Sunteți pe pagina 1din 16

Learning to Think in a Second

Language: Effects of Proficiency


and Length of Exposure in
English Learners of German
PANOS ATHANASOPOULOS JULIE BURNAND
Lancaster University University of Chester
Department of Linguistics and English Language Department of Psychology
County South Parkgate Road
Bailrigg Chester CH1 4BJ
Lancaster LA1 4YL United Kingdom
United Kingdom Email: julieburnand@gmail.com
Email: p.athanasopoulos@lancaster.ac.uk
EMANUEL BYLUND
LJUBICA DAMJANOVIC Stockholm University & Stellenbosch University,
University of Chester Centre for Research on Bilingualism
Department of Psychology Department of Swedish and Multilingualism
Parkgate Road Universitetsvagen 10C
Chester CH1 4BJ 10691 Stockholm
United Kingdom Sweden
Email: l.damjanovic@chester.ac.uk Email: manne.bylund@biling.su.se

The aim of the current study is to investigate motion event cognition in second language learners in a
higher education context. Based on recent findings that speakers of grammatical aspect languages like
English attend less to the endpoint (goal) of events than do speakers of nonaspect languages like Swedish
in a nonverbal categorization task involving working memory (Athanasopoulos & Bylund, 2013; Bylund &
Athanasopoulos, 2015), the current study asks whether native speakers of an aspect language start paying
more attention to event endpoints when learning a nonaspect language. Native English and German (a
nonaspect language) speakers, and English learners of L2 German, who were pursuing studies in
German language and literature at an English university, were asked to match a target scene with
intermediate degree of endpoint orientation with two alternate scenes with low and high degree of
endpoint orientation, respectively. Results showed that, compared to the native English speakers, the
learners of German were more prone to base their similarity judgements on endpoint saliency, rather
than ongoingness, primarily as a function of increasing L2 proficiency and year of university study.
Further analyses revealed a nonlinear relationship between length of L2 exposure and categorization
patterns, subserved by a progressive strengthening of the relationship between L2 proficiency and
categorization as length of exposure increased. These findings present evidence that cognitive
restructuring may occur through increasing experience with an L2, but also suggest that this relationship
may be complex and unfold over a long period of time.
Keywords: German as a foreign language; grammatical aspect; boundedness; linguistic relativity;
instructed SLA; motion events

The Modern Language Journal, 99, Supplement, (2015) THE IDEA THAT THE NATIVE LANGUAGE
DOI: 10.1111/modl.12183 can affect thinking and reasoning about the
0026-7902/15/138153 $1.50/0 perceived world has a long history in scholarly
2015 The Modern Language Journal thought. In modern cognitive science, this idea is
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 139

often referred to as the linguistic relativity obligatorily mark progressive aspect to express
hypothesis (Whorf, 1956). While the hypothesis ongoingness (e.g., John is walking) attend less to
has evolved and diversified into more fine-grained the endpoint (goal) of an action than do speakers
proposals (see Wolff & Holmes, 2010, for a of nonaspect languages like Swedish (where
detailed overview), the basic principle is that ongoingness can be optionally expressed outside
cross-linguistic differences in the semantic parti- of the main verb through adverbials or other
tioning of reality (language diversity) may give lexical means) in a nonverbal similarity judge-
rise to cross-linguistic differences in thought, the ment task involving working memory (Athanaso-
latter typically operationalized as nonverbal poulos & Bylund, 2013). Existing data provide
behaviour related to a range of different cognitive some evidence that functional multilingual speak-
processes, such as reasoning, classification, and ers whose L1 lacks grammatical aspect (and thus
categorical perception (Lucy, 1997). Recent promotes attention to endpoints) but whose L2
studies show effects of linguistic categories on marks progressive aspect grammatically (and thus
cognitive processes in a variety of domains, such as defocuses endpoints) are influenced by their L2
colour (Regier & Kay, 2009), grammatical num- in their event categorization behaviour as a
ber (Lucy & Gaskins, 2003), grammatical gender function of frequency of L2 use and exposure
(Boutonnet, Athanasopoulos, & Thierry, 2012), to the L2 as the language of schooling in
space (Majid et al., 2004), time (Casasanto, 2008), childhood (Bylund et al., 2013; Bylund &
and manner and path of motion (Kersten et al., Athanasopoulos, 2014a). It seems that going
2010). While the extent of linguistic influence from an L1 that lacks grammaticized means to
may vary as a function of domain and task and express progressive aspect to an L2 where such
while the reported effects do not go uncontested means are applied obligatorily may have pro-
(see, e.g., Barner, Inagaki, & Li, 2009; January & found consequences for event cognition.
Kako, 2007; Li & Gleitman, 2002), the linguistic This article contributes to this line of investiga-
relativity hypothesis forms a core part of the tion in two ways. First, we explore whether
investigation into human development and cognitive restructuring is possible when going in
behaviour throughout the cognitive and social the other direction, that is, when the L1 is a
sciences. [ aspect] language and the L2 is a [ aspect]
Given the substantial cross-linguistic diversity language. One may assume that cognitive re-
that exists in how languages semantically partition structuring occurs in the direction of [ aspect]
reality and the correlation of this diversity with L1 to [ aspect] L2 as a function of the
different cognitive patterns in monolingual acquisition of a new, obligatorily applied gram-
populations, recent studies have investigated matical category (in this case, progressive aspect).
the linguistic relativity hypothesis through the However, going in the opposite direction, there
lens of bilingualism. The main question here are no obligatory grammatical categories to be
takes as its starting point cognitive differences in acquired. Therefore one could assume that
monolingual populations in a specific domain cognitive restructuring may not occur at all, since
and examines the extent to which language- obligatoriness of application of a grammatical
specific cognition in the same domain is resistant category may be a key factor in the extent of
to L2 influence, or whether cognitive patterns linguistic influence on cognition (Lucy, 1992).
may change under the influence of the L2 (Jarvis Second, we examine the idea that linguistic
& Pavlenko, 2008). Studies to date have shown relativity effects emerge in humans primarily as
that cognitive categorization may be impervious a function of degree of exposure to a specific
to L2 influence in intermediate L2 users (Atha- language. Recent attempts to identify the theo-
nasopoulos, 2006) or late bilinguals (Boroditsky, retical underpinnings of cognitive restructuring
Schmidt, & Phillips, 2003), but may be influenced in L2 users have adopted models of associative
by the L2 once an advanced level of proficiency learning (see subsequent sections) that assume
has been reached (Athanasopoulos & Kasai, that changes in cognition emerge gradually as a
2008), in early bilinguals (Boroditsky, 2001), function of the degree of exposure to specific
and as a function of language mode (Kersten formmeaning associations, which then become
et al., 2010). part of an individuals cognitive routine (Bylund
The current study is located within the & Athanasopoulos, 2014b; Casasanto, 2008;
grammatical aspect approach to motion event Kersten et al., 2010). If that is so, the longer
cognition (von Stutterheim et al., 2012). More learners are exposed to a second language with
specifically, it builds on recent evidence that grammatical properties different from the first,
native speakers of languages like English that the more their cognitive dispositions are likely to
140 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)

