Sunteți pe pagina 1din 6

Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 940945

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

A ve-dimensional model of individual differences in social exchange styles


Michelle J. Leybman a,, David C. Zuroff a, Marc A. Fournier b
a
McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
b
University of Toronto Scarborough, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: An evolutionary psychology framework was used to develop a ve-factor model of individual differences
Received 27 April 2011 in the domain of social exchange that built on a prior two-factor model (Leybman, Zuroff, Fournier, Kelly,
Received in revised form 19 July 2011 & Martin, 2011). Two studies examined the factor structure of the revised Social Exchange Styles Ques-
Accepted 23 July 2011
tionnaire (SESQ-II), the reliability and validity of its ve scales, and the relationship between social
Available online 23 August 2011
exchange styles (SESs) and social support. Principal components and principal factor analyses of the
SESQ-II in 226 undergraduates found ve social exchange dimensions: Tracking, Fairness, Individualism,
Keywords:
Benet-Seeking, and Overinvestment. These scales showed good internal consistency and retest reliabil-
Personality
Evolutionary psychology
ity. Multiple regression analyses demonstrated that SESs, while distinct from other personality variables,
Reciprocity had expected relationships with several variables and predicted social support patterns.
Scale development 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Social support

1. Introduction behavior evolved as a response to problems in the domains of


attachment, mating, social rank, coalition groups, and reciprocity/
The eld of evolutionary psychology has grown rapidly in inu- social exchange. To date, few personality measures have been
ence over the past two decades, yet it has not been applied to its developed with an explicitly evolutionary rationale. Thus, Zuroff
full potential in the area of personality psychology. Recently, Zur- et al. (2010) proposed identifying domain-specic personality vari-
off, Fournier, Patall, and Leybman (2010) suggested that applying ables, beginning with the social rank domain. Leybman, Zuroff,
an evolutionary psychology framework to understanding individ- Fournier, Kelly, & Martin, 2011 continued by examining individual
ual differences could lead to a unied theory of personality that fo- differences in the social exchange domain.
cused on both content (a description of personality) and process Leybman et al. (2011) found two dimensions of individual dif-
(how personality is acquired and inuences behavior). They pro- ferences in social exchange styles (SESs): Equitable Alliance Build-
posed developing this theory by identifying individual differences ing (EAB: the tendency to seek benets, favor equity, and be willing
within the evolved systems that regulate social behavior. The pres- to invest in establishing and maintaining social exchange relation-
ent article focuses on individual differences in the domain of social ships) and Vigilant Alliance Management (VAM: the tendency to
exchange relationships. monitor costs and benets in exchange relationships, be rm in
negotiations with exchange partners, and be willing to terminate
1.1. Evolutionary psychology exchange relationships). EAB and VAM were distinct from NEO
traits and attachment styles, but showed predictable relationships
Our research has been inuenced by the theme of domain spec- with these variables; i.e., extraverted and agreeable people were
icity within evolutionary psychology, which proposes that recur- higher in EAB, whereas disagreeable and avoidantly attached peo-
rent problems in living in the environment of evolutionary ple were higher in VAM. Moreover, EAB and VAM predicted subjec-
adaptiveness can be categorized into distinct domains (e.g., Bugen- tive and objective performance in small work groups. In this article
tal, 2000). The adaptive problems of each domain resulted in a cor- we propose a more comprehensive alternative to Leybman and col-
responding set of evolved psychological mechanisms that guide leagues two-dimensional model and provide evidence for the reli-
our current thinking and behavior. Bugental suggested that human ability and validity of a measure of our new model.

Abbreviations: SESQ, Social Exchange Styles Questionnaire; SESs, Social


1.2. Social exchange styles
Exchange Styles; EAB, Equitable Alliance Building; VAM, Vigilant Alliance
Management.
Corresponding author. There are a handful of scales that measure individual differ-
E-mail address: mleybman@gmail.com (M.J. Leybman). ences in the area of social exchange, such as the Personal Norms

