Sunteți pe pagina 1din 15

Chapter 7

Ethnomethodology and
Conversation Analysis
Paul ten Have
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Ethnomethodology (EM) and conversation analysis The Emergence of the


(CA) are the somewhat confusing names of two Ethnomethodological Program
related research traditions that were developed in
To summarize a rather complex net of influences and
the 1960s in the United States by Harold Garfinkel
inspirations, it can be said that Harold Garfinkel’s
and by Harvey Sacks (with his coworkers Emanuel
initiative started with his critical confrontation with
Schegloff and Gail Jefferson), respectively. The
the theories of his dissertation supervisor, Talcott
contexts for their initiatives were the theoretical
Parsons at Harvard, from a phenomenological per-
and methodological debates in sociology that were
spective informed by the work of Schutz, Gurwitsch,
current at the time. Since then, EM and CA have
and Husserl (Garfinkel, 1967; Heritage, 1984b; Rawls
become more or less established paradigms that
in Garfinkel, 2002). When he was involved with a
have attracted researchers from a range of disci-
study of jury deliberations and listened to the tapes
plines around the world. As a first characterization,
of those discussions, he was struck by the carefulness
one can note their interest in the detailed ways in
of the jury members’ commonsense reasoning: their
which members of society collaboratively consti-
serious “lay methodology.” So, in line with anthropo-
tute the situations in which they find themselves
logical disciplines like ethnomedicine and ethnobot-
and the (inter)actions that take place in those situ-
any, he coined the term ethnomethodology for the
ations. The general sociological issue of social
study of the methods that ordinary members of soci-
order is reconsidered as a local achievement of
ety use and take for granted. As a professor in sociol-
members in situations, and the research objective
ogy at the University of California, Los Angeles, he
is to explicate how this is done in an accountable
undertook a series of empirical studies to further
way. CA can be seen as a specialized form of EM,
develop EM as a research program. Some of these
originally focusing on verbal interaction and later
studies used a procedure of breaching commonsense
also considering nonvocal aspects. It has developed
background expectancies as demonstrations of their
a rather specific, relatively conventionalized
importance in everyday sense making. It turned out
research style using audio or video recordings and
that such breaches were often experienced as upset-
detailed transcripts as basic data. Other forms of
ting and that they led to instant efforts to normalize
EM use a much wider range of data types, such as
the situation (Garfinkel, 1967, pp. 35–75).
ethnographic observations, while the approach is
These background expectancies are part of an
closely fitted to the chosen topic and the properties
immense collection of taken-for-granted, “seen but
of the research site.

The author thanks Paul Camic, Harrie Mazeland, and Jonathan Potter for their remarks on earlier versions of this chapter.

DOI: 10.1037/13620-007
APA Handbook of Research Methods in Psychology: Vol. 2. Research Designs, H. Cooper (Editor-in-Chief)
103
Copyright © 2012 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
Paul ten Have

unnoticed,” and silently presupposed capacities and visibly-rational-and-reportable-for-all-


understandings that can be taken as foundational for practical-purposes, i.e., “accountable,” as
both ordinary and specialized forms of social life. organizations of commonplace everyday
Imagine trying to make a dish by following a recipe activities. The reflexivity of that phe-
from a cookbook without understanding what the nomenon is a singular feature of practi-
terms used meant, how to handle a knife to cut the cal actions, of practical circumstances,
vegetables, how to boil water, and so on. It is this of common sense knowledge of social
level of presupposed, ordinarily unnoticed but structures, and of practical sociological
essential capacities and understandings that is of reasoning. By permitting us to locate and
interest to ethnomethodologists. Of course, they do examine their occurrence the reflexiv-
not limit their attention to things like following a ity of that phenomenon establishes their
recipe. They have, indeed, studied this level of study. (Garfinkel, 1967, vii)
unnoticed phenomena for a very wide range of
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

activities. The first sentence tells us that EM studies focus


A critical point in all this is that the level of con- on the fact that members in their ordinary actions
crete capacities and understandings is not only in various ways make the sense and purpose of
ignored in instructions such as recipes but also in those actions recognizable for other members.
all sorts of plans; reports; and scientific analyses of Accountability refers to the moral obligation among
various sorts, including, of course, sociology. In members to produce their actions in ways that are
that discipline, social life is generally investigated in recognizably reasonable in the situation in which
terms of pregiven concepts or preelaborated theo- they are done. By using reflexivity, Garfinkel
ries, on the basis of data gathered in terms of pre- stresses the self-explicating and self-referencing
given categories and methods. In fact, Talcott aspect of practical actions. What is made clear in
Parsons was a champion of this approach of first the doing of an action is its local and situated mean-
elaborating an extensive, rationally organized cate- ing, what anthropologists might characterize as
gory system, before doing any empirical work. In emic in contrast to etic. The frequent use of words
contrast to this external top-down approach, Gar- like everyday, practical, and common sense further
finkel maintained that members of society should stresses the EM focus on the achieved ordinariness
not be treated as “dopes” whose task it was to act of actions. It is the local sense displays rather than
according to some theories but rather as actually some theorized general sense of actions that is of
capable of “doing social life,” which can be investi- interest to ethnomethodologists. The last sentence
gated “from within actual settings” (Garfinkel, in the excerpt formulates the methodological basis
1967, p. 68). of EM studies. To conduct EM studies, the eth-
To explicate the specifics of his approach, Garfin- nomethodologist has to immerse himself or herself
kel developed a complex writing style, which tends in the situation of interest, mentally if not physi-
to be rather incomprehensible to the uninitiated, cally. One has to acquire “membership knowledge”
and which, in combination with the breaching pro- to become what Garfinkel has elsewhere called
cedures, led quite often to strongly negative reac- “vulgarly competent” in the setting (Garfinkel &
tions from other sociologists. I discuss a key excerpt Wieder, 1992).
from the preface to Studies in Ethnomethodology In later work, Garfinkel elaborated the eth-
(Garfinkel, 1967) to present Garfinkel’s general nomethodological program in various ways, but
ideas and to familiarize readers with his style and most often in terms of the contrast between EM and
basic concepts. the established sociological approach characterized
as constructive or formal analysis. The general theme
Ethnomethodological studies analyze of these elaborations is that in EM studies the phe-
everyday activities as members’ meth- nomena that established approaches study in terms
ods for making those same activities of predefined concepts of social order are respecified

