Sunteți pe pagina 1din 40

Social Neuroscience

ISSN: 1747-0919 (Print) 1747-0927 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/psns20

Social neuroscience: undoing the schism between


neurology and psychiatry

Agustín Ibáñez, Adolfo M. García, Sol Esteves, Adrián Yoris, Edinson Muñoz,
Lucila Reynaldo, Marcos Luis Pietto, Federico Adolfi & Facundo Manes

To cite this article: Agustín Ibáñez, Adolfo M. García, Sol Esteves, Adrián Yoris, Edinson
Muñoz, Lucila Reynaldo, Marcos Luis Pietto, Federico Adolfi & Facundo Manes (2016): Social
neuroscience: undoing the schism between neurology and psychiatry, Social Neuroscience,
DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1245214

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1245214

Accepted author version posted online: 06


Oct 2016.
Published online: 27 Oct 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 54

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=psns20

Download by: [Athabasca University] Date: 17 November 2016, At: 12:11


SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1245214

Social neuroscience: undoing the schism between neurology and psychiatry


Agustín Ibáñez a,b,c,d,e*, Adolfo M. Garcíaa,b,f*, Sol Estevesa, Adrián Yorisa,b, Edinson Muñozg, Lucila Reynaldoa,
Marcos Luis Piettoh, Federico Adolfia and Facundo Manesa,b,e,i
a
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience (LPEN), Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCyT), INECO
Foundation, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina; bNational Scientific and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires,
Argentina; cCenter for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago de Chile, Chile;
d
Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia; eCentre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council
(ACR), Sydney, Australia; fFaculty of Elementary and Special Education (FEEyE), National University of Cuyo (UNCuyo), Mendoza, Argentina;
g
Departamento de Lingüística y Literatura, Facultad de Humanidades, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Santiago, Chile; hUnit of Applied
Neurobiology (UNA, CEMIC), Buenos Aires, Argentina; iDepartment of Experimental Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Multiple disorders once jointly conceived as “nervous diseases” became segregated by the Received 12 May 2016
distinct institutional traditions forged in neurology and psychiatry. As a result, each field specia- Revised 29 July 2016
lized in the study and treatment of a subset of such conditions. Here we propose new avenues for Published online 27 October
interdisciplinary interaction through a triangulation of both fields with social neuroscience. To 2016
this end, we review evidence from five relevant domains (facial emotion recognition, empathy, KEYWORDS
theory of mind, moral cognition, and social context assessment), highlighting their common Social neuroscience;
disturbances across neurological and psychiatric conditions and discussing their multiple patho- neurology; psychiatry;
physiological mechanisms. Our proposal is anchored in multidimensional evidence, including neuropsychiatry
behavioral, neurocognitive, and genetic findings. From a clinical perspective, this work paves the
way for dimensional and transdiagnostic approaches, new pharmacological treatments, and
educational innovations rooted in a combined neuropsychiatric training. Research-wise, it fosters
new models of the social brain and a novel platform to explore the interplay of cognitive and
social functions. Finally, we identify new challenges for this synergistic framework.

On the origins of neurology and psychiatry transdiagnostic approach. In this paper, we delineate a
triangulation among neurology, psychiatry, and social
Currently, neurology and psychiatry represent two dis-
neuroscience. Specifically, we review massive evidence
tinct disciplines with different clinical specializations,
of shared social cognition impairments across neurolo-
research methodologies, and institutional frameworks.
gic and psychiatric diseases, and sketch a prospective
Mr. Gambale loses his memory after a head blow, and
framework to foster more synergistic research and edu-
he is swiftly taken to the neurologist. Instead, if Mr.
cation beyond disciplinary specializations.
Holdsworth believes he is dead, he is referred to a
To understand why this triangulation has not yet
psychiatrist. However, had these patients lived
crystallized, we must trace the historical roots of neu-
200 years ago, both their conditions would have been
rology and psychiatry. In the early and mid-nineteenth
treated as nervous diseases (Price, Adams, & Coyle, 2000;
century, Gall and Spurzheim, followed by Broca and
Reynolds, 1990). Indeed, multiple cognitive and beha-
Wernicke, pioneered the scientific study of complex
vioral domains become similarly compromised in neu-
pathological behaviors in connection to the brain
rological and psychiatric disorders. Social cognition
(Price et al., 2000). Both before and after such contribu-
impairments are prime examples of these commonal-
tions, leading scholars (e.g., Charcot, Freud, Kraepelin,
ities (Ibanez, Kuljis, Matallana, & Manes, 2014).
Bleuler, Alzheimer, Parkinson) favored a joint approach
Nevertheless, while both neurology and psychiatry
to neurological and psychiatric disorders (Martin, 2002).
have established fruitful dialogue with neuroscientific
Admittedly, methodological differences already existed
research, no systematic triangulation with social neu-
between neurologists and psychiatrists. While the for-
roscience has yet been formulated to address social
mer correlated brain syndromes with neuropathological
cognition disturbances from a dimensional,

CONTACT Agustín Ibáñez aibanez@ineco.org.ar Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCyT), Pacheco de Melo 1860, Buenos Aires,
Argentina
*These authors are the First authors.
© 2016 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
2 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

changes via postmortem and microscopic techniques, and practice within social neuroscience. For instance,
the latter relied on analytical descriptions of behavior there are virtually no empirical reports of social cogni-
and symptoms. Yet, their analyses pursued the same tion domains which directly compare neurological and
goal: characterizing the structure and function of the psychiatric conditions. Even in some syndromes sec-
nervous system (Cowan, Harter, & Kandel, 2000). ondary to both types of diseases (e.g., Capgrass syn-
Throughout the twentieth century, however, neurol- drome), direct contrasts of social cognition skills (e.g.,
ogists and psychiatrists began to forge different facial expression recognition) remain unattempted,
approaches to disorders of the nervous system, with which precludes comparisons between different patho-
separate philosophical rationales, research principles, physiological mechanisms for the same socio-cognitive
and treatment methods (Martin, 2002). In the post-war domain. Similarly, explanatory models of social cogni-
Anglo-Saxon and American contexts, the field was tion impairments rooted in neurology (e.g., (Dickerson,
marked by a theoretical and practical divide: neurolo- 2015) or psychiatry (e.g., (Fett, Shergill, & Krabbendam,
gists studied organic brain dysfunctions, searching for 2015))) have largely overlooked evidence from the
anatomic causes of “neurological diseases”; instead, other discipline. In addition, virtually no academic pro-
psychiatrists, influenced by Freudian theories, were grams and funding sources aim to apply the knowledge
interested in functional alterations of the mind and of social neuroscience to neurological and psychiatric
their “psychodynamic causes”. This was particularly disorders featuring similar impairments. Finally, given
reinforced since the 1930s. Relying on pioneering that neuroscience has traditionally ignored contribu-
brain measurement techniques (such as electroence- tions from social neuroscience (Nature Neuroscience
phalography) which offered objective, consistent, and Editorial, 2012), the latter’s interaction with neuropsy-
reproducible findings, neurologists agreed on impor- chiatry has been delayed.
tant definitions and criteria (Price et al., 2000). A neuropsychiatric approach, moving toward a sys-
However, several disorders lacked visible pathological tematic triangulation of social neuroscience with neu-
markers. These became the focus of psychiatrists, who rology and psychiatry, could provide a synergistic arena
favored symptom descriptions over laboratory tests. for clinical and research developments. Here we focus
The opposition between both communities was on the empirical, methodological, and conceptual con-
reflected in their training requirements and geographi- tributions that could be derived directly from social
cal distributions. The prestigious Archives of Neurology neuroscience in the pursuit of such an aim. An emer-
and Psychiatry, for instance, was quickly dissolved into ging agenda is reframing psychiatric diseases as disor-
two journals with separate agendas: JAMA Neurology ders of social cognition (Schilbach et al., 2013).
and JAMA Psychiatry. Extending these claims, below we sketch a social neu-
Such initial separation has had unfortunate conse- roscience approach to various dimensions of neurolo-
quences. In particular, the consolidation of different gical and psychiatric disorders (Baez, García, & Ibanez,
theories, research projects, training methods, and clin- 2016; Ibanez & Manes, 2012).
ical assessment models has hindered the construal of The paper is organized as follows. First, we character-
synergistic neuropsychiatric knowledge (Northoff, 2008; ize the tenets of social neuroscience. Second, we
Price et al., 2000). The complex field of neuropsychiatry describe behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia
partly bridged such a gap, framing structural and func- (bvFTD) (Piguet, Hornberger, Mioshi, & Hodges, 2011)
tional aspects as complementary across neurological as a prototypical model to map the clinical and biologi-
and psychiatric diseases (Northoff, 2008). Similarly, neu- cal overlaps between several neurological and psychia-
roscientists largely agree on the conceptual futility of tric disorders. Third, we selectively review key domains of
strict separations between both disciplines (Martin, social neuroscience, highlighting their common distur-
2002). However, integration efforts so far have been bances in bvFTD and other conditions. We focus on facial
only partially successful, at best. We propose that this emotion recognition (FER), empathy, theory of mind
endeavor could witness major breakthroughs if specific (ToM), moral cognition, and ecological assessment of
knowledge from both fields is triangulated with multi- social context. Fourth, we discuss the prospects of this
level insights from social neuroscience. triangulation, showing how multilevel network-based
Despite general positive beliefs about the role of approaches within social neuroscience can open new
cognitive neuroscience as a bridge between neurology opportunities for development. Then, we sketch avenues
and psychiatry (Northoff, 2008; Price et al., 2000), such a of interaction between social neuroscience and neurop-
threefold interaction has rarely taken place in the social sychiatry, including theoretical, diagnostic, and pharma-
neuroscience arena. To a large extent, this is due to the cological considerations. Finally, we identify novel
scarcity of dimensional and transnosological research challenges for this synergistic framework.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 3

Social neuroscience: a tool kit for triangulation presentations of bipolar disorder (BD), major depressive
disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
Experimental approaches within cognitive neuroscience
(ADHD), schizophrenia (SZ), and borderline personality
—ranging from animal models to behavioral and ima-
disorder (BPD). Thus, it is often difficult to ascertain
ging studies with humans—shed light on cognitive
bvFTD as the underlying clinical entity (Barutta,
(dys)functions in neurological and psychiatric disorders.
Hodges, Ibanez, Gleichgerrcht, & Manes, 2011; Manes,
Despite the shortcomings mentioned above, this joint
2012). Indeed, in approximately half of bvFTD patients,
approach, in itself, reduces the discontinuity between
behavioral and attitudinal changes are ignored or
constructs in each discipline (Cowan et al., 2000). More
attributed to a primary psychiatric disorder (Woolley,
particularly, the subfield of social neuroscience studies
Khan, Murthy, Miller, & Rankin, 2011). Specifically, social
the biological underpinnings of social processes, as
cognition deficits, typified by contextual inappropriate-
shaped by contextual factors. Psychosocial mechanisms
ness (Baez et al., 2016; Ibanez & Manes, 2012), may be
are conceived as multidimensional phenomena, includ-
overlooked before they become severe, especially
ing social behavior (observable interactions between
because such variable is rarely quantified in structured
people), social cognition (psychological processes
clinical (Gleichgerrcht, Ibanez, Roca, Torralva, & Manes,
underling social behavior), socially driven neural activity
2010) and neuropsychological (Torralva, Roca,
(brain structures and networks subserving social pro-
Gleichgerrcht, Bekinschtein, & Manes, 2009) examina-
cesses), and social functioning (the ability to interact
tion. In sum, as further shown below, bvFTD is a multi-
with others in time and space) (Kennedy & Adolphs,
dimensional condition whose comprehensive study
2012). More generally, social neuroscience addresses
requires integrating socially relevant insights from neu-
the normal and pathological interplay among environ-
rology and psychiatry (Ibanez et al., 2014).
ment, performance, cognition, neural substrates, and
genetics (Ibanez, Kotz, Barrett, Moll, & Ruz, 2014).
The social cognition network is so widespread
(Kennedy & Adolphs, 2012) that it proves vulnerable A dimensional and transnosological
to varied neurological and psychiatric diseases. Given comparison of social cognition domains
the omnidirectional relationship among the above
A key step toward the triangulation we advocate con-
dimensions, a disturbance in any of them could com-
sists in reviewing extant evidence from social neu-
promise relevant neural circuits even in the absence of
roscience with a dimensional and transnosological
primary (neurocognitive) social deficits. Yet, that very
perspective (Association, 2000; Kendler, 1990). A dimen-
feature suggests multiple alternatives to foster recov-
sional approach frames psychopathological processes
ery, capitalizing on spared components of the network.
as continua between normal and pathological
Through its sensitive experimental methods, social neu-
extremes. Similarly, social cognition deficits in a specific
roscience sheds light on the anatomy, function, and
domain (e.g., social decision making) can be present
plasticity of such complex circuitry, regardless of the
not only as a dimensional disturbance across patients,
underlying diagnosis.
but also across subdomains (e.g., regarding fairness,
bargaining, risk, or other relevant variables). Besides, a
transnosological conception acknowledges that specific
Behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: A key
mental and social faculties can be similarly compro-
model for disciplinary triangulation
mised across disease types (Hyman, 2007; Insel &
The shortcomings of the neurology/psychiatry divide Quirion, 2005; Jablensky, 2005; McGorry & Van Os,
are evident in bvFTD (Ibanez et al., 2014). This neuro- 2013). Although the transnosological approach has
degenerative condition is characterized by frontal, insu- been mostly restricted to the psychiatric field, it can
lar, and temporal atrophy, although such damage may be extended across psychiatric and neurological disor-
not be evident on structural brain imaging early on ders. These dimensional and transnosological view-
(Rahman, Sahakian, Hodges, Rogers, & Robbins, 1999; points concern not only clinical, but also anatomical,
Rascovsky et al., 2011). Initial manifestations involve neurophysiological, molecular, and genetic dimensions.
pervasive changes in personality and social behavior The study of social cognition impairments profitably
(e.g., disinhibition, compulsivity, depression, impulsivity, lends itself to both approaches, since such deficits can
inappropriate conduct), even before cognitive altera- vary in frequency and intensity across a vast array of
tions are observed (Piguet et al., 2011; Santamaría- neurological and psychiatric diseases. Below we deploy
García et al., 2016). Clinical criteria for early bvFTD this framework through a review of five distinguishable
(Manes et al., 2010) are sometimes met by atypical though interrelated domains.
4 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Facial emotion recognition Huntington’s disease (HD)(Feuerriegel, Churches,


Hofmann, & Keage, 2014; Kumfor & Piguet, 2013;
FER is crucial for social interactions, as it allows people
Novak et al., 2012), multiple sclerosis (MS, Henry et al.,
to infer what others are feeling and act accordingly
2011; Mike et al., 2013), neuromyelitis optica (Cardona
(Adolphs, 2002; Atkinson & Adolphs, 2011; Rutishauser,
et al., 2014), amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS)(Crespi
Mamelak, & Adolphs, 2015). It involves multiple sub-
et al., 2014), mild cognitive impairment (Varjassyova
processes and engages networks distributed along the
et al., 2013), epilepsy (Gomez-Ibanez, Urrestarazu, &
fusiform gyrus, the superior temporal sulcus, the amyg-
Viteri, 2014; Jiang et al., 2014), Cockayne syndrome
dala, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and the basal gang-
(Baez et al., 2013), stroke (Falquez et al., 2014; Yuvaraj,
lia (Adolphs, 2002; Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio, 2003;
Murugappan, Norlinah, Sundaraj, & Khairiyah, 2013),
Allison, Puce, Spencer, & McCarthy, 1999). Facial, emo-
Rett syndrome (Djukic, Rose, Jankowski, & Feldman,
tional, and contextual integration of information occurs
2014), posterior cortical atrophy (Gonzalez-Gadea,
early in the brain (Ibáñez et al., 2010, Ibáñez, Hurtado,
Ibanez, et al. 2014), and genetic disorders such as
et al. 2011). FER deficits hamper social communication
Prader–Willi syndrome (Key, Jones, & Dykens, 2013)
and induce changes in personality and behavior. They
and Klinefelter syndrome (Van Rijn, Swaab, Aleman, &
are more severe in bvFTD (Kumfor et al., 2011; Kumfor &
Kahn, 2006) (see Table 1).
Piguet, 2013; Lavenu & Pasquier, 2005; Werner et al.,
FER impairments are also present in most psychiatric
2007) (and other subtypes of FTD) than in other demen-
conditions, including BD (Ibanez et al., 2012), BPD (Baez,
tias, even controlling for disease severity (Bertoux, Volle,
Marengo, et al. 2014), major depressive disorder
et al. 2014; Fernandez-Duque & Black, 2005; Kumfor &
(Schepman, Taylor, Collishaw, & Fombonne, 2012),
Piguet, 2013). Crucially, in this condition, they are not
obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) (Kang,
explained by other cognitive deficits (Kumfor et al.,
Namkoong, Yoo, Jhung, & Kim, 2012), body dysmorphic
2011; L. A. Miller et al., 2012) and seem to impact the
disorder (Buhlmann, McNally, Etcoff, Tuschen-Caffier, &
implicit awareness of its dysfunction (Ibanez, Velasquez,
Wilhelm, 2004), panic disorder (Wang et al., 2013), ado-
Caro, & Manes, 2013).
lescents under social deprivation (Escobar et al., 2013),
FER impairments in bvFTD are associated with atro-
ex-combatants (Tobón et al., 2015), and anorexia
phy in the amygdala, the insula, the OFC, and the
(Fonville, Giampietro, Surguladze, Williams, &
medial prefrontal cortices (Bertoux et al., 2012; Kipps,
Tchanturia, 2014) (see Table 1). In particular, they are
Nestor, Acosta-Cabronero, Arnold, & Hodges, 2009;
pervasive in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Aoki,
Omar, Rohrer, Hailstone, & Warren, 2011; Rosen et al.,
Cortese, & Tansella, 2014; Dawson, Webb, &
2002). More particularly, impaired recognition of nega-
McPartland, 2005; Feuerriegel et al., 2014; Sachse
tive emotions, such as disgust and fear, has been asso-
et al., 2014). Electrophysiological evidence shows that
ciated with damage to the left insula and the right
early (decoding) and late (integration) face-processing
amygdala, respectively (Kumfor, Irish, Hodges, Piguet,
disruptions in autistic individuals may occur since age
& Kilner, 2013). Thus, the regions supporting FER over-
three and persist into adulthood (Dawson et al., 2005).
lap with those compromised in bvFTD.
Similar deficits have been reported in SZ (Baez, Herrera,
This skill is disturbed in other neurodegenerative
Villarin, et al. 2013; Fujiwara, Yassin, & Murai, 2014;
conditions. In Alzheimer’s disease (AD), for example,
Kohler et al., 2008; Linden et al., 2010; Ventura, Wood,
FER decreases with the progression of dementia
Jimenez, & Hellemann, 2013), with reduced modulation
(Kumfor & Piguet, 2013; Lavenu & Pasquier, 2005), par-
of face-specific ERPs being evident even in first-degree
ticularly in the domain of negative emotions (Phillips,
relatives (Feuerriegel et al., 2014; Ibanez et al., 2012;
Scott, Henry, Mowat, & Bell, 2010; Rosen et al., 2006).
McCleery et al., 2014; Turetsky et al., 2007). Responses
Such an impairment is only partially influenced by other
to facial emotional stimuli in SZ are also associated with
cognitive deficits (Klein-Koerkamp, Beaudoin, Baciu, &
decreased activation of the fusiform gyrus and the
Hot, 2012). In fact, varied emotional domains are com-
amygdala (Habel et al., 2010; Paradiso et al., 2003).
promised in AD, suggesting a global emotional distur-
The N170 to emotional faces is also impaired in ADHD
bance (Braak & Braak, 1991; Bruen, McGeown, Shanks, &
(Ibáñez, Petroni, et al. 2011). Notably, FER deficits are
Venneri, 2008; Horinek, Varjassyova, & Hort, 2007). FER
marked in Capgras syndrome (the delusion that a friend
deficits are also common in other neurological disor-
or relative has been replaced by an identical-looking
ders, with varying degrees of intensity. These include
impostor), which may be present in neurological and
corticobasal syndrome (Kumfor et al., 2014), prosopag-
psychiatric diseases, such as Lewy body dementia
nosia (De Renzi, 1986; Liu & Behrmann, 2014), progres-
(Fiacconi et al., 2014) and paranoid SZ (Ellis et al.,
sive supranuclear palsy (Kumfor & Piguet, 2013),
1993; Ellis, Young, Quayle, & De Pauw, 1997),
Table 1. FER in neurological and psychiatric conditions.
Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarker
Bertoux, De AD (CSF profile) (N = 33); bv-FTD N FER test AD performed worse than HC. Bv-FTD impaired compared Not measured.
Souza, et al. (no CSF) (N = 60) and HC to AD and HC for all emotions except happiness and
(2014) (N = 30) neutral. FER in AD was influenced by disease duration
and overall cognitive impairment.
Piguet, Leyton, nfv-PPA (N = 20); lv-PPA (N = 18); N ER Task: (Ekman 60 Faces nfv-PPA group was impaired compared with the other two Predominant left lateralized brain atrophy in PPA groups.
Gleeson, Hoon, HC (N = 21). Test) groups Greater cortical thinning in the left inferior parietal,
& Hodges, posterior cingulate, and inferior temporal regions in lv-
(2015) PPA, and in the insula and right calcarine in nfv-PPA.
All lv-PPA (but not nfv-PPA) had amyloid B deposition
associated w. Alzheimer (Pib-PET imaging).
Kumfor et al. CBS (N = 16); AD (N = 18); HC N FER: Face Perception Task; CBS performed worse than HC on FER. AD: intact High-level ER deficits in CBS associated with atrophy of
(2014) (N = 22). Face Matching; Emotion performance. Ekman 60 and TASIT: CBS and AD the paracentral gyrus/precuneus region and basal
Matching Task; Emotion performed worse than HC. ganglia. In AD ER deficits associated with cortical
selection task; TASIT. thickness in the anterior cingulate and insula, together
MRI scan with the hippocampus, amygdala, and nucleus
accumbens.
Cardona et al. NMO (N = 10) Emotional Morphing Task Reduced accuracy in patients for emotions of disgust, Not measured.
(2014) HC (N = 10) anger
and fear.
Crespi et al. ALS (N = 22) N ER Task (Ekman 60 Faces Global impairment of ER in ALS compared to HC. Significant microstructural white-matter changes
(2014) HC (N = 55) Test) (Diffusion Tensor Imaging) within the motor pathway
in ALS compared to HC.
Liu and CP (N = 8) N Forty front view Caucasian CP: discrimination performance (Holistic Processing) Not measured.
Behrmann HC 1 (N = 32) and HC 2 (N = 8) male faces w. neutral impaired compared to HC.
(2014) expressions
Djukic et al. Rett Syndrome female patients N ER Task of three basic Rett syndrome patients were impaired in ER compared to Not measured.
(2014) (N = 37) emotions (happy, sad HC.
HC (N = 34) and fear)
Gomez-Ibañez Idiopatic generalized epilepsy N FER Task FER impaired in both MTLE and IGE patients. Not measured.
et al. (2014). (IGE) (N = 20) Facial Identity Recognition FIR impaired in MTLE patients.
Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy Task (FIR)
(MTLE) (N = 19)
HC (N = 23)
Fiacconi et al. DLB w Capgras patient (N = 1) N Famous Face Recognition DLB with Capgras showed impairment in FER. Not measured.
(2014)
DLB w/o Capgras patient (N = 1) Fear Expression Rating These deficits were not present in DLB patient w/o
Capgras.
HC (N = 10).

