Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

Reliability of Rating Visible Landscape Qualities

James F. Palmer

James E Palmer is Associate Profes- Abstract: Reliability is measured by whether an investigation will obtain similar results
sor and Undergraduate Curriculum when it is repeated by another party. It is argued that reliability is important at the level of
Director of the Landscape Architec- individuals. While all landscape assessments are based on individual judgments, they are fre-
ture Program at the State University quently aggregated to form co~npositejudgments. The use of inter-group, intra-gwup and inter-
of New York’s College of Environmen- rater measures of reliability in the landscape perception literature is reviewed. This paper inves-
tal Science and Forestry in Syracuse, tigates the reliability of assessing various visible landscape qualities using data primarily from
New York. He holds an M.L.A. degree previously published studies. The results indicate that there is reason for concern about the reli-
and Ph.D. degree from the University ability of rating scales used in thisJ~eld, and suggest actions for both research and practice.
of Massachusetts in Amherst. His Landscape studies have joined the shifting sands of scholarly inquiry. The empirical pos-
research focuses on perceptions of itivist tradition continues to hold that there is an objective landscape that can be studied
visible landscape qualities. He is par- through the careful application of scientific principles. Post-positivists, among whom I include
ticularly interested in developing GIS myself, stand back from the stranglehold that science recently held on legitimate knowledge.
models to predict landscape percep- They acknowledge other ways of knowing, but favor empirical methods of discovery. In response
tions and the interaction between to positivism, post-modern landscape scholars with diverse viewpoints are investigating new
landscape change and changes in per- ways to explore the personal meaning that landscapes hold for us. For instance, Norberg-Schulz
ceptions. He also hosts LArch-L, the (1979) has contributed to the rising importance of genius loci, Potteiger and Purinton (1998)
electronic discussion group for land- are exploring landscape narratives, and Brook (1998) is adapting Goethe’s approach to scien-
scape architecture. He may be tific inquiry through direct experience. In this flurry of activity some foundational attributes of
reached by email at applied everyday experience are lost or ignored. One such attribute is reliability, which is the
zooey@mailbox.syr.edu. subject of this paper.

Reliability and the Human Condition The poor boy hopes to recover This bud of love, by summer’s
from his blunder: ripening breath,

T o illustrate the tension


between our desire to cap-
Juliet. Do not swear at all;
Or, if thou wilt, swear by thy gra-
May prove a beauteous flower when
next we meet.
Good night, good night! as sweet
ture the uniqueness of the moment cious self, repose and rest
and the need for stability in our lives, Which is the god of my idolatry, Come to thy heart as that within
I call upon one of the greatest chroni- And I’ll believe thee. my breast!
clers of the human condition. Quoting
She embraces the substance of He has twice failed, and she
from the balcony scene in Shake-
his desire (their love); it is only the grows weary. However, she leaves him
speare’s Romeo and Juliet act 2, scene 2:
metaphor for his method that is in with an indication of what she
Romeo. Lady, by yonder blessed question (the unique moment). seeks--reliability. What she wants is
moon I swear
Romeo. If my heart’s dear love .... not rash declarations, but steadfast
That tips with silver all these fruit-
tree tops .... devotion; not unadvised promises, but
He has not learned, and begins considered pronouncements; not sud-
In the middle of Romeo’s poetic another uniquely poetic expression, den like a flash of lightning, but a
attempt to swayJuliet’s heart, she only to be cut off once more: constant and unfailing partnership in
cuts him offl Juliet. Well, do not swear. Although love.Juliet’s response represents the
Juliet. O! swear not by the moon, I joy in thee, need for reliability in our most impor-
the inconstant moon, I have no joy of this contract to- tant experiences as a fundamental
The monthly changes in her circled night: condition in our lives.
orb, It is too rash, too unadvised, too
Lost that thy love prove likewise sudden; Reliability in the (Post-)Positivist Paradigm
variable. Too like the lightning, which doth The National Environmental
cease to be
She will have none of it (this Ere one can say it lightens. Sweet, Policy Act of 1969 has particular
night at least). She makes it clear good night! importance for those of us who con-
that she wants a constant and duct landscape assessments. It is
dependable love: NEPA that declared it national policy
Romeo. What shall I swear by?

166 Landscape Journal


and the "continuing responsibility of for which purposes reliabilities of reliability for a single rater’s assess-
the Federal Government to use all .70 or higher will suffice .... For ment, based on the ratings from a
practicable means to... assure for all basic research, it can be argued group. The landscape assessment lit-
Americans... aesthetically.., pleas- that increasing reliabilities much erature concerning inter-group, intra-
ing surroundings." In particulm; beyond .80 is often wasteful of time group, and inter-rater reliability is
and funds .... In many applied
there is a responsibility to "identify reviewed. The analyses of raw
problems, a great deal hinges on
and develop methods and procedures the exact score made by a datasets, primarily from previously
¯.. which will insure that presently person .... In those applied settings published studies, are presented to
unquantiffed environmental ameni- where important decisions are illustrate the difference between
ties and values may be given appro- made with respect to specific test inter- and intra-group reliabilities for
priate consideration in decision mak- scores, a reliability of .90 is the scenic value ratings. Then three new
ing." As one example, the goals of the minimum that should be tolerated, analyses are presented investigating
Forest Service’s new Scenery Manage- and a reliability of .95 should be the reliability of three types of land-
ment System are to (1) inventory and considered the desirable standard. scape descriptors: landscape dimen-
analyze scenery; (2) assist in estab- Since there are few problems sions (development, naturalism, pref-
lishing overall resource goals and that impact us as directly as decisions erence, and spaciousness), informa-
objectives; (3) monitor the scenic about our common landscape, the tional content (coherence,
resource; and (4) ensure high-quality fundamental importance of using complexity, legibility, and mystery),
scenery for future generations reliable landscape assessment meth- and compositional elements (color,
(USDA 1995). Reliability is the ods should be readily apparent. form, line, and texture). The conclud-
implicit cornerstone upon which ing discussion makes recommenda-
these goals can be achieved. Reliability in Visual Landscape tions to those in research and prac-
Though speaking about unquan- Assessments tice concerning the reliability of land-
tiffed values and amenities, the lan- It is surprising that relatively scape assessment.
guage of NEPA clearly takes a decid- little attention is paid by most Inter-group reliability. At the Our
edly positivist tone. Nunnally (1978, researchers and practitioners to the National Landscape conference,
p. 191) describes the meaning and reliability of landscape assessment Craik and Feimer (1979) made a plea
importance of reliability to science: methods--and there is a lot on which to establish technical standards for
Reliability concerns the extent to to focus¯ For instance, how many pho- the reliability, validity, generality, and
which measurements are repeat- tos are needed to reliably represent utility of observer-based landscape
able--when different persons make different landscapes (Daniel et al. assessments. At this time, most stud-
the measurements, on different 1977; Hoffman 1997)? When a long ies that include reliability estimates
occasions, with supposedly alterna- series of landscape scenes is being use inter-group measures that com-
tive instruments for measuring the evaluated, are the same standards pare the mean assessments made by
same thing and when there are groups of students, lay public, or pro-
being reliably applied throughout the
small variations in circumstances fessionals. Interpretation of inter-
for making measurements that are sequence (Palmer 1998)? Are land-
scape evaluations stable after a year group reliabilities may easily fall prey
not intended to influence results.
or two (Hull and Buhyoff 1984); how to the ecological fallacy (Robinson
¯.. Measurement reliability repre-
sents a classic issue in scientific about after ten years (Palmer 1997)? 1950) of making generalizations
generalization. While these and other questions about individuals based on correla-
of reliability are all important, this tions among groups. Establishing
The measurement of reliability similar rating patterns between
paper focuses on the reliability of
is relatively straightforward.~ "The divergent groups, for example profes-
raters. Three levels of rater reliability
average correlation among the items sionals and the public, does not indi-
are distinguished: inter-group, intra-
can be used to obtain an accurate cate that there is wide agreement
group, and inter-rater. Inter-group
estimate of reliability" for independ- among individuals within or across
reliability compares the mean ratings
ently made assessments (Nunnally these groups. The following examples
assigned by different groups. The
1978, p. 227). There are even gener- illustrate the use of inter-group relia-
intent is to establish whether differ-
ally accepted standards for reliability bility.
ent groups give similar ratings. Intra-
among psychometricians (Nunnally In a study of Southern Con-
group reliability establishes the relia-
1978, p. 245). necticut River Valley landscapes,
bility of a composite or mean rating
What a satisfactory level of reliabil- from a particular group. Inter-rater Zube et al. (1974) reported the corre-
ity is depends on how a measure is reliability establishes the expected lations among mean ratings for thir-
being used. In the early stages of teen groups on eighteen scales.
research.., one saves time and Eighty-five percent of the correla-
energy by working with instruments tions among these groups were above
that have only modest reliability, .83. All of the correlations below .83
involved a single group of center-city

