Sunteți pe pagina 1din 16

Personal Relationships, 24 (2017), 146–161. Printed in the United States of America.

Copyright © 2017 IARR; DOI: 10.1111/pere.12173

Attractiveness and relationship longevity: Beauty


is not what it is cracked up to be

CHRISTINE MA-KELLAMS,a,b MARGARET C. WANG,a,c


AND HANNAH CARDIEL
a

a Harvard University; b University of La Verne; and c Santa Clara University

Abstract
Across four studies, we examined the relational repercussions of physical attractiveness (PA). Study 1 (n = 238) found
that those rated as more attractive in high school yearbooks were married for shorter durations and more likely to
divorce. Study 2 (n = 130) replicated these effects using a different sample (high-profile celebrities). Study 3 (n = 134)
examined the link between PA and the derogation of attractive alternatives, a relationship maintenance strategy. Study 4
(n = 156) experimentally manipulated perceived PA and examined its relation with both derogation of attractive
alternatives and current relationship satisfaction. PA predicted likelihood of relationship dissolution and decreased
derogation of attractive alternatives. Furthermore, PA predicted greater vulnerability to relationship threats—in this
case, relationship alternatives—resulting from poor relationship satisfaction.

Research in social psychology has long sug- In the present work, we use a multimethod
gested that physical attractiveness (PA) is a approach—of longitudinal, archival, survey,
powerful motivator and potent predictor of pos- and lab studies—to examine the consequences
itive outcomes across a variety of domains of PA in romantic relationships.
(for review, see Langlois et al., 2000). In the In a nonrelationship context, there is limited
context of close relationships, PA consistently evidence to suggest that PA does not always
emerges as a highly desirable partner trait confer benefits. Namely, physically attrac-
(e.g., Buss, 1989, 1992; Gallup & Freder- tive individuals are more likely to discount
ick, 2010; Swami & Furnham, 2008) over praise from an opposite-sex evaluator (Major,
which relationship partners strive to maintain Carrington, & Carnevale, 1984). Moreover,
positive illusions (Barelds, Dijstra, Kouden- attractive women tend to be perceived less
brug, & Swami, 2011; Swami & Allum, 2012; positively in managerial contexts (Heilman
Swami, Stieger, Haubner, Voracek, & Furn- & Stopeck, 1985), be less trusting of the
ham, 2009). Nevertheless, a limited number of opposite sex in social interactions (Reis et al.,
existing studies have shown that being phys- 1982), and exhibit lower self-esteem during
ically attractive is not without its liabilities. puberty (Zakin, Blyth, & Simmons, 1984). In
a relationship context, studies on the impact of
PA on outcomes have been more inconsistent
Christine Ma-Kellams, Kennedy School, Harvard Univer- in their findings. Here, we focus primarily
sity; Department of Psychology, University of La Verne; on actor effects, or the effects of a person’s
Department of Psychology, Harvard University; Margaret
C. Wang, Department of Psychology, Harvard University; PA on his/her own relationship satisfaction.1
School of Education and Counseling Psychology, Santa In some cases, no relations have emerged
Clara University; Hannah Cardiel, Department of Psychol-
ogy, Harvard University.
Correspondence should be addressed to Christine 1. For a review on partner effects of PA on relationship sat-
Ma-Kellams, University of La Verne, Department of Psy- isfaction, see the meta-analysis by Eastwick, Luchies,
chology, 1950 Third Street, La Verne, CA 91750, e-mail: Finkel, and Hung (2014), which found strong effects
cma-kellams@laverne.edu. of PA on relationship evaluations for both sexes (but

146
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 147

between PA and marital satisfaction (Murstein linked to popularity, dating, and sexual experi-
& Christy, 1976), whereas in other cases, a ence (Perilloux, Cloud, & Buss, 2013; Prokop
positive relation between husband’s PA and & Fedor, 2011, 2013; Rhodes, Simmons, &
marital satisfaction has emerged (Peterson & Peters, 2005). Although these traits may serve
Miller, 1980). In still other studies, the rela- as resources in certain contexts, we anticipate
tion between attractiveness and relationship that they may also stand as relational liabilities
outcomes depended on sex or relationship insofar as they promote greater likelihood
type. Both husbands and wives were less of interest in extra-relationship partners. It
satisfied in relationships where the husband may be the qualities that are associated with
was more physically attractive than the wife being highly physically attractive (sociability,
(McNulty, Neff, & Karney, 2008). However, a popularity, dating, and sexual experience)
recent longitudinal study found that husbands’ that promote initial mate appeal, but that also
PA only weakly predicted husbands’ initial subsequently threaten the long-term viability
relationship satisfaction (i.e., more physically of the ensuing relationship. Consistent with
attractive husbands were less satisfied with this idea, existing studies have documented
their relationships initially, but that this pattern that more physically attractive individuals
did not emerge with wives); no changes in have more relationship partners (Jokela, 2009;
satisfaction emerged as a function of actor Langlois et al., 2000; Rhodes et al., 2005)
PA over time (Meltzer, McNulty, Jackson, & and attract more short-term mating interest
Karney, 2014a). Furthermore, among men in (Perilloux et al., 2013; van Straaten, Engles,
casual and serious dating relationships, those Finkenauer, & Holland, 2008), suggesting
who were more attractive exhibited increased higher rates of relationship dissolution. Other
self-reported desire for sexual relations with studies have shown that being more physically
extra-relationship others (White, 1980). Thus, attractive correlates with having more poten-
the work that has examined the role of attrac- tial relationship alternatives (Schmitt & Buss,
tiveness in romantic relationships has largely 2001; White, 1980), which may also threaten
been equivocal on the precise nature of the link the current relationship.
between attractiveness, relationship outcomes, Studies on cheating behavior provide
and the conditions under which it emerges.2 further evidence that PA can come with
Perspectives from fitness-related evolu- relational costs. Hughes and Gallup (2003)
tionary theories, models of socialization, and found that women with low waist-to-hip
social expectancy theories all suggest that ratios (WHR)—a paradigmatic feature of
those greater in PA are likely to possess, either female PA (Brooks, Jordan, Shelly, & Dixson,
naturally (i.e., by virtue of good genes) or over 2015; Singh, 1993; Singh, Dixson, Jessop,
time (i.e., by virtual of social forces), more Morgan, & Dixson, 2010; Streeter & McBur-
favorable traits (for review, see Gallup & Fred- ney, 2003; but see also Simpson, Brewer,
erick, 2010; Langlois et al., 2000). Although & Hendrie, 2014)—reported having more
empirical evidence for the strength of the extra-relationship partners relative to women
association between PA and positive traits with high WHR. Similarly, men with lower
varies, among adults, attractiveness is reliably levels of fluctuating asymmetry report having
more extra-relationship partners (Gangestad
& Thornhill, 1997; Thornhill & Gangestad,
see also Eastwick, Neff, Finkel, Luchies & Hunt, 2014;
Meltzer et al., 2014b). 1994). Consistent with these findings, mens’
2. Additional theories also can account for the link facial and bodily attractiveness correlated with
between attractiveness and relationship outcomes—for their number of short-term partners (Rhodes
example, the matching hypothesis suggests that individ-
uals are likely to affiliate with those of similar levels
et al., 2005). Moreover, more physically
of physical attractiveness (e.g., Cash & Derlega, 1978; attractive individuals are more frequently the
Murstein, 1972; Murstein & Christy, 1976). However, target of poaching attempts (Schmitt & Buss,
the recent work by McNulty et al. (2008) and Meltzer
et al. (2014a) suggest that matching attractiveness lev-
2001), and perhaps not surprisingly, more
els may not be as important as having a physically attractive (i.e., taller) men have higher divorce
attractive partner, at least in certain contexts. rates (Mueller & Mazur, 2001). Although PA
148 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

