Sunteți pe pagina 1din 11

Alimentary Pharmacology and Therapeutics

Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease improves on a gluten-


free diet and correlates with histological and serological
indices of disease severity
I. T. Lichtwark*, E. D. Newnham, S. R. Robinson, S. J. Shepherd, P. Hosking, P. R. Gibson, & G. W. Yelland,

*School of Psychological Sciences, SUMMARY


Monash University, Clayton, Australia.

Eastern Health Clinical School,


Monash University, Box Hill Hospital, Background
Melbourne, Australia. Mild impairments of cognition or Brain fog are often reported by patients
with coeliac disease but the nature of these impairments has not been system-
School of Health Sciences, RMIT
University, Bundoora, Australia. atically investigated.

Central Clinical School, Monash


University, Alfred Health, Melbourne,
Australia.
Aim
This longitudinal pilot study investigated relationships between cognitive func-
tion and mucosal healing in people with newly diagnosed coeliac disease com-
Correspondence to: mencing a gluten-free diet.
Dr G. W. Yelland, Department of
Gastroenterology, Central Clinical Methods
School, Alfred Health, Monash Eleven patients (8 females, 3 males), mean age 30 (range 2239) years, were
University, Vic. 3800, Australia.
tested with a battery of cognitive tests at weeks 0, 12 and 52. Information pro-
E-mail: Greg.Yelland@monash.edu
cessing efcacy, memory, visuospatial ability, motoric function and attention
Lichtwark and Newnham are equal were tested. Small bowel biopsies were collected via routine gastroscopy at
rst authors. weeks 12 and 52 and were compared to baseline Marsh scores. Cognitive per-
formance was compared to serum concentrations of tissue transglutaminase
antibodies, biopsy outcomes and other biological markers.
Publication data
Submitted 27 December 2013
Results
First decision 7 January 2014
Resubmitted 2 May 2014 All patients had excellent adherence to the diet. Marsh scores improved signi-
Accepted 7 May 2014 cantly (P = 0.001, Friedmans test) and tissue transglutaminase antibody con-
EV Pub Online 28 May 2014 centrations decreased from a mean of 58.4 at baseline to 16.8 U/mL at week
52 (P = 0.025). Four of the cognitive tests assessing verbal uency, attention
This article was accepted for publication and motoric function showed signicant improvement over the 12 months and
after full peer-review.
strongly correlated with the Marsh scores and tissue transglutaminase antibody
levels (r = 0.3770.735; all P < 0.05). However, no meaningful patterns of cor-
relations were found for nutritional or biochemical markers, or markers of
intestinal permeability.

Conclusions
In newly diagnosed coeliac disease, cognitive performance improves with adher-
ence to the gluten-free diet in parallel to mucosal healing. Suboptimal levels of cog-
nition in untreated coeliac disease may affect the performance of everyday tasks.

Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160170

160 2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


doi:10.1111/apt.12809
Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease

INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND METHODS


Coeliac disease (CD) is an inammatory autoimmune dis-
order that affects at least 1% of the adult population in Participants
many countries, and a strict, lifelong gluten-free diet Participants were recruited via the Box Hill Hospital
(GFD) is the only recommended treatment.1, 2 While the Coeliac Clinic (Melbourne, Australia), advertisements in
disease is primarily an intestinal disorder that is histologi- local Coeliac Society publications, and from a local pri-
cally characterised by intraepithelial lymphocytosis, crypt vate dietetic practice. Patients were eligible for the study
hyperplasia and villous atrophy, there is increasing support if they were aged between 18 and 40 years, had been
for a broader concept of a systemic inammatory disease. diagnosed with CD and had not commenced a GFD
Support for this view comes from clinical observations of more than 4 weeks prior to enrolment. The diagnosis of
extra-intestinal manifestations such as dermatologic, hepa- CD was made on the basis of duodenal biopsies showing
tic, osteologic, endocrine and neurological signs. Reported villous atrophy (at least Marsh 3a) or Marsh 1 or 2 with
neurological manifestations include amnesia, ataxia, acal- positive CD-specic antibodies. All participants were
culia, epilepsy, chronic neuropathies, confusion and per- required to express the HLA-DQ2 and/or DQ8 haplo-
sonality changes.36 Some of these severe neurological type. Exclusion criteria comprised other signicant gas-
symptoms can improve upon treatment with a GFD.3, 7, 8 trointestinal or inammatory diseases, pregnancy, a
A milder form of cognitive impairment frequently history of neurological or psychiatric events or of brain
reported by people with CD has been colloquially termed injury resulting in unconsciousness for more than
brain fog. Brain fog can include difculty concentrating, 30 min, existing neurodegenerative disease, and an
problems with attentiveness, lapses in short-term mem- inability to speak and write in English or to give written
ory, word-nding difculties, temporary loss in mental informed consent.
acuity and creativity, and confusion or disorientation.9
In our experience, patients often report that brain fog Protocol
dissipates after treatment on a GFD or returns after The timeline for interventions in the study is summar-
inadvertent gluten exposure. ised in Figure 1. During screening and at dened time
Few studies have investigated such subtle cognitive points over the following 12 months, patients were
decits. In a group of eight cognitively normal partici- assessed clinically by a gastroenterologist, had blood and
pants with CD and idiopathic cerebellar ataxia signicant intestinal permeability tests, underwent cognitive testing,
impairments were found in immediate recall from epi- and completed prospective 7-day food diaries to assess
sodic and semantic memory, and there was a trend dietary compliance. Duodenal biopsies were performed
towards decits in phonemic verbal uency and execu- at 12 and 52 weeks. The study protocol was approved
tive function.10 Further, a study of 15 elders with cogni- by the Human Research and Ethics Committees at
tive decline that began within 2 years of the onset of CD Monash University and Eastern Health in Melbourne,
symptoms suggested a link might exist between cognitive Australia. The longitudinal design alleviated issues asso-
impairment and CD.3 The possibility of such a link has ciated with the small sample size. For instance,
been strengthened by the ndings of a retrospective inter-participant variability was eliminated because the
study of elderly patients in which CD was diagnosed participants served as their own control through their
after the age of 60 years.11 Of the seven patients identi- baseline measures.
ed, two presented with cognitive decline that had been
attributed to Alzheimers disease, but was ameliorated Compliance measures
after the initiation of a GFD, while a third patient had At baseline screening and at weeks 12 and 52, partici-
peripheral neuropathy that completely resolved after the pants completed a food diary for seven consecutive days,
initiation of a GFD. recording all food and drink consumed as well as ingre-
The present pilot study investigated relationships dients for home-prepared meals. The diaries were
between levels of cognitive functioning and mucosal assessed by an experienced dietitian, with face-to-face
healing in people commencing on a GFD after being consultations provided if concerns were identied by the
diagnosed with CD. It was hypothesised that the level of participant, clinician or dietitian. Adherence was judged
cognitive function would improve concomitantly with by an arbitrary scale comprising excellent when no
mucosal healing. sources of gluten ingestion were identied, good if

Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170 161


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
I. T. Lichtwark et al.

Compliance assessment

Cognitive tests/Intestinal permeability/Blood tests Figure 1 | Timeline of the


study protocol. Compliance of
the 15 participants to the GFD
was assessed via 7-day food
Week 0 Week 12 Week 52
diaries and interview by a
dietitian. Intestinal
permeability was assessed
using a dual sugar
methodology. Blood tests
ENDOSCOPY AND BIOPSY
examined biochemical,
nutritional and serological
STRICT GLUTEN FREE DIET markers.

minor gluten intake (estimated <0.3 g gluten per Cognitive measures


episode) occurred no more than six times in the Cognitive tests are outlined in detail in Table 1. They
12 months, fair if minor gluten intake (<0.3 g) occurred were administered in a consistent order to keep the test-
up to one episode per week, and poor if gluten intake ing time to a minimum while ensuring that there was
was more frequent than once a week. adherence to the temporal requirements of tests with
multiple components (e.g., delayed memory compo-
Biological measures nents). The Subtle Cognitive Impairment Test (SCIT)
Duodenal biopsies were obtained during routine upper was administered, followed by Trail Making Test A & B,
gastrointestinal endoscopy at weeks 12 and 52 of the Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT),
study. Two biopsies were taken from the rst part of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) immediate
duodenum and at least four from the second part. Histo- recall, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (RAVLT) (tri-
logical assessment was performed by an experienced als 15, foil trial and immediate recall trial), Anxiety and
pathologist (PH) who was blinded to the baseline histol- Depression subscales of the State-Trait Personality
ogy. Marsh scores were assigned using the most severe Inventory (STPI), Grooved Pegboard Task, RAVLT
lesion found in the biopsies. The index biopsies were delayed recall trial, and nally the ROCF delayed recall
reviewed by the same (blinded) pathologist and scored trial. All cognitive testing was conducted during morning
retrospectively in a similar way. visits. Cognitive test performances were scored according
Intestinal permeability (IP) was assessed by a dual to their respective published manuals.
sugar permeability test. After an overnight fast, the blad-
der was emptied, and 1 g L-rhamnose and 5 g lactulose Data analysis
dissolved in 120 mL water was ingested. Urine was col- Most datasets violated the assumption of normality
lected for 5 h after ingestion. Concentrations of both (ShapiroWilkes analyses) and, since Marsh scores are
sugars were measured by high performance liquid chro- an ordinal measure, the Friedman test, a nonparametric
matography and the lactulose:rhamnose (L:R) ratio was alternative to the repeated measures ANOVA, was used to
calculated. investigate whether signicant changes in any of the
Venous blood samples were collected at baseline variables had occurred over the duration of the study.
(screening) and at weeks 12 and 52. Sera were assessed Wilcoxon signed rank tests were used as post hoc tests to
for tissue transglutaminase (tTG) IgA antibodies determine when signicant changes may have occurred.
(QUANTA Lite ELISA, Inova Diagnostics, Inc., San Given the exploratory nature of this study and to avoid
Diego, CA, USA), and haemoglobin (Hb), vitamins D rejecting potential true effects that could be followed up
and B12, iron studies and liver function tests were per- in a larger study, P < 0.1 was considered to represent
formed as routine pathology tests. Thyroid function statistical signicance. To investigate whether signicant
tests were assessed at baseline and weeks 26 and 52. changes had occurred for tTG, IP and serum ferritin