Varjassyova et al. Amnesic MCI (N = 22) N Test of Facial Emotion Deficits in FER but not in FFI in patients compared to HC. Not measured.
(2013) Recognition (FER)
HC (N = 18) Famous Faces
Identification (FFI)
Mike et al. (2013) MS (N = 49) N Faces Test; Eyes Test; Faux Patients performed poorer than HC in the Faces and Eyes Poor Faces test performance correlated with thinning of
Two groups of HC (N = 24 and PasMRI Test (but not left and right fusiform face area of the fusiform gyrus
18) Faux Pas). and an area in the right entorhinal cortex. Performance
on FER and eye gazes tests correlated with T1-lesion
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

load and regional of T1-lesion of association fiber


tracts interconnecting cortical regions related to visual
and emotional processing.
5

(Continued )
6

Table1. (Continued).
Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarker
Hot et al. (2013) Mild AD (N = 17) N Emotional Morphing Task Impairment of fear identification in AD patients compared Not measured.
to HC.
Young HC (N = 17) and Older HC
A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

(N = 17)
Donix et al. AD (N = 12) N Pictures of: Face/Place— Face recognition was associated to neural networks. Reduced bilateral superior frontal cortical activity among
(2013) HC (N = 12) Familiar/Unfamiliar fMRI AD for familiar versus unfamiliar faces and places.
Also reduced activity in the right middle orbital gyrus.
Group differences in right cerebellum when contrasting
familiar and unfamiliar faces.
Kumfor and Review N Emotion Recognition AD: ER deficits mild on early stages but worse over time. AD: Increased amygdala activity during passive view of
Piguet (2013) AD, FTD (bvFTD; SD; PNFA) Tasks BvFTD: significant ER deficits. (SD & PNFA subtypes also emotional faces (not related to ER nor to specific
PSP and HD present deficits. emotions)
HD: ER deficits for negative emotions (especially disgust). Amnestic MCI: poor FER linked to changes on the left
PSP: Deficits in emotion recognition. uncinate fasciculus on DTI.
FTD: Atrophy in frontal and temporal regions: (amygdala,
insula, orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices)
associated with overall impaired of ER (bv and SD).
Premanifest HD: Ability to detect and process disgust is
related to insula integrity. Happiness and anger
recognition associated with amygdala and striatal
integrity, respectively.
Baez et al. (2013) Genetic Cockayne Syndrome N Emotional Morphing Task Difference in recognizing basic emotions of disgust and Global atrophy in the frontal structures, bilateral
(1) patient (N = 1) anger. cerebellum, basal ganglia, temporal lobe, and
occipitotemporal and occipitoparietal regions.
Klein-Koerkamp Meta-analysis: 24 studies. AD and N Emotional naming task AD patients showed significant impairment in emotional Not measured.
et al. HC Emotional selection task decoding abilities and were significantly impaired in
Emotional matching task decoding emotions regardless of the task used, the
Emotional discrimination stimuli involved, the emotion to be processed or the
task severity of the disease.
Henry et al. MS (N = 64) N FER Test Lower scores for patients in emotion FER. Not measured.
(2011) HC (N = 30)
Bediou et al. MCI (N = 10) N Facial Expression Task Mild AD patients performed worse than HC in facial Not measured.
(2009) Mild dementia (N = 10) expression.
FTD (N = 10) Facial Gender Task FTD performed worse than controls in facial expression
HC (N = 10) and gaze.
Eye Gaze Direction Task FTD and AD differed in expression detection and gaze
discrimination.
MCI showed no impairment.
Werner et al. Frontotemporal Lobar N Emotional Experience and FLD were less likely than HC to recognize that the main Greater accuracy for the fear film associated with greater
(2007) Degeneration (N = 28) Emotion Recognition lobar volumes in the left frontal, right frontal and right
temporal lobes.
Emotional Behavior No group diff. for emotional reactivity (self-reported For the sad film: Accuracy associated w. greater lobar
subjective experience) volumes in the left frontal, left temporal and right
temporal lobes.
(Continued )
Table1. (Continued).
Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarker
De Renzi, Perani, Review. Prosopagnosia Patients N Matching and age One patient showed impairment in all of the face Right cerebral and occipital lesions. Signal abnormalities
Carlesimo, (N = 27) estimation of unknown recognition tests; one in detected in the temporal mesial regions.
Silveri, & Fazio, faces all but age estimation and one only on memory tests.
(1993)
Famous people (familiar Right hemisphere metabolic reduction in cortical and
or unfamiliar) deep cerebral areas (PET) that were structural
unaffected
27 reported cases of prosopagnosia and RH lesions.

Feuerriegel et al. Review (67 articles): ASD/SZBD/ N/P Facial/emotional N170/ N170 and VPP ERP components associated with FER ability. SZ: smaller amplitudes for the N170 and VPP to
(2014) AD/ADHD/Social M170/VPP emotional and no-emotional tasks. Alcohol
PhobiaDepression/Alcohol dependence, ADHD, AD, MCI, BD, Bulimia and HD
dependenceParkinson/CP/ showed smaller N170 and slower peak latencies to
BulimiaFibromialgia/HD faces. Except SZ and ASD all results using FER tasks
showed impairment in this processing.
Sachse et al. Paranoid SZ (N = 19) P FER No differences for SZ Autism: deficit in the recognition of Not measured.
(2014) High function ASD (N = 22) basic and complex emotions compared to HC and SZ.
HC (N = 20)
Baez, Marengo, et Borderline (N = 15) P Emotion Morphing No group differences for E-Morphing. Not measured.
al. (2014)
HC (N = 15) TASIT Lower scores for TASIT and difficulty w. disgust
categorization (signif. though effect of depression and
anxiety on disgust).

Aoki et al. (2014) Meta-Analysis: 13 studies. ASD P Face recognition Atypical FER in ASD compared to HC. ASD-related hyperactivation in the hypothalamus during
(N = 226) emotional face processing. Most robust finding:
hyperactivation of the left caudate. Abnormalities in
the subcortical structures, such as amygdala,
hypothalamus and basal ganglia associated w. atypical
emotional face processing in ASD. Hypoactivation in
the hypothalamus during emotional-face processing
(2013) Anorexia patients (N = 31) P Implicit facial emotion Both groups showed high accuracy on gender decision on FMRI analysis revealed a greater blood-oxygenation level
HC (N = 35) processing (IFAP) task the implicit FER task but patients with anorexia were dependent (BOLD) response in patients with anorexia
slower on the task than HC. in the right fusiform gyrus to all facial expressions.
Baez, Herrera, SZ (N = 15) P Emotional Morphing Task Poorer accuracy performance in SC for disgust, anger and Not measured.
Villarin, et al. fear.
(2013) (2)
BD (N = 15) TASIT BD performed poorer on fear and sadness recognition.
HC (N = 30) BD slower RT for disgust, surprise and sadness
Both groups performed worse than controls on TASIT but
SC
performed worse than BD
Ibanez et al. Euthymic BP (N = 13) P Dual Valence Task (angry/ No group difference found for DFT. Reduced N170 and facial emotion modulation/N170
(2012) happy—please/ analysis of facial stimuli evidenced fusiform gyrus
unpleasant)
HC (N = 13) Valence effect on accuracy found. activation: Reduced in BD.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Schepman et al. Children and adolesc. w. P Face Emotion Task No group differences for ER. Not measured.
(2012) Depression (N = 29)
(Continued )
7
8

Table1. (Continued).
Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarker
Depression and conduct disorder Better discrimination of low intensity happy faces by
(N = 23) depressed subjects.
HC (N = 37)
A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Kang et al. (2012) OCD (N = 107) P Emotional Perception Task Impaired Emotional Awareness such as lower perspective Not measured.
HC (N = 130) of face express. taking
and fantasy seeking had a perception bias toward disgust
in response
to ambiguous facial expressions in OCD patients.
Turetsky et al. SZ (N = 16) P Penn Facial Emotion Patients performed poorer in ER than HC. No group diff in P100 nor N250
(2007) HC (N = 16) Stimuli SZ exhibited smaller N170 responses
Response to neutral faces was smaller (N170)
Patients had reduced P300
Dawson et al. Review. ASD and HC. P Face recognition ASD showed impairments in face discrimination and ERP: Neural system that mediates face processing is
(2005) recognition. disrupted by age three and persist in adulthood.
Use of atypical strategies for processing faces Early (encoding) and later (representation) stages of face
characterized by processing affected
reduced attention to the eyes and piecemeal rather than impairments in perception of facial expression of
holistic strategies. emotions
Abnormal gamma band activity during processing of
upright and inverted faces suggesting decreased
perceptual binding and consistent with impairment of
holistic processing of faces/Abnormal regional brain
(fMRI) activity during face processing.
Buhlmann et al. BDD (N = 20) P Short Form of Benton BDD performed worse than HC in ER tasks. No group Not measured.
(2004) OCD (N = 20) Facial Recognition difference for face recognition. BDD were impaired in
HC (N = 20) Emotion recognition task recognizing disgusted expressions.
OCD showed an emotion recognition bias for angry
expressions.
Ellis et al. (1997) Capgras w Psychiatric Diagnostic P Recognition of familiar Capgras patients showed deficits in face processing tasks Not measured.
(N = 5) faces compared to HC, although they showed no difference in
Psychiatric Patients w/o Capgras Matching photos of SCR between familiar and unfamiliar faces.
(N = 5) unfamiliar faces
HC (N = 5) Recognition memory of
faces
Skin Conductance
Response (SCR)
Ellis et al. (1993) Capgras delusion (N = 3) P Pictures of objects and Reversal in normal asymmetry for the Capgras group in Not measured.
Paranoid SZ (N = 3) faces shown on the face recognition (HC showed left visual field advantage
HC right visual field; left and in Capgras this was reversed).
visual field and bilateral
F: Field (neurology or psychiatry). AD: Alzheimer’s disease; bvFTD:behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; SD: semantic dementia; PNFA: progressive nonfluent aphasia; CSF: cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers; nfv-PPA: non
fluent/agrammatic nonsemantic primary progressive aphasia; lv-PPA: logopenic non-semantic primary progressive aphasia; CBS: corticobasal syndrome; NMO: neuromielitis optica; ALS: Amiotrophic lateral sclerosis; CP:
congenital prosopagnosia; MS: Multiple sclerosis; ASD: Autism spectrum disorder; DLB: Capgras syndrome in Lewy body dementia; MCI: mild cognitive impairment; SZ: schizophrenia; BD: bipolar disorder; OCD: obsessive
compulsive disorder; BDD: body dysmorphic disorder; HD: Huntington’s disease; PSP: progressive supranuclear palsy; ADHD: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; HC: healthy controls; FER: facial emotion recognition; ER:
emotion recognition; TASIT: The Awareness of Social Inference Test; VBM: voxel-based morphometry; DTI: diffusion tensor imaging; SCR: skin conductance response.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 9

respectively. Moreover, relatively similar affectation of neurodevelopmental (Baron-Cohen, 2009) conditions


early brain markers of FER is observed across psychiatric (Table 2).
conditions (Ibanez et al., 2014). Empathy deficits are noteworthy and consistent in
FER dysfunctions are ubiquitously observed across bvFTD (Baez & Ibanez, 2014b; Eslinger, Moore,
neurological and psychiatric conditions (Table 1). Anderson, & Grossman, 2011; Lough et al., 2006;
Across individuals and patient groups, dimensional dif- Rankin et al., 2006; Rankin, Kramer, & Miller, 2005),
ferences are observed in various stimulus-related (arou- which involves atrophy in frontotemporal networks
sal, valence, types of emotions) and performance- extending throughout the above structures (Cerami
related (accuracy, reaction times) variables. Similarly, et al., 2014b; Eslinger et al., 2011; Rankin et al., 2005).
imaging results show different degrees of damage in Loss of empathy in this population has been related
relevant neural hubs (fusiform, amygdala, insula), while to early impairment in social functioning (Piguet
electromagnetic techniques evidence alterations in et al., 2011). Suggestively, core (affective) deficits in
either early or late brain dynamics (Ibanez et al., empathic concern appear to be unrelated to execu-
2012). These dimensional differences are transnosologi- tive dysfunction (Baez & Ibanez, 2014b) and triggered
cally observed within and across neurological and psy- by regions of early atrophy, such as the OFC and the
chiatric diseases. Also, the deficits in question may amygdala (Baez et al., 2016).
involve similar (e.g., N170 amplitude/latency alterations) Research on other neurological conditions (Table 2)
as well as differential (neurodegeneration, focal lesions, largely supports a network-based conceptualization of
aberrant connectivity) mechanisms. No empirical com- empathy (Cerami et al., 2014a; Kraemer et al., 2013;
parisons of these dimensions are available across con- Leigh et al., 2013; Narme et al., 2013; Rankin et al.,
ditions and related neural mechanisms, reducing the 2006; Shamay-Tsoory, Tomer, Goldsher, Berger, &
chances of developing multilevel models addressing Aharon-Peretz, 2004). For instance, cognitive and
different outcomes. Moreover, little is known about affective empathy are doubly dissociated following
how similar FER deficits impacts everyday behaviors lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex
across conditions. Thus, future research is required to (vmPFC) and the inferior frontal gyrus, respectively
integrate behavioral and cerebral measures of FER with (S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, Aharon-Peretz, & Perry, 2009).
clinical associations across diseases. Similarly, a comparison among neurodegenerative
conditions (Rankin et al., 2005) suggests that affective
empathy is likely mediated by temporal structures,
Empathy
whereas cognitive empathy is partially mediated by
Empathy is the ability to understand and experience frontal structures. These studies align with previous
other people’s emotional states (Decety & Jackson, evidence in showing that several neuroanatomical loci
2004). Some research programs distinguish between play a role in this vast functional network (Kennedy &
affective and cognitive empathy. The former (which Adolphs, 2012).
usually includes empathic concern and personal dis- Empathy impairments have also been reported innu-
tress) refers to the ability to adequately express or merous psychiatric disorders (Table 2), such as SZ
recognize emotions in response to others’ mental (Derntl et al., 2009; McCormick et al., 2012; Montag,
states. Cognitive empathy, sometimes identified with Heinz, Kunz, & Gallinat, 2007), BD (Cusi, Macqueen, &
ToM (see below), refers more specifically to the capacity McKinnon, 2010; Seidel et al., 2012; Shamay-Tsoory,
to imagine and understand others’ perspectives or Harari, Szepsenwol, & Levkovitz, 2009), and ASD (Baez
mental states. Several cognitive and affective functions & Ibanez, 2014a; Baez et al., 2012; Baron-Cohen &
are involved in this process, which engages the insula, Wheelwright, 2004; Dziobek et al., 2008). Despite the
the somatosensory cortex, the periaqueductal gray, and variability in associated neural abnormalities (Derntl
the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Decety & Jackson, et al., 2012; Di Martino et al., 2009; Fakra, Salgado-
2004; Decety & Lamm, 2006; Jackson, Rainville, & Pineda, Delaveau, Hariri, & Blin, 2008; Gur et al., 2007;
Decety, 2006; Melloni, Lopez, & Ibanez, 2013). The Habel et al., 2010; Pugliese et al., 2009), they rely on a
amygdala and related frontotemporal networks are cri- partially shared network. For example, studies on SZ
tical for the detection of intentional harm in empathy- point to abnormal frontoinsular-temporal systems as a
for-pain paradigms (Hesse et al., 2016). Empathy is one key correlate of empathy deficits (Benedetti et al., 2009;
of the most widely compromised social cognition Lee et al., 2010). Such impairments can be tapped
domains across a range of neurodegenerative (Rankin through ecological methods involving implicit cues,
et al., 2006), cerebrovascular (Leigh et al., 2013), psy- which more closely replicate real-life settings (Baez,
chiatric (Derntl, Seidel, Schneider, & Habel, 2012), and Herrera, Villarin, et al. 2013; Torralva et al., 2012).
10

Table 2. Empathy in neurological and psychiatric conditions.


Study Subjects F Task Behavioral findings Biomarker
Jiang et al. (2014) Idiopathic generalized N Interpersonal Reactivity Index and eye emotion Impaired eye emotion recognition and cognitive Not measured.
epilepsy (N = 42); controls recognition tasks. empathy abilities. Preserved affective empathy.
(N = 47)
Baez and BvFTD (N = 37); controls N Empathy-for-pain task. Deficits in affective cognitive and moral aspects of Not measured.
Ibanez (2014b) (N = 30) empathy. Empathic concern was the only aspect
not related to deficits in EFs or other social
cognition domains.
A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Yeh and Tsai (2014) Stroke patients (N = 34); N Verbal and nonverbal empathy tasks. On the subscale of cognitive empathy, the right Not measured.
controls (N = 40) stroke group had poorer performance on
perspective-taking.
Fujino et al. (2014) MDD (N = 11); controls P Videos showing human hands in painful Lower pain ratings for the painful videos. Reduced cerebral activation (fMRI) in the left
(N = 11) situations. Subjective pain ratings. middle cingulate cortex and the right
somatosensory-related cortices. Greater
cerebral activation in the left inferior frontal
gyrus.
Corbera, Ikezawa, Bell, SZ (N = 19); controls (N = 18) P Pictures. Subjective ratings. Empathic self-reports. Lower ratings of perspective taking and higher Decreased responses to pictures of others in pain
& Wexler, (2014) ratings of personal distress related to decreased (EEG/ERP). Decreased modulation by attention
modulation of late-cognitive empathic of late-cognitive ERPs related to behavioral
responses. deficits.
Abu-Akel, Fischer- SZ (N = 28); controls (N = 27) P Pain evaluation task with oxytocin administration. Absence of empathy bias toward conflictual out- Not measured.
Shofty, Levkovitz, group members.
Decety, & Shamay-
Tsoory, (2014)
Schroeter et al. (2008) BvFTD (N = 417); Controls N Meta-analysis. Anatomical and activation Empathy processing impairments. Compromised networks were associated with
(N = 408) likelihood estimates. emotion and reward processing, empathy, and
executive functions (mainly inhibition)
Cerami et al. (2014b) BvFTD (N = 18); controls N Intention attribution and emotion attribution Intention attribution and emotion attribution Emotion attribution correlated with gray-matter
(N = 36) using non-verbal test. impairments. reduction in the right amygdala, left insula,
and posterior-superior temporal sulcus.
Cerami et al. (2014a) ALS (N = 20); controls (N = 56) N Attribution of emotional versus cognitive states in Defective emotional empathy attribution. Deficit correlated with reduced grey-matter
comic strips. density in the anterior cingulate cortex and
right inferior frontal gyrus.
Kraemer et al. (2013) RRMS (N = 25); controls N Baron-Cohen’s Empathy Quotient Significantly lower level of empathy in the self- Not measured.
(N = 25) rating questionnaire.
Narme et al. (2013) PD (N = 23); controls (N = 46) N Interpersonal Reactivity Index PD patients showed lower levels of empathy Not measured.
Leigh et al. (2013) Stroke patients (N = 27); N Videos and stories. Impairment in affective empathy. Acute impairment of affective empathy was
controls (N = 24) associated with infarcts in the temporal pole
and anterior insula.
Wojakiewicz, Januel, SZ (N = 29); controls (N = 27) P Sensitivity to expressions of pain (STEP) test. Pain Significant differences in all three measures. Not measured.
Braha, Prkachin, Video. Situational pain questionnaire (SPQ)
Danziger, &
Bouhassira, (2013).
Driscoll, Dal Monte, Vietnam combat veterans N Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale. Diminished emotional empathy. Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping revealed
Solomon, Krueger, & with focal penetrating damage in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, left
Grafman, (2012) traumatic brain injuries and right temporal lobes, and insula, was
(N = 192); Non-injured associated with diminished emotional
veterans (N = 54) empathy.
Kang et al. (2012) OCD (N = 107); controls P Interpersonal Reactivity Index, Toronto Lower perspective taking, and higher personal Not measured.
(N = 130) Alexithymia Scale-20 and emotional perception distress, and higher alexithymia.
task of face expression.
McCormick et al. SZ (N = 25); controls (N = 22) P Interpersonal Reactivity Index and hand Higher affective empathy driven by personal Actively psychotic participants showed
(2012) movement observation task. distress subscale. significantly greater mu suppression over the
sensorimotor cortex (EEG). Abnormal “mirror”
activity was positively correlated with
psychotic symptoms.
(Continued )
Table2. (Continued).
Study Subjects F Task Behavioral findings Biomarker
Seidel et al. (2012) BD (N = 21); first-degree P Facial Identities, scenes showing social interaction, Significantly lower scores in patients for emotion Not measured.
relatives (N = 21); controls short written sentences describing situations, recognition and affective responsiveness.
(N = 21) and Interpersonal Reactivity Index
Baez et al. (2012) ASD (N = 15); controls P Empathy-for-pain task and Interpersonal Reactivity ASD scored higher on PD subscale and lower on PT Not measured.
(N = 15) Index (IRI) of IRI. ASD rated the intentional pain situations
with lower scores. No difference between scores
for intentional and accidental harm situations
(expected for controls).
Derntl, Finkelmeyer, et SZ (N = 15); controls (N = 15) P Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy, the Significant empathic deficits in patients, reflected Hypoactivation in a fronto-temporoparietal
al. (2012) German Questionnaire for Assessment of in worse performance in all three domains: network including the amygdala in patients
Empathy, Social Attitude and Aggression, and emotion recognition, perspective-taking, and (fMRI). Moreover, amygdala activation
the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Facial affective responsiveness. correlated negatively with severity of negative
Identities, Scenes showing social interaction, symptoms.
short written sentences describing situations.
Derntl, Finkelmeyer, et SZ (N = 24); BD (N = 24); + 24 P Facial identities depicting five basic emotions, SZ showed the strongest impairment in empathic Not measured.
al. (2012) MDD (N = 24); controls pictures depicting scenes showing social performance followed by BD, while MDD
(N = 24) interaction and short written sentences performed similar to controls, except for
describing real-life situations. affective responsiveness.
Eslinger et al. (2011) FTD (N = 26); controls N Interpersonal Reactivity Index BvFTD patients were significantly impaired on VBM revealed that reduced empathic
(N = 16) caregiver assessments of empathy, although perspective-taking was related to bilateral
self-ratings were normal. frontal and left anterior temporal atrophy,
whereas empathic emotions were related to
right medial frontal atrophy.
Cusi et al. (2010) BD (N = 20); controls (N = 20) P Interpersonal Reactivity Index and the Social Reduced perspective taking and higher personal Not measured.
Adjustment Scale Self-Report distress. Altered affective empathic abilities
correlated with reduced psychosocial
functioning and with increased symptom
severity.
Chen, Chen, Decety, Healthy participants from Visual stimuli depicting body parts being injured Older adults reported less dispositional emotional The response in anterior insula and anterior mid-
and Cheng (2014) three age groups (N = 65). and Interpersonal Reactivity Index empathy and their unpleasantness ratings were cingulate cortex showed an age-related
more sensitive to intentional harm. decline (fMRI).
Shamay-Tsoory, Harari, Stroke patients, ventromedial N Interpersonal Reactivity Index Patients with ventromedial lesions were impaired Not measured.
et al. (2009) lesions (N = 11); inferior in cognitive empathy compared to the HC and
frontal gyrus lesions other patient groups. Patients with IFG lesions
(N = 8); posterior lesions were impaired in emotional empathy compared
(N = 11); controls (N = 34) to the HC and the PC group.
Minio-Paluello, Baron- ASD (N = 16); neurotypical P Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation Not measured. Contrary to controls, no amplitude reduction of
Cohen, Avenanti, controls (N = 20). during observation of painful and nonpainful motor-evoked potentials (EEG-ERP).
Walsh, & Aglioti, stimuli affecting another individual.
(2009)
Derntl et al. (2009) SZ (N = 24); controls (N = 24) P Facial identities, scenes showing social interaction Significant empathic deficits in patients, reflected Not measured.
and short written sentences describing in worse performance in emotion recognition,
situations. perspective taking and affective responsiveness
Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy, as well as self-report empathy questionnaires.
German Questionnaire for Assessment of
Empathy, Social Attitude and Aggression, and
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Interpersonal Reactivity Index


Dziobek et al. (2008) ASD (N = 17); controls P Multifaceted Empathy Test Impairment in cognitive empathy. Not measured
(N = 18)
Montag et al. (2007) SZ (N = 45); controls (N = 45) P Interpersonal Reactivity Index Significantly lower scores in cognitive empathy. Not measured.
11

(Continued )
12 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Investigations in both neurological and psychiatric

F: Field (neurology or psychiatry); SZ: schizophrenia; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; MS: Multiple sclerosis; SD: semantic dementia; BD: bipolar disorder; OCD: obsessive compulsive disorder; ASD: Autism
gyrus correlated with empathy score. Empathy

temporal structures in SD, and with subcallosal


score correlated positively with volume of right

AS activated the frontotemporal regions but not


Voxels in the right temporal pole, right fusiform
disorders point to a complex network subserving empa-

gyrus, right caudate, and right subcallosal


thy processing. Different dimensions (empathic con-

the amygdala when making mentalistic


cern, perspective taking, distress, intentionality) are

inferences from the eyes (fMRI).


affected to varying degrees across disorders and related
to different and distributed regions of the empathizing
Biomarker

gyrus volume in FTD. network. Moreover, other aspects of empathy, such as


the distinctions between cognitive and affective or
explicit and implicit dimensions, are not well character-
Not measured.

