Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

Behavioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29 – 36

www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway model of


behaviour and cognition
Edmund J.S. Sonuga-Barke *
Centre for Research into Psychological De6elopment, Department of Psychology, Uni6ersity of Southampton, Southampton SO17 1BJ, UK

Received 23 December 2000; accepted 13 August 2001

Abstract

Psychological accounts have characterised attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) as either a neuro-cognitive disorder
of regulation or a motivational style. Poor inhibitory control is thought to underpin AD/HD children’s dysregulation while delay
aversion is a dominant characteristic of their motivational style. A recent ‘head to head’ study of these two accounts suggest that
delay aversion and poor inhibitory control are independent co-existing characteristics of AD/HD (combined type). In the present
paper we build on these findings to propose a dual pathway model of AD/HD that recognises two quite distinct sub-types of the
disorder. In one AD/HD is the result of the dysregulation of action and thought resulting from poor inhibitory control associated
with the meso-cortical branch of the dopamine system projecting in the cortical control centres (e.g. pre-frontal cortex). In the
other AD/HD is a motivational style characterised by an altered delay of reward gradient linked to the meso-limbic dopamine
branch associated with the reward circuits (e.g. nucleus accumbens). The two pathways are further distinguished at the levels of
symptoms, cognitive and motivation profiles and genetic and non-genetic origins. © 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder; Heterogeneity; Inhibitory control; Executive function; Delay aversion; Dopamine; Meso-limbic;
Meso-cortical; Birth complications

1. Introduction counter findings relating to the involvement of particu-


lar psychological mechanisms in the disorder are regu-
Despite the great scientific interest it has aroused, larly published and even where some consistency of
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) re- findings is observed the size of statistical effects is often
mains among the least well characterised of mental only small to moderate. One possible contributory fac-
disorders. Historically, there have been difficulties in tor to this situation is the way that theoretical develop-
precisely specifying the symptom structure of a disorder ments tend to be driven by the search for one
that is heterogeneous in nature and has close associa- underlying mechanism that could provide the basis for
tions with other problems of childhood [44,53]. Many a ‘grand theory’ of AD/HD. This tendency has been
of these nosological and definitional ambiguities have encouraged by currently dominant cognitive neuro-psy-
been resolved in the most recent diagnostic formula- chological models of AD/HD. These accounts first reify
tions with the specification of separate diagnoses for AD/HD as an ontological and psychological reality [17]
sub-types of the disorder [26,14]. A refined phenotype, (rather than just a useful clinical construct) and then
which limits diagnosis to more severe and pervasive attempt to explain it by reference to endogenous psy-
patterns of symptoms, has also been proposed [55]. chological dysfunction caused by impairments in spe-
Despite these developments, there are still inconsisten- cific brain modules [19]. Even those theorists, who have
cies in findings regarding the underlying psychological rejected the modular accounts of dysfunction proposed
mechanisms responsible for AD/HD. Findings and by the neuro-psychological model, have tended to
search for grand theories. The heterogeneity of its
clinical expression and its multi-factorially determined
* Tel.: +44-23-80594604. aetiology makes achieving the sort of theoretical unity
E-mail address: ejb3@soton.ac.uk (E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke). required by such models of AD/HD unlikely [58].

0166-4328/02/$ - see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 1 6 6 - 4 3 2 8 ( 0 1 ) 0 0 4 3 2 - 6
30 E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36