Palmer 167
residents, suggesting that they "may but not the average reliability for Schroeder (1984) reports the
in fact have different perceptions of individuals (Brown et al. 1988; Brown inter-rater reliability of attractive-
scenic quality in the rural land- and Daniel 1987; Daniel et al. 1989; ness or scenic beauty from ten stud-
scape." The need for further study is Hetherington et al. 1993; Rudis et al. ies, as well as one study of visual air
indicated. 1988). Typically these reliabilities are quality, two studies of enjoyableness,
Buhyoff and his colleagues above .90 for scenic beauty. and another of safety for urban parks.
(1979) compare the perception of Reports of the intra-group relia- The attractiveness studies have a
insect damage to forest scenes by bility of ratings other than scenic mean reliability of .530. Patsfall and
foresters, environmentalists, and the beauty or preference are less com- his colleagues (1984) report single-
public. There was an overall similar- mon. Herzog (1987) used ratings of rater reliabilities of.23 for SBE rat-
ity among the groups, but ratings identifiability, coherence, spacious- ings of vistas along the Blue Ridge
were affected by awareness of the dis- ness, complexity, mystery, texture, Parkway. Anderson (1976) extended
ease. Groat (1982) compared percep- and preference to characterize sev- Zube’s (1974) study and found an
tions of modern and post-modern enty natural mountainous, canyon, inter-rater reliability for individuals of
architecture by accountants and and desert scenes. Using groups of .69 for scenic value judgments of 212
architects. She found that the from thirteen to twenty-six students, scenes. He also used the Spearman-
accountants were not sensitive to he obtained intra-group reliabilities Brown prophecy formula to deter-
post-modern principles. for mean scores that ranged from .69 mine that a group of four raters
Some of these and other studies for coherence to .97 for preference. would achieve a composite reliability
find evidence that there is a strong Intra-group reliabilities are for scenic value of approximately .90.
similarity among ratings by the pub- generally high, and the more raters Craik’s research group used
lic, students, and professionals in there are in a group, the greater will four scenes to evaluate inter-rater
many instances. In other studies, be the group-mean reliability. The reliabilities for fifteen attribute rat-
expert knowledge appears to change reliability of group-means is impor- ings, primarily those used in the
the ratings by professionals and stu- tant if they are going to be used as Bureau of Land Management’s visual
dents, sometimes quite significantly. variables in predictive research mod- resource assessment procedure
However, none of these group com- els or for making environmental deci- (Feimer et al. 1979). Even after
parisons sheds light on the reliability sions. However, they do not reflect extending the analysis to 19 scenes,
of ratings by individuals, whether the reliability of individual raters. they found that "the reliability of rat-
they are environmental professionals Inter-rater reliability. In practice, ings tends to be low for single
or members of the public. landscape assessments conducted in observers and hence it is advisable to
Intra-group reliability. Most the field rarely involve judgments by use composite judgments of panels of
reports of rater reliability are for more than one or perhaps two trained independent observers" (Feimer et al.
group-mean ratings rather than for professionals. Assessing slides or 1981, p. 16). In a later report on this
the average reliability for an individ- other representations makes it more same work, Craik (1983, p. 72) rec-
Ual rater within a group. For instance, convenient to use a panel of evalua- ommends that "given present rating
Gobster and Chenoweth (1989) iden- tors. However, it is still unusual for systems, panels of at least five mem-
tified thirty-four of the most com- more than a few professionals to bers, rendering independent judg-
monly used landscape descriptors apply any of the agency evaluations ments, are required to achieve ade-
representing physical, psychological systems to a series of slides (Smardon quate levels of composite reliability."
and artistic attributes. In two experi- et al. 1988; USDA 1995; USDI 1980). The basis of this recommendation
ments, using groups of thirty and The only common occurrence of a comes from the application of the
twenty-two raters, they report relia- large group evaluating landscape Spearman-Brown prophecy formula
bilities of .64 to .99 for group-mean scenes is to create mean ratings for a to determine the composite reliability
ratings. single attribute (e.g., scenic beauty or for a group of any size based on the
Extensive research on forest preference), normally for research mean intra-group reliability. Kopka
landscape aesthetics has developed purposes. and Ross (1984) also evaluated the
during the past thirty years. Much of inter-rater reliability of BLM’s "level
this research uses the scenic beauty of influence" procedure applied to
estimation (SBE) method (Daniel existing scenes. The findings from
and Boster 1976), a statistical tech- these two independent studies con-
nique that standardizes each rater’s cerning the assessment reliability of
scores in order to control the varia- formalistic design qualities in the
tion in how a rater "anchors" the rat- landscape are summarized in Table 1.
ing scale. The reliability of group- They fall substantially short of
mean ratings is frequently reported,