FTD and SD showed lower levels of empathy than Not measured.

Patients with prefrontal lesions and patients with Not measured.

Not measured.
ized at transnosological level. Thus, despite the initial
step showing pervasive impairment of this function
across neurological and psychiatric conditions, further

spectrum disorder; MDD: major depressive disorder; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; HD: Hungtington’s disease; PD: Parkinson’s disease; RRMS: Relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis.
right parietal lesions were significantly impaired
research is required to tackle the above constructs
transnosologically.
showed disruption of cognitive empathy only.
both emotional and cognitive empathy. FTDs
Lower empathy scores for FTD and SD patients.

either ADs or NCs. SDs showed disruption of

in both cognitive and affective empathy.


Reduced empathy as rated by caregivers.

Theory of mind
Behavioral findings

The ability to infer other people’s mental states is


Significantly lower scores for AS.

Significantly worse performance.

known as ToM (Premack & Woodruff, 1978). It impli-


cates several areas, including the medial prefrontal cor-
tex (PFC), the bilateral temporoparietal junction, and
the medial parietal cortex. ToM tasks involve various
levels of complexity and recruit several cognitive pro-
cesses (Ahmed & Stephen Miller, 2011). For example,
the Reading-the-Mind-in-the-Eyes Test only requires
identifying an emotional state associated to a gaze;
instead, the faux pass test is considerably more
N Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy

ASD (N = 6); controls (N = 12) P Inference of mental states with photographs.


FTD (N = 18); SD (N = 19); AD N Interpersonal Reactivity Index by first-degree

(QMEE) and Interpersonal Reactivity Index

demanding, as it engages the ability to implicitly inte-


grate cognitive inferences about mental states together
with emotion understanding (Stone, Baron-Cohen, &
Knight, 1998).
N Empathy reports by caregivers.
N Interpersonal Reactivity Index
Task

ToM is especially relevant for the clinical character-


ization and prognosis of bvFTD, as it is more impaired
in this disease (Gregory et al., 2002; Henry, Phillips, &
P Empathy quotient

Von Hippel, 2014) than in other neurodegenerative


relatives.

conditions (J. D. Henry, Phillips, Crawford, Ietswaart, &


Summers, 2006). BvFTD patients have difficulties infer-
ring others’ intentions (cognitive ToM) and emotional
states (affective ToM) (Gregory et al., 2002; Schroeter,
F

PNFA (N = 8); AD (N = 38);


CBD (N = 15); PSP (N = 6);

Raczka, Neumann, & Von Cramon, 2008). These deficits


(N = 16); controls (N = 10)

lesions (N = 15); controls


lesions (N = 36); parietal
FTD (N = 30); SD (N = 26);

Stroke patients, prefrontal

may reflect damage to the frontomedian network


BvFTD (N = 18); controls

ASD (N = 90); controls

(including the anterior PFC, the ACC, the subcallosal/


controls (N = 20)
Subjects

septal area, and the gyrus rectus) (Bruning, Konrad, &


Herpertz-Dahlmann, 2005; Couto et al., 2013), altera-
(N = 13)

(N = 19)

(N = 90)

tions in the anterior medial frontal cortex—Brodmann


areas (BAs) 9 and 32—hypometabolism in the right
lateral PFC (Le Bouc et al., 2012), and perfusion in the
Table2. (Continued).

Wheelwright, (2004)

left rostral medial PFC (BA 10) (Bertoux, Volle, et al.


Shamay-Tsoory et al.
Rankin et al. (2006)

Rankin et al. (2005)


Lough et al. (2006)

Baron-Cohen et al.

2014).
Baron-Cohen &

ToM impairments are also pervasive in other neuro-


logical conditions such as semantic dementia, temporal
(2004)

(1999)
Study

lobe epilepsy (Hennion et al., 2014), MS (Roca et al.,


2014), focal brain lesions, and Parkinson’s disease
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 13

(Poletti, Vergallo, Ulivi, Sonnoli, & Bonuccelli, 2013). Also, complex ToM skills are more sensitive to impairments
Notably, AD patients feature impairments in the most in non-social mechanisms such as language or executive
complex levels of ToM—correlated with hypometabo- functions. Regarding the multiple neural mechanisms of
lism in the left temporoparietal junction (Le Bouc et al., ToM subtypes (emotional, cognitive, high-level mentaliz-
2012), with preservation of intermediate and simple ing, irony, figurative language, perspective taking), more
levels (Castelli et al., 2011). Similar deficits are observed systematic research is needed. Dimensional and transno-
in stroke patients with temporal and medial frontal sological comparisons across ToM components and asso-
damage (Duval et al., 2012). ciated neural substrates are also missing.
Psychiatric conditions also involve ToM deficits. This
is true of non-remitted and remitted patients diagnosed
Moral judgment
with SZ (Bora, Yucel, & Pantelis, 2009; Brune, 2005;
Harrington, Siegert, & McClure, 2005; Huepe et al., Morality can be defined as the code of values and
2012; Sprong, Schothorst, Vos, Hox, & Van Engeland, customs conventionally accepted within a social group
2007) and BD (Samamé, Martino, & Strejilevich, 2012; (Marazziti, Baroni, Landi, Ceresoli, & Dell’Osso, 2013).
Van Rheenen & Rossell, 2013). ASD individuals are not Moral cognition consists in using such codes to guide
the exception (Yirmiya, Erel, Shaked, & Solomonica-Levi, culturally adequate social behavior (Moll, Zahn, De
1998), although their impairments may be limited to Oliveira-Souza, Krueger, & Grafman, 2005), by virtue of
implicit aspects of the domain (Schneider, Slaughter, cognitive, emotion-related, and ToM processes (Decety
Bayliss, & Dux, 2013). Complex ToM abilities are com- & Howard, 2013; Decety, Michalska, & Kinzler, 2012).
promised in other conditions such as OCD (Sayın, Oral, These domains converge, for example, in the passing
Utku, Baysak, & Candansayar, 2010). Social anxiety dis- of moral judgment (Anderson, Bechara, Damasio,
order patients stand apart from the previous popula- Tranel, & Damasio, 1999), via both cortical (OFC, ante-
tions in that they attribute greater emotional content rior PFC, dorsolateral PFC, vmPFC, anterior temporal
and meaningfulness to others’ mental states than con- lobes, superior temporal sulcus) and subcortical (amyg-
trols (Hezel & McNally, 2014). Finally, context-sensitive dala, ventromedial hypothalamus, septal area and
tasks have revealed subtle ToM impairments in psycho- nuclei, basal forebrain) structures (Moll et al., 2005).
paths (Sharp & Vanwoerden, 2014) and BPD patients Moral judgment is typically impaired in bvFTD
(Preissler, Dziobek, Ritter, Heekeren, & Roepke, 2010). In patients (Lough et al., 2006), who lack moral emotional
the latter population, such deficits are partially reactions, favor utilitarian decisions in personal dilem-
explained by an incapacity to integrate social cues mas, and approve emotion-guided moral violations
(Baez et al., 2014, Baez & Ibanez, 2014b). (Mendez, Anderson, & Shapira, 2005; Mendez &
ToM impairments figure prominently in various neuro- Shapira, 2009). Moreover, these patients have difficulty
logical and psychiatric diseases (Table 3). At a dimensional inferring the intentionality of others’ actions (Baez et al.,
level, complex or high-order ToM deficits are more 2014, 2016), and tend to base their moral decisions
extended than those affecting basic ToM abilities. exclusively on outcomes rather than on the integration
Although the latter are usually affected in very severe of intentions and outcomes (Baez et al., 2014). This
conditions, they are sometimes preserved after focal impairment is triggered by atrophy of extended net-
stroke, early stage neurodegeneration or even diffuse works (but not focalized regions) implicated in moral
alterations, including connectivity aberrations. Also, these processing (Baez et al., 2015). In healthy participants,
basic levels tend to be more affected in conditions with moral judgment tasks engage areas comprised by FTD
strong and extended social cognition impairments (e.g., atrophy such as the lateral OFC and the medial PFC
bvFTD, SZ) than in those conditions with more restrictive (during transgressions of social norms) (Berthoz,
social impairments (e.g., PD, AD, OCD). Similarly, implicit Armony, Blair, & Dolan, 2002) and the posterior cingu-
ToM tasks which depend on more extended brain net- late gyrus, the amygdala, and the vmPFC (in moral
works are often more sensitive than explicit paradigms dilemma solving) (J. D. Greene, Nystrom, Engell,
across neuropsychiatric conditions. Besides, the review Darley, & Cohen, 2004; Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom,
shows that different pathophysiological mechanisms can Darley, & Cohen, 2001).
originate these deficits. Not only core (e.g., direct compro- Deficits in moral judgment are pervasive across neu-
mise following lesions or atrophy of prefrontal or temporal rological conditions. Subsequent to vmPFC damage,
regions) but also distributed or unspecific brain distur- stroke patients fail to generate skin conductance
bances can induce ToM impairments. Similarly, different responses before endorsing personal moral violations
affective (e.g., empathy) and cognitive (e.g., intelligence) (Ciaramelli, Muccioli, Làdavas, & Di Pellegrino, 2007;
domains impacts in ToM performance (Ibanez et al., 2013). Moretto, Làdavas, Mattioli, & Di Pellegrino, 2010). In
14

Table 3. Moral cognition in neurological and psychiatric conditions.


Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarkers
Baez and Ibanez BvFTD (N = 37) and HC (N = 30) N Animated scenarios that involves BvFTD (a) rated neutral situations as more incorrect than HC Not measured.
(2014) the perception of intentional and rated neutral situations higher than controls; (b)
and accidental harm presented deficits in moral aspects of empathy.
Baez et al. (2014) PFC damage (N = 8), bvFTD N Moral judgment task (a) Differences in attempted harm between patients and HC; No differences in patients with vmPFC stroke, other frontal
(N = 19) and HC (N = 37) (b) differences in accidental harm between bvFTD and HC. stroke or frontotemporal atrophy.
Njomboro et al. Neurological damage with N Moral sense test—foreseen and (a) Intended harm: significant differences between patients Not measured.
A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

(2014) apathy symptoms (N = 7) intended harm dimensions with apathy, HC and patients without apathy; (b) Foreseen
and without apathy harm: significant differences between HC and patients
symptoms (N = 7) and HC with apathy.
(N = 17)
Beauchamp et al. Mild-to-Severe TBI (N = 25) and N So-Moral and So-Mature (a) TBI had significantly lower levels of moral reasoning Not measured.
(2013) Typically-developing peers maturity; (b) empathy correlated positively with moral
(N = 66) reasoning abilities.
Gleichgerrcht, BvFTD (N = 22) (13—NO group, N Personal moral dilemmas BvFTD patients who performed utilitarian responses (YES Not measured.
Ibanez, Roca, 9—YES group) group) in front of high-emotional scenarios showed
Torralva, & significantly lower scores on affective ToM relative to
Manes, (2010) those bvFTD patients who did not (NO group).
Calder et al. HD (N = 19) and HC (N = 14) N Photographs of a male or female The HD patients show the largest discrepancy from HC for Not measured.
(2010) model (select which emotional scenarios relating to upper lip curl, the face linked more
expression constituted the most to moral disgust.
appropriate facial reaction)
Miller et al. Anterior-Resected (N = 3), N Reasoning task that required verbal Full and partial callosotomy patients based their moral Not measured.
(2010) complete callosotomy moral judgments judgments primarily on actions “outcomes, disregarding
patients (N = 3) and HC agents” beliefs.
(N = 22)
Young et al. Bilateral vmPFC damage N Moral dilemmas vmPFC patients: (a) judged attempted harms, including Not measured.
(2010) (N = 9), brain damage attempted murder, as more morally permissible relative to
(N = 7) and HC (N = 8) HC; (b) judged attempted harms as more permissible than
accidental harms.
Mendez and FTD (N = 21), AD (N = 21) and N Moral dilemmas (reasoned and FTD patients: (a) retention of knowledge for moral behavior Among the FTD patients, the altered moral judgments
Shapira (2009) HC (N = 21) emotional) and the ability to make reasoned moral judgments; (b) corresponded to right hemisphere frontotemporal
were altered in their ability to make emotional moral involvement (functional neuroimaging).
judgments.
Moretto et al. vmPFC bilateral focal damage N Moral dilemmas and skin vmPFC patients approved more personal moral violations vmPFC patients failed to generate SCRs before endorsing
(2010) (N = 8), damage sparing FC conductance response (SCR) than did HC. personal moral violations; SCRs correlated negatively
(N = 7) and HC (N = 18) with the frequency of utilitarian judgments in normal
participants.
Ciaramelli et al. vmPFC damage (N = 7) and HC N Moral dilemmas (personal and VmPFC patients were more willing to judge personal moral Not measured.
(2007) (N = 12) impersonal) and non-moral violations as acceptable behaviors in personal moral
dilemmas dilemmas, and they did so more quickly.
Koenigs et al. Bilateral, adult-onset vmPFC N Moral dilemmas (high and low Significant differences emerged for the high-conflict Not measured.
(2007) damage (N = 6), brain conflict scenarios) scenarios: The vmPFC group was more likely to endorse
damage (N = 12) and HC the proposed action than either the HC or brain damage
(N = 12) group, with no difference between the HC and brain-
damage participants.
Lough et al. FvFTD (N = 18) and HC (N = 13) N The Moral/Conventional distinction FvFTD patients: (a) moral reasoning was defective; (b) Not measured.
(2006) task executive function was impaired in this group, and
compromised aspects of moral reasoning.
(Continued )
Table3. (Continued).
Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarkers
Mendez et al. FvFTD (N = 26), AD (N = 26) N Inventory of moral knowledge and FTD patients were impaired in their ability to make Not measured.
(2005) and HC (N = 26) moral dilemmas immediate, emotionally based moral judgments
compared with the patients with AD and the HC subjects.
Anderson et al. Early PFC (N = 2) damage, N Moral dilemmas and social The two patients had defective social and moral reasoning. Not measured.
(1999) adult-onset PFC damage situations Thus early-onset PFC resulted in a syndrome resembling
(N = 6) and HC (N = 7) psychopathy.
Bear (1979) Right focal TE (N = 15), left N Five questionnaire items, true–false Some TLE patients exhibit hypermoralism, religiosity and Not measured.
focal TE (N = 12), contrasted in five questionnaire items, true– excessive rumination over the social consequences of
HC (N = 12) and severe false in format actions.
Neuromuscular or ND (N = 9)
Carmona-Perera Alcohol-dependent individuals P Moral judgment task Alcohol-dependent individuals: (a) were more likely to Not measured.
et al. (2014) (N = 31) and HC (N = 34) endorse utilitarian choices in personal moral dilemmas; (b)
poorer decoding of fear and disgust significantly
predicted utilitarian biases in personal moral dilemmas.
Epa, Czyzowska, BD patients (N = 43) and HC. P Defining Issue Test (DIT) BD patients: (a) significantly less often than HC chose Not measured.
Dudek, Siwek, (N = 43) answers indicating the post-conventional thinking; (b)
& Gierowski, patients in mania less often than persons in euthymia
(2014) chose answers indicating the final stage of moral thinking.
Escobar et al. ESD individuals (n19) and P Intentional Inference Task No significant differences between groups on externalizing ESD: (a) atypical early and late frontal cortical markers
(2014) Controls non-adopted and internalizing problems and no between-group associated with attribution of intentionality; (b) reduced
adolescents (N = 18) differences were observed in accuracy measure. activity in the right prefrontal cortex and insula and
bilaterally in vmPFC.
Verdejo-Garcia Cocaine-dependent individuals P Moral dilemmas Cocaine-dependent and HC subjects provided similar Cocaine-dependent subjects: (a) reduced activation
et al. (2014) (N = 10) and nondrug using responses to moral dilemmas, as both study groups did involving frontolimbic structures: (b) less resting-state
controls (N = 14) not differ in the proportion of positive and negative functional connectivity between ACC, thalamus, insula,
responses. and brain stem.
Whitton et al. OCD (N = 23), non-OCD anxiety P Moral dilemmas: benign, OCD: (a) fewer utilitarian responses to impersonal moral Not measured.
(2014) (N = 21) and HC (N = 24) impersonal, personal dilemmas; (b) poorer cognitive flexibility was associated
with fewer utilitarian responses to impersonal dilemmas;
(c) greater trait disgust was associated with increased
utilitarian responding to personal dilemmas.
de Achával et al. SZ patients (N = 13), unaffected P Deontological or utilitarian All participants made the same proportion of utilitarian and Brain activation induced by moral decisions was different
(2013) siblings (N = 13) and HC decisions deontological decisions. in HC, SZ patients, and nonpsychotic siblings in regards
(N = 13) to areas directly concerned with emotion processing.
Gleichgerrcht HFA/AS adults (N = 36) and HC P Moral dilemmas with low and high (a) HFA/AS participants more frequently delivered the Not measured.
et al. (2013) (N = 36) emotional saliency utilitarian judgment; (b) utilitarian HFA/AS participants
showed a decreased ability to infer other people’s
thoughts and to understand their intentions.
Khemiri et al. Alcohol-dependent individuals P Moral dilemmas (personal and AD patients generated increased utilitarian moral judgment Not measured.
(2012) (N = 20) and HC (N = 20) impersonal) and non-moral compared to controls when faced with moral personal
dilemmas dilemmas.
Koenigs et al. Low-anxious psychopaths P Moral dilemmas (a) Both groups of psychopaths were significantly more likely Not measured.
(2012) (N = 12), high-anxious to endorse the impersonal actions; (b) only the low-
psychopaths (N = 12) and HC anxious psychopaths were significantly more likely to
(N = 12) endorse the personal harms when commission of the
harm would maximize aggregate welfare-the “utilitarian”
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

choice.
(Continued )
15
16
A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Table3. (Continued).
Study Subjects F Tasks Behavioral findings Biomarkers
Pujol et al. (2012) Criminals psychopaths (N = 22) P Moral dilemmas Psychopathic individuals and control subjects provided (a) The network subserving moral judgment is underactive
and HC (N = 22) similar responses to most moral dilemmas; the responses in psychopathic individuals during moral dilemma
were all in the same direction psychopaths were more situations; (b) a baseline network alteration with a
likely to endorse the harmful action. functional disconnection between emotional and
cognitive elements that jointly construct moral
judgment.
Zalla et al. (2011) HFA/AS (N = 20) and HC P Transgression scenarios followed HFA/AS failed to distinguish moral and disgust Not measured.
(N = 33) by questions about transgressions along the seriousness dimension and were
permissibility, seriousness, unable to provide appropriate welfare based moral
authority contingency and justifications.
justification
Franklin et al. OCD (N = 20) and HC (N = 18) P Moral dilemmas OCD: The higher patients’ scores on the Responsibility Not measured.
(2009) Attitude Scale, the less likely they were to act to kill one
person to save the lives of others.
Takeda et al. HFPDD students (N = 23)(6– P Human External Action and its HFPDD scored significantly lower on Internal Moral Not measured.
(2007) 14 years old) and HC internal Reasoning Type (HEART) Reasoning than control students.
students (N = 23)
Blair (1995) Psychopaths (N = 10) and HC P Moral stories used to measure the Psychopaths fail to make the moral/conventional distinction Not measured.
(N = 10) moral/conventional distinction and they are significantly less likely to make reference to
the welfare of others.
Benson (1980) SZ (N = 40) and HC (aged P Kohlberg Moral Judgment SZ scored lower than controls on all measures. Not measured.
14–26) Interview
Campagna and 44 (normal and sociopathic P Kohlberg’s Moral Development Level of moral reasoning was higher for normal than for Not measured.
Harter (1975) children) Interview sociopathic children at both mental age levels.
F: Field (neurology or psychiatry); SZ: schizophrenia; bvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; fvFTD frontal variant frontotemporal dementia; MS: Multiple sclerosis; SD: semantic dementia; BD: bipolar disorder; OCD:
obsessive compulsive disorder; ASD: Autism spectrum disorder; HFA/AS: high-functioning autism/Asperger’s syndrome; MDD: major depressive disorder; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; HD: Huntington’s disease; PD: Parkinson’s
disease; RRMS: Relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis; HFPDD: high-functioning pervasive developmental disorder; PFC: prefrontal cortex; TBI: traumatic brain injury; VM: ventromedial; ESD: early social deprivation; FC: frontal
cortex. TE: temporal epilepsy; ND: neuropathic disorder.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 17

addition, when judging harmful intent, these patients comparisons across conditions. As this function
base their moral judgments preferentially on action depends on an extended neural network (Moll &
outcomes (Baez & Ibanez, 2014; Young et al., 2010). Schulkin, 2009; Moll et al., 2005), which is differentially
Although many studies have emphasized the role of affected at different levels, several conditions present
the vmPFC in intuitive/affective moral judgment, similar both similar and dissimilar impairments. Also, subtle
impairments are observed following frontal stroke, with differences among moral sensitivity, emotions, reason-
or without vmPFC involvement (Baez et al., 2014; ing, and dilemmas are not well understood. We do not
Njomboro, Humphreys, & Deb, 2014; Young et al., yet know how these different dimensions interact and
2010) or partial callosotomy (M. B. Miller et al., 2010). manifest each condition. Dimensionality in a single
In neurodegenerative diseases such as HD, impairments domain must also be considered within specific groups.
in recognizing facial expressions correlate with viola- For instance, in studies of moral dilemmas, different
tions of socio-moral norms (Calder et al., 2010) and patient populations present non-absolute differential
moral emotions (e.g., schadenfreude) are also affected patterns of utilitarian and deontological responses.
(Baez et al., 2016). During normal brain ontogeny, early Not all patients with bvFTD develop aberrant moral
PFC lesions compromise the development of moral judgments, and not all OCD patients present hyper-
skills (Anderson et al., 1999); indeed, children with trau- moralistic judgments. Moral cognition impairments are
matic brain injury evince immature socio-moral reason- very variable across individuals (within and between
ing (Beauchamp, Dooley, & Anderson, 2013). diseases). Thus, future neuroscience research should
Similar impairments in moral judgment have also point at individual differences (Dubois & Adolphs,
been observed in psychiatric disorders. Psychopaths 2016) as a strategy to provide a better dimensional
fail to make the moral/conventional distinction (Blair, characterization across conditions. Also, some areas
1995) and more frequently endorse utilitarian choices that have been rarely explored (e.g., moral emotions)
when facing personal dilemmas (Koenigs, Kruepke, should be assessed together with other moral pro-
Zeier, & Newman, 2012). The same occurs in individuals cesses. Thus, a challenge for future neuropsychiatric
with addictions (Carmona-Perera, Clark, Young, Pérez- research into moral cognition is to assess individual
García, & Verdejo-García, 2014; Khemiri, Guterstam, and group variability across relevant processes and
Franck, Jayaram-Lindström, & García, 2012). Such pat- test multilevel models considering different pathophy-
terns are related to functional alterations in the PFC and siological mechanisms.
other regions (Koob & Volkow, 2010; Verdejo-Garcia
et al., 2014). Adults with ASD judge conventional and
Ecological assessment of social context
disgust transgressions as more serious than do controls,
while failing to distinguish between disgust and moral Human interaction and social cognition are multifar-
transgressions (Moran et al., 2011; Zalla, Barlassina, iously influenced by context, namely, the relevant cir-
Buon, & Leboyer, 2011). They also favor utilitarian judg- cumstances surrounding a cognitive event (Adolphs,
ments in moral dilemmas tasks (Gleichgerrcht et al., 2009; Barrett, Lindquist, & Gendron, 2007). Integrating
2013) and show a dissociation between moral knowl- contextual information with our appraisal of others’
edge and moral judgment. The opposite pattern has intentions is critical to deploy adequate social behaviors
been observed in OCD (Franklin, McNally, & Riemann, and access social meaning (Adolph, Meister, & Pause,
2009). Furthermore, immature moral reasoning has 2013; Adolphs, 2009; Baez, Herrera, Villarin, et al. 2013;
been detected across age groups in social deprivation Ibanez & Manes, 2012). Context-dependent effects have
(Escobar et al., 2014), sociopathic conditions been documented in low-level domains—such as visual
(Campagna & Harter, 1975), high-functioning pervasive perception (Bar, 2004)—and in high-level social cogni-
developmental disorders (Takeda, Kasai, & Kato, 2007), tion—including prediction and understanding of
and SZ (Benson, 1980). In psychopaths, addicts, socio- others’ intentions and meanings (Baez et al., 2016,
pathic children, and socially deprived adolescents, 2013; Baez & Ibanez, 2014b; Baez et al., 2012; Ibanez &
moral dilemma tasks yield reduced activation patterns Manes, 2012).
in several areas, including the PFC, the cingulate gyrus, BvFTD clearly illustrates that social cognition impair-
and the insula. ments result from a general inability to integrate con-
In sum, moral judgment is compromised across neu- textual information (Ibanez & Manes, 2012). Such
rological and psychiatric conditions (Table 4), although deficits are present since early stages of the disease
current research is difficult to integrate given the diver- and probably reflect abnormalities in a frontotemporo-
sity of tasks employed, the different moral domains insular network (Ibanez & Manes, 2012), although they
being assessed, and the dearth of systematic are not easily captured in classical and routine
Table 4. ToM in neurological and psychiatric conditions.
18