In the current paper, we describe a recent study HD behaviours are functional expressions of an under-
which provides a strong impetus for a dual pathway lying motivational style rather than the result of
model of combined type AD/HD. Two pathways are dysfunctioning regulatory systems. According to this
proposed. In one, AD/HD is a disorder of dysregula- hypothesis AD/HD children are motivated to escape or
tion of thought and action associated with diminished avoid delay. Their inattentive, overactive and impulsive
inhibitory control. In the other, it is a motivational behaviours therefore represent functional expressions of
style (delay aversion) associated with fundamental al- what has been termed delay aversion. The model pre-
terations in reward mechanisms. In part one of the dicts that when faced with a choice between immediacy
paper we review research on inhibitory control in, and and delay AD/HD children will choose immediacy [50],
the effects of delay on, AD/HD children. In part two when no choice is available they will act on their
we describe a ‘head to head’ study contrasting disinhi- environment to reduce their perception of time during
bition and delay aversion as characteristics of AD/HD. delay by either creating or attending to non-temporal
The dual pathway model is developed in part three. The feature of the environment [1]. The resulting behaviour,
final part extends the discussion of the model by mak- because of its likely task incompatibility, is labelled as
ing predictions about the distinctive characteristics of inattentive and overactive. In this model cognitive
AD/HD arising from the two pathways. deficits associated with the provision, protection and
use of time, such as working memory and planning,
1.1. AD/HD — deficient inhibitory control or delay arise as secondary effects of delay aversion associated
a6ersion with patterns of reduced task engagement [52].
The majority of research on delay aversion in AD/
Neuro-psychological studies converge on the view HD has concentrated on AD/HD children’s unwilling-
that AD/HD is associated with problems of executive ness to wait for delayed rewards and events. There is
or higher order control functions [4]. AD/HD children now good evidence that AD/HD children prefer imme-
lack attentional and strategic flexibility, display poor diate over large delayed rewards under many circum-
planning and working memory and fail to effectively stances [23]. This has been interpreted as further
monitor their behaviour [12,11]. While such difficulties evidence for deficient inhibitory control by some. How-
are shared with a range of psychopathologies [8] the ever, a series of experiments have demonstrated that the
primary role of deficient inhibitory control in the emer- preference for immediacy is exercised only under cer-
gence of this pattern of dysregulation seems to distin- tain conditions. Furthermore, there appears to be a
guish AD/HD from other disorders [3,5,35]. The best double dissociation between preference for delayed re-
evidence in support of this assertion comes from the wards and inhibitory control [50,48,52]. These studies
now large number of studies using the stop signal suggest that in some situations AD/HD children can
paradigm (SSP [42]). This paradigm tests an individu- wait for rewards (to the same extent as controls) even
al’s ability to inhibit an already initiated pre-potent when this involves ongoing inhibition. In other situa-
response to a ‘go signal’ (typically visual) when sig- tions they fail to wait for rewards when no inhibition is
nalled to do so by a ‘stop signal’ (typically auditory) required. But in general they will not wait for rewards
presented at varying intervals prior to the expected time if this increases the total amount of delay experienced.
of the individuals ‘go’ response. Both the slope of the Taken together these results suggest that AD/HD chil-
probability of inhibition given different stop intervals dren’s decisions not to wait for rewards or events,
and the stop signal reaction time (SSRT) provide mea- which appear to be the result of problems of on-going
sures of the efficiency of inhibitory processes. A recent inhibition, are often the results of an aversion to delay.
meta-analysis [30] and a number of subsequent studies The delay aversion and the deficient inhibitory con-
[41,28] show that AD/HD children have a flatter proba- trol accounts both represent attempts to develop a
bility of inhibition slope and longer SSRTs. These unitary ‘grand’ theory of AD/HD. Not surprisingly,
effects are of moderate to large size and appear consis- and given their opposed philosophical origins, the find-
tent at least across clinical samples [22]. The association ings in support of each are difficult to account for with
of AD/HD with deficient inhibitory control seems more reference to the other. However, there are one or two
robust than that with other executive functions (e.g. possibilities worth exploring. First, it is possible that
working memory [31]). the slower SSRT displayed by the AD/HD children are
While models emphasising disinhibtion and dysregu- an artefact related to the temporal structure of the SSP
lation dominate the current literature, a number of rather than a true reflection of inhibitory problems.
alternative accounts have been proposed that emphasise Given the relative infrequency of stop signals compared
the motivational basis of AD/HD [15,63,18]. The delay with go signals the longer SSRTs may be the result of
aversion hypothesis represents the most radical depar- the different inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) durations be-
ture from the dominant neuro-psychological paradigm tween go and stop signals. Such ISI effects have been
[46]. This model is based on the assumption that AD/ found frequently with AD/HD children and have been
E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36 31

