Sunteți pe pagina 1din 353

The Cerebellum Revisited

Rodolfo Llinas Constantino Sotelo


Editors

The Cerebellum
Revisited
With 209 Illustrations, 5 in Full Color

Springer-Verlag
New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris
Tokyo Hong Kong Barcelona Budapest
Rodolfo Llinas Constantino Sotelo
Department of Physiology/Biophysics INSERM U-106
New York University Medical Center H6pital de la Salpetriere
550 First Avenue 47 Boulevard de I'H6pital
New York, NY 10016 75013 Paris
USA France

Library of Congress Cataloguing-in-Publication Data

The Cerebellum revisited/Rodolfo Llim'ts, Constantino Sotelo,


editors.
p. cm.
Based on the Cajal Centenary Meeting on The Neuron Doctrine held
in 1988 in Barcelona, Spain.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN-13:978-1-4612-7691-3
1. Cerebellum-Physiology-Congresses. I. Llinas, Rodolfo R.
(Rodolfo Riascos). II. Sotelo, Constantino. III. Cajal
Centenary Meeting on The Neuron Doctrine (1988:Barcelona, Spain)
[DNLM: 1. Cerebellum-physiology-congresses. WL 320 C414 1988]
QP379.C46 1991
599'.0188-dc20
DNLM/DLC 91-5226

Printed on acid-free paper.

© 1992 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc.


Sof tcover reprint. of the hardcover 1st edition 1992

All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without
the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag New York, Inc., 175 Fifth Avenue,
New York, NY 10010, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly
analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic
adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or
hereafter developed is forbidden.
The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc., in this publication,
even if the former are not especially identified, is not to be taken as a sign that such names,
as understood by the Trade Marks and Merchandise Marks Act, may accordingly be used
freely by anyone.
While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date
of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal
responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no
warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein.

Production managed by Natalie Johnson: manufacturing supervised by Jacqui Ashri.

Typeset by Thomson Press (India) Ltd., New Delhi, India.

9 8 7 6 543 2 1

ISBN-13:978-1-4612-7691-3 e-ISBN -J3: 978-1-4612- 2840-0


DOl: J0.1007/978-1-4612-2840-0
Preface

This book is organized into three parts that correspond with the main
groups of chapters delivered during the Cajal Centenary Meeting on
The Neutron Doctrine. These chapters represent important aspects of the
morphology, development, and function of the cerebellum and related
structures. Clearly an exhaustive analysis of all aspects of the cerebellar
system, as they relate to the legacy of Ramon y Cajal, would be impossible
to contain in just one volume, given its far-reaching impact. Instead, we
deliberately steered away from the traditional handbook approach that
some of us have taken in the past and selected those aspects of cerebellar
research currently under vigorous study that would also represent the
widest scope of interest for neuroscientists in general and for cerebellar
specialists in particular.
In particular, we felt that as the discrete anatomy of the cerebellum is
quite well known, only certain aspects of the structure should be discussed
here. For example, the organization of the pontocerebellar pathways, we
felt, would be particularly interesting given the enormity of the system in
higher vertebrates. Also of interest is the distribution and development
of the synaptology and neurotransmitter properties in this cortex. Indeed,
from the point of view of cerebellar development, this may represent one
of the clearest paradigms in the understanding of rules for neurogenesis
for the central nervous system. More specifically, aspects relating to the
precise organization of the shape of Purkinje cells and the epigenetic
restrictions that govern the crystal-like structure of the cerebellar cortex
are being concretized and are expected to be generalizable to other regions
of the nervous system. This is particularly significant given the precise
crystal-like organization of the morphology of the cerebellar cortex.
Purkinje cells are isoplanar and strictJy regimented with regard to each
other, thereby producing maximum divergence and maximum convergence
for their parallel fiber input. Equally precise is the distribution of the
granular cell-parallel fiber system and the rostrocaudal distribution in
the olivocerebellar system.
On the other hand, the simplicity of connectivity that characterizes this
cortex throughout evolution and the fact that the basic cerebellar circuit
described by Cajal recurs at different levels of phylogeny from the lower
vertebrates to man indicates the presence of powerful laws of morphogenesis

v
VI Preface

where those variables that determine the final structure of neural nets
may be studied more dearly. With regard to the functional aspects of the
cerebellum, some specific issues may be mentioned concerning the electro-
physiology of the Purkinje and the olivary cells that are of general interest
to electrophysiologists and of special interest to those who wish to under-
stand integrative cell physiology 'or the organization of rhythmic para-
computation in the olivocerebellar system.
In terms of electrophysiology, Purkinje cells are the largest neural
elements in the brain and the epitome of neuronal interaction. They have
been studied intracellularly with single and double penetrations, where
the electrical activity of both the soma and dendrites of a given neuron
are simultaneously recorded. Biophysically, the use of the patch-damp
techniques have yielded descriptions of the voltage- and ligated-dependent
currents in these neurons with a degree of detail seldom obtained in other
neuronal elements of the brain. By contrast, the inferior olivary cells are
the prototypical oscillating neurons of the vertebrate brain with their ionic
conductances and are organized in a very special fashion. They represent
the other extreme of neuronal functioning from the Purkinje cell. Their
ability to integrate and transmit information is limited by their powerful
intrinsic oscillatory properties. Thus, it is only through studies of neuron
ensembles, as may be studied in slices, and more particularly with the

Figure 1. Vertical section of the cerebellar folium in chicken stained with the
Golgi method. The three parenthesis (to the right) show, in A the molecular layer,
in B the granular layer, and in C the white substance. (A) is the body of the
Purkinje cell; (B) the Deiters prolongation of the neuron (the axon); (D) a small
stellate cell; (L) the axonal prolongation of those elements; (C) the terminal
branches of this prolongation; (S) the hole left by the descending branch where
the body of the Purkinje cell is lodged; (H) small elements of the granular layer
with the axonal cylinder directly upwards; (F) large stellate cell of the granular
layer; (G) axonal cylinder of that cell. (Reprinted from S. Ramon y Cajal, May
1988. Estructura de los centros nerviosos de las aves. I. Cerebelo. Revist. trimestr.
de Histol. norm. y patol., 1. Mayo de 1888.)
Preface Vll

Figure 2. Section of the cerebellar folium in pigeon stained with the Golgi method.
For the sake of clarity, the Purkinje cells are not depicted. (A) neuroglial cells
from the molecular layer; (B) neuroglial cells from the granular layer; (C) tree-like
neuroglial cells; (D) large stellate cell with its axon terminating in a large number
of varicose ramifications; (E) nodose fiber (mossy fiber) with small ramification;
(F and G) varicose vertical fibers, (P) stellate elements of the molecular layer,
(0) axon from the cell in P. (Reprinted from S. Ramon y Cajal, May 1888. Estructura
de los centros nerviosos de las aves. 1. Cerebelo. Revist. trimestr. de Ristol. norm.
y patol.. 1. Mayo de 1888.)

chimeric hardware-neuronal hybrids developed by Y. Yaron-that we


begin to understand the properties of such neuronal groupings.
The third aspect of the book relates to the physiology of cerebellar
systems, most particularly, the way in which neuronal afferents interact
at the level of the cerebellar cortex. Because the cerebellar cortex is close
to homogeneous along its surface, many of the details of functional
diversity encountered relate to the nature of information that reaches the
cerebellar cortex as well as the nature of afferents that reach it.
The cerebellar cortex may be viewed as a radial with fractured somato-
topy when considering the mossy fiber system and rostrocaudal bonded
projection considering the climbing fiber systems (see Figs. 1 and 2).
Although published in 1888, Figures 1 and 2 depict the cellular elements
in the cerebellar cortex as we know them today. They are also the first
description of the neuron as a distinct cellular element having a body, a
dendritic tree, and axon. These nerve cells were described as "contacting,"
without direct continuity, their target cells, i.e., the author indicated that
the cells maintain their individuality.
Finally, beyond the understanding of the cerebellar mapping and the
different distribution of cerebellar afferents, the issues relating to the role
of the cerebellum in motor coordination continue to challenge us. Indeed,
Vlll Preface

the precise manner in which the neuronal circuits, so well understood,


ultimately control the execution of movement is far from clear.
Nevertheless, progress has occurred; many ideas currently related to
overall cerebellar functioning are under investigation. In short, then, this
book required a personal choice of topics and an incomplete one at that.
And yet we believe we have assembled a comprehensive collection of
writings covering fundamental topics of cerebellar neuroscience from the
molecular to the systems level that we hope would please both the specialist
and the generalist in accordance with what we consider to be the intel-
lectual revolution that commenced a century ago with Ramon y Cajal at
the helm.
Contents