Subjects F Task Behavioral findings Biomarker


Gregory et al. FvFTD (N = 19); AD patients N NPI, first and second order false belief, Faux Pas FvFTD impaired on all tests of TOM. AD failed in Not measured.
(2002) (N = 12) and HC (N = 16) Test and RMET second-order false belief (heavy demands on
working memory).
Torralva et al. Early/mild fvFTD patients N ToM tasks and IGT FvFTD group impaired in both ToM tasks and IGT. Not measured.
(2009) (N = 20) and HC (N = 10)
Henry et al. TBI patients (N = 16) and HC N RMET, faces and verbal fluency (FAS). In TBI participants significantly reduced: Recognition Deficits in some aspects of EF may partially underlie
(2006) (N = 17) of basic emotions and capacity for mental state deficits in social cognition following TBI
attribution.
A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Schroeter et al. Meta-analysis: 9 studies. FTD N Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data Not measured. Frontomedian network impaired
(2008) patients (N = 132) and HC
(N = 166)
Adenzato, Review N Multiple neuroimaging tasks ToM deficits Link between the progressive degeneration of the
Cavallo, & anterior regions of medial frontal structures
Enrici, (2010) characterizing the early stages of the bvFTD and
the ToM deficits.
Castelli et al. AD patients (N = 16) and HC N RMET and strange stories Complex ToM levels are impaired in AD patients. Not measured.
(2011) (N = 16)
Allain et al. HD patients (N = 18) and HC N Intention test, RMET and EF Significant impairment of ToM abilities in HD patients Cortico–subcortical circuits are underlying higher
(2011) (N = 18) social functions such as ToM.
Duval et al. SD patients (N = 15) and HC N Memory tasks, intention test, false-belief task, Cognitive and affective ToM impairment. ToM deficit was consistent with the patients’ atrophy
(2012) (N = 36) RMET and “Tom’s taste” in the left temporal lobe and hypometabolism in
the temporal lobes and the medial frontal cortex.
Eddy & Rickards, HD patients (N = 16) and HC N Social recognition of inappropriate behavior, HD associated with deficits in ToM Not measured.
(2012) (N = 16) RMET, verbal fluency, working memory,
inhibition, and Problem Behaviors
Assessment-short form.
Le Bouc et al. BvFTD (N = 11); AD patients N Belief-reasoning task, EF and clinical AD patients had deficit in inferring someone else’s Beliefs task correlated with hypometabolism in the
(2012) (N = 12) and HC (N = 20) assessment belief, whereas patients with BvFTD were impaired left TPJ. Self-perspective inhibition correlated with
in inhibiting their own mental perspective. hypometabolism in the right LPFC.
Mike et al. (2013) MS patients (N = 49) and HC N MRI images; tests of recognition of mental Areas of emotion of (right and left fusiform face area,
(N = 24) states and emotions from facial expressions frontal eye filed and right entorhinal cortex) and
and eye gazes socially relevant information (left temporal pole).
Roca, Cerebellar stroke patients N FPT and EF Focal cerebellar lesions do not affect ToM Not measured.
Gleichgerrcht, (N = 11)
Ibáñez,
Torralva, &
Manes, (2013)
Hennion et al. TLE patients (N = 50) and HC N FPT, sarcastic remarks & mentalistic actions Cognitive and affective ToM processes impaired in TLE Not measured.
(2014) (N = 50) patients
Roca et al. (2014) Mild relapsing-remitting MS N FPT and EF Cognitive but not affective ToM deficits in mild Not measured.
(N = 18) and HC (N = 16) relapsing-remitting MS
Henry et al. Meta-analysis: 15 studies. N Meta-analysis of ToM tasks ToM deficit. Not measured.
(2014) BvFTD (N = 312); AD
patients (N = 163) and HC
(N = 325).
Bertoux, Volle, BvFTD patients (N = 20) N Mini-SEA Not measured. Detection of Faux pas was related to rostral mPFC
et al. (2014) perfusion (BA 10) while recognition of emotion
involved more dorsal regions within the mPFC (BA
9).
Yeh & Tsai (2014) CVA patients (N = 34) and HC N Tasks testing verbal and nonverbal ToM and CVA patients were impaired in both cognitive and The right hemisphere may play an important role in
(N = 40) empathy. affective ToM. Poorer performance in patients with decoding nonverbal cues to infer others’ minds as
right stroke on the cognitive component of well as the processing of empathy.
nonverbal ToM.
(Continued )
Table4. (Continued).
Subjects F Task Behavioral findings Biomarker
Brüne, Blank, HD patients (N = 25); SZ N/P ToM and neurocognitive functions HD impairment in ToM relative to HC and deficits in Not measured.
Witthaus, & (N = 25) and HC (N = 25) ToM similar to SZ.
Saft, (2011)
Hezel and SAD patients (N = 40) and HC P RMET and MASC SAD patients attribute more intense emotions and Not measured.
McNally (2014) (N = 40) greater meaning to what others were thinking and
feeling.
Baez, Marengo, BPD patients (N = 15) and HC P EF, emotion recognition, and ToM BPD adults exhibited deficits in the three domains. Not measured.
et al. (2014) (N = 15)
Schneider et al. High-functioning ASD P ToM scale, Strange Stories test, implicit ToM Impaired implicit false-belief in ASD patients. Not measured.
(2013) patients (N = 24) and HC movies
(N = 20)
Van Rheenen and BD patients (N = 49) and HC P Picture sequencing Task. ToM impairment in BD Not measured.
Rossell (2013) (N = 49)
Das, Lagopoulos, SZ patients (N = 20) and HC P Functional MRI images + intention test Not measured. Patients with schizophrenia had significantly
Coulston, (N = 19) diminished activity in the right superior temporal
Henderson, gyrus at the TPJ and bilaterally within the inferior
and Malhi frontal gyri.
(2012)
Samamé et al. Review and meta-analysis of P Meta-analysis ToM tasks Emotion processing and ToM deficits in remitted BD. Not measured.
(2012) BD
Sayın et al. (2010) OCD patients (N = 30) and P First and second order false belief, hinting task Significant reduction in their “advanced” ToM abilities Not measured.
HC (N = 30) and double-bluff task, verbal memory in OCD patients, which seem related to their
processes test, Weschler memory test, and reduced memory capacities.
Stroop test
Preissler et al. BPD patients (N = 64) and HC P MASC and RMET Impaired abilities in BPD patients in MASC not in Not measured.
(2010) (N = 38) RMET
Bora et al. (2009) Meta-analysis: 36 studies. SZ P Meta-analysis of ToM tasks ToM impairment in remission phase suggests Not measured.
patients (combined mentalizing impairments in schizophrenia
N = 1,181) and HC
(combined N = 936)
Sprong et al. Meta-analysis: 29 studies. SZ P Meta-analysis of ToM tasks Mentalizing impairments in SZ even in remission Not measured.
(2007) patients (combined
N = 1518)
Adolph et al. Nonclinical students (N = 40) P Emotional recognition under relax and stress High-anxiety participants were highly sensitive to the Not measured.
(2013) divided in high and lower full conditions. contextual anxiety signals
social anxiety groups
F: Field (neurology or psychiatry). SZ: schizophrenia; BvFTD: behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; fvFTD: frontal variant frontotemporal dementia; CVA: cerebrovascular accident; MS: Multiple sclerosis; SD: semantic
dementia; TBI: traumatic brain injury; HC: human controls; BPD: borderline personality disorder; BD: bipolar disorder; OCD: obsessive compulsive disorder; ASD: Autism spectrum disorder; SAD: social anxiety disorder; TLE:
temporal lobe epilepsy; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; HD: Huntington’s disease; RMET: Reading-the-mind-in-the-Eyes Test; MASC: movie for the assessment of social cognition; EF: executive functions; FPT: faux pas test; Mini Sea:
Social cognition & Emotional Assessment; IGT: Iowa Gambling Task; TPJ: temporoparietal junction; MPFC: medial prefrontal cortex; LPFC: lateral prefrontal cortex.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE
19
20 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

neuropsychological assessment. An aberrant frontotem- attentional demands in complex scenarios increase


poro-insular network predicts social cognition deficits in emotion-related deficits in psychopaths (Newman,
bvFTD (Sedeño et al., 2016). The same holds for frontal Curtin, Bertsch, & Baskin-Sommers, 2010; Sadeh et al.,
lobe lesion patients (Mesulam, 1986). Deficits in this 2013) and young offenders (Gonzalez-Gadea, Herrera,
population can be tapped via more ecological tasks et al., 2014).
with implicit contextual cues (Burgess, Alderman, Altogether, the above findings highlight distur-
Volle, Benoit, & Gilbert, 2009). By the same token, dur- bances of implicit contextual appraisal as one further
ing short-term contextual processing tasks, patients commonality between neurological and psychiatric
with prefrontal compromise exhibit abnormal beha- conditions (Table 5). As predicted by the social context
vioral and electrophysiological responses (Fogelson, network model (Baez et al., 2012; Ibanez & Manes,
Shah, Scabini, & Knight, 2009). Both, bvFTD and frontal 2012), prefrontal areas would support focused predic-
lesion patients present difficulties to develop context- tions by dynamically updating associations in a specific
sensitive strategies for successful social bargaining. context (Amoruso et al., 2016, 2014; Bar, 2004; Friston,
These deficits are associated not only with atrophy 2012; Torralva, Gleichgerrcht, Ibanez, & Manes, 2016).
and lesions of prefrontal regions, but also with fronto- Also, target-context links subserved by temporal
temporal aberrant oscillations during social offers as regions would be integrated with feature-based infor-
well as fronto-posterior functional connectivity abnorm- mation subserved by frontal regions (A. J. Greene,
alities (Melloni et al., 2016). The crucial role of context Gross, Elsinger, & Rao, 2006) (Bar, 2004). Finally, the
for social cognition is further highlighted by studies on insular cortex would be critical to merge emotional
HD. Here, deficits in the appraisal of disgust and high-level signals related to the coordination
(Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996) and other negative emo- between external and internal milieus (Canales-
tions (Johnson et al., 2007) during FER are reduced Johnson et al., 2015; Couto et al., 2015, 2015, 2014,
when contextual clues are provided (Aviezer et al., 2013; García-Cordero et al., 2016; Garfinkel & Critchley,
2009; Baez et al., 2015). 2013; Melloni et al., 2013; Sedeño et al., 2014; Uddin,
Similar difficulties have been observed in psychiatric Kinnison, Pessoa, & Anderson, 2014; Yoris et al., 2015).
disorders. In SZ, inefficient integration of situational Thus, deficits in contextual social cognition will emerge
information (Amoruso, Cardona, Melloni, Sedeno, & upon compromise to any of these three main hubs.
Ibanez, 2012; Guerra et al., 2009; Ibáñez, Riveros, et al. However, no dimensional study has revealed the speci-
2011) may influence emotion recognition (Green, fic contributions of these hubs (frontal, insular, and
Waldron, & Coltheart, 2007; Green, Waldron, Simpson, temporal) to each subprocess or their interactions dur-
& Coltheart, 2008; Monkul et al., 2007), supporting the ing relevant tasks. Also, there is no information about
idea that social context processing is impaired (Cohen, how distinct pathophysiological mechanisms differen-
Barch, Carter, & Servan-Schreiber, 1999; Riveros et al., tially impact this network across neuropsychiatric con-
2010). Also, frontal activity engaged in predictive cod- ditions. Finally, most current tasks fail to capture the
ing of upcoming information is also affected in ADHD influence of implicit contextual information on socio-
and ASD (Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2015). Similar to cognitive performance. In this sense, further research is
bvFTD, SZ features deficits to contextually update social needed based on ecological interactive paradigms (A.
decisions during bargaining (Billeke et al., 2015). Also, M. García & Ibáñez, 2014) and situated cross-domain
ADHD and ASD presents reduced and aberrant markers tasks (García & Ibáñez, 2016).
of social cooperation (Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2016),
respectively. Notably, such deficits, which affect both
social and non-social domains (Chung & Barch, 2011),
A triangulation with social neuroscience
become more marked in ecological tasks (Baez, Herrera,
Villarin, et al. 2013), highlighting the need for novel Nearly all neurological and psychiatric disorders involve
context-sensitive approaches (Chung & Barch, 2011). some form of impairment in the above domains, all of
Comparable deficits have been reported in BD research, which are systematically assessed by social neu-
although these are not so severe (Baez, Herrera, Villarin, roscience. Despite the relative youth and internal pro-
et al. 2013; Brambilla et al., 2007). Also, in ASD, aberrant blems of this field, we argue that it is prime time to
implicit recognition of contextual clues (Senju, 2012) triangulate its efforts with those of neurology and psy-
triggers impairments in social cognition (Baez & chiatry. Moreover, basic principles which guide current
Ibanez, 2014a; Baez et al., 2012; Schneider et al., 2013; social neuroscience can provide fruitful advances if they
Senju, Southgate, White, & Frith, 2009). Finally, are applied to neuropsychiatry, as described below.
Table 5. Social context sensitivity in neurological and psychiatric conditions.
Study Subjects F Task Behavioral findings Biomarker
Aviezer et al. HD patients (N = 21) N Recognition of emotional scenes and body HD displayed intact recognition of emotion from non- Not measured.
(2009) and controls (N = 27) language facial visual stimuli.
Recognition of isolated facial expressions HD participants were impaired in disgust recognition.
Couto, Sedeño, et Focal stroke (N = 2) and controls N Multimodal basic emotion recognition tests The insular lesion patient showed delayed reaction times. Not measured.
al. (2013) (N = 10) Emotional inference disambiguation task using The subcortical lesion patient was impaired in
contextual social clues multimodal aversive emotion recognition, and was
variously impaired in subtasks of empathy and
contextual inference of emotions.
Fogelson et al. Unilateral focal PFC stroke (N = 7) and N Visual stimuli (targets) Faster reaction time for controls in predicted targets. No Not measured.
(2009) controls (N = 7) reaction-time differences among targets for PFC
patients.
Barcelo & Knight Prefrontal injured (N = 10) and HC N Predictable and unpredictable task context Deficits in both the selection and the suppression of Disrupted frontocortical and
(2007) (N = 10) (targets). familiar versus novel contextual information. frontosubcortical
connectivity.
Fogelson et al. SZ (N = 20); HC (N = 20) and N/P Visual discrimination task. SZ were significantly less accurate than controls. Not measured.
(2013) PD (N = 15) No differences for PD and controls.
Baez & Ibanez Review P Executive function assessments. Social cognition deficits in ASD imply a reduced ability to Not measured.
(2014) Emotional recognition and social interference implicitly encode and integrate contextual cues
tasks (contextual integration) needed to access social meaning
Gonzalez-Gadea, AO (N = 30) and HC (N = 15) P High and low context-sensitive measure: facial Offenders had a significantly poorer performance in High Not measured.
Herrera, et al. emotion recognition. context-sensitive measure.
(2014) Fluid intelligence and EF.
Dwyer, Peters, Formal thought SZ (N = 18) and HC P Semantic processing in emotional salient No differences in emotional manipulation but more Not measured.
McKenna, (N = 15) sentences and sensibility-judgments. errors on sense-judgments and repetition than the
David, and other two groups.
McCarthy
(2014)
Schneider et al. High-functioning ASD (N = 24) and HC P Explicit and implicit ToM measure. Funneled Deficits in implicit ToM recognition. Not measured.
(2013) (N = 20) debriefing
Baez, Herrera, SZ (N = 15); BP (N = 15); HC (N = 30) P Executive functions (EF) assessments. EF was deficit for SZ and BD relative to controls; similarly Not measured.
Villarin, et al. Emotional recognition and social interference was for disgust and fear recognition and contextual
(2013) tasks (contextual integration) integration performance.
Adolph et al. Nonclinical students (N = 40) divided P Emotional recognition under relaxed and High-anxiety participants were highly sensitive to the Not measured.
(2013) in high and low social anxiety stressful conditions contextual anxiety signals.
Yang, Tadin, BP (N = 16) and HC (N = 23) P Visual context processing (luminance, contrast, No differences with controls. Stronger motion and Not measured.
Glasser, Wook size, orientation, and motion direction) orientation context effects correlated with worse
Hong, Blake, clinical symptoms.
and Park
(2013)
Fogelson et al. SZ (N = 32) and HC (N = 30) P Visual discrimination task No score differences. Context-dependent P3b deficits
(2011)
Senju et al. (2009) AS subjects (N = 19) and HC (N = 17) P Eye-tracking task In ASD individuals, eye movements did not anticipate an Not measured.
actor’s intention.
Brambilla et al. BD patients (N = 15) and HC (N = 26) P Visual context interference BD showed a context processing deficit relative to Not measured.
(2007) healthy controls.
Green et al. SZ (N = 20) and HC (N = 22) P Target facial expressions preceded by vignettes Patients shown deficit in facial recognition and vignettes Not measured.
(2007) integration.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE

Cohen et al. SZ, UD and HC P Attentional interference test (Stroop) and lexical Accentuation of deficits in patients with schizophrenia in Not measured.
(1999) disambiguation task context-sensitive conditions.
F: Field (neurology or psychiatry). HD: Huntington’s Disease; PFC: prefrontal cortex; SZ: Schizophrenic Patients; PD: Parkinson’s disease; ASD: Autism spectrum disorder; AS: Asperger’s syndrome; EF: executive functions; ToM:
Theory of mind; BD: bipolar disorder; UP: unipolar depression; P3b: P300 event-related potential (ERP) component.
21
22 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Tracking the synergies between brain structures leading to behavioral inadequacies (Cryan & Kaupmann,
and functions 2005; Krystal et al., 2002; Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, &
Heim, 2009; Maguire & Mody, 2008) and illness
In both neurological and psychiatric disorders, social
(Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Indeed, soluble mediators
cognition deficits emerge from disturbances of net-
released by immune cells can affect the central nervous
works spanning multiple neural locations. Modern
system and alter behavior (Irwin & Cole, 2011;
imaging techniques reveal intimate and complemen-
Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). In addition, mood and anxi-
tary connections between brain anatomy and function
ety disorders can be the early manifestation of broader
(Northoff, 2008). In the same vein, diseases and phar-
diseases, or they can index a disruption of neural net-
macotherapy may alter not just brain functions, but
works supporting interpersonal and emotional pro-
also neural structures proper. Building upon these
cesses (Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Dulawa, & Palmer, 2014;
findings, cognitive neuroscience conceives mental
Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & Putnam, 2002).
processes as emerging from coordinated neural activ-
Accompanying social deficits might promote stressful
ity over multiple regions (see below). Across disorders,
interactions and contribute to a multilevel vicious circle
different substrates (from neuronal channels and
(Bartolomucci et al., 2005; Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Dulawa,
genes to global ensembles) subserve specific cogni-
et al. 2014; Meyer-Lindenberg, 2014). Social stress may
tive functions through various interactions (Millan
even have an impact on genetic regulation (Cacioppo,
et al., 2012). Moreover, in neuropsychiatric conditions,
Cacioppo, Dulawa, et al. 2014; Cole, Hawkley, Arevalo, &
social cognition impairments are not consistently
Cacioppo, 2011). In particular, perceived social isolation
associated with well-defined structural atypicalities
(loneliness) modifies the expression of genes driving
(Dickinson & Harvey, 2009). At the same time, func-
inflammation, one of the first responses of the immune
tional abnormalities in psychiatric patients have been
system (Cole et al., 2011). Molecular changes induced
associated with aberrant brain structure (Millan et al.,
by perceived or real social isolation may duplicate the
2012). Thus, the interplay among structures and func-
chances of developing dementia (Wilson et al., 2007)
tions in both neurological and psychiatric conditions
and increases mortality (Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Capitanio,
should be thoroughly studied. This agenda would also
& Cole, 2015).
help to understand how macroanatomical (e.g., focal
In turn, pure pathophysiological conditions can
lesions or specific neurotransmitter abnormalities
directly affect social and emotional processes. This
affecting whole-brain dynamics) and microanatomical
occurs in cases of premenstrual dysphoric disorder (a
(e.g., channelopathies compromising in neural net-
disturbance of lifestyle including symptoms of irritabil-
work dynamics) phenomena impact social cognition
ity, tension, dysphoria, and mood lability) (Steiner,
functions. Similarly, we can study how classically func-
1997), toxic psychosis (a substance-related disorder),
tional conditions are associated with enduring struc-
and MS (where the feelings of depression and fatigue
tural atrophy and how changes in aberrant
are common and detrimental to life quality (Krupp,
connectivity induce structural changes. As shown by
Alvarez, LaRocca, & Scheinberg, 1988; Marrie et al.,
social neuroscience, in between and around such con-
2015)). The same is true of a wide range of neurode-
structs lie multiple interacting dimensions that chal-
generative diseases (Levenson, Sturm, & Haase, 2014;
lenge one-to-one associations between structural
Neary et al., 1998). In FTD, pathology involves complex
damage and neurological disorders, one the one
interrelations among genetic, neural, and behavioral
hand, and functional alterations and psychiatric con-
levels, which challenges straightforward conceptualiza-
ditions, on the other.
tions of the disease. Just as an example, the same
mutation in gene C9orf72 can lead to either bvFTD
(with its typical social cognition impairments) or a pre-
Multilevel brain mechanisms for social cognition
dominant motor disease (such as ALS) (Bruijn, Miller, &
impairments
Cleveland, 2004; Mahoney et al., 2012).
Diseases are not monolithic entities; rather, they result Over the years, scientists have garnered convergent
from the interaction of social, neural, hormonal, cellular, information from different levels (genetics, neurobiol-
molecular, and genetic mechanisms underlying and ogy, behavior, social environment). Yet, this mosaic
surrounding cognitive processes (Caspi & Moffitt, strategy has not yet provided an adequate picture of
2006). Social and psychological phenomena have a neuropsychiatric conditions, let alone their vast varia-
profound impact on multiple neural levels, which in bility (Devor et al., 2013). Correlating mental disorders
turn affect the former in various ways. For instance, with brain activity patterns in critical regions is only a
stress can induce molecular and cellular brain changes first step toward specifying the mechanisms behind
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 23

