Sunteți pe pagina 1din 38

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/241065842

The Genetic Architecture of


Honeybee Breeding

Article in Advances in insect physiology December 2010


DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-381387-9.00003-8

CITATIONS READS

7 1,262

2 authors:

Peter Oxley Benjamin P Oldroyd


The Rockefeller University University of Sydney
16 PUBLICATIONS 288 353 PUBLICATIONS 8,261
CITATIONS CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related
projects:

Clonal Raider Ant Project View project

Australian Honey Bees View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Benjamin P Oldroyd on 14 January 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only.
Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.

This chapter was originally published in the book Advances in Insect Physiology,
Vol. 39, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the
author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial
research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your
institution, sending it to specific colleagues who know you, and providing a copy to
your institutions administrator.

All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial
reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your
personal or institutions website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions,
permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at:
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial

From: Peter R. Oxley and Benjamin P. Oldroyd, The Genetic Architecture of


Honeybee Breeding. In Stephen J. Simpson and Jrme Casas, editors: Advances in
Insect Physiology, Vol. 39, Burlington: Academic Press, 2010, pp. 83-118.
ISBN: 978-0-12-381387-9
Copyright 2010 Elsevier Ltd.
Academic Press.
Author's personal copy

The Genetic Architecture of


Honeybee Breeding
Peter R. Oxley and Benjamin P. Oldroyd
Behaviour and Genetics of Social Insects Laboratory, School of Biological
Sciences, The University of Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

1 Introduction 83
2 The social and genetic architecture of a honeybee colony 84
2.1 Biological level 1: the gene 85
2.2 Biological level 2: the individual 87
2.3 Biological level 3: the patriline 89
2.4 Biological level 4: the colony 91
2.5 Implications of the four biological levels for honeybee breeding 92
2.6 The architecture of disease resistance 92
3 Biological properties of honeybees that facilitate breeding 94
4 Biological properties of honeybees that hinder breeding 97
4.1 Mating behaviour 97
4.2 Inbreeding and the sex locus 98
5 Methods to address the difficulties in honeybee breeding 100
5.1 Marker-assisted selection 102
5.2 Increasing control of mating 104
6 Minimizing inbreeding and loss of brood viability 105
7 Success stories 107
8 Concluding remarks 108
References 108

1 Introduction

The western honeybee (Apis mellifera) has been utilized by humans for honey
and wax production for millennia, yet unlike most other domesticated animals,
the biology, physiology and behaviour of domestic honeybees have changed
little during this time. For most of this period, bees were not managed so much
as kept: people provided rudimentary containers (which were often destroyed
during honey harvesting), and hoped that wild bee colonies would take up
residence (Crane, 1983). Active management and manipulation of honeybee
colonies has only become possible more recently due to a few transformational
developments in husbandry. These were the discovery of the bee space and

ADVANCES IN INSECT PHYSIOLOGY VOL. 39 # 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
ISBN 978-0-12-381387-9
DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2806(10)39003-5
Author's personal copy

84 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

invention of the moveable frame hive in 1851 (Langstroth, 1853), the honey
extractor in 1867 (Langstroth, 1867) and the queen excluder in 1849 (Dadant,
1975).
The first significant advance in bee breeding was the development of proce-
dures that allowed the production of large numbers of queens from a single
queen mother (Doolittle, 1889). For the first time, it was possible to produce
large numbers of queens from superior colonies. The discovery that queens and
drones mate in flight outside the hive (Huber, 1814) had important implications
for honeybee breeding, for it showed that paternity was random. Isolation of
desired breeding colonies occurred at least as early as 1928 (Weatherhead,
1986). Complete control of mating was made possible by the development of
instrumental insemination (II) by Watson in 1927, though it was not until
additional developments by Laidlaw in 1944 that instrumental insemination
became routinely reliable (Laidlaw, 1944; Cale and Rothenbuhler, 1975).
Application of the techniques of queen propagation and artificial insemina-
tion has permitted the establishment of bee breeding programmes. Most bee
breeding programmes have focussed on honey production, temperament and
disease resistance (for descriptions and examples, see Rothenbuhler, 1958;
Ruttner, 1988; Szabo, 1988; Manning, 1996; Rinderer et al., 1999; van
Engelsdorp and Otis, 2000; Rinderer et al., 2001; Spivak and Reuter, 2001b;
Harbo and Harris, 2005; Harris, 2008). Sadly, few bee breeding programmes
have been successful in the long term, constrained by limited progress in trait
improvement, the detrimental effects of inbreeding and poor returns on invest-
ment. However, recent advances in honeybee genetics (Weinstock et al., 2006;
Bienefeld et al., 2007; Oldroyd and Thompson, 2007) have allowed greater
understanding of the genetic architecture of the honeybee colony, and have
provided new opportunities to utilize novel genetic techniques for the enhance-
ment of honeybee breeding. These advances may usher in a new era for bee
breeding in which cheaper and more refined molecular methods are used to
identify and propagate superior individuals.
A comprehensive review of honeybee genetics and its implications for breed-
ing was last undertaken by Rinderer (1986). In light of the new technologies that
have arisen since then, a new review is timely. Here, we consider the social and
genetic architecture of honeybee colonies, and how this relates to issues of
honeybee genetic improvement. We also review recent developments in honey-
bee genomics and the promise that these hold for future efforts in bee breeding.

2 The social and genetic architecture of a honeybee colony

Honeybee breeding requires identifying colonies that show superior traits and
ensuring that the alleles that contribute to these traits are passed on to the next
generation. However, the relationships between genes and desired commercial
traits are more complex in honeybees than in other livestock.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 85

Colony
Queen Drone fathers

First generation

0.5 1.0

Second generation
0.5 0.25 0.75 0.75

Drone Workers New


offspring queen

FIG. 1 Coefficients of genetic relationship between individuals in a colony (ignoring


worker reproduction). Arrows indicate pathways of inheritance of genetic material.
Numbers between two individuals indicate genetic relatedness between those individuals
(assuming the queen and drone fathers are unrelated). Workers sharing the same
mother and father are full sibs, workers sharing the same mother but different fathers
are half sibs.

Honeybee colonies are not a single individual, but a family of many related
individuals (Fig. 1). The performance of a colony is therefore dependent upon
the work of its many members and the interactions among them. Furthermore,
a honeybee colony can be considered to consist of four levels of biological
organization (sensu Reeve and Keller, 1999) and genetic architecture: the gene,
the individual, the patriline and the colony (Fig. 2). Each level of biological
organization can be considered to express a set of observable characteristics that
are analogous to the phenotype of an individual organism. This phenotype
arises from the influence of lower biological levels (forward effects, Fig. 2),
environmental effects (including feedback from higher and lower biological
levels) and interactions within each level.

2.1 BIOLOGICAL LEVEL 1: THE GENE

The availability of the A. mellifera genomic sequence (Weinstock et al., 2006)


and the development of honeybee microarrays (Whitfield et al., 2002;
Weinstock et al., 2006; Navajas et al., 2008) have made the honeybee the
primary model organism for sociogenomic research (Robinson et al., 2005;
Oldroyd and Thompson, 2007; Toth and Robinson, 2007; Smith et al., 2008). As
such, quantitative trait loci (QTL) and candidate genes have been identified for
number of (predominantly behavioural) traits, including learning in drones
(Chandra et al., 2001), defensiveness and stinging (Hunt et al., 1998; Hunt
et al., 1999; Guzman-Novoa et al., 2002b; Arechavaleta-Velasco et al., 2003;
Author's personal copy

Gene expression Behaviour thresholds Response stability Honey production


Phenotypes of Mutation Disease resistance Within-colony genetic diversity Hygienic behaviour
biological level: Genetic variability Metabolism Foraging preferences Overwintering ability
Haplodiploidy and CSD Pheromone production Task specialization Swarming behaviour
Multiple mating Aggression
Caste Disease resistance
Egg-laying capacity

Forward effects of
phenotype:

Biological level: Gene Individual Patriline Colony

Resource availability
Climate
Backward effects
Pests/diseases
(environmental):

Genetic interactions Caste differences Group interactions Colony interactions


Epistasis Worker/queen/drone Patrilinial thresholds Phenotypic correlations
Interactions within Proportion interaction
Dominance Workerworker interactions
each biological level: Pleiotropy: Communication
genetic correlation Brood care
between traits/castes Policing
Queenworker interactions
Pheromone reception

FIG. 2 The biological organization of a honeybee colony. Direct effects of each biological level are indicated with vertical green arrows.
Influences of lower levels on higher levels is indicated by horizontal green arrows. Environmental effects and influences of higher levels on lower
levels are indicated by horizontal blue arrows, while interactions within a biological level are indicated by vertical blue arrows. (For interpretation
of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this chapter.)
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 87

Lobo et al., 2003; Arechavaleta-Velasco and Hunt, 2004), body size (Hunt
et al., 1998), age of first foraging and foraging preference (Hunt et al., 1995;
Page et al., 1998; Page et al., 2000; Rueppell et al., 2004; Ruppell et al., 2004;
Rueppell, 2009), hygienic behaviour (Oxley et al., 2010a), worker sterility
(Oxley et al., 2008) and sex determination (Beye et al., 2003).
One of the most extensively studied behavioursforaging behaviourhas
revealed a complex genetic architecture, which is influenced by multiple loci of
moderate to small effect (Page et al., 2000; Rueppell et al., 2004; Ruppell et al.,
2004), and which exhibit significant dominance and epistatic effects. Further-
more, these QTL have been shown to affect multiple traits (Hunt et al., 2007), a
fact further reinforced by studies showing significant genetic correlations
between a variety of traits related to foraging and food storage (Collins et al.,
1984; Rueppell et al., 2004).
A negative genetic correlation between two traits (e.g. between hoarding and
time to sting; see Collins et al., 1984) means that a beneficial change in one trait
will lead to a detrimental change in the other. A trade-off between two traits will
reduce the rate or even limit the extent to which the traits can be improved by a
breeding scheme. Negative correlations between traits may exist as a result of
either linkage or pleiotropy. Linkage arising from non-random mating (such as
the introduction of unrelated individuals into a population) decreases with each
successive generation after mixing (Hazel, 1943). Pleiotropy, in contrast, will
permanently hinder the breeders ability to improve one trait without altering
others affected by the same genes. As any pleiotropic genes that cause a positive
genetic correlation quickly reach fixation in a breeding population under selec-
tion (Ruane and Colleau, 1995), pleiotropic genes that cause a negative genetic
correlation increase in their relative impact on the ability of the breeder to
improve performance over time (Lerner, 1950).
Factors affecting gene expression operate at different biological levels. Lar-
vae exposed to pathogens upregulate a number of antimicrobial peptides
(Evans, 2004). Genes regulating worker maturation and behaviour are influ-
enced by the social environment of the colony (Grozinger et al., 2003). Most
significantly, the diet fed to young female larvae influences the expression of
many genes (Evans and Wheeler, 1999; Evans and Wheeler, 2000; Hepperle
and Hartfelder, 2001), which in turn determines the developmental fate of the
offspring. Recent work suggests DNA methylation may also have a significant
role in determining gene expression in relation to caste determination
(Kucharski et al., 2008; Elango et al., 2009).

