or tenance, decision to quit or reduce, efforts to
the community at large will lead to continued do so, and success in maintaining quitting and
declines in smoking but that the programs
reduction all occur in a social context. The
could achieve a greater impact on smoking
reduction if they would make better use of norms and values of the context direct the
existing theoretical knowledge. We also be- smoker's attention and provide interpreta-
lieve these campaigns could benefit by adopt- tions or meaning for specific experiences, ac-
ing more flexible goals, such as urging smok- tions, and success and failure in reduction
ers to smoke less, to smoke low-tar and low- efforts. Although this goal is ambitious and
nicotine cigarettes if they do not increase the possibly unrealistic, it is impossible to under-
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 397

stand the meaning of chemical self-regulation Ashton, H., & Watson, D. W. Puffing frequency and
without understanding its context. nicotine intake in cigarette smokers. British Medi-
cal Journal, 1970, 3, 679-681.
Finally, new approaches are needed to deal Baer, D. J., & Katkin, J. M. Limitation of smoking
with the emotional structures involved in the by sons and daughters who smoke and smoking
maintenance of smoking. The development behavior of parents. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
and addition of these innovations to our al- 1971, 118, 293-296.
Baker, T., & Cannon, D. S. Taste aversion therapy
ready considerable armamentarium could re-
with alcoholics: Techniques and evidence of a
sult in a fairly dramatic increase in the prac- conditioned response. Behaviour Research and
tical success of antismoking efforts and a Therapy, 1979,17, 229-242.
substantial increase in the sophistication of Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying the-
our theoretical models for educational inter- ory of behavioral change. Psychological Review,
1977, 84, 191-215.
vention in other risk-inducing life-style be- Bandura, A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ritter, B. Relative
haviors. efficacy of desensitization and modeling approaches
for inducing behavioral, affective, and attitudinal
changes. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Reference Notes chology, 1969, 13, 173-199.
Barec, L., MacArthur, C., & Sherwood, M. A study
1. Horn, D. Factors affecting the cessation of cigar- of health education aspects of smoking in preg-
ette smoking: A prospective study. Address pre- nancy. International Journal of Health Education,
sented at the meeting of the Environmental Pro- 1976, 19, 1-17, Supplement 1.
tection Agency, Washington, B.C., April 1968. Barrett, T. J., & Sachs, L. B. Test of the classical
2. Perry, C. L., McAlister, A. L., & Farquhar, J. W. conditioning explanation of covert sensitization.
Peer leadership to help adolescents resist pressures Psychological Reports, 1974, 34, 1312-1314.
to smoke: A one-year follow-up. Paper presented Bartlett, W. A, & Whitehead, R. W. The effective-
at the meeting of the American Psychological Asso- ness of meprobamate and lobeline as smoking de-
ciation, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, September 1978. terrents. Journal oj Laboratory and Clinical Medi-
cine, 19S7, 50, 278-281.
3. Horn, D. Personal communication, June 1977. Bauer, R. A., & Bauer, A. H. America, mass society
4. Shiffman, S. M. The tobacco withdrawal syndrome, and mass media. Journal of Social Issues, 1960, 16,
In N. A. Krasnegor (Ed.), Cigarette smoking as a 3-66.
dependence process [NIDA Research Monograph Bejerot, N. Theory of addiction as an artificially
No. 23, U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Wel- induced drive. American Journal oj Psychiatry,
fare Publication No. (ADM) 79-800], Rockville, 1972, 128(1), 842-846.
Md.: National Institute on Drug Abuse, 1979. Berecz, J. M. Reduction of smoking through self-
5. Russell, M. A. H. Tobacco dependence: Is nico- administered aversion conditioning of imagined
tine rewarding or aversive? In N, A. Krasnegor behavior. Proceedings of the 79th Annual Conven-
(Ed.), Cigarette smoking as a dependence process tion oj the American Psychological Association,
[NIDA Research Monograph No. 23, U.S. Dcpt. 1971, 6, 429-430. (Summary)
of Health, Education and Welfare Publication No. Berecz, J. Modification of smoking behavior through
(ADM) 79-800]. Rockville, Md.: National In- self-administered punishment of imagined behav-
stitute on Drug Abuse, 1979. ior: A new approach to aversion therapy. Journal
6. Newman, I. M., Martin, G. L., & Petersen, C. P. oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1972, 38,
Attititdinal and normative factors associated with 244-250. (a)
adolescent cigarette smoking. Paper presented at Berecz, J. M. Reduction of cigarette smoking through
the meeting of the School Health Section, Ameri- self-administered aversion conditioning: A new
can Public Health Association, Los Angeles, Oc- treatment model with implications for public health.
tober 1978. Social Science and Medicine, 1972, 6, 57-66. (b)
7. Horn, D. An analysis of the educational problems Bergen, B. J., & Olesen, E. Some evidence for a peer
of controlling cigarette smoking. Paper presented group hypothesis about adolescent smoking. Health
at the Ninth International Cancer Congress, Tokyo, Education Journal, 1963, 21, 113-119.
October 1966. Bernstein, D. A. Modification of smoking behavior:
An evaluative review. Psychological Bulletin, 1969,
71, 418-440.
References Bernstein, D. A. Modification of smoking behavior:
An evaluative review. In W. A. Hunt (Ed.), Learn-
Armitage, A. K. Some recent observations relating to ing mechanisms in smoking. Chicago: Aldine, 1970.
the absorption of nicotine from tobacco smoke. In Best, J. A. Tailoring smoking withdrawal procedures
W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.), Smoking behavior: Mo- to personality and motivational differences. Jour-
tives and incentives, Washington, D.C.: V. H. Win- nal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 197S,
ston, 1973. 43, 1-8.
398 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

Best, J. A., Bass, F., & Owen, L. E. Mode of service Cartwright, A., Martin, F. M., & Thompson, J. G.
delivery in a smoking cessation program for public Distribution and development of smoking habits.
health. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 1977, Lancet, 1959, 2, 725-727.
68, 469-473. r Cartwright, D. Some principles of mass persuasion.