shift toward those of native speakers of the L2. scene ends. An observer who is asked to describe
Thus the current study focuses on length of L2 this scene may choose to adopt a holistic
exposure in additional language learners who are perspective, or a maximal temporal viewing frame,
not immersed in the target language context and according to which the event is interpreted in its
on the evolution of relationships between other entirety, including the endpoint (i.e., two men
biographical variables and event cognition as such walking to a bridge). The observer, however, could
length unfolds. also adopt an alternative interpretation, or
construal, choosing that of an immediate tempo-
ral viewing frame, whereby only the ongoing
BACKGROUND phase of the event is zoomed in on and the event
Theoretical Approach endpoint is excluded (i.e., two men walking)
(Langacker, 1987, 2008). A growing number of
The current study is located within the studies have shown that the choice of event
theoretical framework of Cognitive Linguistics construal (holistic or immediate) is tightly linked
(CL) (see Evans, Bergen, & Zinken, 2007; to the presence or absence of the grammatical
Geeraerts & Cuyckens, 2007, for detailed insight- category of aspect in the observers/speakers
ful introductions), which places the task of native language. Evidence from speech produc-
language learning within general principles of tion data demonstrates that when describing
human cognitive processing. It views language not unfolding goal-oriented motion events, speakers
as an autonomous modular system with its own of languages with grammatical aspect are more
rules and completely unrelated to other cognitive prone to adopting an immediate viewing frame
processes, but as an integrative component of and exclude reference to endpoints. Speakers of
general human cognitive processing. As such, it languages lacking grammatical aspect, in con-
resonates with linguistic relativity and Thinking trast, show a higher preference to take a maximal
for Speaking approaches to L2 learning (Pav- viewing frame, mentioning the event endpoint.
lenko, 2011), and lends importance to nonverbal This finding has been shown to be consistent
behaviour alongside verbal behaviour in describ- across a number of different languages, such as
ing phenomena of language learning, represen- Algerian Arabic, English, Russian, Spanish (as-
tation, and use. pect languages), and Dutch, German, Norwegian,
Three central concepts or key insights from a Swedish (nonaspect languages) (Bylund, 2011;
Cognitive Linguistic approach to L2 learning are Schmiedtova & Flecken, 2008; von Stutterheim,
relevant for the present investigation, namely 1997, 2003).
construal, categorization, and the usage-based Varying the construal of the same event may
nature of language. The first two refer to the basic have immediate consequences for the second
cognitive operations at play here. The third refers cognitive process mentioned earlier, namely
to the learning mechanism to which these categorization. Generally speaking, categoriza-
operations are amenable. tion is an essential element of human cognition.
Construal refers to the ability to take different Indeed, under many cognitive linguistic and
perspectives or interpretations of an event or, for psychological accounts cognition is essentially
that matter, any perceptual phenomenon (Lan- categorization (Harnad, 1987, 2005; Roberson,
gacker, 1987, 1991). In his discussion on the 2005). The central operating principle of this
similarity between lexical and grammatical cate- process is that similarity is the basis of categorza-
gories, Langacker (2008) postulates that gram- tion (Ameel et al., 2005; Nosofsky, 1986), a
matical constructions are formmeaning pairings position that constitutes the basic methodological
above the word level, with cross-linguistic differ- tenet of modern neo-Whorfian investigations,
ences in grammar essentially referring to differ- where nonverbal behaviour is often operational-
ent conventionalized linguistic constructions for ised along a continuum of cognitive tasks with an
encoding different construals of the same exter- inherent categorization component (Casasanto,
nal event. A change in grammatical or syntactic 2008; Lucy, 1997). A classic example of this is the
form entails a change in meaning, which in turn triads matching task, in which participants are
entails a shift in construal. Grammatical aspect, asked to match a target stimulus with one out of
the focus of the current study, is a primary two alternate stimuli. Among other domains, such
linguistic marker of different temporal con- as object categorization (Lucy, 1992) and colour
struals. Imagine a scene where two men are categorical perception (Roberson, 2005), the
walking along a path, at the end of which there is a triads matching paradigm has also been success-
bridge. Before the men reach the bridge, the fully implemented in the domain of grammatical
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 141

aspect and event construal. The basic assumption associative learning has gained some prominence
is that if two events are construed (interpreted) in L1 development accounts (Colunga & Smith,
similarly, then they will tend to be perceived and 2005; Samuelson, 2002; Smith, 2000, 2003), in L2
categorized as more similar in a triads matching acquisition research (Ellis, 1998, 2002, 2006) and
task. in Cognitive Linguistic theory of language (By-
Athanasopoulos and Bylund (2013) examined bee, 2006; Ellis & FerreiraJunior, 2009). More
the influence of grammatical aspect on non- recently, it has been proposed as a theoretical
verbal event cognition in a triads matching framework to explain linguistic relativity effects
design, asking participants to watch triads of (Casasanto, 2008; Kersten et al., 2010) and
video clips showing motion events with different cognitive restructuring in L2 acquisition/multi-
degrees of goal orientation. The target clip lingualism (Bylund & Athanasopoulos, 2014b).
showed motion toward a goal (intermediate The starting point in linking associative learning
degree of goal orientation), whereas the alter- to cognitive restructuring in L2 is the observation
nates showed either motion without an obvious that representations in memory are multi-modal
endpoint (low degree of goal orientation) or and massively interconnected. Thus, when par-
motion with arrival at a goal (high degree of goal ticipants hear verbs like kick and grasp, and
orientation). The clips were presented in three when participants actually kick or grasp some-
different conditions: one where the clips in each thing, neuroscientific evidence shows that the
triad were played simultaneously in a loop until same neural assemblies are activated (Kiefer &
the participant had made his/her judgment Pulvermuller, 2012). Processing of colour words
online condition; one where the clips played activates both the expected language areas as well
one after another and the participant provided as areas in the visual cortex associated with colour
his/her judgment after having watched the last vision (Regier & Kay, 2009). Representations then
clip in each triad memory condition; and one build up, or emerge, over exposure to a number
similar to the memory condition with the excep- of specific instances of associations. For example,
tion that the participant had to repeat a string of speakers of English will form distinct form
digits while watching each triad verbal inference meaning representations for blue and green,
condition. whereas speakers of Berinmo and Himba (whose
The results showed that in the memory languages have only one term for blue and green)
condition speakers of Swedish paired the target will associate only one term with the same physical
clip with the high degree alternate significantly space of the colour spectrum, because during L1
more than English speakers did. No cognitive development two distinct terms will statistically co-
differences between the two populations of occur with two different visual representations in
speakers were found in the other conditions. the input English children are exposed to,
Athanasopoulos and Bylund (2013) argued that whereas for Berinmo and Himba children only
the reason why differences between groups were one term will frequently co-occur with visual
found in the memory condition was due to the representations of colour space that English
fact that working memory is, to some extent, speakers would normally distinguish into green
verbally mediated (Baddeley, 2003). These results and blue (Roberson et al., 2004).
thus suggest that the effects of grammatical aspect Linguistic relativity effects arise because we
on nonverbal event cognition are primarily draw on co-occurrence patterns that we have
confined to situations where the speaker has to encountered when we construe and categorize a
commit facts to memory and is able to rely on specific event, and when we encounter a new
language to do so. Such nonverbal behaviour event. Regularities in categorization emerge as a
open to verbal mediation probably forms the function of the degree of exposure to specific
majority of real-life instances of categorization associations, which then become part of an
and higher level problem solving in humans individuals cognitive routine. The more routin-
(Kersten et al., 2010). ized an association becomes, the easier it is to
How do such effects play out in instances of retrieve and utilize it for purposes of categoriza-
multilingualism and additional language learn- tion (Langacker, 2008). Indeed, the evidence
ing? To begin to answer this question, we turn to shows that the emergence of cross-linguistic
the third key insight from a Cognitive Linguistic differences in colour categorization is a graded,
account of L2 learning, namely the usage-based linear process, occurring as soon as children show
nature of language that is anchored to more signs of acquisition of their language-specific
general principles of associative learning. Over colour lexicons, and increasing with age (Franklin
the past 10 years or so, the psychological theory of et al., 2008; Roberson et al., 2004). Similar
142 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)

