Sunteți pe pagina 1din 7

293

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Dual-Task Complexity Affects Gait in People With Mild


Cognitive Impairment: The Interplay Between Gait
Variability, Dual Tasking, and Risk of Falls
Manuel Montero-Odasso, MD, PhD, Susan W. Muir, PhD, Mark Speechley, PhD
ABSTRACT. Montero-Odasso M, Muir SW, Speechley M.
Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive
impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking,
F ALL RISK IS DOUBLED among older adults with cog-
nitive impairment, but the mechanisms of the increased fall
1,2
risk are not completely understood. Mild cognitive impair-
and risk of falls. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2012;93:293-9. ment (MCI) is considered a transitional phase between normal
aging and dementia.3,4 People with MCI represent a highly
Objective: To determine the effect of 2 different dual tasks on vulnerable population, as they face an elevated risk for con-
gait variability in people with mild cognitive impairment version to dementia. Interestingly, it has been recently de-
(MCI) compared with control subjects. scribed that they are also at an increased risk for falls and
Design: Cross-sectional study. mobility decline.5,6
Setting: Memory clinic at a university hospital. Walking has long been considered primarily an automatic
Participants: Older adults with MCI (n43) and cognitively motor task, but an emerging body of evidence suggests that this
normal control subjects (n25) from the community. Gait was view is simplistic, and by contrast, cognitive function plays a
assessed under single (usual walking) and dual tasking (naming key role even in the regulation of routine walking.7-9 The
animals and subtracting serial 7s), using an electronic walkway. importance of this cognitive control is most easily demon-
Interventions: Not applicable. strated in older people with memory problems and dementia.
Main Outcome Measures: The dependent variable was the Since the seminal stops walking while talking study,10 which
coefficient of variation of gait variability, a marker of gait demonstrated that the inability to maintain a conversation while
stability and an established risk factor for falls. Two-way walking is a marker for future falls, observing people walking
repeated-measures analysis of variance was used to examine while they perform a secondary task, the dual-task paradigm,
main effects (group, task) and their interaction. has been used to assess the interaction between cognition, gait,
Results: A significant difference was found within and be- and the risk of falling. During dual tasking, the subject per-
tween groups (P.016) of increasing gait variability as dual- forms an attention-demanding task while walking to assess for
task complexity increased. Gait velocity decreased within groups any modifications, compared with the reference task, on either
as dual-task complexity increased. The magnitude of increased the cognitive or the walking subtasks.11 The underlying hy-
gait variability across tasks was greater for the MCI group pothesis is that 2 simultaneously performed tasks interfere and
(2.68% 9.84%) than for the control group (1.86%3.74%), show- compete for brain cortical resources.11,12-14 The change in an
ing a significant between-group difference (P.041). individuals gait performance from a single task to a dual task
Conclusions: Dual-task load significantly increased gait vari- is known as the dual-task cost. The magnitude of these
ability in the MCI group compared with the control group, an dual-task costs reveals the insufficiency of the cortical control
effect that was larger than the changes in gait velocity. The to regulate walking and has been associated with an increased
magnitude of this impairment on gait stability was related to fall risk.15,16 Dual tasking is also clinically relevant because
the complexity of the dual task applied. Our findings help to most activities of daily living involve the simultaneous perfor-
explain the high risk of falls recently described in older adults mance of 2 or more cognitive and motor tasks, making it
with MCI, and may help in the identification of fall risk in representative of real-life situations where falls are likely to
cognitively impaired persons. occur.9
Key Words: Gait; Rehabilitation; Cognition; Attention; Gait is a complex motor behavior with many measurable
Aged. facets beside gait velocity,17 and with the advent of equipment
2012 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation to precisely measure several quantitative aspects of walking,
Medicine the variability of the stride time is a growing research field that
provides an interesting window for the study of the cognitive
control of gait and risk of falls.18 For instance, low stride-to-
stride variability reflects automatic processes that require min-
From the Department of Medicine, Division of Geriatric Medicine, Parkwood
Hospital, University of Western Ontario, London (Montero-Odasso, Muir); Gait and
Brain Lab, Lawson Health Research Institute, London (Montero-Odasso); and De-
partment of Epidemiology & Biostatistics, University of Western Ontario, London
(Speechley), ON, Canada. List of Abbreviations
Supported by operating grants from the Drummond Foundation, Montral, the
Physician Services Incorporated Foundation, Toronto and the Canadian Institutes of a-MCI amnestic mild cognitive impairment
Health Research. ANOVA analysis of variance
No commercial party having a direct financial interest in the results of the research CDR Clinical Dementia Rating Scale
supporting this article has or will confer a benefit on the authors or on any organi-
CoV coefficient of variation
zation with which the authors are associated.
Reprint requests to Manuel Montero-Odasso, MD, PhD, Parkwood Hospital, Di- MCI mild cognitive impairment
vision of Geriatrics, 801 Commissioners Rd E, Room 280, London, Ontario, Canada na-MCI nonamnestic mild cognitive impairment
N6A 5A5, e-mail: mmontero@uwo.ca. MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination
0003-9993/12/9302-00564$36.00/0 MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment
doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.026

