Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

Journal of Services Marketing

Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees


David A. Gilliam Steven W. Rayburn
Article information:
To cite this document:
David A. Gilliam Steven W. Rayburn , (2016),"Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees", Journal of Services
Marketing, Vol. 30 Iss 3 pp. 290 - 301
Permanent link to this document:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JSM-05-2015-0194
Downloaded on: 22 May 2016, At: 19:21 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 80 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 55 times since 2016*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

(2016),"Does a retailers charisma matter? A study of frontline employee perceptions of charisma in the retail setting", Journal of
Services Marketing, Vol. 30 Iss 3 pp. 266-276 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JSM-05-2015-0160
(2016),"Multiple paths to customer delight: the impact of effort, expertise and tangibles on joy and surprise", Journal of Services
Marketing, Vol. 30 Iss 3 pp. 277-289 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JSM-05-2015-0172
(2016),"The effect of workplace incivility on service employee creativity: the mediating role of emotional exhaustion and intrinsic
motivation", Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 30 Iss 3 pp. 302-315 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JSM-10-2014-0342

Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-srm:573577 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald for Authors service
information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission guidelines are available for all. Please visit
www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company manages a portfolio of
more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as well as providing an extensive range of online
products and additional customer resources and services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the Committee on Publication Ethics
(COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for digital archive preservation.

*Related content and download information correct at time of download.


Propensity for reciprocity among
frontline employees
David A. Gilliam
Department of Marketing, University of Arkansas at Little Rock, Little Rock, Arkansas, USA, and
Steven W. Rayburn
Department of Marketing, Texas State University, San Marcos, Texas, USA

Abstract
Purpose This paper aims to examine how other-regarding personality traits relate to reciprocity among frontline employees (FLEs).
Design/methodology/approach Other-regarding personality variables were used to model the propensity for reciprocity and actual reciprocal
behaviors with coworkers. Surveys of 276 FLEs were examined via structural equations modeling.
Findings Other-regarding personality traits proved to be antecedents of reciprocity. Cynicism was particularly interesting in that it was positively
related to reciprocity contrary to findings in other research.
Research limitations/implications Among the interesting findings relating personality to reciprocity are a more affective type of reciprocity
based on empathy and altruism, and a more calculative type based on cynicism related to Machiavellianism.
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

Practical implications Managers can use the effects of personality traits on reciprocity and cooperation to hire and place FLEs in ways that
provide superior service and increased profits.
Social implications This paper indicates that certain individuals who might not typically be thought of as cooperative can in fact reciprocate.
Specific ideas about cynicism and Machiavellian reciprocity in FLEs are discussed.
Originality/value The findings will aid researchers and managers in understanding personality and FLEs cooperation. The findings on cynicism
are particularly valuable in that they contradict some earlier research and commonly held managerial ideas.
Keywords Cooperation, Personality, Machiavellianism, Reciprocity, FLE
Paper type Research paper

Introduction receiving or anticipation of the future return of a benefit.


Reciprocity is an important element of cooperation, which is
Reciprocity is so integral to human society that it facilitates the
working together for a common purpose. Most research
study of social dilemmas by anthropologists, biologists,
political scientists, economists, sociologists and psychologists involving reciprocity focuses on specific reciprocal behaviors
(Brandsttter and Knigstein, 2001; Mauss, 1966; Ostrom or the consequences of reciprocity rather than on the causes of
and Walker, 2005). Some of the heavily researched areas in reciprocity itself (Perugini et al., 2003). Marketers in general
marketing, such as relationship marketing (Gruen et al., 2000; and service providers especially could benefit from a clearer
Morgan and Hunt, 1994; Wetzel et al., 2014), brands (Desai framework of the desire to reciprocate and the ensuing
and Keller, 2002), services (Boukis and Gounaris, 2014; cooperation with coworkers.
Gounaris and Boukis, 2013), and innovation and research This research examines reciprocity and cooperation among
(OBrien and David, 2014), also rely on reciprocity to explain FLEs. FLEs are critical to service provision and cooperation
cooperative relations between actors. Cooperation by frontline by coworkers facilitates their work (Barnes et al., 2014; Melton
employees (FLEs) is also recognized as a key element of and Hartline, 2015; Menguc and Boichuk, 2012; Yoo, 2013).
service delivery (Arndt et al., 2011). The causes of reciprocity Though the social norm of reciprocity is well known, there is
and cooperation by FLEs, however, are not well understood. evidence that people vary in their individual propensity to
Since Gouldners (1960) seminal work on the norm of engage in reciprocity, and FLEs are no exception (Perugini
reciprocity, hundreds of authors have cited reciprocity as the et al., 2003). Other-regarding individual difference variables
fount of manifold human behaviors, but fewer have inquired such as empathy and cynicism should affect the level of this
into the root causes of participation in reciprocity. Reciprocity propensity. Cooperation with coworkers is necessary to enact
is defined here as giving a benefit to another in response to the customer service experience and those FLEs high in
propensity for reciprocity should cooperate more.

The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on
Emerald Insight at: www.emeraldinsight.com/0887-6045.htm
The authors would like to thank John C. Mowen for his kind assistance
with the early conceptual draft of this paper. We also thank the associate
editor and three anonymous reviewers who contributed to improving the
paper with their insightful comments.
Journal of Services Marketing
30/3 (2016) 290 301 Received 28 May 2015
Emerald Group Publishing Limited [ISSN 0887-6045] Revised 18 September 2015
[DOI 10.1108/JSM-05-2015-0194] Accepted 31 October 2015

290
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

A model is proposed relating different levels of Reciprocity


other-regarding individual difference variables to the
It is not surprising therefore that one of the least controversial hypotheses in
propensity to have reciprocal relationships. Upon examination social science is that human beings have a general tendency to reciprocate
of the model via structural equation modeling, FLEs showed (Perugini et al., 2003, p. 251).
a variation in their propensity to participate in reciprocity. Reciprocity is defined here as giving a benefit to another in
Other-regarding variables had interesting effects as drivers of response to receiving or anticipation of the future return of a benefit.
the propensity for reciprocity, including one described herein The benefits returned need not be in-kind and may come after
as Machiavellian reciprocity. Higher cooperation with a considerable delay. This imbues reciprocity with some
coworkers was shown to be an outcome of the propensity for uncertainty and ambiguity that can lead to opportunistic
reciprocity; this gives researchers and managers a clearer behavior. There is also negative reciprocity or retaliation in
picture of how personality affects cooperation by FLEs. which harm is repaid with harm. Gouldner (1960) illustrates
Researchers interested in reciprocal outcomes at work and the norm of reciprocity as a sociological and psychodynamic
explaining service, sales, retailing and other marketing phenomenon which evolved in human prehistory and
phenomena will thus benefit. Managers will benefit from an continues to serve important functions in social interaction.
increased ability to match FLEs with the amount of Many of the surviving writings from antiquity show activities
cooperation required in various positions. The ability to more recognizable as reciprocity (Cohen and Bradford, 2005). In
effectively facilitate reciprocity and thus cooperation by FLEs his study of archaic societies, Mauss (1966) highlighted the
should have a positive effect on service quality and firm profits social importance of gift exchange and the obligations thus
(Bosse et al., 2009). incurred by the participants.
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

