Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

A Theory of

Care-seeking Behavior
Diane Lauver

Although much research has focused upon why people do and do not
engage in recommended health behaviors, there is a need to develop more
accurate theories to explain and predict engagement in health behaviors.
Because health behaviors differ in important ways, it could be most fruitful to
understand one type of health behavior, such as secondary prevention
behavior. Thispaper proposes a theory of care-seekingbehavior (CSB), based
on a theory of general behavior by Triandis. Other popular theories about
health behavior (i.e., Health Belief Model and Theory of Reasoned Action)
also are reviewed. Empirical support for the variables identified in the theory
of CSB is presented, drawing from the literature on secondary prevention for
cancer. The theory of CSB is applied to the situation of seeking care for cancer
symptoms. Directions for future research based upon this theoryare delineated.

[Keywords: prevention; health-related behavior; cancer control;


adherence; care-seeking behavior]

T
he fact that people do not engage in health behaviors as theoretical models of health behavior.
recommended has been discussed frequently and docu- To develop accurate theories of health behavior, it is critical to
mented consistently (Becker, 1985; Becker & Maiman, recognizethe heterogeneityof health-relatedbehaviors (Andersen,
1975;Eraker, Kirscht & Becker, 1984;Kaplan & Simon, 1968; Baric, 1969; Lauver, 1987; Rosenstock, 1990).Preventive
1990; Montano, 1986; Sackett & Snow, 1979). This behaviors are characteristicallydifferent from behaviors to man-
phenomena has been called noncompliance, noncooperation and age illness or disease (Rosenstock, 1990). Preventive behaviors
nonadherence (Becker, 1985; Heiby & Carlson, 1986; Stunkard, are most often undertaken in an asymptomatic state, whereas
1981). Because these terms imply clients’ failures in meeting illness behaviors are often associated with symptoms. Many,
professional expectations, terms such as engagement and lack of although not all, preventive behaviors are undertaken as part of
engagement in recommended health behaviors are preferred a person’s life style, and are independent of the health care
(Kristeller & Rodin, 1984); these terms suggest that useful system. Many, although not all, illness behaviors involve seeking
explanations of health behaviors may be derived from general care either for evaluations or treatment from health care practi-
theories of behavior and convey less judgment and value. tioners and, thus, are undertaken in the context of the health care
Both researchers and clinicians have addressed the problem of system. However, preventive behaviors are not homogenous.
the lack of client engagement in recommended health behaviors. More specifically, prevention behaviors include both primary
The Health Belief Model and the Theory of Reasoned Action are and secondary prevention. The goal of primary prevention is to
popular theories that have guided such research. Although much prevent disease. A person engages in primary prevention behav-
research has focused on understanding health behavior, most ior in the absence of symptoms, such as by adopting a high-fiber,
theories have not provided the desired accuracy in prediction and
adequacy of explanation (Mullen, Hersey & Iverson, 1987; Diane Lauver, RNC, PhD, Beta Eta is Assistant Professor, School of
Wallston & Wallston, 1984).If better theories could be identified Nursing, University of Wisconsin. The reviews of an earlier draft of this
manuscript by Karen A. Pridham, RN, PhD and RichardW. Hoops, MS are
to predict and explain engagement in particular health behaviors, most appreciated as is the assistance of Patricia Feltskog and Lisa
then health care practitioners could use these theories to guide Sornermeyer in the preparation of this manuscript. Correspondence to
interventions to promote recommended behavior. The main School of Nursing, University of Wisconsin, 600 Highland Avenue CSC-
purpose of this paper is to propose a theory of care-seeking K6/262, Madison, WI 53792-2455.
Accepted for publication on April 6, 1992.
behavior. Additional aims are to review and compare alternative
IMAGE: journaf of Nursing Scholarship- Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992 28 1
A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior

low-fat diet to prevent cancer. However, some primary preven- such as feelings about engaging in the recommended behavior,
tion behaviors, for example, exercise or diet, meet goals other could be important to explanations of health behaviors associated
than disease prevention and have positive consequences, such as with a threat.
feeling and looking good. Also, research guided by the HBM has not yielded consistent
In contrast to primary prevention, the goals of secondary support for the model variables. For example, when studied in
prevention are to diagnose disease, detect disabilities in early combination with other variables, perceived severity has been
stages and treat diseases to prevent sequelae. The different goals associated consistently with illness behavior (Janz & Becker,
of primary and secondary prevention may be recognized more 1984)but less often associated with prevention behavior (Becker
frequently by clinicians than clients. Clients engage in secondary & Maiman, 1975;Champion, 1985,1987; Rimer, 1990). Tests of
prevention not only when they seek screening tests in an relationships of the HBM variables with breast self-examination
asymptomatic state, such as hemoccults and mammograms, but (BSE) performance have not revealed consistent relationships
also when they seek care with symptoms or for diagnosed between these variables and BSE performance (Champion, 1985,
diseases. Secondary prevention behaviors share an important 1987,1988; Jacob, Penn & Brown, 1989; Lauver, 1987; Lauver
characteristic: the possibility of discovering an abnormal condi- & Angerame, 1988; Rutledge, Hartmann, Kinmann & Winfield,
tion that could have a negative consequences, such as being 1988). More specifically, 26-28 percent of the variance in BSE
diagnosed with a chronic disease or dysfunction. Engaging in frequency has been explained by some of the HBM variables,
care-seeking behaviors, especially for the goals of screening or especially barriers, but not necessarily by all of these variables
evaluating symptoms, means that clients inherently expose or only by them (Champion, 1985, 1987; Rutledge, 1987).
themselves to the threat of learning that something is seriously Differences in findings from studies using the HBM may be
wrong (Baric, 1969; Lauver, 1987). explained partly by differences in measurement of the HBM
Recognizing special characteristics of one type of preventive variables; they have not been operationalized consistently
health behavior, such as care seeking for secondary prevention, (Cummings,Jette & Rosenstock, 1978;Hill, Gardner & Rassaby,
would help to explain this type of health behavior more fully. If 1985;Jette, Cummings, Brock, Phelps & Naessens, 1981;Mullen
a valid theory for explaining one type of care-seeking behavior et al., 1987; Rimer, 1990; Rutledge et al., 1988; Wallston &
could be identified and supported empirically, then this theory Wallston, 1984).
could be applied to other, similar types of care-seeking behav- Support for the HBM may be limited because of different
iors. interpretations about how to test it. Although indirect relation-
In the following sections, alternative theoretical models of ships are suggestedby the model and in literature about it, explicit
health-related behavior are compared. Variables central to the propositions about how the variables could interrelate have not
theory of care-seeking behavior (CSB) are defined and proposi- been articulated by proponents of the HBM (Becker & Maiman,
tions delineated. Empirical support for the theory is reviewed, 1975; Janz & Becker, 1984). Most researchers have tested only
drawing frequently from the literature on care-seeking and direct relationships between the HBM variables and health
secondary prevention for early detection of cancer. Although the behavior (e.g., Becker, Maiman, Kirscht, Haefner & Drachman,
theory was developed with a focus on care-seeking behavior for 1977; Champion, 1987; Maiman, Becker, Kirscht, Haefner &
secondary prevention, it is not limited to such behavior. Direc- Drachman, 1977; Rutledge et al., 1988). Furthermore, the appli-
tions for future theoretically guided research are delineated. cations of the HBM to prevention behaviors differ sometimes
from applications to illness-focused behaviors (Rimer, 1990;
Models of Health Behavior Rosenstock, 1990). Also, variables that were not identified
originally in the HBM can offer significant explanation of health-
Health Belief Model related behaviors, such as social influences, prior experience
One widely known model used to study the lack of engagement with the health care system and perceived confidence in or
in recommended health behaviors is the health belief model efficacyofthebehavior(Champion, 1985,1987,1988;Cummings,
(HBM). Originally developed to explain preventive behaviors, Becker & Maile, 1980; Lauver & Angerame, 1988; Mullen et al.,
the HBM also has been used to study illness behaviors (Becker 1987; O’Leary, 1985; Olson & Mitchell, 1989; Walker & Glanz,
& Maiman, 1975; Rosenstock, 1990). The variables central to 1986).
this model include: perceived susceptibility and perceived sever-
ity of a disease or condition, barriers and benefits to action, a cue Theory of Reasoned Action
to action and general health motivation. From this model, it is Another theoretical perspective of health behavior, the theory
proposed that clients would be more likely to engage in such of reasoned action (TRA) (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein,
behavior if they had high perceptions of susceptibility to and 1980),has been used frequently to guide health-related research,
severity of a disease or condition, low barriers and high benefits for example, on contraceptive and weight-reduction behavior
to engaging in a related preventive behavior, a cue to act, and a (Davidson, Jaccasd, Triandis, Morales, Diaz-Guerrero, 1976;
high motivation for health behavior in general. This set of Jaccard & Davidson, 1975; Pender & Pender, 1986; Valois,
concepts is intuitively appealing to both clinicians and research- Desharnais & Godin, 1988). Variables central to the original
ers and has guided voluminous research on health behavior. theory include attitude, subjective norm and intention. Attitudes
However, there are limitations of the HBM. Cognitions are and social norms determine intentions, which, in turn, determine
central to the HBM and affect is not integrated explicitly. Affect, behavior. The relationship between intentions and behavior rests

282 IMAGE: Journal of Nursing Scholarship Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992
~ ~~ A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior

on the assumption that the behavior of interest is one that people Triandis’ Theory of Behavior
can initiate voluntarily. More specifically, attitudes are deter-
mined by both perceived consequences of the behavior and Triandis’ theory of behavior (1977, 1980, 1982) could guide
evaluations of those consequences. Subjective norms are deter- research on health behavior. Variables central to the theory
mined by beliefs of social referents that one should or should not include: affect, perceived consequences and values of such
perform the behavior and by the motivation to comply with these consequences, social factors, habit, facilitating conditions and
beliefs. An extension of the TRA is the theory of planned physiologic arousal regarding a particular behavior. Affect is
behavior. In it, clients’ perceived behavioral control (i.e., per- defined as an individual’s “direct emotional response” and
ceived resources and opportunitiesto engage in the behavior) also “immediate and certain emotion” connected with the thought of
influencebehavior, both directly and indirectly, through intention engaging in the behavior (Triandis, 1980, 1982). Perceived
(Ajzen & Madden, 1986). consequencesrefer to the probabilities that salient consequences
There are limitations of the TRA, however. Inherent in the will result from the behavior. Values refer to the evaluation of
theory is an emphasis on explaining intentions rather than behav- these consequences. Social factors include: social norms, that is,
ior (Montano, 1986). Although intention to act is often strongly perceptions of how others believe one should behave; personal
correlated with behavior that is under volitional control, the norms, that is, self-instructions to behave in a manner that is
ultimate goal of clinicians and researchers in health care is to morally acceptable to oneself; and interpersonal agreements to
understand health behavior (Fishbein, 1980; Montano, 1986; engage in the behavior. Habit refers to the degree of an estab-
Smetana & Adler, 1980; Valois et al., 1988). Also, the theory rests lished pattern regarding the behavior and to prior experiences in
heavily on cognitions and rational thought. However, affective carrying out the behavior. Facilitating conditions are objective
dimensions are incorporated somewhat in the concept of attitudes environmental conditions that enable persons to engage in the
and sometimes in the measurement of attitudes (Lierman, Young, behavior. Physiologic arousal refers to the degree of relative
Kasprzyk & Benoliel, 1990; Rimer, 1990). arousal that would drive or promote behavior, such as the role of
Furthermore, research has not supported propositions from the hunger in buying food.
TRA consistently.The influence of attitudes on behavior has been Intentions and habits, as modified by facilitating conditions
found to be direct, rather than indirect through intentions (Bentler and physiological arousal, determine the probability of action.
& Speckart, 1979; Liska, 1984). Also, subjective norms have not Intentions are a function of affect, perceived consequences of
been found to explain intentions consistently (Lierman et al., action as well as values of those consequences,and social factors.
1990; Pender & Pender, 1986; Valois et al., 1988). Variables that Triandis (1980) has expressed these theoretical relationships
have not been incorporated in the theory, such as previous mathematically, stating explicitly how these variables could be
experience with the behavior or symptoms, have been found to be related.
important in explaining behavior (Bentler & Speckart, 1979; The Probability of a Behavior = (wJntentions + w,Habit)
Pender & Pender, 1986; Songer-Nocks, 1976). For example, (Facilitating conditions X Physiological arousal). Intentions and
whereas 39 percent of the variance in BSE frequency was habit are weighted based on individual and situational factors.
explained by variables from the TRA, 60 percent of the variance However, these weights are defined as theoretical rather than
in BSE frequency was explained by variables from the TRA plus statistical constructs (Triandis, 1980). Intentions may influence
variables representing facilitating factors for BSE (Lierman, an established behavior only weakly whereas habits may do so
Kasprzyk, Benoliel, 1991; Lierman et al., 1990). Also, proposi- strongly. Intentions may strongly influence behavior in a novel
tions from the TRA may not hold similarly for those with and situation whereas habits may do so weakly.
without prior personal experience with the target behavior Furthermore,Intention = waAffect+ wc(Probabilitycx Valuesc)
(Brubaker, Prue & Rychtarik, 1987). Propositionsfrom the theory + wnNorms. Theoretical weights are incorporated to reflect
of planned behavior may not hold for persons with and without a individual and situational factors. The middle term represents a
family history of cancer (DeVellis, Blalock & Sandler, 1990). subjective utility factor; it is the sum of the products of the
The measurement of the variables in the TRA deserves consid- expected likelihood of consequences of the behavior and the
eration. Proponents of the TRA maintain that the behavior of values of those consequences. Propositions regarding how these
interest must be specified regarding temporal and contextual variables are related (e.g., regarding the subjective utility term)
factors. To maximize validity of measures, specific items are complement ideas from the HBM that address only what vari-
derived from open-ended interviews about the behavior of inter- ables are important (RoNs & Harel, 1989). Because this theory
est. Also, measures of each predictor variable refer specifically to incorporates a direct measure of affect as well as measures of
the particular behavior. To explain jogging behavior of 20 habit and facilitating conditions, it incorporates concepts that are
minutes duration every other day for a month, measures of social lacking in the theories previously discussed. Thus, it could
norms and intentions would refer to jogging every other day for explain behavior more fully.
20 minutes in the next month (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Carter, Variables from Triandis’ theory have explained 33 percent to
1990). The specificity of such measures may explain why inten- 66 percent of the variance in health behaviors or intentions
tions to engage in a particular behavior and actual engagement in (Montano, 1986; Seibold & Roper, 1979; Valois et al., 1988).
it are highly related. However, the development of situation- Intentions have been used as an indicator of health behaviors
specific measures for each health behavior is time consuming and because the two have been highly associated when persons can
may be impractical (Rimer, 1990). engage voluntarily in the behavior (Fishbein, 1980; Montano,
1MAGE:)ournalof Nursing Scholarship Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992 283
A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior

1986; Smetana & Adler, 1980; Valois et al., 1988). Two studies differently from the latter.
based upon and offering support for the Triandis model are Normative influences include social and personal norms as
noteworthy in that a longitudinal design was used, measuring the well as interpersonal agreements to engage in care seeking.
explanatory and outcome variables separately (Montano, 1986; Social norms are others’ beliefs about care seeking. Personal
Valois et al., 1988). In another study, 60 percent of the variance norms are one’s own beliefs about morally correct behavior
in BSE frequency was explained with the variables from the regarding care seeking. Interpersonal agreement to act reflects a
TRA plus measures of facilitating factors (Lierman, Kasprzyk, promise with another to engage in care seeking.
Benoliel, 1991). Habit refers to how one usually acts when one has symptoms
Concepts similar to Triandis’ theory (i.e., expectations, values (e.g., whether or not one seeks care promptly with symptoms).
and norms) have been compared with the concepts from the Thus, habit reflects one’s usual care-seeking behavior and re-
HBM (i.e., perceived susceptibility and severity, barriers and flects past experience with care seeking. Facilitating conditions
benefits) and the former have been found to be more useful in are specific, objective, external conditions that enable one to seek
explaining behavior (Hill et al., 1985; Mullen et al., 1987; care, such as having health insurance. They are the opposite of
Wallston & Wallston, 1984). When Triandis’ theory has been conditions that would inhibit seeking care. As such, measures of
compared with the TRA, Triandis’ theory: a) is more compre- facilitating conditions may reflect barriers identified in the
hensive because it includes the concepts of affect, habit, and HBM.The selection of these variables as the major explanatory
facilitating conditions; b) can be more powerful and sensitive to variables of behavior rests on the assumption that behavior is
differences among persons of differing socioeconomic status; influenced by many factors (i.e., persons’ feelings, cognitions,
and c) is as useful in explaining behavior (Jaccard & Davidson, social influences, past experiences and objective, external facili-
1975; Montano & Taplin, 1991; Seibold & Roper, 1979; Valois tators regarding the behavior).
et al., 1988). Although Triandis’ model may not be simple, it The relationships among the theoretically identified vari-
may be more complete and useful in explaining health behaviors ables are proposed as follows. Psychosocial variables could
(Wallston & Wallston, 1984). influence behavior in interaction with facilitating conditions
(i.e.. be conditional upon), as proposed by Triandis (1977,
A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior 1980). This proposition rests on the assumption that the pres-
ence of the conditions reflected by the psychosocial variables
Because Triandis’ (1977, 1980, 1982) theory of behavior are necessary, but not sufficient, to engage in care-seeking
could be especially useful in explaining health behavior, a behavior. However, the influences of psychosocial variables on
theory of care-seeking behavior (CSB) has been developed behavior could be direct, as suggested by clinically based
based upon it (Figure 1).The probability of engaging in health research that is reviewed below. Psychosocial variables are
behavior is a function of psychosocial variables (affect, expec- proposed to be the sum of a) affect, b) the products of
tations and values about outcomes, habit and norm) and facili- corresponding expectations and values about outcomes (i.e.,
tating conditions regarding the behavior. utility), c) norms and d) habit. Variables extrinsic to the theory,
Affect refers to feelings associated with care-seeking behav- such as clinical and demographic factors, are proposed to
ior, such as anxiety about a serious diagnosis or embarrassment influence behavior only indirectly, that is, by mediation through
about an examination. Expectations refer to beliefs about the the theoretically identified variables. The latter proposition
likelihood of relevant outcomes of care seeking and values rests on the assumption that clinical and demographic factors do
refer to the importance of those outcomes. When correspond- not influence behavior per se. These factors could influence the
ing expectation and value ratings about particular desired theoretical variables, (e.g.. expectations, values, norms) which,
outcomes of care seeking are multiplied together and summed, in turn, influence behavior.
a utility term is computed, based on subjective expected utility The theory of CSB differs from Triandis’ theory in two ways.
theory (Schoemaker, 1982). Utility reflects the overall worth of Intention is not included as a separate concept focusing on
care seeking; it is similar to the measure of attitudes in the TRA variables that could explain health behavior. Also, physiologic
and the idea of benefits from the HBM, but is measured arousal is not included as a separate predictor of behavior. The
physiological arousal associated with a health threat (e.g., cancer
or dysfunction) would be likely to result in negative affect (e.g.,
I Clinical l F l anxiety about receiving a serious diagnosis or depression about
confirming a loss of function). Thus, by including affect, which
and

socio-
- Utility
IExpecraclnrrs
and Values &)uf
Oufcorne\’,
Swking
is assumed to be an indicator of arousal, the inclusion of arousal
would not be necessary. Furthermore, actual physiologic arousal
that is relevant to health behavior is difficult to identify and
Behavior
demographic Norms Facilitating measure.
Empirical findings regarding these propositions are reviewed
Variables Habits below, drawing on examples of care seeking for secondary
prevention of cancer. Affective variables, such as anxiety, fear of
Figure 1 : Theory of Care-seeking Behavior cancer, depression and denial, have been related directly and
negatively to prompt care seeking among breast cancer clients

284 IMAGE: Journal of Nursing Scholarship Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992
A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior

(Green & Roberts, 1974; Magarey, Todd & Blizard, 1977; Smith symptoms were found to interact to influence clients’ use of
& Anderson, 1987; Worden & Weisman, 1975). In contrast, mammography (Lane & Fine, 1983). To the extent that facilitat-
anxiety and intense emotional responses about breast changes ing conditions are a function of economic status, research that
also have been associated positively with intentions to seek care addressed socioeconomic status could be relevant. Socioeco-
and actual prompt care seeking for breast symptoms (Cameron & nomic status has been found to modify the relationship of affect
Hinton, 1968; Lauver & Chang, 1991; Timko, 1987). The and beliefs about cervical cancer with intentions to seek Pap tests
direction of the relationship between negative affect and behav- as well as the relationship of personal norms about family
ior is not clear. Also, negative affect may be related to care- planning with intentions to engage in contraceptive behavior
seeking behavior in a nonlinear manner or in combination with (Davidson et al., 1976;Seibold & Roper, 1979).Whether particu-
other variables, such as prior experience with a similar situation lar facilitating conditions interact with the psychosocial vari-
(Green & Roberts, 1974; Hackett, Cassem & Raker, 1973). ables to influence care-seeking behavior is unclear. Whether
Expectations about outcomes of care seeking, (e.g., beliefs findings about the interactive effects of facilitating conditions,
about the prevalence of cancer and effective treatments) have based on studies of asymptomatic health behavior, generalize to
been associated with care seeking for cancer symptoms symptomatic behavior is not known.
(Bransfield, Hankey &Wesley, 1989; Cochran, Hacker & Berek, Clinicalfactors (e.g., presence and type of symptom as well as
1986;Green & Roberts, 1974;Smith & Anderson, 1987;Worden history of prior related problems) have been related directly to
& Weisman, 1975). Furthermore, the sum of the products of cancer screening and to intended and actual care seeking
corresponding expectations and values about outcomes has been (Bransfield et al., 1989; Cochran et al., 1986; Funch, 1988;
related directly to intentions to engage in breast self-examination Gould-Martin, Paganini-Hill, Casagrande, Mack & Ross, 1982;
and cervical cancer screening, intentions to seek care with breast Lauver & Chang, 1991; Lauver & Rubin, 1990; Mettlin, Reese
symptoms, actual breast self-examination and mammography & Murphy, 1980; Michielutte, Diseker, Young & May, 1985;
screening (Hill et al., 1985; Lauver & Chang, 1991; Lierman et Smith & Anderson, 1987). These findings may not be consistent
al., 1990; Montano & Taplin, 1991; Seibold & Roper, 1979; with the proposition that variables extrinsic to the theory explain
Timko, 1987). Social norms and personal norms have been health behavior only by mediation through the theoretical vari-
related to actual and intended health behaviors, including cancer ables. Whether or not clinical factors influence behavior when
screening and mammography (Hill et al., 1985; Lierman et al., controlling for theoretical variables is an issue that warrants
1990; Page1 & Davidson, 1984; Seibold & Roper, 1979; Timko, further study.
1987; Valois et al., 1988; Zapka, Stoddard, Costanza & Greene, In general, demographicfactors (e.g., age and education) have
1989). The influence of interpersonal agreement to engage in a not been related consistently to cancer screening or care seeking
particular behavior rarely has been included in tests of Triandis’ for cancer symptoms (Burack & Liang, 1989; Funch, 1988;
theory (Montano, 1986;Jaccard & Davidson, 1975; Valois et al., Greenwald, Becker & Nevitt, 1978; Lierman, 1988; Samet et al.,
1988). In an exception, the influence of interpersonal agreement 1988;Taplin et al., 1989). These findings support the proposition
was studied in combination with social and personal norms and that variables extrinsic to the theory explain health behavior only
was a significant predictor of intentions to get Pap tests (Seibold through theoretical variables.
& Roper, 1979). Habits (e.g., having engaged in prior breast Overall, empirical findings support the proposed relationships
cancer screening and having sought symptomatic care) have between the variables identified in the theory of CSB with regard
been associated with engaging in screening behaviors for breast to care seeking for secondary prevention. However, few studies
cancer (Burack & Liang, 1989; Lauver, 1989; Samet, Hunt, have incorporated the complete set of the theoretical variables or
Lerchen & Goodwin, 1988; Taplin, Anderman & Grothaus, have assessed fully the propositions identified from Triandis’
1989) and intentions to seek care for breast cancer symptoms theory of behavior. Much of the empirical support comes from
(Lauver & Chang, 1991).Thus, the variables of expectations and separate studies that were guided by different theories of health
values about outcomes, norms and habits could have direct behavior; some support comes from atheoretical research. Test-
influences on care-seeking behavior. ing the combined influences of the theoretical variables and the
Facilitating conditions (e.g., health insurance and having a manner in which they influence behavior is essential to assess the
regular health care practitioner) have been related directly to usefulness of the theory.
health behaviors, including seeking mammography (Lane, Future research on health behavior could build on previous
Poldenak & Burg, 1989; Montano & Taplin, 1991; Safer, clinically based research by using a behavioral theory that
Tharps, Jackson & Leventhal, 1979; Weissman, Stern, Fielding incorporates relevant variables. Also, future research on health
& Epstein, 1989). The interaction effects of facilitating condi- behavior could build on theoretically based research by testing
tions with the other theoretical variables have not been tested ideas derived from the theory of CSB. This research could
often or well (Davidson et al., 1976; Hill et al., 1985; Seibold address: 1) whether facilitating conditions interact with the
& Roper, 1979). In one study, prompt care-seeking behavior for psychosocial variables to influence care-seeking behavior and 2)
breast cancer symptoms was explained, in part, by the interac- whether clinical and demographic variables influence behavior
tion of anxiety and having a regular health practitioner. Women only indirectly by mediation through theoretical variables. De-
with low anxiety and no regular practitioner delayed longer than veloping and using similar measures of variables from the theory
women with either high anxiety or a regular practitioner (Lauver of CSB across different situations not only would be efficient but
& Ho, 1991). Also, having no-cost mammography and breast also important to assess the usefulness of the theory. Findings

IMAGE:Journal of Nursing Scholarship. Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992 285