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012


294 DUAL-TASK COMPLEXITY ON GAIT IN MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT, Montero-Odasso

imal attention and is associated with efficient gait control and ing, and medications were recorded. Basic and instrumental
gait safety.19 By contrast, high stride time variability has been activities of daily living were evaluated using the Lawton-
associated with executive and attentional deficits, Parkinsons Brody25 scale, to ensure absence of functional impairment.
disease, and Alzheimers disease.20-23 Additionally, high stride Objective cognitive status of all participants was assessed using
time variability has been shown to predict future falls in com- the MMSE (score, 0 30)26 and the MoCA (score, 0 30; higher
munity-dwelling older persons even when gait velocity failed score indicating better performance).27,28 A CDR was per-
to distinguish between those who fell and those who had not.24 formed in all participants with MCI. Objective cognitive im-
Gait variability characteristics and how these are affected by pairment in our MCI participants was operationalized as a
dual-task challenges have not been evaluated in older adults normal MMSE score (26) combined with a low MoCA score
with MCI. Therefore, the aim of this study was to characterize (26); this pattern has been previously validated for MCI
the effect of 2 different dual-task challenges on gait variability identification.27,29 Finally, we subcategorized our participants
and to determine whether an increase in the secondary task with MCI as amnestic MCI (a-MCI) and nonamnestic MCI
complexity would yield a predictable cost in gait performance (na-MCI) by using a simple and valid approach29 for descrip-
in people with MCI. We tested the following hypotheses: (1) tive purposes only.
gait performance is affected more by dual-task challenges in
people with MCI than in cognitively normal control subjects; Quantitative Gait Assessment
(2) a worsening gait performance in people with MCI (ie, Gait performance under single- and dual-task challenges was
decreased gait velocity and increased gait variability) will be assessed using an electronic walkway system (GAITRite Sys-
greater while doing more complex mental task such as sub- tema) that is 600cm in length and 64cm in width. As partici-
tracting serials 7s, than when simply naming words; and (3) the pants walk along the mat, imbedded sensors are activated by
increase in gait variability is greater than the reduction in gait the pressure of their feet and deactivated when the pressure is
velocity. released. A computer processed the footsteps, providing data
for both spatial and temporal parameters. Start and end points
METHODS were marked on the floor with tape 1m from either end of the
mat to avoid the recording of acceleration and deceleration
Study Participants phases. Participants performed 1 practice trial walking on the
A convenience sample of participants with MCI and cogni- mat to familiarize themselves with the protocol. Gait velocity
tively normal control subjects were recruited. Participants with (cm/s), stride time (ms), and stride time variability (percentage
newly diagnosed MCI were recruited from the Aging Brain of coefficient of variation [%CoV]), the principal gait measures
and Memory Clinic at Parkwood Hospital. The identification of interest, were measured during the single-task and dual-task
of MCI was based on the most recent consensus criteria4 that trials. The single-task trial consisted of walking the length of
included the presence of subjective memory complaints from the mat at a self-selected usual pace. For the dual-task trials,
the patient and family, objective memory impairment (assessed participants walked the length of the mat while subtracting
using the Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] and Mon- serial 7s from 100 aloud or while naming animals aloud. These
treal Cognitive Assessment [MoCA]), preserved general intel- 2 different dual-task conditions were selected based on previ-
lectual function (assessed clinically), absence of significant ous research which demonstrated that subtractions depend
functional impairment, and absence of clinical dementia.4 Ad- more on working memory and attention, while naming animals
ditionally, participants needed to score 0.5 on the Clinical out loud is more related to verbal fluency, which relies on
Dementia Rating Scale (CDR). Controls were recruited by semantic memory.30,31
newspaper advertisement and from a community-based fitness To balance and minimize the effects of learning and fatigue,
program for older adults. the order of the single and dual tasks was randomized. While
Inclusion criteria for the MCI group were a recent diagnosis performing the dual-task trials, there was no instruction to
of MCI, aged 65 years and older, and able to walk indepen- prioritize the gait or cognitive task. Allowing both gait and
dently without a gait aid (eg, cane or walker). In the control cognitive tasks to vary has previously been shown to provide a
group, inclusion criteria were aged 65 years and older, an more natural representation of daily living activities of older
absence of subjective cognitive complaints, normal objective individuals.8,32 Reliability has been previously established by
cognitive testing, absence of functional impairment, and able to our group for the use of this gait assessment protocol in people
walk independently without a gait aid. Exclusion criteria for with MCI.33
both groups included the inability to understand English, par-
kinsonism or any neurologic disorder with residual motor def- Data Analysis
icits (eg, stroke, epilepsy), musculoskeletal disorders or a his- Demographic and medical characteristics were summarized
tory of knee or hip replacement surgery that affected gait using either means and SDs, or frequencies and percentages, as
performance, use of psychotropic medications that affected appropriate. Comparisons between groups were made using
motor performance (eg, neuroleptics, benzodiazepines), or ac- unpaired t tests or Mann-Whitney tests as deemed appropriate,
tive major depression. During assessment, a formal clinical with adjustment made for multiple comparisons using the Bon-
examination was done to evaluate the effect of diseases and ferroni correction. The CoV for stride time was used to quan-
comorbidities that might affect gait performance. The project tify gait variability under each testing condition. The CoV was
was approved by the University of Western Ontario Health calculated as follows:
Sciences Research Ethics Board, and all participants provided