2.2 BIOLOGICAL LEVEL 2: THE INDIVIDUAL

Honeybee colonies comprise three morphologically distinct castes. Drones are


haploid males that develop as a result of being hemizygous at the complemen-
tary sex determiner (csd) locus (Beye et al., 2003). Queens and workers are
diploid females, and develop differentially not due to genetics, but larval diet.
Author's personal copy

88 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Differences in the tasks performed in the colony by each caste are reflected in
their differences in size (Winston, 1987), morphology and longevity (Page and
Peng, 2001).
Drones do not contribute to colony-level traits of economic importance,
except, perhaps, in the negative sense of resource consumption. However,
they do influence a colonys reproductive fitness as determined by the number
of drones that successfully mate and contribute genes to the next generation.
The size of the population of drones within a colony is highly variable, with
numbers varying between zero and several thousand, depending on the time of
year and condition of the colony (Allen, 1958, 1963; Free and Williams, 1975).
The number of drones produced by colonies selected for breeding is particularly
important when queens are allowed to mate in areas containing unselected
colonies.
A honeybee colonys queen, with few exceptions (Anderson, 1963; Barron
et al., 2001), is the sole reproductive female in a colony. The rate at which the
queen is able to produce eggs therefore influences the growth of a colony.
Workers are facultatively sterile, activating their ovaries only when their colony
becomes queenless and broodless. This division of reproductive tasks has
allowed the evolution of strongly divergent caste traits, with queens and workers
expressing physiological adaptations suited to their respective roles.
The worker population can be subdivided according to the tasks currently
being performed by individual workers. These include nursing, comb building,
cell cleaning, nest defence and foraging (Seeley and Kolmes, 1991). Workers
progress through these tasks in an age-dependent manner (mediated particularly
through titres of juvenile hormone and octopamine circulating in the blood;
Robinson, 1987; Huang and Robinson, 1992; Sullivan et al., 2000; Schulz et al.,
2002). The rate at which workers progress through these tasks is influenced by
the individuals genotype (Page and Robinson, 1991; Ben-Shahar et al., 2002;
Jones et al., 2004) and by the number of workers that are available to perform
the task (Robinson, 1987; Leoncini et al., 2004; Chapman et al., 2007a).
Honeybee colonies have two generations present for most of their exis-
tencethe reproductive queen and her worker offspring. Through the pro-
duction of pheromones, the queen influences worker behaviour such as the
age at which workers commence foraging (Pankiw et al., 1998), foraging
activity (Jaycox, 1969), the rearing of replacement queens (Pettis et al., 1995;
Melathopoulos et al., 1996) and the propensity to swarm (Phamdelegue et al.,
1993; Pankiw et al., 1998; Beggs et al., 2007). The interactions that arise
between castes contribute significantly to colony-level phenotype (Oldroyd
et al., 1990). Because phenotypic correlations between worker and queen
effects on some traits are negative (Bienefeld and Pirchner, 1991), gains made
from selecting on colony-level phenotype alone are less than gains made with
selection accounting for effects of both castes (Bienefeld and Pirchner, 1991).
Drones that mate with a queen do not contribute any genetic material to the
drones produced by a colony, and his worker offspring are normally sterile.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 89

Thus, a drones genetic legacy is mostly via the fathering of new queens. Each
queen produced will only inherit the genetic contribution of one of the drones
that mated with her queen mother. As a result, only 1/k (where k is the effective
number of matings; Boomsma and Ratnieks, 1996) of the paternal genetic
contribution to a colonys performance is inherited by any one offspring
queen, and only half of this is subsequently represented in her worker offspring.
Without identification of a virgin queens paternity, her predicted breeding
value is inaccurate. After mating, the predicted performance of a queens
worker offspring is affected by genotypes of the drones she mates with. New
queens may even be the offspring of unselected drones, potentially undoing
previous benefits gained from selection. If the contribution of the drones to
colony performance is ignored, and evaluation is based solely on the merit of the
mother, the selection response achieved in a programme will be significantly
decreased (Henderson, 1984).

2.3 BIOLOGICAL LEVEL 3: THE PATRILINE

Honeybee colonies comprise a large number of individuals that vary in their


genetic relationships to one another (Fig. 1; Crozier and Pamilo, 1980). The
queen shares 50% of her genome with every other individual in her colony. Due
to multiple mating by the queen, random pairs of workers share between 25%
and 75% of their genomeseven more if the drones with which the queen
mated and the queen herself were related. Due to haplodiploidy, drones result
from the development of unfertilized eggs (Dzierzon, 1845; Nachtsheim, 1913).
This means that a mother shares half her genome with her drone offspring, but a
drone shares all of his genome with his mother (Crozier and Pamilo, 1980).
Because all sperm produced by a drone are clonal, drones can be considered the
gametes of the drones mother and a queen can produce sperm vicariously via
her drone offspring.
Workers are capable of laying unfertilized eggs, which can develop into
viable drone offspring. In a colony, therefore, there are four possible degrees
of relatedness a worker can have to drones, depending on which individual is the
mother (Fig. 3). Multiple mating results in lower average worker relatedness
towards worker-laid drones (Fig. 4), and as a consequence, workers increase
their inclusive fitness by only rearing queen-laid drones (Ratnieks, 1988).
Haplodiploidy leads to higher genetic and phenotypic correlations among
workers of a single patriline than would be expected if the drones were diploid
(Oldroyd and Moran, 1983; Fig. 1). As a consequence, colonies often exhibit
significant patrilineal variation for a number of behavioural and physiological
traits. These include hygienic behaviour (Arathi and Spivak, 2001), expression
of antimicrobial compounds (Evans, 2004), resistance to pathogens (Palmer and
Oldroyd, 2003; Behrens et al., 2007), transition from nursing to foraging beha-
viour (Page et al., 1992; Chapman et al., 2007b), thermoregulation (Jones et al.,
2004), water collection (Kryger et al., 2000), guarding (Robinson and Page, 1988),
Author's personal copy

90 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Self Worker Queen Worker


full sib half sib
1.0 0.75 0.5 0.25

0.5 0.375 0.25 0.125

FIG. 3 Coefficients of genetic relationship between a worker and the possible drone
offspring of a colony. Numbers indicate the average relatedness between the individual
and the reference worker (top left). Arrows indicate pathways of inheritance of genetic
material.

0.4
0.35
Average relatedness

0.3 Relationship
0.25 of worker
to: queens
0.2
0.15 Worker
0.1 offspring
0.05
0
0 5 10 15 20 25
Number of drone fathers

FIG. 4 Average relatedness between a random worker to the drone offspring of a


colony as a function of mating frequency. The x-axis indicates the number of drones
mated to the queen. The dashed line indicates the genetic relatedness between a worker
and queen-laid drones. The solid line indicates average relatedness between a worker and
all worker-laid offspring (Ratnieks, 1988).

undertaking (Robinson and Page, 1988), aspects of foraging behaviour (Oldroyd


et al., 1992a; Oldroyd et al., 1993) and colony defence (Frumhoff and Baker, 1988,
Breed and Rogers, 1991).
Colonies with higher levels of genetic diversity among workers, as a result of
higher mating number, are more successful at regulating temperature
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 91

(Jones et al., 2004), are more disease resistant (Palmer and Oldroyd, 2003;
Seeley and Tarpy, 2007) and show improved foraging efficiency (Mattila et al.,
2008), food storage and colony growth (Oldroyd et al., 1992b; Mattila and
Seeley, 2007). Increased performance is believed to be a result of genetically
influenced differences in the propensity of workers to perform particular tasks
(reviewed in Beshers and Fewell, 2001; Myerscough and Oldroyd, 2004;
Oldroyd and Fewell, 2007). Variation in worker propensities to perform a task
means that for a certain level of stimulus to perform a task, only a particular
subset of workers will engage in that task. This modulation of the number of
workers performing any particular behaviour allows more efficient allocation of
workers to tasks (Myerscough and Oldroyd, 2004; Graham et al., 2006). Work-
ers with a sufficiently low threshold for a task may even become specialists, to
the point of delaying their maturation and progression to other colony tasks
(Arathi et al., 2000; Beshers and Fewell, 2001).
Some tasks, such as hygienic behaviour and colony defence, are frequently
performed by workers from a small subset of the total number of patrilines
(Arathi et al., 2000; Hunt et al., 2003). Thus, when queens are chosen based
only on colony-level phenotype, they may not be from the patrilines contribut-
ing to the desired trait. Smaller breeding populations therefore risk losing rarer
desirable alleles before they can become fixed in the population. As continued
selection in a closed population increases the genetic homogeneity, colony
performance arising from patrilineal genetic diversity will decrease, reducing
the gains made by selection.

2.4 BIOLOGICAL LEVEL 4: THE COLONY

Besides perhaps stinging behaviour, the performance of any one worker is not
considered important to the beekeeper. The commercially important traits of
honeybeeshoney and wax production and pollination efficiencyare measured
at the level of the colony. Other traits such as disease resistance, overwintering
ability and swarming tendency are of concern predominantly for the way in which
they influence the primary traits, or prevent the loss of the colony. While the queen
can be considered the only individual of any lasting importance in a colony, she
does not play a direct role in any of the primary traits, but rather influences them
through her production of eggs, the contribution of her genes to the worker
population and the influence of her pheromones on worker behaviour. Breeders
therefore tend to treat a colony as a single entity. Of all the biological levels,
colony-level traits are assuredly the most important commercially.
Colony performance is an emergent property of its social architecture: the
multiple levels of biological organization that arise as a result of its multiple
generations and varied degrees of relatedness among individuals, both within
and between castes, and, ultimately, the genes that these individuals carry
(Fig. 2).
Author's personal copy

92 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

2.5 IMPLICATIONS OF THE FOUR BIOLOGICAL LEVELS FOR HONEYBEE BREEDING

The accuracy of any estimate of breeding merit is limited by the accuracy with
which the phenotype of interest can be measured. Many commercially impor-
tant traits of honeybees are behavioural (e.g. aggression, tendency to swarm,
foraging efficiency) and are influenced by the interactions that occur within and
between each of the four biological levels. Honeybee breeders need to identify
those aspects of a colonys performance that are genetically variable and
determine how the genomes of different individuals interact in the next genera-
tion to influence colony-level performance. Selection of colonies with superior
genotypic merit for breeding is therefore rarely straightforward.
By way of example, we describe in the following section the interactions that
have been identified at the different biological levels for disease resistance.
Disease resistance is an economically important trait for bee breeders
(Sammataro et al., 2000; van Engelsdorp and Otis, 2000), and mechanisms
behind various aspects of disease resistance have been intensively studied
(reviewed in Spivak and Gilliam, 1998; Wilson-Rich et al., 2009). Examining
the genetic architecture of disease resistance therefore illustrates the scope of
the interactions that occur between the four biological levels, and some of the
issues that need to be addressed in breeding for disease resistance.

2.6 THE ARCHITECTURE OF DISEASE RESISTANCE

Honeybees are susceptible to a range of pathogens and parasites (Heath, 1982;


Morse and Nowogrodzki, 1990; Bailey and Ball, 1991; Matheson, 1993; de
Guzman et al., 1997; de Guzman and Rinderer, 1999; Anderson and Trueman,
2000; Ellis and Munn, 2005), and have evolved a number of resistance mechan-
isms against them. These mechanisms include the gene/individual level innate
immunity arising from the production of antimicrobials such as defensins and
abaecin (Evans, 2004) and the colony-level behavioural response known as
hygienic behaviourthe detection and removal of diseased brood
(Rothenbuhler, 1964b; Wilson-Rich et al., 2009).
The molecular mechanisms behind individual and colony-level immunity
differ in complexity and the extent to which they have been characterized.
Abaecin is one of a number of antimicrobial peptides secreted by honeybees
(Casteels et al., 1989, 1990). In larvae, abaecin provides protection against
infection by Paenibacillus larvae (Evans, 2004). The abaecin gene sequence
has been identified (Casteels-Josson et al., 1994), and the genes expression
levels are used to determine abaecin activity in individual larvae (Evans, 2004).
Abaecin shows extremely high variation in expression level even among highly
related individuals, presumably the result of epistatic interactions between
genes within the abaecin immune pathway (Decanini et al., 2007).
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 93

Hygienic behaviour is influenced by a number of genes of moderate to small


effect (Moritz, 1988; Lapidge et al., 2002; Oxley et al., 2010a). A number of
gene candidates for hygienic behaviour have been nominated (Oxley et al.,
2010a), although no functional genomics studies have yet been conducted. It is
known, however, that the likelihood that a worker will perform hygienic
behaviour is significantly influenced by the number of hygienic alleles carried
at three identified QTL. Furthermore, the individual components of hygienic
behaviourbrood cell uncapping and removal of dead broodare influenced
by independent genetic loci (Rothenbuhler, 1964a; Milne, 1985a; Moritz, 1988;
Oxley et al., 2010a).
Changes in gene expression can lead to differences in the phenotype of the
individual worker, and physiological changes in an individual can also affect
gene expression. Increased expression of abaecin leads to improved disease
resistance of the individual (Evans, 2004), but incurs an energy cost (Zasloff,
1992), and possibly reduces foraging efficiency in adults (Evans and Pettis,
2005). Conversely, changes in physiology of the worker induced by infection
lead to an upregulation in abaecin expression (Evans, 2004). A number of key
immunological proteins are also regulated in response to dietary protein (Alaux
et al., 2010), which may in turn be influenced by genes for worker foraging
activity (Page et al., 1998, 2000).
Abaecin and hygienic behaviour provide examples of different interactions
between the gene and the colony-level phenotype of disease resistance. Abaecin
affects the physiological response of the larva being infected, increasing the
resistance of the larva that produces the gene product. In contrast, hygienic
behaviour protects larvae from infection indirectly through the behaviour of
adult workers on already infected larvae.
Many breeding programmes have been established in an attempt to increase
honeybee tolerance towards the parasitic mite Varroa destructor (reviewed in
Buchler et al., 2010; Rinderer et al., 2010). These programmes utilize a number
of honeybee characteristics that have been shown to confer resistance against
Varroa, including hygienic behaviour (Boecking and Drescher, 1992; Spivak,
1996), grooming behaviour (Ruttner and Hanel, 1992) and physiological attrac-
tiveness of larvae to reproductive mites (Buchler, 1990; de Guzman et al., 1995;
de Guzman et al., 1996).
A number of larval cuticular compounds have been found to determine
attractiveness of larvae to Varroa mites (Aumeier et al., 2002). However, the
majority of bioassays used to determine attractiveness rely on infestation rates
of brood cells (Buchler, 1990; de Guzman et al., 1996; Rinderer et al., 2010),
which is likely to depend on additional unidentified factors (Aumeier et al.,
2002) and may explain the variable relationship between brood attractiveness
and Varroa tolerance typically observed (Buchler et al., 2010).
Hygienic behaviour has been successfully improved by selection of superior
colony-level phenotypes (Spivak and Reuter, 2001b). However, by accounting
for variation in hygienic behaviour performed by workers of different patrilines
Author's personal copy

94 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

and breeding from queens descended from superior patrilines, additional


significant improvements in hygienic behaviour have also been obtained
(Perez-Sato et al., 2009). This method of selection is most suited to colony-
level traits that are disproportionately influenced by a limited number of
patrilines, such as in hygienic behaviour (Arathi et al., 2000) and defensiveness
(Hunt et al., 2003).
The individual characteristics that infer colony-level tolerance against Varroa
have been shown to vary between populations (Arechavaleta-Velasco and
Guzman-Novoa, 2001). Individual breeding efforts to improve tolerance will
therefore likely show greatest improvement by considering multiple traits, includ-
ing measures of colony-level tolerance obtained using either measures of colony
mite levels (Harris et al., 2002) or a colony index (de Guzman et al., 1996).