Preface v

Contributors xi

Part 1. Organization, Development, and Repair


of the Cerebellar Circuits 1
1. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity: Its Role in Organizing
the Topography of the Cerebellar Cortex Connections 5
Marion Wassef, Pierre Angaut, Leonor Arsenio-Nunes,
Franck Bourrat, and Constantino Sotelo
2. Zebrins: Molecular Markers of Compartmentation
in the Cerebellum 22
Richard Hawkes, Gino Brochu, Louise Dore, Claude Gravel,
and Nicole Leclerc
3. Cerebellar Granule Cells and the Neurobiology
of Excitatory Amino Acids 56
Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. Jprgensen
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins
in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 72
Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck
5. Cerebellar Grafting as a Tool to Analyze New Aspects
of Cerebellar Development and Plasticity 84
Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart
6. Light and Electron Microscopic Immunocytochemistry
of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids
in the Cerebellum with Some Observations
on the Distribution of Glutamine 116
Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake
7. The Expanding Role of the Basilar Pontine Nuclei as a Source
of Cerebellar Afferents 135
G.A. Mihailoff, R.J. Kosinski, S.A. Azizi, H S. Lee,
and E.G. Border

IX
x Contents

Part 2. Electrophysiology of Purkinje and Inferior


Olivary Neurons 165
8. The Electrophysiology of the Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Revisited 167
Rodolfo R. LUnas and Mutsuyuki Sugimori
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels in Purkinje Cells
from Organotypic Cerebellar Cultures 182
Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty
10. Electroneuronal Hybridization: A Novel Approach to
Investigate Rhythmogenesis in the Inferior Olivary Nucleus 201
Yosef Yarom

Part 3. Electrophysiology of Movement 213


11. Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements
in the Pigmented Rat 215
Piergiorgio Strata, Leonardo Chelazzi. Filippo Tempia.
Ferdinando Rossi, and Mirella Ghirardi
12. A Possible Connection Between the Mossy
and Climbing Fiber Systems at Precerebellar Level 226
Francisco J. Rubia
13. Eye Movements and the Zonal Structure of the Rabbit Flocculus 255
John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan
14. The Dynamic Selection Hypothesis: A Proposed Function
for Cerebellar Sagittal Zones 267
James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly
15. Cerebellar Output: Multiple Maps and Modes of Control
in Movement Coordination 283
W. Thomas Thach, S.A. Kane, J.w. Mink, and H.P. Goodkin
16. The Role of the Cerebellum in Voluntary and Reflexive
Movements: History and Current Status 301
John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

Index 335
Contributors

Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart. Ph.D. INSERM U-106, Hopital de la


Salpetriere, 75013 Paris, France.
Pierre Angaut. Ph.D. INSERM U-106, Hopital de la Salpetriere, 75013
Paris, France.
Leonor Arsenio-Nunes, M.D. INSERM U-106 Hopital de la Salpetriere,
75013 Paris, France.
S.A. Azizi, Ph.D. Department of Cell Biology, University of Texas
Southwestern Medical School, Dallas, TX 75235, USA.
Robert Balazs. M.D., Ph.D. Netherlands Institute for Brain Research,
1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
James R. Bloedel. M.D .• Ph.D. Division of Neurobiology, Barrow
Neurological Institute, Phoenix, AZ 85013, USA.
B.G. Border, Ph.D. Department of Neurology, University of Texas
Southwestern Medical School, Dallas, TX 75235, USA.
Franck Bourrat. Ph.D. INSERM U-106, Hopital de la Salpetriere, 75013
Paris, France.
Gino Brochu, M.Sc. Laboratory of Neurobiology, Faculty of Medicine,
Laval University, Ste-Foy, Quebec GAl 1Z4, Canada.
Leonardo Chelazzi. Ph.D. Istituto de Fisiologia Umana, University of
Verona, Strada Le Grazie, 37134 Verona, Italy.
Louise Dore. M.D.. Ph.D. Laboratory of Neurobiology, Faculty of
Medicine, Laval University, Ste-Foy, Quebec GAl 1Z4, Canada.
Beat H. Giihwiler. Ph.D. Brain Research Institute, CH-8029, Zurich,
Switzerland.
Mirella Ghirardi. Ph.D. Dipartimento di Anatomia e Fisiologia Umana,
Universita degli Studi di Torino, C.so Raffaello 30, 10125 Torino, Italy.
H.P. Goodkin. M.D .• Ph.D. Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology,
Washington University Medical School, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA.

xi
Xll Contributors

Claude Gravel, Ph.D. Institut de Recherches Cliniques de Montreal,


Montreal, Quebec, P.Q., Canada.
Nicola Hack, Ph.D. Netherlands Institute for Brain Research, 1105 AZ
Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
John A. Harvey, Ph.D. Division of Behavioral Neurobiology, Depart-
ment of Pharmacology, Medical College of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia,
PA 19129, USA.
Richard Hawkes, Ph.D. Department of Anatomy and Neuroscience
Research Group, Faculty of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary,
Alberta T2N 4N1, Canada.
Ole S. j¢rgensen, Ph.D. Psychochemistry Institute, Rigshospitalet, DK-
2100 Copenhagen (j), Denmark.
S.A. Kane, Ph.D. Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Washington
University Medical School, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA.
Thomas M. Kelly, Ph.D. Department of Physiology, University of
Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721, USA.
R.J. Kosinski, Ph.D. Department of Cell Biology, University of Texas
Southwestern Medical School, Dallas TX 75235, USA.
Jon H. Laake, M.D. Department of Anatomy, Institute of Basic Medical
Sciences, University of Oslo, N-0317 Oslo 3, Norway.
Nicole Leclerc, Ph.D. Center for Neurologic Disease, Brigham and
Women's Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA.
H.S. Lee, Ph.D. Department of Cell Biology, University of Texas South-
western Medical School, Dallas, TX 75235, USA.
Isabel Llano, Ph.D. Laboratoire de Neurobiologie, Ecole Normale
Superieure, 46 Rue d'Ulm, Paris 75005, France.
Rodolfo R. Llinas, Ph.D. Department of Physiology/Biophysics, New
York University Medical Center, New York, NY 10016, USA.
Alain Marty, Ph.D. Laboratoire de Neurobiologie, Ecole Normale
Superieure, 46, Rue d'Ulm, Paris 75005, France.
Andrew Matus, Ph.D. Friedrich Miescher-Institut, 4002 Basel, Switzerland.
G.A. Mihailoff, Ph.D. Department of Anatomy, University of Mississippi
Medical Center, Jackson, MS 39216-4505, USA.
J. W Mink, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology,
Washington University Medical School, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA.
Ole P. Ottersen, M.D. Department of Anatomy, Institute of Basic
Medic.al Sciences, University of Oslo, N-0317 Oslo 3, Norway.
Ferdinando Rossi, Ph.D. Dipartimento di Anatomia e Fisiologia Umana,
Universita degli Studi di Torino, C.so Raffaello 30, 10125, Torino, Italy.
Francisco J. Rubia, Ph.D. Departamento de Fisiologia, Facultad de
Medicina, Universidad Complutense, 28040 Madrid, Spain.
Contributors xiii