The paper is organized as follows. First, a general discussion The co-evolution of the genetic predisposition to be
of the literature on reciprocity, its nature and peculiarities as a cooperative and of cultural norms of reciprocity could have
form of cooperative behavior, and its potential antecedents provided a powerful tool for group success. The
informs the selection of appropriate theoretical underpinnings environmental forces that shaped this co-evolution included
for the model. Then a set of hypotheses are proposed and an the need to form non-fratricidal sharing rules, the improved
empirical study expounded followed by a general discussion. efficiency of hunting and gathering in cooperative groups, and
the greater ability to take and hold prime territory from rival
groups of humans by working in concert (Gintis, 2005). Thus,
Cooperation by frontline employees individuals and groups that practiced reciprocity would be
Cooperation by FLEs with both other FLEs and more likely to survive and pass on their genes. Similar
cross-functional coworkers will strongly impact a service firms communal and competitive forces drive employee interactions
performance. Teng et al. (2012, p. 332) focused on in business today, which necessitates cooperative behavior
cooperation as a key factor in coordinating workers in a there as well.
health-care setting saying, collaboration among personnel is There is evidence that this evolutionary path has made
critical. The potentially disastrous consequences of low reciprocation an almost instinctive response in humans (Park
cooperation among coworkers in health services are all too and Antonioni, 2007). This extends to negative actions such
clear. Lack of cooperation may be less life threatening in other as the instinctive reciprocation of threats during conflict (Brett
settings but no less harmful to quality of service, customer et al., 1998). But these responses are also predicated on early
loyalty and financial outcomes. It is hard to imagine a service learning in our specific culture as well, for while we may be
environment in which cooperation does not play a role. born with an innate ability for cooperative behavior responses,
In fact, the need for FLEs to cooperate extends to all facets these are specific to our time, place and culture (Hoffman
of service development and delivery. Knowing how to get et al., 1998). Thus, there appears to be a psychological trait of
FLEs to cooperate is essential for service managers because, propensity for reciprocity that has both genetic and early
it is important for them to integrate into a consistent, learning facets.
seamless customer interface (Arndt et al., 2011, p. 225). Economists have studied reciprocity extensively through
Failure to integrate coworkers can be extremely harmful to game theory experiments such as prisoners dilemma,
service quality (Yoo, 2013). Firms in which FLEs fail to ultimatum and gift exchange games (Dufwenberg and
cooperate will thus experience lower customer satisfaction and Kirchsteiger, 2004). Homo Economicus would be expected to
profits. behave in a selfish, utility maximizing manner, but the
Reciprocity forms the core of marketing thought on discovery that a large percentage of players prefer cooperation
cooperation whether it is between actors or entities. to selfishness undermines a significant portion of neo-classical
Cropanzano and Mitchell (2005) indicate that reciprocity is in economic theory and has given birth to a new paradigm, Homo
fact the foundational social norm used in social exchange Reciprocans (Fehr and Gachter, 1998). The experiments show
theory (SET), one of the most widely used theories in that individuals differ in their propensity to begin reciprocity,
relationship research. Wetzel et al. (2014, p. 2) used SET and continue reciprocity and punish defectors (Falk and
reciprocity to explore how business-to-business customers Fischbacher, 2006). These empirical findings support the
show gratitude to suppliers, i.e. buying behaviors motivated notion that there is a unique propensity for reciprocity that is
by a felt obligation to reciprocate supplier-provided benefits. more profound in some individuals than others.
It is clear in the marketing literature that researchers link Economists couch reciprocity in terms of a desire for
reciprocity tightly to cooperation in all marketing rewarding kindness and punishing unkindness such that
relationships. players are interested not only in the outcomes but also in the

291
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

intensions of other players (Falk and Fischbacher, 2006). an unspecified future time (Fehr and Gachter, 1998).
Intentionality of delivering a benefit may in fact be a Reciprocity and cooperation may be based on several different
prerequisite for gratitude, a potentially significant aspect of desires, such as the wish to reward others who were kind to us
reciprocity (Tesser et al., 1968). Thus, it may be important for or a more calculative motive to engage in self-beneficial or
the player to attribute positive motives to others actions and self-aggrandizing social exchange. However, in the end, it will
believe in the sincere intentions of benefit bestowal for be based on an appraisal of others whether founded in
reciprocity to flourish (McCabe et al., 2003). This indicates extremes of teary-eyed emotions or coldly calculative
how important a workers view of their coworker is in manipulation.
determining whether cooperation will occur.
This may be one way of explaining why people will punish
defectors to the detriment of themselves in game theory Hypotheses
experiments as well as why people will display reciprocity even Because of the importance of evaluations of others by FLEs,
in one shot games, in the case of anonymity and when there is the hypotheses relate five other-regarding individual difference
no chance for another meeting with the same players (Perugini variables to the propensity for reciprocity, which is in turn
et al., 2003). It may be true that some people are concerned related to reciprocal outcomes with coworkers. Other-
about rewarding others who are viewed as being intrinsically regarding variables show evaluation of others when the FLE
nice and that this is one reason why other regarding behavior considers situations and proper responses. The two other
is so important for reciprocity (Segal and Sobel, 2007). regarding variables of the widely used Big-5 personality traits
Another interpretation of these findings holds that for some are included in the study: introversion and agreeableness. The
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

actors, it is self-advancement that drives reciprocity as they unused variables of the Big-5 represent either internal
attempt to manage a portfolio of reciprocal relationships to characteristics such as neuroticism or views as to ones proper
maximize their share, i.e. Homo Economicus is not dead yet nor relationship with the environment, namely, conscientiousness
is this strategy necessarily always suboptimal (Wilson et al., and openness to experience.
1996). In addition to introversion and agreeableness, three widely
The evaluation of reciprocal acts thus hinges on an appraisal used other-regarding variables were selected. Cynicism served
of the intentions of the other person to determine if reciprocity to represent a negative view of others cooperative nature.
is indeed the best response (Dufwenberg and Kirchsteiger, Next, altruism lent a positive view, wherein others are
2004; Falk and Fischbacher, 2006; McCabe et al., 2003). The regarded as potentially deserving of help. Finally, empathy was
interactions of self-regarding constructs, such as self-esteem chosen to represent the degree to which workers can see things
and self-aggrandizement, with our views of others intensions from the others perspective. See Figure 1 for the full model.
are complex, vary by individual and are tied to helping Extraverts are described as friendly, assertive and cheerful
behaviors (Rhodewalt and Morf, 1998). This helps to explain the opposite of introverts (Donnellan et al., 2006; Mechinda
how the self-aggrandizement goals of some lead to excessive and Patterson, 2011). Whereas extraverts are thought of as
estimates of their part in an exchange, while others with a hardworking, ambitious and eager to get ahead, introverts
modesty bias may underestimate their contribution struggle with self-confidence and achievement (Hogan and
(McGuire, 2003, p. 363). Thus, sensitivity to social feedback Holland, 2003). Extraversion is a thus a key predictor of
and the reaction thereto is linked to personality and will popularity (Paunonen, 2003). Consequently, extraverts have
impact the response people exhibit when faced with an act of more social relationships and receive more social support in
giving or helping. return (Berkman et al., 2000). Introversion will thus cause less
For reciprocity to flourish then, the intentions of the social engagement with coworkers, while extraversion will help
reciprocating actors must be congruent with an ongoing create a personality which willingly engages with others to
two-way exchange where present benefits will be returned at achieve success.