3 Biological properties of honeybees that facilitate breeding

Despite the complex social and genetic architecture of honeybee colonies,


which potentially hinder efforts in bee breeding, honeybee biology also affords
some interesting benefits to the bee breeder relative to breeders of other kinds of
livestock. Of primary value is the moderate to high heritability (i.e. the propor-
tion of phenotypic variance attributable to additive, selectable genetic effects)
of commercially important colony-level traits (Table 1). Although methods
of calculation vary (cf. Soller and Bar-Cohen, 1967; Oldroyd et al., 1987;
Bienefeld and Pirchner, 1990), the heritability of honey production is estimated
to be between 0.15 (Bienefeld and Pirchner, 1990) and 0.54 (Bar-Cohen et al.,
1978); hygienic behaviour (the detection and removal of diseased brood) has a
heritability between 0.18 (Boecking et al., 2000) and 0.65 (Harbo and Harris,
1999a), while colony defensiveness is estimated to have heritabilities between
0.3 and 0.57 (Moritz et al., 1987).
When two traits are genetically correlated, it is possible to select for one trait
based on the measurement of the other (Falconer and Mackay, 1996). This is
particularly useful if the genetic correlation is strong, one trait is difficult to
measure and the other trait shows high heritability. It has been claimed that
some commercially important traits of honeybees have a high genetic correla-
tion with other traits that are more easily measured. For example, hoarding
behaviour of workers is claimed to be phenotypically correlated with honey
production (Milne, 1980) and has heritability estimates between 0.19 (Milne,
1985b) and 0.92 (Collins et al., 1984). Hygienic behaviour towards honeybee
pathogens is correlated with hygienic behaviour towards freeze-killed brood
and towards brood killed using a pin (Spivak and Downey, 1998).
In the 1980s, there was considerable interest in using laboratory hoarding
ability (the rate at which groups of c.a. 30 workers store sugar syrup in a
laboratory cage) as a selection criterion for honeybee genetic improvement
(Milne, 1980). However, this technique has largely been ignored in bee breeding
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 95

TABLE 1
Heritability estimates for commercially important traits of honeybees

Trait Heritability

Honey production 0.42g; 0.360.58i; 0.160.19j; 0.54k; 0.26a,w; 0.15a,q


Laboratory hoarding ability 0.92b
Spring development 0.76a,w; 0.46a,q
Wax 0.39a,w; 0.45a,q
Defensiveness 0.300.57f; 0.41a,w; 0.4a,q
Abaecin production 0.350.4c
Hygienic behaviour 0.65l; 0.180.36n; 0.57m
(upper bound of heritability)
Worker development time 0.61d; 0.8e
(post capping)
Mating number 0.449h

w
Refers to estimated worker effect; q Refers to estimated queen effect
a
Bienefeld and Pirchner (1990)
b
Collins et al. (1984)
c
Decanini et al. (2007)
d
Harbo (1992)
e
Moritz (1985)
f
Moritz et al. (1987)
g
Oldroyd et al. (1987)
h
Kraus et al. (2005)
i
Soller and Bar-Cohen (1967)
j
Vesely and Siler (1963)
k
Bar-Cohen et al. (1978)
l
Harbo and Harris (1999a)
m
Lapidge et al. (2002)
n
Boecking et al. (2000)

programmes. In contrast, measuring the rate at which colonies remove freeze-


killed brood to determine a colonys level of hygienic behaviour has been
widely used as a selection criterion (Spivak and Downey, 1998; Spivak and
Reuter, 1998), and demonstrably improves colony-level disease resistance
against Ascosphaera apis (the causative agent of chalkbrood) (Gilliam et al.,
1983; Invernizzi, 2001), P. larvae (American Foulbrood) (Spivak and Reuter,
1998, 2001a) and Varroa mites (Spivak and Reuter, 1998; Harbo and Harris,
1999a). The level of Varroa infestation in a colony has also been shown to be
affected by the length of time pupal cells remain capped during their develop-
ment (Buchler and Drescher, 1990), while Harbo and Harris (1999b) evaluated a
number of specific traits that confer colony-level resistance to Varroa.
High fecundity allows a breeder to maximize the genetic contribution of
superior animals to the next generation. Honeybee queens are highly fecund,
laying thousands of eggs in a single day. Queens can develop from any female
embryo, primarily, through feeding larva with an appropriate diet (Laidlaw, 1992).
Author's personal copy

96 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Therefore, the transfer of worker-destined larvae to suitable conditions (Doolittle,


1889) allows the production of thousands of daughter queens from a single queen
mother. Drones are obligatory monogamists, dying immediately upon mating.
However, as drones can be considered flying gametes of their queen mother, a
queen is potentially capable of mating with thousands of new queens, through her
production of drone offspring. Instrumental insemination also allows a single
drone to inseminate multiple queens.
Because it is possible to directly assess drone phenotypes, it is sometimes
possible to use drones as agents for direct selection of gametes for any trait that
they express. Although drones do not express most traits of economic signifi-
cance, they can be selected for traits such as colour (Laidlaw and el-Banby,
1962), development time (Moritz, 1994) or pathogen resistance (Behrens et al.,
2007). Being hemizygous, drones will always express any recessive traits that
they inherit (provided that they are expressed in males), allowing selection for
traits that may otherwise be masked in heterozygous females.
Just as fecundity allows the rapid dissemination of valuable genes, so too does
the honeybees short generation time. Queens emerge 16 days after egg deposition
(Winston, 1987), and will usually mate within a week of emergence. This means
the time from transfer of a larva for queen development to the emergence of the
young queens first brood is less than 6 weeks. As the average life span of a worker
during the warmer seasons is 2535 days (Maurizio, 1950), it is possible to generate
a colony populated only with a daughter queens offspring in just 6 weeks. It is
therefore theoretically possible to evaluate offspring colonies for most traits (with
the key exception of long term honey production) only 78 weeks after selection of
a superior queen for breeding. If it is not necessary to assess the phenotype of the
colony, the offspring generation can be evaluated even earlier.
Marker-assisted selection (MAS) of honeybees is yet to be utilized by commer-
cial bee breeders. However, with the recent publication of the honeybee genome
(Weinstock et al., 2006), and the development of genome-wide screening tech-
nologies for honeybees, such as a complete microsatellite linkage map (Solignac
et al., 2007), microarrays (Weinstock et al., 2006), single nucleotide polymor-
phism arrays (Whitfield et al., 2006), expressed sequence tag libraries (Nunes
et al., 2004) and massively parallel sequencing technology (Weinstock et al.,
2006), this situation is set to change. These resources have led to the discovery
of several genetic markers for commercially important traits, including hygienic
behaviour (Oxley et al., 2010a; Lapidge et al., 2002), stinging behaviour
(Arechavaleta-Velasco et al., 2003) and Varroa tolerance (Navajas et al., 2008),
The honeybee has an exceptionally high recombination rate across its entire
genome (Beye et al., 2006). A high recombination rate quickly eliminates
linkage disequilibrium between genetic loci. As a result, only genetic markers
that are tightly linked to genes remain correlated over generations. This has two
implications for MAS. First, any selection programme based on linkage dis-
equilibrium between a trait and a marker will rapidly lose its efficiency as the
association between marker alleles and the trait of interest will decline rapidly.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 97

More positively, a high recombination rate means that genetic mapping studies
are able to locate genetic markers that are closely linked to a gene of interest,
aiding the development of gene-assisted selection (which is not affected by
linkage disequilibrium or recombination rates). The honeybee therefore stands
to gain much from the application of genomic technology.

4 Biological properties of honeybees that hinder breeding

Despite the complex genetic architecture of honeybee colonies, traditional selec-


tive breeding remains a straightforward task. Commercially important traits such
as honey production and defensiveness exhibit both high phenotypic variance and
moderate heritability, providing scope for genetic improvement by simple recur-
rent selection. Colony productivity is uncorrelated with product quality, as it is for
many other livestock traits, and therefore selection for productivity is not eco-
nomically constrained by the saleability of the product. However, the social and
genetic architecture of colonies significantly interferes with the process of artifi-
cial selection, slowing its progress and potentially limiting its long term viability.
In honeybees, the selection of superior breeding individuals is hampered
by the complex genetic architecture of colony organization discussed above.
First, the traits being selected predominantly arise from the combined behaviour
of many non-reproductive individuals. This precludes direct selection of
superior individuals, as is possible for physiological traits in other domestic
animals. Second, mating in flight reduces control of the paternal contribution
of genes to the workers and subsequent queens. Third, multiple mating results in
a colony phenotype that contains multiple paternal contributions, and hampers
identification of the singular paternal genetic contribution to any one queen
offspring.
There are further issues that limit the viability of honeybee breeding pro-
grammes, even after performance gains have been made. The molecular mech-
anism of sex determination in honeybees can quickly lead to significant loss of
brood viability in closed populations (Woyke, 1980; Page and Laidlaw, 1982a),
undoing any progress achieved via selection. Honeybees are also sensitive to
inbreeding (Bruckner, 1979; Bienefeld et al., 1989), a consequence of any
selection programme. The effort required to successfully manage breeding
programmes, combined with low returns due the high risk of queen mortality
(particularly during transport and establishment), further reduces the capacity to
establish or maintain any large scale programme.

4.1 MATING BEHAVIOUR

Honeybee queens mate in flight (Koeniger and Koeniger, 1991) with drones
originating from colonies up to 15 km away (Jensen et al., 2005). Drones gather
at specific geographical locations known as drone congregation areas (DCAs)
Author's personal copy

98 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

(Loper et al., 1987; Pechhacker, 1994), which may contain as many as 15,000
drones (Koeniger et al., 2005). Queens fly to these DCAs, where they mate with
between 10 and 50 drones (Palmer and Oldroyd, 2000) over the course of one to
three mating flights (Neumann et al., 1999).
Honeybee mating biology means that queens mate with drones sourced
from a large geographic range. Jensen et al. (2005) noted that only 18% of
matings occur between drones and queens from the same apiary. This makes
it extremely difficult to reproductively isolate selected lines from unselected
and wild colonies. Nonetheless, because the queens direct phenotype and
contribution to the genotype of workers contribute greatly to a colonys pheno-
type (Oldroyd et al., 1990; Bienefeld et al., 2007), the use of selected queens
and unselected drones can still provide high-performing colonies for commer-
cial use. However, the contribution of unselected alleles to new generations of
queens impairs the potential rate of improvement in a breeding population.
Identification of the paternal genetic contribution to the phenotype of a
queens colony is further hindered by multiple mating. Since only a fraction
of the colonys paternal genome is passed on to offspring queens, neither the
remaining drone contributions nor the emergent properties arising from the
interactions between the patrilines are inherited. Single drone inseminations
can be used as a tool to eliminate this problem (Harbo, 1999), but the resulting
queens have reduced useful life, and do not show the full emergent properties of
more diverse colonies (Jones et al., 2004; Mattila and Seeley, 2007; Seeley and
Tarpy, 2007; Mattila et al., 2008). The possibility of selecting queens from a
patriline that did not contribute to colony phenotype further hinders selection
efforts. Thus, the genetic contribution from drones evades the breeders control
at both the queen-mating and the queen-rearing level.