John 1. Simpson, Ph.D. Department of Physiology/Biophysics, New


York University Medical Center, New York, NY 10016, USA.
Constantino Sotelo, M.D., Ph.D. INSERM U-I06, Hopital de la Salpetriere,
75013 Paris, France.
Piergiorgio Strata, Prof. Dipartimento di Anatomia e Fisiologia Umana,
Universita degli Studi di Torino, C.so Raffaello 30, 10125, Torino, Italy.
Mutsuyuki Sugimori, Ph.D. Department of Physiology/Biophysics, New
York University Medical Center, New York, NY 10016, USA.
Joep Tan, Ph.D. Department of Anatomy, Faculty of Medicine, Erasmus
University, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
Filippo Tempia, Ph.D. Dipartimento di Anatomia e Fisiologia Umana,
Universita degli Studi di Torino, C.so Raffaello 30, 10125 Torino, Italy.
W. Thomas Thach, M.D. Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology,
Washington University Medical School, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA.
Richard P. Tucker, Ph.D. Friedrich Miescher-Institut, 4002 Basel,
Switzerland.
Johannes Van der Steen, Ph.D. Department of Physiology, Faculty of
Medicine, Erasmus University, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
Christopher Viereck, Ph.D. Friedrich Miescher-Institut, 4002 Basel,
Switzerland.
Marion Wassef, Ph.D. INSERM U-106, Hopital de la Salpetriere, 75013
Paris, France.
John P. Welsh, Ph.D. Department of Physiology/Biophysics, New York
University Medical Center, New York, NY 10016, USA.
Yosef Yarom, Ph.D. Institute of Life Sciences, Department of Neuro-
biology, Hebrew University, Jerusalem 91904, Israel.
Part 1
Organization, Development, and Repair
of the Cerebellar Circuits

Since the pioneerings studies of Cajal, celebrated in this book, the organ-
ization of cerebellar circuitries has attracted many scholars who among
others, have tried to unravel most of the afferent and efferent connections
of this central region. However, despite all these efforts, as Alf Brodal
wrote in his last edition (1981) of his textbook of Neuroanatomy, "We
do not yet properly understand its function and its cooperation with other
parts of the brain." In fact, although the use of axonal tracing methods
during the last fifteen years or so has greatly improved the selectivity of
the anatomical analysis, the polysynaptic nature of most pathways involved
has hampered progress in fully understanding them.
Studies performed in recent years, owing to the convergence of anatom-
ical and physiological approaches, have greatly improved our present
knowledge on the functional organization of afferent and efferent cere-
bellar connections. The more we know of these pathways, the more they
appear to obey highly consistent patterns of organization.
One of the selected topics in this section concerns the organization of
the cerebellar projections originating from potine neurons, the principal
precerebellar region involved in transferring information to the cerebellum
about the current state of the cerebral cortex, particularly the motor cortex
of prime importance in mammals. The way this system is organized, at
least according to our present knowledge, raises the problem of the origin
of the excitatory input to the dentate nucleus. In fact, many anatomists
have failed to encounter collateral axons to the cerebellar nuclei in the
pontocerebellar projection originating from the basilar pontine nuclei
(BNP), suggesting that this system is not organized as the olivocerebellar
one which consistently sends collaterals to the deep nuclei. However, ponto-
cerebellar neurons are located not only at the BNP but also in the nucleus
reticularis tegmenti pontis (NRT), which undoubtedly project to the deep
nuclei, particularly to the dentate nucleus. If all these results are corrobo-
rated, they suggest that only about 20% of the whole pontocerebellar
system will provide excitatory inputs to the dentate nucleus. The cortico-
pontine projection, an input that emerges from almost every portion of
the cerebral mantle, is topographically organized. Only some restricted
regions, mostly the motor and somatosensory cortices, project to the
NRT. Thus, the very elaborated input from the cerebral cortex to the
2 The Cerebellar Circuits

cerebellum would regulate a nucleus without major excitatory drive. A


set of studies in the cortico-ponto-cerebello-thalamo-cortical loop by
Sasaki et al. reveals a very powerful return activation from the cerebellum
back to the cortex following cortical excitation. Thus, despite the anatom-
ical controversy, it seems appropriate to hypothesize that NRT is just
part of the pontine nuclear complex and that this subnucleus, as do the
others, gives collateral axons-organized with precise topography-to
the deep nuclei. It is clear that more anterograde tracing experiments are
needed to confirm or to invalidate this hypothesis.
Immunocytochemical methods to study potative neurotransmitters in
central synapses have been extremely useful in the search of possible candi-
dates for cerebellar synapses, particularly among amino acids with known
excitatory or inhibitory action. This volume reviews results obtained with
quantitative postembedding immunogold method to assess the ultrastruc-
turallocation of all these amino acids and to establish the pattern of their
distribution within the cortical cerebellar circuit. It is also emphasized
that this immunocytochemical approach can be an essential tool to analyze
synaptic release and glial uptake of amino acids.
The recent advances in screening molecular identities in neurons,
through the attainment of extensive monoclonal antibody libraries, have
provided unequivocal proof of the molecular heterogeneity of Purkinje
cells. A number of monoclonal antibodies, most of them oriented against
still not-completely characterized antigens, are expressed according to a
similar parasagittai band pattern, with subsets of Purkinje cells either
possessing or lacking the respective antigens. The cerebellar cortex is,
therefore, subdivided into precise longitudinal zones or compartments
based on their heterogeneous Purkinje cell population. Results reported
in this part point to the fact that the boundaries of the intrinsic cortical
compartments and those of the projectional maps, particularly the
olivocerebellar system, are congruent.
The correlation of these two maps in adult cerebellum indicates a
correspondence of developmental mechanisms. The organization of two
subpopulations of Purkinje cells into mutually exclusive compartments
could either result from postnatal synaptic connections with differential
afferent inputs or from the neuronal phenotype maturing independently
of afferent influences. The problem of the development of the Purkinje
cell heterogeneity is addressed, and a series of arguments are discussed in
favor of the essential role of these neurons in organizing projectional
maps during ontogenesis of the cerebellum. The matching of region-specific
chemical labels between incoming afferent fibers and heterogeneous sets
of Purkinje cells is the most appealing mechanism for the formation of
cerebellar maps.
This first part is also comprised of two other developmental studies on
(1) the role of cytoskeleton (especially microtubules) in the determinism of
neuronal polarity and the acquisition of neuronal shape and (2) the trophic
effect of excitatory amino acids on the survival of granule cells. These
two studies emphasize the point that the cerebellum is one of the most
idoneal paradigms for the analysis of the mechanisms governing the
sequential phases leading to the formation of central neuronal net-
works.
The Cerebellar Circuits 3

The last paper of this part addresses the problem of cerebellar develop-
ment and plasticity through the study of cerebellar repair by neural grafting
in murine heredo-degenerative ataxia. This experimental approach offers
the exceptional possibility of analyzing in vivo cellular interactions
between embryonic and adult neurons, allowing the synaptic integration
of the grafted neurons, leading to the subsequent restoration of the im-
paired cortico cerebellar circuit. These experiments also raise the possibil-
ity that embryonic Purkinje cells can induce in adult neural cells a new
type of plasticity; that of recreating a permissive microenvironment for
the integration of the grafted neurons.
1
Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity:
Its Role in Organizing the Topography
of the Cerebellar Cortex Connections
Marion Wassef, Pierre Angaut, Leonor Arsenio-Nunes,
Frank Bourrat, and Constantino Sotelo