0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.024
M.J. Leybman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 940945 941

of Reciprocity Questionnaire (PNRQ; Perugini, Gallucci, Presaghi, & expected to show that there was only moderate overlap between
Ercolani, 2003). Leybman et al. (2011) developed the Social Ex- our dimensions and other variables, particularly the PNRQ, given
change Style Questionnaire (SESQ) as a measure of individual dif- its focus on styles of approaching reciprocity. More specic
ferences within the social exchange domain in which social hypotheses regarding construct validity are noted prior to report-
exchange styles SESs were dened in evolutionary psychological ing each analysis.
terms and covered a broader range of the domain than previous
scales. Many personality measures offer both higher- and lower- 2.1. Methods
order factors so that users of the scales can decide whether to
pursue parsimony or comprehensiveness. Leybman et al. (2011) 2.1.1. Participants and procedure
anticipated that EAB and VAM would emerge from four lower-or- We recruited 226 English-speaking undergraduate students
der factors: exploitiveness (promoting benets while seeking to from McGill University (149 women, 77 men) using classied
minimize costs), fairness (promoting benets while assuming advertisements on McGills website and Craigslist. Participants
costs), helping (assuming costs without necessarily expecting or completed a battery of questionnaires during a 1-h lab session
promoting benets), and individualism (disinterest in alliances and were contacted again after 2 weeks and 2 months to complete
with little investment and little benet). one questionnaire again. One hundred and 32 participants (97 wo-
Leybman et al. (2011) were able to identify two broad factors in men, 35 men) were retained for the 2-week retest session, and 100
the SESQ, but they did not obtain the anticipated lower-order participants (70 women, 30 men) were retained for the 2-month
styles. We continue to regard the original model as useful and par- retest session. Participants were compensated $15 for the lab ses-
simonious, but we see behavior in this domain as too complex to sion, and $5 for completed retest sessions.
be fully described by two factors. Thus, we created a revision of
the SESQ in hopes of capturing the exploitiveness, fairness, helping, 2.1.2. Measures
and individualism factors, as well as the two broad factors. To add 2.1.2.1. Revised Social Exchange Styles Questionnaire (SESQ-II).
items to the SESQ-II, we identied facets of the four hypothesized Means and Cronbach alphas for all measures are reported in Ta-
factors: goals/expectations about benets, goals/expectations ble 1. The 75 items were rated on a Likert scale, ranging from 1
about costs, beliefs/behaviors related to maintenance of exchange (not at all like me) to 5 (very much like me). Before completing
relationships, negotiation styles in exchange relationships, and be- the questionnaire, participants were asked to write down the ini-
liefs/behaviors related to monitoring costs and benets. We wrote tials of three people they consider social exchange partners. More-
items for each factor that t within these facets. The nal pool in- over, a standard denition of an alliance was provided. The term
cluded 75 items. alliance is used throughout the scale instead of the term social ex-
In addition to creating a more comprehensive supplement to change relationship in an attempt to circumvent the aversion peo-
our two-factor model, we hoped to differentiate our scale from ple might have to conceptualizing their relationships as
the most commonly measured ve-factor personality traits. We motivated by exchange.
also wanted to differentiate our scale from another measure of
individual differences in approaches to reciprocity, the PNRQ 2.1.2.2. Personal Norms of the Reciprocity Questionnaire (PNRQ;
(Perugini et al., 2003). Finally, we wanted to examine the relation- Perugini et al., 2003). This 27-item measure assesses three dimen-
ships of our dimensions with other relevant variables. We were sions of individual differences in approaching reciprocity: belief in
particularly interested in relationships with the interpersonal vari- reciprocity (expectation that others reciprocate ones behaviors),
ables from the NEO (Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Agreeable- positive reciprocity (reciprocating positive acts), and negative rec-
ness), variables that would lend insight into behavioral iprocity (reciprocating negative acts). Cronbach alphas for these
motivations (perceptions of ones value to others and of others factors were .62, .76, and .84, respectively. Items are rated on a
benevolence), and variables that relate to the adaptive function 7-point Likert scale. This scale has demonstrated good construct
of social exchange styles (social support). Hypotheses about these validity (Perugini et al., 2003).
relationships will be noted in the Results section.
2.1.2.3. NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae,
1992). This 60-item measure assesses Neuroticism, Extraversion,
1.3. Objectives
Openness, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness, but we focused
on Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Agreeableness given their rele-
The overarching goal of this research was to examine the reli-
vance to social exchange. Cronbach alphas for these factors were
ability and validity of the newly developed SESQ-II. To do this,
.88, .82, and .77, respectively. Participants were asked to rate each
we had four specic objectives: (1) examine the dimensions of
item on a 5-point Likert scale. This measure has been found to have
the SESQ-II; (2) test the stability of SESs at 2-week and 2-month
good construct validity (Young & Schinka, 2001), and each subscale
follow-up; (3) examine construct validity by looking at the rela-
demonstrates good internal consistency (Murray, Rawlings, Allen,
tionships between SESs and other personality variables, as well
& Trinder, 2003).
as variables related to perceptions of self and other; (4) demon-
strate the utility of the SESQ-II by exploring its prediction of social
2.1.2.4. Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965). This is a
support.
10-item measure of global feelings of self-worth or self-acceptance
that uses a 5-point Likert scale. The Cronbach alpha for self-esteem
2. Study 1 was .90. This scale has good internal consistency and retest reli-
ability (Fleming & Courtney, 1984), and is valid (Lorr & Wunder-
We hypothesized four factors that would reect the themes of lich, 1986).
exploitiveness, fairness, helping, and individualism. Moreover,
although we conceptualized SESs as dispositional, we expected 2.1.2.5. Philosophies of Human Nature Scale (POHNS; Wrightsman,
that variability emerges as individuals adjust their behavior to dif- 1964). The POHNS measures perceptions of other people on six
ferent exchange situations, so we hypothesized moderate temporal dimensions: altruism, trustworthiness, independence, strength of
stability at 2-week and 2-month follow-up. Finally, in examining will and rationality, simplicity and complexity, and variability
the relationships between our variables and other variables, we among people. We focused on perceptions of others benevolence
942 M.J. Leybman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 940945