is not the only factor involved in relation- originally adaptive desire for foods high in fat
ship dissolution and cheating (other factors, and sugar, but pursuing those desires can lead
like sociosexual orientation and commitment to poor outcomes). The same may be true of
are also key determinants of infidelity; see PA. Second, the desirability of an outcome may
Mattingly et al., 2011), the aforementioned vary depending on the perspective or goals.
studies suggest that PA plays a major role in Although relationship dissolution may be a
promoting extra-relationship interest. negative outcome associated with PA from the
In particular, PA may not only make individ- perspective of the relationship, it may not nec-
uals the target of poaching attempts, but also essarily be a negative outcome from an evolu-
make such individuals more likely to respond tionary perspective.
favorably to (appealing) extra-relationship Furthermore, the aim of the present research
alternatives and derogate them less. The is not to compete with or replace other existing
aforementioned past research has suggested models of close relationships. One of the most
that PA is linked to greater numbers of robust models of relationship maintenance
extra-relationship pairings and attempts. How- is the investment model (Rusbult, Agnew,
ever, the existing literature has not gone so far & Arriaga, 2011; Rusbult, Martz, & Agnew,
as to demonstrate that highly attractive indi- 1998). At its core, the investment model is a
viduals are also more likely to be interested theory of dyadic processes in close relation-
in attractive relationship alternatives and less ships. The goal of the present studies is to
likely to derogate them. We predict that this complement its findings by focusing on an
crucial link—that more physically attractive intrapersonal factor—in this case, PA—that
individuals are likely to positively perceive can influence perceptions of alternatives and
attractive alternatives and less likely to dero- relationship satisfaction. Said differently, the
gate them—is an important feature that can present aim is to build upon the investment
play a critical role in relationship processes. model by highlighting how an intrapersonal
In sum, the present research aims to build force like PA can shape interpersonal forces
upon and expand the existing literature in like extra-relationship interest and contentment
two ways. First, we aim to directly assess with the existing relationship.
the impact of PA on relationship outcomes. Across four studies, we examined the role
Although the existing literature has com- of both actual and perceived PA in predict-
pellingly demonstrated that PA is linked to ing relationship outcomes (Studies 1 and
extra-relationship pairing, it remains unclear 2) and the use of relationship-maintaining
whether such behaviors necessarily lead to strategies—namely, derogation of attractive
relationship dissolution. A second goal is alternatives (Studies 3 and 4). Here, actual
to unpack the moderating factors involved PA refers to ratings by other individuals
in the link between PA and relationship whereas perceived PA refers to self-ratings.
processes—namely, to examine how relation- We predicted that PA would exert a negative
ship satisfaction and attractiveness interact to effect on relationship outcomes and that this
predict relationship maintenance behaviors relationship would relate to derogation of
like the derogation of attractive alternatives. attractive alternatives. We expected individ-
At first glance, the prediction that PA leads uals rated as more physically attractive to
to worse relationship outcomes may appear to be married for shorter periods of time and
be at odds with the vast body of studies show- more likely to be divorced (Studies 1 and 2).
ing the universal desirability of PA in mates. Furthermore, we explored the possibility that
However, the present work does not focus more physically attractive individuals are less
on individuals’ desires for physically attrac- likely to derogate attractive alternatives, an
tiveness mates, but rather on the outcomes important relationship maintenance behavior
that follow after a mate is already secured. (Study 3). Finally, we tested whether PA inter-
As with other features of the human psyche acts with current relationship satisfaction to
that have evolved over time, desires and out- predict extra-relationship interest to test the
comes can be dissociated (e.g., we evolved an prediction that relationship alternatives are
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 149

only threatening when the person (the actor) is rated by two independent female coders4 on
attractive and dissatisfied (Study 4). a scale from 1 (very unattractive) to 10 (very
attractive). Ratings were averaged to yield an
overall measure of PA (M = 3.52, SD = 1.52,
Study 1
range = 1.00 to 9.50). Intercoder reliability was
To test the aforementioned hypotheses, we high (r = .92, p < .001).
examined the relation between individual dif- Marriage and divorce outcomes were
ferences in PA and marriage outcomes col- derived from publicly accessible state records
lected over a 30-year period of adulthood for available online (http://www.ancestry.com);
men3 from two disparate U.S. high schools. men were matched based on name, loca-
Relying on archival analysis allowed us access tion, and age. Men were classified as either
to a relatively large sample of real-world data divorced (scored as 1) or married (scored as
for which measures of PA and relationship out- 0); men for whom no marriage or divorce
comes could be obtained. We predicted that records existed (n = 114 total: n = 38 for the
more physically attractive men would experi- Ohio school, n = 86 for the California school)
ence briefer marriages and be more likely to were not included in the sample; this yielded
divorce. Here, we focused on actual PA. a final sample of 238 individuals that were
included. No men in the sample had more
than one divorce on record (n = 39 for number
Method of divorced participants). Numbers of years
Participants married (as of 2013) was calculated from
date of marriage versus divorce certificate
Following procedures used previously by (M years = 23.09, SD = 9.81).
Harker and Keltner (2001), 238 men from a
Catholic high school in New Philadelphia,
Results and discussion
Ohio (n = 100), and a secular high school in
San Jose, California (n = 138), were randomly An independent samples t test revealed that the
selected to be included in the study. Both high divorced men were, on average, more attractive
school rosters were taken from the list of avail- (M = 4.41, SD = 1.90) than the married men
able high schools available online (http://www. (M = 3.34, SD = 1.51), t(236) = 3.84, p < .001,
classmates.com). The former is a private reli- d = 0.62. Furthermore, averaged PA ratings
gious school located in a semirural, predomi- correlated with number of years married, Pear-
nantly White, working-class county in central son’s r = −.23, p < .001; those rated as more
Ohio. The latter is a public school in a subur- physically attractive were married for shorter
ban, ethnically diverse, and socioeconomically periods of time. One alternative explanation for
privileged southern region of Silicon Valley. this effect could be that attractive men married
later in life and thus were not divorced because,
Procedure relative to less attractive men, they were mar-
ried more recently. To test this alternative
The men were rated for (facial) PA using pho- explanation, we examined the link between PA
tographs from the high school yearbooks of and number of years married among the sam-
1977 to 1980 available online (http://www. ple of married men; PA did not predict years
classmates.com). The men sampled were in of marriage (age at which the men married),
their senior year and, as such, were estimated r = −.04, p = .54.
to be between 17 and 18 years of age when the In addition, we tested the predictive power
pictures were taken. Each of the photos was of both PA and type of school (coded as
0 for the Ohio school, 1 for the Califor-
nia school) when entered simultaneously
3. Only men were included in Study 1 because women’s
marriage and divorce outcomes could not be reliably
tracked; some records listed maiden names while other 4. The coders were both female, aged 22 and 26 at the time
records listed married names. of coding.
150 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

