Sunteți pe pagina 1din 4

LETTER TO THE EDITOR

Baby Hair, Mercury Toxicity and Autism


Judith K. Grether,1 Lisa A. Croen,2 and Cheryl A. Theis1

QUERY SHEET

Q1: Au: Please provide location of kaiser-permanente Division of Research.


Q2: Au: Pls. provide received and accepted dates.
Q3: Au: Please provide affiliation and correspondence address.
Q4: Au: Please complete ref. 1.
LETTER TO THE EDITOR

Baby Hair, Mercury Toxicity and Autism


Judith K. Grether,1 Lisa A. Croen,2 and Cheryl A. Theis1
1
Environmental Health Investigations Branch, California Department of Health Services,
5 Oakland, California, USA
Q1 2
Northern California Kaiser-Permanente Division of Research

Concerns continue to be raised regarding the potential role of et al. 1999; Weihe et al. 2002). While it is possible that excretion
mercury exposure in the etiology of autism. In a recent arti- in infants and very young children follows a different biologic
cle published in this journal (Reduced Levels of Mercury in trajectory than in older children and adults, the interpretation
10 First Baby Haircuts of Autistic Children), Holmes, Blaxill, and put forward by Holmes et al. of their unexpected findings re-
Haley (2003) report intriguing findings of higher reported mer- quires confirmation in a study specifically designed to test this 40
cury exposure but lower mercury levels in baby hair of children hypothesis.
with autism compared to control children and in children with As acknowledged by Holmes and colleagues, their study had
severe autism compared to those more mildly affected. At least several methodological limitations that might explain the sur-
15 five interesting, testable hypotheses can be derived from the prising findings. Both the case and control groups represent
Holmes et al. report. convenience samples that undoubtedly bias the findings to an 45
unknown extent. The children with autism were referrals to the
H1: Low hair mercury concentration is a reliable and valid mea- lead author who is an advocate for chelation therapy for chil-
sure of impaired excretion capability in young children. dren with autism. Although the overall gender pattern among
H2: Impaired ability to excrete mercury in some children may, the children with autism in the study is consistent with the lit-
20 in the presence of mercury exposure, contribute to the de- erature, the high percentage of females in the mildly affected 50
velopment of autism. case group is contrary to available data from numerous sources
H3: The greater the impairment in excretion, the more severe that consistently show increasing imbalance in the sex ratio with
the autism. decreasing severity of impairment. Similarly, controls were re-
H4: Autistic children have normal mercury excretion but have cruited through autism parent groups and autism newsletters and
25 a lowered uptake in first baby hair. are unlikely to be representative of the population of typically 55
H5: Study bias or chance account for the findings in this study. developing children.
To our knowledge, the relationship of mercury concentra- As reported in the article, the data collection methods may
tion in hair to overall excretion capability in infants and young also have introduced further bias. Interviews with parents were
children has not been established. High hair levels are consid- conducted by the lead author, ASH, who was not blinded to
30 ered markers of high methylmercury exposure, at least in adults the study hypothesis nor to the diagnostic status of the chil- 60
(Grandjean, welhe, and Nielsen 1994). High maternal hair lev- dren. It is not stated if study respondents were kept blinded to
els of methylmercury at delivery have been associated with neu- the hypothesis, but this seems unlikely since parents of affected
ropsychological deficits in their young children and high levels children had sought the lead authors help for evaluation of mer-
in hair of 7-year-old children were associated with subtle deficits cury toxicity in their children. This raises the possibility that
35 in performance (Grandjean, welhe, and white 1997), (Grandjean respondents knew what the study was about and may have re- 65
sponded accordingly. Of further concern, Holmes et al. found
that 46% of mothers in their case group reported being treated
with Rho D immunoglobulin during pregnancy, a strikingly in-
Received ; accepted . creased proportion compared to the control group and to pop-
Q2 Address correspondence to Judith K. Grether, PhD, Environmental
Health Investigations Branch, California Department of Health Ser- ulation estimates. Rho D immunoglobulin contained the ethyl 70
vices, 1515 Clay Street, 17th Floor, Oakland, CA 94612, USA. E-mail: mercury preservative, thimerosal, until late in the 1990s and the
jgrether@dhs.ca.gov authors interpret their case-control difference as indicating that

International Journal of Toxicology, 23:13, 2004


Copyright c American College of Toxicology
ISSN: 1091-5818 print / 1092-874X online
DOI: 10.1080/10915810490502087 1
2 J. K. GRETHER ET AL.