nervous disorders (Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Dulawa, et al. Neuropsychiatry informing social neuroscience
2014). An adequate picture of neuropsychiatric condi-
In the review section, we assessed available evidence
tions requires connecting the dots from all these
for dimensional and transdiagnostic social cognition
dimensions (Devor et al., 2013).
impairments in neurological and psychiatric conditions.
The multilevel view of social cognition poses addi-
This evidence provides a fruitful agenda for new ques-
tional challenges for triangulation. Although some of
tions, as listed at the end of each subsection therein.
them are beyond current agenda of social neu-
Here, we detail how the triangulation can advance a
roscience, a number of steps can be taken in that
deeper understanding of the social impairments.
direction. The development of multimodal assess-
ments of interaction across levels can be boosted by
the direct comparison of pathophysiological mechan- Different models of the social brain
isms. To this end, it would be crucial to expand the
visibility of triangulated approaches via special issues, Parallel models of the social brain emerge from each
specific funding opportunities, and explicit discussion level of impairment. For example, the study of moral
fora. Finally, recent data-driven methods of social cognition can thrive by considering patients with loca-
neuroscience (Adolphs, Nummenmaa, Todorov, & lized vmPFC damage (Koenigs et al., 2007; Young et al.,
Haxby, 2016) can capitalize on large datasets, find 2010). Despite this straightforward association, a more
patterns across levels, and provide a better character- extended anatomy may be postulated for moral sensi-
ization of the transdimensional nature of social tivity by considering diffuse frontotemporal damage in
cognition. neurodegeneration (Baez, Baez et al., 2014; Ibanez &
Manes, 2012). Moreover, white-matter connectivity
models of moral cognition can be advanced by refer-
ence to complete and partial callosotomy (M. B. Miller
How to promote dimensional and transnosological et al., 2010).
research via a brain network approach Additional evidence may be garnered from disorders
Social cognition impairments attributed to a specific lacking precise neuroanatomical foundations.
substrate can be supported by spatiotemporal integra- Psychopathy, ASD, and Williams syndrome present par-
tion of neural hubs. Today, we know that adequate tially opposite social phenotypes which inform dissocia-
cognitive states depend on the spatial and temporal tions within the social cognition domain. Furthermore,
organization of long-range networks (Sporns, 2014). models of moral cognition may stem from research on
This is especially true of the social brain (Kennedy & aberrant neurotransmission in relevant diseases, includ-
Adolphs, 2012). Some network attributes, such as small- ing mesolimbic dopaminergic dysfunction (Khemiri et al.,
world properties, are associated to similar impairments 2012; Koob & Volkow, 2010) and serotonergic dysregula-
in both neurological and psychiatric disorders (Bullmore tion (Koob & Volkow, 2010; Whitton, Henry, & Grisham,
& Sporns, 2009). However, dissimilar pathophysiological 2014). In sum, different pathogenetic processes across
processes impact differentially in network properties neuropsychiatric conditions can provide separate yet
(García-Cordero et al., 2015; Sedeño et al., 2016).The compatible avenues to build complex models of the
default mode network (DMN), which is implicated in social brain. One simple way to organize a research pro-
social cognition processes, actually constitutes a trans- gram consists in advancing empirical comparisons of a
diagnostic marker across these diseases. The DMN is social cognition domain (e.g., moral cognition) across
impaired in AD, SZ, ASD, and many other disorders. different patient populations with different physiopathol-
Further research characterizing the similarities and dif- ogy, including focal damage of critical regions, diffuse
ferences across conditions regarding the role of the alterations of functional networks, disruption of anatomi-
DMN in social cognition will offer finer grained insights cal tracts, and neurotransmitter dysregulation, among
than those currently available. This will also allow others. In the future, this would help to clarify which
answering whether damage to specific hubs lead to mechanisms are more critical to the target domain, how
similar social cognition deficits across disorders. Also, they interact, and what alternative ways may be posed to
the network dynamics can be used to investigate how ameliorate specific disorders.
specific social cognition mechanisms (e.g., the salience
network) interact with other non-social mechanisms
Social dysfunction at the center or the periphery
(e.g., frontoparietal network). Finally, modifications in
the connectivity of affected hubs (Silasi & Murphy, Social cognition can be impaired by primary or sec-
2014) may afford future strategies for treatment. ondary disturbances such as executive or linguistic
24 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

deficits. Empathy, as well as other abilities discussed variability are observed across disorders (e.g.,
in above sections, relies on working memory (Ze, Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2013; Gonzalez-Gadea, Herrera
Thoma, & Suchan, 2014), inhibitory control (Hansen, et al. 2014).
2011; Ze et al., 2014), and other executive functions Unlike what experimental paradigms and ensuing the-
(Rankin et al., 2005). Inhibition of one’s own perspec- ories imply, neuropsychiatric disorders do not involve
tive is a prerequisite for ToM (Samson, Apperly, deficits in a single domain. To incorporate intercognitive
Kathirgamanathan, & Humphreys, 2005). deficits in the agenda of neuroscientific research, less
Furthermore, working memory is required in com- fragmentary methodologies must be developed. In the
plex social situations (Meyer, Spunt, Berkman, future, classical experimental approaches that denatura-
Taylor, & Lieberman, 2012; Rankin et al., 2006). lize participants’ cognitive processes by prioritizing fixed,
Language disorders may also disrupt social function- oversimplified, and isolated tasks should be complemen-
ing in an indirect way. Wernicke’s aphasics, being ted by more naturalistic tasks engaging interconnected
mostly unable to recognize the unintelligibility of cognitive processes (J. D. Henry, Cowan, Lee, & Sachdev,
their own speech, are prone to develop major para- 2015). Current advances of “second-person” (Schilbach
noid syndrome and even manifest antisocial beha- et al., 2013) or “two-person” (Liu & Pelowski, 2014) neu-
vior (Damasio, 1998). Also, in high-functioning ASD, roscience assessing brain-to-brain coupling (Hasson,
ToM deficits are correlated with deficits to compre- Ghazanfar, Galantucci, Garrod, & Keysers, 2012) and active
hend figurative language, including metaphors and forms of social interaction (Pfeiffer, Timmermans,
irony (Huang, Oi, & Taguchi, 2015). All in all, similar Vogeley, Frith, & Schilbach, 2013), together with multi-
impairments in social cognition can be triggered by source recording technologies, cameras, and mobile
either specifically social deficits or other types of devices (e.g., wearable eye trackers), will be key compo-
cognitive dysfunction. nents of this prospective tool kit. In brief, we need to
Thus, although we can identify some cases where move from isolated, disembodied, and passive assess-
social cognition is more primarily or secondarily ments of social processes to more ecological approxima-
affected, the systematic study of the differences tions (Barutta, Aravena, & Ibáñez, 2010; Barutta, Cornejo,
among these conditions is scarce. Further research is & Ibáñez, 2011; Cosmelli & Ibáñez, 2008; Ibanez &
needed to understand how underlying networks under- Cosmelli, 2008).
lying primary or secondary functions overlap and
interact.
Social neuroscience informing neuropsychiatry
Inter-cognition: beyond instrumental parcellation
Historically, several neurological and psychiatric disor-
Classical assessments of cognitive processes are typi- ders have been assessed with an emphasis on specific
cally rooted in isolation paradigms (Becchio, Sartori, & symptomatology (mostly based on behavioral and
Castiello, 2010), experimental set-ups in which a single mood-related observations, respectively). Little atten-
participant views preprogrammed, decontextualized sti- tion has been paid to the patients’ social dysfunc-
muli and repeatedly performs the same task on them. tions, which are equally prominent and affect real-
However, in neurological disorders, such as PD world functioning (Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cacioppo,
(Dirnberger & Jahanshahi, 2013), NMO (Cardona et al., 2014). Social cognition deficits can be detrimental to
2014), and ALS (Watermeyer et al., 2015), social cogni- functional performance, particularly as regards instru-
tion impairments are typically concomitant with deficits mental activities of daily living. Indeed, several neu-
in other cognitive domains. The same is true of psychia- ropsychiatric conditions involve a disproportion
tric conditions (Dere, Pause, & Pietrowsky, 2010). ASD between global cognitive function and daily function-
involves impairments in central components of social ing. Social cognition domains have to be assessed
cognition, including ToM and empathy (Geschwind, taking into consideration the clinical characterization
2009), but these deficits also depend on executive dys- and questions about patients’ everyday difficulties.
function (Robinson, Goddard, Dritschel, Wisley, & Hence, social cognition measures may be more appro-
Howlin, 2009) and even sensory deficiencies. Likewise, priate surrogate markers of functional efficiency for
BD, SZ, MMD, OCD, ADHD, and post-traumatic stress many patients. Through a combination of behavioral
disorder are characterized by both cognitive (e.g., work- measures, experimental manipulations, and physiolo-
ing memory, memory systems, executive functions, pro- gical recordings, social neuroscience may sow the
cessing speed) and social cognition impairments (Millan seeds for a new conception of neuropsychiatric dis-
et al., 2012). Moreover, higher levels of cognitive orders and a novel approach to clinical practice.
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 25

Dimensional and transdiagnostic cross talk in Pharmacological therapeutics


psychiatry and neurology
Stabilization of chemical alterations underlying social
Social cognition impairments in major clinical entities disturbances may have positive effects across disease
from neurology and psychiatry could be better under- types. The neuroprotective effect of oxytocin treat-
stood by providing transnosological research. The need ment may impact social interaction and social cogni-
for new approaches to classify mental disorders has tion performance not only in ASD (Bakermans-
been addressed by the Research Domain Criteria Kranenburg & Van, 2013) but also in other disorders
(RDoC) project. The same dimensional and transdiag- such as stroke (Karelina et al., 2011), genetic diseases
nostic principles are more difficult to implement in (Francis et al., 2014), autism and seizures (Durand,
neurology, but some developments have begun (e.g., Pampillo, Caruso, & Lasaga, 2008), Prader–Willi syn-
(Carter & Ffytche, 2015)), specially under the guidance drome (Meziane et al., 2014), anxiety (Milrod et al.,
of social neuroscience (Brugger & Lenggenhager, 2014). 2014), BPD (Stanley & Siever, 2010), depression and
The RDoC begins with, but is not limited to, symptoms. chronic stress (Durand et al., 2008), and notably
A further advance may crystallize upon joint considera- bvFTD (Finger, 2011). While not yet approved, multi-
tion of biobehavioral dimensions cutting across multi- ple pharmacological treatments could prove benefi-
ple levels of analysis. Neurocognitive states cannot be cial for social behavior in different conditions. The
properly conceived if framed separately from concomi- comparison of clinical outcomes, long-lasting and
tant socio-affective processes (Casey, Oliveri, & Insel, side effects, as well as other treatment-related vari-
2014; Insel, 2014). ables could provide a better understanding of the
benefits and generalizability of specific drugs for
social cognition improvement.
Renewing clinical assessment and professional
training
The challenges ahead: how to bring the
Social neuroscience implies a renewed approach to framework to institutional life
clinical assessment and professional training.
In the above sections, we have updated how social
Improvements in socio-cognitive performance and
neuroscience has an active impact in triangulating neu-
interaction are beneficial to patients’ life quality
rology and psychiatry. Nevertheless, the greatest long-
(Cacioppo, Capitanio, et al. 2014; Millan et al., 2012).
term challenge is how to formally implement this
By including social cognition assessments, clinical
approach. Bringing this framework to life in academic
practice would capitalize on an expert research area
and clinical settings calls for major institutional devel-
with major translational potential. To this end, clinical
opments. A typical problem for clinical training pro-
training should emphasize contributions from social
grams and professional practice is to establish a
neuroscience with a view to broadening current
particular assessment/evaluation/treatment for a speci-
assessment standards. In other words, to ensure excel-
fic disorder. Clinical, funding, and academic programs
lence in clinical practice, both neurologists and psy-
typically target highly specialized training, focus on a
chiatrists should be trained in a dimensional and
particular condition, and support only specific research
transdiagnostic approach to social cognition.
programs. This is inconsistent with current knowledge
Currently, residency programs in neuropsychiatry (in
of social impairments, as it calls for expertise in many
the U.S.A. and Europe) rarely include an in-depth study
conditions through theoretically heterogeneous train-
of social cognition in their curricula, especially during
ing. A new institutional rationale must be forged for
early stages of professional education. Training in dif-
this to happen. First, potential funding sources, both in
ferent social cognitive domains, including behavioral
the public and private sectors, should open their
assessment, knowledge of underlying neural
options for field-specific financing to dimensional psy-
dynamics, and current know-how from social neu-
chiatric and neurological projects. Funds made avail-
roscience is crucial to increase the awareness of social
able for psychiatric research, for instance, should not
cognition in neuropsychiatry and facilitate exchanges
be denied to viable projects on the grounds that they
among neurologists and psychiatrists. A common
include both neurological and psychiatric conditions.
approach to social neuroscience and neuropsychiatry
Second, faculties and departments specialized in either
may ameliorate the adverse effects of subspecializa-
discipline should be formally encouraged to incorpo-
tion and separation of clinical disciplines (Aminoff,
rate literature and courses aimed to show the continu-
2008).
ities among both types of dysfunction.
26 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

World experts must come together to produce Disclosure statement


groundbreaking journals and handbooks which foster
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
developments within a dimensional and integrative
social neuroscience approach to neuropsychiatry.
Clinical institutions should mirror such synergistic Funding
actions in their training programs and ongoing prac-
tices. Thus, a systematic triangulation can only become This work was supported by grants from CONICET, CONICYT/
concrete if major educational and political changes are FONDECYT Regular [1130920], FONDAP [15150012], FONCyT-
PICT [2012-0412], [2012-1309], and the INECO Foundation.
progressively made. Even if shared social cognition
impairments in neurological and psychiatric disorders
are already broadly acknowledged, implementing this ORCID
conception at an institutional level will constitute a
Agustín Ibáñez http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6758-5101
major challenge. The framework presently outlined
seeks to inspire more integrative training, funding,
and academic opportunities. References
Abu-Akel, A., Fischer-Shofty, M., Levkovitz, Y., Decety, J.,
Conclusions Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2014). The role of oxytocin in empathy
to the pain of conflictual out-group members among
Since the first descriptions of neurocognitive diseases in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 44(16),
ancient Egypt (Santoro et al., 2009) and pre-Hippocratic 3523–3532. doi:10.1017/S003329171400097X.
Greece (York & Steinberg, 2001), the history of nervous Adenzato, M., Cavallo, M., & Enrici, I. (2010). Theory of mind
diseases has been marked by exclusionary (sometimes ability in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal demen-
tia: an analysis of the neural, cognitive, and social levels.
indexed as biological or psychological) explanations Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 2–12. doi:10.1016/j.
(Barutta, Cornejo, & Ibáñez, 2011; Barutta, neuropsychologia.2009.08.001.
Gleichgerrcht, Cornejo, & Ibáñez, 2010; Mikulan, Allain, P., Gaura, V., Fasotti, L., Chauviré, V., Prundean, A.,
Reynaldo, & Ibanez, 2014). Current neuroscientific Sherer-Gagou, C., … Verny, C. (2011). The neural substrates
views partially circumvent this tradition but still face of script knowledge deficits as revealed by a PET study in
Huntington's disease. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2673–2684.
several difficulties to gain a deeper understanding of
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia
social cognition impairments. Social neuroscience offers Adolph, D., Meister, L., & Pause, B. M. (2013). Context counts!
a dimensional and transdiagnostic triangulation to social anxiety modulates the processing of fearful faces in
advance toward a better grasp of the impaired social the context of chemosensory anxiety signals. Frontiers in
brain. Human Neuroscience, 7, 283. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00283
Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expres-
sions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms.
Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(1), 21–62.
Review criteria
doi:10.1177/1534582302001001003
References for this Review were identified through Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social
searches of PubMed from 2000 until January 2015, knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693–716.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514
with the terms “social neuroscience”, “social cognition”, Adolphs, R., Nummenmaa, L., Todorov, A., & Haxby, J. V.
“emotion”, “facial emotions”, “empathy”, “theory of (2016). Data-driven approaches in the investigation of
mind”, “context/contextual and social”, and “psychia- social perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
try/psychiatric” or “neurology/neurological”. Articles Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1693), 20150367.
were also identified through searches of the authors’ doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0367
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2003). Dissociable
own files. Each hit was set aside for in-depth analysis.
neural systems for recognizing emotions. Brain and
Only papers published in English were reviewed. The Cognition, 52(1), 61–69. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00009-5
final reference list was generated on the basis of rele- Ahmed, F. S., & Stephen Miller, L. (2011). Executive function
vance to the topics covered in this Review. mechanisms of theory of mind. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 667–678. doi:10.1007/
s10803-010-1087-7
Acknowledgments Allison, T., Puce, A., Spencer, D. D., & McCarthy, G. (1999).
Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I:
This work was partially supported by grants from CONICET, Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face
CONICYT/FONDECYT Regular (1130920), FONDAP 15150012, and non-face stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 9(5), 415–430.
FONCyT-PICT 2012-0412, 2012-1309, and the INECO Foundation. doi:10.1093/cercor/9.5.415
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 27

Aminoff, M. J. (2008). Training in neurology. Neurology, 70(20), Baez, S., & Ibanez, A. (2014a). The effects of context processing
1912–1915. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000312287.53064.d2 on social cognition impairments in adults with Asperger’s
Amoruso, L., Cardona, J., Melloni, M., Sedeno, L., & Ibanez, A. syndrome. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 270. doi:10.3389/
(2012). Contextual impairments in Schizophrenia and the fnins.2014.00270
FN400. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 191. Baez, S., & Ibanez, A. (2014b). The effects of context proces-
doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00191 sing on social cognition impairments in adults with
Amoruso, L., Ibáñez, A., Fonseca, B., Gadea, S., Sedeño, L., Asperger’s syndrome. Front Neurosci, 8. doi:10.3389/
Sigman, M., . . . Fraiman, D. (2016). Variability in functional fnins.2014.00270
brain networks predicts expertise during action observa- Baez, S., Kanske, P., Matallana, D., Montanes, P., Reyes, P.,
tion. Neuroimage. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.041 Slachevsky, A., . . . Ibanez, A. (2015). Integration of intention
Amoruso, L., Sedeño, L., Huepe, D., Tomio, A., Kamienkowski, and outcome for moral judgment in frontotemporal
J., Hurtado, E., . . . Ibáñez, A. (2014). Time to Tango: Expertise dementia: Brain structural signatures. Neurodegenerative
and contextual anticipation during action observation. Diseases. doi:10.1159/000441918
Neuroimage, 98, 366–385. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage. Baez, S., Manes, F., Huepe, D., Torralva, T., Fiorentino, N.,
2014.05.005 Richter, F., . . . Ibanez, A. (2014). Primary empathy deficits
Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & in frontotemporal dementia. Frontiers in Aging
Damasio, A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral Neuroscience, 6, 262. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2014.00262
behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal Baez, S., Marengo, J., Perez, A., Huepe, D., Font, F. G., Rial, V., . . .
cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(9), 820–824. doi:10.1038/ Ibanez, A. (2014). Theory of mind and its relationship with
12194 executive functions and emotion recognition in borderline
Aoki, Y., Cortese, S., & Tansella, M. (2014). Neural bases of personality disorder. Journal of Neuropsychology.
atypical emotional face processing in autism: A meta-ana- doi:10.1111/jnp.12046
lysis of fMRI studies. The World Journal of Biological Baez, S., Morales, J. P., Slachevsky, A., Torralva, T., Matus, C.,
Psychiatry : the Official Journal of the World Federation of Manes, F., & Ibanez, A. (2016). Orbitofrontal and limbic
Societies of Biological Psychiatry, 1–10. doi:10.3109/ signatures of empathic concern and intentional harm in
15622975.2014.957719 the behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex,
Association, A. P. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of 75, 20–32. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.007
mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR) Baez, S., Rattazzi, A., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Torralva, T.,
(Vol. IV). Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Publishing. Vigliecca, N. S., Decety, J., . . . Ibanez, A. (2012). Integrating
Atkinson, A. P., & Adolphs, R. (2011). The neuropsychology of intention and context: Assessing social cognition in adults
face perception: Beyond simple dissociations and func- with Asperger syndrome. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
tional selectivity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal 6, 302. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00302
Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1571), 1726–1738. Baez, S., Santamaría-García, H., Orozco, J., Fittipaldi, S., García,
doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0349 A., Pino, M., & Ibáñez, A. (2016). Your misery is no longer
Aviezer, H. B. S., Hassin, R. R., Meschino, W., Kennedy, J., my pleasure: Reduced schadenfreude in Huntington’s dis-
Grewal, S., Esmail, E., . . . Moscovitch, H. (2009). Not on the ease families. Cortex, 83, 78–85. doi:10.1016/j.
face alone: Perception of contextualized face expressions in cortex.2016.07.009
Huntington’s disease. Brain, 132, 1633–1644. doi:10.1093/ Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van, I. J. M. H. (2013). Sniffing
brain/awp067 around oxytocin: Review and meta-analyses of trials in
Baez, S., Couto, B., Herrera, E., Bocanegra, Y., Trujillo-Orrego, healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharma-
N., Madrigal-Zapata, L., . . . Villegas, A. (2013). Tracking the cotherapy. Translational Psychiatry, 21(3), 34.
cognitive, social, and neuroanatomical profile in early neu- Bar, M. (2004). Visual objects in context. Nature Reviews.
rodegeneration: Type III cockayne syndrome. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5(8), 617–629. doi:10.1038/nrn1476
Aging Neuroscience, 5, 80. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2013.00080 Barcelo, F., & Knight, R. T. (2007). An information-theoretical
Baez, S., Couto, B., Torralva, T., Sposato, L. A., Huepe, D., approach to contextual processing in the human brain:
Montanes, P., . . . Ibanez, A. (2014). Comparing moral judg- Evidence from prefrontal lesions. Cerebral Cortex, 17 Suppl
ments of patients with frontotemporal dementia and fron- 1, i51–60.
tal stroke. JAMA Neurology, 71(9), 1172–1176. doi:10.1001/ Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The empathizing-systemizing
jamaneurol.2014.347 (E-S) theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
Baez, S., García, A. M., & Ibanez, A. (2016). The social context 1156, 68–80. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x
network model in psychiatric and neurological diseases. Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quo-
Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. doi:10.1007/ tient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or
7854_2016_443 high functioning autism, and normal sex differences.
Baez, S., Herrera, E., Gershanik, O., Garcia, A. M., Bocanegra, Y., Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163–
Kargieman, L., . . . Ibanez, A. (2015). Impairments in negative 175. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00
emotion recognition and empathy for pain in Huntington’s Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2007). Language
disease families. Neuropsychologia, 68, 158–167. as context for the perception of emotion. Trends in
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.012 Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 327–332. doi:10.1016/j.
Baez, S., Herrera, E., Villarin, L., Theil, D., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., tics.2007.06.003
Gomez, P., . . . Ibanez, A. (2013). Contextual social cognition Bartolomucci, A., Palanza, P., Sacerdote, P., Panerai, A. E.,
impairments in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. PLoS Sgoifo, A., Dantzer, R., & Parmigiani, S. (2005). Social factors
One, 8(3), e57664. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057664 and individual vulnerability to chronic stress exposure.
28 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(1), 67–81. International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 18(6), 972–
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.009 985. doi:10.1017/S1355617712001300
Barutta, J., Aravena, P., & Ibáñez, A. (2010). The machine Billeke, P., Armijo, A., Castillo, D., Lopez, T., Zamorano, F.,
paradigm and alternative approaches in cognitive science. Cosmelli, D., & Aboitiz, F. (2015). Paradoxical expectation:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44(2), 176– Oscillatory brain activity reveals social interaction impair-
183. doi:10.1007/s12124-010-9116-9 ment in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 78(6), 421–431.
Barutta, J., Cornejo, C., & Ibáñez, A. (2011). Theories and doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.012
theorizers: A contextual approach to theories of cognition. Blair, R. J. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 45(2), 223– mortality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57(1),
246. doi:10.1007/s12124-011-9156-9 1–29. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P
Barutta, J., Gleichgerrcht, E., Cornejo, C., & Ibáñez, A. (2010). Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2009). Theory of mind
Neurodynamics of mind: The arrow illusion of conscious impairment in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Schizophrenia
intentionality as downward causation. Integrative Research, 109(1–3), 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020
Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44(2), 127–143. Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of
doi:10.1007/s12124-010-9117-8 Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82(4),
Barutta, J., Hodges, J., Ibanez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., & Manes, F. 239–259. doi:10.1007/BF00308809
(2011). Argentina’s early contributions to the understand- Brambilla, P., Macdonald, A. W., III, Sassi, R. B., Johnson, M. K.,
ing of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cortex, 47(5), Mallinger, A. G., Carter, C. S., & Soares, J. C. (2007). Context
621–627. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.05.006 processing performance in bipolar disorder patients.
Beauchamp, M. H., Dooley, J. J., & Anderson, V. (2013). A Bipolar Disorders, 9(3), 230–237. doi:10.1111/j.1399-
preliminary investigation of moral reasoning and empa- 5618.2007.00398.x
thy after traumatic brain injury in adolescents. Brain Bruen, P. D., McGeown, W. J., Shanks, M. F., & Venneri, A.
Injury, 27(7–8), 896–902. doi:10.3109/02699052.2013. (2008). Neuroanatomical correlates of neuropsychiatric
775486 symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 131(9), 2455–
Bear, D. M. (1979). Temporal lobe epilepsy–a syndrome of 2463. doi:10.1093/brain/awn151
sensory-limbic hyperconnection. Cortex, 15(3), 357–84. Brugger, P., & Lenggenhager, B. (2014). The bodily self and its
Becchio, C., Sartori, L., & Castiello, U. (2010). Toward you: The disorders: Neurological, psychological and social aspects.
social side of actions. Current Directions in Psychological Current Opinion in Neurology, 27(6), 644–652. doi:10.1097/
Science, 19(3), 183–188. doi:10.1177/0963721410370131 WCO.0000000000000151
Bediou, B., Ryff, I., Mercier, B., Milliery, M., Hénaff, M. A., Bruijn, L. I., Miller, T. M., & Cleveland, D. W. (2004). Unraveling
D'Amato, T., … Krolak-Salmon, P. (2009). Impaired social the mechanisms involved in motor neuron degeneration in
cognition in mild Alzheimer disease. Journal of Geriatric ALS. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 723–749.
Psychiatry and Neurology, 22(2), 130–140. doi:10.1177/ doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144244
0891988709332939. Brune, M. (2005). “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: A review
Benedetti, F., Bernasconi, A., Bosia, M., Cavallaro, R., of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 21–42.
Dallaspezia, S., Falini, A., . . . Smeraldi, E. (2009). Functional doi:10.1093/schbul/sbi002
and structural brain correlates of theory of mind and empa- Brüne, M., Blank, K., Witthaus, H., & Saft, C. (2011). “Theory of
thy deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 114(1– mind” is impaired in Huntington's disease. Movement
3), 154–160. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.06.021 Disorders, 26(4), 671–678. doi:10.1002/mds.23494.
Benson, A. L. (1980). Morality of schizophrenic adolescents. Bruning, N., Konrad, K., & Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2005).
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89(5), 674–677. Relevance and results of theory of mind research for autism
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.89.5.674 and other psychiatric disorders. Zeitschrift Für Kinder- Und
Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An Jugendpsychiatrie Und Psychotherapie, 33(2), 77–88.
fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) doi:10.1024/1422-4917.33.2.77
violations of social norms. Brain, 125(8), 1696–1708. Buhlmann, U., McNally, R. J., Etcoff, N. L., Tuschen-Caffier, B., &
doi:10.1093/brain/awf190 Wilhelm, S. (2004). Emotion recognition deficits in body
Bertoux, M., De Souza, L. C., Sarazin, M., Funkiewiez, A., dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38(2),
Dubois, B., & Hornberger, M. (2014). How preserved is 201–206. doi:10.1016/S0022-3956(03)00107-9
emotion recognition in Alzheimer disease compared with Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks:
behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia? Alzheimer Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional sys-
Disease & Associated Disorders, 1. doi:10.1097/ tems. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10(3), 186–198.
WAD.0000000000000023 doi:10.1038/nrn2575
Bertoux, M., Volle, E., De Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Dubois, Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Volle, E., Benoit, R. G., & Gilbert, S.
B., & Habert, M. O. (2014). Neural correlates of the mini-SEA J. (2009). Mesulam’s frontal lobe mystery re-examined.
(Social cognition and Emotional Assessment) in behavioral Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27(5), 493–506.
variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain Imaging and doi:10.3233/RNN-2009-0511
Behavior, 8(1), 1–6. doi:10.1007/s11682-013-9261-0 Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cole, S. W.
Bertoux, M., Volle, E., Funkiewiez, A., De Souza, L. C., Leclercq, (2015). The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annual
D., & Dubois, B. (2012). Social Cognition and Emotional Review of Psychology, 66, 733–767. doi:10.1146/annurev-
Assessment (SEA) is a marker of medial and orbital frontal psych-010814-015240
functions: A voxel-based morphometry study in behavioral Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Dulawa, S., & Palmer, A. A. (2014).
variant of frontotemporal degeneration. Journal of the Social neuroscience and its potential contribution to
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 29