A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior

from such research could offer better explanations of CSBs, Eraker, S., Kirscht, J. & Becker, M. (1984). Understanding and improving
clarify inconsistencies and address omissions in past research. patient compliance. Annals of Internal Medicine, 100,258-268.
Fishbein, M. (1980). A theory of reasoned action: Some applications and impli-
By applying the theory of CSB to one type of health behavior, cations. In M.M. Page (Ed.), 1979 Nebraska symposiumon motivation, 65-
such as CSB for secondary prevention, researchers could extend 116, Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press.
theoretical research to explain clients’ engagement in behaviors Funch, D. (1988). Predictors and consequences of symptom reporting behaviors
for cancer control. Health care professionals could use these in colorectal cancer patients. Medical Care, 26, 1000-1008.
auld-Martin, K., Paganini-Hill, A., Casagrande, C., Mack, T. & Ross, R.
explanations to design interventions to promote care-seeking (1982). Behavioral and biological determinants of surgical stage of breast
behaviors, for example, early cancer detection in an effort to cancer. Preventive Medicine, 11,429-440.
improve survival with cancer. If explanations of care-seeking Green, L. & Roberts, B. J. (1974). The research literature on why women delay
behavior were supported empirically for one condition, such as in seekingmedical care for breast symptoms.Health Education Monographs,
2, 129-177.
cancer, then these explanations could be tested in relation to care Greenwald, H., Becker, S. & Nevitt, M. (1978). Delay and noncompliance in
seeking for similarly threatening conditions and other health cancer detection. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly Bulletin, 56,212-230.
behaviors.@Q Hackett, T., Cassem, N. & Raker, J. (1973). Patient delay in cancer. New
References England Journal of Medicine, 289.14-20.
Ajzen,I. & Fishbein,M. (1980).Understandingattitudesandpredictingsocial Heiby, E. & Carlson, J. (1986). The health compliance model. Journal of
behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Compliancein Health Care, 1,135-154.
Ajzen, I. & Madden, T. (1986). Prediction of goal directed behavior: Attitudes, Hill, D., Ganlner, G. & Rassaby, J. (1985). Factors predisposing women to take
intentions and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social precautions against breast and cervix cancer. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 22,453-474. Psychology, 15,59-79.
Andersen, R. (1968). A behavioral model of families use of health services. Jaccard, J. & Davidson, A. (1975). A comparison of two models of social
(Research Series No. 25.) Chicago: Center for Health Administration Studies, behavior. Sociometry, 38,497-5 17.
University of Chicago. Jacob, T., Penn, N. & Brown,M. (1989). Breast self-examination: Knowledge,
Baric, L. (1969). Recognition of the at-risk role: A means to influence behavior. attitudes and performance among black women. Journal of National Medical
International Journal of Health Education, 12(1), 24-34. Association, 81,769-776.
Becker, M. (1985). Patient adherence to prescribed therapies. Medical Care, 23, Jam, N. & Becker, M. (1984). The health belief model: A decade later. Health
539-555. Education Quarterly, 11(1), 1-47.
Becker, M. & Maiman, L. (1975). Sociobehavioraldeterminants of compliance Jette, A., Cummings, K., B m k , B., Phelps, M. & Naessens, J. (1981). The
with health and medical care recommendations.Medical Care, 13, 10-24. structureand reliability of health belief indices. Health ServiceResearch, 16,
Becker, M., Maiman, L., Kirscht, J. Haefner, D. & Drachman, R (1977). The 81-93.
health belief model and prediction of dietary compliance: A field experiment. Kaplan, R. 81S i o n , H. (1990). Compliancein medical care: Reconsiderationof
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 18,348-366. self-predictions. AM& of Behavioral Medicine, 12,66-71.
Bentler, P. & Speckart, G. (1979). Models of attitude-behaviorrelations. Psy- Kristeller, J. & Rodin, A. (1984). A three-stage models of treatment continuity:
chological Review, 86,452-464. Compliance, adherence, maintenance. In A. Baum, S.E. Taylor & J.E. Singer
Bransfield,D., Hankey, B. & Wesley, M. (1989, October). Saciodemographic (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and health (Vol. IV:Social psychological
variables associated with patient delay among blacWwhite breast cancer aspects of health, 85-1 12, Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum &. Associates.
patients. Paper presented at the American Public Health Association, Chicago,IL. Lane, D. & Fine, H. (1983). Compliance with mammography referrals. New
Brubaker, R., Prue, D. & Rychtarik, R (1987). Determinants of disulfiram York State Journal of Medicine, 83,173-176.
acceptance among alcohol patients: A test of the theory of reasoned action. Lane,D.,Poldenak,A.&Burg,M.(1989).Barriersto breastcancerscreening:
Addictive Behavior, 12,43-51. Comparison of surveys of women and physicians. Paper presented at the
Burack, R. & Liang, J. (1989). Acceptanceand completionof mammography by American Public Health Association meeting, Chicago, IL.
older black women. American Journal of Public Health, 79,721-726. Lauver, D. (1987). Theoretical perspectives relevant to breast self-examination.
Cameron, A. & Hinton, J. (1968). Delay in seeking treatment for mammary Advances in Nursing Science, 9(4), 16-24.
tumors. Cancer, 21, 1121-26. Lauver, D. (1989). Instructional information and breast self-examination prac-
Carter, W. (1990). Health behavior as a rational process: Theory of reasoned tice. Research in Nursing & Health, 12, 11-19.
actionandmultiattributeutilitytheory. InK.Glanz,F.Lewis&B. Rimer(Eds.), Lauver, D. & Angerame, M. (1988). Developmentof a questionnaire to measure
Health behavior and health education, 63-91. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass beliefs and attitudes about breast self-examination. Cancer Nursing, 1,51-57.
Publishing. Lauver, D. & Cbang, A. (199 1). Testing theoreticalexplanationsof intentionsto
Champion, V. (1985). Use of the health belief model in determiningfrequency of seek care for a breast cancer symptom.Journal of Applied SocialPsychology,
breast self-examination. Research in Nursing & Health, 8,373-379. 21, 1440-1458.
Champion V. (1987). The relationshipof breast self-examinationto health belief Lauver, D. & Ho, C. (1991). Explaining care-seekingbehavior for breast cancer
model variables. Research in Nursing & Health, 10,375-382. symptom. Manuscript under review.
Champion, V. (1988). Attitudinal variables related to intention, frequency and Lauver, D. & Rubin, M. (1990). Message framing, dispositional optimism and
proficiency of breast self-examination in women 35 and over. Research in follow-upfor abnormalPapanicolaou smears. Research in Nursing &Health,
Nursing & Health, 11,283-291. 13, 199-207.
Cochran, S., Hacker, N. & Berek, J. (1986). Correlates of delay in seeking Lierman, L. (1988). Response to the discovery of breast changes: Women’s
treatment for endometrial cancer. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & responses and nursing implications. Cancer Nursing, 11,352-361.
Gynecology, 5,245-252. Lierman, L., Kasprzyk, D. & Benoliel, J. (1991). Understanding adherence to
Cummings,K., Becker, M. & Maile, M. (1980). Bringing the models together: breast self-examination in older women. Western Journal of Nursing Re-
An empirical approach to combining variables used to explain health actions. search, 13,46-66.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 3, 123-145. Lierman, L., Young, H., Kasprzyk, D. & Benoliel, J. (1990). Predicting breast
Cummings, K., Jette, A. & Rosenstock, I. (1978). Construct validation of the self-examinationusing the theory of reasoned action. Nursing Research, 39,
health belief model. Health Education Monographs, 6,394-405. 97-101.
Davidson, A., Jaccard, J., Triandis, H., Modes, M. & Dim-Guerrero, R. Liska,A. (1984). A critical examination of the causal structure of the Fishbeid
(1976). Cross-cultural model testing: Toward a solution of the etic-emic Ajzen attitude-behaviormodel. Social Psychology Quarterly, 47,61-74.
dilemma. International Journal of Psychology, 11,1-13. Maiman, L., Becker, M., Kirscht, J., Haefner, D. & Drachman, R. (1977).
DeVellis, B., Blalock, S. & Sandier, R. (1990). Predictingpaaicipation in cancer Scalesfor measuring health belief model dimensions:A test of predictive value,
screening:The role of perceived behavioral control.Journal of Applied Social internal consistency and relationshipsamong beliefs. Health EducationMono-
Psychology, 20,639-660. graphs, 5,215-230.