Best, J. A., & Hakstian, A. R. A situation specific Human Relations, 1949, 2, 253-267.
model for smoking behavior. Addictive Behaviors, Coan, R. W. Research strategy in the investigation
1978, 3, 79-92. of personality correlates. In S. V. Zagona (Ed.),
Best, J. A., Owen, L. E., & Trentadue, L. Compari- Studies and issues in smoking behavior. Tucson:
son of satiation and rapid smoking in self-managed University of Arizona Press, 1967.
smoking cessation. Addictive Behaviors, 1978, 3, Conway, J. B. Behavioral self-control of smoking
71-78. through aversive conditioning and self-manage-
Best, J. A., & Steffy, R. Smoking modification pro- ment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
cedures tailored to subject characteristics, Behav- ogy, 1977, 45, 348-357.
ior Therapy, 1971, 2, 177-191. Cornfield, J., et al. Smoking and lung cancer: Re-
Bewley, B. R., & Bland, J. M. Academic performance cent evidence and a discussion of some questions.
and social factors related to cigarette smoking by Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 1959, 22,
school children. British Journal of Preventive and 173-203.
Social Medicine, 1977, 31, 18-24. Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Leventhal, H. Effects of varying
Bland, J. M., Bewley, B. R., & Day, I. Primary the recommendations in a fear-arousing communi-
schoolboys: Image of self and smoker. British cation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Journal of Preventive and Social Medicine, 1975, ogy, 1966, 4, 525-531.
29, 262-266. Damon, A. Smoking attitudes and practices in seven
Borgatta, E. F., & Evans, R. R. (Eds.). Smoking, preliterate societies. In W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.),
health, and behavior. Chicago: Aldine, 1968. Smoking behavior: Motives and incentives. Wash-
Borland, B. L., & Rudolph, J. P. Relative effects of ington, D.C.: V. H. Winston, 1973.
low socioeconomic status, parental smoking and Danahcr, B. G. Research on rapid smoking: Interim
poor scholastic performance on smoking among summary and recommendations. Addictive Behav-
high school students. Social Science and Medicine, iors, 1977, 2, 151-166.
1975, 9, 27-30. Dawley, H. H., Jr. Aversive cigarette smoking with
Bozzetti, L. P. Group psychotherapy with addicted an habitual excessive smoker. Perceptual and Motor
smokers. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 1972, Skills, 1975, 41, 1008-1010.
20, 172-175. Dclahunt, J., & Curran, J. P. Effectiveness of nega-
Brccher, E. M. Licit and illicit drugs. Boston: Little, tive practice and self-control techniques in the
Brown, 1972. reduction of smoking behavior. Journal of Con-
Brown, B. B. Additional characteristic EEC differ- sulting and Clinical Psychology, 1976, 44, 1002-
ences between smokers and nonsmokcrs. In W. L. 1007.
Dunn, Jr. (Ed.), Smoking behavior: Motives and Doll, R., & Hill, A. B. Mortality in relation to smok-
incentives. Washington, D.C.: V. H. Winston, 1973. ing: Ten years observations of British doctors.
Burns, B. H. Chronic chest disease, personality and British Medical Journal, 1964, 1, 1399-1410.
success in stopping cigarette smoking. British Jour- Doll, R., & Peto, R. Mortality in relation to smok-
nal of Preventive and Social Medicine, 1969, 23, ing: 20 years observation in male British doctors.
23-37. British Medical Journal, 1976, 2, 1525-1536.
Burnum, J. F. Outlook for treating patients with Domino, E. F, Xeuropsychopharmacology of nicotine
self-destructive habits. Annals of Internal Medi- and tobacco smoking. In W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.),
cine, 1974, 81, 387-393. Smoking behavior: Motives and incentives, Wash-
Bynner, J. M. Behavioral research into children's ington, D.C.: V. H. Winston, 1973.
smoking: Some implications for anti-smoking strat- Dubitzky, M., & Schwartz, J. L. Ego-resiliency, ego-
egy. Royal Society of Health Journal, 1970, 90, control, and smoking cessation. Journal of Psy-
159-163. chology, 1968, 70, 27-33.
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. Experimental and Dubitzky, M., & Schwartz, J. L. Cognitive dissonance
quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: and changes in cigarette smoking in an organized
Rand McNally, 1963. control program. Journal of Social Psychology,
Caplan, R. D., Cobb, S., & French, J. R. P., Jr. Re- 1969, 79, 219-225,
lationships of cessation of smoking with job stress, Dunn, W. L., Jr. (Ed.). Smoking behavior: Motives
personality, and social support. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 197S, 60, 211-219. and incentives. Washington, D.C.: V, H. Winston,
Carlin, A. S., & Armstrong, H. E., Jr. Aversive con- 1973.
ditioning: Learning or dissonance reduction? Jour- Ebert, R. V. Cessation of cigarette smoking and pul-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1968, monary disease. Journal of the American Medical
32, 674-678. Association, 1978, 240, 2159-2161.
Carlson, R. J. The end of medicine. New York: Eckholm, E. The unnatural history of tobacco. Natu-
Wiley, 1971 ral History, 1977, 86, 22-32.
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 399

Edwards, G. Double-blind trial of lobeline in an Journal of the Irish Medical Association, 1973, 66,
anti-smoking clinic. Medical Officer, 1964, 112, 697-698.
158-160. Fuller, R. The smoking syndrome. New Behavior,
Eisinger, R. A. Psychosocial predictors of smoking May 15, 1975, pp. 150-153. *
recidivism. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Fuller, R. G. C., & Forrest, D. W. Behavioral as-
1971, 12, 355-362. pects of cigarette smoking in relation to arousal
Ejrup, B. Treatment of tobacco addiction: Experi- level. Psychological Reports, 1973, 33, 115-121.
ences in tobacco withdrawal clinics. In, Can we Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G., & Rusiniak, K. W. Be-
help them stop? Chicago: American Cancer So- havioral regulation of the milieu interne in man
ciety, Illinois Division, 1964. and rat. Science, 1974, 18S, 824-831.
Elliott, C. H., & Denney, D. R. A multiple-compo- Garflnkel, L. Changes in the cigarette consumption
nent treatment approach to smoking reduction. of smokers in relation to changes in tar/nicotine
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, content of cigarettes smoked. American Journal
1978, 46, 1330-1339. of Public Health, 1979, 69, 1274-1276.
Engeln, R. G. A comparison of desensitization and Gcrson, P., & Lanyon, R. I. Modification of smok-
aversive conditioning as treatment methods to re- ing behavior with an aversion-desensitization pro-
duce cigarette smoking (Doctoral dissertation, cedure. Journal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
Washington State University, 1969). Dissertation ogy, 1972, 38, 399-402.