4.2 INBREEDING AND THE SEX LOCUS

Inbreeding, the mating of relatives, results in increasing homozygosity and is


often associated with a decline in fitness (Wright, 1977; Lande, 1988; Falconer
and Mackay, 1996; for specific effects on honeybees, see Bruckner, 1979;
Oldroyd and Goodman, 1988; Bienefeld et al., 1989; Clarke et al., 1992). In
the honeybee, the effects of inbreeding are particularly severe due to the method
of sex determination.
Sex is determined by heterozygosity at a single sex locus called csd (Beye
et al., 2003). Individuals that are homozygous or hemizygous at csd develop as
males; heterozygous individuals develop into females (Fig. 5). Because males
are haploid (diploid males that are homozygous at csd are consumed by work-
ers; Woyke, 1963), honeybee queens are able to determine the sex of their
offspring by allowing or withholding fertilization of each egg, and unmated
females are capable of laying eggs that will develop into viable male offspring
(Mackensen, 1951; Beye et al., 1999).
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 99

Drone
Queen
father

A B B

A B A B B B

Viable haploid Diploid Non-viable


drones worker/queen diploid
drone

FIG. 5 Sex determination in honeybees. Letters represent two possible alleles of the
complementary sex determination (csd) locus. Viable drones arise from unfertilized eggs
(Beye et al., 2003). Diploid drones, though capable of developing, are killed by workers
(Woyke, 1963), rendering them effectively non-viable. In this example, half of all
offspring sired by the drone father will therefore be non-viable.

Because diploid males are killed by the workers, these offspring are effec-
tively non-viable. Therefore, half of all the offspring sired by a drone carrying
an allele at the sex locus that is identical with one of the queens alleles will be
non-viable. This leads to spotted brood as workers remove all the homozygous
larvae, which reduces the population growth rate achievable by the colony
(Tarpy and Page, 2001).
The average brood viability (V) in a population of honeybee colonies is a
function of the number of csd alleles in the population, given by

V 1  1=a; 1

where a is the number of alleles (Page and Marks, 1982). The number of alleles
maintained at equilibrium in a population is dependent on the effective popula-
tion size and the mutation rate (Yokoyama and Nei, 1979). Since the average
brood viability is greater than 85% when there are seven or more alleles, it
would seem advantageous to maintain at least this many alleles in a breeding
population. However, maintaining seven alleles at equilibrium in a randomly
mating closed population requires an effective population size of approximately
200 (Yokoyama and Nei, 1979), which is equivalent to approximately 93 colo-
nies (assuming 10 matings per queen, using the formula of Wright (1933) and
Moran (1984)). Any population smaller than this, or any breeding design that
does not maximize maintenance of csd allelic diversity, will therefore
Author's personal copy

100 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

experience a gradual decline in the number of csd alleles, with a consequential


decrease in brood viability (Page and Laidlaw, 1982a).
In a closed population, selecting for superior individuals leads to an
over-representation of those individuals alleles in the population. Selective
breeding will therefore increase the rate of inbreeding within the population,
and decrease the number of csd alleles. The resulting inbreeding depression and
reduction in brood viability mean that the gains from selection can be seriously
eroded.

5 Methods to address the difficulties in honeybee breeding

A significant advance in the evaluation and improvement of livestock came


with the development of best linear unbiased predictors (BLUP) (Henderson,
1984). BLUP estimates the genetic component of an animals performance
(its breeding value), based on the performance of the individual and its relatives
and the degree of relatedness between individuals, while removing, as far as is
possible, non-genetic (environmental) effects. In theory, BLUP provides up to
63% improvement in selection response in animals when compared to selection
based only on an individuals own performance (Kerr et al., 1994). Due to the
complexity of accounting for multiple paternity and queenworker effects,
BLUP has only recently been adapted for use with honeybees (Bienefeld
et al., 2007).
A further advantage of BLUP comes from its ability to maximize selection
response across multiple traits, simultaneously accounting for any genetic
correlation between them. Selecting for multiple traits simultaneously reduces
the selection intensityand the improvement realizedfor each trait individu-
ally, but can maximize the overall genetic merit of selected individuals. To
maximize improvement across all traits, a selection index can be applied, giving
weighting to each trait based on its economic importance, heritability and
genetic correlation to other traits. A selection index for honeybees based on
economic weights has been developed (van Engelsdorp and Otis, 2000), as well
as an index accounting for genetic correlations between traits and queenworker
interactions (Bienefeld and Pirchner, 1991).
The multiple paternity of honeybee colonies can be dealt with using BLUP in
several ways (Fig. 6). These include removing paternal effects from the model,
assigning fathers to a single genetic group, creating a pseudo-father from
related drone producing sisters, or averaging the genetic contribution of all
possible fathers to their offspring.
The most straightforward method of accounting for multiple paternity is to
ignore all paternal effects. Mother/maternal grandmother models can be easily
constructed for honeybee BLUP by adapting the equivalent sire/paternal grand-
sire model that has been well developed for other livestock (Henderson, 1984;
Mrode, 2005). While such a model would be easy to implement, it has the
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 101

A C

ps

ps

B D

ps ps ps

FIG. 6 Four primary methods of accounting for multiple matings in BLUP model of a
honeybee breeding programme. (A) All paternal contributions are ignored. (B) Males
considered a genetic group and contribute a common effect to each individual in a
generation. (C) Drones from related sister queens are combined to form a pseudo-father
(indicated by subscript ps), and then added to pedigree. (D) When multiple pseudo-
fathers are used, they each contribute equally to the breeding value of their offspring
(using average relatedness).

disadvantage of accounting for only a limited proportion of the total genetic


variability present in a population, reducing the gains due to selection made in
each generation.
Another method for accounting for unknown paternity is to assign all possible
fathers to a genetic group, which is then incorporated into the model as a fixed
effect. This method is based on the assumption that all fathers that contribute to
the effect of the genetic group are unrelated to each other and to the population
with which they matewhich is clearly not the case in a closed breeding
scheme. The result will therefore be an underestimation of the inbreeding in
the population (Westell et al., 1988) and bias the estimation of breeding values
(Perez-Enciso and Fernando, 1992).
When the drones originate from closely related queens, it is possible to
construct a pseudo-father, which accounts for the breeding values of the
Author's personal copy

102 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

drones and the average relatedness between them. Such a model was success-
fully implemented by Bienefeld et al. (2007) for a closed breeding programme
in Germany. The pseudo-fathers represent a group of sister queens that are all
the offspring of a single queen. This simplification means that the genetic
relatedness of the drones can be calculated (Bienefeld et al., 1989), as well as
the likelihood of desirable genes being passed to the next generation (i.e. the
relatedness path coefficient). Pseudo-fathers can be incorporated into the BLUP
model with their own estimated breeding value and pedigree, having a single
mother (the mother of the sister drone producing colonies) and a single father
(another pseudo-father).
Another method of incorporating unknown fathers into the breeding pedigree
is to calculate the average breeding value inherited from all possible fathers
(through creation of the average numerator relationship matrix (NRM); Kerr
et al., 1994). This provides the biomatrician with the ability to account for
drones that are not descended from full sisters, which is the case in most
breeding schemes. This method is most likely to be of benefit when combined
with the creation of pseudo-fathers for all sister drone producing colonies, as
this reduces the total number of fathers that need to be accounted for, improving
the accuracy of the average NRM model (Kerr et al., 1994), as well as being
logistically simpler.

5.1 MARKER-ASSISTED SELECTION

MAS is the use of a molecular marker that is known to be associated with a


commercially important trait to identify individuals carrying a desired geno-
type. The ability to directly select superior individuals based on their genotype
allows for the possibility of substantial gains in honeybee improvement. Once
molecular markers for a trait have been identified, they can be used to augment
the evaluation of the estimated breeding value of colonies. This will be of
greatest value for behavioural traits that are difficult to accurately quantify.
Additionally, queens and drones can be genotyped prior to mating (Oldroyd and
Thompson, 2007), providing extremely strong selection intensity (Lande and
Thompson, 1990) and faster gains in trait improvement.
MAS can be categorized according to the relationship between a genetic
marker and the associated QTL that is actually responsible for variation in
the phenotype (Dekkers, 2003). As discussed in Section 3 above, a genetic
marker that is not closely linked with a QTL can be identified from a screen
of (comparatively) few potential markers (a minimum of 96 markers for
honeybees, due to their large map size; Lande and Thompson, 1990). This is
done by establishing linkage disequilibrium between the marker and the QTL
through the crossing of two unrelated inbred lines. However, markers that
are not strongly linked to a QTL are only useful for a limited number of
generations, before recombination between the marker and the QTL removes
the linkage disequilibrium. Markers that are at population wide linkage
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 103

disequilibrium (due to being located very close to the QTL), or are causative
mutations, have the greatest utility, but it is substantially more difficult
to identify such markers, and requires screening of a much larger number
of potential markers.
Because the honeybee has an extremely high recombination rate (Beye et al.,
2006), a QTL with a confidence interval of a given size will result in a smaller
number of gene candidates than in species with a lower rate of recombination.
Nonetheless, the use of standard linkage mapping will rarely provide a resolving
power greater than 1020 cM, except in cases where thousands of individuals
are mapped (Darvasi and Soller, 1997; Weller and Soller, 2004). This interval
can still generate up to 40,000 genes/4000 cM  20 cM 200 gene candidates,
depending on the density of genes within the confidence interval of the QTL. It
is therefore necessary to rank primary candidates from those located in the QTL
region.
Ron and Weller (2007) proposed four criteria for identifying primary gene
candidates following a QTL study:

(1) Genes should have an identifiable link to physiological processes


required for the phenotype.
(2) Gene knockouts, mutation and transgenic studies in other species show
a causative affect on the phenotype.
(3) Genes should be preferentially expressed in organs related to the trait.
(4) Genes should be expressed at key stages of development that affect the
phenotype.

Ron and Weller (2007) concede that most identified genes would fail to
meet all four of their criteria successfully. Nevertheless, their sifting schema
provides a useful method for determining the most likely gene candidates from a
list, and indicates several directions that can be taken to verify the role of the
candidates in affecting phenotype.
Most markers are based on genetic variation in a population, as described
above. However, it is also possible to use the expression profiles of known
candidates. Abaecin expression is correlated with colony-level P. larvae resis-
tance (Evans and Pettis, 2005) and is the only known expression candidate for a
commercial honeybee trait. Although abaecin expression has not been used as a
selection criterion for disease resistance in any breeding programme, assess-
ment of its protein expression levels may in theory be used as a complement to
selection based on sequence level variation at other loci.
Markers can be incorporated into a selection strategy in two primary ways.
Individuals can be initially screened for a molecular marker (or set of markers),
and from this selected subset of the population individuals are then chosen
based on phenotype or estimated breeding value. Alternatively, both phenotypic
and molecular information can be considered together, using a selection index
to give appropriate weighting to the two components.
Author's personal copy

104 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

For single traits, simulation studies have shown that MAS gives greatest
improvement when a trait has a low heritability (Lande and Thompson, 1990;
Ruane and Colleau, 1995). Selectionboth with and without MASleads to
the superior alleles becoming fixed in the selected population. Directly selecting
these alleles, therefore, leads to a faster rate of genetic improvement. It also
reduces the possibility that the superior alleles are lost due to genetic drift before
they have a chance to become fixed in the breeding population. However, after
three or four generations of MAS, most individuals in a population will carry the
preferred allele (Ruane and Colleau, 1995; Pong-Wong and Woolliams, 1998).
From this point, the incorporation of marker information in the selection scheme
becomes detrimental to the selection process, and standard BLUP actually
performs better (Ruane and Colleau, 1995; Dekkers and Van Arendonk, 1998).
For selection of multiple traits, MAS will enhance selection for a longer
period due to the lower selection intensity on each trait. When more loci are
being selected, there is also a greater probability that superior alleles will be lost
if MAS is not used. MAS provides the greatest benefit over other selection
methods when the traits being selected have a high economic weighting, have
negative genetic correlation (but not between their QTL) or their heritability is
large (Togashi and Lin, 2009).
Hygienic behaviour and aggression exhibit many of these properties. They
are both given substantial economic weighting by beekeepers (van Engelsdorp
and Otis, 2000), have negative genetic correlation (Guzman-Novoa et al.,
2002a), and show moderate to high heritability (Moritz et al., 1987; Harbo
and Harris, 1999a). Furthermore, both these traits are close to having identified
molecular markers available (Lobo et al., 2003; Oxley et al., 2010a). Therefore,
honeybee breeders appear to stand to benefit greatly from the use of MAS.