104
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

as members’ methods for creating a locally account- gives them access to the fine details of the verbal and
able order. In their search for analytic generalities, visible activities in ordinary settings of the specific
mainstream sociologists ignore or gloss over the spe- form of life.
cific, locally required concrete details of actions, which In their Invitation to Ethnomethodology, Francis
are the phenomena of interest for ethnomethodolo- and Hester (2004) formulated the process of EM
gists (cf. Garfinkel, 2002). research in the following summary way:
Doing ethnomethodology involves tak-
Ethnomethodology’s Methods
ing three methodological steps: 1. Notice
As the phenomena of interest for an ethnomethodol-
something that is observably-the-case
ogist occur on a level of seen but unnoticed or used
about some talk, activity or setting. 2.
but taken for granted, a first practical problem for
Pose the question “How is it that this
EM is how to make those phenomena noticeable.
observable feature has been produced
Garfinkel’s own efforts, as reported in his Studies in
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

such that it is recognizable for what it


Ethnomethodology (Garfinkel, 1967), were quite
is?” 3. Consider, analyse and describe
strongly oriented to this problem of noticeability, or
the methods used in the production and
rather as an effort to demonstrate the existence and
recognition of the observable feature.
relevance of phenomena of this type. His so-called
(pp. 25–26)
breaching experiments were part of a pedagogy, a
strategy to convince his students and colleagues that And they added,
he was after something relevant, not just for sociol-
In other words, then, ethnomethodol-
ogy but for everyday life. By breaching expectancies,
ogy starts out with what might be called
or having them breached by his students, such as
the “common-sense appearance of the
asking for the meaning of ordinary expressions, he
social world” and then seeks to describe
did two things. He made noticeable their relevance
how they will have been produced “from
and moral quality in the production of everyday sit-
within” such that they do indeed have
uations, and he provoked extra sense-making activi-
the appearances they have. (p. 26)
ties to repair the experienced disturbances. The
people exposed to the breaches showed they were
puzzled and shocked, but they immediately made Some Examples of Ethnomethodological
efforts to normalize the situation, by taking it as a Study
joke or finding some other motive or explanation for Scanning any list of ethnomethodological studies,
the breach. In addition to these provocative inter- one may be struck by the enormous variety of top-
ventions, Garfinkel also studied what might be ics, or rather of settings and activity types studied.
called natural breaches, such as the efforts by a bio- There are, for instance, studies of the ways in which
logical male to pass as a natural female. both staff and inmates used a so-called convict code
Later ethnomethodologists turned to other to explain situations in a halfway house for former
means to study the details of situated practices, most drug users (Wieder, 1974), of laboratory practices
often intense ethnography supplemented with focusing on the identification of artifacts (Lynch,
audiovisual recordings of in situ activities (cf. Chap- 1985), of the art of piano improvization (Sudnow,
ter 10 of this volume). This serves two purposes. On 1978), of the interactions of users with a complex
the one hand, it allows researchers to acquire a cer- copy machine (Suchman, 1987, 2007), or of the rea-
tain measure of membership knowledge by immers- soning practices of Tibetan monks (Liberman,
ing themselves in the social life of the setting under 2004).
study. In this way, they gain access to relevant local These examples can be invoked to illustrate
understandings and even some competence to act as several of the methodological aspects of EM
a member. And on the other hand, the ethnographic research. Wieder (1974) has done intensive eth-
fieldwork (and especially the audiovisual recordings) nography, which Lynch (1985) and Liberman

105
Paul ten Have

(2004) have also used, together with recordings. The studies by Lynch (1985, 1993), Sudnow
Sudnow (1978) has studied his own actions. Wie- (1978), and Liberman (2004) illustrated the fact
der, in his report, referred to a tradition of ethnog- that to do an ethnomethodological study of some
raphy in which a local subculture, such as the specialized activity, the researcher has to have or
convict code, is invoked to explain the actions of a acquire a sufficient level of competence in doing
local gang of people. In contrast to this tradition, that activity (Garfinkel & Wieder, 1992; Lynch,
he described the ways in which such a subculture 1993, pp. 265–308). This is a special case of the
is actually used by its members to account for the more general, noted requirement that an eth-
situation in which they have to act as they do, as a nomethodologist has to have the relevant member-
“folk sociology.” Here access was mainly gained by ship knowledge to understand, in the sense used
his experience in the setting, by seeing and hearing earlier, members’ practical activities (ten Have,
the inmates and staff members telling the code, 2002). So Lynch had to have some competence in
and also in accounting for their actions in dealing reading images produced by the electronic micro-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

with him. scope, Sudnow had to be able to play the piano, and
In the study by Suchman (1987, 2007), a special Liberman had to learn the Tibetan language as well
setup was created in which two subjects were asked as acquire a basic understanding of Tibetan Bud-
to carry out some tasks at the copier, with one hav- dhist philosophy. The practical issue, of course, is
ing to read out the instructions and the displays, how far the researcher should go in this matter.
while the other had to handle the practical actions at A persistent theme in both the programmatic
the machine. In this way, she gained access to the statements of EM as well as most if not all eth-
users’ situated reasoning, which she could confront nomethodological studies is the discrepancy
with the preplanned reasoning implemented in the between, on the one hand, general statements and
machine, as part of an artificial intelligence project. ideas about practical activities, like plans, instruc-
In her analysis she contrasted the plans imple- tions, protocols, and formal accounts, and on the
mented in the machine with the observed situated other, the activities to which they refer in their
actions. She stressed the inability of such plans to actual, particular, and methodic detail. This theme
foresee the local circumstances and ad hoc practical has surfaced in the discussions of Wieder’s and
reasonings that guide the actual actions of the users Suchman’s studies, but is also discernable in the oth-
at the machine in situ. ers mentioned. As Heap (1990, pp. 42–43) has
In whatever way access to the phenomena of noted, ethnomethodological studies can bring differ-
interest is acquired, the researcher will have to ent kinds of “news”: In a “critical news approach,”
produce a record in one way or another, as the researcher claims that things are not as they
ethnographic notes, audio or visual recordings, or appear or are presented, whereas in a “positive news
transcriptions thereof, as an aid to the process of approach,” the message is that something is orga-
noticing in the sense of the quote from Francis and nized in a particular way, which is of interest in
Hester (2004). These records constitute the material itself. I return to these issues in a later section on
the researcher works on, fixing what has happened, applied studies.
in a way objectifying these happenings to help make
them “anthropologically strange” rather than just to
A Major Offshoot: Conversation
be taken for granted (Garfinkel, 1967, p. 9). Almost
Analysis
always, EM research reports contain quotes, longer
fragments or pictures from such records, inviting Garfinkel’s (1967) explorations quite soon became a
the readers to check what is reported with the data source of inspiration for a number of other sociolo-
as rendered in such quotes and excerpts. So record- gists of younger generations. A prominent one was
ings serve a double purpose: as research materials Sacks, who came to focus on spontaneous talk, that
to explore and as demonstrations to support the is, conversation. Both Sacks and his close colleague
arguments. Schegloff had studied with Goffman, who was