to predict marital outcomes. Given that stronger for the larger school (i.e., the school
the former was a religious school in the with more participants included in the sample),
Midwest whereas the latter was a secular this was likely attributed to power; importantly,
one in Silicon Valley, controlling for type of the same pattern of results held across the
school allowed us to test the potential role of two subsamples.
demographic factors in influencing the pattern Study 1, however, was limited in that it
of results. Given the dichotomous and non- relied on publicly accessible state records; not
normal nature of the outcome variable (i.e., all records could be found for all the men
marital outcomes—divorced vs. not), a binary in both high schools, and no information is
logistic regression analysis was conducted. A available for the men for whom no state records
test of the full model against a constant-only of marriage or divorce exist. Thus, the sample
model was statistically significant, indicating may not have been representative of the greater
that the predictors (in this case, PA and school) population, or of the two chosen high schools;
reliably distinguished the married versus the given the high rate of marriage in the United
divorced, χ2 = 22.49, p < .001 with df = 2. The States, it is likely that a larger proportion of
Wald criterion demonstrated that PA made a each sample was married in the past 30 years
significant contribution to prediction, B = 0.30, than what could be included in the analyses. As
SE = 0.11, p = .004; an Exp(B) of 1.35 suggests such, participants who were not included in the
that more attractive men were more likely to sample could have varied systematically from
be divorced. Interestingly, type of school also those who were included.
made a significant contribution to prediction, Second, Study 1 relied on attractiveness
B = −1.16, SE = 0.39, p = .003; thus, we reran ratings based on high school yearbook photos.
the logistic regression separately by school, Although past studies have successfully relied
and the same pattern of results emerged for on the same stimuli to predict other important
both schools, although it was stronger for the life outcomes, including marriage (Harker &
larger school (i.e., California school: B = 0.63, Keltner, 2001), it is nevertheless possible that
SE = 0.21, p = .004, Exp(B) of 1.88) than the relative levels of PA changed during adulthood,
smaller school (i.e., Ohio: B = 0.20, SE = 0.12, and thus attractiveness in high school may not
p = .092, Exp(B) = 1.22). be the best indicator of attractiveness later on
By assessing marriage and divorce out- in life. A related point is that this study exam-
comes over a 30-year period for men from ined only facial attractiveness, which is merely
two different high schools, we found evidence one component of PA (Jonason, Raulston, &
that those higher in PA experienced shorter Rotolo, 2012; Rhodes et al., 2005). Moreover,
marriages and were more likely to be divorced. Study 1 used an all-male sample. Given that
Importantly, this effect held even when con- past studies that have shown differences in
trolling for the type of high school the men the impact male versus female attractiveness
attended. Given that the two schools sam- has on relationship satisfaction (McNulty
pled were of disparate origins (secular, upper et al., 2008; Meltzer et al., 2014a, 2014b), this
middle class, and from the West Coast vs. reli- study does not address the possibility that the
gious, working class, and from the Midwest) findings were specific to male attractiveness.
with divergent levels of socioeconomic status, Finally, the sample used in Study 1 was, on
religiosity, and ethnic diversity, an alternative average, of low to moderate attractiveness,
explanation is that the effects of PA were and it is unclear whether these effects would
confounded with one of these other sociode- extend to a more attractive sample. Thus, to
mographic factors. However, the detrimental test our predictions in a sample not restricted
relationship outcomes associated with being by these limitations, Study 2 was conducted.
highly physically attractive continued even
when the type of school was held constant,
Study 2
suggesting that PA exerted unique predic-
tive power in predicting marital outcomes. The aim of Study 2 was to replicate the find-
Although the predictive power of PA was ings from Study 1 using a different sample
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 151

that included both men and women for whom represent two disparate but well-established
PA, and not just facial attractiveness, could be and widely respected third-party sources for
assessed, and for whom marriage data would information on actors and other high-profile
be consistently available across all participants individuals.
in the sample. Here, we examined marriage and
divorce outcomes among actors, actresses, and Procedure
celebrities because we wanted to test the effects
of PA on marriage outcomes in a sample that The same two independent coders from Study
was disparate from the one used in Study 1 1 rated the PA of the celebrities on a scale from
to demonstrate that the aforementioned effects 1 (very unattractive) to 10 (very attractive).
were not specific to men, individuals for whom Ratings were averaged to yield an overall mea-
public state marriage records were available, sure of PA (M = 6.97, SD = 1.47, range = 3.00
or those particular cohorts. We chose to exam- to 10.00). Intercoder reliability was accept-
ine only high-profile individuals, as opposed able (r = .61, p < .001). Relationship history
to a larger, more inclusive set of high- and (number and duration of marriages; number
low-profile individuals, because of the wide of divorces; identities of spouses) and other
availability of data on their relationship history. details relating to actors’ and actresses’ pro-
One potential concern with using this fessional and personal lives (age; net worth)
select subsample is the popular notion that were gathered from various established online
celebrities possess “infinite” relationship sources, including www.The-Numbers.com,
alternatives relative to the average person. www.celebritynetworth.com, and www.imdb.
However, popular belief about celebrities’ com; these websites were chosen because
alternatives may not be accurate—recent of their comprehensive databases of infor-
research found that the wealthier the man, mation related to celebrities’ personal and
the stronger his preferences for physically professional histories. Marriage and divorce
attractive partners; wealthy women also had data were available for all participants, but
stringent preferences—although in this case, it number of responses varied by item for the
was for wealthier partners (Fales et al., 2016). control variables included given that not all
Along similar lines, experimental manipula- information was available or applicable for
tions of wealth led men to have higher mating all individuals in the sample; as a result,
standards (Yong & Li, 2012). Thus, similar to degrees of freedom will vary as a function
the wealthy, celebrities may not have unlim- of which control variables were included.
ited relationship alternatives if their idea of a The majority of individuals had been married
viable relationship partner comes from a more at least once (54% had been married only
limited and selective pool. once, M years = 11.28, SD = 8.89; 18% married
twice, M years = 9.90, SD = 8.49; 5% married
three times, M years = 6.57, SD = 3.40; and 1%
Method married four times).
Participants
Results and discussion
High-profile individuals from the follow-
ing sources were used: IMDb’s lists of the An independent samples t test revealed that a
top 20 actors and top 20 actresses in the main effect of sex emerged on PA, such that
past 20 years (http://www.imdb.com/list/ female celebrities were rated as more attractive
4qTRP4Hj3xQ/ and http://www.imdb.com/ (M = 7.45, SD = 1.37) than male celebrities
list/77pbwLyeM0U/), and Forbes’s list of the (M = 6.66, SD = 1.45), t(128) = 3.06, p = .003,
100 most powerful celebrities (http://www. d = 0.56. However, there was no effect of sex
forbes.com/celebrities/list/). Duplicates were on the dependent variable of interest (e.g.,
only included once. This yielded a total of 130 duration of marriage, t <1). Thus, sex will no
celebrities (M age = 45.29; 62% male). IMDb’s longer be mentioned for the remainder of the
and Forbes’s lists were used because they analyses.
152 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