interpreted as either a problem with state regulation [7] lems or anxiety disorders were taken into account.
or due to the impact of delay on attention (i.e. an Third, using a stepwise discriminant function procedure
expression of delay aversion; [51]). Second, it is possible the two measures together proved highly diagnostic,
that deficient inhibitory control leads to poor perfor- correctly identifying nearly 90% of cases.
mance on tasks extended over time so that delay itself While confirming that delay aversion and poor in-
becomes associated with negative emotions and ac- hibitory control are core, but unrelated, characteristics
quires an aversive quality. On an empirical level both of of AD/HD these results are clearly difficult to reconcile
these explanations of the co-existence of delay aversion within either ‘grand theory’ of AD/HD. In fact the
and inhibitory control problems would predict a high independence of these measures combined with their
level of association between performance on the SSP high diagnostic value encourages the view that AD/HD
and delay choice tasks. is the product of two quite distinct processes (one
underpinned by deficient inhibitory control and the
1.2. Delay a6ersion and poor inhibitory control other mediated by delay aversion). The model of AD/
‘head-to-head’ HD outlined below builds on this idea by specifying
these processes in terms of two pathways from neuro-
In order to clarify the relationship between these two biological systems through psychological processes to
core characteristics of AD/HD a ‘head to head’ study behaviour and performance.
was recently performed in conjunction with the NIMH
multi-treatment study [45]. The ‘head to head’ ap- 1.3. A dual pathway model of the de6elopment of
proach employed in this study represented an extremely AD/HD
potent test of the competing theories because,
1. both theoretical camps ‘signed up’ to the study and The model (schematically represented in Fig. 1) de-
its outcome. scribes AD/HD as a developmental outcome of two
2. both camps selected what was in their view the best quite distinct psychological/developmental processes.
and most appropriate measure of ‘their’ theoretical At a more abstract level, the model represents a recon-
constructs (inhibitory control vs. delay aversion). ciliation of two philosophically distinct views of be-
3. the data was collected at multiple sites indepen- havioural disorder— one that seeks to identify the site
dently of either theoretical camp. of dysfunction in disorder while the other seeks to
Children with a diagnosis of AD/HD (combined explore the role of function. One route characterises
type) and control children performed: (i) the standard AD/HD as predominantly a motivational style medi-
stop signal task [56] and (ii) a choice delay task which ated by the emergence of delay aversion during child-
was essentially the same as the task used in the first hood. The second sees it as predominantly a disorder of
delay aversion study [50]. In this task children chose the regulation of thought and action resulting from
between small immediate (1 point after 3 s) and large inhibitory dysfunction. Each route has a number of
delayed reward (2 points after 30 s). Trials followed one different components expressed at different conceptual
after the other as soon as rewards were delivered and levels. First, there is the developmental outcome. In the
there was a fixed number of trials per session. This model this is separated into behavioural symptoms
meant that AD/HD children could reduce delay by (impulsiveness, inattention and overactivity) and task
choosing the small reward but that in doing so they engagement— the quality and quantity of task or prob-
would reduce the amount of rewards earned over a lem focused activity. Second, are the psychological
session. The key feature of this type of task, in relation processes that underpin these developmental outcomes.
to its current use, is that the response inhibition load in These can be subdivided into the primary or core
the task is trivial and no greater for a choice of the characteristic and secondary process characteristics. In
large delayed than the small immediate reward. This is this sense deficient inhibitory control is the primary
because, receiving the large reward does not depend on characteristic and cognitive and behavioural dysregula-
withholding a response over the 30 s of the pre-reward tion are its secondary manifestations. According to the
delay period. The child is active when making the model delay aversion and inhibitory deficits exist at
choice, but passive between the choice and the delivery different conceptual levels; delay aversion being a sec-
of the reward. ondary effect of a combination of fundamental alter-
Three aspects of the results were particularly signifi- ations in reward mechanisms and characteristics of the
cant for the current discussion. First, performance on child’s early environment.
the two tasks as represented on the key measures The dysregulation of thought and action pathway
(SSRT and preference for the large delayed reward) was (DTAP-solid line) is characterised by a core dysfunc-
not correlated. Second, there was an effect of AD/HD tion in inhibitory control. In the current model, this has
on performance of both tasks (with a large effect size) two mediated effects on the developmental outcome—
which was not altered when comorbid conduct prob- it causes both behavioural symptoms and poor quality
32 E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36