Figure 1 Hypothesized model

Introversion

Cynicism
+ Propensity for + Reciprocity with
Agreeableness Coworkers
Reciprocity

Altruism +

+
Empathy

292
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

Extraversion is linked to the way individuals appraise and seen as a precursor to the virtue of compassion (Ozer and
react to environmental rewards and punishments (Ozer and Benet-Martinez, 2006). Another virtue essential for helping
Benet-Martinez, 2006, p. 404). Ozer and Benet-Martinez behaviors, gratitude, is likewise empirically linked to
(2006) assert that this may cause extraverts to view the world agreeableness (McCullough et al., 2002). In general, research
positively, ignore negative feedback and disregard unavailable has strongly linked high agreeableness to prosocial and helping
opportunities. Extraversion is also linked to the virtue of behaviors (Caprara et al., 2012; Graziano et al., 2007).
gratitude, which aids in positive attribution about others Agreeableness is also correlated with forgiveness/
actions (McCullough et al., 2002). In fact, experiments using non-retaliation. Thompson et al. (2005) showed that
bargaining games have demonstrated that high extraversion agreeableness is an antecedent of forgiveness. Forgiveness is
can help produce reciprocal behaviors (Brandsttter and important to restarting reciprocity in game theory experiments
Knigstein, 2001). (such as the prisoners dilemma games) after one party
The strong links shown between extraversion and defects, as it forms one aspect of the highly successful tit for tat
helpfulness lead to more regular prosocial behavior and strategy (Ashton et al., 1980; Ostrom and Walker, 2005).
increased rates of volunteerism (Penner, 2002). Highly In fact, low agreeableness results in the exhibition of
introverted persons should thus find it more psychologically hostility toward others and can result in the inability to form
uncomfortable than extraverts do to engage in the and maintain successful relationships with both friends and
interpersonal exchanges that promote reciprocal relationships: family members (Ozer and Benet-Martinez, 2006). Persons
high in agreeableness should more freely provide help to
H1. Introversion will be negatively related to the propensity others to begin with and will prove more likely to continue to
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

for reciprocity. reciprocate even after a defection by their partners:


Cynicism refers to regarding others with deep suspicion as to H3. Agreeableness will be positively related to the
their motives. Persons high in cynicism presume that others propensity for reciprocity.
are motivated by greed and will take advantage of them if given
an opportunity (Andersson and Bateman, 1997). Inesi et al. Altruism was used as a trait in a study of volunteerism where
(2012) demonstrated that cynical attributions reduce it was found to be a significant predictor of volunteer
thankfulness for favors thus inhibiting reciprocity in the orientation (Mowen and Sujan, 2005). These authors defined
workplace. Persons high in cynicism may believe that others altruism as a general predisposition to selflessly seek to help
will not return the given benefit at a future time. Thus, they others. This distinguishes altruism from reciprocity in that
will be reluctant to enter and continue reciprocal acts of altruism do not attach an expectation of return of the
relationships. benefit.
Cynicism is a core aspect of Machiavellianism, which causes Altruism is thus said to occur if the utility of ego is increased
high Machs to manipulate and exploit others for personal by Alters success (Unger, 1991). Researchers debate the
gain (Christie et al., 1970; Daunt and Harris, 2012). exact nature of altruism in the psychology literature (Batson
Researchers have expected low Machs to reciprocate with and Powell, 2003; Maner et al., 2002). But whether motivated
greater reliability: by pure benevolence, promotion of the givers self-esteem, or
self-aggrandizement, altruism serves as a valuable tool in the
We do not expect high Machs to defect under all circumstances, but we do
expect them to perceive defection as a viable option, to be more skilled at the study of helping behaviors in the management literature. For
art of defection, and to use it in a greater range of social situations than low instance, in the study of work in teams by Li et al. (2014) and
Machs (Wilson et al., 1996, p. 290).
the examination of managerial concern for others by Haynes
Gunnthorsdottir et al. (2002, p. 49) explored how high and et al. (2015).
low Machs behave in game-theoretic experiments on trust and Thus, those who act based on altruism do so out of a need
reciprocity. Machiavellianism did not predict trust levels but, to help others and a concern for others well-being. Persons
It does, however, predict reciprocity. They found that high high in this trait do not need the prospect of immediate returns
Machs reciprocate less and defect more than low Machs. to engender helping behaviors and they will be more likely to
Propensity for reciprocity is a stable personality trait enter and maintain reciprocal relationships:
indicating how likely it is for the individual to engage in
reciprocity. Thus, while cynics could exhibit a high propensity H4. Altruism will be positively related to the propensity for
for reciprocity due to a desire for personal gain, researchers reciprocity.
have assumed this is overridden by viewing others as
Empathy as a personality disposition relates to perceptiveness,
opportunistic and unlikely to reciprocate if they can avoid
compassion and taking the perspective of others (Johnson,
doing so:
1990). Those high in empathy more easily see the world from
H2. Cynicism will be negatively related to the propensity for others point of view rather than just their own.
reciprocity. Empathy has been characterized as compassion in response
to the suffering of others (Barron, 1993). Not surprisingly
Agreeableness denotes the need to express kindness and then empathy is often related to helping behaviors (Kurzban
sympathy to others (Mowen, 2000). Agreeable people display et al., 2015; MacKay et al., 1990). In fact, studies of humans
sensitivity to others needs, desire popularity and concentrate and similar animals show empathy to be a phylogenetically
on getting along rather than getting ahead (Hogan and ancient capacity deeply linked to helping behaviors such that,
Holland, 2003). It is not surprising then that agreeableness is Empathy could well provide the main motivation making

293
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

individuals who have exchanged benefits in the past to response rate of 87 per cent. Of these, 276 were identified as
continue doing so in the future (de Waal, 2008, p. 263). FLEs, resulting in a net response rate of 61 per cent. FLEs
This exchange behavior can benefit both individuals and were limited to those who indicated substantial customer
may explain why humans are such social animals in the first contact in their daily work. Respondent average age was 34
place. In describing why this is so for empathetic individuals, years, average time spent working was 36-h per week and
Kurzban et al. (2015, p. 583) stated: average coworkers interacted with per week was 22. The
That is, those who are (temporarily) in some sort of distress are sources of
sample was approximately split between women and men
potential gains in trade because helping in their moment of need might (males 52.5 per cent).
produce reciprocal aid in the future.
Those exhibiting high empathy should tend to see others in a Measures
positive light which will make them willing to take risks in All the constructs were vetted for conceptualizations
helping others. Such workers will start and maintain reciprocal consistent with the literature and prevention of vagueness in
relationships at higher rates than those low in empathy: definition (Gilliam and Voss, 2013). All scales for the data
collection were adapted from previous research except for the
H5. Empathy will be positively related to the propensity for
reciprocity with coworkers scale which was developed for this
reciprocity.
study. All scales were measured on a nine-point basis, with
Perhaps more than any other researchers, economists have 1 strongly disagree and 9 strongly agree. Three items
lead the way in demonstrating that people reciprocate at were included in the scales to ensure brevity, as they were
likely to be completed in the workplace. For items and
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