5.2 INCREASING CONTROL OF MATING

Gaining control over the mating of queens and drones can be achieved through
three primary mechanisms: the use of instrumental insemination, geographical
isolation of breeding colonies using islands or mountains and control of mating-
flight time.
Instrumental insemination (II), although the most technically difficult of the
three methods, provides the greatest degree of control over mating. Indeed, II
even allows the breeder to achieve mating outcomes that are biologically
impossible in vivo. Semen from a single drone can be used to inseminate
multiple queens, or the semen from hundreds of drones can be homogenized
and used to inseminate many hundreds of queens, providing complete unifor-
mity in the paternal contribution to each colony in the population (Kaftanoglu
and Peng, 1980; Kuhnert et al., 1989). Instrumental insemination also allows for
the opportunity to genotype the drones prior to semen collection (Oldroyd and
Thompson, 2007).
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 105

Geographical isolation has been exploited for at least 90 years (Weatherhead,


1986). Regions that have been cleared of wild colonies (and are usually selected
due to a scarcity of suitable habitat for wild colonies) can be used to locate the
breeding colonies during mating. This method is still used by commercial
breeders in Australia (Chapman et al., 2008) and Germany (Bienefeld et al.,
2007). However, because queens and drones are capable of mating over a
distance of 15 km (Jensen et al., 2005), a very large area needs to be free of
unselected colonies. The size of the area to be managed may be effectively
reduced by the use of isolated valleys (Jensen et al., 2005).
Open water appears to be the most effective means of maintaining isolation:
queens mated on an island separated from the mainland by 9.3 km have been
shown to be inseminated by drones only from the island (Scharpenberg et al.,
2006). The use of islands therefore provides two benefitsthey can isolate the
breeding population from populations on the mainland, and small islands can
themselves be maintained free from feral colonies with less effort. Islands have
been successfully used as mating stations for a number of commercial breeding
programmes (Allan and Carrick, 1988; Scharpenberg et al., 2006).
In areas that cannot provide geographic isolation, it is possible to manipulate
the time at which the breeder queens and drones undergo their mating flights,
thus minimizing undesired liaisons with ferals and unselected stock. By main-
taining colonies in cool dark conditions during the day, an Australian honeybee
breeder successfully delays the flight of his queens and drones until after the
majority of the feral drones in the area have returned to their colonies (Oxley
et al., 2010b). This ensures that at least 80% of the drones that mate with his
queens originate from his selected colonies. This is sufficient for him to
maintain his breeding lines and make modest genetic improvement.

6 Minimizing inbreeding and loss of brood viability

Having established a closed breeding population, there are several broad meth-
ods available to minimize inbreeding, particularly at the sex locus. These
methods involve some or all of the following components: maintaining a large
population size, using queen replacement or line breeding, identifying and
monitoring the csd alleles in the population, selecting for brood viability and
importation of unrelated stock. Each of these methods can be used indepen-
dently of the others, or combination (for a detailed analysis of many of these
methods, see Page and Laidlaw, 1982b).
The most straightforward method is to maintain a sufficiently large closed
population that can maintain enough csd alleles to ensure high brood viability.
Page and Laidlaw (1982a) have shown that an effective population size of
107 has a greater than 90% chance of maintaining brood viability greater than
85% over 40 generations. The effective population size can be increased
through artificial insemination of homogenized semen from a large number of
Author's personal copy

106 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

drones (Moritz, 1984). This has the added advantage of eliminating the variation
between colonies that arises from differences in paternity that occur when
semen is not homogenized. Because selection tends to favour more closely
related individuals, selection within the population will require a substantially
larger population size to offset the effects of selection on inbreeding (Omholt
and Adnoy, 1994). This means that large population size alone is unlikely to be
sufficient to maintain high brood viability in a selection programme.
It is possible to increase the effective population size, and reduce loss of csd
alleles, through various forms of population subdivision (Page and Laidlaw,
1982b). The least constrained system consists of maintaining two reproduc-
tively isolated populations, which are then crossed to increase the heterozygos-
ity across all alleles. The most constrained involves the selection of at least one
daughter queen from the existing population to constitute the next generation. In
each of these schemes, the level of inbreeding generally decreases with increas-
ing subdivision, but the level of trait improvement is simultaneously decreased.
Simulations by Omholt and Adnoy (1994) predict that the loss of viability from
selection with queen replacement will be similar to that in a population with
random mating of individuals and no selection.
With the identification of the csd locus (Beye et al., 2003), it is now possible
to genotype an individuals sex locus (Hasselmann et al., 2008). This allows the
number of alleles in a population to be monitored directly, and can ensure that
every allele present initially in the population is passed on in each subsequent
generation. However, even with limited genotyping within a population, this
method may not provide sufficient benefit in most schemes to merit the eco-
nomic cost of genotyping. Furthermore, tracking sex alleles will not reduce the
level of inbreeding at other loci, which may still lead to undesirable losses in
productivity.
It is also possible to select for csd allelic diversity indirectly, by selection for
high brood viability. This procedure is effective because queens carrying rare
csd alleles are unlikely to mate with drones carrying identical alleles, and will
therefore have a higher brood viability. Selection for high brood viability
therefore selects for the rarer alleles in the population, reducing the probability
that they are lost due to genetic drift. When combined with mass selection (no
population subdivision, queen replacement, etc.), this method gives greater trait
improvement and higher brood viability than using selection with queen
replacement (Omholt and Adnoy, 1994).
The final method of reducing inbreeding is the introduction of new alleles to
the population from external stock. This procedure has the advantage that new
traits not present in the current population can be introduced directly, if found in
suitable external stock. Because this method does not merely reduce the rate of
loss of alleles, it can be used to maintain a breeding population indefinitely.
Further, it will not only prevent loss of alleles at the sex locus, but also across
the genome, effectively reducing the effects of inbreeding depression.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 107

The difficulty of introducing new individuals into a closed population is that


unless their performance is at least equivalent to that of the population they are
being introduced into, the end result will be an overall decrease in the perfor-
mance of the population (Omholt and Adnoy, 1994). It is therefore desirable to
introduce as few new individuals as possible. However, Page and Laidlaw
(1982b) note that to prevent new sex alleles being immediately lost to drift,
they should be introduced close to equilibrium frequency (1/a). This would
therefore require about (1/a)N (where N is the size of the breeding population)
individuals being introduced for each new allele.
What is needed is to minimize losses of productivity, while maximizing the
introduction of new csd alleles. This can be done by establishing a test popula-
tion, consisting of offspring of the new queens that have been inseminated
(preferably with homogenized semen) from the existing population. This allows
not only their performance to be compared to the existing population, but rarer
csd alleles are more likely to be selected if colonies are also chosen on brood
viability. Drones from the best performing colonies in the test population are
then added to the drones used for inseminating the breeding population, either
through insemination or by isolated mating.

7 Success stories

While there have been many successful honeybee breeding programmes over
the years, we highlight two that have been successfully operating for many
years. These examples have incorporated several of the techniques we have
described in this review.
A breeding scheme established on Rottnest Island, west of Perth in Western
Australia, has successfully maintained a closed population honeybee breeding
scheme since 1979. To prevent inbreeding, replacement queens are reared from
each of 20 separate lines each generation. Furthermore, drones from four
evaluated colonies that are external to the scheme are introduced each year.
Between 1983 and 1991, semen from drones from the external colonies, and
from each of the 20 lines, was homogenized and used to inseminate the
replacement queens (Allan and Carrick, 1988). Colony evaluation in 1991
(Manning, 1996) showed an average 34.27% greater productivity than unse-
lected colonies from outside the programmea 3.1% increase per year. Since
1991, instrumental insemination was abandoned in favour of isolated mating on
Rottnest Island. After more than 25 years as a semi-closed population, there is
no evidence that the breeding lines have greater homozygosity at random
microsatellite loci relative to feral colonies (Chapman et al., 2008).
A programme run by the German Beekeepers Association (DIB) has been
using a number of islands in the North Sea for isolated mating of colonies since
1972. Drones used in each mating are the offspring of 410 full sisters, each the
Author's personal copy

108 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

daughter of a high performance queen (Bienefeld et al., 1989). This resulted in a


realized selection response per year of only 0.04% and 0.03% for honey
production and defensiveness, respectively (Bienefeld et al., 2007), and an
increase in inbreeding of 0.15% and 0.06% per year for queens and workers,
respectively (Bienefeld et al., 1989). From 1994, however, colonies have been
evaluated using a BLUP methodology (Bienefeld et al., 2007), but using the
same management practises as before. This has resulted in a 14-fold and 21-fold
increase in selection response in the respective traits (a study on the levels of
inbreeding in this population using BLUP has yet to be undertaken).

8 Concluding remarks

Honeybee breeders now have at their disposal the most advanced genome
technologies, similar to those available for any livestock, as well as appropriate
statistical models for breeding value estimation. As our understanding of the
genetics of honeybee behaviour continues to improve, we will begin to see that
genetic markers for commercially important traits become an economically
viable option for evaluation of colonies. Our ability to control mating with
instrumental insemination and geographical or temporal isolation also continues
to improve. Combined with MAS and breeding value estimation, honeybee
breeding is beginning to realize significant increases in trait improvement.

References

Alaux, C., Ducloz, F., Crauser, D. and Le Conte, Y. (2010). Diet effects on honeybee
immunocompetence. Biol. Lett. 6, 562565.
Allan, L. F. and Carrick, M. J. (1988). The Western Australian bee breeding program.
Aust. Beekeep. 90, 7278.
Allen, M. D. (1958). Drone brood in honey bee colonies. J. Econ. Entomol. 51, 4648.
Allen, M. D. (1963). Drone production in honey-bee colonies (Apis mellifera L). Nature
199, 789790.
Anderson, R. H. (1963). The laying worker in the Cape honeybee Apis mellifera capensis.
J. Apic. Res. 2, 8592.
Anderson, D. L. and Trueman, J. W. H. (2000). Varroa jacobsoni (Acari: Varroidae) is
more than one species. Exp. Appl. Acarol. 24, 165189.
Arathi, H. S. and Spivak, M. (2001). Influence of colony genotypic composition on the
performance of hygienic behaviour in the honeybee, Apis mellifera L. Anim. Behav.
62, 5766.
Arathi, H. S., Burns, I. and Spivak, M. (2000). Ethology of hygienic behaviour in the
honey bee Apis mellifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae): behavioural repertoire of
hygienic bees. Ethology 106, 365379.
Arechavaleta-Velasco, M. E. and Guzman-Novoa, E. (2001). Relative effect of four
characteristics that restrain the population growth of the mite Varroa destructor in
honey bee (Apis mellifera) colonies. Apidologie 32, 157174.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 109