Rho D immunoglobulin treatment is a risk factor for autism. and control groups and data collection methods that minimize 115
Alternatively, the findings could indicate substantial recall bias recall and reporting biases. For evaluation of first baby hair, the
75 in maternal reports (which were not validated through review of concern about lack of standardization in collection procedures
medical records) and/or a highly selected and unrepresentative can probably only be solved in a prospective study that enrolls
group of cases. The authors also report that maternal dietary con- baby sibs of ASD-affected and typically developing children.
sumption of fish was not significantly associated with autism in Other concerns about laboratory analyses can be alleviated by 120
their study. This is surprising given that maternal fish consump- use of research rather than commercial laboratories and by as-
80 tion during pregnancy is typically the source of greatest prenatal suring that case and control specimens are randomly and blindly
mercury exposure. analyzed. Research on etiology of autism is, unfortunately, still
The study also raises concerns related to specimen gathering, in very early stages and we dont yet have sufficient answers
processing, and laboratory analysis. Under the best of circum- to guide prevention and medical treatment. We urge caution in 125
stances, analysis of hair samples is a highly imperfect method- acceptance of the conclusions of this interesting study and join
85 ology for measuring past exposures to metals. Unfortunately, with the authors in calling for additional, rigorously designed,
sometimes it is the only available way to obtain such data. An research to replicate the findings.
expert panel on hair analysis convened by the federal Agency
for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry (ATSDR) in 2001 re-
ported that the lack of standard procedures for collecting, wash- REFERENCES
90 ing, and analyzing hair samples presents notable limitations in Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry (ATSDR). 2003. Analysis of 130
using hair samples in etiologic studies (ASTDR 2003). Holmes hair samples: How do hair sampling results relate to environmental exposures?
online report http://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/HAC/hair analysis/.
et al. reported that the laboratory testing was conducted in a Grandjean, P., P. Welhe, and J. B. Nielsen. 1994. Methylmercury: Significance
blinded fashion but it is not clear that the specimens were sub- of intrauterine and postnatal exposures. Clin. Chem. 40:13951400.
mitted to the laboratory during the same time period and thus Grandjean, P., P. Welhe, and R. White. 1997. Cognitive deficit in 7-year-old chil- 135
95 randomly analyzed. Without further information to evaluate this dren with prenatal exposure to methylmercury. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 19:417
possibility, we are concerned that the observed differences be- 428.
Grandjean, P., E. Budtz-Jorgensen, R. F. White, P. J. Jorgensen, P. Weihe, F.
tween mercury levels in specimens from case and control sub- Debes, and N. Keiding. 1999. Methylmercury exposure biomarkers as indica-
jects could be due to variability over time in laboratory analysis. tors of neurotoxicity in children aged 7 years. Am. J. Epidemiol. 150:301305. 140
Whether or not the degree of bias introduced by these limi- Holmes, A. S., M. F. Blaxill, and B. E. Haley. 2003. Reduced levels of mercury
100 tations is sufficient to invalidate the provocative findings of this in first baby haircuts of autistic children. Int. J. Toxicol. 22:277285.
study can only be determined by further study. An adequate Weihe, P., J. C. Hansen, K. Murata, F. Debes, P. Jorgensen, U. Steuerwald, R. F.
White, and P. Grandjean. 2002. Neurobehavioral performance of Inuit chil-
test of the hypothesis that impaired capability to excrete mer- dren with increased prenatal exposure to methylmercury. Int. J. Circumpolar 145
cury contributes to autism would require population-based case Health. 61:4149.

RESPONSE
Sound and Fury, Signifying Nothing
105
Mark F. Blaxill,1 Boyd E. Haley,2 and Amy S. Holmes3
1
Safe Minds,
2
University of Kentucky
3
Q3

110
Grether and colleagues raise three types of concern with re- Our analysis demonstrated a marked departure from the null hy-
spect to our study of mercury levels in baby hair of autistic pothesis of no difference between first baby haircut mercury
children. In order, these include (1) issues of logic and fact, (2) levels in autistic and control children. Grether and her col-
methodological limitations of our study, and (3) political impli- leagues offer five hypotheses with which to test our data, two of 150
cations of our findings. whichH2 and H3are consistent with our interpretation, two
of whichH1 and H4suggest different interpretations, and
Received ; accepted . oneH5that dismisses our analysis out of hand. Although
Address correspondence to Mark F. Blaxill, Safe Minds, wed have been intrigued to hear them defend the fourth, they
LETTER TO THE EDITOR 3