168 Landscape Journal


Table 1. Inter-rater reliability for BLM’s level of influence variables. Inter-Group and Inter-Rater Reliability of
Scenic Quality
Variable Kopka & Ross 1984 Feimer et al. 1981 Most landscape assessment
research has sought to explain why
Form .54 .45 people like certain landscapes more
Line .63 .19 than others (Zube, Sell and Taylor
Color .25 .13 1982). The term used to describe this
Texture .53 .41 liking has varied among authors.
Zube (1974) refers to scenic value,
the Kaplans (1989, 1998) use prefer-
acceptable levels, even for ticular purpose. "Preferential ratings
exploratory research, let alone for ence, and Daniel and Boster (1976)
[judgments] reflect all sorts of indi-
professional assessments. choose scenic beauty. These terms
vidual and subgroup tastes, inclina-
Craik (1972; Brush 1976) sug- have been found to be essentially the
tions, and dispositions" (Craik 1972,
gests that a distinction be made same (Zube et al. 1974). Because sce-
p. 257). High inter-rater reliability nic quality, preference or beauty is so
between evaluative appraisals and may be an indication that an attrib- widely measured, it is possible to use
preferential judgments. Appraisals ute assessment is understood to be an this measure to compare the relative
are based on a widely-accepted and appraisal using common culturally
culturally-determined standard. size of inter- and intra-group reliabili-
accepted criteria, while low reliability ties. It also creates a standard for
"Esthetic appraisals reflect the lay- may indicate a more personal judg- comparison to other attributes:
man’s attempt to employ a commonly ment (Brush 1976). For instance, Methods. The inter-group and
but imperfectly understood external Coughlin and Goldstein (1970) inter-rater reliability of landscape
standard" (Craik 1972, p. 257). In obtained higher inter-rater reliabili- preference is evaluated based on data
contrast, personal judgments are ties for attractiveness than for resi- from four previous research efforts.
more specialized assessments from a dential or sightseeing preference rat- Palmer and Smardon (1989) surveyed
particular perspective and for a par- ings of a series of scenes.

Table 2. Study datasets used to compare inter-group and inter-rater reliability.


Sites Represented Participants
Location Media Year n Format Type Selection Year n Study Citation

Juneau, AK B/W offset press ’86 16 Rating-9 Residents Random ’86 406 Palmer & Smardon 1989
Juneau, AK B/W offset press ’86 t6 Rating-9 Public meeting Random ’86 41 ibid.
Dennis, MA Color photos ’76 56 Q-sort-7 Registered voter Random ’76 68 Palmer 1983
Dennis, MA B/W offset press ’76 56 Q-sort-7 Town list Random ’87 34 Palmer 1997
Dennis, MA Color slides ’76 56 Rating-10 Resident Available ’96 31 unpublished
Dennis, MA B/W offset press ’76 56 Q-sort-7 Env. Prof. Selected ’83 5t Palmer t985
Dennis, MA B/W offset press ’76 56 Q-sort-7 Env. Prof. Selected ’85 67 Palmer 1985
White Mtn, NH Color web press ’92 64 Rating-10 Residents Random ’95 77 Palmer 1998
White Mtn, NH Color web press ’92 64 Rating-10 USFS prof. Random ’95 205 ibid.
White Mtn, Nit Color web press ’92 64 Rating-10 Opinion leaders Census ’94/5 97 ibid.
US impact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Austrian students Available ’90 59 Palmer et al. t990
US ~mpact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 French students Available ’87 99 ibid.
US ampact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 German students Available ’88/9 47 ibid.
US tmpact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Hong Kong stds. Available ’90 53 ibid.
US ~mpact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Italian students Available ’87 26 ibid.
US ~mpact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Japanese students Available ’87 52 ibid.
US ampact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Korean students Available ’87 128 ibid.
US ampact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Puerto Rican stds. Available ’87 14 ibid.
US ~mpact pmrs Color webpress n/a 32 Compare-100 Spanish students Available ’87 100 ibid.
US ampact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Utah students Available ’88 40 ibid.
US ~mpact pmrs Color webpress n/a 32 Compare-100 Yugoslav students Available ’87 47 ibi&
US ~mpact pmrs Color web press n/a 32 Compare-100 Central NY stds. Available ’87 59 ibid.

Palmer 169
Table 3. Inter-group reliability of scenic ratings for Dennis, Massachusetts.
Citizen ’76 Citizen ’87 Citizen ’96 Env. Prof. ’83 Env. Prof. ’85

Citizens ’76 -- -0.947 0.904 0.955 0.952


Citizens ’87 0.947 -- -0.941 0.940 0.942
Citizens ’96 0.904 0.941 -- -0.899 0.895
Env. Prof. ’83 0.955 0.940 0.899 -- -0.992
Env. Prof. ’85 0.952 0.942 0.895 0.992 --

a random sample of residents and forests, and environmental profes- In the White Mountain
attendees at a public workshop to sionals stationed in National Forests clearcutting study, the inter-group
study the human-use values of wet- in the northeastern quarter of the correlation of citizens with opinion
lands in Juneau, Alaska. The survey United States. The final study leaders is .980, and with Forest Ser-
included sixteen photos representing involves twelve groups of college stu- vice professionals it is .978. The cor-
the range of local wetland types and dents from around the world (Palmer relation between opinion leaders and
conditions. The second study began et al. 1990). They evaluated sixteen Forest Service employees is .974. The
as part of a community effort to matched simulations from the north- correlations among the four groups of
develop a comprehensive plan for eastern and southwestern United opinion leaders are shown in Table 4,
Dennis, Massachusetts. In 1976, a States portraying pre- and post- with an average correlation of .894.
random sample of registered voters impact conditions. Citations for these Table 5 shows the correlations among
evaluated fifty-six photographs repre- data-sets and the general characteris- the seven groups of Forest Service
senting the town (Palmer 1983). Resi- tics of the respondents and simulation environmental professionals. Their
dents evaluated the same scenes in media are summarized in Table 2. average correlation is .971.
1987 and 1996 (Palmer 1997). These Results. The correlation between In Table 6 are the correlations
ratings are compared to those from the mean scenic ratings of Juneau between twelve student groups from
employees of the U.S. Army Corps of residents and workshop attendees is around the world. Even with such
Engineers which were gathered in .971 for scenic ratings of sixteen diverse respondent groups, the aver-
preparation for a training course in diverSe wetlands. Table 3 shows the age inter-group correlation is .804.
landscape aesthetics (Palmer 1985). correlations among groups evaluating The lowest correlation is .496
The third study evaluated simula- the Dennis scenes. The average cor- between students from Japan and
tions of different harvesting intensi- relation among the five groups is Germany, while the highest is .969
ties, patterns, and patch sizes of .937. The highest correlation is .992 between the Austrian and German
clearcuts in the White Mountain between the two groups of environ- students. The inter-group correla-
National Forest (Palmer 1998). mental professionals, and the lowest tions from these four studies indicate
Respondents included a random sam- is .895 between 1996 citizens and why the quality of landscape assess-
ple of regional.residents, opinion lead- 1985 professionals. ments enjoys such a high reputa-
ers in the management of the area’s tion-most measures of reliability
meet the highest standards, and all
but a very few meet standards of
acceptability.