informed consent.
SDst
COVst 100
Medical and Cognitive Assessments meanst
Sociodemographic characteristics, comorbidities, a history
of falls in the past 6 months, fear of falling, self-reported where CoVst is the CoV of stride time, SDst is the SD of stride
physical activity level, functionality on activities of daily liv- time, and meanst is the mean stride time.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012


DUAL-TASK COMPLEXITY ON GAIT IN MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT, Montero-Odasso 295

A 2-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) ences in the mean values between groups on each walking test
was performed to evaluate the effect of cognitive status (group) condition were also found for gait velocity (P.0001), stride
across the increasing complexity of gait tasks (condition) and time (P.0001), and gait variability (P.0001). The magni-
their interaction (group condition). If the overall F test was tude of increased gait variability across tasks was greater for
significant, post hoc testing was performed (Tukey procedure) the MCI (2.68%9.84%) than the control group (1.86%
to identify which pair-wise comparisons between and within 3.74%), showing a between-group significant difference
groups were significantly different from one another. A sensi- (P.041). The test for interaction between cognitive status and
tivity analysis of the ANOVA analysis adjusted for age and the walking test condition for gait velocity was not statistically
history of falls was performed. The level of statistical signifi- significant (P.23) (fig 1). A statistically significant interaction
cance was set at P.05 (2-sided), and the statistical analyses effect was found, though, between cognitive status and walking
were conducted using SAS software version 8.2.b test condition in favor of the MCI group for gait variability,
demonstrating that gait variability increased as complexity of
RESULTS the gait task increased (fig 2). A sensitivity analysis to evaluate
Forty-eight people with a diagnosis of MCI were eligible for the robustness of the main analyses was performed, adjusting
this study, and 43 met the study inclusion criteria. Five people for age and history of falls; the directions and magnitude of
with an MCI diagnosis were excluded for the following rea- associations (a difference 10%) of the results were main-
sons: MoCA greater than 26 (n3), MMSE less than 26 (n1), tained. Figure 3A demonstrates the nonsignificant interaction
and English language difficulties (n1). Twenty-nine people effect of dual tasking on gait velocity; in contrast, this effect
were assessed for the cognitively normal group, and 25 people was evident and significant on gait variability (fig 3B). Finally,
met the study inclusion criteria, with 4 people excluded for age the effect of a complex dual-task challenge (serial 7s subtrac-
less than 65 years (n1), MMSE less than 26 (n1), and tion) on gait variability can be seen in figure 4 when comparing
MoCA less than 26 (n2). All participants were fully func- variability in stride time between a control participant and a
tional in their basic and instrumental activities of daily living. participant with MCI.
Demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in table
1. As expected, the MCI group differed from the control group DISCUSSION
on cognitive status. The MCI group was also older, more often This study has demonstrated that gait performance differs
had a positive fall history, and was more likely to be sedentary. between people with MCI and people with normal cognition,
The level of physical activity and age was highly correlated. particularly under dual tasking. Both groups showed a decrease
Among the MCI group, 31 (72%) met the criteria for a-MCI, 11 in gait velocity and an increase in gait variability with the
(26%) for na-MCI, and 1 participant did not have data available different dual-task challenges. However, the effect of the dual-
to allow group assignment. task load on the regulation of stride-to-stride time, assessed as
Results from the repeated-measures ANOVA are presented gait variability, was significantly greater in those with MCI.
in table 2. Statistically significant differences in the mean This effect on gait variability was much larger than the effect
values were found within each cognitive group across the seen on gait velocity.
different walking test conditions for gait velocity (P.0001), The dual-task cost for gait velocity found in our study is
stride time (P.0008), and gait variability (P.0013). Differ- consistent with previous research among patient populations