IMACE:/ourna/of Nursing Scholarship. Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992


A Theory of Care-seeking Behavior

Magarey, C., Todd, P. & B l h r d , P. (1977). Psychosocial factors influencing Schoemaker, P. (1982). The expected utility model: Its variants, purposes,
delay and breast self-examination in women with symptoms of breast cancer. evidence and limitations.Journal of Economic Literature, 20,529-567.
Social Science & Medicine, 11,229-232. Seibold, D. & Roper, R. (1979). Psychosocial determinants of health care
Mettlin, C., Reese, P. & Murphy, G. (1980). Care-seeking behavior among intentions: Test of the Triandis and Fishbein models. In D. Nimmo (Ed.),
positive screenees. Preventive Medicine, 9,5 18-524. Communication yearbook 3 625-643. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Michielutte, R., Diseker, R., Young, L. & May, J. (1985). Noncompliance in Books.
screening follow-up among family planning clinic patients with cervical Smetana, J. & Adler, N. (1980). Fishbein’s value & expectancy model: An
dysplasia.Preventive Medicine, 14,248-258. examination of some assumptions.Personality Social PsychologyBulletin, 6,
Montano, D. (1986). Predicting and understanding influenza vaccination behav- 89-96.
ior: Alternatives to the health belief model. Medical Care, 24,438-453. Smith, E. & Anderson, B. (1987).Symptomatology,delay and stageof disease in
Montano, D. & Taplin, S. (1991). A test of an expanded theory of reasoned action endometrial cancer. Cancer Detection & Prevention, 10,247-254.
to predict mammographyparticipation. Social Science & Medicine, 32,733- Songer-Nocks, E. (1976). Situationalfactors affecting the weighing of predictor
741. components in the Fishbein model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Mullen, P., Hersey, J. & Iverson, D. (1987). Health behavior models compared. o g ~ 12.56-69.
,
Social Science & Medicine, 24,973-98 1. Stunkard, A. (1981). Adherence to medical treatment: Overview and lessons
O’Leary, A. (1985). Self-efficacyand health. Behavioral Research Theory, 23, from behavioral weight control. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 25,
437-457. 187-197.
Olson, R. & Mitchell, E. (1989). Self-confidenceas a critical factor in breast self- Taplin, S., Anderman, C. & Grothaus, L. (1989). Breast cancer risk and
examination.Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic,and Neonatal Nursing, 18, participationin mammographic screening.American Journal of PublicHealth,
476-48 1. 79, 1494-1498.
Pagel, M. & Davidson, A. (1984). A comparison of three social-psychological Timko, C. (1987). Seeking medical care for a breast cancer symptom: Determi-
models of attitude and behavioral plan: Prediction of contraceptive behavior. nants of intentions to engage in prompt or delay behavior. Health Psychology,
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 47,517-533. 6, 305-328.
Pender, N.J. & Pender, A.R. (1986).Attitudes, subjective norms and intentions Triandis, H.(1977). Intrapersonal behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/ColePub-
to engage in health behaviors. Nursing Research, 35, 15-18. lishing Companies.
Rimer, B. (1990).Perspectives on interpersonal theories in health education and Triandis, H.(1980). Values, attitudes and interpersonal behavior. In M.M. Page
healthbehavior.InK.Glanz,F.Lewis&B.Rimer(Eds.),Healthbehaviorand (Ed.), 1979 Nebraska symposium on motivation, 195-259, Lincoln, NB:
health education 140-157.San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishing. University of Nebraska Press.
Ronis D. & Harel, Y.(1989). Health beliefs and breast examination behaviors: Triandis, H. (1982). A model of choice in marketing. In L. McAlister (Ed.),
Analyses of linear structuralrelations. Psychology and Health, 3,259-285. Choice modelsfor buyer behavior, 147-163,Greenwich, C T JAI Press, Inc.
Rosenstock, I. (1 990). The health belief model: Explaining health behavior Valois, P., Desharnais, R.& Godin, G. (1988). A comparisonof the Fishbein and
through expectancies. In K. Glanz, F. Lewis & B. Rimer (Fds.), Health Ajzen and the Triandis attitudinal models for the prediction of exercise inten-
behavior and health education 39-62. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publish- tion and behavior. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 11,459-472.
ing. Walker, L. & Glanz, K. (1986). Psychosocial determinants of breast self-
Rutledge, D. (1987).Factors related to women’s practice of breast self-examina- examination. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 2,169- 178.
tion. Nursing Research, 36, 117-121. Wallston, B.S. & Wallston, K. (1984). Social psychological models of health
Rutledge, D., Hartmann,W., Kinmann, P. & Winfeld, A. (1988). Exploration behavior: An examination and integration. In A. Baum, S.E. Taylor & J.E.
of factors affecting mammographybehaviors. Preventive Medicine, 17,412- Singer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and heaIth (Vol. IV: Social psycho-
422. logical aspects of health 23-53. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum & Associ-
Sackett, D. & Snow,J. (1979).Themagnitudeofcomplianceandnoncompliance. ates.
In R. Haynes, D. Taylor & D. Sackett (Eds.),Compliance in health care, 11- Weissman, J., Stern, R., Fielding, S. & Epstein, A. (1989, August). Delayed
22, Baltimore, MD & London: The Johns Hopkins University Press. access to health care: Who is at risk, what are their reasons and what does
Safer, M., Tharps, Q., Jackson, T. & Leventhal, H. (1979). Determinants of it mean? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Public Health
three stages of delay in seeking care at a medical clinic. Medical Care, 17, Association, Chicago, IL.
11-29. Worden, J. & Weisman, A. (1975). Psychosocial components of lagtime in
Samet, J., Hunt, W., Lerchen, M. & Goodwin, J. (1988).Delay in seeking care cancer diagnosis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 19,69-79.
for cancer symptoms: A population-based study of elderly New Mexicans. Zapka, J., Stoddard, A., Costanza, M. & Greene, H.(1989). Breast cancer
Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 80,432-438. screening by mammography: Utilization and associated factors. American
Journal of Public Health, 79,1499-1502.