Abstracts International, 1969, 30, 1357B. Gilbert, D. G. Paradoxical tranquilizing and emo-
Essman, W. B. Nicotine-related neurochemical tion-reducing effects of nicotine. Psychological
changes: Some implications for motivational mecha- Bulletin, 1979, 86, 643-661.
nisms and differences. In W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.), Glasgow, R. E. Effects of a self-control manual, rapid
Smoking behavior: Motives and incentives. Wash- smoking, and amount of therapist contact on
ington, D.C.: V. H. Winston, 1973. smoking reduction. Journal of Consulting and
Evans, R. I. Smoking in children: Developing a Clinical Psychology, 1978, 46, 1439-1447.
social psychological strategy of deterrence. Preven- Gordon, T., Kannel, W. B., McGee, D., & Dawber,
tive Medicine, 1976, 5, 122-127. T. R. Death and coronary attacks in men after
Evans, R. I., Henderson, A. H., Hill, P., & Raines, giving up cigarette smoking. Lancet, 1974, 2, 1345-
B. E. Smoking in children and adolescents: Psy- 1348.
chosocial determinants and prevention strategies. Gori, G. B. Low-risk cigarettes: A prescription. Sci-
In, Smoking and health: A report oj the surgeon ence, 1976, 104, 1243-1246.
general [U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Wel- Gorsuch, R. L., & Butler, M. C. Initial drug abuse:
fare Publication No. (PHS) 79-50066]. Washing- A review of predisposing social psychological fac-
ton, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1979. tors. Psychological Bulletin, 1976, 83, 120-137.
Evans, R. I., Rozelle, R. M., Lasater, T. M., Dem- Graham, D. T. Psychosomatic medicine. In N. S.
broski, T. M., & Allen, B. P. Fear arousal, per- Greenfield & R. A. Sternbach (Eds.), Handbook
suasion, and actual versus implied behavioral oj psychophysiology. New York: Holt, Rinehart
change: New perspectives utilizing a real-life den- & Winston, 1972.
tal hygiene program. Journal of Personality and Grant, R. L., & Weitman, M. Cigarette smoking and
Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 220-227. school children: A longitudinal study. In E. F.
Evans, R. I., et al. Deterring the onset of smoking Borgatta & R. R. Evans (Eds.), Smoking, health,
in children: Knowledge of immediate physiological and behavior. Chicago: Aldine, 1968.
effects and coping with peer pressure, media pres- Green, D. E. Youth education. In, Smoking and
sure, and parent modeling. Journal of Applied So- health: A report of the surgeon general [U.S.
cial Psychology, 1978, S, 126-13$. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare Publica-
Ferguson, L. W., Freedman, M., & Ferguson, E. P. tion No. (PHS) 79-50066]. Washington, D.C.:
Developmental self-concept and (self-reported) U.S. Government Printing Office, 1979.
drug use. Psychological Reports, 1977, 41, 531-541, Gutmann, M., & Marston, A. Problems of S's moti-
vation in a behavioral program for reduction of
Fishbein, M. Attitude and the prediction of behav-
cigarette smoking. Psychological Reports, 1967, 20,
ior. In M. Fishbein (Ed.), Readings in attitude
1107-1114.
theory and measurement. New York: Wiley, 1Q67.
Hael'ner, D. P. Arousing fear in dental health edu-
Friedman, G. D., Dales, L. G., & Ury, H. K. Mor- cation. Journal oj Public Health Dentistry, 1965,
tality in middle-aged smokers and nonsmokers. 25, 140-146.
New England Journal of Medicine, 1979, 300, 213- Hall, R. A., Rappaport, M., Hopkins, H. K., &
217. Griffin, R. Tobacco and evoked potential. Science,
Frith, C. D. The effect of varying the nicotine con- 1973, 180, 212-214.
tent of cigarettes on human smoking behavior. Hammond, E. C., & Horn, D. The relationship be-
Psychopharmacologia, 1971, 19, 188-192. (a) tween human smoking habits and death rates.
Frith, C. D. Smoking behavior and its relation to Journal oj the American Medical Association, 1954,
the smoker's immediate experience. British Journal 155, 1316-1328.
oj Social and Clinical Psychology, 1971, 10, 73-78. Handel, S, Change in smoking habits in a general
(b) practice. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 1973, 49,
Fuller, R. G. C. A note on smoking motivation. 679-681.
400 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

Heimstra, N. W. The effects of smoking on mood lated to management of affect. International Jour-
change. In W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.), Smoking be- nal of the Addictions, 1969, 4, 649-659.
havior: Motives and incentives. Washington, D.C.: Ikard, F. F., & Tomkins, S. The experience of affect
V. H. Winston* 1973. as a determinant of smoking behavior: A series
Heimstra, N. W., Bancroft, N. R., & DeKock, A. R. of validity studies. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Effects of smoking upon sustained performance in ogy, 1973, 81, 172-181,
a simulated driving task. Annals of the New York Jacobs, M. A. The addictive personality: Prediction
Academy of Sciences, 1967, 142, 295-307. of success in a smoking withdrawal program. Psy-
Herman, C. P. External and internal cues as deter- chosomatic Medicine, 1972, 34, 30-38.
minants of the smoking behavior of light and Jacobs, M. A., Knapp, P. H., Rosenthal, S., & Haskell,
heavy smokers. Journal of Personality and Social D. Psychological aspects of cigarette smoking in
Psychology, 1974, 30, 664-672. menClinical evaluation. Psychosomatic Medicine,
Hill, D. Peer group conformity in adolescent smok- 1970, 32, 469-485.
ing and its relationship to affiliation and autonomy Jaffe, J. H. Cigarette smoking as an addiction. Ameri-
needs. Australian Journal of Psychology, 1971, 23, can Lung Association Bulletin, May 1976, 10-12.
189-199. Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. Effectiveness of emotional
Hochbaum, G. M. Psychosocial aspects of smoking role-playing in modifying smoking habits and at-
with special reference to cessation. American Jour- titudes. Journal of Experimental Research on Per-
nal of Public Health, 1965, 55, 692-697. sonality, 1965, 1, 84-90.
Hoenig, J., & Reed, G. F. The objective assessment Jarvik, M. E. The role of nicotine in the smoking
of desensitization. British Journal of Psychiatry, habit. In W. A. Hunt (Ed.), Learning mechanisms
1966, 112, 1279-1283. and smoking. Chicago: Aldine, 1970.