task engagement. The emergence of AD/HD symptoms the provision, protection and utilisation of time. In the
is mediated by behavioural dysregulation while the model DTAP AD/HD arises out of alterations within
effects on task engagement are mediated by cognitive the higher order control circuits of the brain and there-
dysregulation. Such cognitive dysregulation can be seen fore implicates the frontal and pre-frontal regions and
by the pattern of difficulties displayed by AD/HD their associated circuitry with projections from the
children on tasks requiring attentional flexibility, be- basal ganglia and into the striatum being particularly
havioural monitoring, planning and working memory. important [36].
In the model there is no direct pathway between execu- Functions sub-served by this circuit are regulated by
tive functions and AD/HD symptoms. This reflects the dopamine activity as part of a functionally and anatom-
more modest associations between measures of these ically distinct meso-cortical branch of the dopamine
functions and AD/HD symptoms reported in the litera- system [13,20].
ture [31]. The motivational style pathway (MSP-dashed line)
In addition to the primary (though mediated) effects provides an alternative route to AD/HD. As mentioned
on the quality of task engagement, the model makes above delay aversion is not the core characteristic of
novel predictions of secondary behavioural effects on the motivational pathway in this model. Rather, it is an
task engagement. Inattentive, impulsive and overactive acquired characteristic, which mediates the link be-
behaviours inevitably reduce the amount of effective tween behavioural symptoms, task engagement and a
processing time on tasks. According to the model, the more fundamental biologically based alteration in re-
effect of primary cognitive deficits on task performance ward mechanisms [18]. In identifying altered reward
is compounded by this secondary factor. Furthermore, mechanisms as underpinning this motivational style the
through a feedback loop from task engagement-quan- model builds on evidence from animal studies showing
tity to executive function we acknowledge the possibil- that AD/HD symptoms can result from a shortened
ity that this reduction in the quantity of task ‘delay of reward gradient’ [38–40]. This means that
engagement can over time limit the opportunity for the AD/HD children discount the value of future events at
development of those higher order skills associated with a higher rate than other children. This leads to a

Fig. 1. A schematic representation of the dual pathway model of AD/HD. The solid line represents the pathway for AD/HD as a disorder of the
regulation of thought and action. The dashed line represents AD/HD as a motivational style.
E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36 33