Table 1: Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Study Participants Stratified by Cognitive Status Group
Variable Controls (n25) MCI (n43) P

Age (y) 71.54.1 75.16.3 .013*


MMSE 29.50.6 27.81.6 .0001*
MoCA 28.21.5 23.02.3 .0001*
Sex (female) 22 (88) 23 (54) .012*
Body mass index (kg/m2) 26.84.1 25.83.8 .31
Had fall in past 6 months 5 (20) 23 (54) .01*
Fear of falling 5 (20) 11 (26) .77
Years of education 13.03.2 12.73.3 .72
No. of current medications 4.13.4 5.83.6 .24
Comorbidities
Hypertension 9 (36) 24 (56) .14
Diabetes 2 (8) 4 (9) 1.00
Stroke or transient ischemic attack 0 (0) 4 (9) .29
Parkinsons disease 0 (0) 1 (2) 1.00
Lung disease 1 (4) 4 (9) .64
Myocardial infarction 0 (0) 6 (15) .08
Congestive heart failure 0 (0) 2 (5) .52
Level of physical activity
Vigorous 17 (68) 15 (35) .0046*
Moderate 8 (32) 17 (40)
Sedentary 0 (0) 10 (23)

NOTE. Values are mean SD, n (%), or as otherwise indicated.


*Testing between groups was conducted using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons to maintain statistically significant
difference at P.05.

Only data from 42 MCI subjects were available to calculate current number of prescription medications.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012


296 DUAL-TASK COMPLEXITY ON GAIT IN MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT, Montero-Odasso

Table 2: Results of 2-Way ANOVA on Gait Parameters by Cognitive Status Group and Walking Test Condition
Walking Test Condition

Repeated-Measures 2-Way
Gait Variable Group Usual Gait Naming Animals Serial 7s ANOVA* (P)

Gait Velocity (cm/s) Control (n25) 134.0021.70 115.8829.26 106.2528.43 Group .0001
MCI (n43) 110.9820.67 89.2625.25 74.7924.40 Condition .0001
Interaction .23

Stride time (ms) Control (n25) 1035.7466.54 1153.42144.77 1219.04230.54 Group .0008
MCI (n43) 1120.8894.72 1390.11361.56 1649.70658.40 Condition .0001
Interaction .0165

Gait variability (% CoV) Control (n24) 1.860.66 3.592.95 3.743.31 Group .0013
MCI (n43) 2.681.31 7.167.76 9.8410.13 Condition .0001
Interaction .0419

NOTE. Values are mean SD or as otherwise indicated.