psychiatry. World Psychiatry, 13(2), 131–139. doi:10.1002/ Schizophrenia Research, 131(1–3), 235–241. doi:10.1016/j.
wps.20118 schres.2011.05.028
Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Toward a Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., & Di Pellegrino, G.
neurology of loneliness. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1464– (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment follow-
1504. doi:10.1037/a0037618 ing damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Young, A. W., Lawrence, A. D., Mason, S., Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 84–92.
& Barker, R. A. (2010). The relation between anger and doi:10.1093/scan/nsm001
different forms of disgust: Implications for emotion recog- Cohen, J. D., Barch, D. M., Carter, C., & Servan-Schreiber, D.
nition impairments in Huntington’s disease. (1999). Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia:
Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2719–2729. doi:10.1016/j. Converging evidence from three theoretically motivated
neuropsychologia.2010.05.019 cognitive tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(1),
Campagna, A. F., & Harter, S. (1975). Moral judgment in socio- 120–133. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.108.1.120
pathic and normal children. Journal of Personality and Social Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T.
Psychology, 31(2), 199–205. doi:10.1037/h0076318 (2011). Transcript origin analysis identifies antigen-present-
Canales-Johnson, A., Silva, C., Huepe, D., Rivera-Rei, Á., ing cells as primary targets of socially regulated gene
Noreika, V., Garcia, M. D. C., . . . Bekinschtein, T. A. (2015). expression in leukocytes. Proceedings of the National
Auditory feedback differentially modulates behavioral and Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3080–3085. doi:10.1073/
neural markers of objective and subjective performance pnas.1014218108
when tapping to your heartbeat. Cerebral Cortex (New Corbera, S., Ikezawa, S., Bell, M. D., Wexler, B. E. (2014).
York, N.Y. : 1991), 25, 4490–4503. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv076 Physiological evidence of a deficit to enhance the empathic
Cardona, J. F., Sinay, V., Amoruso, L., Hesse, E., Manes, F., & response in schizophrenia. European Psychiatry, 29(8), 463–
Ibanez, A. (2014). The impact of neuromyelitis optica on the 72. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.01.005.
recognition of emotional facial expressions: A preliminary Cosmelli, D., & Ibáñez, A. (2008). Human cognition in context:
report. Social Neuroscience, 9(6), 633–638. On the biologic, cognitive and social reconsideration of
Carmona-Perera, M., Clark, L., Young, L., Pérez-García, M., & meaning as making sense of action. Integrative
Verdejo-García, A. (2014). Impaired decoding of fear and Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42(2), 233–244.
disgust predicts utilitarian moral judgment in alcohol-depen- doi:10.1007/s12124-008-9060-0
dent individuals. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Couto, B., Adolfi, F., Sedeño, L., Salles, A., Canales-Johnson, A.,
Research, 38(1), 179–185. doi:10.1111/acer.12245 Alvarez-Abut, P., . . . Ibanez, A. (2015). Disentangling inter-
Carter, R., & Ffytche, D. H. (2015). On visual hallucinations and oception: Insights from focal strokes affecting the percep-
cortical networks: A trans-diagnostic review. Journal of tion of external and internal milieus. Frontiers in Psychology,
Neurology, 13, 13. 6(503). doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00503
Casey, B. J., Oliveri, M. E., & Insel, T. (2014). A neurodevelop- Couto, B., Adolfi, F., Velasquez, M., Mesow, M., Feinstein, J.,
mental perspective on the research domain criteria (RDoC) Canales-Johnson, A., . . . Ibanez, A. (2015). Heart evoked
framework. Biological Psychiatry, 76(5), 350–353. potential triggers brain responses to natural affective
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.006 scenes: A preliminary study. Autonomic Neuroscience, 193,
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Gene-environment interac- 132–137. doi:10.1016/j.autneu.2015.06.006
tions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Couto, B., Manes, F., Montañés, P., Matallana, D., Reyes, P.,
Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 7(7), 583–590. doi:10.1038/ Velasquez, M., . . . Ibáñez, A. (2013). Structural neuroima-
nrn1925 ging of social cognition in progressive non-fluent aphasia
Castelli, I., Pini, A., Alberoni, M., Liverta-Sempio, O., Baglio, F., and behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia.
Massaro, D., . . . Nemni, R. (2011). Mapping levels of theory Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 467. doi:10.3389/
of mind in Alzheimer’s disease: A preliminary study. Aging & fnhum.2013.00467
Mental Health, 15(2), 157–168. doi:10.1080/ Couto, B., Salles, A., Sedeño, L., Peradejordi, M., Barttfeld, P.,
13607863.2010.513038 Canales-Johnson, A., . . . Ibanez, A. (2014). The man who
Cerami, C., Dodich, A., Canessa, N., Crespi, C., Iannaccone, S., feels two hearts: The different pathways of interoception.
Corbo, M., . . . Cappa, S. F. (2014a). Emotional empathy in Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1253–1260.
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: A behavioural and voxel- doi:10.1093/scan/nst108
based morphometry study. Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Couto, B., Sedeño, L., Sposato, L. A., Sigman, M., Riccio, P. M.,
and Frontotemporal Degeneration, 15(1–2), 21–29. Salles, A., . . . Ibanez, A. (2013). Insular networks for emo-
doi:10.3109/21678421.2013.785568 tional processing and social cognition: Comparison of two
Cerami, C., Dodich, A., Canessa, N., Crespi, C., Marcone, A., case reports with either cortical or subcortical involvement.
Cortese, F., . . . Cappa, S. F. (2014b). Neural correlates of Cortex, 49(5), 1420–1434. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.006
empathic impairment in the behavioral variant of fronto- Cowan, W. M., Harter, D. H., & Kandel, E. R. (2000). The emer-
temporal dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10, 827–834. gence of modern neuroscience: Some implications for neu-
doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.005 rology and psychiatry. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23,
Chen, Y. C., Chen, C. C., Decety, J., & Cheng, Y. (2014). Aging is 343–391. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.343
associated with changes in the neural circuits underlying Crespi, C., Cerami, C., Dodich, A., Canessa, N., Arpone, M.,
empathy. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(4), 827–836. Iannaccone, S., . . . Cappa, S. F. (2014). Microstructural
doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.080 white matter correlates of emotion recognition impairment
Chung, Y. S., & Barch, D. M. (2011). The effect of emotional in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Cortex, 53, 1–8.
context on facial emotion ratings in schizophrenia. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.002
30 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Cryan, J. F., & Kaupmann, K. (2005). Don’t worry ‘B’happy!: A Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Voss, B., Eickhoff, S. B., Kellermann,
role for GABA B receptors in anxiety and depression. Trends T., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). Neural correlates of the
in Pharmacological Sciences, 26(1), 36–43. doi:10.1016/j. core facets of empathy in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
tips.2004.11.004 Research, 136(1–3), 70–81. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.12.018
Cusi, A., Macqueen, G. M., & McKinnon, M. C. (2010). Altered Derntl, B., Seidel, E.-M., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). How
self-report of empathic responding in patients with bipolar specific are emotional deficits? A comparison of empathic
disorder. Psychiatry Research, 178(2), 354–358. doi:10.1016/j. abilities in schizophrenia, bipolar and depressed patients.
psychres.2009.07.009 Schizophrenia Research, 142(1–3), 58–64. doi:10.1016/j.
Damasio, A. R. (1998). 2 - Signs of aphasia. In M. T. Sarno (Ed.), schres.2012.09.020
Acquired aphasia (3rd ed., pp. 25–41). San Diego: Academic Devor, A., Bandettini, P. A., Boas, D. A., Bower, J. M., Buxton, R.
Press. B., Cohen, L. B., . . . Yodh, A. G. (2013). The challenge of
Das, P., Lagopoulos, J., Coulston, C. M., Henderson, A. F., & connecting the dots in the B.R.A.I.N. Neuron, 80(2), 270–274.
Malhi, G. S. (2012). Mentalizing impairment in schizophre- doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.008
nia: A functional MRI study. Schizophrenia Research, 134(2– Di Martino, A., Ross, K., Uddin, L. Q., Sklar, A. B., Castellanos, F.
3), 158–164. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.019. X., & Milham, M. P. (2009). Functional brain correlates of
Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., & Putnam, K. social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disor-
(2002). Depression: Perspectives from affective neu- ders: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis.
roscience. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 545–574. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 63–74. doi:10.1016/j.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135148 biopsych.2008.09.022
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Dickerson, B. C. (2015). Dysfunction of social cognition and
Understanding the nature of face processing impairment behavior. Continuum (Minneap Minn), 21(3 Behavioral
in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiological Neurology and Neuropsychiatry), 660–677. doi:10.1212/01.
studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 403–424. CON.0000466659.05156.1d
doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2703_6 Dickinson, D., & Harvey, P. D. (2009). Systemic hypotheses for
de Achával, D., Villarreal, M. F., Salles, A., Bertomeu, M. J., generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: A new take
Costanzo, E. Y., Goldschmidt, M., … Guinjoan, S. M. (2013). on an old problem. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(2), 403–414.
Activation of brain areas concerned with social cognition doi:10.1093/schbul/sbn097
during moral decisions is abnormal in schizophrenia Dirnberger, G., & Jahanshahi, M. (2013). Executive dysfunction in
patients and unaffected siblings. Journal of Psychiatric Parkinson’s disease: A review. Journal of Neuropsychology, 7
Research, 47(6), 774–782. doi:10.1016/j. (2), 193–224. doi:10.1111/jnp.2013.7.issue-2
jpsychires.2012.12.018. Djukic, A., Rose, S. A., Jankowski, J. J., & Feldman, J. F. (2014).
De Renzi, E. (1986). Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT Rett syndrome: Recognition of facial expression and its
scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere. relation to scanning patterns. Pediatric Neurology, 51, 650–
Neuropsychologia, 24(3), 385–389. doi:10.1016/0028-3932 656. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.07.022
(86)90023-0 Driscoll, D. M., Dal Monte, O., Solomon, J., Krueger, F.,
De Renzi, E, Perani, D., Carlesimo, G. A., Silveri, M. C., Fazio, F. Grafman, J. (2012). Empathic deficits in combat veterans
(1994). Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage with traumatic brain injury: Avoxel-based lesion-symptom
confined to the right hemisphere–an MRI and PET study mapping study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 25(4),
and a review of the literature. Neuropsychologia, 32(8), 160–166. doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e318280cf4e.
893–902. Donix, M., Jurjanz, L., Meyer, S., Amanatidis, E. C., Baeumler, D.,
Decety, J., & Howard, L. H. (2013). The role of affect in the Huebner, T., … Holthoff, V. A. (2013). Functional imaging
neurodevelopment of morality. Child Development during recognition of personally familiar faces and places in
Perspectives, 7, 49–54. doi:10.1111/cdep.12020 Alzheimer's disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture (1), 72–80. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs093.
of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Dubois, J., & Adolphs, R. (2016). Building a science of indivi-
Reviews, 3(2), 71–100. doi:10.1177/1534582304267187 dual differences from fMRI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20
Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the (6), 425–443. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.014
lens of social neuroscience. The Scientific World Journal, 6, Durand, D., Pampillo, M., Caruso, C., & Lasaga, M. (2008). Role
1146–1163. doi:10.1100/tsw.2006.221 of metabotropic glutamate receptors in the control of neu-
Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., & Kinzler, K. D. (2012). The con- roendocrine function. Neuropharmacology, 55(4), 577–583.
tribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: A doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.022
neurodevelopmental study. Cerebral Cortex, 22(1), 209–220. Duval, C., Bejanin, A., Piolino, P., Laisney, M., De La Sayette, V.,
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr111 Belliard, S., . . . Desgranges, B. (2012). Theory of mind impair-
Dere, E., Pause, B. M., & Pietrowsky, R. (2010). Emotion and ments in patients with semantic dementia. Brain, 135(1),
episodic memory in neuropsychiatric disorders. Behavioural 228–241. doi:10.1093/brain/awr309
Brain Research, 215(2), 162–171. doi:10.1016/j. Dwyer, K., Peters, E., McKenna, P., David, A., & McCarthy, R.
bbr.2010.03.017 (2014). Verbatim recall in formal thought disorder in schi-
Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Toygar, T. K., Hülsmann, A., zophrenia: A study of contextual influences. Cognitive
Schneider, F., Falkenberg, D. I., & Habel, U. (2009). Neuropsychiatry, 19(4), 337–358. doi:10.1080/
Generalized deficit in all core components of empathy in 13546805.2013.870069.
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 108(1–3), 197–206. Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H.
doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.009 R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 31

and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome Fiacconi, C. M., Barkley, V., Finger, E. C., Carson, N., Duke, D.,
using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Rosenbaum, R. S., . . . KÃhler, S. (2014). Nature and extent of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 464–473. person recognition impairments associated with Capgras
doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x syndrome in Lewy body dementia. Frontiers in Human
Eddy, C. M., & Rickards, H. E. (2015). Cognitive deficits predict Neuroscience, 8, 726. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00726
poorer functional capacity in Huntington's disease: but Finger, E. C. (2011). New potential therapeutic approaches in
what is being measured? Neuropsychology, 29(2), 268–273. frontotemporal dementia: Oxytocin, vasopressin, and social
doi:10.1037/neu0000134. cognition. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience : MN, 45(3),
Editorial, N. N. (2012). Focus on social neuroscience. Nature 696–701. doi:10.1007/s12031-011-9550-2
Neuroscience, 15(5), 645. doi:10.1038/nn0512-645 Fogelson, N., Li, L., Li, Y., Fernandez-Del-Olmo, M., Santos-
Ellis, H. D., De Pauw, K. W., Christodoulou, G. N., Papageorgiou, Garcia, D., & Peled, A. (2013). Functional connectivity
L., Milne, A. B., & Joseph, A. B. (1993). Responses to facial abnormalities during contextual processing in schizophre-
and non-facial stimuli presented tachistoscopically in either nia and in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Cognition, 82(3),
or both visual fields by patients with the Capgras delusion 243–253. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.001.
and paranoid schizophrenics. Journal of Neurology, Fogelson, N., Ribolsi, M., Fernandez-Del-Olmo, M., Rubino, I. A.,
Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 56(2), 215–219. doi:10.1136/ Romeo, D., Koch, G., & Peled, A. (2011). Neural correlates of
jnnp.56.2.215 local contextual processing deficits in schizophrenic
Ellis, H. D., Young, A. W., Quayle, A. H., & De Pauw, K. W. patients. Psychophysiology, 48(9), 1217–1226. doi:10.1111/
(1997). Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras j.1469-8986.2011.01195.x.
delusion. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Fogelson, N., Shah, M., Scabini, D., & Knight, R. T. (2009).
Sciences, 264(1384), 1085–1092. doi:10.1098/ Prefrontal cortex is critical for contextual processing:
rspb.1997.0150 Evidence from brain lesions. Brain, 132(11), 3002–3010.
Epa, R., Czyzowska, N., Dudek, D., Siwek, M., & Gierowski, J. K. doi:10.1093/brain/awp230
(2014). Profile of moral reasoning in persons with bipolar Fonville, L., Giampietro, V., Surguladze, S., Williams, S., &
affective disorder. Polish Psychiatry, 48(3), 489–502. Tchanturia, K. (2014). Increased BOLD signal in the fusiform
Escobar, M. J., Huepe, D., Decety, J., Sedeño, L., Messow, M. K., gyrus during implicit emotion processing in anorexia ner-
Baez, S., . . . Ibánez, A. (2014). Brain signatures of moral vosa. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 266–273. doi:10.1016/j.
sensitivity in adolescents with early social deprivation. nicl.2013.12.002
Scientific Reports, 4, 5354. doi:10.1038/srep05354 Francis, S. M., Sagar, A., Levin-Decanini, T., Liu, W., Carter, C. S.,
Escobar, M. J., Rivera-Rei, A., Decety, J., Huepe, D., Cardona, J. & Jacob, S. (2014). Oxytocin and vasopressin systems in
F., Canales-Johnson, A., . . . García, A. V. (2013). Attachment genetic syndromes and neurodevelopmental disorders.
patterns trigger differential neural signature of emotional Brain Research, 1580, 199–218. doi:10.1016/j.
processing in adolescents. PLoS One, 8(8), e70247. brainres.2014.01.021
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0070247 Franklin, S. A., McNally, R. J., & Riemann, B. C. (2009). Moral
Eslinger, P. J., Moore, P., Anderson, C., & Grossman, M. (2011). reasoning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of
Social cognition, executive functioning, and neuroimaging Anxiety Disorders, 23(5), 575–577. doi:10.1016/j.
correlates of empathic deficits in frontotemporal dementia. janxdis.2008.11.005
Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 23(1), 74–82. doi:10.1176/appi. Friston, K. J. (2012). The history of the future of the Bayesian
neuropsych.23.1.74 brain. Neuroimage, 62(2), 1230–1233. doi:10.1016/j.
Fakra, E., Salgado-Pineda, P., Delaveau, P., Hariri, A. R., & Blin, O. neuroimage.2011.10.004
(2008). Neural bases of different cognitive strategies for facial Fujino, J., Yamasaki, N., Miyata, J., Kawada, R., Sasaki, H.,
affect processing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, Matsukawa, N., … Murai, T. (2014). Altered brain response
100(1–3), 191–205. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.040 to others‫ ׳‬pain in major depressive disorder. Journal of
Falquez, R., Couto, B., Ibanez, A., Freitag, M. T., Berger, M., Affective Disorders, 165, 170–175. doi:10.1016/j.
Arens, E. A., . . . Barnow, S. (2014). Detaching from the jad.2014.04.058
negative by reappraisal: The role of right superior frontal Fujiwara, H., Yassin, W., & Murai, T. (2014). Neuroimaging
gyrus (BA9/32). Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 165. studies of social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychiatry and
doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00165 Clinical Neurosciences. doi:10.1111/pcn.12258
Fernandez-Duque, D., & Black, S. E. (2005). Impaired recogni- García, A., & Ibáñez, A. (2016). A touch with words: Dynamic
tion of negative facial emotions in patients with frontotem- synergies between manual actions and language.
poral dementia. Neuropsychologia, 43(11), 1673–1687. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 68, 59–95.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.005 doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.022
Fett, A. K., Shergill, S. S., & Krabbendam, L. (2015). Social García, A. M., & Ibáñez, A. (2014). Two-person neuroscience
neuroscience in psychiatry: Unravelling the neural mechan- and naturalistic social communication: The role of language
isms of social dysfunction. Psychological Medicine, 45(6), and linguistic variables in brain-coupling research. Frontiers
1145–1165. doi:10.1017/s0033291714002487 in Psychiatry, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00124
Feuerriegel, D., Churches, O., Hofmann, J., & Keage, H. A. García-Cordero, I., Sedeño, L., De La Fuente, L., Slachevsky, A.,
(2014). The N170 and face perception in psychiatric and Forno, G., Klein, F., . . . Ibañez, A. (2016). Feeling, learning
neurological disorders: A systematic review. Clinical from, and being aware of inner states: Interoceptive dimen-
Neurophysiology : Official Journal of the International sions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Philosophical
Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. doi:10.1016/j. Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B,
clinph.2014.09.015 Biological Sciences. doi:10.1098/rstb.2016-0006
32 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