162 Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease

Table 1 | Description of the psychological tests used in this study

Test Purpose Procedure Read-out


Subtle Cognitive Measures the speed and Participants respond to a simple Response time (SCIT-RTH and
Impairment Test effectiveness (efcacy) asymmetrical stimulus appearing on SCIT-RTT)
(SCIT)15 of information the screen for varying duration from Error rate (SCIT-ERH and
processing from iconic 16 to 133 ms by pressing the left or SCIT-ERT)
memory to short-term right mouse button corresponding
memory to the shorter side of the stimulus.
Each of the two versions of the
stimulus is presented six times and
response time and error rate
averages are calculated over 96
trials. Data for the four shortest
exposure durations (1666 ms) are
pooled to provide two scores from
the head of their respective curves:
response time (SCIT-RTH) and error
rate (SCIT-EH). Data from the four
longer presentation durations (83
133 ms) are pooled to provide two
scores from the tail of the data
curve for response time (SCIT-RTT)
and error rate (SCIT-ET)
Trail Making Measures speed of The participant connects letters and The time needed to complete the
Test A & B37 processing, attention, numbers in sequential order from task in seconds
sequencing, exibility, letters and numbers randomly
visual search and ne dispersed on a page
motoric function
Rey-Osterrieth Evaluates visuospatial A copy of a complex gure is drawn Presence and correct placement of
Complex Figure ability and visuospatial by the participant and is then the each element of the
(ROCF)38 memory repeated from memory 3 and drawings
30 min later. Maximum obtainable
score is 36
Controlled Oral Word Measures word uency The participant is asked to produce Sum of the number of words in
Association Task as an indicator of as many words as possible in 1 min, three trials, each with a different
(COWAT)39 frontal lobe functioning starting with a given letter. Each letter
word produced is scored
Rey Auditory Verbal Evaluates verbal learning The participant is asked to recall a Number of correctly recalled
Learning Task and memory 15-word list several times: rst words per trial
(RAVLT)40 directly after it has been read out
loud, then after a 3-min and a
30 min delay
Grooved Evaluates manual Participant inserts small pegs with Sum of time to completion in
Pegboard Task41 dexterity little keys on one side into a seconds, pegs inserted and
pegboard where the holes have number of dropped pegs
randomly placed slots in which to
t the keys. This is a timed activity
and each hand is tested separately
(dominant and nondominant hands)
Anxiety and Depression Evaluates state (at the Self-report questionnaire in which Four separate scores each for
subscales of the State- moment) and trait (in participant is asked to rate forty state and trait
Trait Personality general) anxiety and statements on a four-point scale,
Inventory42 depression ranging from not at all to very
much so
Wechslers Test of Adult Estimates pre-morbid IQ Participant reads aloud 50 words that Number of correctly pronounced
Reading (WTAR)43 are irregular in their grapheme-to- words
phoneme translation

Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170 163


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
I. T. Lichtwark et al.