Arechavaleta-Velasco, M. E. and Hunt, G. J. (2004). Binary trait loci that influence


honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) guarding behavior. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 97,
177183.
Arechavaleta-Velasco, M. E., Hunt, G. J. and Emore, C. (2003). Quantitative trait loci
that influence the expression of guarding and stinging behaviors of individual honey
bees. Behav. Gen. 33, 357364.
Aumeier, P., Rosenkranz, P. and Francke, W. (2002). Cuticular volatiles, attractivity of
worker larvae and invasion of brood cells by Varroa mites. A comparison of Africa-
nized and European honey bees. Chemoecology 12, 6575.
Bailey, L. and Ball, B. V. (1991). Honey Bee Pathology. Academic Press, London.
Bar-Cohen, R., Alpern, G. and Bar-Anan, R. (1978). Progeny testing and selecting Italian
queens for brood area and honey production. Apidologie 9, 95100.
Barron, A. B., Oldroyd, B. P. and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2001). Worker reproduction in
honey-bees (Apis) and the anarchic syndrome: a review. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50,
199208.
Beggs, K. T., Glendining, K. A., Marechal, N. M., Vergoz, V., Nakamura, I.,
Slessor, K. N. and Mercer, A. R. (2007). Queen pheromone modulates brain dopa-
mine function in worker honey bees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 24602464.
Behrens, D., Forsgren, E., Fries, I. and Moritz, R. F. A. (2007). Infection of drone larvae
(Apis mellifera) with American foulbrood. Apidologie 38, 281288.
Ben-Shahar, Y., Robichon, A., Sokolowski, M. B. and Robinson, G. E. (2002). Influence
of gene action across different time scales on behavior. Science 296, 741744.
Beshers, S. N. and Fewell, J. H. (2001). Models of division of labor in social insects.
Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46, 413440.
Beye, M., Hunt, G. J., Page, R. E., Fondrk, M. K., Grohmann, L. and Moritz, R. F. A.
(1999). Unusually high recombination rate detected in the sex locus region of the
honey bee (Apis mellifera). Genetics 153, 17011708.
Beye, M., Hasselmann, M., Fondrk, M. K., Page, R. E. and Omholt, S. W. (2003). The
gene csd is the primary signal for sexual development in the honeybee and encodes an
SR-type protein. Cell 114, 419429.
Beye, M., Gattermeier, I., Hasselmann, M., Gempe, T., Schioett, M., Baines, J. F.,
Schlipalius, D., Mougel, F., Emore, C., Rueppell, O., Sirvio, A. Guzman-Novoa, E.,
et al. (2006). Exceptionally high levels of recombination across the honey bee
genome. Genome Res. 16, 13391344.
Bienefeld, K. and Pirchner, F. (1990). Heritabilities for several colony traits in the
honeybee Apis mellifera carnica. Apidologie 21, 175184.
Bienefeld, K. and Pirchner, F. (1991). Genetic correlations among several colony
characters in the honey-bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) taking queen and worker effects
into account. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 84, 324331.
Bienefeld, K., Reinhardt, F. and Pirchner, F. (1989). Inbreeding effects of aueen and
workers on colony traits in the honey bee. Apidologie 20, 439450.
Bienefeld, K., Ehrhardt, K. and Reinhardt, F. (2007). Genetic evaluation in the honey bee
considering queen and worker effects a BLUP-animal model approach. Apidologie
38, 7785.
Boecking, O. and Drescher, W. (1992). The removal response of Apis mellifera L.
colonies to brood in wax and plastic cells after artificial and natural infestation with
Varroa jacobsoni Oud. and to freeze-killed brood. Exp. Appl. Acarol. 16, 321329.
Boecking, O., Bienefeld, K. and Drescher, W. (2000). Heritability of the Varroa-specific
hygienic behaviour in honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Anim. Breed. Genet.
117, 417424.
Boomsma, J. J. and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (1996). Paternity in eusocial Hymenoptera. Philos.
Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 351, 947975.
Author's personal copy

110 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Breed, M. D. and Rogers, K. B. (1991). The behavioral genetics of colony defense in


honeybees: genetic variability for guarding behavior. Behav. Genet. 21, 295303.
Bruckner, D. (1979). Effects of inbreeding on worker honeybees. Bee World 60, 137140.
Buchler, R. (1990). Possibilities for selecting increased Varroa tolerance in central
European honey bees of different origins. Apidologie 21, 365367.
Buchler, R. and Drescher, W. (1990). Variance and heritability of the capped develop-
mental stage in European Apis mellifera L. and its correlation with increased Varroa
jacobsoni Oud. infestation. J. Apic. Res. 29, 172176.
Buchler, R., Berg, S. and Le Conte, Y. (2010). Breeding for resistance to Varroa
destructor in Europe. Apidologie 41, 393408.
Cale, G. H. J. and Rothenbuhler, W. C. (1975). Genetics and breeding of the honey bee.
In: The Hive and the Honey Bee (ed Dadant & Sons), Dadant & Sons, Hamilton, IL.
Casteels, P., Ampe, C., Jacobs, F., Vaeck, M. and Tempst, P. (1989). Apidaecins
antibacterial peptides from honeybees. EMBO J. 8, 23872391.
Casteels, P., Ampe, C., Riviere, L., Vandamme, J., Elicone, C., Fleming, M., Jacobs, F.
and Tempst, P. (1990). Isolation and characterization of Abaecin, a major anti-
bacterial response peptide in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Eur. J. Biochem. 187,
381386.
Casteels-Josson, K., Zhang, W., Capaci, T., Casteels, P. and Tempst, P. (1994). Acute
transcriptional response of the honeybee peptideantibiotics gene repertoire and
required post-translational conversion of the precursor structures. J. Biol. Chem.
269, 2856928575.
Chandra, S. B. C., Hunt, G. J., Cobey, S. and Smith, B. H. (2001). Quantitative trait loci
associated with reversal learning and latent inhibition in honeybees (Apis mellifera).
Behav. Genet. 31, 275285.
Chapman, N. C., Oldroyd, B. P. and Hughes, W. O. H. (2007a). Differential response of
honeybee (Apis mellifera) genotypes to changes in stimuli for generalist tasks. Behav.
Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 11851194.
Chapman, N. C., Oldroyd, B. P. and Hughes, W. O. H. (2007b). Differential responses of
honeybee (Apis mellifera) patrilines to changes in stimuli for the generalist tasks of
nursing and foraging. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 11851194.
Chapman, N. C., Lim, J. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2008). Population genetics of commercial
and feral honey bees in Western Australia. J. Econ. Entomol. 101, 272277.
Clarke, G. M., Oldroyd, B. P. and Hunt, P. (1992). The genetic basis of developmental
stability in Apis mellifera: heterozygosity vs genic balance. Evolution 46, 753762.
Collins, A. M., Rinderer, T. E., Harbo, J. R. and Brown, M. A. (1984). Heritabilities and
genetic correlations for several characters in the honey bee. J. Hered. 75, 135140.
Crane, E. (1983). The Archaeology of Beekeeping. Gerald Duckworth & Co, London.
Crozier, R. H. and Pamilo, P. (1980). Asymmetry of relatedness: who is related to whom?
Nature 283, 604.
Dadant, C. (1975). Beekeeping Equipment. In: The Hive and the Honey Bee (ed Dadant
& Sons), Dadant & Sons, Hamilton, IL.
Darvasi, A. and Soller, M. (1997). A simple method to calculate resolving power and
confidence interval of QTL map location. Behav. Genet. 27, 125132.
de Guzman, L. I. and Rinderer, T. E. (1999). Identification and comparison of Varroa
species infesting honey bees. Apidologie 30, 8595.
de Guzman, L. I., Rinderer, T. E. and Lancaster, V. A. (1995). A short test evaluating
larval attractiveness of honey bees to Varroa jacobsoni. J. Apic. Res. 34, 8992.
de Guzman, L. I., Rinderer, T. E., Delatte, G. T. and Machhiavelli, R. E. (1996). Varroa
jacobsoni Oudemans tolerance in selected stocks of Apis mellifera L. Apidologie 27,
193210.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 111

de Guzman, L. I., Rinderer, T. E. and Stelzer, J. A. (1997). DNA evidence of the origin of
Varroa jacobsoni Oudemans in the Americas. Biochem. Genet. 35, 327335.
Decanini, L. I., Collins, A. M. and Evans, J. D. (2007). Variation and heritability in
immune gene expression by diseased honeybees. J. Hered. 98, 195201.
Dekkers, J. C. M. (2003). Commercial application of marker- and gene-assisted selection
in livestock: strategies and lessons. J. Dairy Sci. 86, 5.
Dekkers, J. C. M. and Van Arendonk, J. A. M. (1998). Optimizing selection for
quantitative traits with information on an identified locus in outbred populations.
Genet. Res. 71, 257275.
Doolittle, G. M. (1889). Scientific Queen Rearing. Newman, Chicago.
Dzierzon, J. (1845). Gutachten uber die von Herrn Direktor Stohr im ersten und zweiten
Kapitel des General-Gutachtens aufgestellten Fragen. Eichstadter Bienenzeitung 1
(109113), 119121.
Elango, N., Hunt, B. G., Goodisman, M. A. D. and Yi, S. V. (2009). DNA methylation is
widespread and associated with differential gene expression in castes of the honey-
bee, Apis mellifera. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 1120611211.
Ellis, J. D. and Munn, P. A. (2005). The worldwide health status of honey bees. Bee
World 86, 88101.
Evans, J. D. (2004). Transcriptional immune responses by honey bee larvae during
invasion by the bacterial pathogen, Paenibacillus larvae. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 85,
105111.
Evans, J. D. and Pettis, J. S. (2005). Colony-level impacts of immune responsiveness in
honey bees, Apis mellifera. Evolution 59, 22702274.
Evans, J. D. and Wheeler, D. E. (1999). Differential gene expression between developing
queens and workers in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96,
55755580.
Evans, J. and Wheeler, D. (2000). Expression profiles during honeybee caste determina-
tion. Genome Biol. 2, research0001.
Falconer, D. S. and Mackay, T. F. C. (1996). Introduction to Quantitative Genetics.
Longman, Harlow.
Free, J. B. and Williams, I. H. (1975). Factors determining rearing and rejection of drones
by honeybee colony. Anim. Behav. 23, 650675.
Frumhoff, P. C. and Baker, J. (1988). A genetic component to division of labour within
honey bee colonies. Nature 333, 358361.
Gilliam, M., Taber, S. III and Richardson, G. V. (1983). Hygienic behavior of honey bees
Apis mellifera in relation to chalkbrood disease. Apidologie 14, 2940.
Graham, S., Myerscough, M. R., Jones, J. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2006). Modelling the role
of intracolonial genetic diversity on regulation of brood temperature in honey bee
(Apis mellifera) colonies. Insect. Soc. 53, 226232.
Grozinger, C. M., Sharabash, N. M., Whitfield, C. W. and Robinson, G. E. (2003).
Pheromone-mediated gene expression in the honey bee brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 100, 1451914535.
Guzman-Novoa, E., Hunt, G. J., Page, R. E. J. and Fondrk, M. K. (2002a). Genetic
correlations among honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) behavioral characteristics and
wing length. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 95, 402406.
Guzman-Novoa, E., Hunt, G. J., Uribe, J. L., Smith, C. and Arechavaleta-Velasco, M. E.
(2002b). Confirmation of QTL effects and evidence of genetic dominance of honey-
bee defensive behavior: results of colony and individual behavioral assays. Behav.
Genet. 32, 95102.
Harbo, J. R. (1992). Breeding honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) for more rapid
development of larvae and pupae. J. Econ. Entomol. 85, 21252130.
Author's personal copy

112 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Harbo, J. R. (1999). The value of single drone inseminations in selective breeding of


honey bees. In: Apiculture for the 21st Century (eds Hoopingarner, R. and Conner, L.),
pp. 15. Wicwas Press, Cheshire.
Harbo, J. R. and Harris, J. W. (1999a). Heritability in honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae)
of characteristics associated with resistance to Varroa jacobsoni (Mesostigmata:
Varroidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 92, 261265.
Harbo, J. R. and Harris, J. W. (1999b). Selecting honey bees for resistance to Varroa
jacobsoni. Apidologie 30, 183196.
Harbo, J. R. and Harris, J. W. (2005). Suppressed mite reproduction explained by the
behaviour of adult bees. J. Apic. Res. 44, 2123.
Harris, J. W. (2008). Effect of brood type on Varroa-sensitive hygiene by worker honey
bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 101, 11371144.
Harris, J. W., Rinderer, T. E., Kuzentsov, K., Danka, R. G., Delatte, G. T., de
Guzman, L. I. and Villa, J. D. (2002). Imported Russian honeybees: quarantine and
initial selection for Varroa resistance. Am. Bee. J. 142, 591596.
Hasselmann, M., Vekemans, X., Pflugfelder, J., Koeniger, N., Koeniger, G., Tingek, S.
and Beye, M. (2008). Evidence for convergent nucleotide evolution and high allelic
turnover rates at the complementary sex determiner gene of Western and Asian
honeybees. Mol. Biol. Evol. 25, 696708.
Hazel, L. N. (1943). The genetic basis for constructing selection indexes. Genetics 28,
476490.
Heath, L. A. F. (1982). Development of chalk brood in a honeybee colony: a review. Bee
World 63, 119130.
Henderson, C. R. (1984). Applications of Linear Models in Animal Breeding. University
of Guelph, Guelph.
Hepperle, C. and Hartfelder, K. (2001). Differentially expressed regulatory genes in
honey bee caste development. Naturwissenschaften 88, 113116.
Huang, Z. Y. and Robinson, G. E. (1992). Honeybee colony integration. Worker-worker
interactions mediate hormonally regulated plasticity in division-of-labor. Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA 89, 1172611729.
Huber, F. (1814). Nouvelles Observations sur les Abeilles. Barde, Manget & Co,
Geneva.
Hunt, G. J., Page, R. E., Fondrk, M. K. and Dullum, C. J. (1995). Major quantitative loci
affecting honey bee foraging behaviour. Genetics 141, 15371545.
Hunt, G. J., Guzman-Novoa, E., Fondrk, M. K. and Page, R. E. (1998). Quantitative trait
loci for honey bee stinging behavior and body size. Genetics 21, 12031213.
Hunt, G. J., Collins, A. M., Rivera, R., Page, R. E. and Guzman-Novoa, E. (1999).
Quantitative trait loci influencing honeybee alarm pheromone levels. J. Hered. 90,
585589.
Hunt, G. J., Guzman-Novoa, E., Uribe-Rubio, J. L. and Prieto-Merlos, D. (2003).
Genotype-environment interactions in honeybee guarding behaviour. Anim. Behav.
66, 459467.
Hunt, G. J., Amdam, G. V., Schlipalius, D., Emore, C., Sardesai, N., Williams, C. E.,
Rueppell, O., Guzman-Novoa, E., Arechavaleta-Velasco, M., Chandra, S.,
Fondrk, M. K. Beye, M., et al. (2007). Behavioral genomics of honeybee foraging
and nest defense. Naturwissenschaften 94, 247267.
Invernizzi, C. (2001). Chalkbrood disease resistance in Apis mellifera colonies with
efficient hygienic behaviour (Hymenoptera, Apidae) [Resistencia a la enfermedad
de Cra yesificada por colonias de Apis mellifera con eficiente comportamiento
higienico (Hymenoptera, Apidae)] [in Spanish]. Iheringia Ser. Zool. 91, 109114.
Jaycox, E. R. (1969). Honey bee queen pheromones and worker foraging behavior. Ann.
Entomol. Soc. Am. 63, 222228.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 113