Holland, W. W., & Elliott, A. Cigarette smoking, Jarvik, M. E. Further observations on nicotine as
respiratory symptoms and anti-smoking propa- the reinforcing agent in smoking. In W. L. Dunn,
ganda. Lancet, 1968, 1, 41-43. Jr. (Ed.), Smoking behavior: Motives and incen-
Horn, D. Modifying smoking habits in high school tives. Washington, D.C.: V. H. Winston, 1973.
students. Children, 1960, 7, 63-6S. Jefferys, M., & Westaway W. R. Catch them before
Horn, D. Behavioral aspects of cigarette smoking. they start. Health Education Journal, 1961, 1C,
Journal of Chronic Disease, 1963,16, 383-39S. 3-17.
Horn, D. The health consequences of smoking. In Jenks, R., Schwartz, J. L., & Dubitzky, M. Effect of
E. F. Borgatta & R. R. Evans (Eds.), Smoking, the counselor's approach to changing smoking be-
health, and behavior. Chicago: Aldine, 1968. havior. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969,
Horn, D., Courts, F. A., Taylor, R. M., & Solomon, 16, 215-221.
E. S. Cigarette smoking among high school stu- Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. C. Problem behavior and psy-
dents. American Journal of Public Health, 1959, chosocial development: A longitudinal study of
40, 1497-1511. youth. New York: Academic Press, 1977.
Hovland, C. I. Effects of the mass media of com- Johnston, E., & Donoghue, J. R. Hypnosis and smok-
munication. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of ing: A review of the literature. American Journal
social psychology (Vol. 2). Cambridge, Mass.: Ad- of Clinical Hypnosis, 1971,13, 265-272.
dison-Wesley, 1954. Kandel, D. Adolescent marihuana use: Role of par-
Hovland, C. I, Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. Com- ents and peers. Science-, 1973, 181, 1067-1069.
munication and persuasion. New Haven, Conn.: Kandel, D. Stages in adolescent involvement in drug
Yale University Press, 1953. use. Science, 1975, ISO, 912-914.
Hull, C. L. Quantitative aspects of the evolution Kanfer, F. H. Self-management methods. In F. H.
of concepts: An experimental study. Psychological Kanfer & A. P. Goldstein (Eds.), Helping people
Monographs, 1920, 25(1, Whole No. 123). change. New York: Pergamon Press, 1975.
Hunt, W. A. Learning mechanisms in smoking. Chi- Kapel, S. A. The effects of self-control, booster
cago: Aldine, 1970. sessions and cognitive factors on the maintenance
Hunt, W. A., & Bespalec, D. A. An evaluation of of smoking reduction. Unpublished doctoral dis-
current methods of modifying smoking behavior. sertation, University of Oregon, 1974.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1974, 30, 431-438. Karoly, P., & Doyle, W. W. Effects of outcome ex-
Hunt, W., & Matarazzo, J. D. Three years later: pectancy and timing of self-monitoring on ciga-
Recent developments in the experimental modifica- rette smoking. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1975,
tion of smoking behavior. Journal of Abnormal 31, 351-355.
Psychology, 1973, SI, 107-114. Katz, E., & Lazarsfeld, P. F. Personal influence. New
Hutchinson, R. R., & Emley, G. S. Effects of nico- York: Free Press of Glencoe, 1955.
tine on avoidance, conditioned suppression and Kazdin, A. E., & Wilcoxon, L. A. Systematic de-
aggression response measures in animals and man. sensitization and nonspecific treatment effects: A
In W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.), Smoking behavior: methodological evaluation. Psychological Bulletin,
Motives and incentives. Washington, D.C.: V. H. 1976, 83, 729-758.
Winston, 1973. Kelley, H. H. Salience of membership and resistance
Ikard, F. F., Green, D. E., & Horn, D. A. A scale to change of group-anchored attitudes. Human
to differentiate between types of smoking as re- Relations, 1958, 8, 275-289.
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 401

Kelley, H. H., & Volkart, E. H. The resistance to Leventhal, H. Experimental studies of anti-smoking
change of group-anchored attitudes. American So- communications. In E. F. Borgatta & R. R. Evans
ciological Review, 1952,17, 453-465. (Eds.), Smoking, health and behavior. Chicago:
Kelman, H. C, Compliance, identification, and in- Aldine, 1968.
ternalization: Three processes of attitude change. Leventhal, H. Findings and theory in the study of
Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1958, 2, 51-60. fear communications. In L. Bcrkowitz (Ed.), Ad-
Keutzer, C. S., Lichtenstein, E., & Mees, H. L. Modi- vances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 5).
fication of smoking behavior: A review. Psycho- New York: Academic Press, 1970.
logical Bulletin, 1968, 70, 520-533. Leventhal, H. Changing attitudes and habits to re-
Kiesler, D. J. Some myths of psychotherapy research duce risk factors in chronic disease. American Jour-
and the search for a paradigm. Psychological Bulle- nal oj Cardiology, 1973, 31, 571-580.
tin, 1966, 65, 110-136. Leventhal, H. Attitudes: Their nature, growth, and
Klapper, J. T. The effect of mass communication. change. In C. Nemeth (Ed.), Social psychology:
New York: Free Press of Glencoe, 1961. Classic and contemporary integrations. Chicago:
Knapp, P. H., Bliss, C. M., & Wells, H. Addictive Rand McNally, 1974.
aspects in heavy smoking. American Journal oj Leventhal, H. A perceptual-motor processing model
Psychiatry, 1963, 119, 966-972. of emotions. In P. Pliner, K. Blankstein, & I. M.
Koenig, K. P., & Masters, J. Experimental treatment Spigel (Eds.), Advances in the study of communi-
of habitual smoking. Behaviour Research and Ther- cation and affect: Perception oj emotion in self
apy, 1965, 3, 235-243. and others (Vol. 5). New York: Plenum Press,
Kornitzer, M., DeBacker, G., Dramaix, M., & Thilly, 1979.
C. The Belgian Heart Disease Prevention Project.
Leventhal, H. Toward a comprehensive theory of
Circulation, 1980, 61, 18-25.
emotions. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
Kornzweig, N. D. Behavioral change as a function of
experimental social psychology (Vol. 13). New
fear arousal and personality (Doctoral dissertation, York: Academic Press, 1980.