106
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

already famous for his studies of interaction, organization is that a turn-at-talking consists of one
although in a different framework than the one Sacks or more units, such as sentences or single words like
came to develop. At first, Sacks explored various yes, called turn-constructional units (TCUs). When
aspects of verbal interaction, including the use of one such unit comes to a possible end, the turn
categorization of persons, in what later became might go to another participant; this moment is
known as membership categorization analysis (MCA; called a transition-relevance place (TRP). Then the
see the section A Minor Offshoot), but gradually he current speaker may select another to take over, or
concentrated on the actual organization of talking another may self-select to speak, or the current
together, in collaboration with Schegloff and later speaker may continue, either immediately or a bit
Jefferson. This became what is now known as CA. later (see Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson, 1974, for
Since their publication of a paper on the organization details and illustrations).
of turn-taking in 1974, CA became a recognizable The organization of sequences, more or less
paradigm in the Kuhnian sense, a research tradition coherent sets of subsequent turns, is rather more
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

with its own approach to data and analysis, with a complicated, as can be seen in a recent book-length
prominent exemplar, and a growing set of people overview by Schegloff (2007b). The core idea is that
working along the lines set out by the originators. a sequence can minimally consist of a pair of turns,
The relations between EM and CA can be charac- one acting as an initiative such as a question, to be
terized as ambivalent (cf. Clayman & Maynard, followed immediately by a response such as an
1995). Like EM, CA also studies the ways in which answer. The technical term for such a two-part
members of society organize their dealings with each sequence is adjacency pair (Schegloff & Sacks, 1973,
other with an eye on the local circumstances in which pp. 295–296), and other examples besides question–
they find themselves and with a stress on the local answer are invitation–acceptance/decline, greeting–
achievement of a social order. But whereas EM tended greeting, and so forth. In sequences consisting of
more and more to stress the local specificity of these more than two parts, the core of such sequences
methods, CA would often formulate those in more quite often consists of one basic adjacency pair to
general terms, as is clearly the case in the paper on which other parts are added in a sequence expansion.
turn-taking. Furthermore, whereas ethnomethodolo- The first speaker may, for instance, react to the sec-
gists have studied many aspects of an immense variety ond part with a third-turn response, such as a thanks
of situations, CA concentrated, at least originally, on or an evaluation of the response. The core sequence
the organization of talking together in itself, whatever may also be prepared in some way by a so-called
the situation. Later, many conversation analysts presequence, such as, “May I ask you a question?”
turned to studying how the specifics of talk related to Another possibility is that the response to the first
aspects of the situation in which it occurred, quite part of the pair is delayed, for instance, by an
often an institutional setting. Although the expression inquiry like “Now or tomorrow?” which starts an
CA is well established, at the initiative of Schegloff inserted sequence. And, of course, the core sequence
(1987), CA’s object is now mostly termed talk-in- may be followed up in many ways by postexpansions.
interaction. And although CA originally was con- A few more technical terms deserve to be expli-
ducted almost exclusively on the basis of audio cated, if only to show the kinds of complexities of
recordings, video is now in general use. In a way, this sequences considered in CA. First-pair parts like
seems to have brought EM and CA closer together invitations, requests, proposals, or accusations have
again, as current studies of multimodal activities often possible second-pair parts that can be divided into
combine inspirations from both EM and CA, as I two alternate types, such as acceptance or declina-
illustrate later in this chapter. tion. The ways in which responses of these two
The specificity of CA’s analytic perspective can types are designed tend to differ: Some are relatively
be characterized in terms of a restricted set of orga- short and produced promptly, whereas others
nizations, including most prominently those of turn- may be much more elaborated, delayed in various
taking and sequence. The basic idea of turn-taking ways, and accounted for in often a rather complex

107
Paul ten Have

manner. The first type has been called a preferred ering background data. It is only through the obser-
response and the latter dispreferred. Accepting an vation of the details of interaction that CA can be
invitation tends to be done using the first, preferred convincingly done. As in EM generally, CA prefers
format, whereas a declination will be done in the naturally occurring situations, that is, situations in
second, dispreferred way. This is one example of which the impact of the researcher is minimal or
what more generally is called preference organiza- absent. This, of course, is an important contrast with
tion, which can be observed in many other sequen- other types of social and behavioral sciences, but it
tial environments as well. It should be stressed that fits EM’s and CA’s situationalist interests and their
preference here does not refer to any psychological generally observational approach.
entity, such as the willingness or not to accept an The most important aspect of working with
invitation, but just to the conventional formatting of recordings is that it allows repeated inspection, lis-
turns. It has been suggested that these various forms tening, or viewing, as the case may be. The results of
of preference have to do with an underlying very this observational process are laid down in a tran-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

general preference for progressivity, that is, to get on script, in which the words spoken are rendered in a
or to move forward in any interactive activity (Sche- way that suggests how this was done, partly with a
gloff, 1979; Stivers & Robinson, 2006). specialized use of typographical symbols. Jefferson
Insertion sequences are quite often initiated to has developed a set of conventions for this job,
clarify the meaning and purpose of a first-pair part. which is now in general use (cf. Jefferson, 2004).
In that case it can also be called a repair sequence. The example in Extract 7.1 may illustrate some of
Repair can be initiated any time a speaker decides to the complexities involved.
do something about a preceding utterance, the so-
called trouble source, which is in some way problem-
atic. And, of course, a repair can be initiated and Extract 7.1
done by the speaker of that trouble source, which is Coworkers on the Phone
called self-initiated self-repair. That is in fact the pre-
ferred way of doing repair (Schegloff, Jefferson, &
Sacks, 1977), compared with other-initiated self- (Coworkers Maggie and Sorrell went to a wedding reception
where Maggie had some sort of momentary blackout
repair or other-repair. Here also, the preference for and felt ill. Next morning she phones Sorrell at work to
progressivity can be seen at work; self-repair, espe- say that she will not be coming to work, is going to the
cially when done quickly, is the least disturbing to doctor)
the flow of the conversation of these alternatives. 1  Maggie: .hh because I (.) you know I told Mother
what”d ha:ppened yesterday
Whereas the original concepts of CA, like the 2  there at the party,
ones noted thus far, have been mainly developed on 3  Sorrell: [° Yeah.°]
the basis of audio recordings, it has become clear by 4  Maggie: [ a:: ] n d uh, .hhhhh (0.2) uh you know she
analyzing videos that visual impressions also may asked me if it was
impact heavily on the social organization of face-to- 5  because I’d had too much to dri:nk and I
said no=
face interactions. Goodwin (1981), who initiated the
6  Sorrell: =[No :::::.]
use of video for CA study, has shown, for instance, 7  Maggie: =[because at the t]i:me I’d only ha:d,h you
that speakers may adapt their utterances-in-course know that drink “n
in response to visual displays of attention, agree- 8  a ha:lf when we were going through the
ment, disagreement, puzzlement, and so forth. receiving line.
9  Sorrell: Ri:ght.