Table 4. Inter-group reliability among opinion leaders’ scenic ratings for clearcutting alternatives in the
White Mountains, New Hampshire.
Appalachian Forest Resources North Country Roundtable on
Trail Council Steering Committee Council Forest Law
Appalachian Trail Council -- -0.934 0.890 0.881
Forest Resources Steering Com. 0.934 -- -0.902 0.890
North Country Council 0.890 0.902 -- -0.868
Roundtable on Forest Law 0.881 0.890 0.868 --

170 Landscape Journal


Table 5. Inter-group reliability among USFS employees’ scenic ratings for clearcutting alternatives in the
White Mountains, New Hampshir,e.
Archaeol. Engineer Forester Land Arch Manager Rec Spec Wild,Bio.
Archaeologist -- -0.985 0.968 0.937 0.948 0.973 0.977
Engineer 0.985 -- -0.973 0.947 0.950 0.975 0.980
Forester 0.968 0.973 -- -0.970 0.984 0.995 0.989
Landscape Arch 0.937 0.947 0.970 -- -0.986 0.975 0.945
Management 0.948 0.950 0.984 0.986 -- -0.987 0.960
Recreation Spec 0.973 0.975 0.995 0.975 0.987 -- -0.984
Wildlife Biologist 0.977 0.980 0,989 0.945 0.960 0.984 --

However, the inter-rater relia- If landscape assessments are and scenic quality. His landscape
bilities in Table 7 are much less made primarily by individuals and not dimensions were all measured from
encouraging. The average inter-rater by large panels of evaluators, then USGS 1:24,000 topography or Massa-
correlation is .307 for Juneau resi- these results indicate that the relia- chusetts MapDown land use maps.
dents, and .355 for workshop atten- bility of scenic assessments is unlikely Palmer (1996) used a similar
dees. That is approximately one-third to be reaching acceptable levels. The approach to validate a GIS model of
the inter-rater correlation between next sections will consider the relia- spaciousness. A regression analysis
the two groups. The average of the bility of other visual qualities. found landscape dimensions
intra-group correlations for the five explained approximately half of the
Dennis study groups is .608. Again, Inter-group and Inter-rater Reliability of variation in Zube’s perceived scenic
this is a substantial drop in reliability Landscape Dimensions value and Palmer’s perceived spa-
from the average inter-group correla- Zube (1974) defines landscape ciousness.
tion of .937. The average inter-rater dimensions "as physical characteris- Shafer (1969) employed a dif-
correlation is .554 among the three tics or attributes of the landscape ferent approach to measure land-
major groups in the White Mountain which can be measured using either scape dimensions. He divided an eye-
study. This is down from an average normal ratio scales or psychometric level photograph into foreground,
inter-group correlation of .977. The scaling." Examples of such dimen- middle ground and background. Then
average of the twelve inter-rater cor- sions include: percent tree or water the area and perimeter of content
relations from the international study cover, length or area of the view, rela- areas were measured in each zone.
is .427, down from an average inter- tive elevation change, and various Examples of content include water,
group correlation of .804. edge and contrast indices. These trees, buildings, ground cover, and
dimensions bear a remarkable resem- pavement. This approach to measur-
blance to those employed by quantita- ing landscape dimensions also
tive landscape ecologists today accounts for approximately half the
(Tnrner and Gardner 1991). Zube variation in visual preference.
investigated the relationship between
twenty-three landscape dimensions

Table 6. Inter-group reliability in a multi-national study of visual impact perceptions.


Au CNY Fr Gr HK It Ja Ko PR Sp Ut Yu
Austrian -- -0.958 0.952 0.969 0.681 0.863 0.604 0.723 0.802 0.934 0.891 0.906
Central NY 0.958 -- -0.928 0.952 0.709 0.881 0.641 0.750 0.813 0.945 0.914 0.923
French 0.952 0.928 -- -0.958 0.631 0.784 0.599 0.712 0.712 0.902 0.870 0.887
German 0.969 0.952 0.958 -- -0.587 0.813 0.496 0.661 0.731 0.880 0.832 0.853
Hong Kong 0.681 0.709 0.631 0.587 -- -0.800 0.791 0.704 0.765 0.790 0.855 0.821
Italian 0.863 0.881 0.784 0.813 0.800 -- -0.714 0.731 0.853 0.733 0.783 0.796
Japanese 0.604 0.641 0.599 0.496 0.791 0.714 -- -0.843 0.619 0.733 0.783 0.844
Korean 0.723 0.750 0.712 0.661 0.704 0.731 0.843 -- -0.663 0.821 0.800 0.844
Puerto Rican 0.802 0.813 0.712 0.731 0.765 0.853 0.619 0.663 -- -0.782 0.767 0.855
Spanish 0.934 0.945 0.902 0.880 0.790 0.855 0.733 0.821 0.782 -- -0.963 0.933
Utah 0.891 0.914 0.870 0.832 0.855 0.847 0.783 0.800 0.767 0.963 -- -0.916
Yugoslav 0.906 0.923 0.887 0.853 0.821 0.902 0.796 0.844 0.855 0.933 0.916 --

Palmer 171
Table 7. Inter-Rater Reliability of Scenic Ratings from Four Studies.
Location Respondents n Mean 95%-ile Median 5%-ile

Juneau, AK Residents 406 .307 .793 .424 -.249


Public meeting attendees 41 .355 .824 .490 -.400
Dennis, MA Registered voter 68 .603 .824 .636 .272
Town list 34 .563 .837 .606 .076
Residents 31 .539 .853 .655 -. 198
Env. Prof. in Corps of Engineers 51 .635 .818 .674 .315
Env. prof. in Corps of Engineers. 67 .701 .971 .807 .089
White Mtn, NH Citizens 69 .512 .800 .593 -.067
Opinion leaders 95 .532 .812 .630 -.147
Appalachian Trail Council 24 .574 .840 .709 -.645
Forest Res. Steering Committee 18 .344 .737 .441 -.321
North Country Council 43 .653 .833 .701 .275
Roundtable on Forest Law 10 .554 .815 .676 -.095
USFS employees 205 .619 .837 .672 .215
Archaeologist 9 .690 .820 .697 .543
Engineer 13 .537 .814 .598 .122
Forester 87 .601 .830 .658 .798
Landscape Architect 10 .684 .882 .735 .434
Management 30 .584 .838 .651 .002
Recreation Specialist 20 .687 .848 .706 .457
Wildlife Biologist 36 .666 .839 .717 .216
US impact pairs Austrian students 59 .602 .827 .646 .176
French students 29 .451 .774 .461 .090
German students 47 .543 .844 .598 .002
Hong Kong students 53 .344 .675 .383 -.154
Italian students 26 .314 .699 .322 -. 147
Japanese students 52 .347 .704 .390 -.180
Korean students 128 .248 .606 .251 -.121
Puerto Rican students 14 .273 .685 .364 -.274
Spanish students 100 .505 .785 .528 .149
Utah State students 40 .469 .805 .497 .049
Yugoslav students 47 .450 .74 t .479 .061
SUNY ESF students 59 .583 .812 .624 .226

The approaches developed by Methods. Respondents are thirty Naturalism refers to aspects of the
Zube and Shafer use physical tools to advanced landscape architecture or landscape that could exist without
measure the landscape’s dimensions. environmental science students at human care. Nature is an expres-
Human judgment can also be used to State University of New York’s Col- sion of how much vegetation is in a
estimate these measurements, for lege of Environmental Science and view, how organic are its elements
instance the relative area of a view and patterns, and how uncontrolled
Forestry in 1997 and 1998. They eval- are the natural processes.
covered by forest. However, when uated offset printed photographs of Development refers to aspects of
people are used as the measuring Dennis, Massachusetts taken in 1976. the landscape that are human cre-
device, more complicated constructs They were instructed to identify the ations. Development is an expres-
can also be measured, such as natu- highest, lowest, and intermediate sion of human control over natural
ralism or spaciousness. It is the relia- quality scenes and describe the crite- processes or patterns, and the dom-
bility of using people to measure ria for their decisions. Using these inance of structures, such as build-
landscape dimensions that is tested in scenes as anchor points on a seven- ings, roads, or dams.
this section. point scale, they sorted the remaining
scenes among the seven rating levels.
Each quality was evaluated on a dif-
ferent day. The four landscape dimen-
sions were described as follows:

172 Landscape Journal


Spaciousness is the landscape’s hypothesis is that humans have It is suggested that the land-
enclosure or expansiveness. It evolved to seek and understand infor- scape dimensions are descriptions of
describes how much room there is mation in a particular type of land- physical condition. However, the four
to wander in the view, or how far scape, namely the savanna (Kaplan Preference Matrix variables describe
you could go before you reach the
and Kaplan 1982, p. 75-77). As such, our experience and interpretation of
boundaries.
the preference for visual conditions information in the landscape. In a
Preference is how much you like or
dislike a landscape. It is also called that enhance the acquisition of infor- sense they are more removed from an
scenic quality, attractiveness, or mation in savanna-like landscapes is objective physical condition and
beauty. People’s preferences are in the genes, so to speak. closer to a psychological outcome.
descriptions of their personal expe- The Kaplans posit a framework Methods. The respondents, photo-
rience. that characterizes information from graphs, and procedures are the same
two perspectives. First, information as those used for the landscape dimen-
Results. The inter-rater correla-
contributes to either understanding sions ratings. The students were given
tions reported in Table 8 show that
or exploration. "Understanding a reading assignment (Kaplan and
acceptable levels of reliability are
refers to the desire people have to Kaplan 1989, p. 49-58) in preparation
achieved for perceived naturalism
make sense of their world, to compre- for a thirty minute lecture about the
(.796), development (.762), and spa-
hend what goes on around them. Kaplan’s information framework. As
ciousness (.715). The response pat-
Understanding provides a sense of before, students rated the attributes
terns for naturalism and development
security.... People want to explore, on different days. The four informa-
are nearly mirror images of each
to expand their horizons and find out tional attributes were described in the
other. The reliability of development
what lies ahead. They seek more instructions as follows:
is brought down somewhat by one
information and look for new chal- Coherence is the landscape’s order-
student whose responses correlate
lenges" (Kaplan, Kaplan & Ryan liness or confusion. It describes how
negatively with the other evaluators.
1998, p. 10)¯ Second, visual informa- well a view "hangs together," or
The reliability of the preference
tion is presented in two forms: two- how easy it is to understand what
ratings (.582) in Table 8 is compara-
dimensional and three-dimensional. you see. It is enhanced by anything
ble to that for scenic preference from
Two-dimensional information that helps organize the patterns of
the other studies presented in this light, size, texture, or other ele-
"involves the direct perception of the
paper. However, it is substantially ments into a few major units.
elements in the scene in terms of
lower than for the three landscape Complexity is the landscape’s
their number, grouping, and place-
dimensions. intricacy or simplicity. It describes
ment .... When viewing scenes, peo- how much is going on in a view;
ple not only infer a third dimension,

Table 8. Inter-rater Reliability for Perceived Landscape Dimensions in Dennis, MA.


Attribute n Mean 95%-ile Median 5%-ile

Naturalism 30 .796 .916 .826 .466


Development 30 .762 .9 l0 .828 -.046
Spaciousness 30 .715 .859 .728 .514
Preference 30 .582 .823 .613 .226

but imagine themselves in the scene. how many eletnents of different


Inter-group and Inter-rater Reliability of
¯.. involve the inference of what kinds it contains. It is the promise
Informational Content
being in the pictured space would of further information, if only there
The Kaplans (1982, 1989, 1998)
entail" (Kaplan et al. 1998, p. 13). were more time to look at it from
have proposed an elegant theory that the present vantage point.
Four information concepts are
relates the informational content of a Legibility is whether a landscape
scene to its preference. It is related to derived from this framework. Two-
dimensional understanding is called is memorable or indistinguishable.
Appleton’s (1975) prospect-refuge It describes how easy it would be to
coherence, and two-dimensional
theory of environmental preference¯ find one’s way around the view; how
exploration is complexity. Three- easy it would be to figure out where
They both refer to adaptive pressures
dimensional understanding is legibil- one is at any given moment or to
during human evolution to lend
ity, and three-dimensional explo- find one’s way back to any given
authority to their theories¯ Their
ration is mystery. The Kaplans call place.
this two-by-two classification grid the
Preference Matrix. It is thought that
people prefer landscapes that are
coherent, complex, legible, and mys-
terious.

Palmer 173
Mystery in the landscape is the (USDI 1980; Smardon et al. 1988). Line is a path, real or imagined,
result of incomplete perception. It More recent manuals developed in that the eye follows when perceiv-
describes the extent to which fur- Great Britain build on the work of ing abrupt differences in color or
ther information is promised to the Dame Sylvia Crowe and demonstrate texture, or when objects are
obsetwer if she were to walk deeper aligned in a one-ditnensional
how a more complete palette of aes-
into the scene. This is not a prom- sequence, described by its boldness,
thetic factors can be used to describe
ise of surprise, but of information complexity, and orientation. It is
that has continuity with what is and evaluate landscapes (Lucas 1990; usually evident as the edge of forms
already available. Bell 1993). in the landscape. Bold vertical lines
Methods. Respondents were which interrupt the skyline tend to
These definitions are based on twenty-five professionals in a two-day dominate weak horizontal lines.
descriptions given by the Kaplans and visual assessment continuing educa- Texture is small forms or color
their graduate students (Kaplan and tion course taught in early-December mixtures aggregated into a contin-
Kaplan 1989; Herzog 1989; Herzog, 1997 in Albany, New York. The indi- uous surface pattern. The aggrega-
Kaplan and Kaplan 1982). vidual contrast ratings are the maxi- tion is sufficient that the parts do
Results. Table 9 lists the reliabil- mum contrast from the land/water, not appear as discrete objects in
ities obtained for the informational the composition of the scene. Tex-
vegetation, or structures components
attributes. These reliabilities are tures are described by their grain,
of the landscape. The ratings involve density, regularity, and internal
highest for the exploratory variables two visual principles, contrast and contrast. Coarse and high-contrast
complexity (.315) and mystery (.262). dominance. Visual elements are the textures tend to dominate fine-
The three-dimensional variables, source of visual contrast in the land- grained textures of low internal
legibility (.214) and ~nystery (.26), scape, creating the patterns that we contrast.
are higher than the two-dimensional see. An object may differ from its set- Scale is the relative size of an
variables. The lowest reliability was ting or other objects in one or more object in relation to its surrounding
found for the two-dimensional under- element. When there is significant landscape. The scale may be in
standing variable, coherence(. 186). contrast in one or more of the ele- relation to the landscape setting as
However, all of these intra-group reli- a whole, the proportion of field-of-
ments, one object may dominate
abilities are unacceptably low. view, or other distinct objects.
other parts of the landscape. The con- Large, heavy, massive objects
trast or dominance of the following within a confined space dominate
Inter-Group and Inter-Rater Reliability of six visual elements are evaluated: small, light, delicate objects in
Compositional Elements more expansive settings.
A common approach to assess- Color is the major visual property
of surfaces attributed to reflected Space is the three-dimensional
ing visual impacts involves evaluating arrangement of objects and voids.
light of a particular intensity and
the amount of change in the scene’s wavelength. Described by its hue Compositions are described as
visual composition. Litton (1968; (tint or wavelength), value (light or panoramic, enclosed, feature, focal,
USDA 1973) initiated what became or canopied. Position of objects or

Table 9. Inter-rater reliability for informational attributes of the Dennis, MA landscape.