First, we examined the link between PA analyses was conducted with PA and wealth
and duration of (first) marriages using linear (operationalized as total net worth) predicting
regression; for all subsequent analyses, linear marriage and divorce outcomes; in both cases,
regression was used for all analyses involving number of times married was included as a
marriage duration. We opted to examine mar- covariate. PA continued to significantly predict
riage duration for first marriages because this marriage duration, β = −0.50, t(94) = 5.90,
allowed us to examine the maximum possi- p < .001, with more physically attractive
ble number of individuals in our sample, given individuals experiencing briefer marriages.
that fewer numbers of individuals had sec- As expected, number of times married was
ond, third, or fourth marriages. PA predicted also related to marriage duration, β = −0.29,
number of years married for first marriages, t(94) = 3.23, p = .002; briefer first marriages
β = −0.50, t(96) = 5.72, p < .001. Those rated were associated with more subsequent mar-
as more physically attractive were married for riages. No relation emerged between net worth
shorter periods of time. and marriage duration (t < 1, p = .94). The
Second, we examined the link between same pattern emerged when number of times
PA and marriage outcomes (i.e., among all divorced was the outcome of interest: PA
marriages). Given the count nature of marriage significantly predicted likelihood of divorce,
outcomes (in this case, number of divorces) in B = 0.21, SE = 0.08, 95% Wald CI [0.05, 0.37].
this sample and its nonnormal nature, Poisson Study 2 replicated the findings from Study
generalized linear models were fitted to predict 1 and showed that among high-profile celebri-
number of divorces using PA while controlling ties, PA predicted marital outcomes. This find-
for the number of marriages. (In all subsequent ing is notable because it demonstrates that the
analyses, this type of model was used for all link between attractiveness and relationship
analyses involving number of divorces.) An dissolution is robust, not specific to the men
omnibus test of the fitted model against an in Study 1, and occurred in a restricted sam-
intercept-only model was statistically signif- ple of highly attractive, highly likely to divorce
icant, χ2 = 6.51, p = .011 with df = 1, and PA individuals. Importantly, the link between a
made a significant contribution to prediction, celebrity’s PA and his or her likelihood of
B = 0.21, SE = 0.08, 95% Wald CI [0.047, divorce held even when important covariates
0.37]. and confounds were held constant (includ-
As we did with Study 1, to account for ing net worth and age at which the celebrity
the alternative explanation that attractive peo- was first married). Together, Studies 1 and
ple married later in life and thus were not 2 demonstrated that across disparate samples,
divorced because, relative to the less attractive, those higher in PA were less likely to stay mar-
they were married more recently, we reran the ried and experienced briefer marriages. How-
above analyses with age of first marriage as a ever, marriage length may be an imperfect
covariate. Doing so did not change the pattern proxy for relationship quality. Thus, to test the
of findings, χ2 = 6.82, p = .009 with df = 1; PA effects of PA on other relationship outcomes
continued to make a significant contribution to more directly related to relationship quality,
prediction, B = 0.21, SE = 0.08, 95% Wald CI Study 3 was conducted.
[0.05, 0.36]. Similarly, PA continued to predict
length of marriage controlling for age of first
Study 3
marriage, β = −0.56, t(68) = 5.28, p < .001.
One additional alternative explanation for Why are the more physically attractive less
these findings is that the poor marital out- likely to maintain long-lasting relationships?
comes among particularly attractive actors Relationship maintenance models posit that
and actresses may be explained by other con- one of the mechanisms involved in relation-
founds that accompany celebrity—namely, ship maintenance is perceptual derogation
wealth. Thus, to demonstrate that the effects of attractive, potential alternatives, which
of PA continue to predict marital dissolution has been shown to occur on a variety of
even when wealth is held constant, regression dimensions—including intelligence, humor,
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 153

similarity, dependableness, and faithfulness recruited for this study. Fifty-four were volun-
(Johnson & Rusbult, 1989). However, another tary undergraduate participants recruited from
work has argued that derogation on the PA a local university campus. Eighty were paid
dimension should be one of the most impor- community participants recruited from the
tant strategies to protect a relationship from greater Boston area who participated as part
dissolution, because very physically attrac- of a larger study. Of the sample, 41% were
tive opposite-sex individuals can pose one involved in an exclusive romantic relationship
of the worst potential threats to an existing at the time of the study.
relationship (Simpson, Gangestad, & Lerma,
1990). In line with this idea, Maner, Rouby, Procedure
and Gonzaga (2008) found that individuals
in relationships automatically paid less visual All participants completed an initial question-
attention to attractive alternatives when made naire concerning their relationship history,
to think romantic thoughts about their current including sexual orientation, current rela-
relationship partner. Importantly, attractive tionship status, duration of their current
opposite-sex individuals do not need to be relationship, and number of past relation-
actually viable alternative partners to threaten ships. Afterward, following the procedures
existing relationships (Simpson et al., 1990), outlined by Simpson et al. (1990), participants
given that the mere exposure to an attractive, were shown a photograph of an attractive,
opposite-sex individual can cause current part- opposite-sex target and asked to rate the
ners to appear less attractive by comparison. target’s attractiveness on a scale from 1(not
In support of their argument, Simpson et al. at all attractive) to 7 (very attractive). Dur-
(1990) noted past studies showing that even ing the study, the PA of the participants
viewing attractive television stars can nega- was surreptitiously rated independently by
tively impact men’s evaluations of a potential two experimenters, whose responses were
date (Kenrick & Gutierres, 1980), and viewing averaged to yield an overall measure of PA
attractive pornographic targets can make men (M = 5.95, SD = 1.65, range = 2.00 to 10.00),
less happy with their current relationship part- r = .57, p < .001. The experimenters were not
ner (Kenrick, Gutierres, & Goldberg, 1989). blinded to the hypothesis of the study but were
To test the link between PA and the dero- blind to the participants’ relationship status,
gation of attractive alternatives, we asked history, and ratings. Number of participants’
participants, both in exclusive romantic rela- responses varied by item because of omitted
tionships and not, to rate the attractiveness of responses; as a result of pairwise deletion,
opposite-sex targets. We predicted that among only participants who completed all responses
those in exclusive romantic relationships, were included in the analyses.
greater PA of the participant would predict
less derogation of (or increased perceptions of
attraction toward) opposite-sex targets. Fur- Results and discussion
thermore, this effect should not be accounted An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with
for by a general tendency for physically attrac- sex, PA, and relationship status predicting
tive individuals to perceive greater attractive- extra-relationship interest (or lack of dero-
ness in others; thus, among those not currently gation of attractiveness alternatives) revealed
involved in exclusive romantic relationships, no main effects or interactions involving
the relationship between PA and perceived sex, all ps > .19. Thus, sex will no longer be
attractiveness of targets should not emerge. discussed. To test the prediction that among
those currently in romantic relationships, more
Method physically attractive individuals would be more
likely to engage in relationship-threatening
Participants
behaviors (in this case, engage in less dero-
One hundred and thirty-four participants gation of an opposite-sex target), a linear
(45% men) from the New England area were regression was conducted with relationship
154 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