developmental trends have been reported for ing by new information. This observation, along
categorization of objects and substances (Imai & with the multifaceted instantiations of L2 linguis-
Gentner, 1997; Lucy & Gaskins, 2001). At the tic experience that are subject to considerable
same time, because we are continuously exposed individual differences, means that the degree of
to novel events throughout our lifetime represen- strength of the direction of this interaction (L1 to
tation is subject to constant restructuring. The L2, and L2 to L1) will depend on a combination of
individual constantly updates the relative statisti- the individual learners history, as well as the
cal weighting of the recurring associations against specific linguistic categories under investigation.
new contexts and new instances of associations
(Ellis, 2008). The question of interest from an L2 Grammatical Aspect and Motion Event Construal in
learning perspective then concerns the extent to L2 Users
which construal and categorization are affected
by the acquisition of novel formmeaning associ- Based on the robust evidence that speakers of
ations in the L2, and the factors that modulate aspect languages tend to exclude endpoints when
internalization of these associations. describing dynamic goal-oriented stimuli, while
From an associative learning perspective, the speakers of nonaspect languages display a
key factor is increasing experience with specific tendency instead to focus on the endpoints in
formmeaning associations. However, the con- their verbal descriptions, a recent study by Bylund
struct of learning experience itself is multiface- and Jarvis (2011) analyzed the relation between
ted, and must be operationalized in light of the grammatical aspect and verbal processing of
learning context. For example, in L1 acquisition, endpoints vs. ongoingness in goal-oriented
experience is instantiated as repeated exposure to motion events in SpanishSwedish bilinguals.
the ambient language environment pre- and post- They asked their participants to describe the
natally, which has specific consequences for how scenes in Spanish (their L1). Results showed that
infants understand phonemic features of the the bilinguals behaved differently from Spanish
ambient language and begin segmenting the monolinguals. First, bilingual speakers produced
input based on statistical regularities (Johnson & more endpoints in motion events and simple verb
Jusczyk, 2001) and for how they construct forms than Spanish monolinguals. Second, profi-
morphophonological representations (Szagun, ciency in L1, measured through the ability to
2001). Frequency of exposure to specific word identify aspectual errors in a grammaticality
classes has consequences for categorization of judgment test, was the best predictor of endpoint
objects and events (Gopnik, Choi, & Baumberger, encoding: The weaker the bilinguals command
1996). Arguably, for L2 learners, the construct of of Spanish aspectual distinctions, the higher his/
experience can be manifested in even more her endpoint encoding frequencies. This finding
complex ways than in L1 development. A possible provides evidence of a direct link between
outcome of experience with an L2 is increasing grammatical aspect and endpoint encoding.
expertise or proficiency in the second language. Age of onset of L2 acquisition was also a
But such expertise can be modulated by a significant factor that negatively correlated with
multitude of other instrumentations of experi- endpoint encoding frequencies in the L1.
ence, such as frequency of speaking a particular Flecken (2011) investigated effects of gram-
language, age of onset of L2 learning, context of matical aspect on verbal encoding in early
use, which can take the form of learning setting bilingual speakers of Dutch (an aspect language)
(naturalistic, instructed) and language setting and German (a nonaspect language). Her main
(L1 vs. L2 monolingual speaking environments vs. question was the extent to which these early and
bi- or multilingual speaking environments), simultaneous bilinguals would encode endpoints
length of immersion in a specific speaking in their L1 speech, thus showing an effect of the
environment, and so on (for a comprehensive L2 on the L1. Additionally, eye movements were
discussion of such variables, see Bylund & recorded with eye-tracking equipment before and
Athanasopoulos, 2014b). during the description task in order to study
In addition to the diverse instantiations of planning and organization of content that was
experience that the process of L2 learning may going to be expressed. The results showed that
entail, the L2 learner already has a rich set of bilinguals displayed patterns of language use that
conceptual categories associated with L1 linguis- were dissimilar to either monolingual norm.
tic categories. In an associative learning account, Specifically, these early bilinguals frequently
such past knowledge will inevitably interact with used the progressive form in Dutch, but they
new information and is always open to restructur- also tended to combine progressive aspect with
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 143

the mention of endpoints, a combination that is the act is complete and the conceptual viewer
not at all frequent in monolingual Dutch speak- can construe the boundaries of the action. This
ers. The eye-tracking analysis demonstrated that analysis is entirely compatible with Langackers
bilinguals also allocated attention differently than theoretical framework presented earlier. Imme-
the monolingual groups. They tended to look diate viewing frames entail unbounded con-
earlier and for longer periods to the action than struals, whereas holistic viewing frames entail
to the agent of the event, and this correlated with bounded construals. The presence or absence of
a high frequency of use of progressive forms, grammatical aspect in a language may serve as a
showing a tight link between frequency of use of reliable and distinctive cue to the construal of
specific linguistic features and attention alloca- events as bounded or unbounded and thus direct
tion to specific aspects of a dynamic event. differential attention to event boundaries such as
More recently, studies have also attempted to endpoints.
study motion event categorization outside of overt Reinterpreting the findings of Athanasopoulos
speech production. Bylund and Athanasopoulos and Bylund (2013) and subsequent studies
(2014a) correlated frequency of language use (Bylund et al., 2013; Bylund & Athanasopoulos,
with categorization tendencies in native speakers 2014a) under Niemeier and Reifs (2008) acco-
of IsiXhosa who were multilingual in a number of unt, native speakers of [ aspect] languages
languages. The researchers repeated the memory tend to focus more on the unbounded nature of
version of Athanasopoulos and Bylunds (2013) actions as a basis for categorization than native
study, which had shown cognitive differences speakers of [ aspect] languages do, because
between English and Swedish speaking popula- [ aspect] languages offer more linguistic cues
tions. IsiXhosa being a nonaspect language, the (such as obligatory morphological marking) to
researchers investigated how the knowledge and their speakers to construe events as unbounded
use of additional languages with grammatical than [ aspect] languages do. This clarifies what is
aspect may influence cognition of endpoint- at stake in an L2 learning context between certain
oriented motion events in these functional multi- language pairs. If the second language employs
linguals. Results from a triads matching task grammaticized progressive aspect then it offers an
showed that multilinguals who often used aspect important linguistic cue to construe events as
languages and had greater exposure to English unbounded. If the second language does not
(an aspect language) in primary education were obligatorily apply such aspectual means to express
less prone to relying on endpoints when making ongoingness, then this is a reliable cue to
their categorization decisions. Bylund et al. interpret events as bounded (unless some adver-
(2013) additionally examined event categoriza- bial or other means is used to specifically denote
tion in L1 speakers of Afrikaans (a nonaspect the unbounded nature of the event in nonaspect
language) who had English as an L2. Results from languages). Learning and using an L2 with
a triads matching task showed that those who used different aspectual characteristics than the L1
English more often were less prone to relying on effectively means learning to adjust the relative
the reaching of endpoints while categorizing the weight of boundedness and unboundedness as a
motion events, thus exhibiting categorization basis for categorization. Under this interpreta-
behaviour closer to that of English speakers. tion, going from a nonaspect to an aspect
While the recent studies show a tight link language should have similar consequences for
between grammatical aspect and motion event categorization as going from an aspect language
construal, some theoretical frameworks view to a nonaspect language. In the former case,
grammatical aspect as the linguistic realization linguistic cues to construe events as unbounded
of a more general cognitive representation, that will increase, while in the latter case, such cues are
of boundedness. Specifically, Niemeier and Reifs absent, promoting a construal of events as
(2008) pedagogically oriented analysis of gram- bounded.
matical aspect posits that sentences containing Under an associative learning account, the key
progressive aspect such as Mr. Karabatsos is to such readjustment is cumulative experience
walking entail an unbounded construal, since the with the second language. Indeed, the studies
focus is on the ongoing nature of the action, reviewed in the previous section are in line with
leaving out the boundaries (i.e., the source and the idea that the strength of the weightings of
the goal of the action). When no progressive the elements that make up conceptual represen-
marker is used, such as in the sentence Mr. tation of motion events as a basis for categori-
Karabatsos walked (home/to the bowling lane), zation will change as a result of individual
the speaker adopts a bounded construal, because differences in experience, with experience
144 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)

typically operationalized as frequency of language second research question addresses the following
use and proficiency. issue:

RQ2. Does length of exposure to a second


Aims of the Current Study language with grammatical properties
that differ from those of the first affect
The current study focuses on native speakers of the likelihood that learners cognitive
English who are users of L2 German. The study dispositions will shift toward those of native
does not distinguish between L2 learners and speakers of the L2?
L2 users because the students in our sample use
the L2 as part of their university degree program
and continue to be L2 learners. METHOD
German, like Swedish, is a nonaspect language,
and previous studies have established that Participants
German speakers tend to focus on endpoints in
both their verbal descriptions and in their The participants were 14 monolingual English-
attention allocation as measured by eye-tracking speaking adults who were students in the United
(von Stutterheim et al., 2012). We thus first Kingdom (UK) at the University of Bangor, 14
seek to establish whether these cross-linguistic monolingual German-speaking adults who were
differences occur in a nonverbal paradigm students in Germany1 at Otto-von-Guericke Uni-
(Athanasopoulos & Bylund, 2013); after estab- versity, and 76 native speakers of English who were
lishing that they do we compare the behaviour using L2 German as part of their degree in
of monolinguals to that of the L2 user group, German language or German literature at a UK
and investigate the several variables (see university (recruited from Bangor and Chester
Table 1) that may modulate L2 user behaviour Universities, henceforth referred to as L2 users).
in general terms. This leads to our first research Permission to recruit and advertise the study was
question: granted by the programme leaders of the target
courses of study, who also allowed access to
RQ1. Does learning an L2 with fewer cues for
students on a group testing basis. Given the lack of
unboundedness (such as the absence of a formal measure of general proficiency in the
progressive aspect) shift categorization German language that can be administered
behaviour toward a bounded construal, quickly, we asked our participants to self-assess
indexed by an increased preference for their German proficiency on a scale of 1 poor to
motion endpoints as a basis for nonverbal 4 advanced in six different categories, namely
similarity judgements? speaking, understanding, writing, reading, gram-
mar, and pronunciation (following previous
We then focus on those L2 users who began similar studies like Flecken, 2011, and a large
learning the L2 sequentially via formal classroom body of L2 acquisition research that uses self-
instruction and who have had minimal or no ratings in the absence of more formal proficiency
immersion in the L2-speaking environment. We measures, see Marian, Blumenfeld, & Kaushan-
investigate effects of length of L2 exposure in an skaya, 2007, for a review). By means of a
L1 setting and the relationship between cognitive biographical background questionnaire, we also
behaviour with other factors in L2 users with recorded the L2 learners age of L2 acquisition
different periods of L2 exposure. Thus, our (defined here as the age at which they self-

TABLE 1
Summary of EnglishL2 German Learners Details

Measure Mean (Range)


Self-rated L2 proficiency 3.09/4 (14)
L2 acquisition age 12 years old (250 years old)
Length of stay in German-speaking country 3 months (027 months)
German-speaking media exposure in hours per week 2.31 (050)
Hours per week interacting with German native speakers 8.76 (080)
Degree of enjoyment of learning L2 German on a 15 cm 11.79 cm (7 cm14 cm)
visual analogue scale
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 145

reported first started learning German), the time classroom after the end of their tutorial, where
they had previously spent in a German-speaking they were introduced to the experimenter. The
country, the amount of exposure to German- experimenter then gave task instructions to the
speaking media (including printed press, radio, group and distributed informed consent forms
television, and internet, measured in hours per and biographical questionnaires for students to
week), the amount of interaction with German complete while the projector screen was set up for
native speakers (measured in hours per week), the experiment. Once all details had been filled
and how much they enjoy learning German in, the main experiment was administered using
(measured on a 15 cm visual analogue scale, PowerPoint. Participants were informed that they
where 0 cm was not enjoyed at all and 15 cm was would see video clips arranged in triads on the
enjoyed very much). Table 1 provides a summary computer screen, with clip A appearing first, then
of these details. clip B, and finally clip X (the target). Participants
Participants ranged in age from 1851 years; all were instructed to indicate whether they thought
came from lower middle class suburban back- clip X was more similar to clip A or more similar to
grounds. They were given a monetary reward for clip B, by circling the relevant answer on their
their participation. participant response sheet. Thirty-eight triads
were thus presented in an ABX format, where
Materials in a counter-balanced design half of the time the
[ endpoint] alternate appeared first (clip A) and
We elicited nonverbal similarity judgement half of the time it appeared second (clip B), and
data using the memory triads matching task used vice versa for the [ endpoint] alternate. All 38
previously in Athanasopoulos and Bylund (2013). triads were presented in different random orders
The stimuli consisted of 31 video clips from the across groups. The precise sequence of the clips in
stimulus pool of the research group of von each triad was as follows: Clip A played, followed
Stutterheim, Carroll, and Schmiedtova (see e.g., by clip B, followed by clip X. Participants were
Athanasopoulos & Bylund, 2013; Schmiedtova, instructed to give their responses only after they
Carroll, & von Stutterheim, 2011), and were used had watched clip X in its entirety. Clips played
in all permissible combinations to create 19 triads, immediately after one another, with no pause in
consisting of a target clip and two alternate clips. between. Inter-trial interval was self-paced until all
The target clip was a scene with an intermediate participants within the group had circled their
degree of goal orientation in which there was a answer, at which point the experimenter initiated
visible, possible endpoint for the motion, but its the next trial.
arrival was not overtly shown (for example a person
walking toward a cafe). One type of alternate, the RESULTS
so-called [ endpoint] alternate, was a video with a
low degree of goal orientation showing an entity We first examined the overall pattern of results
moving along a trajectory without an obvious (shown in Table 2) by conducting a one-way
endpoint (for example a person walking along ANOVA with Group (English, German, and L2
a road). The other type of alternate, the so-called users) as the independent variable and the
[ endpoint] alternate, was a video with a high number of times participants matched the target
level of goal orientation that contained a moving clip (X) with the [ endpoint] alternate (indicat-
entity that actually reached an endpoint (for ing an endpoint preference) as the dependent
example a person walking into a building). Triads variable. This ANOVA showed a significant main
were created (and clips edited where necessary) in effect of Group, F(2, 101) 5.46, p < 0.05.
such a way as to control for manner and direction
of motion and number of agents. All clips were 6
seconds long. The videos had been normed for TABLE 2
visual similarity in the study of Athanasopoulos Mean Endpoint Preferences (%) Across Groups in
and Bylund (2013), such that the [ endpoint] the Triads Matching Task.
alternates looked similar to the [ endpoint]
Endpoint
alternates.
Groups Preference (%)
German monolinguals 36.62 (8.86)
Procedure English users of L2 German 29.16 (12.34)
Students were tested after their small group English monolinguals 22.37 (7.57)
tutorials at the university. They remained in the Note. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.
146 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)

Post-hoc Scheffe tests confirmed that the differ- proportion of all 76 L2 users [ endpoint]
ence was located between the two monolingual preferences was entered into a Pearson correla-
groups, with German monolinguals selecting tion analysis, along with their year of university
significantly more [ endpoint] alternates than study, their self-perceived L2 proficiency, mean
English monolinguals, p < 0.05. The L2 user age of L2 acquisition, mean length of stay in a
group did not differ significantly from either German-speaking country, mean exposure to
monolingual group, a pattern typically observed German-speaking media, mean hours per week
in bilingual cognition studies (Athanasopoulos, interacting with German native speakers, and
2011). mean enjoyment of learning German as an L2. As
We then sought to probe further the perfor- expected based on the previous parametric tests,
mance of the L2 users according to their year of proportion of [ endpoint] preference was
German studies at university. To this end, we significantly correlated with the L2 users year
conducted another one-way ANOVA, this time of university study, r .461, p < 0.05. The higher
dividing the L2 users into three different groups. the number of the year of study, the more
This yielded 39 first-year students, 17 second-year participants were prone to select the [ end-
students, and 20 fourth-year students (we could point] alternate. The L2 users [ endpoint]
not get access to third-year students because they preferences also correlated significantly with their
study abroad in a German-speaking country). We self-rated L2 proficiency, r .285, p < 0.05. As
compared similarity judgements using the same proficiency advances, so does the tendency to
dependent variable as before in these three show preference for the [ endpoint] alternate.
groups, as well as the two monolingual groups. The proportion of [ endpoint] preferences did
Group averages are shown in Figure 1. The not correlate significantly with any of the other
ANOVA showed a significant main effect of variables.
group, F(4, 99) 7.37, p < 0.05. Post-hoc Scheffe We then focused on length of L2 exposure.
tests revealed that the fourth-year L2 users and the Because the group of L2 users included individu-
German monolinguals selected significantly more als who had begun learning German simulta-
[ endpoint] alternates than the English mono- neously or shortly after English in a naturalistic
linguals and the first-year L2 users did, p < 0.05 in setting (see the age of L2 acquisition variable in
all cases. This indicates that significant shifts in L2 Table 1), we targeted those L2 users that began
users motion event cognition can come about learning German exclusively and continuously
when going from a [ aspect] language to a [ through formal instruction in secondary school,
aspect] language, as a function of experience of high school, college, or university. In addition, we
studying for a German language related degree. excluded from the subsequent analysis individua-
The next step in the analysis probed the ls who self-reported that they had lived in a
linguistic or extra-linguistic factors that modulate German-speaking country for more than
the degree of L2 user cognitive shift. Thus, the 14 months. Given that previous studies show L2
cognitive effects only after 24 months (Athanaso-
FIGURE 1 poulos et al., 2010) or 36 months (Cook et al.,
Endpoint Preferences (%) in the Triads Matching 2006) in the L2-speaking country, it would be
Task Across German and English Monolinguals, and reasonable to assume that this variable would have
English Users of L2 German Split by Year of German minimal effect in this sample. These measures
Studies at University together would ensure homogeneity in terms of
learning context (primarily instructed), age of
70
onset of bilingualism (late childhood learners),
60
and length of immersion in the L2-speaking
Endpoint preference (%)