Attribute n Mean 95%-ile Median 5%-ile

Coherence 28 .186 .518 .227 -.205


Complexity 28 .315 .727 .350 -.358
Legibility 28 .214 .630 .224 -.246
Mystery 28 .262 .581 .278 -.081

the common use of form, line, color, darkness), and chroma (saturation view in the landscape is relative to
and texture as the attributes most or brilliance). Lighter, warmer, topography. Backdrop is the sky,
brighter colors tend to "advance", water, or land background against
commonly used to describe landscape
while darker, cooler, duller colors which objects are seen. Objects
character and change. In particular; which occupy vulnerable positions
procedures have been developed that tend to "retreat" in a scene. Dark
next to light tends to attract the within spatial compositions, which
evaluate changes in contrast associ- are high in the landscape, and/or
eye and this contrast becomes a
ated with forIn, line, color, texture, as visual focal point. which are seen against the sky dom-
well as scale and spatial dominance Form is the mass or shape of an inate in the scene. The sum of the
object or objects which appear uni- contrast and dominance ratings is
fied. Forms are described by their used to create an index of visual
geometry, complexity, and orienta- impact severity.
tion in the landscape. Forms that
are bold, regular, solid, or vertical
tend to be dominant in the land-
scape.

174 Landscape Journal


The evaluation forms are ity of the index of visual impact genetically based or human evolu-
adapted from those prepared by severity is even more reliable (.664). tionary significance in their meaning.
Smardon and his colleagues (1981) Reliability of an index is normally Perhaps their relevance to our every-
for the Bureau of Land Management. higher than the reliability of its com- day survival has changed as our cul-
These judgments were made for five ponents (Nunnally 1978). However, tural and environmental conditions
sets of pre- and post-impact slide these levels still fall well below pro- have changed. Another possibility is
pairs. The impacts evaluated were fessionally acceptable levels. that static photographs are inade-
eleven wind turbines installed in Ver- quate to effectively trigger our infor-
mont as seen from 1.25 and 4.0 miles, Discussion mation-seeking instincts. Perhaps
a forest view 15 percent of which is In general, landscape attributes more attention needs to be paid to
harvested in four or five acre that have a more denotative charac- how" movement through the land-
clearcuts and another in twenty-five ter seem to have greater inter-rater scape influences both landscape pref-
to thirty acre clearcuts in the White reliability than those with a more erence and information-seeking
Mountains, and a new on-ramp to a connotative charactex: Denotative behavior.
limited access highway near Bing- attributes provide clear designation At least with regard to scenic
hamton, New York. or referential meaning. These are quality, these results also indicate
Results. The reliability of the generally agreed upon "objective" that evaluations from landscape pro-
compositional attributes is shown in facts. They are particularly appropri- fessionals are more reliable than
Table 10. The average inter-rater ate for evaluative appraisals. Conno- from public individuals. Table 11
reliability of scenic value ratings for tative attributes provide emotive or compares the average individual (i.e.,
the nine slides was .539, which is metaphorical meaning. They include inter-rater) reliability for profes-
comparable, though slightly lower any suggestion or implication beyond sional and public respondents to the
than the scenic value ratings reported denotative meaning. This more per- Dennis, Massachusetts and White
above. There is a range of reliability sonal attribution suggests that conno- Mountains studies. This is one aspect
for the contrast ratings. Unaccept- tative meaning is largely a matter of of Carlson’s (1977) argument that
ably low contrast reliabilities are preferential judgments. Naturalism, landscape evaluations should be left
found for scale (.280) and line (.423). development, and spaciousness seem to specially trained environmental
The contrast reliabilities for color to be examples of denotative attrib- professionals. However; the difference

Table 10. Inter-rater Reliability of Landscape Composition Attributes.


Rating n Mean 95%-ile Median 5%-ile

Scenic Value 25 .539 .912 .592 -.005


Contrast (Sum) 25 .630 .950 .681 .193
Color Contrast 25 .503 .952 .583 -.272
Form Contrast 25 .563 .923 .664 -. 144
Line Contrast 25 .423 .922 .497 -.392
Scale Contrast 25 .280 .885 .395 -.608
Texture Contrast 25 .620 .948 .716 -. 154
Scale Dominance 25 .561 .923 .620 .000
Spatial Dominance 25 .376 .896 .463 -.453
Visual Impact Severity 25 .664 .960 .742 .006

(.503), and form (.563) are minimal. utes. Landscape preference and the is not so great as to nullify the useful-
Only texture contrast (.620) has a compositional elements form, line, ness of evaluations by random sam-
minimally acceptable reliability. colo~; and texture may be in a gray ples of the public, nor can it be used
These results are comparable to area between connotative and deno- to justify evaluations by only one or
those from the studies summarized in tative meaning. The reliability of two professionals.
Table 1. The reliability of scale domi- coherence, complexity, mystery, and There are several possible rea-
nance (.561) and spatial dominance legibility is sufficiently low to suggest sons that occur to the author for the
(.376) is also low. that they have highly connotative generally poor results reported here.
The sum of these five contrast meaning. One possibility is that photographs
ratings forms a contrast index that is The relatively poor reliability of
more reliable (.630) than any of its the information variables may con-
individual components. The reliabil- tribute to the discussion of nature
versus nurture, whether there is a

Palmer 175
scape assessments are most com-
Table 11. Inter-rater reliability of scenic ratings by professionals and the public.
monly made by a single professional.
Dennis, MA White Mtns. Published evaluations of the reliabil-
ity of a single evaluator (i.e., intra-
Professionals .668 .619 group reliability) for various land-
Public .568 .512 scape qualities do not ever meet pro-
fessional standards (i.e., greater than
provide insufficient information or For some time, the practice of .9) and normally fall below minimally
that different people fill in missing landscape assessment has been domi- acceptable levels (i.e., .7). Table 9
information differently. This is a nated by methods that use rating shows the number of evaluators
question of the validity of photo- scales or checklists. The mixed find- needed for each of the landscape
graphic representations. Many stud- ings reported here suggest that it qualities considered in this paper.
ies have reported that photographs may be appropriate to investigate the The size of these evaluation panels is
appear to be valid representations, reliability of other landscape assess- determined by applying the Spear-
but recent work by Hoffman (1997; ment methods. Those who used rat- man-Brown Prophecy Formula (Nun-
Hoffman and Palmer 1994) suggests ing scales did so because they sought nally 1978; Feimer et al. 1979; Ander-
that there are serious validity con- reliable results; they never made son et al. 1976).
cerns under some circumstances. claims to uncover deep meaning. In Several recommendations seem
Another possible explanation is that contrast, other researchers chose to appropriate given these findings.
the instructions describing the land- develop qualitative methods to search Research involving landscape assess-
scape attributes to be evaluated were for deeper meaning and to gain a ments by the public or professionals
must include: (1) field validations of