Abbreviation: % CoV, percent coefficient of variation in stride time.
*P value reported for Main Effect of Group, Condition, and the interaction of Group x Condition with statistical significance set at P.05.

with MCI and Alzheimers disease2,34,35; however, this is 1 of lenge in our study is in agreement with a previous study39,40 of
the first studies to assess and compare the effect of 2 different physically frail older adults. In the same vein, the present
types of dual tasks and to assess their effect on gait variability findings are consistent with a recent study41 evaluating the
in people with MCI. effect of different types of dual-task challenges in healthy older
Previously, it has been demonstrated that motor dysfunction adults. In that study, 2 different types of dual tasks, phoneme
is present in older adults who will eventually develop cognitive monitoring and serial subtractions, had different effects on gait
decline and dementia.36-38 This motor dysfunction, evaluated performance. The authors postulated that these 2 types of dual
as gait velocity, may have its onset up to 12 years before MCI tasks might tax distinct cognitive resources since the nature of
conversion.36 Therefore, the presence of a slower gait in our the dual task also differs: attentional demands of phoneme
MCI group is consistent with these findings. Interestingly, both monitoring are essentially uniform over time, when compared
groups in our study had a normal gait velocity (1m/s) under with the increased attention needed while doing serial subtrac-
the single-task test condition. However, and in line with our tions.
first hypothesis, the type of dual task applied showed a differ- In the present study, gait variability was differentially
ential effect on the gait performance, principally in the MCI affected by the type of dual task performed, more signifi-
group. A more complex cognitive task with a profound cortical cantly in people with MCI (see fig 3), demonstrating that
demand, such as subtracting serial 7s from 100, produced a this gait-derived parameter is a very sensitive quantitative
greater dual-task detrimental effect on gait performance when measure of the concurrent task. These results underscore the
compared with a less demanding task such as naming animals, observation that the effects on gait are related to the nature
as shown in figure 3. The significant and graded decline on gait
velocity while engaging in a more demanding dual-task chal-

Fig 2. Mean gait variability in older adults with MCI (n43) and
Fig 1. Mean gait velocity, with SDs, in participants with MCI (n43) normal cognition (n25) while performing a usual walking task and
and normal cognition (n25) while performing a usual walking task 2 dual-task walking conditions. Abbreviation: % CoV, percent coef-
and 2 dual-task walking conditions. ficient of variation in stride time.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012


DUAL-TASK COMPLEXITY ON GAIT IN MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT, Montero-Odasso 297

Fig 3. Interaction between cognitive status and 3 different walking conditions for gait velocity (A) and gait variability (B). Abbreviation: %
CoV, percent coefficient of variation in stride time.

of the dual task, the aspect of the gait under study, and the loads might generate differential decrements in gait perfor-
cognitive reserve of the subject. mance. This is particularly important for the comparison of
Our findings have novel research and clinical implications. results across studies, as different tasks have the potential to
From a research perspective, we found that gait variability is identify different populations of at-risk individuals.
more sensitive to dual-tasking load than gait velocity, which A potential clinical implication of our findings relates to the
may help to refine the measures used in future research studies identification of possible fall risk and mobility dysfunction in
for fall risk in cognitively impaired populations. Additionally, people with MCI. Gait variability is an established marker of
we showed that the type of dual tasking with different cognitive fall risk, and it has been prospectively proven to predict falls

Fig 4. Effect of complex dual-task load (serials subtractions by 7s) in stride time in a participant with normal cognition (A) compared with
a participant with MCI (B).