Doing CA: Recordings and Transcriptions Note. From “Is ‘No’ an Acknowledgment Token?
EM tends to use two kinds of data-gathering meth- Comparing American and British Uses of (+)/(−)
Tokens,” by G. Jefferson, 2002, Journal of Prag-
ods: intensive ethnography or recordings. For CA
matics, 34, p. 1346. Copyright 2002 by Elsevier.
the use of recordings is absolutely required, whereas Reprinted with permission.
ethnography may be used as an adjunct way of gath-

108
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

The extract is introduced, within double brack- In a typical CA approach (see ten Have, 2007, for
ets, with a description of the interactional context, a more extensive treatment), the researcher starts
to assist the reader in understanding the episode. In with inspecting some recordings or transcripts look-
the actual transcript a number of special symbols are ing for episodes that seem somehow interesting,
used to render the specific, hearable details of the maybe puzzling or especially apt. That episode
speech production. Underlining indicates stress; should be analyzed in depth using CA’s conceptual
square brackets ([]) are used to indicate those parts repertoire. As argued elsewhere (ten Have, 2007),
that were spoken in overlap; when one utterance such an analysis essentially takes two steps, which I
follows another very quickly, this is indicated by an call understanding and the analysis proper. Listening
equal sign (=); and a marked prolongation of a to the recording and reading the transcript, the ana-
sound is indicated by the colons (:). Punctuation lyst first tries to understand what the interactants
marks are used to indicate intonation: a question are doing organizationally when they speak as they
mark indicates a rising tone, a comma indicates a do. They may, for instance, be requesting informa-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

nonfinal flat tone, and a dot indicates a downward tion, offering to tell a story, changing the topic, and
falling tone. The dot-preceded .hhh, finally, renders so forth. Such understandings will be based, at first,
an inbreath. In interaction the shaping of an utter- on the researcher’s own membership knowledge, as,
ance and the timing of one in relation to others is one might say, a cultural colleague of the speakers.
essential, therefore such details have to be noted as Second, however, the analyst will check the sequen-
exactly as possible. tial context and especially the uptake of the utter-
ances in question in subsequent talk, immediately
CA as a Data-Driven Approach following or later in the conversation, for instance,
It has often been noted that CA uses a data-driven by granting a request. Understanding the actions,
approach. This means that at first the researcher although not the purpose of the research, is a neces-
takes an attitude that has been characterized as sary requirement for the next step, the analysis
unmotivated looking, that is, without a prespecified proper, which is to formulate the procedures used to
problem or question in mind. This does not deny the accomplish the actions as understood. Because CA’s
fact that one has a broadly focused interest in CA in interest is organizational and procedural, the ulti-
the how of interactional organization. Furthermore, mate object of CA research is what Schegloff (1992)
now that there is a well-defined and elaborate tradi- has called the procedural infrastructure of interaction,
tion, the CA is structured by an extensive conceptual and, in particular, the practices of talking in conver-
apparatus, which would be silly to ignore (see the sation. This means that conversational practices are
section A Major Offshoot). So although much is not analyzed in terms of individual properties or
already known about the organization of interactive institutional expectations, but as situated collabora-
talk, the interest in much current CA work is still to tive accomplishments.
discover previously unnoticed phenomena. At the Such an analysis results in an analytic formula-
same time, one would also want to extend and refine tion of a device, a typical sequence, or whatever may
what is known or to apply CA to new substantive be reported as such in a single-case study. Most of
areas. The news that CA can offer to existing the time, however, the researcher will go on to
non-CA knowledge is often that the latter is too sim- inspect other cases, which may be relevantly com-
ple in the sense that it ignores or glosses over the pared with the first one. This may lead to a confir-
actual details of interactive talk or that it starts from mation, a reformulation, a specification, or a
and is limited by current cultural, mostly individual- differentiation into types. The researcher may for-
istic and mentalistic, preconceptions. Like EM more mulate conditions for, and effects of, the device or
generally, it demonstrates in detail that persons act sequence, in general, its functions. This more
in more intersubjectively responsible ways than is extended type of research is often called a collection
recognized in most other approaches in the human study. The idea is that the analysis of a first case can
sciences (Button, 1991). be used as a starting point for a more systematic

109
Paul ten Have

exploration of an emerging analytic theme. The the possibility to compare conversation to other
researcher searches an available data set of newly kinds of speech-exchange systems (1974, pp. 729–731).
collected data for instances that seem to be similar Some years later, CA researchers began to take up
to the candidate phenomenon, the first formulation the implied challenge and started to study interac-
of the theme as well as data that seem to point in a tional talk in a variety of settings, such as law courts,
different direction, the so-called deviant case analy- medical consultations, new interviews, and many
sis. In short, the researcher builds a collection of rel- others. Such studies can be characterized as applied
evant cases in search of patterns that elucidate some CA and I will discuss some examples in a later sec-
procedural issues. This may seem to suggest a kind tion. But at the same time, pure CA continued to be
of principled independence of a single research proj- elaborated, by the originators, like Schegloff and
ect from existing knowledge, but this is not the case Jefferson, and by others who started doing CA later,
for all of CA reports. An investigation may take off many of whom had a background in functional or
from an issue internal to the CA tradition, or even interactional linguistics (cf. Ford, Fox, & Thomp-
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

from some problem or idea external to it, but all the son, 2002; Ochs, Schegloff, & Thompson, 1996).
same each and every piece of data should be first One of the interests that this strand of CA has added
analyzed in its own terms. to the earlier ones is the issue of linguistic resources
This stress on analyzing each instance in its own that speakers have at their disposal as members of a
terms is, of course, at odds with a general preference speech community. By comparing findings on the
for quantification, which is dominant in the social basis of the study of English-speaking interactants
sciences at large. Whether quantification makes with data on conversations in Finnish or Japanese, it
sense in CA has been debated by some of its major became clear that the devices actually used partly
practitioners, notably Schegloff (1993) and Heritage depend on what particular linguistic systems allow
(1999). The general upshot seems to a need for cau- or facilitate (cf. Hayashi, 2005; Sidnell, 2009; Sor-
tion and a limitation of sensible quantification to jonen, 2001; Tanaka, 1999). The English system, for
some of the most easily differentiable aspects, such instance, facilitates the early projection of the action
as the choice of a word in an identical position or a an utterance will be doing, whereas in Japanese the
clearly defined (yes–no) outcome (cf. examples in verbs that embody the action tend to be produced in
Heritage, 1999, and an experiment reported in Heri- sentence final positions. So Japanese speakers can
tage, Robinson, Elliott, Beckett, & Wilkes, 2007; see still change the action in their talk at a relatively late
also the discussions in Chapter 8 of this volume moment, but they may also at times use an early
contrasting discursive psychology with mainstream placeholder word if they need to inform their recipi-
methods). ents on what they are up to (Hayashi, 2005).