The cerebellar cortex comprises a few cell types The deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN), the PCs,
and two main afferent systems arranged in a ste- and the Golgi cells are generated by the ventri-
reotyped synaptic pattern that is repeated mono- cular neuroepithelium in successive waves ex-
tonously throughout. The regularity of the tending over 2 to 3 days for the projection neurons
laminated structure of the cerebellar cortex per- and somewhat longer for the Golgi cells. The
mits the identification of the main cell types, even DCN are produced first, peaking at Ell in the
on conventionally stained sections, based on their mouse and E13 in the rat, followed by PC, peak-
position and size. Contrasting with its regular ing at E12 in the mouse and E14 in the rat. Golgi
architecture, the cerebellar cortex is subdivided cells are produced between E12 and E15 in the
into a mosaic of small functional zones defined mouse and between E18 and P2 in the rat (Altman
by the precise pattern of their afferent and efferent and Bayer, 1978; Miale and Sidman, 1961).
connections. In this chapter, we report and dis- Beginning from E16 in the rat, the external
cuss work from our laboratory aimed at under- granular layer arises from the "germinal tri-
standing how such a precise organization of the gone," which is located at the periphery of the
cerebellar projection maps is achieved during cerebellar plate, close to the attachment of the
development. Different lines of evidence are pre- tela choroidea. The EGL spreads progressively
sented in favor of the hypothesis that, during over the surface of the cerebellar plate, which
development, the cerebellar cortex develops an is eventually completely covered by E20 (Altman
intrinsic topographic map through its subdivi- and Bayer, 1978; see also Feirabend, 1983; and
sion into small sets of biochemically different Hanaway, 1967). The cerebellum is often de-
Purkinje cells (PC). We propose that this PC scribed as a late maturing structure because all
heterogeneity is subsequently recognized by the interneurons of the cerebellar cortex, with the
afferent fibers and thus underlies the topography exception of Golgi cells, are produced postna-
of the cerebellar cortex connections. Before pre- tally in small rodents. The onset of synaptoge-
senting our results, a brief description of the nesis, as observed by physiological (Crepel, 1971;
early stages of the embryonic development of Puro and Woodward, 1977a, b; Shimono et al.,
the cerebellum in rats and mice is worthwhile. 1976) and morphological (Altman, 1972a, b, c;
A unique feature of cerebellar development is Larramendi, 1969) criteria, is also mainly
that its neurons are generated by two neuro- postnatal; it may be dated from P2-P3 in the
epithelia. Early in development, neurons com- rat, although occasional synapses have been
prising the projection neurons of the cerebellum observed in late embryos (West and del Cerro,
arise from the ventricular neuroepithelium. 1976). To determine whether or not synapto-
Later on, a secondary neuroepithelium, the exter- genesis is related to the mechanisms underlying
nal granular layer (EGL), produces the bulk of the formation of projection maps, we have studied
cerebellar neurons that are local-circuit neurons. the development of the olivocerebellar and

5
6 Marion Wassef et al.

spinocerebellar projections in rat pups, using birth (Arsenio-Nunes and Sotelo, 1985), indicat-
anterograde and retrograde axonal tracing ing that both olivary and spinal afferent fibers
methods. We observed that, despite the imma- reach the cerebellum during fetal development.
turity of the cerebellum around birth, the afferent Contrasting with the ubiquitous distribution of
fibers are already well organized and that their olivary fibers in the white matter, at birth spinal
broad distribution is indistinguishable from that afferents are restricted to the territories that they
of adults, considering that the available methods normally innervate in adult rats: the vermis
of investigation are rather crude. Therefore, and intermediate cortex of the anterior lobe
except perhaps in its finest details, the topogra- (Fig. l.1A) and the copula pyramidis. Spinal
phic organization of the cerebellar cortex afferents fibers begin to penetrate the cortical gray matter
seems to be independent from synapse formation.

Development of the
Olivo cerebellar Projection
The olivocerebellar fibers are already present,
arrested in the prospective white matter of the
cerebellum at birth (Sotelo et aI., 1984); a few
fibers begin at that time to invade the overlying
gray matter. Although the bulk of the olivary
fibers have not yet contacted their target PCs,
they are already distributed in their proper
terminal territories. As in adult rats, restricted
horseradish peroxidase (HRP) injections in the
cerebellar cortex result in the retrograde labeling
of neurons in restricted zones of the contralateral
inferior olive: the caudal half of the medial
accessory olive (MAO) projects to the vermal
cortex, the dorsal accessory olive (DAO) and
part of the rostral MAO to the intermediate
cortex, the principal olive to the hemispheric
cortex. At P5 a more precise analysis of the
topography of the olivocerebellar projection is
possible. The fibers have moved from the
prospective white matter toward the PC layer.
Although the peak of multiple innervation of
PCs by climbing fibers is reached at P5 (Crepel
et aI., 1976, 1981; Mariani and Changeux, 1981),
no later changes in the organizational pattern Figure 1.1. Organization of the spinocerebellar pro-
of the olivocerebellar projection have been jection in an adult normal rat (A) and in the agranular
observed. In P5 rats this pattern is identical to cerebella of an x-irridiated rat (B) and a staggerer
that observed at P15 after the one-to-one mutant mouse (C) injected at the same level of the
relationship of climbing fibers to PCs has been spinal cord (lower thoracic, upper lumbar) with
WGA-HRP. The sections are viewed under polarized
attained.
light. In the anterior lobe of the vermis of the control
rat the spinal fibers display a characteristic columnar
Development of the organization (A). This columnar distribution is pre-
Spinocerebellar Projection served in the irradiated rat (B) but severely altered
in the cerebellum of the staggerer mutant mouse (C)
Like olivary fibers, the spinal afferents are where the fibers are evenly distributed throughout the
present in the white matter of the cerebellum at vermal cortex. Bar = 200 flm in A and C, 100 flm in B.
1. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 7

by P3 in the anterior lobe. The mediolateral the spinocerebellar projection is disorganized, it


variations in the density of the spinal fibers can be suggested that they are involved in the
result in a "protocolumnar organization" topographical organization of this projection.
(Arsenio-Nunes and Sotelo, 1985). At P5, the The preceding observations indicate that the
fibers, as in adult rats, are organized in columns olivocerebellar projection is already well organ-
but some labeled fibers still remain dispersed ized at birth. On the other hand, although the
between the columns. From P7 on, the adult spinocerebellar fibers have not yet adopted their
topography of the spinocerebellar projection is columnar pattern of projection at birth, it seems
achieved. Although some granule cells are present that the cues necessary for their organization are
in the internal granular layer before P5, their produced before birth. At least it is difficult to
number is very low. The observations about the interfere postnatally with the establishment of
early organization of the spinocerebellar projec- the characteristic columnar pattern of this pro-
tion support two kinds of interpretations: either jection even by deleting the target neurons. This
the small number of early produced granule cells prompted us to examine earlier events, seeking
is sufficient to organize the spinocerebellar for some early organizing mechanism that could
projection or this organization does not depend explain our postnatal observations about the
on granule cells. This question has been addres- development of the olivocerebellar and spino-
sed by studying the organization of the spino- cerebellar projections. Of particular interest was
cerebellar projection in different models of the development of the projection neurons of the
agranular cerebella. cerebellum (i.e., the deep cerebellar neurons and
the PCs). We report below some observations
about the embryonic and early postnatal devel-
Pattern of the Spinocerebellar opment of PCs. We discuss how the early gene-
Projection in Agranular Cerebella ration of subsets of biochemically different PCs
The organization of the spinocerebellar projec- and their subsequent pattern of migration and
tion was analyzed by anterograde axonal wheat interdigitation in the cortex built up an intrinsic
germ agglutinin HRP (WGA-HRP) tracing in topography of the cerebellar cortex. This marking
three types of adult agranular cerebellar cortices of the cortex through PC heterogeneity could
induced either experimentally by postnatal x-ray give cues to afferent fibers and allow them to reach
irradiation or occurring spontaneously in weaver their proper site of termination.
and staggerer mutant mice (Arsenio-Nunes et aI.,
1988). In x-irradiated rats (Fig. l.lB), as in Transient PC Heterogeneity
weaver mice, the granule cells are directly affected
and die early in development (Sotelo and
During Perinatal Development
Changeux, 1974b), before migration and dif- Several proteins and glycoproteins have been
ferentiation. Nevertheless, the spinocerebellar described that may be considered as PC markers
projection in the anterior vermis displays its in the adult rat and mouse cerebellum, in the sense
normal columnar pattern. Conversely, in stag- that antibodies against these antigens stain all
gerer mutants, the spinocerebellar organization the PCs and that, in the cerebellum, PCs are the
is severely modified (Fig. 1.1 C), although granule only immunoreactive cell type. By extension, the
cell death occurs later, after migration and term "PC marker" in the following description
differentiation, probably because of a PC de- refers indifferently to the antigens or their anti-
fect that prevents synaptogenesis and the subse- bodies. Among others, antibodies against cyclic
quent synaptic stabilization of granule cells (Sotelo guanosine monophosphate (GMP)-dependent
and Changeux, 1974a). These data indicate that protein kinase (cGK); (De Camilli et aI., 1984;
specific synaptogenesis and even the presence of Lohmann et aI., 1981), vitamin D-dependent
granule cells are not essential for the establish- calcium-binding protein (calbindin or CaBP)
ment of the normal spinocerebellar topography. (Jande et aI., 1981; Legrand et aI., 1983), Purkinje
On the other hand, since PCs are the primary cell-specific glycoprotein (PSG); (Langley et aI.,
target ofthe mutation in staggerer mice, in which 1982; Reeber et aI., 1981), and PEP 19 (Ziai et aI.,
8 Marion Wassef et al.