García-Cordero, I., Sedeño, L., Fraiman, D., Craiem, D., De La Green, M. J., Waldron, J. H., Simpson, I., & Coltheart, M. (2008).
Fuente, L. A., Salamone, P., . . . Ibañez, A. (2015). Stroke and Visual processing of social context during mental state
neurodegeneration induce different connectivity aberra- perception in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry &
tions in the insula. Stroke, 46(9), 2673–2677. doi:10.1161/ Neuroscience : JPN, 33(1), 34–42.
STROKEAHA.115.009598 Greene, A. J., Gross, W. L., Elsinger, C. L., & Rao, S. M. (2006). An
Garfinkel, S. N., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Interoception, emo- FMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: Inference, con-
tion and brain: New insights link internal physiology to text, and task awareness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
social behaviour. Commentary on:: “Anterior insular cortex 18(7), 1156–1173. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1156
mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety” by Terasawa Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., &
et al. (2012). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8 Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict
(3), 231–234. doi:10.1093/scan/nss140 and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389–400.
Geschwind, D. H. (2009). Advances in autism. Annual Review of doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027
Medicine, 60, 367–380. doi:10.1146/annurev. Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., &
med.60.053107.121225 Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional
Gleichgerrcht, E., Ibanez, A., Roca, M., Torralva, T., & Manes, F. engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105–
(2010). Decision-making cognition in neurodegenerative 2108. doi:10.1126/science.1062872
diseases. Nature Reviews. Neurology, 6(11), 611–623. Gregory, C., Lough, S., Stone, V., Erzinclioglu, S., Martin, L.,
doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2010.148 Baron-Cohen, S., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Theory of mind in
Gleichgerrcht, E., Torralva, T., Rattazzi, A., Marenco, V., Roca, patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and
M., & Manes, F. (2013). Selective impairment of cognitive Alzheimer’s disease: Theoretical and practical implications.
empathy for moral judgment in adults with high function- Brain, 125(4), 752–764. doi:10.1093/brain/awf079
ing autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), Guerra, S., Ibanez, A., Martin, M., Bobes, M. A., Reyes, A.,
780–788. doi:10.1093/scan/nss067 Mendoza, R., . . . Sosa, M. V. (2009). N400 deficits from
Gomez-Ibanez, A., Urrestarazu, E., & Viteri, C. (2014). semantic matching of pictures in probands and first-degree
Recognition of facial emotions and identity in patients relatives from multiplex schizophrenia families. Brain
with mesial temporal lobe and idiopathic generalized epi- and Cognition, 70(2), 221–230. doi:10.1016/j.
lepsy: An eye-tracking study. Seizure, 23, 892–898. bandc.2009.02.004
doi:10.1016/j.seizure.2014.08.012 Gur, R. E., Loughead, J., Kohler, C. G., Elliott, M. A., Lesko, K.,
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Baez, S., Torralva, T., Castellanos, F. X., Ruparel, K., . . . Gur, R. C. (2007). Limbic activation associated
Rattazzi, A., Bein, V., . . . Ibanez, A. (2013). Cognitive varia- with misidentification of fearful faces and flat affect in
bility in adults with ADHD and AS: Disentangling the roles schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(12), 1356–
of executive functions and social cognition. Research in 1366. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1356
Developmental Disabilities, 34(2), 817–830. doi:10.1016/j. Habel, U., Chechko, N., Pauly, K., Koch, K., Backes, V., Seiferth,
ridd.2012.11.009 N., . . . Kellermann, T. (2010). Neural correlates of emotion
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Chennu, S., Bekinschtein, T. A., Rattazzi, recognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 122
A., Beraudi, A., Tripicchio, P., . . . Ibanez, A. (2015). Predictive (1–3), 113–123. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.009
coding in autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit Hansen, S. (2011). Inhibitory control and empathy-related
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(5), personality traits: Sex-linked associations. Brain and
2625–2636. doi:10.1152/jn.00543.2015 Cognition, 76(3), 364–368. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.04.004
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Herrera, E., Parra, M., Gomez Mendez, Harrington, L., Siegert, R. J., & McClure, J. (2005). Theory of mind
P., Baez, S., Manes, F., & Ibanez, A. (2014). Emotion recogni- in schizophrenia: A critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry,
tion and cognitive empathy deficits in adolescent offenders 10(4), 249–286. doi:10.1080/13546800444000056
revealed by context-sensitive tasks. Frontiers in Human Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., &
Neuroscience, 8, 850. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00850 Keysers, C. (2012). Brain-to-brain coupling: A mechanism
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Ibanez, A., Damm, J., Ramirez Romero, for creating and sharing a social world. Trends in Cognitive
D. A., Abrevaya, S., Manes, F., . . . Roca, M. (2014). Different Sciences, 16(2), 114–121. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.007
levels of implicit emotional recognition in posterior cortical Hennion, S., Delbeuck, X., Duhamel, A., Lopes, R., Semah, F.,
atrophy (PCA). Neurocase, 1–8. doi:10.1080/ Tyvaert, L., . . . Szurhaj, W. (2014). Characterization and pre-
13554794.2014.919325 diction of theory of mind disorders in temporal lobe epi-
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Sigman, M., Rattazzi, A., Lavin, C., Rivera- lepsy. Neuropsychology. doi:10.1037/neu0000126
Rei, A., Marino, J., . . . Ibanez, A. (2016). Neural markers of Henry, A., Tourbah, A., Chaunu, M. P., Rumbach, L., Montreuil,
social and monetary rewards in children with Attention- M., & Bakchine, S. (2011). Social cognition impairments in
Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Journal of the
Scientific Reports, 6, 30588. doi:10.1038/srep30588 International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 17(6), 1122–
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Tripicchio, P., Rattazzi, A., Baez, S., Marino, 1131. doi:10.1017/S1355617711001147
J., Roca, M., . . . Ibanez, A. (2014). Inter-individual cognitive Henry, J. D., Cowan, D. G., Lee, T., & Sachdev, P. (2015). A
variability in children with Asperger’s syndrome. Frontiers in review of different methods of assessing social cognitive
Human Neuroscience, 8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00575 function in clinical settings. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 1.
Green, M. J., Waldron, J. H., & Coltheart, M. (2007). Emotional doi:10.1097/yco.0000000000000139
context processing is impaired in schizophrenia. Cognitive Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Crawford, J. R., Ietswaart, M., &
Neuropsychiatry, 12(3), 259–280. doi:10.1080/ Summers, F. (2006). Theory of mind following traumatic
13546800601051847 brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 33

dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1623–1628. Ibanez, A., Kotz, S. A., Barrett, L., Moll, J., & Ruz, M. (2014).
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.020 Situated affective and social neuroscience. Frontiers in
Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., & Von Hippel, C. (2014). A meta- Human Neuroscience, 8, 547. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00547
analytic review of theory of mind difficulties in behavioural- Ibanez, A., Kuljis, R. O., Matallana, D., & Manes, F. (2014).
variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia, 56, 53– Bridging psychiatry and neurology through social neu-
62. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.024 roscience. World Psychiatry, 13(2), 148–149. doi:10.1002/
Hesse, E., Mikulan, E., Decety, J., Sigman, M., Garcia, M. D. C., wps.20125
Silva, W., . . . Ibanez, A. (2016). Early detection of intentional Ibanez, A., & Manes, F. (2012). Contextual social cognition and
harm in the human amygdala. Brain, 139(Pt 1), 54–61. the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia.
doi:10.1093/brain/awv336 Neurology, 78(17), 1354–1362. doi:10.1212/
Hezel, D. M., & McNally, R. J. (2014). Theory of mind impair- WNL.0b013e3182518375
ments in social anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy, 45(4), Ibanez, A., Melloni, M., Huepe, D., Helgiu, E., Rivera-Rei, A.,
530–540. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.010 Canales-Johnson, A., . . . Moya, A. (2012). What event-related
Hot, P., Klein-Koerkamp, Y., Borg, C., Richard-Mornas, A., potentials (ERPs) bring to social neuroscience? Social
Zsoldos, I., Paignon Adeline, A., Thomas Antérion, C., Neuroscience, 7(6), 632–649. doi:10.1080/
Baciu, M. (2013). Fear recognition impairment in early- 17470919.2012.691078
stage Alzheimer's disease: When focusing on the eyes Ibáñez, A., Petroni, A., Urquina, H., Torrente, F., Torralva, T.,
region improves performance. Brain and Cognition, 82(1), Hurtado, E., . . . Manes, F. (2011). Cortical deficits of emo-
25–34. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.02.001. tional face processing in adults with ADHD: Its relation to
Horinek, D., Varjassyova, A., & Hort, J. (2007). Magnetic reso- social cognition and executive function. Social
nance analysis of amygdalar volume in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience, 6(5–6), 464–481. doi:10.1080/
Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20(3), 273–277. doi:10.1097/ 17470919.2011.620769
YCO.0b013e3280ebb613 Ibáñez, A., Riveros, R., Aravena, P., Vergara, V., Cardona, J. F.,
Huang, S. F., Oi, M., & Taguchi, A. (2015). Comprehension of García, L., . . . Manes, F. (2011). When context is difficult to
figurative language in Taiwanese children with autism: The integrate: Cortical measures of congruency in schizophre-
role of theory of mind and receptive vocabulary. Clinical nics and healthy relatives from multiplex families.
Linguistics & Phonetics, 24, 1–12. Schizophrenia Research, 126(1–3), 303–305. doi:10.1016/j.
Huepe, D., Riveros, R., Manes, F., Couto, B., Hurtado, E., schres.2010.04.008
Cetkovich, M., . . . Ibanez, A. (2012). The relationship of Ibanez, A., Riveros, R., Hurtado, E., Gleichgerrcht, E., Urquina,
clinical, cognitive and social measures in schizophrenia: A H., Herrera, E., . . . Manes, F. (2012). The face and its emotion:
preliminary finding combining measures in probands and Right N170 deficits in structural processing and early emo-
relatives. Behavioural Neurology, 25(2), 137–150. tional discrimination in schizophrenic patients and rela-
doi:10.1155/2012/194159 tives. Psychiatry Research, 195(1–2), 18–26. doi:10.1016/j.
Hyman, S. E. (2007). Can neuroscience be integrated into the psychres.2011.07.027
DSM-V? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 8(9), 725–732. Ibanez, A., Urquina, H., Petroni, A., Baez, S., Lopez, V., Do
doi:10.1038/nrn2218 Nascimento, M., . . . Baune, B. T. (2012). Neural processing
Ibanez, A., Aguado, J., Baez, S., Huepe, D., Lopez, V., Ortega, R., of emotional facial and semantic expressions in euthymic
. . . Manes, F. (2014). From neural signatures of emotional bipolar disorder (BD) and its association with theory of
modulation to social cognition: Individual differences in mind (ToM). PLoS One, 7(10), e46877. doi:10.1371/journal.
healthy volunteers and psychiatric participants. Social pone.0046877
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(7), 939–950. Ibanez, A., Velasquez, M. M., Caro, M. M., & Manes, F. (2013).
doi:10.1093/scan/nst067 Implicit emotional awareness in frontotemporal dementia.
Ibanez, A., & Cosmelli, D. (2008). Moving beyond computa- Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(3–4), 204–206. doi:10.1080/
tional cognitivism: Understanding intentionality, intersub- 17588928.2013.854756
jectivity and ecology of mind. Integrative Psychological and Insel, T. R. (2014). The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC)
Behavioral Science, 42(2), 129–136. doi:10.1007/s12124-007- Project: Precision medicine for psychiatry. American Journal
9045-4 of Psychiatry, 171(4), 395–397. doi:10.1176/appi.
Ibáñez, A., Gleichgerrcht, E., Hurtado, E., González, R., Haye, A., ajp.2014.14020138
& Manes, F. (2010). Early neural markers of implicit atti- Insel, T. R., & Quirion, R. (2005). Psychiatry as a clinical neu-
tudes: N170 modulated by intergroup and evaluative con- roscience discipline. Jama, 294(17), 2221–2224. doi:10.1001/
texts in IAT. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 188. jama.294.17.2221
doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00188 Irwin, M. R., & Cole, S. W. (2011). Reciprocal regulation of the
Ibanez, A., Huepe, D., Gempp, R., Gutiérrez, V., Rivera-Rei, A., & neural and innate immune systems. Nature Reviews.
Toledo, M. I. (2013). Empathy, sex and fluid intelligence as Immunology, 11(9), 625–632. doi:10.1038/nri3042
predictors of theory of mind. Personality and Individual Jablensky, A. (2005). Categories, dimensions and prototypes:
Differences, 54(5), 616–621. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.022 Critical issues for psychiatric classification. Psychopathology,
Ibáñez, A., Hurtado, E., Riveros, R., Urquina, H., Cardona, J. F., 38(4), 201–205. doi:10.1159/000086092
Petroni, A., . . . Manes, F. (2011). Facial and semantic emotional Jackson, P. L., Rainville, P., & Decety, J. (2006). To what extent
interference: A pilot study on the behavioral and cortical do we share the pain of others? Insight from the neural
responses to the dual valence association task. Behavioral bases of pain empathy. Pain, 125(1), 5–9. doi:10.1016/j.
and Brain Functions, 7(1), 8. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-7-8 pain.2006.09.013
34 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Jiang, Y., Hu, Y., Wang, Y., Zhou, N., Zhu, L., & Wang, K. (2014). Krupp, L. B., Alvarez, L. A., LaRocca, N. G., & Scheinberg, L. C.
Empathy and emotion recognition in patients with idio- (1988). Fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Archives of Neurology,
pathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 37, 139– 45(4), 435–437. doi:10.1001/archneur.1988.0052028
144. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.06.005 0085020
Johnson, S. A., Stout, J. C., Solomon, A. C., Langbehn, D. R., Krystal, J., Sanacora, G., Blumberg, H., Anand, A., Charney, D.,
Aylward, E. H., Cruce, C. B., . . . Paulsen, J. S. (2007). Beyond Marek, G., . . . Mason, G. F. (2002). Glutamate and GABA
disgust: Impaired recognition of negative emotions prior to systems as targets for novel antidepressant and mood-
diagnosis in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 130(7), 1732–1744. stabilizing treatments. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, S71–S80.
doi:10.1093/brain/awm107 doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001021
Kang, J. I., Namkoong, K., Yoo, S. W., Jhung, K., & Kim, S. J. Kumfor, F., Irish, M., Hodges, J. R., Piguet, O., & Kilner, J. (2013).
(2012). Abnormalities of emotional awareness and percep- Discrete Neural Correlates for the Recognition of Negative
tion in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal Emotions: Insights from Frontotemporal Dementia. PLoS
of Affective Disorders, 141(2–3), 286–293. doi:10.1016/j. One, 8(6), e67457. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0067457
jad.2012.04.001 Kumfor, F., Miller, L., Lah, S., Hsieh, S., Savage, S., Hodges, J. R.,
Karelina, K., Stuller, K. A., Jarrett, B., Zhang, N., Wells, J., & Piguet, O. (2011). Are you really angry? The effect of
Norman, G. J., & DeVries, A. C. (2011). Oxytocin mediates intensity on facial emotion recognition in frontotemporal
social neuroprotection after cerebral ischemia. Stroke, 42 dementia. Social Neuroscience, 6(5–6), 502–514.
(12), 3606–3611. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.628008 doi:10.1080/17470919.2011.620779
Kendler, K. S. (1990). Toward a scientific psychiatric nosology. Kumfor, F., & Piguet, O. (2013). Emotion recognition in the
Strengths and limitations. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47 dementias: Brain correlates and patient implications.
(10), 969–973. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810220085011 Neurodegenerative Disease Management, 3(3), 277–288.
Kennedy, D. P., & Adolphs, R. (2012). The social brain in doi:10.2217/nmt.13.16
psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends in Cognitive Kumfor, F., Sapey-Triomphe, L.-A., Leyton, C. E., Burrell, J. R.,
Sciences, 16(11), 559–572. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006 Hodges, J. R., & Piguet, O. (2014). Degradation of emotion
Key, A. P., Jones, D., & Dykens, E. M. (2013). Social and emo- processing ability in corticobasal syndrome and Alzheimer’s
tional processing in Prader-Willi syndrome: Genetic subtype disease. Brain, 137(11), 3061–3072. doi:10.1093/brain/
differences. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 5(1), 7. awu246
doi:10.1186/1866-1955-5-7 Lavenu, I., & Pasquier, F. (2005). Perception of emotion on
Khemiri, L., Guterstam, J., Franck, J., Jayaram-Lindström, N., & faces in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease:
García, A. V. (2012). Alcohol dependence associated with A longitudinal study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive
increased utilitarian moral judgment: A case control study. Disorders, 19(1), 37–41. doi:10.1159/000080969
PLoS One, 7(6), e39882. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039882 Le Bouc, R., Lenfant, P., Delbeuck, X., Ravasi, L., Lebert, F.,
Kipps, C. M., Nestor, P. J., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Arnold, R., & Semah, F., & Pasquier, F. (2012). My belief or yours?
Hodges, J. R. (2009). Understanding social dysfunction in Differential theory of mind deficits in frontotemporal
the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: The dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 135(10), 3026–
role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain, 132(3), 592– 3038. doi:10.1093/brain/aws237
603. doi:10.1093/brain/awn314 Lee, S. J., Kang Do, H., Kim, C.-W., Gu, B. M., Park, J.-Y., Choi, C.-
Klein-Koerkamp, Y., Beaudoin, M., Baciu, M., & Hot, P. (2012). H., . . . Kwon, J. S. (2010). Multi-level comparison of empathy
Emotional decoding abilities in Alzheimer’s disease: A in schizophrenia: An fMRI study of a cartoon task. Psychiatry
meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease : JAD, 32(1), Research: Neuroimaging, 181(2), 121–129. doi:10.1016/j.
109–125. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-120553 pscychresns.2009.08.003
Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., & Newman, J. P. (2012). Leigh, R., Oishi, K., Hsu, J., Lindquist, M., Gottesman, R. F.,
Utilitarian moral judgment in psychopathy. Social Cognitive Jarso, S., . . . Hillis, A. E. (2013). Acute lesions that impair
and Affective Neuroscience, 7(6), 708–714. doi:10.1093/scan/ affective empathy. Brain, 136(8), 2539–2549. doi:10.1093/
nsr048 brain/awt177
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Levenson, R. W., Sturm, V. E., & Haase, C. M. (2014). Emotional
Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal and behavioral symptoms in neurodegenerative disease: A
cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446 model for studying the neural bases of psychopathology.
(7138), 908–911. doi:10.1038/nature05631 Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 581–606.
Kohler, C. G., Martin, E. A., Stolar, N., Barrett, F. S., Verma, R., doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153653
Brensinger, C., . . . Gur, R. C. (2008). Static posed and evoked Linden, S. C., Jackson, M. C., Subramanian, L., Wolf, C., Green,
facial expressions of emotions in schizophrenia. P., Healy, D., & Linden, D. E. J. (2010). Emotion-cognition
Schizophrenia Research, 105(1–3), 49–60. doi:10.1016/j. interactions in schizophrenia: Implicit and explicit effects of
schres.2008.05.010 facial expression. Neuropsychologia, 48(4), 997–1002.
Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry of addiction. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.023
Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 217–238. doi:10.1038/ Liu, T., & Pelowski, M. (2014). Clarifying the interaction types in
npp.2009.110 two-person neuroscience research. Frontiers in Human
Kraemer, M., Herold, M., Uekermann, J., Kis, B., Wiltfang, J., Neuroscience, 8. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00276
Daum, I., . . . Abdel-Hamid, M. (2013). Theory of mind and Liu, T. T., & Behrmann, M. (2014). Impaired holistic processing
empathy in patients at an early stage of relapsing remitting of left-right composite faces in congenital prosopagnosia.
multiple sclerosis. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 115 Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 750. doi:10.3389/
(7), 1016–1022. doi:10.1016/j.clineuro.2012.10.027 fnhum.2014.00750
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 35

Lough, S., Kipps, C. M., Treise, C., Watson, P., Blair, J. R., & and social cognition. Behavioral and Brain Functions : BBF, 9,
Hodges, J. R. (2006). Social reasoning, emotion and empa- 47. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-9-47
thy in frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia, 44(6), Mendez, M. F., Anderson, E., & Shapira, J. S. (2005). An inves-
950–958. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.009 tigation of moral judgement in frontotemporal dementia.
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18(4), 193–197.
Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000191292.17964.bb
behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10 Mendez, M. F., & Shapira, J. S. (2009). Altered emotional
(6), 434–445. doi:10.1038/nrn2639 morality in frontotemporal dementia. Cognitive
Maguire, J., & Mody, I. (2008). GABA A R plasticity during Neuropsychiatry, 14(3), 165–179. doi:10.1080/
pregnancy: Relevance to postpartum depression. Neuron, 13546800902924122
59(2), 207–213. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.019 Mesulam, M. M. (1986). Frontal cortex and behavior. Annals of
Mahoney, C. J., Beck, J., Rohrer, J. D., Lashley, T., Mok, K., Neurology, 19(4), 320–325. doi:10.1002/ana.410190403
Shakespeare, T., . . . Warren, J. D. (2012). Frontotemporal Meyer, M. L., Spunt, R. P., Berkman, E. T., Taylor, S. E., &
dementia with the C9ORF72 hexanucleotide repeat Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Evidence for social working mem-
expansion: Clinical, neuroanatomical and neuropathologi- ory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proceedings of
cal features. Brain, 135(3), 736–750. doi:10.1093/brain/ the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 1883–1888.
awr361 doi:10.1073/pnas.1121077109
Manes, F. (2012). Psychiatric conditions that can mimic early Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2014). Social neuroscience and
behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: The impor- mechanisms of risk for mental disorders. World Psychiatry,
tance of the new diagnostic criteria. Current Psychiatry 13(2), 143–144. doi:10.1002/wps.20121
Reports, 14(5), 450–452. doi:10.1007/s11920-012-0294-7 Meziane, H., Schaller, F., Bauer, S., Villard, C., Matarazzo, V., Riet,
Manes, F. F., Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., F., . . . Muscatelli, F. (2014). An early postnatal oxytocin treat-
Bekinschtein, T. A., & Hodges, J. R. (2010). Frontotemporal ment prevents social and learning deficits in adult mice
dementia presenting as pathological gambling. Nature deficient for Magel2, a gene involved in prader-willi syn-
Reviews. Neurology, 6(6), 347–352. doi:10.1038/ drome and autism. Biological Psychiatry, 20(14), 00887–00887.
nrneurol.2010.34 Mike, A., Strammer, E., Aradi, M., Orsi, G., Perlaki, G., Hajnal, A.,
Marazziti, D., Baroni, S., Landi, P., Ceresoli, D., & Dell’Osso, L. . . . Paul, F. (2013). Disconnection mechanism and regional
(2013). The neurobiology of moral sense: Facts or hypoth- cortical atrophy contribute to impaired processing of facial
eses? Annals of General Psychiatry, 12(1), 6. doi:10.1186/ expressions and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: A
1744-859X-12-6 structural MRI study. PLoS One, 8(12), e82422. doi:10.1371/
Marrie, R. A., Reingold, S., Cohen, J., Stuve, O., Trojano, M., journal.pone.0082422
Sorensen, P. S., . . . Reider, N. (2015). The incidence and Mikulan, E. P., Reynaldo, L., & Ibanez, A. (2014). Homuncular
prevalence of psychiatric disorders in multiple sclerosis: A mirrors: Misunderstanding causality in embodied cognition.
systematic review. Multiple Sclerosis (Houndmills, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 299. doi:10.3389/
Basingstoke, England). doi:10.1177/1352458514564487 fnhum.2014.00299
Martin, J. B. (2002). The integration of neurology, psychiatry, Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S.,
and neuroscience in the 21st century. American Journal of Clayton, N. S., . . . Young, L. J. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction
Psychiatry, 159(5), 695–704. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.695 in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the
McCleery, A., Lee, J., Joshi, A., Wynn, J. K., Hellemann, G. S., & quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews. Drug
Green, M. F. (2014). Meta-analysis of face processing event- Discovery, 11(2), 141–168. doi:10.1038/nrd3628
related potentials in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 5 Miller, L. A., Hsieh, S., Lah, S., Savage, S., Hodges, J. R., & Piguet,
(14), 015. O. (2012). One size does not fit all: Face emotion processing
McCormick, L. M., Brumm, M. C., Beadle, J. N., Paradiso, S., impairments in semantic dementia, behavioural-variant
Yamada, T., & Andreasen, N. (2012). Mirror neuron function, frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease are
psychosis, and empathy in schizophrenia. Psychiatry mediated by distinct cognitive deficits. Behavioural
Research: Neuroimaging, 201(3), 233–239. doi:10.1016/j. Neurology, 25(1), 53–60. doi:10.1155/2012/683052
pscychresns.2012.01.004 Miller, M. B., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Young, L., King, D., Paggi,
McGorry, P., & Van Os, J. (2013). Redeeming diagnosis in A., Fabri, M., . . . Gazzaniga, M. S. (2010). Abnormal moral
psychiatry: Timing versus specificity. The Lancet, 381(9863), reasoning in complete and partial callosotomy patients.
343–345. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61268-9 Neuropsychologia, 48(7), 2215–2220. doi:10.1016/j.
Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., De La Fuente, L., neuropsychologia.2010.02.021
Forno, G., . . . Ibanez, A. (2016). Your perspective and my Milrod, B., Markowitz, J. C., Gerber, A. J., Cyranowski, J.,
benefit: Multiple lesion models of self-other integration Altemus, M., Shapiro, T., . . . Glatt, C. (2014). Childhood
strategies during social bargaining. Brain, 26. 10.1093/ separation anxiety and the pathogenesis and treatment of
brain/aww1231 adult anxiety. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(1), 34–43.
Melloni, M., Lopez, V., & Ibanez, A. (2013). Empathy and con- doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13060781
textual social cognition. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Minio-Paluello, I., Baron-Cohen, S., Avenanti, A., Walsh, V., &
Neuroscience. doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0205-3 Aglioti, S. M. (2009). Absence of embodied empathy during
Melloni, M., Sedeño, L., Couto, B., Reynoso, M., Gelormini, C., pain observation in Asperger syndrome. Biological
Favaloro, R., . . . Ibanez, A. (2013). Preliminary evidence Psychiatry, 65(1), 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.
about the effects of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity biopsych.2008.08.006.
36 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