varying rates. Dufwenberg and Kirchsteiger (2004,


p. 268-270) state that the intension to be kind to someone construct properties, see Table I.
who was kind sometimes replaces pure selfishness as a
motivation in exchange. Fehr and Gachters (1998, p. 846) Measurement analysis
games showed that, Such reciprocation occurs even in Item generation for the new reciprocity with coworkers
one-shot encounters among strangers and when it is costly for (RECOW) scale began after completing the literature review.
the responder, while pointing out that utility maximizing The primary researchers developed seven items reflective of
examples of Homo Economicus still persist in the population. thought within the reciprocity literature across the disciplines.
There exists among workers a variation in the desire to engage A review of these initial items by two other faculty and five
in reciprocity. doctoral students served for item reduction. Items deemed by
In conceptualizing the norm of reciprocity, Gouldner the judges to be double barreled, prone to misinterpretation,
(1960, p. 171) posited there exists a feeling that, people outside the core aspects of reciprocity, or otherwise unfit were
should help those who have helped them. The degree to dropped. The review caused the elimination of four items.
which individuals inculcate and act on this norm is governed An initial dataset collected without regard to worker type
by personality traits causing a significant variance in the served to validate the new RECOW scale (n 390). An
propensity for reciprocity among workers (Perugini et al., exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was run in SPSS 23. The
2003). Reciprocity with coworkers measures actual KaiserMeyerOlkin measure of sampling adequacy was in
engagement in reciprocal acts with other workers on the job. the meritorious range above 0.8 and Bartletts test of
The variation in desire for reciprocity impacts actual sphericity was significant with 2 value 5,697.4, df 231 and
reciprocal actions performed among coworkers. Those p 0.000, indicating that an EFA could be successfully used
exhibiting a high propensity for reciprocity will engage in more (Hair et al., 2010). The EFA scree plot was inconclusive and the
reciprocity with coworkers and produce more cooperative Kaiser 1 rule of using Eigen values above 1 yielded two fewer
outcomes at work: factors than expected by theory. The propensity for the Kaiser 1
rule to extract too few factors in studies with a modest number
H6. The propensity for reciprocity will be positively related of variables, a desire to extract maximum variance and strong
to reciprocity with coworkers. theoretical underpinnings indicated the extraction of seven
factors (Hair et al., 2010). The data were amenable to EFA via
principle axis factoring and promax rotation which resulted in
Methods
readily interpretability factors that explained 79.0 per cent of
Data collection and sample characteristics the variance (Brown, 2006; Costello and Osborne, 2005). All
To empirically examine the hypotheses data collection was loadings were well into the highly acceptable range above 0.5
performed in a small town in the south-central USA. The data and cross-loadings were well below 0.3 (Hair et al., 2010).
came from FLEs using a variant of the snowball sampling The three RECOW items loaded on the factor at 0.961, 0.992
method. The snowball sampling method is particularly and 0.675 with the largest cross-loading being 0.152 onto the
advantageous in identifying and accessing specific populations propensity for reciprocity factor. The results of the EFA thus
of interest (Biernacki and Waldorf, 1981). In this case, the indicated that the RECOW scale was appropriate for further
population was FLEs. examination.
Students at a local university acted as data collection agents Analysis of scale alphas, identification of scale composite
after being trained in key privacy and voluntary consent reliabilities and average variance extracted, and a test for
procedures (Gilliam et al., 2014). The students collected pen discriminant validity was conducted. No coefficient alphas or
and paper surveys from local service and retail workers. A total construct reliabilities were less than 0.75, thereby meeting or
of 450 surveys were distributed; 390 were returned for a gross exceeding recommended minimums (Bagozzi and Yi, 1988).

294
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

Table I CFA results


Constructs with items (source scales were adapted from) STD loading t-values CR AVE
Reciprocity with coworkers: In the past three months, . . . 0.93 0.82
I have exchanged favors with most of my coworker 0.90 18.93
I have engaged frequently in reciprocity with coworkers at work 0.98 22.04
I have often freely given and received help from coworkers 0.85 17.34
Propensity to reciprocity (Perugini et al., 2003, pp. 276-277) 0.90 0.75
I offer assistance when a coworker needs helps so I can get help when I need it 0.71 13.41
I feel good about helping a coworker who has helped me 0.97 21.40
I typically reciprocate when a coworker helps me 0.90 19.04
Introversion (Licata et al., 2003, pp. 269-270) 0.86 0.68
I am quiet when with other people 0.62 10.98
I dont like being the center of attention 0.93 18.16
I dont like to draw attention to myself 0.89 17.11
Cynicism (Wrightsman, 1974, p. 233) 0.84 0.64
Most people would cheat on their taxes if they had a chance 0.68 12.11
People pretend to care more about one another than they really do 0.85 15.81
People take advantage of unselfish individuals 0.84 15.62
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

Agreeableness (Licata et al., 2003, pp. 269-270) 0.94 0.83


I am tender hearted with others 0.90 19.18
I am interested in people 0.89 18.86
I sympathize with others feelings 0.94 20.64
Altruism (Mowen and Sujan, 2005, p. 182) 0.86 0.66
I am giving to other 0.78 14.81
I will sacrifice my goals to help others 0.78 14.73
I am unselfish in giving time to others 0.89 17.90
Empathy (Davis, 1980, pp. 10-11) 0.90 0.76
I try to imagine how things look from others perspective 0.78 15.14
I often have feelings of concern for people less fortunate than me 0.93 20.14
I feel sorry for people when they are having problems 0.89 18.49
Notes: CFA statistics: 2 486.62; df 168; p 0.00000; RMSEA 0.083; SRMR 0.0694; NFI 0.945; CFI 0.963

Discriminant validity was tested by comparing average Lance, 2010; Podsakoff et al., 2012). Confirmatory factor
variance extracted to the squared correlations of all pairs of analysis (CFA) was conducted using LISREL 9.10 to verify
constructs (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). Smallest AVE is 0.64 measurement properties and to establish acceptable
which is larger than the largest squared correlation, 0.429, measurement statistics; the measurement model provided
indicating discriminant validity. Multicollinearity was assessed acceptable fit to the data[1] (2 486.62; df 168; p
through variance inflation factors. All are between 1.02 and 0.000; RMSEA 0.083, SRMR 0.0694; NFI 0.945;
2.15 indicating multicollinearity was not an issue. Common CFI 0.963). For complete results of the CFA, see Table II.
method bias was controlled for by: considering the theoretical
relevance of self-report data collection for personality Analytical approach
measures; reducing item overlap between constructs; using Structural equation modeling (SEM) with LISREL 9.10 was
well-tested measures; and ensuring respondent anonymity used to test the hypothesized paths from selected individual
while minimizing other measurement stressors (Conway and difference variables through propensity to reciprocate to