1986) have been used to label specifically adult


PCs in the cerebellum. Apparently, apart from
being highly concentrated in PCs, these PC
markers have nothing in common. The onset of
PC immunoreactivity varies for each marker
(Wassef et aI., 1984, 1985, unpublished observa-
tions). The first immunoreactive PCs are observed
at E16 with anti-CaBP, E17 with anti-cGK, and
around E20 with anti-PSG and anti-PEP 19. In
addition to being the earliest PC marker, anti-
CaBP is also the one that gives the most detailed
picture of embryonic PCs. At E16, CaBP immu- cGK
noreactive PCs are still located against the
receding ventricular neuroepithelium. Most of
them are radially oriented and begin to extend
B f •

their axons dorsally. The CaBP immunoreactive

CaBP

Figure 1.3. Successive coronal sections through the


cerebellum of an E20 rat embryo stained for cGK (A)
B and CaBP (B). The fact that CaBP+, cGK- (tri-
angles) and CaBP-, cGK+ (stars) clusters of PC
coexist indicates that the immunoegativity of PCs for
a given antibody does not simply reflect a global delay
in their stage of development. Bar = 400 11m.

axons gather in the "intermediate fibrous layer"


(Altman and Bayer, 1978). Beginning from day
17, PCs migrate dorsally to the cortical region
and it becomes apparent that all the PCs are not
CaBP immunoreactive (Fig. 1.2). For each of the
PC markers tested (CaBP, cGK, PSG, and PEP
19), PCs are arranged in alternating positive and
negative clusters (Figs. 1.2, 1.3, 1.4) that are sym-
metrically arranged in relation to the midline
Figure 1.2. Successive coronal sections from caudal
(Fig. 1.4). With each antibody, the pattern is re-
(top) to rostral (bottom) through the cerebellum of producible at a given age and changes progres-
an E18 rat embryo stained for CaBP showing the sively with the spreading and migration of PCs as
succession of CaBP immunopositive (white stars) development proceeds. Interestingly, the pattern
and immunonegative (black stars) clusters of PCs. of positive and negative clusters is different with
Bar = 500 11m. each antibody (Fig. 1.3), although some bound-
I. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 9

PCs ofthe negative clusters, however, do contain


lower amounts of the antigens, which increase
progressively as development proceeds until
around P5, when adjacent clusters become
indistinguishable.
The basic PC compartment (Wassef et a!.,
1985) is not the immunopositive or immuno-
negative cluster of PCs by itself but the inter-
section of such clusters where, by definition, all
the PCs are identical. The basic PC compartment
is characterized by a combination of proteins or
glycoproteins (markers) expressed at high or low
levels (A +, B -, C -, D +, ...); this combination
constitutes the "label" of the PC compartment.
Because the position in the cerebellar cortex of
each basic PC compartment is characteristic and
reproducible at a given age, the PC heterogeneity
makes up a reproducible map of the cerebellar
cortex. Even with a small number of markers,
the combinatory mechanism described above
can give a reasonably high number of different
labels. Because the basic PC compartments are
not directly amenable to analysis, it is difficult

c PE P 19
to appreciate their sizes and shapes.
We have hypothesized that PC heterogeneity
serves as a labeling of the cortical structure and
Figure 1.4. Coronal sections at similar levels in the
is involved in the establishment of the cerebellar
rostral cerebellum of 1-day-old rat pups labeled for
cGK (A), CaBP (B), and PEP 19 (C). After birth, the projection maps. Obviously, as the antibodies
PCs are still organized in clusters characterized by a used to study PC development label all adult
high or low level of expression of each of these PCs, the PC heterogeneity that we observe during
markers. Note that the pattern of immunoreactivity development is transient. Beginning from P5, the
is different for each antibody. Bar = 1 mm. differences in PC immunoreactivity with these
markers have faded. Nevertheless, the adult cere-
bellar cortex is still subdivided into sagittal strips
and patches of biochemically different PCs. This
aries are shared by several markers. This obser- heterogeneity of adult PCs can be evidenced
vation indicates that the PCs in the negative either by a differential sensitivity to mutations
clusters are not simply delayed in their overall affecting the postnatal survival of PCs or by
stage of maturation. In addition, it shows that differences in reactivity using various histochemi-
subsets of PCs adopt different timetables for the calor immunocytochemical techniques.
expression of some of their characteristic proteins,
thus following different developmental pathways.
The organization of the cerebellar cortex in
Cell Heterogeneity in the
alternating immunopositive and negative clus- Adult Cerebellum
ters of PCs is still observed after birth (Fig. 1.4), Cerebellar Mutations Disclose
ruling out the possible existence of another cell
Adult PC Heterogeneity in the Mouse
population mingled with PCs in embryos. It
must be stressed that the immunopositive and Several mutations have been described in the
immunonegative PC clusters are characterized mouse that affect the postnatal survival of PCs.
by marked differences in staining intensities. The We have studied (Wassef et a!., 1987) three such
10 Marion Wassef et al.