over the duration of the study, a one-way repeated mea- Both participants with Marsh 1 lesions had an elevated
sures ANOVA was performed. Correlations between mea- tTG and were HLA-DQ2 positive.
sures across time points were calculated using
Spearmans rho, and the False Discovery Rate was used Changes in biological measures over time
to adjust for type 1 errors.12, 13 The signicance levels At week 0, the median Marsh score was 3b (range 13c),
for the correlations were kept at P < 0.05 to provide a at 12 weeks it was 3a (03a) and by week 52 it had
manageable and meaningful number of correlations. decreased to 1 (03a). Marsh scores improved across the
three time points [v2F(2) = 15.70, P = 0.001; (Friedmans
RESULTS test), Figure 2]. Histological improvement was marked in
the rst 12 weeks (T = 1.15, P = 0.01), but improvement
Characterisation of the participants between weeks 12 and 52 was not signicant
Sixteen participants enrolled for the study. Five with- (P = 0.371). At 52 weeks, mucosal remission was
drew: three due to an inability to meet the time commit- achieved in four and mucosal response (Marsh 1) in ve,
ments, one due to pregnancy before the week 12 while one stayed at Marsh 3a and biopsy data was not
endoscopy and one due to a clinical diagnosis of depres- obtained for the last participant.
sion at week 4 based on analysis of the HADS from the Serum tTG levels signicantly decreased in all patients
screening visit (symptoms that predated the diagnosis of from a mean of 58.4 at baseline to 32.5 at 12 weeks and
CD). Thus, 11 participants with a mean age of 30 (range 16.8 U/mL at 52 weeks [F(1.14, 10.26) = 6.8, P < 0.023,
2239) years completed the 12-month study. Demo- Figure 2]. Further, differences between weeks 0 and 12,
graphical, clinical, laboratory and histopathological char- as well as between weeks 12 and 52, were also signicant
acteristics of the patients at baseline are summarised in [F(1, 9) = 6.15, P = 0.03 and F(1,9) = 5.42, P = 0.04
Table 2. Participants commenced a GFD 426 days respectively]. tTG had normalised in two patients by
(mean 14 days) before study enrolment. Based on dietary 12 weeks and in seven patients at 52 weeks. Normalisa-
history and food diary analysis, adherence to the GFD tion of serum tTG concentrations was associated with
was considered to be excellent in all participants across mucosal healing in the two patients with normalised tTG
all time points in the study. One participant with Marsh levels at 12 weeks, and for ve patients at 52 weeks. For
3c histology and DQ2 haplotype was antibody-negative. the remaining two patients in whom tTG concentrations
fell over time, the duodenal mucosa either did not
improve from the diagnostic biopsy at Marsh 3a or dete-
riorated from Marsh 0 to 1 by 52 weeks. Furthermore,
Table 2 | Characteristics at baseline of the 11 patients
Marsh scores improved in two of the four patients who
studied. Data are shown as (median) where applicable
did not achieve normal tTG levels.
Age (years) 2239 (33) One participant had abnormally high vitamin B12 lev-
Female: gender 8 (73%)
els as a result of supplementation. Mild iron deciency
Body mass index (kg/m2) 23.028.6 (25.3)
Marsh score of duodenal biopsy anaemia (Hb concentration 112 g/L) was present in one
1 2 participant who remained mildly anaemic throughout
2 0 the study. Two other participants were found to be iron
3a 3 decient at baseline, but with normal Hb concentration.
3b 2
3c 4 Only one of the three iron-decient patients was given
Anti-tissue 4203 (49) iron repletion therapy via a total dose iron infusion
transglutaminase IgA (<20 U/mL)* between weeks 12 and 26. The serum ferritin levels over
Haemoglobin (120150 g/L)* 112156 (142) the 12 months are (Figure 2d) did not differ between the
Ferritin (30400 lg/L)* 18399 (71)
B12 (156698 pmol/L)* 215708 (297)
three time points [F(1.08, 8.67) = 0.64, P = 0.46]. Vita-
Vitamin D (>75 nmol/L)* 35165 (71) min D concentrations were low in six participants at
Thyroid stimulating 0.752.58 (1.33) baseline. These remained low in all but one participant
hormone (0.274.2 mIU/L)* despite oral vitamin D replacement therapy (adminis-
Intestinal permeability: 0.040.91 (0.06)
tered to all six). In two further participants, the initially
lactulose:rhamnose ratio (<0.20)*
Intelligence Quotient (IQ) 90112 (114) normal vitamin D levels fell below normal at 12 and
52 weeks. No patient had abnormal thyroid function and
* Normal reference range in parenthesis.
no changes were noted in thyroid function tests or liver

164 Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease

(a) Marsh scores


Baseline
Marsh 3C
Week 12
Week 52
Marsh 3B

Marsh 3A

Marsh 2

Marsh 1

Healed

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Participant number

(b) tTG (c) 1.0 Intestinal permeability


200

0.8 Individual participants

Intestinal permeability
150 Mean IP
Individual participants
Median IP
Mean tTG 0.6
IU/mL

100 Median tTG


0.4

50
0.2

0 0.0
0 12 52 0 12 52
Weeks Weeks

(d) 600 Ferritin

Individual participants
Mean ferritin
Median ferritin
400
mcg/L

200

0
0 12 52
Weeks

Figure 2 | Changes in selected physiological measures during 12 month on a gluten-free diet. (a) Median Marsh
scores for each participant (n = 11) improved signicantly across the three time points (P = 0.001; Friedmans test).
(b) Tissue transglutaminase antibodies concentrations (shown as mean  1 SEM) (n = 11) fell signicantly across the
three time points (P = 0.02). (c, d) Intestinal permeability and serum ferritin are shown as mean  1 SEM (n = 11).
Neither measure differed signicantly between the time points.

Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170 165


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
I. T. Lichtwark et al.

function tests (data not shown). IP was elevated in three P = 0.066). Similarly, the scores for accurately recalling
participants, one of these at baseline the other two at the complex gure of the ROCF after 3 min improved
week 12. All IP levels were within the normal range at from 21.1 to 27.7 (T = 52.00; P = 0.012), and the scores
52 weeks (Figure 2c). Changes in IP across the three for accurately recalling of the ROCF after 30 min
time points were not statistically signicant [F improved from 20.5 to 26.5 (T = 49.0; P = 0.028). The
(2,18) = 0.90, P = 0.42]. changes in performance between weeks 0 and 52 on all
other cognitive tests were not signicant at P < 0.1 and
Cognitive changes over time are not included in Figure 3.
Four of the eight cognitive tests demonstrated a signi-
cant improvement in performance between time 0 and Correlation between cognitive outcomes, other
52 weeks (Figure 3). The time taken to perform the indices and Marsh scores
TRAILS A part of the Trail Making Task improved from Performance on the following cognitive tests and physio-
23.7 s in week 017.0 s in week 52 (T = 6.50; logical measures did not correlate signicantly with
P = 0.032). Mean performance on SCIT-RTH improved Marsh scores across the three time points: SCIT-EH and
from 580 ms in week 0538 ms in week 52 (T = 7.00; -ET, Trail making task parts B and B minus A, ROCF,

(a) SCIT-RTH (b) TRAIL A time


1000 Worse 50 Worse

40 Individual participants
Mean TRAIL A time
800 Median TRAIL A
Individual participants 30
Mean SCIT-RTH scores
ms

Median SCIT-RTH
20
600

10

Better Better
400 0
0 12 52 0 12 52
Week Week

(c) ROCF 3 min delay (d) ROCF 30 min delay


Better Better

30 30
Score

Score

20 20 Individual participants
Individual participants ROCF 30 min mean score
ROCF 3 min mean score Median ROCF 30 min delay

Worse Median ROCF 3 min delay Worse


10 10
0 12 52 0 12 52
Week Week

Figure 3 | Changes in selected cognitive measures during 12 month on a GFD. Results are shown as mean  1 SEM,
(n = 11). Data for SCIT-RT, TRAILS A and tTG are plotted with improvement indicated by a downward trend. The
difference in performance on SCIT-RTH between weeks 0 and 52 was signicant at P = 0.06, for TRAILS A at
P = 0.03, ROCF 3 min P = 0.01 and ROCF 30 min P = 0.028. COWAT differences were not signicant at P = 0.184.