Table II Scale statistics


Scale statistics Correlations
Alpha Mean SD CR AVE RECOW PROPEN INTRO CYNIC AGREE ALTRU EMPAT
RECOW 0.91 6.2 1.94 0.93 0.82
PROPEN 0.86 6.86 1.56 0.9 0.75 0.622
INTRO 0.83 5.09 1.95 0.86 0.68 0.092 0.121
CYNIC 0.82 5.73 1.88 0.84 0.64 0.095 0.032 0.14
AGREE 0.93 7.04 1.94 0.94 0.83 0.316 0.448 0.068 0.13
ALTRU 0.84 6.35 1.57 0.86 0.66 0.363 0.455 0.043 0.092 0.619
EMPAT 0.88 6.78 1.61 0.9 0.76 0.283 0.5 0.046 0.156 0.642 0.655

Notes: Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed); Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed)

295
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

reciprocating with coworkers. The data exhibited some slight Table III Results for structural equation analyses
departures from normal, to accommodate a maximum Structural model statistics Results
likelihood estimator was used. This generates less biased
estimates of fit and standard error than other methods in the 2
499.53
presence of non-normality. For the structural model with df 173
standardized loadings, see Figure 2. The model exhibits SRMR 0.074
acceptable fit (2 499.53; df 173; p 0.00; RMSEA RMSEA 0.083
0.083; SRMR 0.0736; NFI 0.944; CFI 0.962) Normed fit index 0.944
explaining approximately 39 per cent of the variance in Comparative fit index 0.962
reciprocity with coworkers.
Standard path
Coefficient
Results Structural paths (SE) t-values
H1H5 dealt with anticipated relationships between Introversion Propensity to reciprocate 0.12 2.22
other-regarding individual difference variables and FLEs Cynicism Propensity to reciprocate 0.11 2.05
propensity for reciprocity and H6 related propensity for Agreeableness Propensity to
reciprocity to reciprocity with coworkers. Results of reciprocate 0.05 0.56ns
hypothesis testing using SEM can be viewed graphically in Altruism Propensity to reciprocate 0.30 2.97
Figure 2 and numerically in Table III. Empathy Propensity to reciprocate 0.37 3.94
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

H1 predicted that introversion would have a negative


Propensity to reciprocate Reciprocity
relationship with propensity for reciprocity and was significant w/co-workers 0.62 9.52
( 0.12; p 0.05). Introverts thus lack the friendly and
Notes: p 0.01; p 0.05; ns non-significant; n 276
assertive manner that fosters social interaction. The
psychological discomfort introverts experience during
interpersonal engagement apparently thwarts efforts to H3 related agreeableness to propensity for reciprocity.
establish and maintain reciprocal relationships Though the coefficient was positive as expected it was
H2 predicted cynicism to have a similar negative nonsignificant ( 0.05; p 0.10). In conjunction with the
relationship to propensity for reciprocity, and while significant findings in H2 about cynicism, this further illuminates the
( 0.11; p 0.05), the direction of the effect is opposite that calculative dimensions of reciprocity. We conceptualized
predicted. Prior research predicted the negative relationship. reciprocity as having two sides, a positive side for returning
A priori we expected the existence of what might be called good benefits and a negative side to retaliate for bad actions by
Machiavellian reciprocity; that is a manipulative form of others. A picture of positive reciprocity is emerging in the
reciprocity based purely on self-interest. It seems to have empirical findings that also have two sides, one for emotional
actually overwhelmed any misgivings cynics have about the or affective motivations and one for more calculative
likelihood of opportunistic behaviors by others. This intentions.
counterintuitive finding sheds considerable light on selecting H4 predicted a positive relationship between altruism and
employees who must cooperate with coworkers: it need not be propensity for reciprocity and was significant ( 0.30; p
based entirely on just being nice or for that matter agreeable, 0.01). Altruism would represent the affective elements of
as the next hypothesis test showed. reciprocity, wherein others are more likely to appear

Figure 2 Empirical model

*
Introversion 0.12

*
Cynicism 0.11

ns **
0.05 Propensity for 0.62 Reciprocity with
Agreeableness Coworkers
Reciprocity

**
Altruism 30

**
Empathy 0.37

Notes: SEM statistics: 2 = 499.53; df = 173; p = 0.00000; RMSEA = 0.083;


SRMR = 0.074; NFI = 0.944; CFI = 0.962; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01

296
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

deserving. Altruism once again appears as a fount of helping (Gustafsson et al., 2005; Rayburn, 2014). Affective-based
behaviors in the case of reciprocity. exchange may be regarded as a strategy which emphasizes fair
H5 predicted empathy to be positively related to propensity division. Calculative exchange is perhaps more guarded,
for reciprocity and was significant ( 0.37; p 0.01). wherein ensuring ones own share takes precedence over fair
Another of the emotional or affective traits driving reciprocity, division. Altruism and empathy fuel affectively driven
empathy allows perspective taking from the others point of reciprocity, while cynicism is the hallmark of the more
view. This may soften the inevitable bumps in the road that calculative variety.
occur in long-term reciprocal relationships. Those high in Gustafsson et al. (2005) point to affective commitment as
empathy appear more likely to give others the benefit of the the more desirable of the two believing it to be more durable.
doubt during exchanges. This may not be the case with reciprocity. Affective-based
Finally, H6 predicted that the propensity for reciprocity reciprocity has an attractively warm and fuzzy feel to it, but it
would lead to action, specifically to reciprocity with coworkers can also have its downsides. When cooperative behaviors are
and was also significant ( 0.62; p 0.01). The variation in undertaken largely based on an evaluation of coworkers
individuals desire to engage in reciprocity leads to differing needs, business imperatives may receive less consideration
amounts of actual reciprocity with coworkers. than personal desires. The less emotional judgments of
In the overall inquiry, all but one hypothesized relationship calculative reciprocity may well steer cooperation in more
was significant. However, one was opposite the predicted
productive directions. Seeing Machiavellianism as one
effect, cynicism, though it nonetheless has an interesting and
strategy for adapting to the environment may reveal
useful interpretation. It appears that other-regarding
Machiavellian reciprocity as merely a useful form of realpolitik
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