Later Developments in CA
A Minor Offshoot: Membership
A particular aspect of the development of CA is that
Categorization Analysis
during the first 15 years of its existence, its basic
approach and its core concepts had been developed As noted, apart from the approach that later became
by a very small number of people, mainly Sacks, known as CA, Sacks also for some time worked on a
Schegloff, and Jefferson (1974). So everything com- rather different enterprise that is currently called
ing after that could be seen as extensions, additions, MCA. He noted that a large part of the knowledge
or applications, in one or another direction. The that people use and rely on in their interactions is
CA as it emerged in the 1960s and 1970s might be organized in terms of categories of people, either in
characterized as CA-in-general, or pure CA, in the general terms (as in children) or in reference to a
sense that it took as its core task to study talk-in- particular person (as in my husband). These insights
interaction as such, without taking into account its and their elaborate explication was at first part of his
particular setting or interactional genre. In their doctoral research on calls to a suicide prevention
turn-taking paper, Sacks et al. had already hinted at center (cf. Sacks, 1972a; also a number of lectures in

110
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

Sacks, 1992) in which callers explained their life sit- Although MCA more or less disappeared from
uation and their feeling that they had “no one to the CA enterprise, it was later taken up again by
turn to.” What Sacks noted, among other things, more ethnomethodologically inclined authors,
was that people use person-categories as part of sets including Hester and Eglin (1997), Jayyusi (1984),
of categories, which he called membership categori- and Watson (1997). In the introduction to a volume
zation devices (MCDs; Sacks, 1972a, 1972b). For collecting some of these later MCA studies, Hester
instance, within the MCD sex, people use two basic and Eglin (1997, pp. 11–22) commented on an
categories, female and male, whereas the MCD age ambiguity in Sacks’s observations on membership
does not have a fixed number of categories because categorization. Some of his formulations suggest a
their use depends on situational considerations. decontextualized model of membership categories
Sometimes two categories suffice, old and young, but and collections of categories as preexisting any occa-
often more subtle differentiations are called for. sion of use, whereas at other times, he stresses the
Categories are not just named or implied, they occasionality of any actual usage. Hester and Eglin
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

also carry a number of different associated proper- (1997, p. 21) stressed that for EM “membership cat-
ties, later called category predicates, like the one that egorization is an activity carried out in particular
Sacks used a lot: category-bound activities. So, for local circumstances.” It should be seen as “in situ
instance, he noted that the activity crying may be achievements of members’ practical actions and
considered bound to the category baby, and the practical reasoning.” In recent years MCA continues
activity picking up (a child) is typical of the category to be used and debated (cf. Carlin, 2010; Schegloff,
mother (Sacks, 1972b). Other kinds of predicates 2007a, 2007c).
might involve properties like rights and responsibili- Currently, MCA is often used to analyze written
ties, specialized knowledge, and competencies. texts (Watson, 2009), but the underlying theme of
Sacks (1972a) also made an effort to explicate “rules MCA, namely, the social organization of knowledge,
of application,” such as an economy rule (one cate- emerges regularly in CA proper, for instance, in a
gory is often sufficient) and a consistency rule (once series of papers by Heritage (1984a) on the use
a category from a specific MCD is used, other cate- of Oh as a marker of a change of knowledge of
gories from that device tend to be used also). the speaker and more recently on what he has called
Although many different categories may be correct, epistemics, or more particularly epistemic rights and
there are most often only a few that are also relevant epistemic authority (Heritage, 2005; Heritage &
in the situation at hand. Raymond, 2005; Raymond & Heritage, 2006). As he
It is remarkable, as can be seen by reading Sacks’s wrote, “Interactants not only keep score on who
(1992) Lectures on Conversation in chronological knows what, they also keep rather close watch over
order, that his interest in these matters became less the relevant rights that each may have to know par-
prominent after about 1967, being more strongly ticular facts” (Heritage, 2005, p. 196), and that they
focused on issues of turn-taking and sequence. He do can be shown in the details of their actions and
did, however, publish some of these mid-1960s reactions, when analyzed in sequential terms. In my
explorations of categorization much later (Sacks, view, then, terms like EM, CA, and MCA denote
1972a, 1972b), so apparently he did not disavow variants of one big enterprise to study the local
them. In his introduction to the first volume of organization of human action or even of human
Sacks’s (1992) Lectures, Schegloff commented on sociality.
this shift as mainly a methodological one. Although
the work on membership categorization tended to
Applied Studies
stress the recognizability of expressions such as cat-
egory terms, for any member of the culture, Sacks As used in this section, the notion of application can
later sought to substantiate such claims in terms of refer both to projects in which the insights and
the demonstrable understanding by the participants, methods of EM and CA are applied to specific sub-
as visible in their uptake. stantive themes or areas and ones in which EM and

111
Paul ten Have

CA insights are used to criticize or educate specific which physicians do react to (or anticipate) patients’
practices, although the first is much more frequent moves. In other words, whereas at first it was the
than the second. The basic idea is that the detailed asymmetry that was noted, it was later seen that the
attention that EM and CA give to everyday practices asymmetry-prone context of medical interaction
can be used to extend or correct available, often also allowed for half-hidden negotiations, especially
quantitative, knowledge about such practices. Estab- about treatment options. Application in the second,
lished knowledge about some practice is often too practical sense also seems to be on the rise in recent
general to cover the details of such practices in the years, as CA-based insights and studies are used
situations in which they occur. Therefore such in medical education and professional training
knowledge, as available in theories, plans, or (Maynard & Heritage, 2005; Stein, Frankel, &
reports, is quite often not able to effectively predict Krupat, 2005).
or understand the relative success or failure of prac- When CA emerged in the 1960s, some psycho-
tical projects. This theme was already discernable in therapists were already recording their sessions with
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Suchman’s work (1987, 2007), as the instructions patients. Sacks, for instance, used some recordings,
provided by an “intelligent” copier were misunder- in his case of group therapy with teenagers, to
stood by the users, while the machine also misun- explore conversation (cf. Sacks, 1992). That these
derstood some of the users’ actions, together leading interactions were not just conversations but rather
to various type of practical troubles. therapy sessions was largely ignored in his com-
In the past 3 decades an increasing number of ments. Later, the tendency was to use data from
studies in EM and CA have been of an applied char- ordinary interactions. It was only since about the
acter. For reasons of space only a few themes can be turn of the century that CA researchers turned to
developed at some length, and only a small number psychotherapy as a topic for the application of CA.
of topical areas are mentioned in this chapter. Most Since then, a substantial number of papers and a
applied CA studies deal with interactions between book (Peräkylä, Antaki, Vehviläinen, & Leudar,
institutional agents and their clients of some sort, 2008) have been published. Of course, therapists
such as patients, defendants, interviewees, and so use quite a varied set of the verbal practices that, for
on. In line with the remarks by Sacks et al. (1974) them, constitute therapy. The challenge for CA is to
about the comparison of speech-exchange systems, explicate these practices in greater detail than has
these studies quite often (especially in the 1980s) been done before and to point out and describe fea-
used a comparison between the institutional interac- tures of interaction that are part of psychotherapy
tion studied with informal conversation as an but that psychotherapeutic theories have not recog-
implicit or explicit frame of reference. Early studies nized or discussed (Peräkylä et al., 2008). A major
of doctor–patient interaction, for instance, noted the difference between CA research and previous stud-
restrictions on turn-taking opportunities and turn- ies of psychotherapy lies in CA’s stress on sequence
type selection to which patients seemed to be sub- organization: how one utterance relates to previous
jected (cf. Frankel, 1984, 1990; West, 1984). They and subsequent utterances in detail.
also made it clear that such encounters tended to be A major characteristic of psychotherapy ses-
organized in a restricted number of phases, such as sions, in contrast to physician–patient encounters,
history taking, examination, diagnosis, and treat- is their lack of an overall structural organization
ment. What was discernable then, and confirmed in phases. Therefore, CA studies in this field tend
later (cf. Beach, 2001; Heritage & Maynard, 2006; to focus on local sequential relations, for instance,
Stivers, 2007), was that the speaking opportunities the ways in which a therapist’s comments relate to
for patients depend very much on the phase of the previous utterances by the patient, or the ways in
encounter. One can note a shift, in these later stud- which a patient reacts to such a recipient action.
ies, from a somewhat moral noting of restrictions to In psychotherapy, recipient actions are among the
a more diversified description of the uses by patients major means at the therapist’s disposal to influ-
of their speaking opportunities, and the ways in ence the patient is a more or less subtle manner.