Jensen, A. B., Palmer, K. A., Chaline, N., Raine, N. E., Tofilski, A., Martin, S. J.,
Pedersen, B. V., Boomsma, J. J. and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2005). Quantifying honey
bee mating range and isolation in semi-isolated valleys by DNA microsatellite
paternity analysis. Conserv. Genet. 6, 527537.
Jones, J. C., Myerscough, M. R., Graham, S. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2004). Honey bee nest
thermoregulation: diversity promotes stability. Science 305, 402404.
Kaftanoglu, O. and Peng, C. Y.-S. (1980). A washing technique for collection of
honeybee semen. J. Apic. Res. 19, 205211.
Kerr, R. J., Hammond, K. and Kinghorn, B. P. (1994). Effects of multiple-sire matings on
genetic evaluations, selection response and rates of inbreeding. Livest. Prod. Sci. 38,
161168.
Koeniger, N. and Koeniger, G. (1991). An evolutionary approach to mating behaviour
and drone copulatory organs in Apis. Apidologie 22, 581590.
Koeniger, N., Koeniger, G., Gries, M. and Tingek, S. (2005). Drone competition at drone
congregation areas in four Apis species. Apidologie 36, 211221.
Kraus, F. B., Neumann, P. and Moritz, R. F. A. (2005). Genetic variance of mating
frequency in the honeybee (Apis mellifera L.). Insect. Soc. 52, 15.
Kryger, P., Kryger, U. and Moritz, R. F. A. (2000). Genotypic variability for the tasks of
water collecting and scenting in a honey bee colony. Ethology 106, 769779.
Kucharski, R., Maleszka, J., Foret, S. and Maleszka, R. (2008). Nutritional control of
reproductive status in honeybees via DNA methylation. Science 319, 18271830.
Kuhnert, M. E., Carrick, M. J. and Allan, L. F. (1989). Use of homogenized drone semen
in a bee breeding program in Western Australia. Apidologie 20, 371381.
Laidlaw, H. H. (1944). Artificial insemination of the queen bee (Apis mellifera L.):
morphological basis and results. J. Morphol. 74, 429.
Laidlaw, H. H. (1992). Production of queens and package bees. In: The Hive and the
Honey Bee (ed Graham, J. M.),, pp. 9891042. Dadant & Sons, Hamilton, IL.
Laidlaw, H. H. and el-Banby, M. A. (1962). Inhibition of yellow body color in the honey
bee, Apis mellifera L.. J. Hered. 53, 171173.
Lande, R. (1988). Genetics and demography in biological conservation. Science 241,
14551460.
Lande, R. and Thompson, R. (1990). Efficiency of marker-assisted selection in the
improvement of quantitative traits. Genetics 124, 743756.
Langstroth, L. L. (1853). Langstroth on the Hive and the Honey-Bee: A Bee Keepers
Manual. Bridgman & Co., Northampton.
Langstroth, L. L. (1867). Improved Apparatus for Extracting Honey from the Comb. US
Patent Office.
Lapidge, K. L., Oldroyd, B. P. and Spivak, M. (2002). Seven suggestive quantitative trait
loci influence hygienic behavior of honey bees. Naturwissenschaften 89, 565568.
Leoncini, I., Le Conte, Y., Costagliola, G., Plettner, E., Toth, A. L., Wang, M., Huang, Z.,
Becard, J.-M., Crauser, D., Slessor, K. N. and Robinson, G. E. (2004). Regulation of
behavioral maturation by a primer pheromone produced by adult worker honey bees.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 1755917564.
Lerner, I. M. (1950). Population Genetics and Animal Improvement. Cambridge Univer-
sity Press, Cambridge.
Lobo, N. F., Ton, L. Q., Hill, C. A., Emore, C., Romero-Severson, J., Hunt, G. J. and
Collins, F. H. (2003). Genomic analysis in the sting-2 quantitative trait locus for
defensive behavior in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Genome Res. 13, 25882593.
Loper, G. M., Wolf, W. W. and Taylor, O. R. (1987). Detection and monitoring of drone
congregation areas by radar. Apidologie 18, 163172.
Mackensen, O. (1951). Viability and sex determination in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.).
Genetics 36, 500509.
Author's personal copy

114 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Manning, R. (1996). Evaluation of the Western Australian queen bee breeding program.
Aust. J. Exp. Agric. 36, 513518.
Matheson, A. (1993). World bee health report. Bee World 74, 176212.
Mattila, H. R. and Seeley, T. D. (2007). Genetic diversity in honey bee colonies enhances
productivity and fitness. Science 317, 362364.
Mattila, H. R., Burke, K. M. and Seeley, T. D. (2008). Genetic diversity within honeybee
colonies increases signal production by waggle-dancing foragers. Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B 275, 809816.
Maurizio, A. (1950). The influence of pollen feeding and brood-care on longevity and
physiology of bees. Schweiz. Bienen Zeitung 73, 5864.
Melathopoulos, A. P., Winston, M. L., Pettis, J. S. and Pankiw, T. (1996). Effect of queen
mandibular pheromone on initiation and maintenance of queen cells in the honey bee
(Apis mellifera L.). Can. Entomol. 128, 263272.
Milne, C. P. J. (1980). Laboratory measurement of honey production in the honey bee
Apis mellifera. 1. A model for hoarding behavior by caged workers. J. Apic. Res. 19,
122126.
Milne, C. P. J. (1985a). Estimates of the heritabilities of and genetic correlation between
two components of honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) hygienic behaviour: uncapping
and removing. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 78, 841844.
Milne, C. P. J. (1985b). A heritability estimate of honeybee hoarding behavior. Apido-
logie 16, 413420.
Moran, C. (1984). Sex-linked effective poplation size in control populations, with
particular reference to honeybees (Apis mellifera L). Theor. Appl. Genet. 67,
317322.
Moritz, R. F. A. (1984). Semen diluents and homogenous semen mixing for artificial
insemination of the honeybee queen Apis mellifica. Apidologie 15, 269271.
Moritz, R. F. A. (1985). Heritability of the postcapping stage in Apis mellifera and its
relation to varroatosis resistance. J. Hered. 76, 267270.
Moritz, R. F. A. (1988). A reevaluation of the two-locus model hygienic behavior in
honey bees, Apis mellifera L. J. Hered. 79, 257262.
Moritz, R. F. A. (1994). Selection for varroatosis resistance in honeybees. Parasitol.
Today 10, 236238.
Moritz, R. F. A., Southwick, E. E. and Harbo, J. B. (1987). Genetic analysis of defensive
behaviour of honeybee colonies Apis mellifera L. in a field test. Apidologie 18, 2742.
Morse, R. A. and Nowogrodzki, R. (eds.) (1990). In: Honey Bee Pests, Predators and
Diseases, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.
Mrode, R. A. (2005). Linear Models for the Prediction of Animal Breeding Values.
CABI Publishing, Oxford.
Myerscough, M. R. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2004). Simulation models of the role of genetic
variability in social insect task allocation. Insect. Soc. 51, 146152.
Nachtsheim, H. (1913). Cytologische Studien uber die Geschlechtsbestimmung bie der
Honigbiene (Apis mellifera L.). Arch. Zellforsch 11, 119241.
Navajas, M., Migeon, A., Alaux, C., Martin-Magniette, M. L., Robinson, G. E.,
Evans, J. D., Cros-Arteil, S., Crauser, D. and Le Conte, Y. (2008). Differential gene
expression of the honey bee Apis mellifera associated with Varroa destructor infec-
tion. BMC Genomics 9, 301.
Neumann, P., Moritz, R. F. A. and van Praagh, J. (1999). Queen mating frequency in
different types of honey bee mating apiaries. J. Apic. Res. 38, 1118.
Nunes, F. M. F., Valente, V., Sousa, J. F., Cunha, M. A. V., Pinheiro, D. G., Maia, R. M.,
Araujo, D. D., Costa, M. C. R., Martins, W. K., Carvalho, A. F., Monesi, N.
Nascimento, A. M., et al. (2004). The use of Open Reading-frame ESTs (ORESTES)
for analysis of the honey bee transcriptome. BMC Genomics 5, 84.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 115

Oldroyd, B. P. and Fewell, J. H. (2007). Genetic diversity promotes homeostasis in insect


colonies. Trends Ecol. Evol. 22, 408413.
Oldroyd, B. P. and Goodman, R. D. (1988). Inbreeding and heterosis in queen bees in
relation to brood area and honey production. Aust. J. Agric. Res. 39, 959964.
Oldroyd, B. P. and Moran, C. (1983). Heritability of worker characters in the honeybee
(Apis mellifera). Aust. J. Biol. Sci. 36, 323332.
Oldroyd, B. P. and Thompson, G. J. (2007). Behavioural genetics of the honey bee, Apis
mellifera. Adv. Insect Physiol. 33, 149.
Oldroyd, B. P., Moran, C. and Nicholas, F. W. (1987). Diallele crosses of honeybees. II A
note presenting the heritability of honey production under Australian conditions.
Aust. J. Agric. Res. 38, 651654.
Oldroyd, B. P., Goodman, R. D. and Allaway, M. A. (1990). On the relative importance
of queens and workers to honey production. Apidologie 21, 153159.
Oldroyd, B. P., Rinderer, T. E. and Buco, S. M. (1992a). Intra-colonial foraging special-
ism by honey bees (Apis mellifera) (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
30, 291295.
Oldroyd, B. P., Rinderer, T. E., Harbo, J. R. and Buco, S. M. (1992b). Effects of
intracolonial genetic diversity on honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) colony perfor-
mance. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 85, 335343.
Oldroyd, B. P., Rinderer, T. E., Buco, S. M. and Beaman, L. D. (1993). Genetic variance
in honey bees for preferred foraging distance. Anim. Behav. 45, 323332.
Omholt, S. W. and Adnoy, T. (1994). Effects of various breeding strategies on diploid
drone frequency and quantitative traits in a honey bee population. Theor. Appl. Genet.
89, 687692.
Oxley, P. R., Thompson, G. J. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2008). Four quantitative trait loci that
influence worker sterility in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Genetics 179, 13371343.
Oxley, P., Spivak, M. and Oldroyd, B. (2010a). Six quantitative trait loci influence task
thresholds for hygienic behaviour in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Mol. Ecol. 19,
14521461.
Oxley, P. R., Hinhumpatch, P., Gloag, R. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2010b). Genetic evaluation
of a novel system for controlled mating of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J. Hered.
101, 334338.
Page, R. E. and Laidlaw, H. H. (1982a). Closed population honeybee breeding. 1. Popu-
lation genetics of sex determination. J. Apic. Res. 21, 3037.
Page, R. E. and Laidlaw, H. H. (1982b). Closed population honeybee breeding. 2. Com-
parative methods of stock maintenance and selective breeding. J. Apic. Res. 21, 3844.
Page, R. E. and Marks, R. W. (1982). The population genetics of sex determination in
honey bees: random mating in closed populations. Heredity 48, 263270.
Page, R. E. and Peng, C. Y.-S. (2001). Aging and development in social insects with
emphasis on the honey bee, Apis mellifera L. Exp. Gerontol. 36, 695711.
Page, R. E. and Robinson, G. E. (1991). The genetics of division of labour in honey bee
colonies. Adv. Insect Physiol. 23, 117169.
Page, R. E., Robinson, G. E., Britton, D. S. and Fondrk, M. K. (1992). Genotypic
variability for rates of behavioral development in worker honeybees (Apis mellifera L).
Behav. Ecol. 3, 173180.
Page, R. E., Erber, J. and Fondrk, M. K. (1998). The effect of genotype on response
thresholds to sucrose and foraging behavior of honey bees (Apis mellifera L.).
J. Comp. Physiol. A 182, 489500.
Page, R. E., Fondrk, M. K., Hunt, G. J., Guzman-Novoa, E., Humphries, M. A.,
Nguyen, K. and Greene, A. S. (2000). Genetic dissection of honeybee (Apis mellifera
L.) foraging behavior. J. Hered. 91, 474479.
Author's personal copy