individual difference variables do predict the propensity for


in the workplace (Wilson et al., 1996). At any rate, the
reciprocity by FLEs and this leads further to predicting
presence of some FLEs with higher levels of cynicism in the
cooperative behaviors among coworkers.
workforce will not likely lower cooperation; it may also bring
more balanced decision-making to what would otherwise be a
Discussion
process dominated by affect alone.
This study showed a significant difference in individuals In practice then the cynic does not necessarily try to harm
propensity for reciprocity which lead to expressions of actual coworkers during cooperation, they are likely just cautious and
cooperation with coworkers. Other-regarding individual share maximizing. The fact that agreeableness was not a
difference variables predicted this propensity in interesting significant contributor bolsters the notion that reciprocity and
ways. One of the Big-5 personality variables, introversion, was cooperation can be a very utilitarian and functional behaviors
shown to have a negative relationship to reciprocity while the for many FLEs. They need not be based on an eagerness to get
other, agreeableness was positively signed but not significant. along to be successful and fruitful for both parties. Perhaps we
The two variables in the study which tend to drive a positive must recognize that all exchanges have facets that are at once
view of others, altruism and empathy, both showed strong cooperative and exploitative and that no exchanges are
positive relationships to reciprocity. The final variable, completely devoid of manipulation (Patterson and Baron,
cynicism, embodies a deep suspicion of others and fosters 2010; Wilson et al., 1998). Leaning toward a functional, less
Machiavellian behavior but this may still be expressed as emotional approach will not necessarily stifle cooperation, and
cooperation. Contrary to expectations, cynicism had a positive there apparently exists a variety of behavioral strategies for
relationship to reciprocity. The empirical findings thus present
successful reciprocal relationships. Thus, there are also a
two sides to positive reciprocity and the resulting cooperation
variety of personality types that can engage successfully in
that are further discussed under implications.
reciprocity and cooperation to varying degrees.
The inquiry focuses on positive reciprocity by FLEs, but has
broader implications for marketing. In the opening paragraph,
we mentioned many other areas to which marketing Managerial implications
researchers have already applied reciprocity as an explanatory Managers of FLEs in services may wish to hire, train, place
construct. The power of reciprocity in sales, knowledge and especially to select for teams based on an individuals
sharing, cross-functional collaboration, product development, propensity for reciprocity with coworkers. Treating all
relationship marketing and brand alliances is clear and worthy employees as though they have an equal propensity for
of the managers consideration. When managers take the reciprocity likely leads to improperly matching persons to
personality of the participants and their varying propensity for positions. Knowing an individual FLEs propensity for
reciprocity into account, the coworkers involved may reciprocity may be the managers most valuable tool for
participate more in reciprocity and exhibit greater creating a mix of workers who will be highly cooperative
cooperation. This should in turn affect service quality and together. The manager may view their workforce as a portfolio
profits. of individuals with varying capabilities to cooperate. Many
firms use personality testing as a tool to develop this portfolio;
Theoretical implications our new measure of propensity for reciprocity can easily be
Overall this indicates reciprocity may have two basic driving included in these tests to benefit managers, as they attempt to
forces, one affective and emotional, while the other is more place employees appropriately into specific work roles.
calculative having its roots in a desire to improve ones own Properly aligning workers with specific duties and coworkers
situation. Of course, there are similar dichotomies noted in the based on the expected level of reciprocity can help maximize
literature such as affective and calculative commitment cooperative outcomes and the value of the portfolio of FLEs.

297
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

The empirical results give direct guidance in determining when dealing with model fit. They propose RMSEA
the propensity for reciprocity. Persons high in altruism and between 0.03 and 0.08 and immediately state that
empathy and low in introversion should possess a higher absolute cutoffs are unwise. They state SRMR above 1.0
propensity for reciprocity thus producing greater amounts of is bad fit suggesting anything below is acceptable and
cooperation with coworkers. These individuals should further below is better. They offer that NFI 1.0 is
produce reciprocity and cooperation with an affective nature. perfect fit, closer is better, and CFI above 0.9 is
A possible constructive feature of such cooperation is its ease acceptable. We have taken this information to suggest that
of maintenance and thus longevity. It may in fact represent the our measurement and structural models exhibits
kind of touchy-feely behavior that the term reciprocity acceptable fit based on aggregation of the fit statistics.
typically summons in our minds. It is not, however, requisite
for cooperation and could possibly lead to a preponderance of
cooperation based on coworkers needs and desires as much as References
on business imperatives.
Andersson, L.M. and Bateman, T.S. (1997), Cynicism in the
The more calculative reciprocity of cynics or even high
workplace: some causes and effects, Journal of
Machs may prove just as valuable. It may introduce a clearer
Organizational Behavior, Vol. 18 No. 5, pp. 449-469.
eyed perspective that leans toward maximizing outcomes.
Arndt, A.D., Karande, K. and Landry, T.D. (2011), An
Thus, cynicism may not forestall cooperation, but care should
examination of frontline cross- functional integration during
be taken till more is known about the overall effects of this
retail transactions, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 87 No. 2,
personality trait in FLEs. It may well prove to be the case that
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

pp. 225-241.
a variety of personality types can prove productive in meeting
Ashton, M.C., Paunonen, S.V., Helmes, E. and Jackson,
different challenges in the workplace.
D.N. (1980), Kin altruism, reciprocal altruism, and the big
As an enduring individual difference variable, propensity for
five personality factors, Evolution and Human Behavior,
reciprocity should pay dividends throughout the employees
Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 243-255.
tenure. Failing to consider it before hiring and placing an
Bagozzi, R.P. and Yi, Y. (1988), On the evaluation of
individual will probably lead to suboptimal cooperation with
structural equation models, Journal of the Academy of
other FLEs and perhaps cross-functional coworkers, not to
Marketing Science, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 74-94.
mention the possibility for reduced cooperative relationships
Barnes, D.C., Collier, J.E. and Robinson, S. (2014),
with customers and suppliers.
Customer delight and work engagement, Journal of
Services Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 5, pp. 380-390.
Limitations and future research
Barron, R.A. (1993), Affect and organizational behavior:
As with any research, this effort had limitations. This research
when and why feeling good (or bad) matters, in
represents a beginning of exploration in the area rather than a
Murnighan, J.K. (Ed.), Social Psychology in Organizations:
conclusion; that is to say future research should address the
Advances in Theory and Research, Prentice Hall, Englewood
current limitations. There is as usual a need to replicate the
Cliffs, NJ.
findings and extend them to other settings. Methodological
Batson, C.D. and Powell, A.A. (2003), Altruism and
triangulation must be pursued, for instance via creating ways
prosocial behavior, in Millon, T and Lerner, M.J. (Eds),
to replicate the work by measures other than self-reports. It
Handbook of Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology,
should be noted that this inquiry is itself an effort at
John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, Vol. 5, pp. 463-484.
triangulation with most work on reciprocity occurring in a
Berkman, L.F., Glass, T., Brissette, I. and Seeman, T.E.
laboratory. We respond to the need for studies, outside the
(2000), From social integration to health: durkheim in the
laboratory in real-life situations (Wilson et al., 1996).
new millennium, Social Science & Medicine, Vol. 51 No. 6,
Future efforts should further address all the variables in the
pp. 843-857.
study, especially cynicism. Researchers need to delimit the
Biernacki, P. and Waldorf, D. (1981), Snowball sampling:
boundaries of the unexpected positive relationship of cynicism
problems and techniques of chain referral sampling,
to reciprocity. Exploring other positive and the negative
Sociological Methods & Research, Vol. 10 No. 2, pp. 141-163.
aspects of cynicism in FLEs should prove useful as well.
Bosse, D.A., Phillips, R.A. and Harrison, J.S. (2009),
Further, investigating negative reciprocity or retaliation may
Stakeholders, reciprocity, and firm performance, Strategic
be just as important to management of FLEs as the present
Management Journal, Vol. 30 No. 4, pp. 447-456.
focus on positive cooperative behaviors. The effect of
Boukis, A. and Gounaris, S. (2014), Linking IMO with
perceived intentions of coworkers on reciprocity is a natural
employees fit with their environment and reciprocal
extension of this work and an area that could potentially be
behaviours towards the firm, Journal of Services Marketing,
managerially manipulated, perhaps via the enhancement of
Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 10-21.
channels of communication. Finally, research into the
Brandsttter, H. and Knigstein, M. (2001), Personality
interaction effects of mixing affective and calculative
influences on ultimatum bargaining decisions, European
reciprocators in the workforce could be a challenging yet
Journal of Personality, Vol. 15 No. 1, pp. 53-70.
rewarding effort.
Brett, J.M., Shapiro, D.L. and Lytle, A.L. (1998), Breaking
the bonds of reciprocity in negotiations, Academy of
Note Management Journal, Vol. 41 No. 4, pp. 410-424.
1 Hair et al. (2010) offers recommendations of some rules of Brown, T.A. (2006), Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied
thumb for fit, but admonishes that there is no absolute Research, Guilford Press, New York, NY.