50 country (minimal), and thus isolate the variable


40 of length of L2 exposure as much as possible in
30 order to study its effects on cognition. Sixty-three
20
individuals were thus selected for further analysis.
A one-way ANOVA with Group (English and
10
German monolinguals, and L2 users) as the
0
English Year 1 Year 2 Year 4 German
independent variable and proportion of [
monolinguals students students students monolinguals endpoint] preference as the dependent variable
Group
yielded a significant main effect of group,
Note. Columns show group means. Error bars show F(2, 88) 5.43, p < 0.05, with the difference
standard error of the mean. located, as expected, between the monolingual
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 147
TABLE 3 FIGURE 2
Mean Endpoint Preferences (%) in the Triads Endpoint Preferences (%) in the Triads Matching
Matching Task Across Monolingual Groups and L2 Task Across German and English Monolinguals and
Users Who Began Learning German via Formal/ English Users of L2 German, Differentiated by
Classroom Instruction Length of L2 Exposure

Endpoint 70
Groups Preference (%)
60
German monolinguals 36.62 (8.86)
English users of L2 German 28.66 (12.62) 50

Endpoint preference (%)


via formal instruction
40
English monolinguals 22.37 (7.57)
30
Note. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.
20

groups (post-hoc Scheffe p < 0.05). The L2 users 10


did not differ significantly from either monolin-
gual group. Table 3 shows the groups averages. 0
English Short exposure Medium Long exposure German
As with the whole group, we also conducted a monolinguals L2 users exposure L2 L2 users monolinguals
users
Pearsons correlation with this group, this time
adding the variable of years of L2 exposure. The Note. Columns show group means. Error bars show
results showed a significant relationship between standard error of the mean.
year of study and [ endpoint] preferences,
r .451, p < 0.05, while the correlation between
self-rated proficiency and [ endpoint] prefer- monolingual speakers of their L1, toward the
ences just about reached significance, r .247, direction of their L2, resembling typical in-
p 0.05. None of the other variables correlated between behaviour. Medium exposure L2 users
significantly with [ endpoint] preferences. on the other hand appear to revert back to the L1
The findings show clearly that length of L2 pattern, showing behaviour that is near-identical
exposure alone can bring about changes in event with their monolingual L1-speaking counterparts.
cognition of the type typically observed in other Finally, long exposure L2 users shifted completely
populations of L2 users, that is, cognitive behav- toward the pattern of monolingual speakers of the
iour that is in between that of their monolingual L2. A one-way ANOVA comparing the three
peers. However, the fact that length of L2 groups of L2 users and their monolingual
exposure did not correlate significantly with [ peers showed a significant main effect of Group,
endpoint] preferences suggests that the relation- F(4, 86) 6.83, p < 0.05. Post-hoc Scheffe tests
ship between the two variables may be nonlinear. showed that the German monolinguals and the
This would contrast with the relationship between long exposure L2 users selected the [ endpoint]
cognitive behaviour and other variables such as alternate significantly more frequently than did
proficiency, where across many studies, para- the English monolinguals and the group of
digms, linguistic domains, and different L2 user medium exposure L2 users (all p values < 0.05).
populations, that relationship has been found to The short exposure L2 users did not differ
be monotonic linear. To probe further into this significantly from any of the other groups.
possibility, we evenly divided up the 63 L2 users We then sought to discover which factors are
who began learning German through formal related to the cognitive behaviour of each group
instruction into 3 groups based on the years of L2 of L2 users according to length of L2 exposure.
exposure. This yielded a group of short expo- Thus, for each group of short, medium, and long
sure L2 users with an average of 3.8 years of exposure L2 users we performed correlations with
exposure (SD 2.4 years), a group of medium the proportion of [ endpoint] alternate selec-
exposure L2 users with an average of 7.5 years tion and the variables used in the previous
of exposure (SD 0.5 years), and a group of correlation. Interestingly, in the short instruction
long exposure L2 users with an average of 10.8 group, [ endpoint] preferences did not corre-
years of exposure (SD 2.5 years). As Figure 2 late significantly with any other variable. In the
shows, our hypothesis about the nonlinear medium instruction group, [ endpoint] prefer-
relationship between length of L2 exposure and ences correlated with year of study at university,
[ endpoint] preference was confirmed. Short r .386, p < 0.05, while the correlation with self-
exposure L2 users appear to shift away from rated proficiency was marginally significant,
148 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)

r .361, p 0.05. In the long instruction L2 user It is well known that higher levels of L2
group, [ endpoint] preferences correlated proficiency consistently relate to cognitive restr-
significantly with self-rated L2 proficiency, ucturing in L2 users in a number of domains, such
r .483, p < 0.05, and with year of study at as grammatical number and object categorization
university, r .454, p < 0.05. (Athanasopoulos, 2006), grammatical gender
(Kurinski & Sera, 2011), and spatial cognition
DISCUSSION (Park & Ziegler, 2014). However, studying the
evolution of the relationship between proficiency
The discussion is divided into two main and motion event categorization in learners with
sections: First, we will focus on the main findings different lengths of L2 exposure is able to reveal
of the study, looking at global and specific key insights into the nature of cognitive restruc-
patterns of performance, and suggest an inter- turing, at least in this and similar samples of
pretation of the documented effects of length of learners. Short exposure learners seem to display
L2 exposure, and the evolution of the relation- the typical in-between behaviour that is character-
ship between proficiency and event categoriza- istic in bilingual cognition studies and that is
tion patterns; second, we will discuss pedagogical indicative of a shift from L1 behaviour patterns to
aspects of cognitive restructuring in L2 learning. L2 behaviour patterns. Medium exposure learn-
ers seem to revert back to the L1 pattern, showing
Cognitive Restructuring and Exposure to a Second resistance to restructuring. Long exposure learn-
Language ers appear to have shifted completely toward the
L2 cognitive pattern. We interpret this apparent
We set out to extend the investigation of U-shape of cognitive patterns in groups with
motion event cognition to a new population of different lengths of instruction as a special case of
native speakers (German). Results showed that the nonlinearity that is characteristic of all
native speakers of English and German differed developmental processes (GershkoffStowe &
systematically in their event categorization pat- Thelen, 2004). Specifically, behavioural regres-
terns, establishing the existence of cognitive sion can be understood as part of the ordinary
differences between English and German- mechanisms of change, which are characterized
speaking populations in event construal beyond by collective dynamics of multiple, contingent
overt language production. These findings are processes (Rogers, Rakison, & McClelland, 2004).
consistent with those of previous studies using the In the case of L2 users, such processes include
same nonverbal paradigm (Athanasopoulos & sensitivity to statistical regularities in the input,
Bylund, 2013; Bylund et al., 2013). suppression of pre-potent or routinized categori-
Our primary goals, however, concern L2 user zation schemas, and redirection of attention to a
behaviour. Our findings are the first to show an novel event construal (Ameel et al., 2005). Under
effect of the second language on cognition in this interpretation, the state of representation at
individuals whose L1 has progressive aspect but any given developmental stage always depends on
whose L2 lacks that grammatical feature. Altho- its past history. Therefore regression is not really
ugh one might not expect cognitive restructuring regression, but reflects a developmental stage in
in the learning of a language that does not involve changing behaviour (GershkoffStowe & Thelen,
the acquisition of a specific grammatical construc- 2004). New cognitive patterns may emerge as a
tion linked to said hypothetical cognitive restruc- function of the changing relationships between
turing, we provide a principled account for such internal processes (suppression and redirection)
an influence by referring to the theoretical and contextual influences (statistical regularities
assumptions used by Niemeier and Reifs in the input).
(2008). Specifically, what is being restructured How might one explain the nonlinear curve in
is not grammatical aspect per se, but the construal this particular sample of learners? Similar curves
of boundedness and unboundedness, which in can be observed in associative learning that takes
turn affects how motion events are categorized by place over a long period of time, when a specific
L2 users. Under this interpretation, construal of construal is consistent with input only in one
events as bounded was an aspect of the L2 that specific domain, but inconsistent in other do-
initially lacked saliency for the learners. However, mains (Rogers et al., 2004). In such cases,
the weight of bounded construal as a basis for statistical learning will initially result in changing
categorization changed under the influence of cognitive representations as the system is affected
long exposure, as a function of increasing L2 at a general level. This can explain the shift
proficiency, and with year of study at university. observed in the short instruction group. Later on,
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 149