166 Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease

RAVLT immediate recall, and recall at 30 min, STPI, This study provides the beginnings of an evidence-base
Hb, IP, and serum levels of ferritin and vitamins B12 and for the ill-dened, yet frequently reported symptoms of
D. Performance on the following tests correlated signi- brain fog in CD. While participants improved on all nine
cantly with Marsh scores and tTG levels: TRAILS A and cognitive tests, only TRAILS A, ROCF and SCIT-RTH
COWAT at P < 0.01, and SCIT-RTH, SCIT-RTT, RAV- demonstrated a signicant improvement over the course
LT 3 min at P < 0.05. As shown in Figure 4, the correla- of the study. Two of these tests are strongly dependent on
tions (Spearmans r) were large and ranged from 0.377 processing speed: and include speed of response in the
to 0.735. SCIT-RTH, and visuomotor speed in the TRAILS A test.
Performance on the following cognitive tests and ROCF is mainly a test for visuospatial short-term mem-
physiological measures did not correlate signicantly ory. Although improved performance on the TRAILS A
with tTG serum titre across the three time points: SCI- test might be due to practice effects, the published tes-
T-EH and -ET, all of the ROCF measures, RAVLT t-retest reliability over 312 months is high, suggesting
immediate recall and recall at 30 min, STPI, Hb and that any practice effect would be minor.14 Alternative ver-
serum levels of ferritin and vitamins B12 and D. Perfor- sions were used for the ROCF and COWAT, and the
mance on the following tests correlated signicantly with SCIT does not have learning or practice effects.15, 16
tTG levels: SCIT-RTH, SCIT-RTT, TRAILS A, B and It is notable that the level of impairment on the
COWAT (all at P < 0.01) as well as TRAILS B minus SCIT-RTH at week 0 was similar to that in people with a
A, RAVLT1-5, grooved pegboard (dominant and non- blood alcohol level of 0.05 g/100 mL,16 which is the
dominant hands) and IP (at P < 0.05). No meaningful upper legal limit for driving in Australia. It is also equiv-
patterns could be found for the relationships between alent to level of impairment in SCIT-RTH observed in
the cognitive test results and the serum concentrations participants with severe jetlag (SR Robinson & GW
of ferritin, Hb, vitamin B12 and vitamin D, or between Yelland, unpublished data). Further, the 8% relative
cognitive test outcomes and IP for the participants with change in SCIT performance in people with untreated
elevated IP. CD to their paired test after being adherent to the GFD,
was similar to that demonstrated between 0.05 and 0.02
DISCUSSION blood alcohol levels and to the recovery from jetlag over
This pilot study examined the relationships between cog- a 24-h period. In people with undiagnosed CD, such
nitive function and mucosal healing in patients who had cognitive decits might result in impaired performance
been recently diagnosed with CD, and who adhered to a in driving and at work.17 If these ndings are conrmed
GFD over the rst year of treatment. The study demon- in a larger study, they may have important health and
strated the presence of cognitive impairment that safety implications. When viewed together, the present
improved with therapy and was correlated with histolog- results indicate that short-term memory, movement and
ical evidence of mucosal recovery and/or healing. processing speed are impaired in untreated CD and that

0.515**
Tissue
transglutaminase Marsh scores

0.
73
0.

5*
0
50

*
0.494**

0.516**

.4
3*

94
*
0.487**

Figure 4 | Participants **
(n = 11) performance on four 3*
cognitive tests showed 42
0.
0.631**
signicant correlations with SCIT-RTT TRAILS A
** 0
each other and with 6 .5
95 69
histological scores and tTG 0. 0.644** **
**
levels (*P < 0.05, **P < 0.001; .679
SCIT-RTH
0.569** COWAT 0
Spearmans Rho).

Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170 167


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
I. T. Lichtwark et al.

they improve during adherence to a GFD. These impair- of the effect sizes. The strength of these correlations
ments, although subclinical, may contribute to the sensa- raises the interesting possibility that a small battery of
tion of brain fog that is commonly reported by people cognitive tests might offer a noninvasive means to reg-
with CD. ularly screen CD patients for intestinal healing. Cur-
There are three reasons why cognition might be rently, there is need for an accurate biomarker of