155 focused their substantive attention only on the first, which is not 3 ppm (compared to our control mean of 3.63 ppm).4 We look 205
even a valid hypothesis and based on a logical error. Contrary forward to learning the results of ongoing replication efforts and
to their suggestion, low mercury hair concentration can provide do not expect that they will reveal material biases in our methods.
a reliable and valid measure of impaired excretion capability Finally, Grether and colleagues reveal their real concerns near
only when maternal mercury exposure is high enough to warrant the end of their letter when they profess their preference to in-
160 an expectation of high mercury levels in hair. We welcome tests validate the findings, largely because they are provocative. In 210
of our arguments, but only when they reflect an understanding that spirit, they urge caution in the acceptance of [our] conclu-
of our work, an understanding not demonstrated here. sions.
Grether and colleagues also question the validity of the con- We see little scientific benefit in such caution. If increased
nection between infant hair mercury levels and mercury excre- mercury exposure is indeed implicated in the increases in autism
165 tion, claiming that such a connection has not been established. rates, then the investigation into the mechanism of action and 215
We strongly support additional research on infant mercury ex- potential treatments must proceed with a sense of urgency and
cretion. But the small amount of work that has been done is con- a bias to action. Autism rates have reached disturbing levels
sistent with our findings and interpretations. One research group in recent years and large numbers of children may have been
has reported preliminary findings in autistic hair samples con- affected by increased mercury exposure and/or retention. Of-
170 sistent with ours.1 In addition, Faeroe Island researchers have fering false comfort in the guise of weak hypotheses does little 220
shown that higher infant hair mercury levels were associated more than divert resources and attention away from this large
with faster progress to developmental milestones.2 This finding emerging population. We note with disappointment that Grether
puzzled the researchers, who never considered a differential ex- and Croen have sponsored similar diversionary tactics before.
cretion hypothesis and assumed that higher hair levels would In a recent publication, they offered what they would probably
175 produce slower development. But the results provide support consider a less provocative hypotheses, in this case the sugges- 225
for the idea that high mercury hair levels in a high-exposure tion that a diagnostic substitution effect might explain recent
population are associated with relatively positive developmen- autism increases.5 Although this was an explanation they might
tal outcomes. have found more comforting, it was simply wrong. Indeed, it
Next, Grether and colleagues offer additional speculation was so clearly flawed that they subsequently withdrew their hy-
180 (H5) on a number of methodological points. Many of these points pothesis based on analysis they could have performed with their 230
reiterate cautions we offered ourselves, including the benefits of original data.6
prospective design, careful case and control recruitment to min- For the health and welfare of affected children and their fam-
imize any source of potential bias, and the use of research as ilies, we suggest they and others worry less about whether our
opposed to commercial laboratories. conclusions are provocative and more about whether they are
185 Nevertheless, it is very hard to dismiss our findings as a con- biologically plausible and consistent with other evidence. We 235
sequence simply of study bias and nearly impossible, statisti- strongly encourage investigation of the environmental causes of
cally, to dismiss them as chance findings. Perhaps Grether and autism and urge that only the highest standards of science and
colleagues believe we simply fabricated the data (indeed, the reason be applied when developing and disseminating hypothe-
authors hint at a hidden commercial agenda around chelation ses for scientific work.
190 therapy). We strongly reject any such allegation.
As to more subtle potential biases, we suggest that skeptics of
our findings pay closer attention to our control group findings. REFERENCES 240
This is the group in which any argument for study bias is at 1. Hu, L-W., J. A. Bernard, and J. Che. 2003. Neutron activation analysis of
its weakest. Yet the variance from an expected finding of low hair samples for the identification of autism. Transactions of the American
195 dosage is high. In 3 of the 45 normal children, the mercury Nuclear Society 89: Q4
concentration in hair was well over 10 ppm, above levels that 2. Grandjean, P., P. Weihe, and R. F. White. 1995. Milestone development in
infants exposed to methylmercury from human milk. Neurotoxicology 16:27 245
would be considered neurotoxic in adult populations,3 and over
33.
1 in 4 had concentrations exceeding 4 ppm. We have responded 3. National Academy of Science. 2000. Toxicological effect of methylmercury.
to some comments that it is these control results that are most National Academy Press: Washington, DC.
200 likely to be biased (whereas the autistics samples are closer to 4. Hightower, J. M., and D. Moore. 2003. Mercury levels in high-end consumers
expected outcomes), but where we have seen evidence on normal of fish. Environ. Health Perspect. 111:604608. 250
5. Croen, L. A., J. K. Grether, J. Hoogstrate, and S. Selvin. 2002. The changing
infant controls we have found it consistent with our work. One
prevalence of autism in California. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 32:207215.
recent report provided examples of similarly high mercury hair 6. Croen, L. A., and J. K. Grether. 2003. Response: A response to Blaxill, Baskin
levels in a California population with high mercury exposures, and Spitzer on Croen et al. (2002). The changing prevalence of autism in
including an additional infant mercury hair concentration of over California. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 33:227229. 255

S-ar putea să vă placă și