Table 1
Descriptive statistics of all variables and Pearson correlations with SESQ.

Variable Correlation M SD
Tracking Fairness Individualism Benet-Seeking Overinvestment
2-Week Follow-up
Tracking .68*** 2.75 0.72
Fairness .69*** 4.06 0.59
***
Individualism .74 2.85 0.61
Benet-Seeking .68*** 3.5 0.52
Overinvestment .64*** 2.89 0.44
2-Month Follow-up
Tracking .61*** 2.78 0.76
Fairness .69*** 3.92 0.57
***
Individualism .75 2.88 0.66
Benet-Seeking .60*** 3.4 0.47
Overinvestment .50*** 2.99 0.47
Five-Factor Traits
Neuroticism .18** 0.05 0.12 .15* 0.11 2.89 0.75
Extraversion 0.04 0.11 .37*** .25** 0.01 3.43 0.58
Agreeableness .30*** .36*** .22** 0.03 0.1 3.6 0.53
Norms of Reciprocity
Belief in reciprocity .28*** .28*** 0.07 0.1 0.12 4.4 0.73
Positive reciprocity 0.02 .40*** .18** 0.06 0.01 5.41 0.68
Negative reciprocity .36*** .14* 0.06 0.13 0.07 3.52 1
Self-Esteem 0.08 0.1 0.06 .19** .24** 3.74 0.73
Beliefs about others
Trust .24** .17** .16* 0.05 0.05 0.17 0.73
Altruism .21** .16* .26** 0.09 0.01 0.08 0.87
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .0001.