status, participants’ PA, and the interaction 3.5


between the two predicting attractiveness
3
ratings of the opposite-sex target. There was
no main effect of PA, t <1, but a main effect 2.5
of relationship status emerged, t(124) = 2.00,
β = −0.70, p = .047. Consistent with past 2
studies, those in relationships, on average, 1.5
perceived the opposite-sex target to be less
physically attractive. However, this main 1
effect was qualified by a significant interaction In a relationship
0.5
between relationship status and participants’ Not in a relationship
PA, β = 0.82, t(124) = 2.25, p = .026. Among 0
those in exclusive romantic relationships, 1 SD below 1 SD above
participants’ PA predicted attraction to the Attractiveness
opposite-sex target, with those higher in PA
reporting less derogation or greater attraction, Figure 1. Attraction to extrarelationship tar-
β = 0.37, t(50) = 2.78, p = .008. In contrast, gets as a function of researcher-rated attractive-
among those not currently involved in exclu- ness and relationship status (i.e., for individu-
sive relationships, no relationship emerged als in a relationship vs. not in a relationship;
between participants’ PA and attraction toward Study 3).
the target, t < 1 (see Figure 1).5
Among those in exclusive romantic rela- A potential related concern is the fact that we
tionships, greater PA predicted less derogation measured ratings of PA of the target rather than
of attractive, opposite-sex alternatives. Impor- explicit attraction to the target. However, pre-
tantly, this effect could not be accounted for vious studies (Simpson et al., 1990) have reli-
by a general tendency for physically attractive ably used photographs to measure derogation
individuals to perceive greater attractiveness in of attractive alternatives among those in rela-
others. The present findings suggest that PA tionships and used attractiveness ratings as a
serves as a relational liability insofar as it pro- valid proxy for attraction.
motes perceived interest in alternative partners The findings from Study 3 extended the
and is linked with a lower likelihood of dero- findings from Studies 1 and 2 by demon-
gating attractive alternatives. Previous research strating that PA not only predicts relationship
suggested that partnered individuals may auto- duration and likelihood of dissolution, but it
matically show inattention to attractive alterna- also predicts how individuals perceive rela-
tives (Maner et al., 2008), and the present find- tionship alternatives and derogate attractive
ings suggest that this relationship maintenance alternatives in particular. One possibility
strategy may be moderated by the attractive- is that being physically attractive leads to
ness of the individual at hand. increased interest in relationship alternatives,
One limitation of Study 3 was the use of and the latter predicts (worse) relationship
photographs, which does not allow for signs outcomes (i.e., a mediational path with PA
of reciprocity. Thus, photographs may not ade- predicting extra-relationship interest, and
quately reflect actual threats to a relationship. extra-relationship interest predicting relation-
ship dissolution). However, although previous
5. We reran the analyses with only those who self-reported research (e.g., Mueller & Mazur, 2001) has
as heterosexual (among those in a relationship, five suggested that more physically attractive indi-
were excluded; among those not in a relationship, six viduals may have increased opportunities for
were excluded). This did not change the pattern of
results. In addition, we reran the analysis with relation- extra-relationship pairings, there is little to
ship length as a covariate (relationship length measured suggest that simply being attractive would
from 1 to 6, with 1–5 reflecting number of months and make someone more interested in a specific
6 reflecting relationships that have been in effect for 6 or
more months); doing so also did not change the pattern extra-relationship target. Said differently, there
of results. is little theoretical or empirical evidence that
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 155

individual differences in PA reliably predicts via Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT). Past
perceptions of attractiveness of opposite sex research has shown that AMT stands as a
targets. Another possibility is that PA and reliable source of data that is comparable to
interest in alternatives interact, such that alter- samples obtained from more traditional meth-
natives only implicate relationship outcomes ods (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011;
when the person is physically attractive, Horton, Rand, & Zeckhauser, 2011; Mason &
but does not negatively impact relationship Suri, 2012).
outcomes when the person is not (i.e., a mod-
eration relationship, with extra-relationship Measures
interest moderating the link between PA and
Participants were randomly assigned to view
relationship outcomes). However, previous
a series of five images containing either
research has consistently demonstrated that
physically attractive or unattractive same-sex
attractive, opposite-sex targets are one of the
targets. Images were (predominantly color)
most threatening risks in the context of a het-
photographs taken from Google searches for
erosexual relationship (Rusbult, 1983). Finally,
“attractive female,” “unattractive female,”
a third, more compelling possibility is that PA “attractive male,” and “unattractive male”
and relationship satisfaction interact, such that and contained both faces and bodies.6 To
only dissatisfied, attractive people are likely ensure that participants successfully viewed
to be interested in relationship alternatives. To all images, they were asked to briefly describe
test this possibility of a moderation relation- each image; no time limits were placed. After-
ship with relationship satisfaction moderating ward as a manipulation check, participants
the link between PA and extra-relationship rated their own attractiveness on a 1 (not at
interest, Study 4 was conducted. all attractive) to 7 (very attractive) scale. As
an attentional check, participants were also
Study 4 asked to rate the attractiveness of the pho-
tographs using two different questions: “The
In Study 4, we experimentally manipulated individuals in the images were physically
perceived PA among individuals currently attractive” and “The individuals in the images
involved in a romantic relationship. We mod- were NOT physically attractive”; in both cases,
eled our PA manipulation on past research participants rated their agreement on a 1 to 7
that has shown reliable contrast effects to scale.
emerge when comparing the self with physi- Participants subsequently completed
cally attractive or unattractive targets. Viewing the same task assessing their interest in
highly physically attractive targets leads to extra-relationship partners used in the previ-
decreased levels of self-perceived PA, but ous study with one change: Instead of viewing
viewing physically unattractive targets leads a single target, participants viewed a series of
to increased levels of self-perceived PA (Agli- three attractive, opposite-sex targets, which
ata & Tantleff-Dunn, 2004; Brown, Novick, allowed us to more stringently assess the
Lord, & Richards, 1992). We subsequently replicability of the previous finding with a
examined the link between PA, perceptions broader array of stimuli. As before, they rated
of relationship alternatives (i.e., derogation of the degree to which they were attracted to each
attractive alternatives), and current relationship one. Finally, participants evaluated their satis-
satisfaction. faction with their current romantic relationship

Method 6. Only images that contained a single target were


included. Targets were predominantly White/
Participants European American (9/10 of the men; 8/10 of the
women). Google images were used because Google
One hundred and fifty-six participants who search represents an updated source of stimuli that
were currently involved in a romantic relation- participants are unlikely to have been exposed to in the
ship (59% men, M age = 31.17) were recruited context of previous psychological studies.
156 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

and partner by completing the satisfaction Table 1. Study 4 manipulation check of attrac-
subscale of the Investment Model Scale tiveness: Mean ratings (standard deviations in
(Rusbult et al., 1998).7 Number of responses parentheses) of targets’ ratings of the target
varied because of omitted responses. and self as a function of condition