preference for immediacy—i.e. behavioural impulsive- are predicted to have distinct functional and struc-
ness. However, while clearly a key element of the tural characteristics. DTAP AD/HD arises from
disorder, the AD/HD combined type symptom pattern dysfunction while MSP AD/HD is a functional
is not reducible to impulsiveness. Furthermore, as we expression of a motivational style. Therefore, MSP
have demonstrated AD/HD impulsiveness (choice of AD/HD is more likely to vary as a function of
immediacy over delay) is conditional on the choice environmental context (especially its delay structure
context. In the present model, the link between AD/HD and stimulus quality). DTAP AD/HD should be
behaviours and altered reward mechanisms is mediated predominantly context independent. Furthermore,
by the emergence over time of a generalised aversion to given its dysfunctional origins, DTAP AD/HD is
delay. We employ a simple associative conditioning more likely to ‘behave’ like a disease category with
mechanism to account for this development. The short- DTAP AD/HD children appearing qualitatively dif-
ened delayed reward gradient leads the child to repeat- ferent from children within the normal range. The
edly fail to effectively respond to contextual demands behaviour of MSP AD/HD children, on the other
related to waiting and delay. Delay rich settings there- hand, may be best characterised as an extreme
fore come to acquire aversive properties through associ- expression of a normally distributed trait. There has
ation with the negative emotions associated with such been debate about the underlying structure of AD/
failure. Explicit in this mechanism is the moderating HD. Is it a category or a continuum [25,47,61]? This
role of cultural practices on the effect of these altered model suggests, it depend on the sub-type in
reward mechanisms. Such effects are also likely to differ question.
between families. Parents who set unrealistically high 2. Cognitive profile: it is expected that DTAP AD/HD
standards and are unforgiving of failures to wait are will be associated with significantly more severe and
more likely to create the context for the emergence of generalised cognitive impairment. This is because,
delay aversion and AD/HD in impulsive children. In the cognitive dysregulation associated with poor
keeping with previous formulations of the hypothesis, inhibitory control is compounded by the effects of
the expression of delay aversion is context dependent. behavioural symptoms on the child’s opportunity to
Inattention and overactivity occurring in no choice develop the ability to effectively provide, protect
delay settings and impulsiveness in choice settings. and utilise processing time. Impairment associated
While this pathway represents a predominantly motiva- with MSP AD/HD will be restricted to this latter
tional route, there are secondary acquired cognitive form. MSP AD/HD children should not show
characteristics. The impact of AD/HD behaviour on deficits on those tasks and in those functions that
the quantity and quality of task engagement will be do not tap inter-temporal competencies. Further-
similar to that proposed for the DTAP (disrupting the more, it is possible that given their intact control
provision, protection and utilisation of time) and there systems children with MSP have the potential to
are parallel lines for the two pathways connecting be- adapt to the constraints imposed on performance by
haviour to engagement and through to executive dys- their condition so developing alternative informa-
function. In this case, however, this does not represent tion processing strategies [48,49].
a compounding influence on existing cognitive dysregu- 3. Genetic and non-genetic aetiology: it is possible that
lation, but rather might be regarded as leading to a the two subtypes of AD/HD can be distinguished
cognitive profile that mimics elements of such dysregu- on the basis of activity within the meso-cortical
lation. In this model MSP AD/HD is associated with (DTAP) and meso-limbic (MSP) dopamine systems.
alterations in brain reward circuits especially the ven- Most recent attention has focused on genetic varia-
tral –striatal network (including the nucleus accumbens) tions identifying associations between AD/HD and
[33,34,37,43] associated with the meso-limbic branch of polymorphisms in the D4 receptor [24,29]. The sig-
the dopamine system [6,21]. nificance of this finding with regard to the dual
pathway model is unclear. The D4 receptor is
1.4. Distincti6e features of the two pathways widely distributed in the brain at relatively low
densities and may not be specifically related to
While the DTAP and the MSP are potential routes to either dopamine branch [27]. Of more interest for
the same AD/HD combined type diagnosis (i.e. inatten- the current model would be abnormalities in two
tive, overactive and impulsive behaviour), the model other specific dopamine receptors. The D1 receptor
allows a number of predictions about their distinctive seems to play a central role in regulating pre-frontal
properties to be made. Some of these are outlined activity [2,32], while alterations in the D2 receptor
below: are important for reward processes [6,21] and may
1. Structural and functional aspects of symptoms: be directly implicated in moderating the value of
while both DTAP and MSP AD/HD will meet delayed rewards [60]. The role of the dopamine
criteria for combined type diagnosis their symptoms transporter (DAT), which regulates levels of extra-
34 E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36