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012


298 DUAL-TASK COMPLEXITY ON GAIT IN MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT, Montero-Odasso

even when gait velocity failed to identify fallers in community- International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment.
dwelling older adults. Additionally, the recent advent of por- J Intern Med 2004;256:240-6.
table gait mats allows for easy quantification for gait variability 5. Camicioli R, Majumdar SR. Relationship between mild cognitive
in clinical settings, facilitating fall risk identification in older impairment and falls in older people with and without Parkinsons
people who have an otherwise unremarkable gait performance. disease: 1-year prospective cohort study. Gait Posture 2010;32:
Recent recommendations concerning fall prevention and inter- 87-91.
ventions to decrease falls among cognitively impaired older 6. Liu-Ambrose TY, Ashe MC, Graf P, Beattie BL, Khan KM.
adults advocate to screen/test people with MCI according to the Increased risk of falling in older community-dwelling women with
evidence available for cognitively intact, community-dwelling mild cognitive impairment. Phys Ther 2008;88:1482-91.
older adults.42 However, we have found that changes in gait 7. Hausdorff JM, Yogev G. Cognitive function may be important for
behavior under dual tasking are a clear marker of fall risk in fall injury prevention trials. J Am Geriatr Soc 2006;54:865-6.
older adults with MCI. This suggests that people with MCI, 8. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of exec-
when they are exposed to a cognitive challenge while walking, utive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord 2008;23:329-42.
have an increased risk of falls. Interestingly, gait velocity under 9. Hausdorff JM, Yogev G, Springer S, Simon ES, Giladi N. Walk-
single-task condition was within normal limits in both groups ing is more like catching than tapping: gait in the elderly as a
(1m/s), and only dual-task testing revealed abnormal slow- complex cognitive task. Exp Brain Res 2005;164:541-8.
ing (1 m/s) and gait instability (high variability) in the MCI 10. Lundin-Olsson L, Nyberg L, Gustafson Y. Stops walking when
group. Therefore, dual-task assessment may be a more sensi- talking as a predictor of falls in elderly people. Lancet 1997;349:
tive measure of gait impairment than single-task assessment, 617.
thereby providing a means to identify cognitively impaired 11. Abernethy B, Hanna A, Plooy A. The attentional demands of
persons at risk of falls. A future prospective study is needed to preferred and non-preferred gait patterns. Gait Posture 2002;15:
test this hypothesis. 256-65.
12. Marquis S, Moore MM, Howieson DB, et al. Independent predic-
Study Limitations tors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly persons. Arch Neurol
Some limitations of this study should be noted. In the data 2002;59:601-6.
analysis, we were not able to adjust for all potential confound- 13. Camicioli R, Howieson D, Lehman S, Kaye J. Talking while
ers that may affect gait and cognition; however, our findings walking: the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimers
were maintained after controlling for age and a history of falls. disease. Neurology 1997;48:955-8.
The limited sample size precluded our ability to further explore 14. Ble A, Volpato S, Zuliani G, et al. Executive function correlates
associations between MCI subtypes or impairment in specific with walking speed in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Am
cognitive domains, such as executive and attentional function, Geriatr Soc 2005;53:410-5.
with the use of the dual-task cost approach.43 These limitations 15. Beauchet O, Annweiler C, Dubost V, et al. Stops walking when
do not detract or alter the importance of our findings, which talking: a predictor of falls in older adults? Eur J Neurol 2009;
showed a differential effect of dual-task challenges in gait 16:786-95.
variability in people with MCI when compared with cogni- 16. Montero-Odasso M, Bergman H, Phillips NA, Wong CH, Sourial
tively normal older adults. N, Chertkow H. Dual-tasking and gait in people with mild cog-
nitive impairment. The effect of working memory. BMC Geriatr
CONCLUSIONS 2009;9:41.
17. Montero-Odasso M, Schapira M, Soriano ER, et al. Gait velocity
Dual-task load impairs and increases gait variability in peo- as a single predictor of adverse events in healthy seniors aged 75
ple with MCI, an effect that was larger than the effect seen on years and older. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2005;60:1304-9.
gait velocity. These findings also suggest that cognitive control 18. Newell KM, Corcos DM. Issues in variability and motor control.
of gait is differentially challenged by the complexity of dual- In: Newell KM, Corcos DM, editors. Variability and motor con-
task load. Older people with MCI had an impaired ability to trol. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1993. p 1-12.
regulate and minimize gait variability while performing dual- 19. Hausdorff JM. Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning.
task challenges, and the magnitude of this impairment is related J Neuroeng Rehabil 2005;2:19.
to the kind and complexity of the dual task performed. Our 20. Brach JS, Studenski SA, Perera S, VanSwearingen JM, Newman
findings may explain the high risk of falls recently described in AB. Gait variability and the risk of incident mobility disability in
this population, and suggest that their fall risk should not be community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
assessed using methodology established among cognitively 2007;62:983-8.
normal older adults. 21. Brach JS, Perera S, Studenski S, Newman AB. The reliability and
Acknowledgments: We thank Maggie Hall for her help in the validity of measures of gait variability in community-dwelling
participants assessment and data gathering, and Kevin Hansen for his older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2008;89:2293-6.
help in the manuscript preparation. 22. Herman T, Mirelman A, Giladi N, Schweiger A, Hausdorff JM.
Executive control deficits as a prodrome to falls in healthy older
References adults: a prospective study linking thinking, walking, and falling.
1. Shaw FE. Falls in cognitive impairment and dementia. Clin Geri- J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2010;65:1086-92.
atr Med 2002;18:159-73. 23. Verghese J, Holtzer R, Lipton RB, Wang C. Quantitative gait
2. Sheridan PL, Hausdorff JM. The role of higher-level cognitive markers and incident fall risk in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol
function in gait: executive dysfunction contributes to fall risk in Sci Med Sci 2009;64:896-901.
Alzheimers disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007; 24. Hausdorff JM, Rios DA, Edelberg HK. Gait variability and fall
24:125-37. risk in community-living older adults: a 1-year prospective study.
3. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001;82:1050-6.
J Intern Med 2004;256:183-94. 25. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-
4. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impair- maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontol-
ment beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the ogist 1969;9:179-86.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012