Rating of Rating
Results and discussion
Condition target of self
Prior to analyses, participants’ responses to
the attentional checks were examined and Attractive (i.e., 3.08 (1.73) 5.16 (1.19)
those who failed attentional checks were not viewed unattractive
included in the analysis. Namely, those who targets)
provided identical ratings to the statements Unattractive (i.e., 6.03 (1.11) 4.72 (1.53)
“the individuals in the images were physi- viewed attractive
cally attractive” and “the individuals in the targets)
images were NOT physically attractive” were
excluded, as were those who did not describe
the images (final N = 139). As a manipulation found the attractive targets more attractive than
check, an ANOVA with both condition and the unattractive targets, but this discrepancy
sex predicting ratings of self-perceived PA and was greater for women than for men (namely,
perceived PA of the same-sex targets was con- women found the unattractive targets less
ducted. A main effect of condition emerged attractive than men did, but the two groups did
on both self-rated PA, F(1, 135) = 4.02, not differ in their ratings of attractive targets):
p = .047, and perceived PA of the targets, M = 3.63, SE = 0.24, 95% CI [3.15, 4.11]
F(1, 135) = 156.59, p < .001. As expected,
versus M = 5.97, SE = 0.21, 95% CI [5.56,
unattractive targets were rated as less attrac-
6.38] for men; M = 2.48, SE = 0.25, 95% CI
tive than attractive targets, Cohen’s d = 2.02,
[1.99, 2.97] versus M = 6.10, SE = 0.25, 95%
and those exposed to unattractive same-sex
CI [5.60, 6.60] for women. In sum, there was
targets perceived themselves as more attractive
no effect of sex and no Sex × Condition inter-
than those exposed to attractive same-sex
action on the effectiveness of the manipulation
targets, d = 0.32; for full means and standard
(self-perceived PA). Moreover, sex did not
deviations, see Table 1. In addition, a main
interact with condition to predict the dependent
effect of sex emerged on target ratings, F(1,
variable of interest, relationship satisfaction
135) = 4.68, p = .032, but not on self-ratings of
(namely, when sex was entered simulta-
PA, F < 1. Men rated the targets overall as more
neously with condition, extra-relationship
attractive (M = 4.99, SD = 1.86) than women
interest, and the interaction between sex and
did (M = 4.26, SD = 2.22), d = 0.35. Finally, a
condition to predict satisfaction, no inter-
Sex × Condition interaction emerged on target
action emerged); thus, sex will no longer
ratings, F(1, 135) = 7.16, p = .008, but not on
be discussed.
self-ratings, F(1, 135) = 2.47, p = .12. Pairwise
For the main analyses, responses to the sat-
comparisons reveal that both men and women
isfaction scale were averaged to yield an over-
all measure of satisfaction with one’s current
7. We specifically examined responses to the satisfaction relationship. In addition, attraction to all three
subscale of the Rusbult et al. (1998) measure, which extra-relationship alternatives was also aver-
has additional subscales that measure other relation-
ship features (quality of alternatives, investment size). aged. We subsequently tested different mod-
To avoid multicollinearity concerns with the interest in els of the link between PA, extra-relationship
extra-relationship partners measure, we did not include interest, and relationship satisfaction in sep-
the quality of alternatives subscale. The investment size
subscale was not relevant to our hypothesis. The Invest- arate linear regressions. To address potential
ment Model also contains a commitment scale, which concerns about multicollinearity, we assessed
is the proposed outcome of these three aforementioned tolerance and variance inflation factor (VIF);
factors (satisfaction, alternatives, investment), but we
did not include it in the interest of keeping the survey extra-relationship interest and condition exhib-
brief. ited high tolerance (0.89) and low VIF (1.12),
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 157

suggesting that multicollinearity is not a con- are noteworthy because they demonstrate that
cern. PA predicts the likelihood of relationship being
We subsequently tested our predicted threatened—in this case, by poor relationship
moderation model, which proposes that rela- satisfaction.
tionship satisfaction interacts with PA to
influence evaluation of relationship alter-
General Discussion
natives. In this case, the outcome variable
of interest is extra-relationship interest, and Across four studies, being more attrac-
the prediction is that PA and satisfaction tive physically predicted greater likelihood
interact to determine evaluation of attractive, of relationship dissolution, briefer
opposite-sex targets. A regression with con- relationships, and decreased engagement
dition, relationship satisfaction, and the in relationship-maintenance processes. In
interaction between the two predicting Studies 1 and 2, more physically attractive
extra-relationship interests revealed that individuals were more likely to experience
all three factors were significant. Those shorter marriages and higher divorce rates. In
in the attractiveness condition (coded as Study 3, more physically attractive individuals
1 = attractive condition, where participants involved in dating relationships expressed
viewed unattractive targets and thus were less derogation of attractive alternatives. In
made to feel attractive; 2 = unattractive con- Study 4, PA hurt relationship processes by
dition, where participants viewed attractive predicting increased interest in attractive alter-
targets and thus were made to feel unattractive) natives among those who were dissatisfied
showed increased extra-relationship interest, with their relationship. Study 4 is impor-
β = −1.33, t(143) = 3.68, p < .001. In addition, tant in showing that being more physically
lower levels of relationship satisfaction also attractive makes one’s relationship more vul-
predicted increased extra-relationship interest, nerable to threats—namely, those stemming
β = −0.76, t(143) = 2.51, p = .013. These main from poor satisfaction with one’s existing
effects were qualified by a condition by sat- relationship.
isfaction interaction, β = 1.23, t(143) = 2.85,
p = .005. Among those in the attractive con-
PA and evolutionary perspectives
dition, current relationship satisfaction was
inversely related to extra-relationship interest, These findings suggest that the more favorable
β = −0.25, t(65) = 2.02, p = .048, whereas the traits possessed by physically attractive indi-
opposite pattern emerged among those in the viduals (Gallup & Frederick, 2010; Langlois
unattractive condition, β = 0.24, t(78) = 2.22, et al., 2000) can stand as a relational liability
p = .029. In other words, among individuals and thus may confer benefits to the individual
made to feel attractive, those who were less at hand (the physically attractive person), but
satisfied with their current relationship were not the relationship at stake. Thus, from an
more interested in extra-relationship alterna- evolutionary perspective, PA remains a trait
tives. Those made to feel unattractive did not that is highly selected for, but from a rela-
exhibit this pattern. tionship maintenance perspective, this highly
Study 4 demonstrates that PA interacts with desirable trait can come at a cost to the rela-
other relationship features to predict relation- tionship itself. Moreover, the present studies
ship outcomes. The findings support the idea also offer one potential explanation for why
that PA is moderated by current relationship physically attractive individuals have more
satisfaction to predict extra-relationship inter- relationship partners (Langlois et al., 2000;
est. That is, poor relationship satisfaction only Rhodes et al., 2005), more extra-relationship
threatens relationships under conditions of pairings (Dijkstra & Buunk, 2001; Gangestad
PA—in such cases, low levels of satisfaction & Thornhill, 1997; Singh, 1993; Streeter &
with one’s current relationship predicts greater McBurney, 2003; Thornhill & Gangestad,
interest in extra-relationship partners, but only 1994), and attract more short-term mating
among those who feel attractive. The findings interest (van Straaten et al., 2008). Although
158 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