298
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

Caprara, G.V., Alessandri, G. and Eisenberg, N. (2012), Gouldner, A.W. (1960), The norm of reciprocity: a
Prosociality: the contribution of traits, values, and preliminary statement, American Sociological Review,
self-efficacy beliefs, Journal of Personality and Social Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 161-178.
Psychology, Vol. 102 No. 6, pp. 1289-1303. Gounaris, S. and Boukis, A. (2013), The role of employee
Christie, R., Geis, F.L. and Berger, D. (1970), Studies in job satisfaction in strengthening customer repurchase
Machiavellianism, Academic Press, New York, NY. intentions, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 27 No. 4,
Cohen, A.R. and Bradford, D.L. (2005), The influence pp. 322-333.
model: using reciprocity and exchange to get what you Graziano, W.G., Habashi, M.M., Sheese, B.E. and Tobin,
need, Journal of Organizational Excellence, Vol. 25 No. 1, R.M. (2007), Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: a
pp. 57-80. person situation perspective, Journal of Personality and
Conway, J.M. and Lance, C.E. (2010), What reviewers Social Psychology, Vol. 93 No. 4, pp. 583-599.
should expect from authors regarding common method bias Gruen, T.W., Summers, J.O. and Acito, F. (2000),
in organizational research, Journal of Business and Relationship marketing activities, commitment, and
Psychology, Vol. 25 No. 3, pp. 325-334. membership behaviors in professional associations, Journal
Costello, A.B. and Osborne, J.W. (2005), Best practices in of Marketing, Vol. 64 No. 3, pp. 34-49.
exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for Gunnthorsdottir, A., McCabe, K. and Smith, V.L. (2002),
getting the most from your analysis, Practical Assessment, Using the Machiavellianism instrument to predict
Research and Evaluation, Vol. 10 No. 7, pp. 1-9. trustworthiness in a bargaining game, Journal of Economic
Cropanzano, R. and Mitchell, M.S. (2005), Social exchange Psychology Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 49-66.
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

theory: an interdisciplinary review, Journal of Management, Gustafsson, A., Johnson, M.D. and Roos, I. (2005), The
Vol. 31 No. 6, pp. 874-900. effects of customer satisfaction, relationship commitment
Daunt, K.L. and Harris, L.C. (2012), Motives of dimensions, and triggers on customer retention, Journal of
dysfunctional customer behavior: an empirical study, Marketing, Vol. 69 No. 4, pp. 210-218.
Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 26 No. 4, pp. 293-308. Hair, J.F., Black, W.C., Babin, B.J. and Anderson, R.E.
Davis, M.H. (1980), A multidimensional approach to
(2010), Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed., Prentice Hall,
individual differences in empathy, JSAS Catalog of Selected
Upper Saddle River, NJ.
Documents in Psychology, Vol. 10, p. 85.
Haynes, K.T., Josefy, M. and Hitt, M.A. (2015), Tipping
De Waal, F.B.M. (2008), Putting the altruism back into
point: managers self-interest, greed, and altruism, Journal
altruism: the evolution of empathy, Annual Review of
of Leadership & Organizational Studies, Vol. 22 No. 3,
Psychology, Vol. 59, pp. 279-300.
pp. 265-279.
Desai, K.K. and Keller, K.L. (2002), The effects of
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K.A. and Smith, V.L. (1998),
ingredient branding strategies on host brand extendibility,
Behavioral foundations of reciprocity: experimental
Journal of Marketing, Vol. 66 No. 1, pp. 73-93.
economics and evolutionary psychology, Economic Inquiry,
Donnellan, M.B., Oswald, F.L., Baird, B.M. and Lucas, R.E.
Vol. 36 No. 3, pp. 335-352.
(2006), The mini-ipip scales: tiny-yet-effective measures
Hogan, J. and Holland, B. (2003), Using theory to evaluate
of the big five factors of personality, Psychological
Assessment, Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 192-203. personality and job-performance relations: a socioanalytic
Dufwenberg, M. and Kirchsteiger, G. (2004), A theory of perspective, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 88 No. 1,
sequential reciprocity, Games and Economic Behavior, pp. 100-112.
Vol. 47 No. 2, pp. 268-298. Inesi, M.E., Gruenfeld, D.H. and Galinsky, A.D. (2012),
Falk, A. and Fischbacher, U. (2006), A theory of How power corrupts relationships: cynical attributions for
reciprocity, Games and Economic Behavior, Vol. 54 No. 2, others generous acts, Journal of Experimental Social
pp. 293-315. Psychology, Vol. 48 No. 4, pp. 795-803.
Fehr, E. and Gachter, S. (1998), Reciprocity and economics: Johnson, J.A. (1990), Empathy as a personality disposition,
the economic implications of Homo Reciprocans, in MacKay, R.C., Hughes, J.R. and Carver, E.J. (Eds),
European Economic Review, Vol. 42 Nos 3/5, pp. 845-859. Empathy in the Helping Relationship, Springer Publishing
Fornell, C. and Larcker, D.F. (1981), Evaluating structural Company, New York, NY, pp. 49-64.
equation models with unobservable variables and Kurzban, R., Burton-Chellew, M.N. and West, S.A. (2015),
measurement error, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 18 The evolution of altruism in humans, Annual Review of
No. 1, pp. 39-50. Psychology, Vol. 66, pp. 575-599.
Gilliam, D.A., Flaherty, K.E. and Rayburn, S.W. (2014), Li, N., Kirkman, B.L. and Porter, C.O.L.H. (2014), Toward
The dimensions of storytelling by retail salespeople, a model of work team altruism, Academy of Management
International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Review, Vol. 39 No. 4, pp. 541-565.
Research, Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 231-241. Licata, J.W., Mowen, J.C., Harris, E.G. and Brown, T.J.
Gilliam, D.A. and Voss, K. (2013), A proposed procedure (2003), On the trait antecedents and outcomes of service
for construct definition in marketing, European Journal of worker job resourcefulness: a hierarchical model approach,
Marketing, Vol. 47 Nos 1/2, pp. 5-26. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 31 No. 3,
Gintis, H. (Ed.) ( 2005), Moral Sentiments and Material pp. 256-271.
Interests: The Foundations of Cooperation in Economic Life, McCabe, K.A., Rigdon, M.L. and Smith, V.L. (2003),
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, Vol. 6. Positive reciprocity and intentions in trust games, Journal