however, the system moves from general to more the categories they were exposed to in the
specific instances of the newly learned property as laboratory, but over time the weights of the newly
it tries to represent it in as fine-grained a way as learned categories as categorization cues are not
possible. Because for the learners in this study the strong enough to suppress individuals previous,
majority of instances that the system encounters routinized way of categorizing. In other words, the
promotes unbounded construalsthe L2 learn- problem is not one of loss, but of interference
ers continue to use their L1 and interact with suppression, which is amenable to input frequen-
other L1 native speakersbehaviour may revert cy effects.
to reflect L1 conceptual dominance. Over time, Our findings regarding the shift in long
the L2 user must learn to defocus unboundedness exposure learners contrast with those of Malt
as a basis for categorization, and activate a and Sloman (2003), who found that advanced L2
bounded schema as a basis for categorization. users with very similar lengths of exposure as our
Finally, when sufficient repeated exposure to long exposure group exhibited some discrepan-
bounded events in a specific context (e.g., the L2 cies from native speakers naming patterns and
German classroom, or studying German at typicality judgments for household objects. It
university) occurs, the L2 user is able to overcome appears, then, that different domains of experi-
any pre-potent bias and shifts toward the L2 ence may afford differential opportunities for
cognitive pattern. Evidence that is compatible restructuring. It is important to note, however,
with the onset of this process can be observed in that the developmental trends described and
the medium exposure learners. Even though examined here and in Malt and Slomans (2003)
group averages do not indicate cognitive restruc- study are not developmental trends in the literal
turing, individual patterns of behaviour (as sense because these studies are cross-sectional,
revealed by correlational analyses) do show a and focus on adults, relying on their previous
propensity to focus on boundedness as a basis for linguistic history and their self-assessments of
categorization at higher levels of proficiency and knowledge of the L2 as variables worth investigat-
with more years of university study. That is, ing in the context of cognitive restructuring in L2
although performance may seemingly deterio- acquisition (cf. Athanasopoulos, 2011).
rate, there are long-term changes that indicate Nevertheless the trajectory of cognitive behav-
overall positive growth, as attested to by the iour observed in this study as a function of L2
significant correlations in the medium exposure exposure and the progression of influence of
group. These correlations then become stronger increasing expertise in the L2 (as indicated by self-
and more significant in the long exposure group. rated proficiency and year of study at university)
Thus the apparent U-shaped curve uncovered in conforms to well-known general learning theo-
the study is unlikely to indicate loss or regression ries, with learners displaying behaviour that is
to a former state of representation. fully consonant with theories that postulate
The shift observed in short exposure learners orderly progression underlying apparent U-
may be attributed to frequency of input alone at a shaped learning. This progression indicates that
general level, just like the effects of laboratory the apparent L1-like behaviour in medium
training of new categories in linguistic relativity exposure learners is not just a reflection of
studies. Research shows that it is possible to unlearning or of L1 entrenchment, but is rather
induce changes in categorical perception and the surface instantiation of a much more complex
object recognition by training participants to reorganization of the cognition of these L2
learn new artificial categories (Goldstone & learners, which provides first evidence for true
Barsalou, 1998; Notman, Sowden, & Ozgen, internalization of L2-derived motion event cogni-
2005; Ozgen & Davies, 2002). However, such tion, consistent with theories of domain-general
effects are transient and short-lived. In line with U-shaped learning trajectories. Our assumptions
Ellis (2008), for input to be internalized it needs in this regard need to be verified in a longitudinal
to be interpreted meaningfully and in context. design that spans several years (cf. Stam, 2015),
We view the emergence of a link between L2 but the current findings lay a useful methodolog-
proficiency and categorization behaviour in ical and theoretical foundation for such future
medium exposure learners as the beginnings of inquiry.
such meaningful associative learning, and the
progressive strengthening of this correlation in Placing Cognitive Restructuring Within L2 Pedagogy
long exposure learners as true internalization of a
novel event construal. Even in category training As already mentioned, in a Cognitive Linguistic
studies it is not that participants lose or unlearn approach language is understood to be another
150 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)

instance of general human cognition, inextricably overt verbal behaviour, as well as an array of
linked to other cognitive abilities and to the multi- instances of nonverbal behaviour, which may be
modal nature of memory representation, which verbally mediated to different degrees. In the case
contains both verbal and nonverbal experiential of the German L2 English user described earlier,
conceptual elements. Such a view is very much in attaching undue importance to the possible
line with neuroscientific and clinical approaches endpoint of some action that she herself is
to representation, where linguistic categories are performing, or some action that she is recalling
seen as constituent elements of larger complexes from memory in an eye-witness account, may be
of knowledge (Humphreys & Forde, 2001; perceived as displaying a foreign cognitive or
Pulvermuller, 2003). As Tyler (2012) puts it, behavioural accent. While it may be very difficult
language is understood as part and parcel of for L2 learners to notice, let alone modulate, their
general cognitive organization and processes (p. L1 semantic accents (since L2 teaching rarely
19). focuses on such aspects of language use), it may
Is cognitive restructuring of the type investigat- be even more difficult for L2 learners to notice
ed and demonstrated in this study an essential and try to overcome a behavioural or cognitive L1
part of L2 learning? If verbal communication is accent. Living in an L2 setting and frequently
part of humans general cognitive make-up and is using the L2 in that setting may help (Bylund &
constrained and constructed from the same Athanasopoulos, 2014a; Bylund et al., 2013), but
building blocks as other cognitive processes, the task seems a particularly arduous challenge in
then the answer is yes and cognitive restructur- an instructed L2 learning setting in the L1-
ing in SLA is just as important an outcome of L2 speaking country, for several reasons. First, new
learning as any other aspect of linguistic restruc- information conflicts with fully developed cogni-
turing, such as phonetic, lexical, grammatical, or tive routines. If humans learn new information
pragmatic restructuring. Beyond that, we suggest more easily and with less effort when they can
that a more nuanced and also more principled relate it to established cognitive schemas (Ru-
treatment of the phenomenon might be attained melhart, 1984), then learning novel or different
through the notion of cognitive or behavioural categorical distinctions in the L2 may pose a
accent, here exemplified by endpoint saliency as a particular challenge. Second, frequency of usage
function of the presence or absence of progressive of the L2 (which is an important variable in
aspect in a language. Scholars looking at informa- cognitive restructuring occurring in immersed or
tion structure in L2 users have already introduced multilingual settings) tends to be limited to a few
the notion of semantic accent to refer to L2 user hours per week in a classroom, possibly augment-
communication that may in all respects be target- ed by individual interactive self-study activities. As
like with regard to grammar and vocabulary, but demonstrated in this study, this may have
may nonetheless still be perceived as nonnative consequences for the internalization of novel
because of the way L2 users choose to segment construals and shifting categorizations.
information into grammatical and lexical units Finally, rich opportunities for learners to
(Tyler, 2012). Thus, if a German L2 user of interact and internalize the behaviour of native
English produces sentences containing progres- speakers of the L2 tend to be confined to second-
sive aspect while consistently mentioning the hand exposure via media interactive teaching
endpoint (e.g., I was driving to the petrol station materials, or via interaction with the teacher, in
when John rang) even in contexts where the cases where the teacher is a native speaker of the
endpoint is irrelevant to the current conversation L2. Absent other native speakers in the classroom,
schema, then an otherwise grammatical utterance even in those cases the teachers behaviour may
may be perceived as having a foreign semantic not be typical of native speakers. Learners rarely,
accent because it presents additional information if ever, receive explicit instruction on information
that a native speaker would not include. structure, and are even less likely to be made
Attaching salience to endpoints is not confined aware of a nonverbal cognitive or behavioural
only to overt language use, but to a number of accent. For the L2 learner, a behavioural L1
nonverbal activities as well, such as driving, accent would not necessarily cause communica-
walking, running, cycling, as well as interpreting tion breakdown and, indeed, may not even be
and remembering acts of motion and the relevant in cases where the learner has not lived or
constituent elements of such motion, such as does not intend to live in the L2-speaking
the agent, trajectory, manner, and endpoint. In community. However, if the primary goal of all
other words, construal of a motion event is a cognitive activities (including language) is mental
multifaceted phenomenon that manifests itself in contact with other humans (Langacker, 1987,
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 151