DUAL-TASK COMPLEXITY ON GAIT IN MILD COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT, Montero-Odasso 299

26. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. A 36. Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Aggarwal NT, Tang Y, Bennett DA. Mild
practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the cognitive impairment: risk of Alzheimer disease and rate of cog-
clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-98. nitive decline. Neurology 2006;67:441-5.
27. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, et al. The Montreal 37. Buracchio T, Dodge HH, Howieson D, Wasserman D, Kaye J.
Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment.
cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53:695-9. Arch Neurol 2010;67:980-6.
28. Smith T, Gildeh N, Holmes C. The Montreal Cognitive Assess- 38. Camicioli R, Howieson D, Oken B, Sexton G, Kaye J. Motor
ment: validity and utility in a memory clinic setting. Can J Psy- slowing precedes cognitive impairment in the oldest old. Neurol-
chiatry 2007;52:329-32. ogy 1998;50:1496-8.
29. Montero-Odasso M, Muir SW. Simplifying detection of mild
39. Beauchet O, Dubost V, Gonthier R, Kressig RW. Dual-task-
cognitive impairment subtypes. J Am Geriatr Soc 2010;58:992-4.
related gait changes in transitionally frail older adults: the type of
30. Hittmair-Delazer M, Semenza C, Denes G. Concepts and facts in
the walking-associated cognitive task matters. Gerontology 2005;
calculation. Brain 1994;117(Pt 4):715-28.
31. Weiss EM, Siedentopf C, Hofer A, et al. Brain activation pattern 51:48-52.
during a verbal fluency test in healthy male and female volunteers: 40. Beauchet O, Dubost V, Aminian K, Gonthier R, Kressig RW.
a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett Dual-task-related gait changes in the elderly: does the type of
2003;352:191-4. cognitive task matter? J Mot Behav 2005;37:259-64.
32. Verghese J, Kuslansky G, Holtzer R, et al. Walking while talking: 41. Hausdorff JM, Schweiger A, Herman T, Yogev-Seligmann G,
effect of task prioritization in the elderly. Arch Phys Med Rehabil Giladi N. Dual-task decrements in gait: contributing factors
2007;88:50-3. among healthy older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci
33. Montero-Odasso M, Casas A, Hansen KT, et al. Quantitative gait 2008;63:1335-43.
analysis under dual-task in older people with mild cognitive 42. Shaw FE. Prevention of falls in older people with dementia.
impairment: a reliability study. J Neuroeng Rehabil 2009;6:35. J Neural Transm 2007;114:1259-64.
34. Maquet D, Lekeu F, Warzee E, et al. Gait analysis in elderly adult 43. Persad CC, Jones JL, Ashton-Miller JA, Alexander NB, Gior-
patients with mild cognitive impairment and patients with mild dani B. Executive function and gait in older adults with cog-
Alzheimers disease: simple versus dual task: a preliminary report. nitive impairment. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2008;63:
Clin Physiol Funct Imaging 2010;30:51-6. 1350-5.
35. Sheridan PL, Solomont J, Kowall N, Hausdorff JM. Influence of
executive function on locomotor function: divided attention in- Suppliers
creases gait variability in Alzheimers disease. J Am Geriatr Soc a. CIR Systems Inc, 69 Garlor Dr, Havertown, PA 19083.
2003;51:1633-7. b. SAS Institute Inc, 100 SAS Campus Dr, Cary, NC 27513.

Arch Phys Med Rehabil Vol 93, February 2012

S-ar putea să vă placă și