Yale University, 1967). Dissertation Abstracts In-
ternational, 1968, 20, 674A. Leventhal, H., & Avis, N. Pleasure, addiction, and
Kozlowski, L. T. Psychosocial influences on cigarette habit: Factors in verbal report on factors in smok-
smoking. In, Smoking and health: A report oj the ing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
surgeon general [U.S. Dept. of Health, Education 1976, 85, 478-488.
and Welfare Publication No. (PHS) 79-50066]. Leventhal, H., Brown, D., Shacham, S., & Engquist,
Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing G. Effects of preparatory information about sen-
Office, 1979. sations, threat of pain, and attention on cold
Lamontagne, Y., Gagnon, M. A., & Gaudette, G. pressor distress. Journal of Personality and Social
Thought-stopping, pocket timers and their com- Psychology, 1979, 37, 688-714.
bination in the modification of smoking behavior. Leventhal, H., & Cleary, P. D. Experimental studies
British Journal of Addiction, 1978, 73, 220-224. of risk factor modification: Technology or sci-
Lando, H. A. A comparison of excessive and rapid ence? In M. L. Pollock & D. Schmidt (Eds.),
smoking in the modification of chronic smoking Heart disease and rehabilitation. New York:
behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- Houghton Mifflin, 1979.
chology, 1975, 43, 350-355. Leventhal, H., & Everhart, D. Emotion, pain, and
Lando, H. A. Aversive conditioning and contingency physical illness. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Emotion
management in the treatment of smoking, Journal and psychopathology. New York: Plenum Press,
oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1976, 44, 1979.
312. Leventhal, H., Hochbaum, G., & Rosenstock, I. Epi-
Lando, H. A. Successful treatment of smokers with demic impact on the general population in two
a broad spectrum behavioral approach. Journal of cities. In I. M. Rosenstock, G. M. Hochbaum, H.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1977, 45, 361- Leventhal, et al. (Eds.), The impact oj Asian
366. influenza on community life: A study in five cities
Lando, H. A., & Davison, G. C. Cognitive dissonance (U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare,
as a modifier of chronic smoking behavior: A U.S. Public Health Service Publication No. 766).
serendipitous finding. Journal oj Consulting and Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing
Clinical Psychology, 1975, 43, 750. Office, 1960.
Lang, P. J., Lazovik, A. D., & Reynolds, D. J. De- Leventhal, H., & Niles, P. Persistence of influence
scnsitization, suggestibility, and pseudo-therapy. for varying durations of exposure to threat stimuli.
Journal oj Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 395^02. Psychological Reports, 1965, 16, 223-233.
Lang, P. J., Melarned, B. G., & Hart, J, A psycho- Leventhal, H., Safer, M. A., Cleary, P. D., & Gut-
physiological analysis of fear modification using mann, M. Cardiovascular risk modification by
an automated desensitization procedure. Journal community-based programs for life-style change:
of Abnormal Psychology, 1970, 76, 220-234. Comments on the Stanford study. Journal oj Con-
Leeder, S. R., & Woolcock, A. J. Cigarette smoking sulting and Clinical Psychology, 1980, 48, 150-158.
in Sydney schoolchildren aged 12 to 13 years, Leventhal, H., Singer, R., & Jones, S. Effects of fear
Medical Journal oj Australia, 1973, 2, 674-678. and specificity of recommendation upon attitudes
402 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Mausner, B. Report on a smoking clinic. American
Psychology, 196S, 2, 20-29. Psychologist, 1966, 21, 251-255.
Leventhal, H., & Watts, J. C. Sources of resistance Mausner, B. An ecological view of cigarette smok-
to fear-arousing communications on smoking and ing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1973, 81,
lung cancer. Journal oj Personality, 1966, 34, 155- 115-126.
175. Mausner, B., & Platt, E. S. Smoking: A behavioral
Leventhal, H., Watts, J. C., & Pagano, F. Effects of analysis. New York: Pergamon Press, 1971.
fear and instructions on how to cope with danger. Mausner, J. S., Mausner, B., & Rial, W. Y. The in-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, fluence of a physician on the smoking of his pa-
6, 313-321. tients. American Journal of Public Health, 1968,
Levinson, B. L., Shapiro, D., Schwartz, G. E., & 58, 46-53.
Tursky, B. Smoking elimination by gradual reduc- McFall, R. Effects of self-monitoring on normal
tion. Behavior Therapy, 1971, 2, 477-487. smoking behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
Lichtenstein, E. Modification of smoking behavior: cal Psychology, 1970, 35, 135-142.
Good designsIneffective treatments. Journal of McFall, R. M., & Hammen, C. L. Motivation, struc-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1971, 36, 163-
ture, and self-monitoring: Role of nonspecific fac-
166. tors in smoking reduction. Journal of Consulting
Lichtenstein, E., & Danaher, B. G. Modification of and Clinical Psychology, 1971, 37, 80-86.
smoking behavior: A critical analysis of theory,
research and practice. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eislcr, McGuire, W. J. Inducing resistance to persuasion:
& P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in behavior modi- Some contemporary approaches. In L. Berkowitz
fication (Vol. 3). New York: Academic Press, 1975. ( E d . ) , Advances in experimental social psychology
Lichtenstein, E., Harris, D. E., Birchler, G. R., Wahl, (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press, 1964.
J. M., & Schmahl, D. P. Comparison of rapid McKennell, A. C. British research into smoking be-
smoking, warm, smoky air. and attention placebo havior. In E. F. Borgatta & R. R. Evans (Eds,),
in the modification of smoking behavior. Journal Smoking, health and behavior. Chicago: Aldine,
oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973, 40, 1968.
92-98. McKennell, A. C. Is addictive smoking an indepen-
Lichtenstein, E., & Keutzer, C. S. Modification of dent trait? International Journal oj the Addictions,
smoking behavior: A later look. In H. Fenster- 1973, <?, 505-509.
heim, C. M. Franks, A. A. Lazarus, & R. D. Rubin Meichenbaum, D. Teaching children self-control. In
(Eds.), Advances in behavior therapy: Proceedings. B. Lahey & A. Kazden (Eds.), Advances in child
New York: Academic Press, 1971. clinical psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Plenum
Locke, E. A. Is "behavior therapy" behavioristic? Press, 1979.