which are assessed by the altruism and trustworthiness scales, the scale (as per Little, Cunningham, Shahar, and Widaman (2002))
each of which consists of 14 items rated on 7-point Likert scales. with three parcels per factor entered into the model. This model
Cronbach alphas for these variables were .75, and .83, respectively. had a good t, GFI = .91, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .07. These results led
us to adopt the ve-factor model with eight items per scale as
2.2. Results and discussion our nal model. We named the factors: Tracking, Fairness, Individu-
alism, Benet-Seeking, and Overinvestment. Cronbach alphas were
2.2.1. Social exchange dimensions .84, .81, .81, .75, and .68, respectively. The items that map onto
Our rst objective was to examine the dimensional structure of each dimension and their factor loadings are available upon re-
the SESQ-II. A principal components analysis (PCA) with a promax quest. The inter-factor correlations for the ve-factor model are
rotation was conducted on the 75 items. We used a promax rota- presented in Table 2 (all <.35). The only signicant gender differ-
tion to assess possible correlations between the factors. The scree ence was for Fairness, F(1, 224) = 6.71, p < .05, with women endors-
plot suggested a 7-factor solution, but because only 3 items loaded ing more Fairness than men.
onto the seventh factor, we discarded it and interpreted a six-fac- The content of the ve factors was largely consistent with our
tor solution. The six factors reected themes of: (1) monitoring hypothesized model, but some differences arose. The Fairness
costs and benets, (2) favoring equity and fairness, (3) preference and Individualism dimensions resembled our hypothesized factors,
for self-reliance and low investment, (4) reluctance to abandon ex- with Fairness reecting a desire for equal relationships and a will-
change relationships, (5) desire to benet as much as possible, and ingness to invest in order to promote mutual benets, and Individ-
(6) investing in exchange relationships without requiring equal ualism reecting a preference to rely on oneself and avoid
investments from others. We found that the third and fourth fac- investments in exchange relationships. The content of the explo-
tors were substantially correlated, r(224) = 0.47, p < .001. Because itiveness dimension shifted towards a more specic focus on ones
of the magnitude of this correlation, and because we had not own benets in a social exchange relationship, and the content of
hypothesized a relationship-maintenance factor, we do not report the helping dimension shifted towards a specic focus on ones
the results related to the fourth factor. own investments in a social exchange relationship. Finally, the
Approximate factor scores were created by averaging the eight Tracking facet emerged as a unique dimension instead of merging
highest loading items for each factor. We did not have a minimum into the existing factors.
factor loading score and we did not omit items if they cross-loaded Next we explored the relationship between our new ve factors
across factors, preferring to keep more items in this phase of item and the two factors initially reported by Leybman et al. (2011). A
selection. Five items had loadings higher than .30 on more than second-order PCA with a promax rotation on the ve factors of
one item. We then conducted a principal factor analysis (PFA) the SESQ-II disclosed two higher-order factors. The rst higher-or-
using a promax rotation on the 40 items that emerged from the ini- der factor correlated with EAB, r(224) = .84, p < .001, and the sec-
tial screening. The PFA conrmed our ve-factor model with each ond correlated with VAM, r(224) = .63, p < .001 showing a strong
item loading onto the same factor that it did in the initial PCA. The relationship with the two factors from the rst version of our scale.
lowest loading was .34 for the last item on the Benet-Seeking fac- To further examine the relationship, we used Structural Equation
tor. Finally, we conducted a Conrmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) on Modeling to determine whether the two higher-order factors and
item parcels that were constructed by balancing the loadings from the two original factors reected two latent variables. Indeed, this
M.J. Leybman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 940945 943

Table 2
Inter-factor correlations, and second-order PCA loading values.

Tracking Fairness Individualism Benet-Seeking Overinvestment EAB VAM Second order factor 1 Second order factor 2
*** ***
Tracking 1.00 .05 .06 .28 .09 .12 .63 .78
Fairness 1.00 .33*** .34*** .02 .81*** .20* .79
Individualism 1.00 .23** .01 .54*** .40*** .80
Benet-Seeking 1.00 .15* .13* .29*** .52 .54
Overinvestment 1.00 .13 .30*** .58

Note: Only factor loadings >.50 are reported.


*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .0001.