SOURCES AND RESOURCES


E.T. Net is anonline computer conferencenetwork whose purpose is to electronically link developersand users ofinteractive technology in health
care education.E.T. Net allows users and developersto share reviews on available s o b a r e , hardware and videodiscs (information on those under
development and those which they would like to see developed) as well as new applications of interactive hardware to health science education.
Sponsored by the Educational Technology Branch of the Lister Hill National Center for Biomedical Communications, E.T. Net is open to
professionals engaged in either the development or use of interactive technology in health professional education. It is free, available 24 hours a
day, seven days a week. E.T. Net also includes NUCARE (NUrsing CAre REsearch) conference which provides opportunities for researchers to
exchange information significant to nursing care research. E.T. Net may be reached through a local Telenet number or via the Internet. For more
information, or help with access, contact: E.T. Net Coordinator, The LearningCenter for Interactive Technology, Lister Hill National Center for
BiomedicalCommunications,National Library OfMedicine, 8600 RockvillePike, Bethesda, Maryland 20894. Phone: (301)496-0508, FAX (301)
480-3035; E-mail: TL.C@lhc.n.m.nih.gov

SOURCES AND RESOURCES

IMAGE:/ourna/ ofNursing Scholarship. Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992 287


Key Aspects of Caring for the Acutely Ill:
Technological Aspects, Patient Education, and Quality of Life
Fifih National Conference on Research for Clinical Practice

“Key Aspects of Caringfor the Acutely Ill”


Kathleen Dracup, DNSc, RN, FAAN
University of California, Los Angels

“Managing Technology and Complex Care” 6cCoorciinatingPatient and Family Education ”


Kathleen Stone, PhD, RN, FAAN Carol Lindeman, PhD, RN, FAAN
Ohio State University Oregon Health Sciences University

“Maintainingthe Patient’s Quality of Life” “Caringfor the Acutely Ill Child”


Carolyn Cooper, PhD, RN Barbara Turner, DNSc, RN, FAAN
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Madigan Army Medical Center,Tacoma W A
April 22-24,1993,Chapel Hill, North Carolina
Clinicians, administrators, and researchers will examinecurrent will focusonassessingtheapplicability ofresearch forpradcesires,
research and practice in caring for acutely ill patients--including overcoming barriers to using research, and implementing research
surgical patients, trauma patients, patients suffering from exacerba- findings from the conference; individual guidance will be provided.
tions of chronicillness, immuno-compromised patients, and psychi- Workshop fee is $50, space limited.
atricpatients. Keytopicsincludemanagementofcomplexandhighly The conferencehorkshop is funded by the Division of Nursing,
technical care, patient and family education and preparation for DHHS, and co-sponsored by the University of North Carolina at
discharge, quality of life, and related issues. Discussions of the Chapel Hill School of Nursing, Alpha Alpha Chapter of Sigma Theta
practical uses of current research and suggestions for further study Tau, and the Southern Nursing Research Society.
will follow research presentations. The conference fee of $160 Contact:Ruth Wiese, Projectcoordinator, CW7460Camngton
includes a book of research and discussions based on the conference. Hall, UNC-CH School of Nursing, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-7460,
A preconference workshop for clinicians and administrators (919)966-2263for registration materials.

I
I ~ THE UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN
SCHOOLOF

Postdoctoral Nursing Research


Fellowship in Neuroscience
Neurobehavior
Family Relations is helping family professionals find
TRAINING PROGRAM G O A L S To develop nurse sci- the information they need. An applied scholarly
entists capable of sustaining independent research careers journal for family practitioners and educators, like
focused on generating knowledge about human responses yourself. Family Relations is the SOURCE for
and behaviors associated with altered braidcognitive func- multidisciplinaryapplied research. Are you a user of
tioning. family related research?
TRAINING SUPPORT: is for 1 to 2 year period. Annual
stipends depend on qualifications and experience. Positions Let us prove that Family Relations can be your
open for 1992 and 1993. Funded by the National Center for
SOURCE, call or write for a FREE SampleIssue.
Nursing Research. Inquiries about the program are welcome
at any time. Candidates of color are encouraged to apply.
F O R APPLICATION GUIDELINES AND INF’ORMA- For more information call or write:
National Council on Family Relations

b
TION: Barbara Therrien, PhD, RN, FAAN, School of
Nursing, 400 N. Ingalls, Ann Arbor, MI48109-0482, Phone 3989 Central Ave. NE, #550
I (313)
. , 763-0002 Minneapolis, MN 55421
Equal Opportunity/Aficmatke Action Employer -0 612/781-9331 FAX 612/781-9348
288 1MACE:)ournal of Nursing Scholarship- Volume 24, Number 4, Winter 1992

S-ar putea să vă placă și