(An analysis of Wolpe's psychotherapeulic meth- Melzack, R. The puzzle of pain. New York: Basic
ods). Psychological Bulletin, 1971, 76, 318-327. Books, 1973.
Lucchcsi, B. R, Schuster, C. R., & Emley, G. S. The Merki, D. J., Creswell, W. H., Stone, D. B., Huff-
role of nicotine as a determinant of cigarette smok- man, W., & Newman, J. The effects of two educa-
ing frequency in man with observations of certain tional methods and message themes on rural youth
cardio-vascular effects of the tobacco alkaloid. smoking behavior. Journal of School Health, 1968,
Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 1967, 8, 38, 448-454.
789-796. Meyer, A., & Henderson, J. B. Multiple risk factor
Maccoby, N., & Farquhar, J. W. Communication for reduction in the prevention of cardiovascular dis-
health: Unselling heart disease. Journal oj Com- ease. Preventive Medicine, 1974, 3, 225-236.
munication, 1975, 25, 114-126. Meyer, A. J., Nash, J. D., McAlister, A. L., Maccoby,
Maiman, L. A., & Becker, M. H. The health belief N., & Farquhar, J. W. Skills training in a cardio-
model: Origins and correlates in psychological the- vascular health education campaign. Journal oj
ory. Health Education Monographs, 1974, 2, 336- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1980, 48, 129-
353. 142.
Malmo, R. B. On emotions, needs, and our archaic Miller, A., & Gimpl, M. Operant conditioning and
brains. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1975. self-control of smoking and studying. Journal of
Marlatt, G. A. Alcohol, stress, and cognitive con- Genetic Psychology, 1971, 119, 181-186.
trol. In C. D. Spielberger & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Miller, M. M. Hypnoaversion treatment in alcohol-
Stress and anxiety (Vol. 3). New York: Hemi- ism, nicotinism and weight control. Hospital Week,
sphere, 1976. October 17, 1975, pp. 129-130.
Man-one, R. L., Merksamer, M. A., & Salzberg, P. Mitchell, V. Instant answers for students on what
M. A short duration group treatment of smoking
smoking does to the body. American Lung Associa-
behavior by stimulus saturation. Behaviour Re-
search and Therapy, 1970, 8, 347-352. tion Bulletin, 1978, 10(1), 6-9.
Marston, A. R., & McFall, R. M. Comparison of be- Monk, M., Tayback, M., & Gordon, J. Evaluation
havior modification approaches to smoking reduc- of an anti-smoking program among high school
tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- students. American Journal of Public Health, 1965,
ogy, 1971, 36, 153-162. 55, 994-1004.
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 403

Morison, J. B., & Medovy, H. Smoking habits o year. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 1971, 62,
Winnipeg school children. Canadian Medical Asso- 432-441.
ciation Journal, 1961, 84, 1004-1012. Piper, G. W., Jones, J. A., & Matthews, V. L. Sas-
Morison, J. B., Medovy, H., & MacDonell, G. T. katoon Smoking Study: Results of the second year.
Health education and cigarette smoking: A report Canadian Journal of Public Health, 1974, 65, 127-
on a three-year program in the Winnipeg School 129.
Division, 1960-1963. Canadian Medical Association Posncr, M. I. Cognition: An introduction. Glenview,
Journal, 1964, 91, 49-56. 111.: Scott, Foresman, 1973.
Nesbitt, P. D. Smoking, physiological arousal, and Rabinowitz, H. S., & Zimmerli, W. H. Effects of a
emotional response. Journal of Personality and health education program on junior high school
Social Psychology, 1973, 25, 137-144. student's knowledge, attitudes, and behavior con-
Newman, I. M. Adolescent cigarette smoking as com- cerning tobacco use. Journal of School Health,
pensatory behavior. Journal of School Health, 1970, 1974, 44, 324-330.
40, 316-321. (a) Resnick, J. H. Effects of stimulus satiation on the
Newman, I. M. Status configurations and cigarette overlearned maladaptive response of cigarette smok-
smoking in a junior high school. Journal of School ing. Journal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Health, 1970, 40, 28-31. (b) 1968, 32, 501-505.
Niles, P. The relationship of susceptibility and anxi- Robins, L. N., Davis, D. H., & Goodwin, D. W.
ety to acceptance oj fear-arousing communications. Drug use by U. S. Army enlisted men in Vietnam:
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale Univer- A follow-up on their return home. American Jour-
sity, 1964. nal of Epidemiology, 1974, 99, 235-249.
Ober, D. C. Modification of smoking behavior. Jour- Rogers, E. M., & Shoemaker, F. F. Communication
nal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1968, oj innovations: A cross-cultural approach. New
32, S43-549. York: Free Press, 1971.
O'Brien, C. P., Testa, T., O'Brien, T. J., Brady, J. Rosen, G. M., Glasgow, R. E., & Barrera, M., Jr.
P., & Wells, B. Conditioned narcotic withdrawal A controlled study to assess the clinical efficacy
in humans. Science, 1977, 175, 1000-1002. of totally self-administered systematic desensitiza-
O'Keefe, M. T. The anti-smoking commercials: A tion. Journal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
study of television's impact on behavior. Public ogy, 1976,44, 208-217.
Opinion Quarterly, 1971, 3$, 242-248. Rosen, G. M., Glasgow, R. E., & Barrera, M., Jr. A
Palmer, A. B. Some variables contributing to the two-year follow-up on systematic desensitization
onset of cigarette smoking among junior high with data pertaining to the external validity of
school students. Social Science and Medicine, 1970, laboratory fear assessment. Journal of Consulting
4, 359-366. and Clinical Psychology, 1977, 45, 1188-1189.
Peak, H. Attitude and motivation. In M. R. Jones Rosen, T. J. False feedback, perceived competancy,
(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. and need for fear control in a fear communication
3). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1955. setting. Unpublished master's thesis, University
Pechacek, T. F. Modification of smoking behavior. of WisconsinMadison, 1970.
In, Smoking and health: A report of the surgeon Rosenberg, M. J., & Abelson, R. P. An analysis of
general [U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Wel- cognitive balancing. In M. J. Rosenberg, C. I.
fare Publication No. (PHS) 79-50066]. Washing- Hovland, W. J. McGuire, R. P. Abelson, & J. W.
ton, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1979. Brehm (Eds.), Attitude organization and change.
Pederson, L. L., Scrimgeour, W. G., & Lefcoe, N. M. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press, 1960.