mutations: nervous (NR), Purkinje cell degener-


ation (PCD), and tambaleante (TBL). In all three
mutations pathological changes are not observed
before the end of the second postnatal week, at
a time when PCs may be considered as almost
mature.
In NR (Landis, 1973; Sidman and Green,
1970), the first pathological change, an anomaly
in the size, shape, and location of PC mitochondria,
is observed throughout the cerebellar cortex at
P15. Apparently all the PCs are affected. However,
whereas most PCs undergo degenerative changes
and die before the end of the second month, a
small population (about 10%) recover and survive
throughout the whole life of the animal, although
some sporadic PC death may be observed
(Sotelo and Triller, 1979).
In PCD (Mullen et aI., 1976), PC loss is almost
complete: it begins during the third week of life
and then proceeds rapidly. By 4 months, fewer
than 120 PCs remain in the cerebellum (Wassef
et aI., 1986).
TBL is a recessive mutation, still not fully
characterized, which appeared spontaneously in
the CNRS breeding center of Orleans-La Source;
TBL seems to affect primarily PC survival
(Sotelo and Guenet, unpublished observations).
Figure 1.5. Coronal section through the vermal re-
The number of PCs is not reduced before about
gion of the cerebellum of a 4-month-old nervous
2 months, but between 2 and 3 months there is mutant mouse stained for CaBP by immunofluore-
a massive, but subtotal, degeneration of PCs. scence. Surviving PCs are grouped into longitudinal
Each of these mutations reveals a sub po pula- bands that are symmetrically arranged in relation to
tion of more resistant PCs. The pattern of these the midline. Bar = 400 flm.
"resistant" PCs was studied using CaBP immuno-
cytochemistry. In NR (Figs. 1.5, 1.6) and I-month-
old PCD (Fig. 1.6), surviving PCs are arranged
in clusters separated by areas of unstained
molecular layer. This topographic arrangement results as showing that each mutation unmasks
of PC survival results in a checkerboard pattern the underlying PC heterogeneity in a way that
of CaBP immunostaining in the cerebellar cortex depends both on the mutation and on the
of these mutants. In TBL, patches differing in physiological characteristics of PCs, the most
the density of surviving PCs are also observed. important implication probably being that the
At a given age, the pattern of surviving PCs is physiological heterogeneity of PCs is related to
reproducible in individuals bearing the same their position in the cerebellar cortex (topogra-
mutation. In all three mutations, the clusters of phically determined). The populations of "resis-
surviving PCs are oriented sagittally and they tant" PCs are partially overlapping, as was the
are symmetrically arranged in relation to the case for the immunoreactive clusters of PCs ob-
midline. Interestingly, the patterns of surviving served during development, an observation that
PCs differ for each mutation, indicating that we supports the same interpretation: the clusters of
are not simply dealing with a subpopulation of resistant PCs that are disclosed by the mutations
more robust PCs. We have interpreted these are not the basic PC compartments.
1. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 11

Level of PC Heterogeneity:
Single Cell or Cell Population?

We propose that the cerebellar cortex is built up


through the assembly of small heterogeneous
subsets of PCs. Each subset has a single signature,
its "label," constituted by a characteristic com-
bination of properties (the high or low expression
of a given set of markers) that individually are
shared with other PC subsets. The labeling of
PCs through a combination of different markers
offers interesting possibilities: a) a few different
markers may give rise, by combination, to a
variety of labels, or b) a hierarchy of the labels
is possible, allowing the progressive subdivision
of broad territories into smaller specialized zones.
The size of the smallest subdivisions, that we
termed "basic PC compartments," is, however,
unknown: do they comprise a single PC or a
subset of PCs?
The idea that each PC in the monolayer is
unique is supported by considerations about the
specificity of its developmental history and con-
nections. Apparently, it also has a strong anthro-
pomorphic background. The assumption that
individual target cells may be identified by the
afferent growth cones through a distinct bio-
chemical label is commonly referred to as Sperry's Figure 1.6. Coronal sections through the cerebellum
hypothesis (Sperry, 1944, 1963). Sperry specu- of a 3-month-old nervous (A: caudal; B rostral) and
lated that the conservation of the topographic I-month-old PC degeneration (C) mutant mice stain-
order from the retina onto the tectal surface ed for CaBP. Surviving PCs are arranged in sagittal
bands whose pattern is reproducible in individuals of
during the development or regeneration of optic
the same age bearing the same mutation. Compare
fibers in amphibians and fishes could be explained the pattern in the vermis in B with Fig. 1.5 taken ap-
by the existence of "an orderly cytochemical proximately at the same level from another nervous
mapping in terms of two or more gradients of mouse. The population of resistant PCs, however,
embryonic differentiation with their axes roughly is different in each mutation (compare A and C).
perpendicular. These separate gradients would Bar= Imm.
stamp each cell with its appropriate latitude and
longitude expressed in a kind of chemical code"
(Sperry, 1963). The graded property of the in marking out the cerebellar cortex and sub-
chemical code in Sperry's hypothesis is impor- dividing it into physiologically relevant zones.
tant to bring about a continuous ordering of the The labeling would allow afferent fibers to invade
projection. The usefulness of a discontinuous set their proper zone of termination. In this context,
of labels generated at the single cell level by a the basic compartments may be supposed to be
combination of unrelated markers is question- of variable sizes throughout the cortex but gen-
able: it seems difficult to imagine a matching erally not reduced to a single Pc.
mechanism for such a labeling. The role of the In theory, the organization of the cortex in
PC heterogeneity that we observe during develop- patches of biochemically different PCs could, in
ment and in mutants is, in our view, different. addition, be involved in the local ordering of
We propose that this heterogeneity is involved afferents. The common boundary between two
12 Marion Wasser et al.

patches of different PCs may be used in the antibodies directed against unidentified epitopes
generation of gradients of positional informa- (Bl: Ingram et ai., 1985; mabQl13 or zebrin 1:
tion, as has been proposed for the organization Hawkes et ai., 1985; Hawkes and Leclerc, 1987)
of subfields during development (Meinhardt, react with molecules that are clearly localized to
1982). As discussed by Meinhardt (1982, 1983), a subset of PCs. The populations of immuno-
we need only to assume that two patches of PCs reactive PCs observed with each antibody are
cooperate for the production of a label, present characteristically organized in sagittal strips that
at their surface or in the extracellular matrix. are symmetrically arranged in relation to the
This required cooperation makes the synthesis midline and whose pattern is reproducible in a
of the label only possible at the boundary. By given species.
diffusion and decay, a gradient of positional
information is formed, with the highest concen- Relationship Between Heterogeneity
tration of label at the limit between two PC and the Pattern of Connections
patches. If the afferent fibers are supposed to
have some mechanism allowing them to maintain of the Cerebellar Cortex
the neighboring relationship of their neurons of In order to demonstrate that the PC heterogeneity
origin, this further refinement of the cortical is directly involved in the segregation of afferent
labeling could allow the establishment of a pre- fibers and thus underlies the topography of the
cise topographic order of the cerebellar cortex cerebellar projection maps, much new infor-
afferent fibers even before the beginning of synap- mation about early cerebellar development is
togenesis (Von der Malsburg and Wills haw, needed. For example, the timing of arrival of
1977). early afferents into the cerebellar plate and their
position in relation to the PC clusters are still
Histochemical and Immunohistochemical unknown. Even the comparison between the
Evidence of PC Heterogeneity in the pattern of afferents and the pattern of the PC
immunoreactive clusters observed transiently
Adult Cerebellar Cortex
after birth is technically uneasy. It seemed
Several histochemical and immunohistochemical worthwhile to investigate first the relationship
techniques provide histological evidence of the between the adult pattern of PC heterogeneity,
subdivision of the cerebellar cortex, and parti- as revealed by the zebrin 1 innunoreactivity, and
cularly its molecular layer, into sagittal bands the pattern of projection of one of the main
differing in their reactivity. Thus, variations cerebellar afferent systems. The climbing fiber
in the staining intensity for the enzymes 5'- projection seemed the most appropriate for this
nucleotidase in the mouse cerebellum (Marani study since a) the termination of climbing fibers
and Voogd, 1973; Scott, 1964) and acetylcholin- on the dendritic tree of PCs makes the compari-
esterase in the cat and monkey cerebellum (Brown son between the pattern of projection and the
and Graybiel, 1983; Hess and Voogd, 1986; pattern of PC immunoreactivity easy, b) the
Marani and Voogd, 1977) have been described". location of the terminal axonic field of the
Binding experiments using radioactive ligands climbing fiber in the molecular layer allows its
for muscarinic acetylcholine receptors (Neustadt unambiguous identification even if the tracer
et ai., 1988) also disclose a sagittal banding pat- injection is not restricted to the inferior olive
tern of the molecular layer. In these experiments and involves neighboring structures projecting
the cellular localization of the enzymes or the in the granular layer as mossy fibers, and c) the
receptors is not always clear. olivocerebellar projection could share the same
By contrast, several antibodies against peptides principles of organization, in parasagittal strips,
or enzymes (antimotilin: Chan-Palay et ai., 1981; as the zebrin 1 PC bands.
Nilaver et ai., 1982; see also Lange et ai., 1986; Hawkes and collaborators (Gravel et ai.,
anti-glutamate decarboxylase: Chan-Palay et ai., 1987) have compared the pattern of the climbing
1981; anti-cystein sulfinic acid decarboxylase: fiber projection with that of PC immunoreacti-
Chan-Palay et ai., 1982) as well as monoclonal vity using the monoclonal antibody Ql13, which
1. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 13