impaired in patients with untreated CD. First, nutrient healing that can lessen the requirement for repeated
deciencies involving, for example, iron, vitamin D and intestinal biopsies. A larger prospective study will be
folate have been associated with cognitive impairment.1820 needed to determine whether cognitive tests have suf-
As part of the malabsorption that might occur in patients cient reliability to serve as a proxy of intestinal heal-
with CD, such micronutrient deciencies do occur. In this ing.
study however, changes in iron levels were not associated In conclusion, this pilot study indicates that cogni-
with cognitive performance, and the persisting low levels of tion is impaired in people with untreated CD. Cognitive
vitamin D in six participants argues against a link to the function improves after commencement of a strict
cognitive improvement found over the duration of the GFD, and this improvement is correlated with a nor-
study. malisation of histopathological markers of disease sever-
Second, cognitive impairment in patients with ity. These results support patient reports of brain fog in
untreated CD may be due to the high levels of circulat- untreated CD, and demonstrate that impairments in
ing cytokines associated with systemic inamma- cognitive performance are an additional extra-intestinal
tion.21, 22 Elevated concentrations of circulating or systematic manifestation of CD. Our ndings intro-
cytokines have been associated with changes in behav- duce the possibility that cognitive tests have the poten-
iour, mood and cognition.2329 The brain possesses tial to provide a noninvasive, cost-efcient marker of
receptors for circulating cytokines, whilst cytokine activa- intestinal healing.
tion of neurones and subsequent central signalling may
also be important.26 AUTHORSHIP
Third, the cognitive improvement may be related to Guarantor of the article: G.W. Yelland.
reduced exposure to gluten per se. Animal studies have Author contributions: Irene Lichtwark analysed and
shown dietary gluten may reduce brain tryptophan con- interpreted the psychological test data, wrote the manu-
centrations. Since tryptophan is the precursor of the neu- script and prepared the illustrations. Evan Newnham
rotransmitter, serotonin, it has been speculated that contributed to study design, recruited participants,
gluten may impair cognitive function by lowering brain administered psychological assessments, collected blood
serotonin levels.30 Alternatively, opioid peptides derived samples, collated the biological data and contributed to
from partially digested gluten, so-called exorphins, can writing the clinical sections of the manuscript. Stephen
have several effects on higher brain function in Robinson contributed to study design, data interpretation
rodents,31 although no studies have been reported in and writing of the manuscript. Susan Shepherd per-
humans. Furthermore, changes to the diet can alter the formed dietary/compliance assessments, recruited
gut microbiota, and this has been found to strongly participants and reviewed the manuscript. Patrick
inuence behaviour in rats.3235 Humans might be simi- Hosking interpreted the histology from biopsies. Peter
larly affected, since a recent brain imaging study Gibson contributed to study design, data interpretation
reported that a 4-week intake of probiotic fermented and writing of the manuscript. Gregory Yelland
milk by healthy women affected the activity of brain contributed to study design, administered psychological
regions that control central processing of emotion and assessments, data interpretation and writing of the
sensation.36 manuscript. All authors approved the nal version of the
This study demonstrated that improvements in cog- manuscript.
nition in CD are signicantly correlated with the
extent of intestinal healing, as indicated by Marsh ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
scores. Indeed, the strength of these correlations was as Declaration of personal interests: Susan Shepherd has
good as those obtained for serum tTG. The fact that published several books on coeliac disease and dietary
signicant correlations between disease activity mea- management relevant to coeliac disease. Peter Gibson
sures and cognitive function were obtained with a sam- has published two books on dietary management rele-
ple of just 11 participants, underscores the magnitude vant to coeliac disease. Stephen Robinson is a patent