Comparison of hypnosis plus counseling, counsel- Rosenstock, I. M. The health belief model and pre-
ing alone, and hypnosis alone in a community ventive health behavior. Health Education Mono-
service smoking withdrawal program. Journal of graphs, 1974, 2, 354-386.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1975, 43, 920. Russell, M. A. H. Cigarette dependence: 1. Nature
Peele, S. Redefining addiction: I. Making addiction and classification. British Medical Journal, 1971,
a scientifically and socially useful concept. Inter- 2, 330-331. (a)
national Journal of Health Services, 1977, 7, 103- Russell, M. A. H. Cigarette smoking: Natural history
124. of a dependence disorder. British Journal of Medi-
Perry, C. L., & Mullen, G. The effect of hypnotic cal Psychology, 1971, 44, 1-16. (b)
susceptibility on reducing smoking behavior treated Russell, M. A. H. Nicotine chewing gum as a sub-
by a hypnotic technique. Journal oj Clinical Psy- stitute for smoking. British Medical Journal, 1977,
chology, 1975, 31, 387-390, 1, 1060-1063.
Pincherle, G., & Wright, H. B, Smoking habits of Russell, M. A. H., Feyerabend, C., & Cole, P. V.
business executives. Practitioner, 1970, 205, 209- Plasma nicotine levels after cigarette smoking and
212. chewing nicotine gum. British Medical Journal,
Piper, G. W., Jones, J. A., & Matthews, V. L. The 1976, 1, 1043-1046.
Saskatoon Smoking Project: The model. Cana- Russell, M. A. H., Wilson, C., Feyerabend, C., &
dian Journal oj Public Health, 1970, 61, 503-508. Cole, P. V. Effect of nicotine chewing gum in
Piper, G. W., Jones, J. A., & Matthews, V. L. The smoking behavior and as an aid to cigarette with-
Saskatoon Smoking Study: Results of the first drawal. British Medical Journal, 1976, 2, 391-393.
404 HOWARD LEVENTHAL AND PAUL D. CLEARY

Russell, M. A. H., Wilson, C., Taylor, C., & Baker, Scriven, M. The methodology of evaluation. In R.
C. D. Effect of general practitioners' advice against W. Tyler, R. M. Gagne, & M. Scriven (Eds.), Per-
smoking. British Medical Journal, 1979, 2, 231-235. spectives of curriculum evaluation. Chicago: Rand
Ryan, F. J. Cold turkey in Greenfield, Iowa: A McNally, 1967.
follow-up study. In W. L. Dunn, Jr. (Ed.), Smok- Seligman, M. E. P. Helplessness. San Francisco:
ing behavior: Motives and incentives. New York: Freeman, 197S.
Wiley, 1973. Seltzer, C. G., Friedman, G. D., & Siegelaub, A. B.
Safer, M. A., Tharps, Q. J., Jackson, T. C., & Leven- Smoking and drug consumption in white, black
thal, H. Determinants of three stages of delay in and oriental men and women. American Journal
seeking care at a medical clinic. Medical Care, of Public Health, 1974, 64, 466-473.
1979, 17, 11-29. Shapiro, D., Tursky, B., Schwartz, G. E., & Shnid-
Salber, E. J., Freeman, H. E., & Abelin, T. Needed man, S. R. Smoking on cue: A behavioral approach
to smoking reduction. Journal of Health and Social
research on smoking: Lessons from the Newton
Behavior, 1971, 12, 108-113.
study. In E. F. Borgatta & R. R. Evans (Eds.),
Smoking, health and behavior. Chicago: Aldine, Sherman, A. R. Real-life exposure as a primary thera-
peutic factor in the desensitization treatment of
1968. fear. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1972, 79,
Salber, E. J., Goldman, E., Buka, M., & Welsh, B. 19-28.
Smoking habits of high school students in New- Shiffman, S. M., & Jatvik, M. E. Smoking with-
ton, Massachusetts. New England Journal of Medi- drawal symptoms in two weeks of abstinence.
cine, 1961, 265, 969-974. Psychopharmacology, 1976, SO, 35-39.
Salber, E. J., Reed, R. B., Harrison, S. V., & Green, Siegel, S. Evidence from rats that morphine toler-
J. H. Smoking behavior, recreational activities and ance is a learned response. Journal of Compara-
attitudes toward smoking among Newton second- tive and Physiological Psychology, 1975, 80, 498-
ary school children. Pediatrics, 1963, 32, 911-918. 506.
Sarbin, T., & Nucci, L. P. Self-reconstruction pro- Siegel, S. Morphine tolerance acquisition as an asso-
cesses: A proposal for reorganizing the conduct ciative process. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
of confirmed smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psy- ogy: Animal Behavior Processes, 1977, 3, 1-13.
chology, 1973, 81, 182-195. Singer, R. P. The effects of fear-arousing communi-
Schachter, S. 1. Nicotine regulation in heavy and cations on attitude change and behavior (Doctoral
light smokers. Journal of Experimental Psychol- dissertation, University of Connecticut, 1965).
ogy: General, 1977, 106, 5-12. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1965, 26, 5574.
Schachter, S., Kozlowski, L. T., & Silverstein, B. (University Microfilms No. 66-00,891)
2. Effects of urinary pH on cigarette smoking. Smith, G. M. Personality and smoking: A review
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, of the empirical literature. In W. A. Hunt (Ed.),
1977, 106, 13-19. Learning mechanisms in smoking. Chicago: Aldine,
Schachter, S., Silverstein, B., Kozlowski, L. T., Her- 1970.
man, C. P., & Liebling, B. 4. Effects of stress on Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. An opponent-process
cigarette smoking and urinary pH. Journal oj Ex- theory of motivation: II. Cigarette addiction. Jour-
perimental Psychology: General, 1977, 106, 24-30. nal of Abnormal Psychology, 1973, 81, 158-171.
Schachter, S., Silverstein, B., & Perlick, D. 5. Psy- Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. An opponent-process
chological and pharmacological explanations of theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of
smoking under stress. Journal of Experimental affect. Psychological Review, 1974, 81, 119-145.