116 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Palmer, K. A. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2000). Evolution of multiple mating in the genus Apis.
Apidologie 31, 235248.
Palmer, K. and Oldroyd, B. P. (2003). Evidence for intra-colonial genetic variance in
resistance to American foulbrood of honey bees (Apis mellifera): further support for
the parasite/pathogen hypothesis for the evolution of polyandry. Naturwissenschaften
90, 265268.
Pankiw, T., Huang, Z. Y., Winston, M. L. and Robinson, G. E. (1998). Queen mandibular
gland pheromone influences worker honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) foraging ontogeny
and juvenile hormone titers. J. Insect Physiol. 44, 685692.
Pechhacker, H. (1994). Physiography influences honeybee queens choice of mating
place (Apis mellifera carnica Pollmann). Apidologie 25, 239248.
Perez-Enciso, M. and Fernando, R. L. (1992). Genetic evaluation with uncertain parent-
age a comparison of methods. Theor. Appl. Genet. 84, 173179.
Perez-Sato, J. A., Chaline, N., Martin, S. J., Hughes, W. O. H. and Ratnieks, F. L. W.
(2009). Multi-level selection for hygienic behaviour in honeybees. Heredity 102,
609615.
Pettis, J. S., Winston, M. L. and Collins, A. M. (1995). Suppression of queen rearing in
European and Africanized honey bees Apis mellifera L. by synthetic queen mandibu-
lar gland pheromone. Insects Sociaux 42, 113121.
Phamdelegue, M. H., Trouiller, J., Caillaud, C. M., Roger, B. and Masson, C. (1993).
Effect of queen pheromone on worker bees of different ages behavioural and
electrophysiological responses. Apidologie 24, 267281.
Pong-Wong, R. and Woolliams, J. A. (1998). Response to mass selection when an
identified major gene in segregating. Genet. Sel. Evol. 30, 313337.
Ratnieks, F. L. W. (1988). Reproductive harmony via mutual policing by workers in
eusocial Hymenoptera. Am. Nat. 132, 217236.
Reeve, H. K. and Keller, L. (1999). Levels of selection: burying the units-of-selection
debate and unearthing new issues. In: Levels of Selection in Evolution (ed Keller, L.),,
pp. 314. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J.
Rinderer, T. E.(ed.) In: Bee Genetics and Breeding, Academic Press, Orlando, FL.
Rinderer, T. E., Delatte, G. T., de Guzman, L. I., Williams, J., Stelzer, W. J. and
Kuznetsov, V. N. (1999). Evaluations of the Varroa-resistance of honey bees
imported from far-eastern Russia. Am. Bee. J. 139, 287467.
Rinderer, T. E., de Guzman, L. I., Delatte, G. T., Stelzer, J. A., Lancaster, V. A.,
Williams, J. L., Beaman, L. D., Kuznetsov, V., Bigalk, M., Bernard, S. J. and
Tubbs, H. (2001). Multi-state field trials of ARS Russian honey bees. 2. Honey
production 1999, 2000. Am. Bee. J. 141, 726729.
Rinderer, T. E., Harris, J. W., Hunt, G. J. and de Guzman, L. I. (2010). Breeding for
resistance to varroa destructor in North America. Apidologie 41, 409425.
Robinson, G. E. (1987). Regulation of Honey-bee age polyethism by juvenile-hormone.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 20, 329338.
Robinson, G. E. and Page, R. E. (1988). Genetic determination of guarding and under-
taking in honey-bee colonies. Nature 333, 356358.
Robinson, G. E., Grozinger, C. M. and Whitfield, C. W. (2005). Sociogenomics: social
life in molecular terms. Nat. Rev. Genet. 6, 257270.
Ron, M. and Weller, J. I. (2007). From QTL to QTN identification in livestock winning
by points rather than knock-out: a review. Anim. Genet. 38, 429439.
Rothenbuhler, W. C. (1958). Genetics and breeding of the honey bee. Annu. Rev.
Entomol. 3, 161180.
Rothenbuhler, W. C. (1964a). Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. IV.
Responses of F1 and backcross generations to disease-killed brood. Am. Zool. 12,
578583.
Author's personal copy

THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF HONEYBEE BREEDING 117

Rothenbuhler, W. C. (1964b). Behaviour genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees.


I. Responses to disease-killed brood. Anim. Behav. 12, 578583.
Ruane, J. and Colleau, J. J. (1995). Marker assisted selection for genetic improvement of
animal populations when a single QTL is marked. Genet. Res. 66, 7183.
Rueppell, O. (2009). Characterization of quantitative trait loci for the age of first foraging
in honey bee workers. Behav. Genet. 39, 541553.
Rueppell, O., Pankiw, T., Nielsen, D. I., Fondrk, M. K., Beye, M. and Page, R. E. (2004).
The genetic architecture of the behavioral ontogeny of foraging in honeybee workers.
Genetics 167, 17671779.
Ruppell, O., Pankiw, T. and Page, R. E. (2004). Pleiotropy, epistasis and new QTL: the
genetic architecture of honey bee foraging behavior. J. Hered. 95, 481491.
Ruttner, F. (1988). Breeding Techniques and Selection for Breeding of the Honeybee.
The British Isles Bee Breeders Association by arrangement with Ehrenwirth Verlag,
Munich.
Ruttner, F. and Hanel, H. (1992). Active defense against Varroa Mites in a Carniolan
strain of honeybee Apis mellifera carnica Pollmann. Apidologie 23, 173187.
Sammataro, D., Gerson, U. and Needham, G. (2000). Parasitic mites of honey bees: life
history, implications, and impact. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 45, 519548.
Scharpenberg, H., Neumann, P., Van Praagh, J. and Moritz, R. F. A. (2006). Reliability
of an island mating apiary under routine management. J. Apic. Res. 45, 153154.
Schulz, D. J., Barron, A. B. and Robinson, G. E. (2002). A role for octopamine in honey
bee division of labor. Brain Behav. Evol. 60, 350359.
Seeley, T. D. and Kolmes, S. A. (1991). Age polyethisms for hive duties in honey bees
illusion or reality. Ethology 87, 284297.
Seeley, T. D. and Tarpy, D. R. (2007). Queen promiscuity lowers disease within
honeybee colonies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 274, 6772.
Smith, C. R., Toth, A. L., Suarez, A. V. and Robinson, G. E. (2008). Genetic and genomic
analyses of the division of labour in insect societies. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 735748.
Solignac, M., Mougel, F., Vautrin, D., Monnerot, M. and Cornuet, J.-M. (2007). A third
generation microsatellite-based linkage map of the honey bee, Apis mellifera, and its
comparison with the sequence-based physical map. Genome Biol. 8, R66.
Soller, M. and Bar-Cohen, R. (1967). Some observations on the heritability and genetic
correlation between honey production and brood area in the honey bee citrus-D.
J. Apic. Res. 6, 3743.
Spivak, M. (1996). Honey bee hygienic behavior and defence against Varroa jacobsoni.
Apidologie 27, 245260.
Spivak, M. and Downey, D. L. (1998). Field assays for hygienic behavior in honey bees
(Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 91, 6470.
Spivak, M. and Gilliam, M. (1998). Hygienic behaviour of honey bees and its application
for control of brood diseases and varroa Part II. Studies on hygienic behaviour since
the Rothenbuhler era. Bee World 79, 169186.
Spivak, M. and Reuter, G. S. (1998). Performance of hygienic honey bee colonies in a
commercial apiary. Apidologie 29, 291302.
Spivak, M. and Reuter, G. S. (2001a). Resistance to American foulbrood disease by honey
bee colonies Apis mellifera bred for hygienic behavior. Apidologie 32, 555565.
Spivak, M. and Reuter, G. S. (2001b). Varroa destructor infestation in untreated honey
bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) colonies selected for hygienic behavior. J. Econ.
Entomol. 94, 326331.
Sullivan, J. P., Jassim, O., Fahrbach, S. E. and Robinson, G. E. (2000). Juvenile hormone
paces behavioral development in the adult worker honey bee. Horm. Behav. 37, 114.
Szabo, T. I. (1988). Progress report on selective breeding of honey bees for resistance to
parasitic mites. Am. Bee. J. 138, 464466.
Author's personal copy

118 PETER R. OXLEY AND BENJAMIN P. OLDROYD

Tarpy, D. R. and Page, R. E. (2001). The curious promiscuity of queen honey bees (Apis
mellifera): evolutionary and behavioral mechanisms. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 38, 255265.
Togashi, K. and Lin, C. Y. (2009). Theoretical efficiency of multiple-trait quantitative
trait loci-assisted selection. J. Anim. Breed. Genet. 127, 5363.
Toth, A. L. and Robinson, G. E. (2007). Evo-devo and the evolution of social behavior.
Trends Genet. 23, 334341.
van Engelsdorp, D. and Otis, G. W. (2000). Application of a modified selection index for
honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 93, 16061612.
Vesely, V. and Siler, R. (1963). Possibilities of the application of quantitative and
population genetics in bee breeding. Proc. Int. Apic. Congr. 19, 120121.
Watson, L. R. (1927). Demonstration of instrumental insemination of the queenbee.
J. Econ. Entomol. 20, 530536.
Weatherhead, T. (1986). Boxes to Bar Hives. International Colour Productions,
Stanthorpe.
Weinstock, G. M., Robinson, G. E., Gibbs, R. A., Worley, K. C., Evans, J. D.,
Maleszka, R., Robertson, H. M. Weaver, D. B., et al. (2006). Insights into social
insects from the genome of the honeybee Apis mellifera. Nature 443, 931949.
Weller, J. I. and Soller, M. (2004). An analytical formula to estimate confidence interval
of QTL location with a saturated genetic map as a function of experimental design.
Theor. Appl. Genet. 109, 12241229.
Westell, R. A., Quaas, R. L. and Van Vleck, L. D. (1988). Genetic groups in an animal
model. J. Dairy Sci. 71, 13101318.
Whitfield, C. W., Band, M. R., Bonaldo, M. F., Kumar, C. G., Liu, L., Pardinas, J. R.,
Robertson, H. M., Soares, M. B. and Robinson, G. E. (2002). Annotated expressed
sequence tags and cDNA microarrays for studies of brain and behavior in the honey
bee. Genome Res. 12, 555566.
Whitfield, C. W., Behura, S. K., Berlocher, S. H., Clark, A. G., Johnston, J. S.,
Sheppard, W. S., Smith, D. R., Suarez, A. V., Weaver, D. and Tsutsui, N. D.
(2006). Thrice out of Africa: ancient and recent expansions of the honey bee, Apis
mellifera. Science 314, 642645.
Wilson-Rich, N., Spivak, M., Fefferman, N. H. and Starks, P. T. (2009). Genetic,
individual, and group facilitation of disease resistance in insect societies. Annu.
Rev. Entomol. 54, 405423.
Winston, M. L. (1987). The Biology of the Honey Bee. Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, MA.
Woyke, J. (1963). What happens to diploid drone larvae in a honeybee colony? J. Apic.
Res. 2, 7375.
Woyke, J. (1980). Effect of sex allele homo-heterozygosity on honeybee colony popula-
tions and on their honey production. J. Apic. Res. 19, 5163.
Wright, S. (1933). Inbreeding and homo-zygosis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 19,
411420.
Wright, S. (1977). Evolution and the genetics of populations. Experimental Results and
Evolutionary Deductions Vol. 3. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Yokoyama, S. and Nei, M. (1979). Population dynamics of sex determining alleles in
honey bees and self incompatibility alleles in plants. Genetics 91, 609626.
Zasloff, M. (1992). Antibiotic peptides as mediators of innate immunity. Curr. Opin.
Immunol. 4, 37.

View publication stats

S-ar putea să vă placă și