model had a good t on two of the three commonly used t indices from others, we expected them to be related to Extraversion, as
in CFA, GFI = .91, CFI = .91. The third commonly used indicator did it is the Big Five trait that encompasses assertiveness. We expected
not indicate good t, RMSEA = .33, but the GFI and CFI led us to ac- Individualism to be negatively related to Extraversion, which
cept the model. Factor loadings for the higher-order factors and encompasses warmth and gregariousness, because Individualism
correlations between our initial factors and the new factors are re- is partly dened by avoidance of exchange relationships. Finally,
ported in Table 2. we expected a willingness to invest in exchange relationships to
be predicted by Agreeableness. Thus we anticipated that Fairness
2.2.2. Testretest reliability and Overinvestment would be predicted by Agreeableness, while
Our second objective was to test the stability of SESs. Pearson Individualism and Benet-Seeking would be negatively related to
correlation coefcients were calculated to examine the retest reli- this variable.
ability at 2-week and 2-month follow-up (Table 1). The results The overall models were signicant (all ps < .001) for predicting
were consistent with our prediction of moderate stability, with Tracking (R2 = .11), Fairness (R2 = .13), Individualism (R2 = .17), and
correlations ranging from .64 (Overinvestment) to .74 (Individual- Benet-Seeking (R2 = .07). Tracking was positively predicted by
ism) at 2-week follow-up, and from .50 (Overinvestment) to .75 Neuroticism, = .15, p < .05, and negatively predicted by Agree-
(Individualism) at 2-month follow-up. Paired samples t-tests were ableness, = .37, p < .001. Fairness was positively predicted by
performed between each factor at the initial session, with the same Agreeableness, = .40, p < .001. Individualism was negatively pre-
factor at 2-week follow-up and 2-month follow-up. Only one of the dicted by both Extraversion, = .50, p < .001, and Agreeableness,
10 t-tests performed was signicant, indicating a slight decrease in = .23, p < .05. Finally, Benet-Seeking was positively predicted
Fairness from the initial session to the 2-month follow-up, by Extraversion = .24, p < .001.
t(99) = 2.08, p < .05. In summary, as expected, people high in Tracking tended to be
more neurotic. Additionally, as expected, people high in Fairness
2.2.3. Construct validity tended to be agreeable, while people high in Individualism tended
Our third objective was to examine construct validity. Correla- to be disagreeable. An unexpected nding was that people high in
tions were small to moderate in magnitude, demonstrating good Tracking were disagreeable; this gives us insight into the interper-
discriminant validity; the highest observed correlation was be- sonal patterns that relate to this SES. Finally, as predicted, people
tween Fairness and Positive Reciprocity, r(224) = .40, p < .001 (Ta- high in Benet-Seeking were more extraverted, while people high
ble 1). Multiple regressions were performed with Tracking, in Individualism were more introverted.
Fairness, Individualism, Benet-Seeking, and Overinvestment as
dependent variables to examine the relationship of SESs with other 2.2.3.3. Perceptions of self and others. Analyses related to ones per-
personality variables and with a persons representation of self and ceptions of themselves and others were conducted to contribute to
others. Four separate analyses were conducted using the following understanding the motives underlying different SESs. We reasoned
sets of predictors: (1) personal norms of reciprocity, (2) NEO traits, that people with high self-worth would see themselves as valuable
(3) self-esteem, and (4) philosophies of human nature. Predictors trading partners and would therefore use strategies focused on
were entered simultaneously into models. Interactions with gen- receiving benets. People with positive self-perceptions would ex-
der were tested, but none were signicant so they were deleted pect that others would want to share resources with them. Simi-
from all models. larly, we hypothesized that low self-worth would predict
strategies in which people focus on investing resources to be per-
2.2.3.1. Personal norms of reciprocity. Because of its relationship to ceived as valuable exchange partners, anticipating that people with
the social exchange domain, we were interested in demonstrating low self-esteem would feel they could not offer anything to others
that there would be only small to moderate relationships between unless it was material or concrete. Consistent with these hypothe-
the PNRQ and our scales. The overall models were signicant ses, Benet-Seeking was positively predicted by self-esteem,
(p < .05) for predicting Tracking (R2 = .15), Fairness (R2 = .26), and = .19, p < .01, and Overinvestment was negatively predicted by
Individualism (R2 = .04). These results conrm that SESs are not self-esteem, = .24, p < .01.
redundant with other measures of individual differences in Regarding perceptions of others, we expected perceptions of
reciprocity. benevolence to motivate strategies that focus on Fairness and
Overinvestment, whereas we expected negative perceptions to re-
2.2.3.2. Personality traits. We explored the relationships between sult in strategies that involved monitoring others behavior (Track-
SESs and the interpersonal NEO traits (Neuroticism, Extraversion, ing), beneting without investing (Benet-Seeking), or being
and Agreeableness). We understand Tracking as a response to anx- detached from exchange partners (Individualism). We therefore
iety about being cheated in exchange relationships; we therefore tested models in which perceptions of Altruism and Trustworthi-
expected it to be related to Neuroticism. Additionally, because both ness predicted the SESQ-II dimensions. The overall models were
Fairness and Benet-Seeking are related to obtaining resources signicant (all ps < .05) for predicting Tracking (R2 = .06), Fairness
944 M.J. Leybman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 940945