cellular dopamine also needs to be clarified with tive/motivational profile, their genetic and non-genetic
regard to these two AD/HD pathways [62]. Non-ge- origins and the role of environmental factors in moder-
netic factors are also likely to be important in the ating risk. The model predicts that DTAP AD/HD will
aetiology of the disorder and the extent of involve- be context independent, associated with relatively
ment of such factors may also allow us to distin- severe and generalised cognitive dysregulation, be cate-
guish between the two pathways. A recent study has gorical in nature and be less strongly associated with
suggested no association between inhibitory control genetic factors. MSP AD/HD will be context depen-
and D4 receptor polymorphisms in a group of dent, have a more limited pattern of cognitive impair-
AD/HD children [54]. It is possible that factors ment associated with the provision, protection and
such as birth related trauma, may make a significant utilisation of time, be a continuously distributed trait
non-shared genetic contribution to DTAP AD/HD and be more closely associated with genetic factors.
so masking the absolute contribution of genetic Future research should focus on exploring the coher-
factors. There is evidence that both executive func- ence of these two pathways, the extent to which they
tion performance and AD/HD symptoms are asso- act interactively or additively and their combined
ciated with this factor [10,16] and that these power to characterise AD/HD.
processes may be mediated by dopamine activity [9]
and alterations in the fronto– striatal system [59].
The central role of associative learning in the MSP References
opens up the possibility that AD/HD could be
associated with environmental risks of a purely [1] Antrop I, Roeyers H, Van Oost P, Buysse A. Stimulation
social nature rather than those mediated by neuro- seeking and hyperactivity in AD/HD children. J Child Psychol
biology. For instance, delay aversion might arise Psychiatry 2000;41:225 – 32.
under situations where parents or other significant [2] Arnsten AMT. Catecholamine regulation of the pre-frontal cor-
tex. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:151 – 62.
adults fail to follow through on promised rewards [3] Barkley RA. Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and
leading to a distrust of future events and an unwill- executive functions: constructing a unified theory of AD/HD.
ingness to wait. AD/HD, which has developed in Psychol Bull 1997;121:65 – 94.
this situation, should be amenable to psycho-social [4] Barkley RA, Grodzinsky G, DuPaul G. Frontal lobe functions
interventions. in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: a
review and research report. J Abnorm Child Psychol
4. Environmental moderation: Taylor [57] has pro- 1992;20:163 – 88.
vided a number of developmental models for the [5] Bayliss DM, Roodenrys S. Executive processing and attention
emergence of AD/HD through the interaction of deficit hyperactivity disorder: an application of the supervisory
genetic risk and environmental factors. Although, a attentional system. Dev Neuropsychol 2000;17:161 – 80.
discussion of these factors is beyond the scope of [6] Blum K, Wood RC, Braverman ER, Chen TJH, Sheridan PJ.
The D2 dopamine-receptor gene as a predictor of compulsive
the current paper, the current model also highlights disease —Bayes theorum. Funct Neurol 1995;10:37 – 44.
the role of social and cultural factors in moderating [7] Borger N, van der Meere J. Motor control and state regulation
the effects of biological factors. In the MSP path- in children with ADHD: a cardiac response study. Biol Psychol
way inflexible and demanding parenting, combined 2000;51:247 – 67.
with unrealistically high expectations for self-con- [8] Bradshaw JL, Sheppard DM. The neurodevelopmental frontos-
triatal disorders: evolutionary adaptiveness and anomalous later-
trol, is seen as a condition for the emergence of alization. Brain Language 2000;73:297 – 320.
delay aversion so providing the setting for the emer- [9] Brake WG, Sullivan RM, Gratton A. Perinatal distress leads to
gence of AD/HD. lateralized medial prefrontal cortical dopamine hypofunction in
Further, although no specific predictions are made adults rats. J Neurosci 2000;20:5538 – 43.
within the model about the protective significance of [10] Bylund B, Cervin T, Finnstrom O, Gaddlin PO, Leijon I, Mard
S, Samuelsson S, Sandstedt P, Warngard O. Very low-birth-
effective and positive parenting, it is likely that this weight children at 9 years: school performance and behavior in
could ameliorate the negative effect of genetic and relation to risk factors. Prenatal Neonatal Med 2000;5:124 – 33.
non-genetic factors at a number of levels within the [11] Cepeda NJ, Cepeda ML, Kramer AF. Task switching and
model. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol
2000;28:213 – 26.
[12] Clark C, Prior M, Kinsella GJ. Do executive function deficits
differentiate between adolescents with AD/HD and oppositional
2. Concluding remarks defiant/conduct disorder? A neuropsychological study using the
six elements test and hayling sentence completion test. J Abnorm
In the current paper, we have argued that combined Child Psychol 2000;28:403 – 14.
type AD/HD has two quite distinct sub-types that can [13] Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine mediated mechanisms in the
pre-frontal cortex. Semin Neurosci 1992;4:149 – 60.
be distinguished primarily in terms of their underlying [14] Gomez R, Harvey J, Quick C, Scharer I, Harris G. DSM-IV
psychological mechanisms but secondarily in terms of AD/HD: confirmatory factor models, prevalence and gender and
the context dependant nature of symptoms, their cogni- age differences based on parent and teacher ratings of Australian
E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36 35