labels a subset of PCs (zebrin 1 +) in the cerebel- Random Tracer Injections in the
lum of adult rats. Two days after an injection of Inferior 0 live Disclose Intrinsic
WGA-HRP in the inferior olive of adult rats, Subdivisions of the
alternate cerebellar sections were treated either Olivocerebellar Projection
with the tetramethyl benzidine (TMB) method
to reveal the anterogradely labeled climbing Some properties of the olivo cerebellar projec-
fibers, or for zebrin 1 immunocytochemistry. tion are well established: a) the projection covers
These authors found that "the mabQ113 + / the whole cerebellar cortex (Campbell and
mabQ113 - boundaries and the bands of climb- Armstrong, 1983a, b) climbing fibers from a cir-
ing fibers seen by using anterograde tracer typi- cumscribed region of the inferior olive terminate
cally coincide and that it is likely that the climb- in the cerebellar cortex in narrow sagittal zones
ing fiber projection to the cerebellar cortex and that extend through several lobules (Armstrong
the distribution of the two PC phenotypes share et aI., 1974; Groenewegen and Voogd, 1977;
a common compartmental organization." These Groenewegen et al., 1979). In general, a small
results were of particular interest; however, several random injection in the inferior olive results in
considerations weaken the conclusions of Gravel the labeling of several sagittal strips of climbing
et al. (1987). First, the comparison between the fibers (Figs. 1.7 through 1.11), c) in some parts
two patterns relies on the observation of succes- of the inferior olive, in particular the rostral
sive sections, one of which has shrunk in the DAO, a single olivary axon may supply branches
acidic solutions used to reveal the WGA-HRP to two or more nonadjacent sagittal zones in the
anterograde tracer. In this condition it is difficult anterior lobe (Armstrong et al., 1973; Ekerot and
to appreciate whether there is a coincidence Larson, 1982), d) in both the anteroposterior and
between the bands oflabeled climbing fibers and the mediolateral axes a band of cerebellar cortex
the zebrin 1 + or zebrin 1 - PC bands rather a, b, c, d, may originate from a band of olivary
than a simple similarity between two longitudi- neurons whose organization is d, b, a, c (Azizi and
nally oriented patterns of labeling in the molec- Woodward, 1987). Figure 1.7B is a schematic
ular layer. Second, the authors observed that drawing that considers some of the above-
the bands of labeled climbing fibers coincide mentioned proper ties of the olivo cerebellar
generally with either a zebrin 1 + or a zebrin projection. Each of the olivary zones (a, b, c, d)
1- band of PCs. This is surprising because it is terminates respectively in, and fills completely,
unlikely that a random injection in the inferior the a', b', c', and d' sagittal strips of the cerebellar
olive could repeatedly result in the complete cortex. It is assumed that the local topography
labeling of determined cortical bands, although of the projection of a onto a', b onto b', and so
some zebrin 1 cortical bands may be completely forth are conserved. There is a rostrocaudal shift
filled with labeled climbing fibers locally. in the projection maps between a and b, and an
Because of the great interest of these results for inversion between c and d. As shown in Figure
us, in collaboration with Hawkes we repeated 1.7B, a random tracer injection represented by
the experiments of Gravel et aI. (1987) using tri- stippling in the inferior olive results in the label-
tiated leucine as anterograde axonal tracer from ing of sagittal bands of climbing fibers (stippled
the inferior olive. This allowed us to reveal both in the cerebellar cortex). In several areas (indi-
the bands of anterogradely labeled climbing fibers cated by big dots) the bands of labeled climbing
and those of immunoreactive PCs on the same fibers abut against the boundaries of the a', b', c',
section. In addition, we used this material for a and d' sagittal zones. These boundaries are char-
statistical analysis aiming at disclosing if the acteristic of the topography of the olivocerebel-
observed coincidences between the discontinuities lar projection and predate the actual injection.
in the olivocerebellar projections and the shifts Thus, a random injection can uncover intrinsic
in PC immunoreactivity could occur by chance. boundaries of the olivocerebellar projection
Before reporting our results, a brief account of (local discontinuities in this projection). Another
the rationale of such an experiment is worth- illustration of this property is given in Fig. 1.8,
while. which is slightly modified from Groenewegen
14 Marion Wassef et al.

result in the outlining of intrinsic boundaries of


the olivo cerebellar projection. This notion is
fundamental for the analysis and interpretation
of the experiments of Gravel et ai. (1987) and
our own, which tested the possibility that the
CEREBELLAR PC heterogeneity evidenced by zebrin 1 differen-
CORTEX
tial immunolabeling coincides with intrinsic sub-
B divisions of the olivocerebellar projection.
Nine Wistar rats were used for this study. One
or two injections of 50 to 100 nl of tritiated
INFERIOR OLIVE

a b c d

Figure 1.7. Schematic representation of some of the


features of the topography of the olivocerebellar
projection. A tracer injection in the inferior olive
results in the anterograde labeling of sagittal climbing
fiber bands extending over several lobules in the
cerebellar cortex (A). A map of the olivocerebellar
projection is represented in B. The projections of the
contiguous territories of the inferior a, b, c, dare DAO MAO PO
organized in sagittal bands in the cerebellar cortex,
respectively a', b', c', d'. Due to local discontinuities in
the topography of the olivocerebellar projection (a
rostrocaudal shift between the projections of a and b
and an inversion of the terminal fields of c and d), a
tracer injection (stippling in the inferior olive) results
in the labelling of bands of climbing fibers (stippling
in the cerebellar cortex) that are partially delimited
(big dots) by the intrinsic boundaries (between a', b',c',
and d') of the olivocerebellar projection. Figure 1.8. The topologic properties of the olivo-
cerebellar projection are illustrated in a real injec-
tion case (stippled on a flattened representation of
the inferior olive, bottom) in the inferior olive of a
et al. (1979). The pattern of the climbing fiber
cat (cat H8854; redrawn with permission from
projection labeled in the cerebellar cortex of a
Groenewegen et aI., 1979). It is clear that the limits
cat (case H8854 of Groenewegen et aI., 1979) of the climbing fiber bands labeled by this injection
after an injection in the inferior olive was may frequently coincide with the boundaries of the
reported on the map. It is clear in this case that A... D sagittal zones. (Adapted with permission from
the rostrocaudal as well as mediolateral dis- Groenewegen et aI., 1979. Copyright by Wiley-Liss, a
continuities in the olivocerebellar projection Division of John Wiley and Sons, Inc.)
I. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 15

Climbing Fibers Labeling


The injections were large, and resulted in two
kinds of labeling in the cerebellar cortex. First,
in some cortical areas the climbing fiber labeling
was extensive, and the ARG labeling intensity
varied along sagittal cortical bands. Second, in
other regions, the labeling was fractionated and
restricted to several sagittal bands of molecular
layer extending over several folia separated by
unlabeled regions (Figs. 1.9, 1.10, 1.11). The pat-
terns of ARG labeling were closely similar in