Table 12. The number of assessors needed to obtain minimally and professionally reliable ratings of landscape attributes.
Number of assessors to reach:
Attribute Tested reliability .9 reliability .7 reliability
Scenic value .582 7 2
Naturalism .796 3 l
Development .762 3 1
Spaciousness .715 4 1
Complexity .315 20 6
Mystery .262 25 7
Legibility .214 33 9
Coherence .186 40 11
Color Contrast .503 9 3
Form Contrast .563 7 2
Line Contrast .423 13 4
Scale Contrast .280 24 6
Texture Contrast .620 6 2
Scale Dominance .561 7 2
Spatial Dominance .376 15 4

not understood or were inadequate in richer understanding from the land-


other ways. For instance, only written scape. Perhaps it is time to also inves- photographic representations when
or oral instructions are typically given tigate and refine the reliability of possible; (2) validation of the evalua-
to respondents. It seems reasonable these methods. They may be just as tion instructions; and (3) use of pho-
that some form ofvisuat instructions reliable as some of the rating scales! tographs or images to help explain
may be necessary to provide more the landscape attributes being evalu-
reliable evaluations of visible attrib- Conclusions ated. The professional application of
utes. Finally, low retiabilities may be The results presented here give landscape assessments must include:
an indication of low salience. Some of reason for some concern in the way (1) multiple trait~ed evaluators; (2) a
these landscape attributes may be landscape assessments are conducted reliability assessment of the evalua-
fuzzy concepts or constructs that in both research and practice. Land- tions; and (3) field validation of photo-
need further development to reach graphic representations when possible.
professional standards of reliable
assessment.

176 Landscape Journal


Burry-Stock,J. A., D. G. Shaw, C. Laurire and Guilford,J. P. 1954. Psychometric Methods. New
Acknowledgments B. S. Chissom. 1996. "Rater Agreement York: McGraw-Hill.
Indexes for Performance Assessment." Herzog, T. R. 1987. "A cognitive analysis of
Previously unpublished data used for this
Educational and Psychological Measurement preference for natural environments:
research were funded by the North Central
56(2): 25t-262. mountains, canyons, and deserts." Land-
Forest Research Station, Chicago, Illinois.
Carlson, A. A. 1977. "On the possibility of scape Journal 6(2): 140-152.
Slides for the landscape compositional assess-
quantifying scenic beauty." Landscape ¯ 1989. "A cognitive analysis of prefer-
ment were provided by Vermont Environmen-
Planning 4(2): 131-172. ence for urban nature."Journal of Envi-
tal Research Associates, Waterbury Center,
Coughlin, R. E. and K. A. Goldstein. 1970. ronmentalPsychology 9(1): 27-43.
Vermont and Integrated Site, Syracuse, New
"The Extent of Agreement Among , S. Kaplan and R. Kaplan 1982. "The
York.
Observers on Environmental Attractive- prediction of preference for unfamiliar
Ness." Discussion Paper 37. Philadel- urban places." Population and Environment
phia: Regional Science Research Insti- 5(1): 43-59.
tute. Hetherington,J., T.C. Daniel and T.C. Brown.
Craik, C. H. 1972. "Psychological Factors in 1993. "Is motion more important than it
Note Landscape Appraisal." Environment and sounds?: the medium of presentation in
Behavior 4(3): 255-266. environmental perception research."
1. The intraclass correlation (ICC) is an alter- . 1983. "The psychology of large scale Journal of Environmental Psychology 13 (4):
native approach to estimating reliability that environments. In Environmental Psychol- 283-291.
normally gives roughly comparable values ogy: Directions and Perspectives. New York: Hoffrnan, R.E. 1997. Testing the validity and
(Ebel 1951;Jones et al. 1983). The ICC is cal- Praeger Publications. reliability of slides as representations of
culated using variance components from an -. and N. R. Feimer. 1979. "Setting Tech- northern hardwood forest conditions.
ANOVA table. There is an extensive literature nical Standards for Visual Assessment (Ph.D. dissertation). SUNY College of
on the appropriate valance components to Procedures." In Our National Landscape. Environmental Science and Forestry.
include in an ICC (e.g., Shrout & Fleiss 1979). General Technical Report. PSW-35. ., andJ. F. Palmer. 1994. "Validity of
Burry-Stock et al. (1996) considers ICC calcu- Berkeley, CA: USDA Forest Service, using photographs to represent visible
lations "beyond the statistical expertise of the Pacific Southwest Forest and Range qualities of forest environments." In
average observer using observation or perform- Experiment Station. History and Culture: Proceedings of the Coun-
ance data" in their research. Daniel, T. C., L. M. Anderson, H. W. Schroeder cil of Educators in Landscape Architecture
and L. Wheeler III. 1977. "Mapping the 1994 Coherence. Washington, D. C.:
Scenic Beauty of Forest Landscapes." Landscape Architecture
Leisure Sciences 1 (1): 335-52. Foundation/Council of Educators in
., and R. S. Boster. 1976. "Measuring Landscape Architecture.
Landscape Esthetics: the Scenic Beauty Hull, R. B., IV and G.J. Buhyoft: 1984. "Indi-
References Estimation Method." Research Paper vidual and group reliability of landscape
Anderson, T. W., E. H. Zube and W. P. Mac- RM-167. Fort Collins, CO: USDA Forest assessments." Landscape Planning 11 (!):
Connell. 1976. "Predicting scenic Service, Rocky Mountain Forest and 67-71.
Resource Values." In Studies in Landscape Range Experiment Station. Jones, A. P., L. A.Johnson, M. C. Butler and
Perception. Amherst: Institute for Man ., T. C. Brown, D. A. King, M. T. D. S. Main. 1983. "Apples and oranges:
and Environment, University of Massa- Richards, and W. P. Stewart. 1989. "Per- an emperical comparison of commonly
chusetts. ceived Scenic Beauty and Contingent used indices of interrater agreement."
Appleton, J. 1975. The Experience of Landscape. Valuation of Forest Campgrounds." For- Academy of Management Journal 26(3):
London: Wiley. est Science, 35(1): 76-90. 507-519.
--. 1984. "Prospects and Refuges Re- Ebel, R. L. 1951. "Estimation of the reliability Kaplan, R., and S. Kaplan. 1989. The Experience of
visited." Landscape Journal 3 (XX): 91-103. of ratings." Psychometrica 16 (4): 407-424. Nature: A Psychological Perspective. New
Bell, S. 1993. Elements of Visual Design in the Feimer, N. R., K. H. Craik, R. C. Smardon, and York: Cambridge University Press.
Landscape. London: E & FN Spon. S. R.J. Sheppard. 1979. Evaluating the ., S. Kaplan and R. L. Ryan. t998. With
Brook, I. 1998. "Goethean Science as a Way to Effiectiveness of Observer Based Visual People in Mind. Washington, D. C: Island
Read Landscape." Landscape Research Resource and hnapact Assessment Press.
23(1): 51-69. Methods." In Our National Landscape. Kaplan, S., and R. Kaplan. 1982. Cognition and
Brown, T. C., and T. C. Daniel. 1987. "Context General Technical Report. PSW-35. Environment. New York: Cambridge Uni-
Effects in Perceived Environmental Berkeley, CA.: USDA Forest Service, versity Press.
Quality Assessment: Scene Selection Pacific Southwest Forest and Range Kopka, S., and M. Ross. 1984. "A study of the
and Landscape Ratings."Journal of Envi- Experiment Station. reliability of the Bureau of Land Man-
ronmental Psychology 7(3): 233-250. , R. C. Smardon, and K. H. Craik. 1981. agement visual resource assessment
Brown, T. C., T. C. Daniel, M. T. Richards and "Evaluating the effectiveness of scheme." Landscape Planning 11 (2):
D. A. King. 1988. "Recreation Participa- observer based visual resource and 161-166.
tion and the Validity of Photo-based impact assessment methods." Landscape Litton, B: R.,Jr. 1968. "Forest landscape
Preference Judgements."Journal of Research 6(1): 12-16. description and inventories--a basis for
Leisure Research 20(4): 40-60. Gobster, P. H., and R. E. Chenoweth. 1989. land planning and design." Research
Brush, R. O. 1976. "Perceived Quality of Scenic "The dimensions of aesthetic prefer- Paper PSW-49. Berkeley, CA: SUDA
and Recreational Environments." In Per- ence: a quantitative analysis."Journal of Forest Service, Pacific Southwest Forest
ceiving Environmental Quality: Research and Environmental Management 29 (1): 47-72. and Range Experiment Station.
Applications. New York: Plenum Press. Great Literature: Personal Library Series. 1992. Lucas, O. W. R. 1991. The Design of Forest Land-
Bnhyoff, G.J., W. A. Leuschner andJ. D. Well- (CD-ROM) Parsippany, NJ: Bureau scapes. New York: Oxford University
man. 1979. "Aesthetic Impacts of South- Development, Inc. Press.
ern Pine Beetle Damage."Journal of Groat, L. 1982. "Meaning in post-modern
Environmental Management 8(3): 261-267. architecture: an examination using the
multiple sorting task."Journal of Environ-
mentalPsychology 2(1): 3-22.