past studies have shown that PA can make important variables—for example, sociode-
individuals the target of poaching attempts mographic factors like class, or individuating
(Schmitt & Buss, 2001), the present studies factors like earning power, confidence, overall
show that PA can also make such individuals positivity of self-views or other views, and
express more interest in and derogate less so on—and that the effects of attractiveness
(appealing) extra-relationship opportunities, at can by accounted for by its correlates. Two
least when the current relationship is less than considerations cast strong doubt on this expla-
satisfying. nation. First, in Studies 1 and 2, we tested the
link between PA and marriage outcomes using
three disparate samples: two sociodemograph-
PA and the investment model of close
ically divergent high school samples (in which
relationships
case, after controlling for the sample demo-
Taken together, Studies 3 and 4 highlight how graphic [high school attended], PA continued
an intrapersonal force, PA, bears upon the to predict marital dissolution) and one sample
important interpersonal forces of relationship of high-profile celebrities. Second, in Study
alternatives and satisfaction in the reigning 3, those not currently involved in exclusive
model of close relationships, the investment romantic relationships did not show a relation
model (Rusbult et al., 1998). At its core, between PA and perceived attractiveness of
the investment model is about how relation- targets, suggesting that the effect of PA on
ship satisfaction and alternatives (along with interest in extra-relationship partners could
investment size) predict relationship outcomes not be accounted for by a general tendency for
(the likelihood of persistence vs. dissolution; physically attractive individuals to perceive
Rusbult et al., 1998). The present findings greater attractiveness in others.
build upon this model by demonstrating that Nevertheless, the present research is
relationship satisfaction and individual differ- not without limitations. All studies were
ences in PA can interact to shape maintenance cross-sectional in nature insofar as their
behavior—in this case, perceptions of attrac- assessment of attractiveness at a single point
tive alternatives. Those who are dissatisfied in time and does not address the possibil-
with their relationship partners are more likely ity that PA changes over the course of the
to positively perceive attractive alternatives, lifespan. More longitudinal data would be
but only if they are physically attractive. necessary to demonstrate that PA at any given
These findings also contribute to the lit- point in time predicts subsequent relationship
erature on the processes that contribute to satisfaction or outcomes, and that shifts in
the derogation of attractive alternatives, and PA over time is accompanied by shifts in
whether they involve perceptual (Maner et al., relationship-maintaining behaviors. Addi-
2008) versus motivated (Johnson & Rusbult, tionally, rather than manipulating perceived
1989) mechanisms. Consistent with the more PA (as we did in Study 4), future laboratory
recent studies, derogation oftentimes involves studies can directly manipulate actual PA (e.g.,
automatic, perceptual processes (e.g., Maner through “makeovers”) and test its subsequent
et al., 2008); however, these previous studies effects on relationship-related behaviors.
have largely focused on derogation that serves An additional limitation is that the present
to maintain relationships. The present studies, work does not examine idiosyncratic fea-
in contrast, highlight how the (lack of) dero- tures of the relationship, such as the role
gation can also serve to hinder relationship of relationship-specific norms or commit-
maintenance, at least among the physically ment. One possibility is that more physically
attractive. attractive individuals may be likely to pos-
sess different relationship norms about
extra-relationship pairing, and these norms
Limitations and future directions
may influence responses to alternatives or
An alternative explanation for the present the likelihood of dissolution. A related pos-
findings is that PA is confounded with other sibility is the idea that level of commitment
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 159

to a given relationship may also shape how physically attractive partners) sheds doubt on
partners respond to alternatives (Lydon, this possibility, future studies can directly the
Meana, Sepinwall, Richards, & Mayman, role of partner attractiveness interacting with a
1999), given that finding extra-relationship tar- person’s own PA in predicting relationship out-
gets attractive and acting upon those feelings comes among a broader population.
of attraction are not the same. In the current In short, previous research has long estab-
studies, we only examined extra-relationship lished PA as a powerful motivator and potent
interest and derogation; we did not test actual predictor of positive outcomes across a variety
cheating behaviors. Whether extra-relationship of domains, and a highly desirable attribute in
interest versus derogation predicts actual the context of romantic relationships (Langlois
extra-relationship behavior and how addi- et al., 2000). Despite the apparently universal
tional relationship features stand as important valuation of PA in prospective mates (Buss,
potential moderators of this link are topics that 1989; Weeden & Sabini, 2005), the present
future studies can investigate. research shows that physically attractive rela-
Future studies can also more closely exam- tionship partners are less likely to derogate
ine the precise mechanisms involved in the extra-relationship partners and exhibit worse
link between PA and relationship outcomes. relationship outcomes. These findings suggest
Extra-relationship interest may be one of mul- that being physically attractive is not without
tiple processes associated with PA in the con- its relational liabilities.
text of close relationships. Future work can
examine other possible models in order to References
elucidate the underlying processes that can Agliata, D., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2004). The impact of
account for the link between attractiveness and media exposure on males’ body image. Journal of
relationship outcomes. For example, PA may Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 7–22.
Barelds, D. P. H., Dijstra, P., Koudenbrug, N., & Swami,
also influence relationship outcomes via per- V. (2011). An assessment of positive illusions of the
ceptions of others’ interest in the self or the physical attractiveness of romantic partners. Journal of
perceived likelihood of acquiring new relation- Social and Personal Relationships, 28, 706–719.
ship partners. Additionally, concerns about the Berscheid, E., Dion, K., Walster, E., & Walster, G. W.
liabilities of PA might increase the use of mal- (1971). Physical attractiveness and dating choice: A
test of the matching hypothesis. Journal of Experimen-
adaptive relationship processes such as jeal- tal Social Psychology, 7, 173–189.
ousy or possessive behaviors among partners Brooks, R. C., Jordan, A. L., Shelly, J., & Dixson, B.
of those who are highly physically attractive. J. (2015). The multivariate evolution of female body
Future studies can also test the potentially shape in an artificial digital ecosystem. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 36, 351–358.
moderating role of partner attractiveness in
Brown, J. D., Novick, N. J., Lord, K. A., & Richards,
changing the relation between PA and rela- J. M. (1992). When Gulliver travels: Social context,
tionship outcomes. Across the present stud- psychological closeness, and self-appraisals. Journal
ies, we focused exclusively on the attractive- of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 717–727.
ness of participants but not the attractiveness Buhrmester, M. D., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011).
Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpen-
of their partners. Per the matching hypothe-
sive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psycholog-
sis (Berscheid, Dion, Walster, & Walster, 1971; ical Science, 6, 3–5.
Cash & Derlega, 1978; Murstein, 1972), effects Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate pref-
of PA could be moderated by partner attrac- erences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures.
tiveness (namely, physically attractive individ- Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–14.
Buss, D. M. (1992). International preferences in selecting
uals may not experience worse relationship mates: A study of 37 societies. Journal of Cross Cul-
outcomes when their partners are also highly tural Psychology, 21, 5–47.
physically attractive). Thus, one possibility is Cash, T. F., & Derlega, V. J. (1978). The matching
that PA is only a liability in contexts where the hypothesis: Physical attractiveness among same-sexed
partner is equally or less attractive. Although friends. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4,
240–243.
the fact that we found the same pattern of Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (2001). Sex differences in
results in Study 2 with a highly attractive the jealousy-evoking nature of a rival’s body build.
and wealthy subsample (who tends to attract Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 335–341.
160 C. Ma-Kellams, M. C. Wang, and H. Cardiel