1991), then being aware of and being able to Athanasopoulos, P., Dering, B., Wiggett, A., Kuipers, J., &
modulate ones cognitive accent becomes one of Thierry, G. (2010). Perceptual shift in bilingualism:
the central processes of being a bi/multilingual Brain potentials reveal plasticity in pre-attentive
individual, and as such one of the major goals of colour perception. Cognition, 116, 437443.
Athanasopoulos, P., & Kasai, C. (2008). Language and
additional language learning.
thought in bilinguals: The case of grammatical
What is perhaps needed at the moment to number and nonverbal classification preferences.
facilitate cognitive restructuring in instructed L2 Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 105123.
learners is the fundamental role of interaction Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back
between the expert and the novice, as initially and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4,
conceptualized by Vygotsky (1934/1986) and 829839.
later filtered through the sensibilities of L2 Barner, D., Inagaki, S., & Li, P. (2009). Language,
teaching researchers (e.g., Lantolf, 2003; Lantolf thought, and real nouns. Cognition, 111, 329344.
& Appel, 1994). Under this account, cultural Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?
knowers (i.e., teachers or native speakers of the English and Mandarin speakers conceptions of
time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 122.
L2) act as models for novices (i.e., L2 learners),
Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L., & Phillips, W. (2003). Sex,
who through apprenticeship learning develop the syntax, and semantics. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin
basic cognitive tools and skills needed to accom- Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the
plish a particular activity, in this case, L2-based study of language and thought (pp. 6180). Cam-
cognitive behaviour. Unless such tools and skills bridge, MA: The MIT Press.
are built into a long-term L2 curriculum (see Boutonnet, B., Athanasopoulos, P., & Thierry, G.
Byrnes, Maxim, & Norris, 2010; Cook, 2011; (2012). Unconscious effects of grammatical gen-
Pavlenko, 2009, for some preliminary discus- der during object categorisation. Brain Research,
sions), cognitive restructuring will remain a 1479, 7279.
Bybee, J. (2006). Frequency of use and the organization of
significant challenge for the L2 classroom
language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
learner.
Bylund, E. (2011). Language-specific patterns in event
conceptualization: Insights from bilingualism. In
A. Pavlenko (Ed.), Thinking and speaking in two
languages (pp. 108142). Bristol, UK: Multilingual
NOTE
Matters.
Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2014a). Language
1
Given that it is virtually impossible to find complete- and thought in a multilingual context: The case of
ly monolingual German native speakers who are also isiXhosa. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17,
educated to university level, we made sure that these 431441.
participants were not using English as part of their Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2014b). Linguistic
degree; we also administered the Quick Oxford relativity in SLA: Towards a new research pro-
Placement Test, a quick English proficiency test. The gramme. Language Learning, 64, 952985.
mean score on the test was 47.86% (SD 8.92), Bylund, E., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2015). Televised
indicating that most participants had basic to lower Whorf: Cognitive restructuring in advanced for-
intermediate knowledge of English. eign language learners as a function of audiovisual
media exposure. Modern Language Journal, 99,
Supplement 2015, 123137.
REFERENCES
Bylund, E., Athanasopoulos, P., & Oostendorp, M.
(2013). Motion event cognition and grammatical
Ameel, E., Storms, G., Malt, B. C., & Sloman, S. A. aspect: Evidence from Afrikaans. Linguistics, 51,
(2005). How bilinguals solve the naming problem. 929955.
Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 6080. Bylund, E., & Jarvis, S. (2011). L2 effects on L1 event
Athanasopoulos, P. (2006). Effects of the grammatical conceptualization. Bilingualism: Language and
representation of number on cognition in bilin- Cognition, 14, 4759.
guals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, Byrnes, H., Maxim, H. H., & Norris, J. M. (2010).
8996. Realizing advanced L2 writing development in a
Athanasopoulos, P. (2011). Cognitive restructuring in collegiate curriculum: Curricular design, pedago-
bilingualism. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), Thinking and gy, assessment. Modern Language Journal, 94, s1,
speaking in two languages (pp. 2965). Bristol, UK: ivvi, 1235.
Multilingual Matters. Casasanto, D. (2008). Whos afraid of the Big Bad Whorf?
Athanasopoulos, P., & Bylund, E. (2013). Does gram- Cross-linguistic differences in temporal language
matical aspect affect motion event cognition? A and thought. Language Learning, 58, 6379.
crosslinguistic comparison of English and Swed- Colunga, E., & Smith, L. B. (2005). From the lexicon to
ish speakers. Cognitive Science, 37, 286309. expectations about kinds: A role for associative
152 The Modern Language Journal 99 (2015)
learning. Psychological Review, 112, 347382. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in
Cook, V. J. (2011). Linguistic relativity and language cognitive science (pp. 2045). London: Elsevier.
teaching. In V. J. Cook & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Humphreys, G. W., & Forde, E. M. E. (2001). Hierar-
Language and bilingual cognition (pp. 509518). chies, similarity, and interactivity in object recog-
Hove, UK: Psychology Press. nition: Category-specific neuropsychological
Cook, V. J., Bassetti, B., Kasai, C., Sasaki, M., & deficits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 453509.
Takahashi, J. A. (2006). Do bilinguals have Imai, M., & Gentner, D. (1997). A crosslinguistic study of
different concepts? The case of shape and material early word meaning: Universal ontology and
in Japanese L2 users of English. International linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169200.
Journal of Bilingualism, 10, 137152. January, D., & Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence
Ellis, N. C. (1998). Emergentism, connectionism and for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001).
language learning. Language Learning, 48, 631 Cognition, 104, 417426.
664. Jarvis, S., & Pavlenko, A. (2008). Crosslinguistic influence
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language in language and cognition. New York: Routledge.
processing: A review with implications for theories Johnson, E. K., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Word
of implicit and explicit language acquisition. segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143188. cues count for more than statistics. Journal of
Ellis, N. C. (2006). Selective attention and transfer Memory and Language, 44, 548567.
phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue Kersten, A., Meissner, C., Lechuga, J., Schwartz, B.,
competition, salience, interference, overshadow- Albrechtsen, J., & Iglesias, A. (2010). English
ing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied speakers attend more strongly than Spanish
Linguistics, 27, 164194. speakers to manner of motion when classifying
Ellis, N. C. (2008). Usage-based and form-focused novel objects and events. Journal of Experimental
language acquisition: The associative learning of Psychology: General, 139, 638653.
constructions, learned attention and the limited Kiefer, M., & Pulvermuller, F. (2012). Conceptual
L2 end state. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), representations in mind and brain: Theoretical
Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language developments, current evidence and future direc-
acquisition (pp. 372406). London: Routledge. tions. Cortex, 48, 805825.
Ellis, N. C., & FerreiraJunior, F. (2009). Constructions Kurinski, E., & Sera, M. D. (2011). Does learning
and their acquisition: Islands and the distinc- Spanish grammatical gender change English-
tiveness of their occupancy. Annual Review of speaking adults categorization of inanimate
Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 188221. objects? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14,
Evans, V., Bergen, B. K., & Zinken, J. (2007). Cognitive 203220.