(R2 = .03), Individualism (R2 = .07), and Benet-Seeking (R2 = .04). were considered missed. Participants were told that they could
Tracking was negatively predicted by viewing people as trustwor- miss a maximum of one diary and make it up on the 8th day. Par-
thy, = .19, p = .05. Individualism was negatively predicted by ticipants were compensated $16 for the initial baseline session, $2
viewing people as altruistic, = .31, p < .01. Lastly, Benet-Seek- for each completed online diary and a $20 bonus for completing se-
ing was negatively predicted by viewing people as trustworthy, ven diaries in 8 days.
= .28, p < .05, and was positively predicted by viewing people
as altruistic = .29, p < .05. Because the Benet-Seeking results 3.2. Results and discussion
were inconsistent with the zero-order correlations, they were
attributed to suppression effects. In summary, our hypotheses 3.2.1. Predictive validity
about peoples beliefs about others were partially conrmed in that We aggregated both perceived and received social support
people who viewed others as untrustworthy were more likely to scores across the seven diaries. Next, we conducted two multiple
use Tracking or Benet-Seeking strategies, and people who viewed regressions in which all ve SES dimensions were entered simulta-
others as less altruistic were likely to use Individualism strategies. neously as predictors. Interactions with gender were tested but
none were signicant, so gender was deleted from all models. SESs
3. Study 2 were signicant in predicting both perceived (R2 = .24, p < .001)
and received (R2 = .14, p < .05) social support. Specically, Tracking,
Our nal objective was to demonstrate the utility of the SESQ-II = .43, p < .01, and Overinvestment, = .19, p < .05, each pre-
by exploring its ability to predict perceived and received social dicted perceiving less social support, while Fairness predicted per-
support. Perceived support refers to ones belief that they could ceiving more support, = .21, p < .05. Received support was
have access to support if they needed it, while received support negatively predicted by Individualism, = .35, p < .01.
is a measure of supportive behaviors provided to someone in a gi-
ven time frame. We anticipated that expecting/seeking benets 3.2.2. Incremental validity
(Fairness and Benet-Seeking) would lead to increased relational To examine the utility of the SESQ-II, we compared the original
benets because people using these strategies would surround two-factor model with our expanded ve-factor model. We found
themselves with others who satised their expectations. On the that while the two-factor model accounted for 11% of the variance
other hand, we anticipated that focusing on monitoring benets in perceived support, and 5% of the variance in received support,
(Tracking) would have the ironic consequence of reducing rela- the ve-factor model accounted for 20% and 9% of the variance in
tional benets. We also expected that either detaching from social these variables, respectively, based on the adjusted R2. Finally,
exchange relationships or investing without requiring reciprocity we conducted a hierarchical regression to determine whether our
would result in less relational satisfaction. In terms of incremental variables predicted social support beyond Neuroticism, Extraver-
validity, we expected that the new model would account for more sion, and Agreeableness. For both perceived and received support
variance in these social outcomes than the old two-factor model, we entered the NEO variables in a rst step, and then entered
and that the SESQ-II would signicantly improve a model in which our ve SESQ-II factors. For perceived support, the R2 increased
Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Agreeableness predicted social from .30 to .40 when the SESQ-II variables were included,
support. F(8, 92) = 7.58, p < .001. For received support, the R2 increased from
.10 to .18, F(8, 92) = 1.21, although this test was not signicant.
Overall, our hypotheses were partially conrmed. As expected,
3.1. Methods
Fairness resulted in more relational satisfaction, whereas Tracking,
Individualism and Overinvestment resulted in less. Moreover, we
3.1.1. Participants and procedure
found an advantage to using the more comprehensive model com-
We recruited 100 English-speaking undergraduate students
pared to the two-factor model and showed that the SESQ-II offered
from McGill University (50 men; 50 women) using classied adver-
an advantage in predicting perceived social support over using
tisements on McGills website and Facebook. The study lasted one
NEO traits alone.
week, involving an initial lab session and a daily diary component.
At the lab session, participants completed the SESQ-II, as well as
the Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Agreeableness scales from the 4. General discussion
NEO-FFI on Survey Monkey, an online questionnaire administra-
tion program. 4.1. Summary
For seven days beginning the day after the lab session, partici-
pants were emailed a link to complete a social support question- We sought to elaborate a model based on evolutionary psychol-
naire on Survey Monkey. Social support was assessed using the ogy that would explain individual differences in approaching social
Social Provisions Scale (SPS; Cutrona & Russell, 1987), which as- exchange relationships. Specically, we sought to provide a more
sesses the extent to which individuals feel that, in times of stress, comprehensive alternative to our two-factor model of SESs. The re-
others provide them with six social provisions. The current study sults generally supported a ve-factor model, with the original two
administered an amended 3-item version of the SPS that asked par- factors emerging as higher-order factors. Moreover, our results
ticipants to rate the extent to which they perceived and received indicated good convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity
the three provisions: guidance (To what extent did another per- and show that SESs predict patterns of social support. Although
son(s) provide you with advice or guidance today?), reliable alli- more research is needed to document the origins and implications
ance (To what extent did another person(s) come to your of SESs, these results demonstrate the potential for a personality
assistance today?), and emotional closeness (To what extent psychology that uses evolutionary psychology as a starting point.
did another person(s) provide you with a sense of emotional secu-
rity and well-being today?). These three items are frequently 4.2. Limitations
combined to measure social support (e.g., Cutrona, 1989).
A research assistant emailed a link to participants every evening Our results provide encouraging evidence for the reliability and
at 6 pm and participants were asked to complete the questionnaire validity of the SESQ-II, yet some limitations should be noted. First,
between 6 pm and 6 am each day. Diaries completed after 6 am research is needed to conrm the factor structure of the SESQ-II.
M.J. Leybman et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 51 (2011) 940945 945