Palmer 177
Norberg-Schulz, C. 1979. Genius Loci: Towards Patsfall, M. R., N. R. Feimer, G.J. Buhyoff, and Smardon, R. C., S. R.J. Sheppard and S. New-
a Phenomenology of Architecture. New York: J. D. Wellamn. 1984. "The prediction of man. 1981. Prototype Visual hnpact Assess-
Rizzoli International Publications. scenic beauty from landscape content ment Manual. Syracuse: SUNY College of
Nunnally, J. C. 1978. Psychometric Theory. New and composition."J0urna/ofEnvironmen- Environmental Science and Forestry.
York: McGraw-Hill. talPsychology 4(1): 7-26. --,J. F. Palmer, A. Knopf, K. Grinde,J. E.
Palmer,J. F. 1983. "Assessment of coastaI wet- Potteiger, M. andJ. Purinton. 1998. Landscape Henderson and L. D. Peyman-Dove.
lands in Dennis, Massachusetts." In The Narratives. New York:John Wiley & 1988. Visual Resources Assessment Procedure
Future of Wetlands: Assessing Visual-Cul- Sons. for US Army Corps of Engineers. Instruction
tural Values of Wetlands. Montclair, New Robinson, W. S. 1950. "Ecological correlations Report EL-88-1. Vicksburg, Mississippi:
Jersey: Allanheld, Osmun Co. and the behavior of individuals." Ameri- U.S. Army Engineer Waterways Experi-
¯ 1985. "The perception of landscape can Sociological Review 15(3): 351-357. ment Station.
visual quality by environmental profes- Rudis, V. A.,J. H. Gramann, E.J. Ruddell, and Turner, M. G., and R. H. Gardner (eds.). 1991.
sionals and local citizens." Syracuse: J. M. Westphal. 1988. "Forest inventory Quantitative methods in landscape ecology: the
Faculty of Landscape Architecture, and management-based visual prefer- analysis and interpretation of landscape het-
SUNY College of Environmental Sci- ence models of southern pine stands." erogeneity. New York: Springer-Verlag.
ence and Forestry. Forest Science 34(4): 846-863. U.S. Department of Agriculture, Forest Ser-
--. 1996. Modeling Spaciousness in the Dutch Schroeder, H.W. 1984. "Environmental percep- vice. 1973. National forest management. Vol.
Landscape. Report 119. Wageningen, The tion rating scales: a case for simple 1. Agriculture Handbook No. 434.
Netherlands: Agricultural Research methods of analysis." Environment and Washington, D. C.: U.S. Government
Department, Winand Staring Centre. Behavior 16(5): 573-598. Printing Office.
--. 1997. "Stability of landscape percep- Shafer, E. L.,J. E. Hamilton and E. A. Schmidt. --. 1995. Landscape Aesthetics: A Handbook for
tions in the face of landscape change." 1969. "Natural landscape preferences: a Scenery Management. Agriculture Hand-
Landscape and Urban Planning 37(1/2): predictive lnodel.’Journal of Leisure book No. 701. Washington, D. C.: USDA
109-113. Research 1(1): 1-19. Forest Service.
--. 1998. Cleareutting in the White Mountains: Shakespeare, W. 1992. Romeo and Juliet. In Great U.S. Department of Interior, Bureau of Land
Perceptions of Citizens, Opinion Leaders and Literature: Personal Library Series. 1992. Management. 1980. VisualResource
U.S. Forest Service Employees. [NYCFRD (CD-ROM) Parsippany, NJ: Bureau Management Program. Washington, D. C.:
98-0i] Syracuse, NY: New York Center Development, Inc. U.S. Government Printing Office.
for Forestry Research and Develop- Shrout, P. E. andJ. L. Fleiss. 1979. "Intraclass Zube, E. H., D. G. Pitt and T. W. Anderson.
ment, SUNY College of Environmental correlations: uses in assessing rater reli- 1974. Perception and Measurement of Scenic
Science and Forestry. ability." Psychological Bulletin 86 (2): Resources in the Southern Connecticut River
--., S. Alonso, K. Dong-hee,J. Gury, Y. 420-428. Valley. Pub. No. R-74-1. Amherst: Insti-
Hernandez, R. Ohno, G. Oneto, A. tute for Man and His Environment,
Pogacnik, and R. Smardon. 1990. "A University of Massachusetts.
multi-national study assessing perceived ,J. L. Sell andJ. G. Taylor. 1982. "Land-
visual i~npacts." Impact Assessment Bulletin scape perception, research, application
8(4): 31-48. and theory." Landscape Planning 9(1):
--, and R.C. Smardon. 1989. "Measuring 1-35.
human values associated with wet-
lands." In Intractable ConJlicts and their
Transformation. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse
University Press.

178 Landscape Journal

S-ar putea să vă placă și