112
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

Peräkylä et al. (2008) distinguished different types This situation is contrasted with naturally occurring
of such actions. A therapist may (a) propose a differ- situations in which an aphasic wants to tell the story
ent, probably more focused term for one used by the of a personal experience to a close relative, for
patient, (b) extend a patient’s sentence as a display instance, or participate in the arrangement of eating
of understanding, (c) reformulate what was just out with a family group. In such situations it is the
said, or (d) offer a different interpretation than the achievement of shared understanding that matters
one suggested by the patient. In so doing, the thera- and not the objective assessment of the linguistic
pists may be seen as “stretching the boundaries of quality of an individual performance.
ownership of knowledge” (Peräkylä et al., 2008, The concept of aphasia covers a range of impair-
pp. 192–193), suggesting that they know the patient ments, and the patients whose speech is exemplified
better than the individual. Such reworkings are and analyzed in the various chapters suffer from a
mostly selective and thus reshape what was said by variety of types of linguistic insufficiencies, includ-
the other party. Therapists’ questioning initiatives ing quite prominently the inability to find the right
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

and patients’ responses to therapist sayings are also word in time. In such cases, the speakers often use
discussed. CA studies of psychotherapy offer a general terms instead of a more specific ones, requir-
detailed sociological account of therapeutic pro- ing a more specific interpretation or (implicit or
cesses rather than a psychological one. CA fore- explicit) guess made on the basis of shared knowl-
grounds the collaborative and negotiative character edge or situational cues. This also accounts for the
of such interactions. What therapists do is a special- fact that mutual understanding is best achieved in
ized use of quite ordinary conversational means. active collaboration with intimates.
From the mid-1990s onward, there have been Although these three types of applied studies
quite a number of CA-based studies of interaction in mostly applied CA concepts and methods and based
which at least one of the participants is communica- their findings in most cases just on audio or video
tively impaired in one way or another. In Goodwin’s recordings, a different type of applied studies
edited book Conversation and Brain Damage (2003), emerged in the late 1980s that, in addition to video
he collected work by the major contributors to this recordings, also used ethnographic fieldwork. I have
area, focusing on aphasia. The core message that is mentioned the early work of Suchman (1987) on
stressed in all of these contributions is that whatever users’ work on an advanced copier, which she car-
communicative success is achieved in the encoun- ried out as part of a research job in an industrial
ters under study has to be seen as a collaborative firm. She continued her studies there focusing on
achievement. It depends on the use of quite ordinary specialized work activities involving complex tech-
conversational methods, adapted to the particulari- nological support devices like computers and vari-
ties of the impairment in the case at hand, in which ous communication technologies. Later, other
both the aphasics and their interlocutors have to be researchers were able to start similar projects, so
creative. In this way, the book’s essays have a now one can speak of a tradition of “workplace
polemical subtheme in relation to conventional psy- studies.” Apart from Suchman, important contribu-
cholinguistic and neurological approaches, which tions have been made by Button, Heath, and Whalen
are oriented to individual failings rather than collec- (cf. collections edited by Button, 1993; Heath &
tive communicative successes. Luff, 2000; Luff, Hindmarsh, & Heath, 2000). Heath
A number of contributions deal explicitly with has led a particularly successful Work, Interaction
the contrast between ordinary, real-life interactions and Technology Research Group at King’s College
with an aphasic and formal tests. In the latter type of in London, which has studied a wide range of set-
situations, an aphasic may be asked, for instance, to tings, including control rooms for the London
tell a story on the basis of a set of cartoon drawings, Underground system, museums, and art galleries as
or to answer standardized questions, at the request well as journalists working in a news agency.
of a tester who has only a professional interest in the In these studies, the use of video is crucial
performance displayed in those tellings or answers. because the activities often take place in a group

113
Paul ten Have

setting involving team members and the extensive hidden work and the competencies involved in it vis-
use of various technological artifacts. Analyzing ible to outsiders, including the overseeing manage-
such activities requires, on the one hand, visual ment (cf. Whalen & Vinkhuyzen, 2000).
access to the local environment to study bodily To illustrate how CA can figure in this more
actions and nonvocal exchanges in situ, and, on the encompassing endeavor, let me refer shortly to some
other, a deeper, locally specialized understanding of Nevile’s (2004) findings when studying airline
of the activities under study than is generally used cockpits. In the first part of his book, he analyzed the
for agent–client interactions. Some participant use of personal pronouns: I, you, and we. Some of
observation—as well as studying relevant docu- these uses are officially prescribed in the relevant
ments and interviewing experts—is used to acquire manuals, and others are voluntary and impromptu,
the necessary background to really understand what chosen on the spot. They function, in any case, as
is going on. To mention one example, Nevile designators of local and momentary cockpit identi-
(2004), from Australia, collected the core data for ties, with associated tasks and responsibilities. In the
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

his studies of talk-in-interaction in the airline cock- second part of the book, Nevile, by discussing a range
pit by video-recording the activities of flight crews of concrete examples, examined how pilots coordi-
on scheduled flights by commercial airlines. But nate their talk and nontalk activities as they perform
before he even approached the airlines to ask for the routine tasks necessary to fly their plane. Such
their cooperation, he prepared his research by coordination is essential to maintain a shared under-
extensively reading whatever he could find about standing of where they are in the flight and what has
the operation of commercial airlines, training and to be done then and there, as pilots have to perform
operations manuals, official accident reports, and so their tasks with split-second precision and strictly in
on. He also watched available information videos sequence, one after the other. In doing this work, talk
showing pilots at work, visited conferences, and is just one of the resources that participants use and
talked to research psychologists working with flight orient to; others include gaze direction, gesture, and
crews and accident investigators. placement and movement of parts of the body, such
In such workplace studies, the concepts, findings, as head, arms, hands, legs, and eyebrows. In the third
and methods of CA are applied as part of a wider and final part of his book, Nevile widened his per-
undertaking that is broadly inspired by EM. I men- spective on talk in the cockpit to investigate how it is
tion a few general insights that can be gained from coordinated with talk to participants outside the cock-
these studies. First, such specialized (team)work pit, such as air traffic controllers and cabin crew
requires an experience-based practical knowledge members. Again, the issue is that the two pilots have
that goes far beyond what can be acquired through to attain and demonstrate a shared understanding of
formal instruction in a nonwork setting. Second, the local consequences of the outside talk, which they
effective teamwork not only requires the individual can do by internal talk or nonvocal but visible activi-
worker’s understanding of the task at hand but also ties. Investigation of such fine-tuned activities
an understanding at a glance of what others are requires finely detailed transcriptions of talk with
doing. A lot of actual coordination of activities descriptions of the associated activities.
occurs in an implicit fashion, often by seeing in To give an impression of how this is done, I
peripheral vision or by overhearing talk and adapting quote one of Nevile’s examples in Extract 7.2, which
one’s own activities accordingly. So, for instance, the in the original text is accompanied by a picture of
worker in a London Underground control room who the pointing finger that is mentioned.
is tasked with making announcements to the travel- The arrows indicate the moment in the talk at
ing public knows from observing and overhearing which the nonvocal activities start or stop. Work-
his colleagues at work on some crisis situation what place studies are a rather demanding kind of study,
to announce, to whom, and when without getting a but they are also extremely rewarding and necessary
request to do so (see Heath & Luff, 2000). Third, because they reflect the subtle but too often ignored
formal accounts of work activities cannot make this complexity of specialized work.