linguistic reader. London: Equinox. Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar,
Flecken, M. (2011). Event conceptualization by early Vol. 1. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
DutchGerman bilinguals: Insights from linguistic Langacker, R. (1991). Foundations of cognitive grammar,
and eye-tracking data. Bilingualism: Language and Vol. 2. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Cognition, 14, 6177. Langacker, R. (2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic
Franklin, A., Drivonikou, G. V., Clifford, A., Kay, P., introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.
Regier, T., & Davies, I. R. L. (2008). Lateralization Lantolf, J. P. (2003). Intrapersonal communication and
of categorical perception of color changes with internalization in the second language classroom.
color term acquisition. Proceedings of the National In A. Kozulin, V. S. Ageev, S. Miller, & B. Gindis
Academy of Sciences, 105, 1822118225. (Eds.), Vygotskys educational theory in cultural context
Geeraerts, D., & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.). (2007). The Oxford (pp. 349370). Cambridge: Cambridge University
handbook of cognitive linguistics. Oxford: Oxford Press.
University Press. Lantolf, J. P., & Appel, G. (Eds.). (1994). Vygotskian
GershkoffStowe, L., & Thelen, E. (2004). U-shaped approaches to second language research. Norwood, NJ:
changes in behaviour: A dynamic systems perspec- Ablex.
tive. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 1136. Li, P., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Turning the tables:
Goldstone, R. L., & Barsalou, L. W. (1998). Reuniting Language and spatial reasoning. Cognition, 83,
conception and perception. Cognition, 65, 231 265294.
262. Lucy, J. A. (1992). Grammatical categories and cognition: A
Gopnik, A., Choi, S., & Baumberger, T. (1996). Cross- case study of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. Cam-
linguistic differences in early semantic and cogni- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
tive development. Cognitive Development, 11, 197 Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of
227. Anthropology, 26, 291312.
Harnad, S. (1987). Categorical perception: The groundwork of Lucy, J. A., & Gaskins, S. (2001). Grammatical categories
cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. and the development of classification preferences:
Harnad, S. (2005). To cognize is to categorize. A comparative approach. In M. Bowerman & S.
Cognition is categorization. In H. Cohen & C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and
Panos Athanasopoulos et al. 153
conceptual development (pp. 257283). New York: two languages: A longitudinal study. Journal of
Cambridge University Press. Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 554571.
Lucy, J. A., & Gaskins, S. (2003). Interaction of language Rogers, T. T., Rakison, D. H., & McClelland, J. L. (2004).
type and referent type in the development of U-shaped curves in development: A PDP ap-
nonverbal classification preferences. In D. Gent- proach. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5,
ner & S. GoldinMeadow (Eds.), Language in mind: 137145.
Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. Rumelhart, D. E. (1984). Schemata and the cognitive
465492). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. system. In R. Wyer & T. K. Thomas (Eds.),
Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Handbook of social cognition, Vol 1 (pp. 161188).
Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence, Erlbaum.
cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Samuelson, L. K. (2002). Statistical regularities in
Sciences, 8, 108114. vocabulary guide language acquisition in connec-
Malt, B. C., & Sloman, S. (2003). Linguistic diversity and tionist models and 15- to 20-month-olds. Develop-
object naming by non-native speakers of English. mental Psychology, 38, 10161037.
Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 4767. Schmiedtova, B., & Flecken, M. (2008). Aspectual
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. concepts across languages: Some considerations
(2007). The Language Experience and Proficien- for second language learning. In S. de Knop (Ed.),
cy Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language Pedagogical grammar (pp. 357384). Berlin: Mou-
profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of ton de Gruyter.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 940 Schmiedtova, B., Carroll, M., & von Stutterheim, C.
967. (2011). Implications of language-specific patterns
Niemeier, S., & Reif, S. (2008). Making progress of event construal of advanced L2 speakers. In A.
simpler? Applying cognitive grammar to tense Pavlenko (Ed.), Thinking and speaking in two
aspect teaching in the German EFL classroom. In languages (pp. 66107). Bristol, UK: Multilingual
S. De Knop & T. De Rycker (Eds.), Cognitive Matters.
approaches to pedagogical grammar (pp. 325356). Smith, L. B. (2000). Learning how to learn words: An
Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. associative crane. In R. Golinkoff et al. (Eds.),
Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the Becoming a word learner (pp. 5180). New York:
identification-categorisation relationship. Journal Oxford University Press.
of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 3957. Smith, L. B. (2003). Learning to recognize objects.
Notman, L., Sowden, P. T., & Ozgen, E. (2005). The Psychological Science, 14, 244250.
nature of learned categorical perception effects: Stam, G. (2015). Changes in thinking for speaking: a
A psychophysical approach. Cognition, 95, longitudinal case study. Modern Language Journal,
B1B14. 99, Supplement 2015, 8399.
Ozgen, E., & Davies, I. R. L. (2002). Acquisition of Szagun, G. (2001). Learning different regularities: The
categorical color perception: A perceptual learn- acquisition of noun plurals by German-speaking
ing approach to the linguistic relativity hypothesis. children. First Language, 21, 109141.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 477 Tyler, A. (2012). Cognitive linguistics and second language
493. learning. London: Routledge.
Park, H. I., & Ziegler, N. (2014). Cognitive shift in the von Stutterheim, C. (1997). Zum Ausdruck von Raum-
bilingual mind: Spatial concepts in Korean und Zeitkonzepten in deutschen und englischen
English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Texten [The expression of spatial and temporal
Cognition, 17, 410430. concepts in German and English texts]. Zeitschrift
Pavlenko, A. (2009). Conceptual representation in the fur Germanistische Linguistik, 25, 147166.
bilingual lexicon and second language vocabulary von Stutterheim, C. (2003). Linguistic structure and
learning. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), The bilingual mental information organisation. The case of very ad-
lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 125160). vanced learners. EuroSLA Yearbook, 3, 183206.
Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. von Stutterheim, C., Andermann, M., Carroll, M.,
Pavlenko, A. (Ed.). (2011). Thinking and speaking in two Flecken, M., & Schmiedtova, B. (2012). How
languages. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters. grammaticized concepts shape event conceptuali-
Pulvermuller, F. (2003). The neuroscience of language: On zation in language production: Insights from
brain circuits of words and serial order. Cambridge: linguistic analysis, eye tracking data, and memory
Cambridge University Press. performance. Linguistics, 50, 833867.
Regier, T., & Kay, P. (2009). Language, thought and Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1986). Thought and language.
color: Whorf was half right. Trends in Cognitive Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Sciences, 13, 439446. Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Selected
Roberson, D. (2005). Color categories are culturally writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (J. B. Carroll, Ed.).
diverse in cognition as well as in language. Cross- Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Cultural Research, 39, 5671. Wolff, P., & Holmes, K. J. (2010). Linguistic relativity.
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I., & Shapiro, L. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2,
(2004). The development of color categories in 253265.

S-ar putea să vă placă și