Second, the extent to which SESs are generalizable across relation- Cutrona, C. E. (1989). Ratings of social support by adolescents and adult informants:
Degree of correspondence and prediction of depressive symptoms. Journal of
ships has not yet been explored. Third, the question remains
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 723730.
whether scores on our scale will predict overt behavior in ex- Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. W. (1987). The provisions of social relationships and
change situations. Finally, as we have previously noted, other adaptation to stress. In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.). Advances in personal
existing measures target constructs that have some overlap with relationships (Vol. 1, pp. 3767). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Fleming, J. S., & Courtney, B. E. (1984). The dimensionality of self-esteem. II.
ours. Hierarchical facet model for revised measurement scales. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 46, 404421.
Leybman, M. J., Zuroff, D. C., Fournier, M. A., Kelly, A. C., & Martin, A. E. (2011). Social
4.3. Future directions exchange styles: Measurement, validation, and application. European Journal of
Personality, 25, 198210.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not
Several lines of research could stem from this study. In previous
to parcel: Exploring the questions, weighing the merits. Structural Equation
work, we described potential applications of the SESQ in the areas Modeling, 9, 151173.
of organizational and clinical psychology (Leybman et al., 2011). Lorr, M., & Wunderlich, R. A. (1986). Two objective measures of self-esteem. Journal
Other directions for future research include focusing on modera- of Personality Assessment, 50, 1823.
Murray, G., Rawlings, D., Allen, N. B., & Trinder, J. (2003). NEO Five-Factor inventory
tors of social exchange styles, such as holding compassionate goals scores: Psychometric properties in a community sample. Measurement &
versus goals focused on building reputation, and using game para- Evaluation in Counseling & Development, 36, 140149.
digms from behavioral economics that could permit experimental Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Presaghi, F., & Ercolani, A. P. (2003). The personal norm of
reciprocity. European Journal of Personality, 17, 251283.
investigations of the behavioral correlates and situational modera- Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
tors of SESs. University Press.
Wrightsman, L. S. (1964). Measurement of philosophies of human nature.
Psychological Reports, 14, 743751.
References Young, M. S., & Schinka, J. A. (2001). Research validity scales for the NEO-PI-R:
Additional evidence for reliability and validity. Journal of Personality Assessment,
Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A domain based 76, 412420.
approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 187219. Zuroff, D. C., Fournier, M. A., Patall, E. A., & Leybman, M. J. (2010). Steps toward an
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R. Professional manual. Odessa, FL: evolutionary personality psychology: Individual differences in the social rank
Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. domain. Canadian Psychology, 51, 5866.

S-ar putea să vă placă și