299
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

of Economic Behavior & Organization, Vol. 52 No. 2, Paunonen, S.V. (2003), Big five factors of personality and
pp. 267-275. replicated predictions of behavior, Journal of Personality
McCullough, M.E., Emmons, R.A. and Tsang, J. (2002), and Social Psychology, Vol. 84 No. 2, pp. 411-422.
The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical Penner, L.A. (2002), Dispositional and organizational
topography, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, influences on sustained volunteerism: an interactionist
Vol. 82 No. 1, pp. 12-27. perspective, Journal of Social Issues, Vol. 58 No. 3,
McGuire, A.M. (2003), It was nothing extending pp. 447-467.
evolutionary models of altruism by two social cognitive Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Presaghi, F. and Ercolani, A.P.
biases in judgments of the costs and benefits of helping, (2003), The personal norm of reciprocity, European
Social Cognition, Vol. 21 No. 5, pp. 363-394. Journal of Personality, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 251-283.
Mechinda, P. and Patterson, P.G. (2011), The impact of Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B. and Podsakoff, N.P.
service climate and service provider personality on (2012), Sources of method bias in social science research
employees customer-oriented behavior in a high-contact and recommendations on how to control it, Annual Review
setting, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 25 No. 2, of Psychology, Vol. 63, pp. 539-569.
pp. 101-113. Rayburn, S.W. (2014), Improving service employee work
MacKay, R.C., Hughes, J.R. and Carver, E.J. (1990), affect: the transformative potential of work design, Journal
Empathy in the Helping Relationship, Springer Publishing of Services Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 71-81.
Company, New York, NY. Rhodewalt, F. and Morf, C.C. (1998), On
Maner, J.K., Luce, C.L., Neuberg, S.L., Cialdini, R.B., self-aggrandizement and anger: a temporal analysis of
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

Brown, S. and Sagarin, B.J. (2002), The effects of narcissism and affective reactions to success and failure,
perspective taking on motivations for helping: still no Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 74 No. 3,
evidence for altruism, Personality and Social Psychology pp. 672-685.
Bulletin, Vol. 28 No. 11, pp. 1601-1610. Segal, U. and Sobel, J. (2007), Tit for tat: foundations of
Mauss, M. (1966), The Gift: Forms and Functions of Exchange preferences for reciprocity in strategic settings, Journal of
in Archaic Societies, Cohen and West, London.
Economic Theory, Vol. 136 No. 1, pp. 197-216.
Melton, H. and Hartline, M.D. (2015), Customer and
Teng, C., Lee, I., Chu, T., Chang, H. and Liu, T. (2012),
employee co-creation of radical service innovations,
How can supervisors improve employees intention to help
Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 112-123.
colleagues? Perspectives from social exchange and
Menguc, B. and Boichuk, J.P. (2012), Customer orientation
appraisal- coping theories, Journal of Service Research,
dissimilarity, sales unit identification, and
Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 332-342.
customer-directed extra-role behaviors: understanding the
Tesser, A., Gatewood, R. and Driver, M. (1968), Some
contingency role of coworker support, Journal of Business
determinants of gratitude, Journal of Personality and Social
Research, Vol. 65 No. 9, pp. 1357-1363.
Psychology, Vol. 9 No. 3, pp. 233-236.
Morgan, R.M. and Hunt, S.D. (1994), The
Thompson, L.Y., Snyder, C.R., Hoffman, L., Michael, S.T.,
commitment-trust theory of relationship marketing,
Rasmussen, H.N., Billings, L.S., Heinze, L., Neufeld, J.E.,
Journal of Marketing, Vol. 58 No. 3, pp. 20-38.
Mowen, J.C. (2000), The 3M Model of Motivation and Shorey, H.S., Roberts, J.C. and Roberts, D.E. (2005),
Personality: Theory and Empirical Applications to Consumer Dispositional forgiveness of self, others, and situations,
Behavior, Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston, MA. Journal of Personality, Vol. 73 No. 2, pp. 313-360.
Mowen, J.C. and Sujan, H. (2005), Volunteer behavior: a Unger, L.S. (1991), Altruism as a motivation to volunteer,
hierarchical model approach for investigating its trait and Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 71-100.
functional motive antecedents, Journal of Consumer Wetzel, H.A., Hammerschmidt, M. and Zablah, A.R. (2014),
Psychology, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 170-182. Gratitude versus entitlement: a dual process model of the
OBrien, J.P. and David, P. (2014), Reciprocity and R&D profitability implications of customer prioritization,
search: applying the behavioral theory of the firm to a Journal of Marketing, Vol. 78 No. 2, pp. 1-19.
communitarian context, Strategic Management Journal, Wilson, D.S., Near, D.C. and Miller, R.R. (1996),
Vol. 35 No. 4, pp. 550-565. Machiavellianism: a synthesis of the evolutionary and
Ostrom, E. and Walker, J. (Eds) ( 2005), Trust and Reciprocity: psychological literatures, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 119
Interdisciplinary Lessons from Experimental Research, Vol. 6, No. 2, pp. 285-299.
Russell Sage Foundation Series on Trust, New York, NY. Wilson, D.S., Near, D.C. and Miller, R.R. (1998),
Ozer, D.J. and Benet-Martinez, V. (2006), Personality and Individual differences in Machiavellianism as a mix of
the prediction of consequential outcomes, Annual Review cooperative and exploitative strategies, Evolution and
of Psychology, Vol. 57 No. 1, pp. 401-421. Human Behavior, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 203-212.
Park, H. and Antonioni, D. (2007), Personality, reciprocity Wrightsman, L.S. (1974), Assumptions About Human Nature:
and strength of conflict resolution strategy, Journal of A Social-Psychological Approach, Brooks/Cole Publishing,
Research in Personality, Vol. 41 No. 1, pp. 110-125. Monterey, CA.
Patterson, A. and Baron, S. (2010), Deviant employees and Yoo, J. (2013), The influence of social undermining on the
dreadful service encounters: customer tales of discord and service employees customer oriented boundary-spanning
distrust, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 24 No. 6, behavior, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 27 No. 7,
pp. 438-445. pp. 539-550.

300
Propensity for reciprocity among frontline employees Journal of Services Marketing
David A. Gilliam and Steven W. Rayburn Volume 30 Number 3 2016 290 301

About the authors spanner issues for salespeople and service workers,
especially those regarding communication and
David A. Gilliam (PhD, Oklahoma State University) is an
relationships. David A. Gilliam is the corresponding author
Assistant Professor of Marketing and Assistant Director of
and can be contacted at: dagilliam@ualr.edu
the Center for Professional Selling at the University of
Arkansas at Little Rock. He has extensive work experience
with a multinational corporation and as an entrepreneur. Steven W. Rayburn (PhD, Oklahoma State University) is an
His research has appeared or is forthcoming in the European Assistant Professor at Texas State University. His research
Journal of Marketing, Journal of Retailing and Consumer takes a Transformative Service Approach and often focuses on
Services, International Review of Retail Distribution and front-line service issues for both employees and consumers.
Consumer Research, Industrial Marketing Management, His research is published in Journal of Services Marketing,
Journal of Marketing Management and a number of Journal of Business Research, Service Industries Journal, and
conference proceedings. His research focuses on boundary other Marketing Journals.
Downloaded by University of Sussex Library At 19:21 22 May 2016 (PT)

For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com

301

S-ar putea să vă placă și