114
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

does not take off from the individual and his or her
Extract 7.2 mind. Instead, ethnomethodologists observe human
In the Cockpit action as inherently social and situated. Therefore
they largely abstain from precategorized and
C = Captain researcher-provoked ways of collecting data but
PNF = Pilot not flying rather use intense ethnography and audio or video
FO = First officer recordings. In so doing, they can reveal news about
PF = Pilot flying human life that was previously unavailable. Quite
1 (50.3)
2 C/PNF: one thousand to altitude.
often this can lead to what has been called a respeci-
↑————-↑ fication of previously developed concepts in the
2a C/PNF: ((moves right hand up from lap, then left human sciences, including sociology and psychol-
to right, at chest height, with index finger ogy, as members’ situated practices (Button, 1991;
extended))
Garfinkel, 2002). This kind of work as applied to
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

↑———↑
2b C/PNF: ((holds right hand still, just to psychological concepts has been taken up by discur-
the right of own chest, index sive psychology, which is discussed in Chapter 8 of
finger points to FO/PF’s side of this volume. That chapter also provides an exem-
main instrument panel))
↑ plary sketch of the different stages of a research
2c C/PNF: ((moves right hand project, including an analysis of some interactional
down and left, back to episodes that clearly demonstrate the impact of
right leg)) sequentiality in human interaction.
3 (3.2) = (0 > 1.4 > 2.4 > 3.2))
↑———↑
4 ((sound of altitude alert buzzer)) References
5 FO/PF >alert (.) for level (.) two fi::ve zero
Beach, W. A. (Ed.). (2001). Introduction: Diagnosing “lay
6 (27.5)
diagnosis.” Text: Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study
Note. From Beyond the Black Box: Talk-in-Interaction of Discourse, 21, 13–18.
in the Airline Cockpit (p. 131), by M. Nevile, 2004, Button, G. (Ed.). (1991). Ethnomethodology and the
Aldershot, England: Ashgate. Copyright 2004 by human sciences. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Maurice Nevile. Reprinted with permission. University Press.
Button, G. (Ed.). (1993). Technology in working order:
Studies of work, interaction and technology. London,
There are many other types of applied studies England: Routledge.
using EM, CA, or MCA. In all these it has been Carlin, A. P. (2010). Discussion note: Reading “A tuto-
proven that it is useful to explicate the ways in rial on membership categorization” by Emanuel
which (inter)action and communication depend on Schegloff. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 257–261.
doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2009.06.007
the local application of infrastructural practices. The
Clayman, S. E., & Maynard, D. W. (1995). Ethnomethod­
stress on restrictions that are operative in institu- ology and conversation analysis. In P. ten Have &
tional settings, which was stressed in early applied G. Psathas (Eds.), Situated order: Studies in the social
CA, can now be broadened in saying that task- organization of talk and embodied activities (pp. 1–30).
oriented interactions basically depend on general Washington, DC: University Press of America.
interactional capacities, which in the setting at hand Ford, C. E., Fox, B. A., & Thompson, S. A. (Eds.). (2002).
get a specialized application. The language of turn and sequence. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Francis, D., & Hester, S. (2004). An invitation to eth-
Conclusion nomethodology: Language, society and interaction.
London, England: Sage.
Within the limits of the space available, I have
Frankel, R. M. (1984). From sentence to sequence:
offered a summary characterization of EM and its Understanding the medical encounter through
offshoots CA and MCA. Contrary to most of the micro-interactional analysis. Discourse Processes, 7,
human sciences and Western culture at large, EM 135–170. doi:10.1080/01638538409544587

115
Paul ten Have

Frankel, R. M. (1990). Talking in interviews: A dispref- in assessment sequences. Social Psychology Quarterly,
erence for patient-initiated questions in physician– 68, 15–38. doi:10.1177/01902725050
patient encounters. In G. Psathas (Ed.), Interactional 6800103
competence (pp. 231–262). Washington, DC:
Heritage, J., Robinson, J. D., Elliott, M. N., Beckett, M.,
University Press of America.
& Wilkes, M. (2007). Reducing patients’ unmet
Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. concerns in primary care: The difference one word
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. can make. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22,
1429–1433. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0279-0
Garfinkel, H. (2002). Ethnomethodology’s program:
Working out Durkheim’s aphorism. Lanham, MD: Hester, S., & Eglin, P. (Eds.). (1997). Culture in action:
Rowman & Littlefield. Studies in membership categorization analysis.
Washington, DC: University Press of America.
Garfinkel, H., & Wieder, D. L. (1992). Two incommensu-
rable, asymmetrically alternate technologies of social Jayyusi, L. (1984). Categorization and the moral order.
analysis. In G. Watson & R. M. Seiler (Eds.), Text in Boston, MA: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
context: Studies in ethnomethodology (pp. 175–206).
Jefferson, G. (2002). Is “no” an acknowledgment token?
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Comparing American and British uses of (+)/(−)


Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organization: tokens. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 1345–1383.
Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York, doi:10.1016/S0378-2166(02)00067-X
NY: Academic Press.
Jefferson, G. (2004). Glossary of transcript symbols with
Goodwin, C. (Ed.). (2003). Conversation and brain dam- an introduction. In G. H. Lerner (Ed.), Conversation
age. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. analysis: Studies from the first generation (pp. 13–31).
Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.
Hayashi, M. (2005). Referential problems and turn con-
struction: An exploration of an intersection between Liberman, K. (2004). Dialectical practice in Tibetan
grammar and interaction. Text: Interdisciplinary philosophical culture: An ethnomethodological inquiry
Journal for the Study of Discourse, 25, 437–468. into formal reasoning. Lanham, MD: Rowman &
Littlefield.
Heap, J. L. (1990). Applied ethnomethodology: Looking
for the local rationality of reading activities. Human Luff, P., Hindmarsh, J., & Heath, C. (Eds.). (2000).
Studies, 13, 39–72. doi:10.1007/BF00143040 Workplace studies: Recovering work practice and inform-
ing systems design. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Heath, C., & Luff, P. (2000). Technology in action.
University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511628122
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489839 Lynch, M. (1985). Art and artifact in laboratory science: A
study of shop work and shop talk. London, England:
Heritage, J. (1984a). A change-of-state token and aspects
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
of its sequential placement. In J. M. Atkinson & J.
Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies Lynch, M. (1993). Scientific practice and ordinary action:
in conversation analysis (pp. 299–345). Cambridge, Ethnomethodology and social studies of science. New
England: Cambridge University Press. York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Heritage, J. (1984b). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Maynard, D. W., & Heritage, J. (2005). Conversation
Cambridge, England: Polity Press. analysis, doctor–patient interaction and medical
communication. Medical Education, 39, 428–435.
Heritage, J. (1999). CA at century’s end: Practices of
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2929.2005.02111.x
talk-in-interaction, their distributions and their out-
comes. Research on Language and Social Interaction, Nevile, M. (2004). Beyond the black box: Talk-in-interaction
32, 69–76. doi:10.1207/S15327973RLSI321&2_9 in the airline cockpit. Aldershot, England: Ashgate.
Heritage, J. (2005). Cognition in discourse. In H. Ochs, E., Schegloff, E. A., & Thompson, S. A. (Eds.).
te Molder & J. Potter (Eds.), Conversation and (1996). Interaction and grammar. Cambridge,
cognition (pp. 184–202). Cambridge, England: England: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ CBO9780511620874
CBO9780511489990.009
Peräkylä, A., Antaki, C., Vehviläinen, S., & Leudar, I.
Heritage, J., & Maynard, D. W. (Eds.). (2006). (Eds.). (2008). Conversation analysis and psycho-
Communication in medical care: Interaction between therapy. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
primary care physicians and patients. Cambridge, Press.
England: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
Raymond, G., & Heritage, J. (2006). The epistemics
CBO9780511607172
of social relationships: Owning grandchildren.
Heritage, J., & Raymond, G. (2005). The terms of agree- Language in Society, 35, 677–705. doi:10.1017/
ment: Indexing epistemic authority and subordination S0047404506060325

116
Ethnomethodology and Conversation Analysis

Sacks, H. (1972a). An initial investigation of the Stein, T., Frankel, R. M., & Krupat, E. (2005). Enhancing
usability of conversational data for doing sociology. clinician communication skills in a large healthcare
In D. Sudnow (Ed.), Studies in social interaction organization: A longitudinal case study. Patient
(pp. 31–74). New York, NY: Free Press. Education and Counseling, 58, 4–12. doi:10.1016/
Sacks, H. (1972b). On the analyzability of stories by chil- j.pec.2005.01.014
dren. In J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions Stivers, T. (2007). Prescribing under pressure: Parent–
in sociolinguistics: The ethnography of communication physician conversations and antibiotics. New York,
(pp. 325–345). New York, NY: Rinehart & Winston. NY: Oxford University Press.
Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on conversation (Vols. 1–2). Stivers, T., & Robinson, J. D. (2006). A preference for
Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell. progressivity in interaction. Language in Society, 35,
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A 367–392. doi:10.1017/S0047404506060179
simplest systematics for the organization of turn Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situated action: The prob-
taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735. lem of human–machine communication. Cambridge,
doi:10.2307/412243 England: Cambridge University Press.
Copyright American Psychological Association. Not for further distribution.

Schegloff, E. A. (1979). The relevance of repair to syntax- Suchman, L. A. (2007). Human–machine reconfigurations:
for-conversation. In T. Givon (Ed.), Syntax and Plans and situated actions (2nd ed.). Cambridge,
semantics 12: Discourse and syntax (pp. 261–286). England: Cambridge University Press.
New York, NY: Academic Press.
Schegloff, E. A. (1987). Analyzing single episodes of Sudnow, D. (1978). Ways of the hand: The organization of
interaction: An exercise in conversation analysis. improvised conduct. London, England: Routledge &
Social Psychology Quarterly, 50, 101–114. Kegan Paul.

Schegloff, E. A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last Tanaka, H. (1999). Turn-taking in Japanese conversation:
structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity A study in grammar and interaction. Amsterdam, the
in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97, Netherlands: Benjamins.
1295–1345. doi:10.1086/229903 ten Have, P. (2002, September). The notion of member is
Schegloff, E. A. (1993). Reflections on quantification the heart of the matter: On the role of membership
in the study of conversation. Research on Language knowledge in ethnomethodological inquiry. Forum:
and Social Interaction, 26, 99–128. doi:10.1207/ Qualitative Social Research, 3(3). Retrieved from
s15327973rlsi2601_5 http://www.qualitative-research.net/fqs/fqs-eng.htm
Schegloff, E. A. (2007a). Categories in action: Person- ten Have, P. (2007). Doing conversation analysis: A practi-
reference and membership categorization. Discourse cal guide (2nd ed.). London, England: Sage.
Studies, 9, 433–461. doi:10.1177/1461445607079162 Watson, R. (1997). Some general reflections on “categori-
Schegloff, E. A. (2007b). Sequence organization in inter- zation” and “sequence” in the analysis of conversation.
action: A primer in conversation analysis (Vol. 1). In S. Hester & P. Eglin (Eds.), Culture in action: Studies
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. in membership categorization analysis (pp. 49–76).
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511791208 Washington, DC: University Press of America.
Schegloff, E. A. (2007c). A tutorial on membership cat- Watson, R. (2009). Analysing practical and professional
egorization. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 462–482. texts: A naturalistic approach. Farnham, England:
doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2006.07.007 Ashgate.
Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The West, C. (1984). Routine complications: Trouble with talk
preference for self-correction in the organization between doctors and patients. Bloomington: Indiana
of repair in conversation. Language, 53, 361–382. University Press.
doi:10.2307/413107
Whalen, J., & Vinkhuyzen, E. (2000). Expert sys-
Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up clos- tems in (inter)action: Diagnosing document
ings. Semiotica, 8, 289–327. doi:10.1515/semi.1973. machine problems over the telephone. In P. Luff,
8.4.289 J. Hindmarsh, & C. Heath (Eds.), Workplace stud-
Sidnell, J. (Ed.). (2009). Conversation analysis: Comparative ies: Recovering work practice and informing sys-
perspectives. Cambridge, England: Cambridge tems design (pp. 92–140). Cambridge, England:
University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511635670 Cambridge University Press.
Sorjonen, M.-L. (2001). Responding in conversation: A Wieder, D. L. (1974). Language and social reality: The
study of response particles in Finnish. Amsterdam, the case of telling the convict code. The Hague, the
Netherlands: Benjamins. Netherlands: Mouton.

117

S-ar putea să vă placă și