primary school children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a
1999;40:265– 74. study with functional MRI. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:891 –6.
[15] Haenlein M, Caul W. Attention deficit disorder with hyperactiv- [37] Sagvolden T. The attention deficit disorder might be a reinforce-
ity: a specific hypothesis of reward dysfunction. J Am Acad ment deficit disorder. In: Georgas J, Manthouli M, Besevegis E,
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1987;26:356 –62. Kokkevi A, editors. Contemporary Psychology in Europe: The-
[16] Harvey JM, O’Callaghan MJ, Mohey H. Executive function of ory, Research and Application. Gottengin: Hogrefe and Huber,
children with extremely low birth weight: a case control study. 1991:131 – 43.
Dev Med Child Neurol 1999;41:292 –7. [38] Sagvolden T, Sergeant JA. attention deficit/hyperactivity disor-
[17] Jensen PS, Hoagwood K. The book of names: DSM-IV in der-From brain dysfunction to behaviour. Behav Brain Res
context. Dev Psychopathol 1997;9:231 –49. 1998;94:1 – 10.
[18] Johansen EB, Aase H, Meyer A, Sagvolden T. Attention deficit/ [39] Sagvolden T. Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hyper-
hyperactivity disorder behaviour explained by dysfunctional re- tensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyper-
inforcement and extinction processes, Behav Brain Res activity disorder (AD/HD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev
2002;130:37– 45. 2000;24:31 – 9.
[19] Karmiloff-Smith A. Development itself is the key to understand- [40] Sagvolden T, Aase H, Zeiner P, Berger D. Altered reinforcement
ing developmental disorders. Trends Cogn Sci 1998;2:389 – 98. mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav
[20] Knable MB, Weinberger DR. Dopamine, the prefrontal cortex Brain Res 1998;94:61 – 71.
and schizophrenia. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:123 –31. [41] Schachar R, Mota VL, Logan GD, Tannock R, Klim P. Confir-
[21] Koob GF. Dopamine, addiction and reward. Semin Neurosci mation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention deficit/hyper-
1992;4:139 – 48. activity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychology 2000;28:227 –35.
[22] Kuntsi J, Stevenson J. Hyperactivity in children: a focus on [42] Schachar R, Logan GD. Impulsivity and inhibitory control in
genetic research and psychological theories. Clin Child Family normal development and childhood psychopathology. Dev Psy-
Psychol Rev 2000;3:1 – 22. chology 1990;26:710 – 20.
[23] Kuntsi J, Oosterlaan J, Stevenson J. Psychological mechanisms [43] Shultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J
in hyperactivity I: response inhibition, deficit, working memory Neurophysiol 1998;80:1 – 27.
impairment, delay aversion or something else. J Child Psychol [44] Simonoff E. Extracting meaning from comorbidity: genetic
Psychiatry 2001;42:199 – 210. analyses that make sense. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
[24] LaHoste GJ, Swanson JM, Wigal SB, Glabe C, Wigal T, 2000;41:667 – 74.
Kennedy JL, Dopamine DR. receptor gene polymorphism is [45] Solanto MV, Abikoff H, Sonuga-Barke E, Schachar R, Logan
associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psy- GD, Wigal T, Hechtman L, Hinshaw S, Turkel E. The ecological
chiatry 1996;1:121 – 4. validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of
[25] Levy F, Hay DA, McStephen M, Wood C, Waldman I. Atten- impulsivity in AD/HD a supplement to the NIMH multi-modal
tion-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a category or a continuum? treatment study of AD/HD. J Abnorm Child Psychol
Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. J Am Acad Child 2001;29:215 – 28.
Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36:737 –44. [46] Sonuga-Barke EJS. Annotation: On dysfunction and function in
[26] McBurnett K, Pffifner LJ, Willcutt E, Tamm L, Lerner M, psychological theories of childhood disorder. J Child Psychol
Ottolini YL, Furman MB. Experimental cross validation of Psychiatry 1994;35:801 – 15.
DSM-IV types of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am [47] Sonuga-Barke EJS. Categorical model in child psychopathology;
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999;38:17 –24. a conceptual and empirical analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
[27] Muglia P, Jain U, Macciardi F, Kennedy JL. Adult attention 1998;39:115 – 33.
deficit hyperactivity disorder and the dopamine D4 receptor [48] Sonuga-Barke EJS, Houlberg K, Hall M. When is impulsiveness
gene. Am J Med Genet 2000;96:273 –7. not impulsive?; the case of hyperactive children’s cognitive style.
[28] Nigg J. The AD/HD response-inhibition deficit as measured by J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994;35:1247 – 53.
the stop task: replication with DSM-IV combined type-extension [49] Sonuga-Barke EJS. Internal length and time use in AD/HD; A
and qualification. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1999;27:393 – 402. comparison of 4 models. J Abnormal Child Psychol, in press.
[29] Oak JN, Oldenhof J, Van Tol HHM. The dopamine D-4 recep- [50] Sonuga-Barke EJS, Taylor E, Sembi S, Smith J. Hyperactivity
tor: one decade of research. Eur J Pharmacol 2000;405:303 – 27. and delay aversion I: the effect of delay on choice. J Child
[30] Oosterlaan J, Logan GD, Sergeant JA. Response inhibition in Psychol Psychiatry 1992;33:387 – 98.
AD/HD, CD, co-morbid AD/HD+CD, anxious, and control [51] Sonuga-Barke EJS, Taylor E. The effect of delay on hyperactive
children; a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. J Child and non-hyperactive children’s response times. J Child Psychol
Psychol Psychiatry 1998;39:411 –25. Psychiatry 1992;33:1091 – 6.
[31] Pennington BF, Ozonoff S. Executive functions and develop- [52] Sonuga-Barke EJS, Williams E, Hall M, Saxton T. Hyperactivity
mental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry & delay aversion III; the effect on cognitive style of imposing
1996;37:51– 87. delay after errors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37:189 –94.
[32] Robbins TW. Chemical neuromodulation of frontal-executive [53] Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T. Attention deficit disorder and
functions in humans and other animals. Exp Brain Res comorbidity. Pediatr Clin North Am 1999;46:915 – 27.
2000;133:130 – 8. [54] Swanson J, Oosterlaan J, Murias M, Schuck S, Flodman P,
[33] Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Functions of dopamine in the dorsal Spence MA, Wasdell M, Ding Y, Chi H, Smith M, Mann M,
and ventral striatum. Semin Neurosci 1992;4:119 –28. Carlson C, Kennedy JL, Sergeant JA, Leung P, Zhang Y, Sadeh
[34] Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Neurobehavioural mechanisms of re- A, Chen C, Whalen CK, Babb KA, Moyzis R, Posner MI.
ward and motivation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1996;6:228 –36. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder children with a 7-repeat
[35] Ross RG, Harris JG, Olincy A, Radant A. Eye movement task allele of the dopamine D4 receptor gene have extreme behaviour
measures inhibition and spatial working memory in adults with but normal performance on critical neuro-psychological tests of
schizophrenia, AD/HD and a normal comparison group. Psychi- attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2000;97:4754 – 9.
atry Res 2000;95:35 – 42. [55] Swanson JM, Sergeant JA, Taylor E, Sonuga-Barke EJS, Jensen
[36] Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SCR, PS, Cantwell DP. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and
Simmons A, Bullmore ET. Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperkinetic disorder. Lancet 1998;351:429 – 33.
36 E.J.S. Sonuga-Barke / Beha6ioural Brain Research 130 (2002) 29–36