Moll, J., & Schulkin, J. (2009). Social attachment and aversion American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(10), 1775–1783.
in human moral cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1775
Reviews, 33(3), 456–465. doi:10.1016/j. Pfeiffer, U. J., Timmermans, B., Vogeley, K., Frith, C., &
neubiorev.2008.12.001 Schilbach, L. (2013). Towards a neuroscience of social inter-
Moll, J., Zahn, R., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, action. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. doi:10.3389/
J. (2005). Opinion: The neural basis of human moral cogni- fnhum.2013.00022
tion. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 799–809. Phillips, L. H., Scott, C., Henry, J. D., Mowat, D., & Bell, J. S.
doi:10.1038/nrn1768 (2010). Emotion perception in Alzheimer’s disease and
Monkul, E. S., Green, M. J., Barrett, J. A., Robinson, J. L., mood disorder in old age. Psychology and Aging, 25(1),
Velligan, D. I., & Glahn, D. C. (2007). A social cognitive 38–47. doi:10.1037/a0017369
approach to emotional intensity judgment deficits in schi- Piguet, O., Hornberger, M., Mioshi, E., & Hodges, J. R. (2011).
zophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 94(1–3), 245–252. Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: Diagnosis,
doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.023 clinical staging, and management. The Lancet Neurology,
Montag, C., Heinz, A., Kunz, D., & Gallinat, J. (2007). Self- 10(2), 162–172. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70299-4
reported empathic abilities in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Piguet, O., Leyton, C. E., Gleeson, L. D., Hoon, C., & Hodges, J.
Research, 92(1–3), 85–89. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.024 R. (2015). Memory and emotion processing performance
Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., contributes to the diagnosis of non-semantic primary pro-
Mavros, P. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Impaired theory of gressive aphasia syndromes. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease,
mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. 44(2), 541–547. doi:10.3233/JAD-141854.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), Poletti, M., Vergallo, A., Ulivi, M., Sonnoli, A., & Bonuccelli, U.
2688–2692. doi:10.1073/pnas.1011734108 (2013). Affective theory of mind in patients with Parkinson’s
Moretto, G., Làdavas, E., Mattioli, F., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2010). disease. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 67(4), 273–
A psychophysiological investigation of moral judgment 276. doi:10.1111/pcn.12045
after ventromedial prefrontal damage. Journal of Cognitive Preissler, S., Dziobek, I., Ritter, K., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S.
Neuroscience, 22(8), 1888–1899. doi:10.1162/ (2010). Social cognition in borderline personality disorder:
jocn.2009.21367 Evidence for disturbed recognition of the emotions,
Narme, P., Mouras, H., Roussel, M., Duru, C., Krystkowiak, P., & thoughts, and intentions of others. Frontiers in Behavioral
Godefroy, O. (2013). Emotional and cognitive social pro- Neuroscience, 4, 182. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00182
cesses are impaired in Parkinson’s disease and are related Premack, D. W. G., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpan-
to behavioral disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 182–192. zee have a theory of mind? Behavioral Brain Sciences, 1,
doi:10.1037/a0031522 515–526. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Price, B. H., Adams, R. D., & Coyle, J. T. (2000). Neurology and
Black, S., . . . Benson, D. F. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar psychiatry: Closing the great divide. Neurology, 54(1), 8-8.
degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. doi:10.1212/WNL.54.1.8
Neurology, 51(6), 1546–1554. doi:10.1212/WNL.51.6.1546 Pugliese, L., Catani, M., Ameis, S., Dell’Acqua, F., Thiebaut De
Newman, J. P., Curtin, J. J., Bertsch, J. D., & Baskin-Sommers, A. Schotten, M., Murphy, C., . . . Murphy, D. G. M. (2009). The
R. (2010). Attention moderates the fearlessness of psycho- anatomy of extended limbic pathways in Asperger syn-
pathic offenders. Biological Psychiatry, 67(1), 66–70. drome: A preliminary diffusion tensor imaging tractography
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.035 study. Neuroimage, 47(2), 427–434. doi:10.1016/j.
Njomboro, P., Humphreys, G. W., & Deb, S. (2014). Exploring neuroimage.2009.05.014
social cognition in patients with apathy following acquired Pujol, J., Batalla, I., Contreras-Rodríguez, O., Harrison, B. J.,
brain damage. BMC Neurology, 14, 18. doi:10.1186/1471- Pera, V., Hernández-Ribas, R., … Cardoner, N. (2012).
2377-14-18 Breakdown in the brain network subserving moral judg-
Northoff, G. (2008). Neuropsychiatry. An old discipline in a ment in criminal psychopathy. Social Cognitive and Affective
new gestalt bridging biological psychiatry, neuropsychol- Neuroscience, 7(8), 917–923. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr075
ogy, and cognitive neurology. European Archives of Rahman, S., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Rogers, R. D., &
Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 258(4), 226–238. Robbins, T. W. (1999). Specific cognitive deficits in mild
doi:10.1007/s00406-007-0783-6 frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 122(8),
Novak, M. J., Warren, J. D., Henley, S. M., Draganski, B., 1469–1493. doi:10.1093/brain/122.8.1469
Frackowiak, R. S., & Tabrizi, S. J. (2012). Altered brain Rankin, K. P., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Allison, S. C., Stanley, C. M.,
mechanisms of emotion processing in pre-manifest Glenn, S., Weiner, M. W., & Miller, B. L. (2006). Structural
Huntington’s disease. Brain, 135(4), 1165–1179. anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative disease. Brain,
doi:10.1093/brain/aws024 129(11), 2945–2956. doi:10.1093/brain/awl254
Omar, R., Rohrer, J. D., Hailstone, J. C., & Warren, J. D. (2011). Rankin, K. P., Kramer, J. H., & Miller, B. L. (2005). Patterns of
Structural neuroanatomy of face processing in frontotem- cognitive and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar
poral lobar degeneration. Journal of Neurology, degeneration. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18(1),
Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 82(12), 1341–1343. doi:10.1136/ 28–36. doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000152225.05377.ab
jnnp.2010.227983 Rascovsky, K., Hodges, J. R., Knopman, D., Mendez, M. F.,
Paradiso, S., Andreasen, N. C., Crespo-Facorro, B., O’Leary, D. S., Kramer, J. H., Neuhaus, J., . . . Miller, B. L., et, al. (2011).
Watkins, G. L., Boles Ponto, L. L., & Hichwa, R. D. (2003). Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural
Emotions in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia dur- variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 134(9), 2456–
ing evaluation with positron emission tomography. 2477. doi:10.1093/brain/awr179
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 37

Reynolds, E. H. (1990). Structure and function in neurology from the heart, through the brain, to the whole body, and
and psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 481– beyond: A journey through Western history, science, and
490. doi:10.1192/bjp.157.4.481 philosophy. Neurosurgery, 65(4), 633–643. doi:10.1227/01.
Riveros, R., Manes, F., Hurtado, E., Escobar, M., Martin Reyes, NEU.0000349750.22332.6A
M., Cetkovich, M., & Ibañez, A. (2010). Context-sensitive Sayın, A., Oral, N., Utku, Ç., Baysak, E., & Candansayar, S. (2010).
social cognition is impaired in schizophrenic patients and Theory of mind in obsessive-compulsive disorder:
their healthy relatives. Schizophrenia Research, 116(2–3), Comparison with healthy controls. European Psychiatry, 25
297–298. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.10.017 (2), 116–122. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.09.002
Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. Schepman, K., Taylor, E., Collishaw, S., & Fombonne, E. (2012).
(2009). Executive functions in children with autism spec- Face emotion processing in depressed children and ado-
trum disorders. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 362–368. lescents with and without comorbid conduct disorder.
doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.007 Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(4), 583–593.
Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Ibáñez, A., Torralva, T., & Manes, F. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9587-2
(2013). Cerebellar stroke impairs executive functions but Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G.,
not theory of mind. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person
Neurosciences, 25(1), E48–49. doi:10.1176/appi. neuroscience. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393–
neuropsych.12030057. 414. doi:10.1017/s0140525x12000660
Roca, M., Manes, F., Gleichgerrcht, E., Ibáñez, A., Gonzalez De Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2013).
Toledo, M. E., Marenco, V., . . . Sinay, V. (2014). Cognitive but A temporally sustained implicit theory of mind deficit in
not affective theory of mind deficits in mild relapsing- autism spectrum disorders. Cognition, 129(2), 410–417.
remitting multiple sclerosis. Cognitive and Behavioral doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.004
Neurology : Official Journal of the Society for Behavioral Schroeter, M. L., Raczka, K., Neumann, J., & Von Cramon, D. Y.
and Cognitive Neurology, 27(1), 25–30. doi:10.1097/ (2008). Neural networks in frontotemporal dementia–a
WNN.0000000000000017 meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 29(3), 418–426.
Rosen, H. J., Perry, R. J., Murphy, J., Kramer, J. H., Mychack, P., doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.023
Schuff, N., . . . Miller, B. L. (2002). Emotion comprehension in Sedeño, L., Couto, B., García-Cordero, I., Melloni, M., Baez, S.,
the temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain, Morales Sepúlveda, J. P., . . . Ibanez, A. (2016). Brain network
125(Pt 10), 2286–2295. doi:10.1093/brain/awf225 organization and social executive performance in fronto-
Rosen, H. J., Wilson, M. R., Schauer, G. F., Allison, S., Gorno- temporal dementia. Journal of the International
Tempini, M.-L., Pace-Savitsky, C., . . . Miller, B. L. (2006). Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 22(2), 250–262.
Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired recognition of doi:10.1017/S1355617715000703
emotion in dementia. Neuropsychologia, 44(3), 365–373. Sedeño, L., Couto, B., Melloni, M., Canales-Johnson, A., Yoris,
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.012 A., Baez, S., . . . Soriano-Mas, C. (2014). How do you feel
Rutishauser, U., Mamelak, A. N., & Adolphs, R. (2015). The when you can’t feel your body? Interoception, functional
primate amygdala in social perception - insights from elec- connectivity and emotional processing in depersonaliza-
trophysiological recordings and stimulation. Trends in tion-derealization disorder. PLoS One, 9(6), e98769.
Neurosciences, 38(5), 295–306. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2015.03.001 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098769
Sachse, M., Schlitt, S., Hainz, D., Ciaramidaro, A., Walter, H., Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. (2004). Psychological stress
Poustka, F., . . . Freitag, C. M. (2014). Facial emotion recogni- and the human immune system: A meta-analytic study of
tion in paranoid schizophrenia and autism spectrum dis- 30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin, 130(4), 601–630.
order. Schizophrenia Research, 159, 509–514. doi:10.1016/j. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601
schres.2014.08.030 Seidel, E.-M., Habel, U., Finkelmeyer, A., Hasmann, A.,
Sadeh, N., Spielberg, J. M., Heller, W., Herrington, J. D., Engels, Dobmeier, M., & Derntl, B. (2012). Risk or resilience?
A. S., Warren, S. L., . . . Miller, G. A. (2013). Emotion disrupts Empathic abilities in patients with bipolar disorders
neural activity during selective attention in psychopathy. and their first-degree relatives. Journal of Psychiatric
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 235–246. Research, 46(3), 382–388. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.
doi:10.1093/scan/nsr092 2011.11.006
Samamé, C., Martino, D. J., & Strejilevich, S. A. (2012). Social Senju, A. (2012). Spontaneous theory of mind and its absence
cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder: Systematic review in autism spectrum disorders. The Neuroscientist, 18(2), 108–
and meta-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 113. doi:10.1177/1073858410397208
125(4), 266–280. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01808.x Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind
Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Kathirgamanathan, U., & eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in
Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Seeing it my way: A case of a Asperger syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885.
selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective. Brain, 128(Pt doi:10.1126/science.1176170
5), 1102–1111. doi:10.1093/brain/awh464 Shamay-Tsoory, S., Harari, H., Szepsenwol, O., & Levkovitz, Y.
Santamaría-García, H., Reyes, P., García, A., Baéz, S., Martinez, (2009). Neuropsychological evidence of impaired cognitive
A., Santacruz, J. M., . . . Ibañez, A. (2016). First symptoms and empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder. The Journal of
neurocognitive correlates of behavioral variant frontotem- Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 21(1), 59–67.
poral dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 54, 957–970. doi:10.1176/jnp.2009.21.1.59
doi:10.3233/jad-160501 Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two
Santoro, G., Wood, M. D., Merlo, L., Anastasi, G. P., Tomasello, systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emo-
F., & Germano, A. (2009). The anatomic location of the soul tional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus
38 A. IBÁÑEZ ET AL.

versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132(3), 617– Turetsky, B. I., Kohler, C. G., Indersmitten, T., Bhati, M. T.,
627. doi:10.1093/brain/awn279 Charbonnier, D., & Gur, R. C. (2007). Facial emotion recogni-
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Goldsher, D., Berger, B. D., & tion in schizophrenia: When and why does it go awry?
Aharon-Peretz, J. (2004). Impairment in cognitive and affec- Schizophrenia Research, 94(1–3), 253–263. doi:10.1016/j.
tive empathy in patients with brain lesions: Anatomical and schres.2007.05.001
cognitive correlates. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Uddin, L. Q., Kinnison, J., Pessoa, L., & Anderson, M. L. (2014).
Neuropsychology, 26(8), 1113–1127. doi:10.1080/ Beyond the tripartite cognition-emotion-interoception
13803390490515531 model of the human insular cortex. Journal of Cognitive
Sharp, C., & Vanwoerden, S. (2014). Social cognition: empirical Neuroscience, 26(1), 16–27. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00462
contribution: The developmental building blocks of psy- Van Rheenen, T. E., & Rossell, S. L. (2013). Picture sequencing
chopathic traits: Revisiting the role of theory of mind. task performance indicates theory of mind deficit in bipolar
Journal of Personality Disorders, 28(1), 78–95. doi:10.1521/ disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151(3), 1132–1134.
pedi.2014.28.1.78 doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.07.009
Silasi, G., & Murphy, T. H. (2014). Stroke and the connectome: Van Rijn, S., Swaab, H., Aleman, A., & Kahn, R. S. (2006). X
How connectivity guides therapeutic intervention. Neuron, Chromosomal effects on social cognitive processing and
83(6), 1354–1368. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.052 emotion regulation: A study with Klinefelter men (47,XXY).
Sporns, O. (2014). Contributions and challenges for network Schizophrenia Research, 84(2–3), 194–203. doi:10.1016/j.
models in cognitive neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 17 schres.2006.02.020
(5), 652–660. doi:10.1038/nn.3690 Varjassyova, A., Horinek, D., Andel, R., Amlerova, J., Laczo, J.,
Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Karnat, A., Sheardova, K., . . . Hort, J. (2013). Recognition of facial emo-
Lange, H., Hömberg, V., . . . Rowland, D. (1996). Loss of tional expression in amnestic mild cognitive impairment.
disgust. Perception of faces and emotions in Huntington’s Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease : JAD, 33(1), 273–280.
disease. Brain, 119(5), 1647–1665. doi:10.1093/brain/ doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-120148
119.5.1647 Ventura, J., Wood, R. C., Jimenez, A. M., & Hellemann, G. S.
Sprong, M., Schothorst, P., Vos, E., Hox, J., & Van Engeland, H. (2013). Neurocognition and symptoms identify links
(2007). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. The between facial recognition and emotion processing in schi-
British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 5–13. doi:10.1192/bjp. zophrenia: Meta-analytic findings. Schizophrenia Research,
bp.107.035899 151(1–3), 78–84. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.015
Stanley, B., & Siever, L. J. (2010). The interpersonal dimension Verdejo-Garcia, A., Contreras-Rodriguez, O., Fonseca, F.,
of borderline personality disorder: Toward a neuropeptide Cuenca, A., Soriano-Mas, C., Rodriguez, J., . . . De La Torre,
model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1), 24–39. R. (2014). Functional alteration in frontolimbic systems rele-
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050744 vant to moral judgment in cocaine-dependent subjects.
Steiner, M. (1997). Premenstrual syndromes. Annual Review of Addiction Biology, 19(2), 272–281. doi:10.1111/j.1369-
Medicine, 48, 447–455. doi:10.1146/annurev.med.48.1.447 1600.2012.00472.x
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal Wang, S. M., Kim, Y., Yeon, B., Lee, H. K., Kweon, Y. S., Lee, C. T.,
lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive & Lee, K. U. (2013). Symptom severity of panic disorder
Neuroscience, 10(5), 640–656. doi:10.1162/ associated with impairment in emotion processing of
089892998562942 threat-related facial expressions. Psychiatry and Clinical
Takeda, T., Kasai, K., & Kato, N. (2007). Moral judgment in high- Neurosciences, 67(4), 245–252. doi:10.1111/pcn.12039
functioning pervasive developmental disorders. Psychiatry Watermeyer, T. J., Brown, R. G., Sidle, K. C., Oliver, D. J., Allen,
and Clinical Neurosciences, 61(4), 407–414. doi:10.1111/ C., Karlsson, J., . . . Goldstein, L. H. (2015). Executive dysfunc-
j.1440-1819.2007.01678.x tion predicts social cognition impairment in amyotrophic
Tobón, C., Ibañez, A., Velilla, L., Duque, J., Ochoa, J., Trujillo, N., lateral sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, 10, 10.
. . . Pineda, D. (2015). Emotional processing in Colombian Werner, K. H., Roberts, N. A., Rosen, H. J., Dean, D. L., Kramer, J.
ex-combatants and its relationship with empathy and H., Weiner, M. W., . . . Levenson, R. W. (2007). Emotional
executive functions. Social Neuroscience, 10(2), 153–165. reactivity and emotion recognition in frontotemporal
doi:10.1080/17470919.2014.969406 lobar degeneration. Neurology, 69(2), 148–155.
Torralva, T., Gleichgerrcht, E., Ibanez, A., & Manes, F. (2016). doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000265589.32060.d3
The frontal lobes. In M. Husain & J. Schott (Eds.), Oxford Whitton, A. E., Henry, J. D., & Grisham, J. R. (2014). Moral
textbook of cognitive neurology and dementia (pp. 27–38). rigidity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Do abnormalities
UK: Oxford University Press. in inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility and disgust play a
Torralva, T., Roca, M., Gleichgerrcht, E., Bekinschtein, T., & role? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Manes, F. (2009). A neuropsychological battery to detect Psychiatry, 45(1), 152–159. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.10.001
specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly,
frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 132(5), 1299–1309. J. F., Barnes, L. L., . . . Bennett, D. A. (2007). Loneliness and
doi:10.1093/brain/awp041 risk of Alzheimer disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64
Torralva, T., Strejilevich, S., Gleichgerrcht, E., Roca, M., Martino, (2), 234–240. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234
D., Cetkovich, M., & Manes, F. (2012). Deficits in tasks of Wojakiewicz, A., Januel, D., Braha, S., Prkachin, K., Danziger, N.,
executive functioning that mimic real-life scenarios in bipo- Bouhassira, D. (2013). Alteration of pain recognition in
lar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 14(1), 118–125. doi:10.1111/ schizophrenia. European Journal of Pain, 17(9), 1385–1392.
j.1399-5618.2012.00987.x doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00310.x
SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE 39

Woolley, J. D., Khan, B. K., Murthy, N. K., Miller, B. L., & Rankin, interoceptive sensitivity and metacognitive interoception
K. P. (2011). The diagnostic challenge of psychiatric symp- in panic. Behavioral and Brain Functions : BBF, 11, 14.
toms in neurodegenerative disease: Rates of and risk fac- doi:10.1186/s12993-015-0058-8
tors for prior psychiatric diagnosis in patients with early York, G. K., & Steinberg, D. A. (2001). The sacred disease of
neurodegenerative disease. The Journal of Clinical Cambyses II. Archives of Neurology, 58(10), 1702–1704.
Psychiatry, 72(2), 126–133. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06382oli Young, L., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Hauser, M., &
Yeh, Z. T., & Tsai, C. F. (2014). Impairment on theory of mind Damasio, A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal
and empathy in patients with stroke. Psychiatry and Clinical cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron, 65(6),
Neurosciences, 68(8), 612–20. doi:10.1111/pcn.12173 845–851. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.003
Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998). Yuvaraj, R., Murugappan, M., Norlinah, M. I., Sundaraj, K., &
Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of indivi- Khairiyah, M. (2013). Review of emotion recognition in
duals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and stroke patients. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders,
normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124 36(3–4), 179–196. doi:10.1159/000353440
(3), 283–307. Zalla, T., Barlassina, L., Buon, M., & Leboyer, M. (2011). Moral
Yang, E., Tadin, D., Glasse,r D. M., Wook Hong, S., Blake, R., & judgment in adults with autism spectrum disorders.
Park, S. (2013). Visual context processing in bipolar disor- Cognition, 121(1), 115–126. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.004
der: A comparison with schizophrenia. Frontiers in Ze, O., Thoma, P., & Suchan, B. (2014). Cognitive and affective
Psychology, 4, 569. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00569 empathy in younger and older individuals. Aging &
Yoris, A., Esteves, S., Couto, B., Melloni, M., Kichic, R., Mental Health, 18(7), 929–935. doi:10.1080/
Cetkovich, M., . . . Sedeño, L. (2015). The roles of 13607863.2014.899973

S-ar putea să vă placă și