Psychology: General, 1977, 106, 31-40. Steffy, R. A., Meichenbaum, D., & Best, J. A. Aver-
Schmahl, D. P., Lichtenstein, E., & Harris, D. E. sive and cognitive factors in the modification of
Successful treatment of habitual smokers with smoking behavior. Behaviour Research and Ther-
warm, smoky air and rapid smoking. Journal of apy, 1970, 8, 115-125.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1972, 3S, 105- Stern, M. P., Farquhar, J. W., Maccoby, N., & Rus-
sell, S. H. Results of a two-year health education
111.
campaign on dietary behavior: The Stanford Three
Schubert, D. S. P. Arousal seeking as a central factor Community Study. Circulation, 1976, $4, 826-833.
in tobacco smoking among college students. Inter- Stewart, L., & Livson, N. Smoking and rebelliousness:
national Journal of Social Psychiatry, 1965, 11, A longitudinal study from childhood to maturity.
221-225. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1966, 30, 225-
Schwartz, J. L. A critical review and evaluation of 229.
smoking control methods. Public Health Reports, Stolerman, I. P., Goldfarb, T., Fink, R., & Jarvik,
1969, 84, 483-506. M. E. Influencing cigarette smoking with nicotine
Schwartz J. L., & Dubitzky, M. Clinical reduction antagonists. Psychopharmacologia, 1973, 28, 247-
of smoking: A California study. Addictions, 1967, 259.
14, 35-44. Suchman, E. A. Stages of illness and medical care.
Schwartz, J. L., & Dubitzky, M, Requisites for suc- Journal of Health and Human Behavior, 1965, 6,
cess in smoking withdrawal. In E. F. Borgatta & 114-128.
R. R. Evans (Eds.), Smoking, health and behavior. Suedfeld, P., & Best, J. A. Satiation and sensory dep-
Chicago: Aldine, 1968. rivation combined in smoking therapy: Some case
THE SMOKING PROBLEM 405

studies and unexpected side effects. International geon general's reports. The health consequences oj
Journal o) the Addictions, 1977, 12, 337-359. smoking, 1973 [Publication No. (HMS) 73-8704].
Suedfeld, P., & Ikard, F. F. Use of sensory depriva- U.S. Government Printing Office, 1973.
tion in facilitating the reduction of cigarette smok- von Hilsheimer, G. Eliminating the smoking habit.
ing. Journal oj Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Human Learning, 1968, S, 1-5.
1974, 42, 888-895. Wagner, M. K., & Bragg, R. A. Comparing behavior
Suedfeld, P., Landon, P. B., Pargament, R., & Ep- modification approaches to habit decrement
stein, Y. M. An experimental attack on smoking Smoking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
(Attitude manipulation in restricted environments, chology, 1970, 34, 258-263.
III). International Journal oj the Addictions, 1972, Warner, K. E. The effects of the anti-smoking cam-
7, 721-733. paign on cigarette consumption. American Journal
Sutherland, A., Amit, Z., Golden, M. & Roseberger, of Public Health, 1977, 67, 645-650.
Z. Comparison of three behavioral techniques in Watts, J. C. The role of vulnerability in resistance
the modification of smoking behavior. Journal of to fear-arousing communications (Doctoral disser-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1975, 43, 443- tation, Bryn Mawr College, 1966). Dissertation
447. Abstracts International, 1968, 28, 4274A. (Univer-
Swinehart, J. W. Changes over time in student reac- sity Microfilms No. 68-04,699)
tions to the surgeon general's report on smoking Weiss, W. Effects of the mass media of communica-
and health. American Journal of Public Health, tion. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The
1966, 56, 2023-2027. handbook of social psychology (Vol. 5). Reading,
Swinehart, J. W., & Kirscht, J. P. Smoking: A panel Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1969.
study of beliefs and behavior following the PHS West, D. W., Graham, S., Swanson, M., & Wilkinson,
report. Psychological Reports, 1966,18, 519-528. G. Five-year follow-up of a smoking withdrawal
Tamerin, J. S. The psychodynamics of quitting smok- clinic population. American Journal oj Public
ing in a group. American Journal of Psychiatry, Health, 1977, 67, 536-544.
1972, 129, 589-595. White, K. L., Williams, T. G., & Greenberg, B. G.
Tennant, F. S., Jr., & Detels, R. Relationship of al- The ecology of medical care. New England Journal
cohol, cigarette, and drug abuse in adulthood with of Medicine, 1961, 265, 885-892.
alcohol, cigarette and coffee consumption in child- Whitman, T. L. Aversive control of smoking behav-
hood. Preventive Medicine, 1976, 5, 70-77. ior in a group context. Behaviour Research and
Thomas, C. B. Characteristics of smokers compared Therapy, 1972, 10, 97-104.
with nonsmokers in a population of healthy young Wilde, G. J. S. Behaviour therapy for addicted ciga-
adults. Annals oj Internal Medicine, 1960, S3, rette smokers: A preliminary investigation. Be-
697-718. haviour Research and Therapy, 1964, 2, 107-109.
Thompson, E. L. Smoking education programs, 1960- Williams, C. L., Arnold, C. B., & Wynder, E. L.
1976. American Journal of Public Health, 1978, 68, Primary prevention of chronic disease beginning in
250-257. childhood. Preventive Medicine, 1977, 6, 344-357.
Tomkins, S. S. Psychological model for smoking be- Williams, H. 0. Routine advice against smoking: A
havior. American Journal of Public Health, 1966, chest clinic pilot study. Practitioner, 1969, 202,
56, Supplement 2, 17-20. 672-676.
Tomkins, S. S. A modified model of smoking be- Wohlford, P., & Giammona, S. T. Personality and
havior. In E. F. Borgatta & R. R. Evans (Eds.), social variables related to the initiation of smok-
Smoking, health and behavior, Chicago: Aldine, ing cigarettes. Journal of School Health, 1969, 39,
1968. 544-552.
U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare, U.S. Zane, P. K., Kunze, M. J., & Kunze, M. Smoking
Public Health Service, Center for Disease Control. behavior: Motivational determinants and modi-
Smoking and health: Report oj the advisory com- fication techniques. A review. Rehabilitation, 1974,
mittee to the surgeon general of the Public Health 27, 1-12.
Service [Publication No. (PHS) 1103]. Washing- Zborowski, M. Cultural components in response to
ton, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1964. pain. Journal of Social Issues, 1952, 8, 16-30.
U.S. Dept. of Health, Education and Welfare, Na-
tional Clearinghouse on Smoking and Health, sur- Received September 25, 1979

S-ar putea să vă placă și