168 Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Cognitive impairment in coeliac disease

holder for the Subtle Cognitive Impairment Test. Gre- Declaration of funding interests: This study was funded
gory Yelland is a patent holder for the Subtle Cognitive by the Coeliac Research Fund (Coeliac Australia).
Impairment Test.

REFERENCES
1. Kang JY, Kang AHY, Green A, Gwee 14. Sanchez-Cubillo I, Perian~ez JA, 27. Kapadia M, Sakic B. Autoimmune and
KA, Ho KY. Systematic review: Adrover-Roig D, et al. Construct inammatory mechanisms of CNS
worldwide variation in the frequency of validity of the Trail Making Test: role damage. Prog Neurobiol 2011; 95: 301
coeliac disease and changes over time. of task-switching, working memory, 33.
Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2013; 38: 226 inhibition/interference control, and 28. OConnor JJ. Actions of the pro-
45. visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol inammatory cytokine IL-1 beta on
2. Gibson PR, Shepherd SJ, Tye-Din JA. Soc 2009; 15: 43850. central synaptic transmission. Exp
For celiac disease, diagnosis is not 15. Yelland GW, Robinson SR, Friedman Physiol 1999; 84: 601.
enough. Clin Gastroenterol Hepatol T, Hutchinson C. Assessment of 29. Banks WA, Farr SA, Morley JE. Entry
2012 ; 8: 9001. cognitive impairment. Australian of blood-borne cytokines into the
3. Hu WT, Murray JA, Greenaway MC, Ofcial Journal of Patents. Australia: central nervous system: effects on
Parisi JE, Josephs KA. Cognitive Monash University, 2004. cognitive processes.
impairment and celiac disease. Arch 16. Friedman TW, Robinson SR, Yelland Neuroimmunomodulation 2002; 10: 319.
Neurol 2006; 63: 14406. GW. Impaired perceptual judgement at 30. Choi S, DiSilvio B, Fernstrom MH,
4. Currie S, Hadjivassiliou M, Clark M, low blood alcohol concentrations. Fernstrom JD. Meal ingestion, amino
et al. Should we be nervous about Alcohol 2011; 45: 7118. acids and brain neurotransmitters:
coeliac disease? Brain abnormalities in 17. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological effects of dietary protein source on
patients with coeliac disease referred for Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University serotonin and catecholamine synthesis
neurological opinion. J Neurol Press, 2004. rats. Physiol Behav 2009; 98: 15662.
Neurosurg Psychiatry 2012; 83: 1216 18. Murray-Kolb LE, Beard JL. Iron 31. Takahashi M, Fukunaga H, Kaneto H,
21. treatment normalizes cognitive Fukudome S-I, Yoshikawa M.
5. Zelnik N, Pacht A, Obeid R, Lerner A. functioning in young women. Am J Behavioral and pharmacological
Range of neurological disorders in Clin Nutr 2007; 85: 77887. studies on gluten exorphin A5, a newly
patients with celiac disease. Pediatrics 19. Balion C, Grifth L, Strier L, et al. isolated bioactive food protein fragment,
2004; 113: 16726. Vitamin D, cognition, and dementia: a in mice. Jpn J Pharmacol 2000; 84:
6. Sapone A, Bai JC, Ciacci C, et al. systematic review and meta-analysis. 25965.
Spectrum of gluten-related disorders: Neurology 2012; 79: 1397405. 32. Bercik P, Denou E, Collins J. The
consensus on new nomenclature and 20. Ramos M, Allen L, Mungas D, et al. Low intestinal microbiota affect central levels
classication. BMC Med 2012; 10: 13. folate status is associated with impaired of brain-derived neurotropic factor and
7. Neto JIS, Costa ACLV, Magalhaes FG, cognitive function and dementia in the behavior in mice. Gastroenterology
Silva GS. Neurological manifestations of Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging. 2011; 141: 599609. e3.
celiacs disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatri Am J Clin Nutr 2005; 82: 134652. 33. Heijtz RD, Wang S, Anuar F. Normal
2004; 62: 96972. 21. Forton DM, Allsop JM, Main J, Foster gut microbiota modulates brain
8. Bardella MT, Velio P, Cesana BM, et al. GR, Thomas HC, Taylor-Robinson SD. development and behavior. Proc Natl
Coeliac disease: a histological follow-up Evidence for a cerebral effect of the Acad Sci 2011; 108: 304752.
study. Histopathology 2007; 50: 46571. hepatitis C virus. Lancet 2001; 358: 34. Neufeld K, Kang N, Bienenstock J,
9. Anderson J. Celiac Disease and Gluten 389. Foster J. Reduced anxiety-like behavior
Sensitivity, 2013. Available at: http:// 22. Senzolo M, Schiff S, DAloiso C, et al. and central neurochemical change in
celiacdisease.about.com/od/symptom Neuropsychological alterations in germ-free mice. Neurogastroenterol
sofceliacdisease/a/Celiac-Disease-Brain- hepatitis C infection: the role of Motil 2011; 23: 255e119.
Fog.htm. Accessed December 8, 2013. inammation. World J Gastroenterol 35. Cryan JF, Dinan TG. Mind-altering
10. Bruck K, Bosch S, M uller CA, et al. 2011; 17: 336974. microorganisms: the impact of the gut
Sporadic cerebellar ataxia associated 23. Maier SF. Cytokines for psychologists: microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat
with gluten sensitivity. Brain 2001; 124: implications of bidirectional immune- Rev Neurosci 2012; 13: 70112.
10139. to-brain communication for 36. Tillisch K, Labus J, Kilpatrick L.
11. Lurie Y, Landau DA, Pfeffer J, Oren R. understanding behavior, mood, and Consumption of fermented milk
Celiac disease diagnosed in the elderly. cognition. Psychol Rev 1998; 105: 83. product with probiotic modulates brain
J Clin Gastroenterol 2008; 42: 24. Turrin NP, Plata-Salaman CR. activity. Gastroenterology 2013; 144:
5961. Cytokinecytokine interactions and the 1394401.
12. Howell D. Statistical Methods for brain. Brain Res Bull 2000; 51: 39. 37. Reitan RM. The relation of the trail
Psychology. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 25. Larson SJ, Dunn AJ. Behavioral effects making test to organic brain damage.
2013. of cytokines. Brain Behav Immun 2001; Consult Psychol 1955; 19: 3934.
13. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling 15: 37187. 38. Knight JA, Kaplan E. The Handbook of
the false discovery rate: a practical and 26. Wilson CJ. Cytokines and cognition Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Usage:
powerful approach to multiple testing. J the case for a head-to-toe inammatory Clinical and Research Applications.
Stat Soc Series B (Methodological) 1995; paradigm. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008; 50: Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment
57: 289300. 2041. Resources, Inc., 2003.

Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170 169


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
I. T. Lichtwark et al.

39. Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of instructions. Available at: http://www. 43. Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult
Neuropsychological Tests. New York, si-instruments.com.au/product/show/ Reading. San Antonio, TX:
NY: Oxford University Press, 1991. 146/grooved-pegboard-test/. Accessed Psychological Corporation, 2001.
40. Rey A. Lexamen clinique en June 25, 2005.
psychologie. Paris, France: Presses 42. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Personality
Universitaires de France, 1964. Inventory (STPI) Research and Manual
41. Lafayette Instrument Company. (2002) Sampler Set. Tampa, FL: Mind Garden,
Grooved pegboard test user Inc, 1995.

170 Aliment Pharmacol Ther 2014; 40: 160-170


2014 John Wiley & Sons Ltd

S-ar putea să vă placă și