Eastwick, P. W., Luchies, L. B., Finkel, E. J., & Hunt, Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson,
L. L. (2014). The predictive validity of ideal partner A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths
preferences: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review.
Personality and Social Psychology, 140, 623–665. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423.
Eastwick, P. W., Neff, L. A., Finkel, E. J., Luchies, L. B., Lydon, J. E., Meana, M., Sepinwall, D., Richards, N.,
& Hunt, L. L. (2014). Is a meta-analysis a foundation, & Mayman, S. (1999). The commitment calibration
or just another brick? Comment on Meltzer, McNulty, hypothesis: When do people devalue attractive alterna-
Jackson, and Karney (2014). Journal of Personality tives? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,
and Social Psychology, 106, 429–434. 152–161.
Fales, M. R., Frederick, D. A., Garcia, J. R., Gildersleeve, Major, B., Carrington, P. I., & Carnevale, P. J. (1984).
K. A., Haselton, M. G., & Fisher, H. E. (2016). Mating Physical attractiveness and self-esteem: Attributions
markets and bargaining hands: Mate preferences for for praise from an other-sex evaluator. Personality and
attractiveness and resources in two national US studies.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 43–50.
Personality and Individual Differences, 88, 78–87.
Maner, J. K., Rouby, D. A., & Gonzaga, G. C. (2008).
Gallup, G. G., & Frederick, D. A. (2010). The science
Automatic inattention to attractive alternatives:
of sex appeal: An evolutionary perspective. Review of
The evolved psychology of relationship main-
General Psychology, 14, 240–250.
tenance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29,
Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1997). The evolution-
343–349.
ary psychology of extrapair sex: The role of fluctuat-
ing asymmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2012). Conducting behavioral
69–88. research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior
Harker, L., & Keltner, D. (2001). Expressions of posi- Research Methods, 44, 1–23.
tive emotion in women’s college yearbook pictures Mattingly, B. A., Clark, E. M., Weidler, D. J., Bullock, M.,
and their relationship to personality and life outcomes Hackathorn, J., & Blankmeyer, K. (2011). Sociosexual
across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social orientation, commitment, and infidelity: A mediation
Psychology, 80, 112–124. analysis. Journal of Social Psychology, 151, 222–226.
Heilman, M. E., & Stopeck, M. H. (1985). Being attrac- McNulty, J. K., Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2008).
tive, advantage or disadvantage? Performance-based Beyond initial attraction: Physical attractiveness in
evaluations and recommended personnel actions as a newlywed marriage. Journal of Family Psychology, 22,
function of appearance, sex, and job type. Organiza- 135–143.
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, Meltzer, A. L., McNulty, J. K., Jackson, G. L., & Karney,
202–215. B. R. (2014a). Sex differences in the implications of
Horton, J. J., Rand, D. G., & Zeckhauser, R. J. (2011). The partner physical attractiveness for the trajectory of
online laboratory: Conducting experiments in a real marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social
labor market. Experimental Economics, 4, 399–425. Psychology, 106, 418–428.
Hughes, S. M., & Gallup, G. G. (2003). Sex differences in Meltzer, A. L., McNulty, J. K., Jackson, G. L., & Karney,
morphological predictors of sexual behavior: Shoulder B. R. (2014b). Men still value physical attractiveness in
to hip and waist to hip ratios. Evolution and Human a long-term mate more than women: Rejoinder to East-
Behavior, 24, 173–178. wick, Neff, Finkel, Luchies, and Hunt (2014). Journal
Johnson, D. J., & Rusbult, C. E. (1989). Resisting temp- of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 435–440.
tation: Devaluation of alternative partners as a means Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (2001). Evidence of uncon-
of maintaining commitment in close relationships.
strained directional selection for male tallness. Behav-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
ioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 302–311.
967–980.
Murstein, B. L. (1972). Physical attractiveness and marital
Jokela, M. (2009). Physical attractiveness and reproductive
choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
success in humans: Evidence from the late 20th century
22, 8–12.
United States. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30,
Murstein, B. I., & Christy, P. (1976). Physical attractive-
342–350.
Jonason, P. K., Raulston, T., & Rotolo, A. (2012). More ness and marriage adjustment in middle-aged couples.
than just a pretty face and a hot body: Multiple cues Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 537.
in mate-choice. Journal of Social Psychology, 152, Perilloux, C., Cloud, J. M., & Buss, D. M. (2013).
174–184. Women’s physical attractiveness and short-term mat-
Kenrick, D. T., & Gutierres, S. E. (1980). Contrast effects ing strategies. Personality and Individual Differences,
and judgments of physical attractiveness: When beauty 54, 490–495.
becomes a social problem. Journal of Personality and Peterson, J. L., & Miller, C. (1980). Physical attractiveness
Social Psychology, 38, 131–140. and marriage adjustment in older American couples.
Kenrick, D. T., Gutierres, S. E., & Goldberg, L. L. (1989). Journal of Psychology, 105, 247–252.
Influence of popular erotica on judgments of strangers Prokop, P., & Fedor, P. (2011). Physical attractiveness
and mates. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, influences reproductive success of modern men. Jour-
25, 159–167. nal of Ethology, 29, 453–458.
Attractiveness and relationship outcomes 161

Prokop, P., & Fedor, P. (2013). Associations between Singh, D., Dixson, B. J., Jessop, T. S., Morgan, B., &
body morphology, mating success and mate prefer- Dixson, A. F. (2010). Cross-cultural consensus for
ences among Slovak males and females. Anthropolo- waist-to-hip ratio and women’s attractiveness. Evolu-
gischer Anzeiger, 70, 121–135. tion and Human Behavior, 31, 176–181.
Reis, H. T., Wheeler, L., Spiegel, N., Kernis, M. H., Streeter, S. A., & McBurney, D. H. (2003). Waist-hip
Nezlek, J., & Perri, M. (1982). Physical attractiveness ratio and attractiveness: New evidence and a critique
in social interaction: II. Why does appearance affect of “a critical test.” Evolution and Human Behavior, 24,
social experience? Journal of Personality and Social 88–98.
Psychology, 43, 979–996. Swami, V., & Allum, L. (2012). Perceptions of the physical
Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005). Attrac- attractiveness of the self, current romantic partners, and
tiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness former partners. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
enhance mating success? Evolution and Human 53, 89–95.
Behavior, 26, 186–201. Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2008). The psychology of
Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the invest- physical attraction. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor
ment model: The development (and deterioration) of & Francis.
satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involve- Swami, V., Stieger, S., Haubner, T., Voracek, M., & Furn-
ments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ham, A. (2009). Evaluating the physical attractiveness
45, 101–117. of oneself and one’s romantic partner: Individual and
Rusbult, C. E., Agnew, C., & Arriaga, X. B. (2011). The relationship correlates of the love-is-blind bias. Jour-
investment model of commitment processes. In P. A. nal of Individual Differences, 30, 35–43.
Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1994). Human fluctu-
Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2 pp. ating asymmetry and sexual behavior. Psychological
218–232). London, England: Sage. Science, 5, 297–302.
Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). van Straaten, I., Engles, R., Finkenauer, C., & Hol-
The Investment Model Scale: Measuring commitment land, R. W. (2008). Sex differences in short-term
level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and mate preferences and behavioral mimicry: A
investment size. Personal Relationships, 5, 357–387. semi-naturalistic experiment. Archives of Sexual
Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2001). Human mate poach- Behavior, 37, 902–911.
ing: Tactics and temptations for infiltrating existing Weeden, J., & Sabini, J. (2005). Physical attractiveness and
mateships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- health in Western societies: A review. Psychological
ogy, 80, 894–917. Bulletin, 131, 635–653.
Simpson, V. J., Brewer, G., & Hendrie, C. A. (2014). White, G. L. (1980). Physical attractiveness and courtship
Evidence to suggest that women’s sexual behavior is progress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
influenced by hip width rather than waist-to-hip ratio. ogy, 39, 660–668.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 1367–1371. Yong, J. C., & Li, N. P. (2012). Cash in hand, want
Simpson, G. A., Gangestad, S., & Lerma, M. (1990). better looking mate: Significant resource cues raise
Perception of physical attractiveness: Mechanisms men’s mating standards. Personality and Individual
involved in the maintenance of romantic relationship. Differences, 53, 55–58.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, Zakin, D. F., Blyth, D. A., & Simmons, R. G. (1984).
1192–1201. Physical attractiveness as a mediator of the impact of
Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical early pubertal changes for girls. Journal of Youth and
attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 439–450.
Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 293–307.

S-ar putea să vă placă și