[56] Tannock R, Schachar R, Logan G. Methylphenidate and cogni- [61] Waldman ID, Robinson BF, Rowe DCA. logistic regression
tive flexibility-dissociated dose effects in hyperactive-children. J based extension of the TDT for continuous and categorical
Abnorm Child Psychol 1995;23:235 –66. traits. Ann Hum Genet 1999;63:329 – 40.
[57] Taylor E. Developmental neuropsychology of attention deficit [62] Waldman ID, Rowe DC, Abramowitz A, Kozel ST, Mohr JH,
and impulsiveness. Dev Psychopathol 1999;11:607 – 28. Sherman SL, Cleveland HH, Sanders ML, Card JHC, Stever C.
[58] Todd RD. Genetics of attention deficit disorder: are we ready for Association and linkage of the dopamine transporter gene and
molecular genetic studies. Am J Med Genet 2000;96:241 – 3. attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children: heterogeneity
[59] Toft PB. Prenatal and perinatal striatal injury: a hypothetical owing to diagnostic subtype and severity. Am J Hum Genet
cause of attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Neurol
1998;63:1767 – 76.
1999;21:602 – 10.
[63] Zentall SS, Zentall TR. Optimal stimulation: a model of disor-
[60] Wade TR, de Wit H, Richards JB. Effects of dopaminergic drugs
dered activity and performance in normal and deviant children.
on delayed reward as a measure of impulsive behaviour in rats.
Psychol Bull 1983;94:446 – 71.
Psychopharmacology 2000;150:90 – 101.

S-ar putea să vă placă și