Figure 1.9. Coronal sections through the cerebellar


cortex of adult rats injected with 3H amino acids in
the inferior olive. The sections were treated for zebrin
1 immunoreactivity and ARG. The ARG-labeled
climbing fibers are visualized in darkfield (white dots)
in A and C and in brightfield (black dots) in B. A
sagittal climbing fiber band contained in a zebrin
1 area and extending over three folia abuts against a
zebrin 1 PC band (A). Either located in a zerbin 1- (B)
or in a zebrin 1 + (C) PC band, the ARG-Iabeled
climbing fiber bands often end exactly at the shift in
PC immunoreactivity. Bar = 400 fl.m in A, 50 fl.m in B
and C.

leucine (5 to 10 j.LCi, 50 Ci/mM) were placed


unilaterally or bilaterally in the inferior olive
using a ventral approach. Two days later, the Figure 1.10. Coronal section through the caudal
animals were fixed by aldehyde perfusion. Frozen vermis of an adult rat injected with 3H amino acids
in the inferior olive, same treatment as in Fig. 1.9. The
sections through the cerebellar region were cut
section is viewed under dark field (A), brightfield (B),
in the coronal or horizontal plane and processed and a combination of both (C). Note the high number
successively for zebrin 1 labeling by immuno- of coincidences (arrows) between the limits of the
cytochemistry and tritiated leucine anterograde ARG-labeled bands of climbing fibers and the zebrin
axonal tracing by autoradiography (ARG). One 1 PC bands. However, some ARG-Iabeled bands cross
out of 6 sections was processed for autoradio- one (squares) or more (triangles) limits of the zebrin
graphy without prior immunolabeling. 1 PC bands. Bar = 500 fl.m.
16 Marion Wassef et al.

zebrin 1+ regions it acquired a reddish reflection


from the diaminobenzidine (DAB) reaction
product. This difference was less marked on
micrographs. However, in order to eliminate a
possible influence of the DAB precipitate on the
intensity of the ARG labeling, the statistical
analysis reported below was restricted to those
areas where the climbing fiber labeling was frac-
tionated. In addition, we supposed that the in-
trinsic discontinuities of the olivocerebellar
projection were probably involved in the forma-
tion of this fractionated projection (see Figs. 1.7B
and 1.8) and more likely to be evidenced in this
region.
In areas of extensive climbing fiber labeling,
marked variations in climbing fiber labeling
intensities were observed. Confirming the obser-
vation of Gravel et al. (1987), in many instances
these variations coincide with shifts in zebrin 1
immunoreactivity. As mentioned previously, this
region was not extensively studied. Our study
focused on the zones of the cerebellar cortex
where the labeling was fractionated. When the
relationship between the climbing fiber and the
zebrin 1 labeled bands were analyzed, several
patterns were observed as expected:
Figure 1.11. Coronal section through the cerebellum 1. The ARG-Iabeled band crosses one or two
of another adult rat injection with 3H amino acids in zebrin 1 zones and its boundaries are not related
the inferior olive. The section is taken at the same to the zebrin 1 immunoreactivity shifts (triangles
level illustrated in Fig. 1.10. The same zebrin 1+ PC in Fig. 1.10). In most cases the limits of these
band was involved by both injections but to different ARG-Iabeled bands are not sharp, but this is not
extents. The arrows point to boundaries that are always the case.
common to the ARG-labeled climbing fiber bands
2. The ARG-Iabeled band crosses one or more
and the zebrin 1 PC bands. Bar = 500 J!m.
zebrin 1 zones and its boundaries may coincide
on one or both sides with a shift in PC immuno-
alternate sections whether or not the sections reactivity (squares in Fig. 1.10).
were treated for immunocytochemistry before 3. The ARG-Iabeled band may be entirely en-
autoradiography. closed within the limits of a zebrin 1 band sharing
no, one, or two boundaries with it (arrows in
Relationship Between the Bands of Figs. 1.9, 1.10, 1.11).
Labeled Climbing Fibers and the
Bands of Zebrin 1 + and Zebrin 1 - PCs Several observations about the correspondence
between the labeled climbing fiber bands and
in Rats Injected with Tritiated Leucine
the zebrin 1 PC bands were made. In many
in the Inferior Olive instances a row of labeled climbing fibers stops
As expected, labeled climbing fibers were located exactly on the last PC of a zebrin 1 band (Figs.
in zebrin 1+ as well as in zebrin 1- molecular 1.9B, C; arrows in Figs. 1.9, 1.10, 1.11). An exact
layer strips. When observed in darkfield, the coincidence between the ARG and zebrin 1 bands
ARG labeling was usually brighter in zebrin 1- is observed more frequently in some regions,
areas, where it appeared white, whereas -in the particularly in the vermis (Figs. 1.9, 1.10, 1.11)
I. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 17

and the intermediate cortex. It is unlikely that together with landmarks on the sections were
this observation is due to a peculiarity of our drawn on a transparent paper superimposed on
injection technique, because our observations in the darkfield photographs. The shifts in zebrin
the caudal vermis (Figs. 1.10, 1.11) are similar 1 immunoreactivity were reported on the same
to those of Gravel et al. (1987), who used a dorsaldrawing positioned on the darkfield photographs
approach. Rather, it may be proposed that the and the coincidences observed between both
regions of the inferior olive projecting inside or kinds oflabeling were checked at higher magnifi-
between the ARG-Iabeled bands are relatively cation under the microscope. The lengths of
distant and rarely affected together by the tracer labeled and unlabeled molecular layer were
injection. Figures 1.10 and 1.11 show the same measured on enlargements of these drawings
region of the caudal vermis in two cases of in- using a Hewlett-Packard microcomputer inter-
ferior olive injections resulting respectively in faced with a graphic tablet.
the total and partial filling of the same zebrin 1+ A total length of 137.8 mm of molecular layer
bands. In both cases there is a striking coinci- was screened, containing 77 ARG-Iabeled bands
dence between the border of the ARG-Iabeled and 137 shifts in zebrin 1 immunoreactivity. The
climbing fiber band and the limit of a zebrin 1 length of ARG-Iabeled molecular layer was
PC band. 38.4 mm (28% of total) and the number of ARG
In several instances, in successive sections of band borders was 152 (in two cases the limit of
the same animal, a band of ARG labeling may the band was too ill-defined). The number of
be observed first inside a zebrin 1 band, then PC shifts in immunoreactivity occurring inside
abutting against its limit with both boundaries an ARG-Iabeled band was 17 and the number
remaining coincident in several sections before of exact coincidences was 43.
either crossing the zebrin 1 boundary or becom- We have compared the observed number of
ing located again inside the zebrin 1 band. This shifts occurring in the ARG-Iabeled molecular
pattern is similar to the one depicted in Fig. 1.7.layer to the number expected under the null
hypothesis of independence between the pattern
Are the Coincidences Observed of zebrin 1 immunoreactivity and the ARG
Statistically Significant? labeling. If the ARG labeling and zebrin 1
immunoreactivity were independent, given an
In order to decide whether there is a significant ARG labeling of 28% of the total length of
correlation between the pattern of the zebrin 1 molecular layer, one would expect 38.4 (28% of
labeled bands and the topography of the olivo- 137 shifts) shifts in zebrin 1 immnoreactivity to
cerebellar projection, we measured several para- fall inside the ARG-Iabeled bands. Comparing
meters in regions where the ARG labeling was the observed number (17) to this expected num-
fractionated. These regions were selected as ber using a chi-square statistic leads to a p value
follows. The sections were screened in darkfield of .0001.
at low magnification (x 10 or x 6.3) and areas Assuming the distance from center to center
where the climbing fiber labeling was intense, between two adjacent PCs to be 30 pm (31 ± 2;
sharply delimited, and fractionated were selected. Altman and Winfree, 1977), the number of PCs
A single field was selected on a given section. It aligned within 137.8 mm of molecular layer is
was photographed under darkfield, brightfield, about 4600. The expected number of coincidences
and a combination of both. All the ARG-Iabeled between shifts in PC immunoreactivity (137) and
bands on the micrographs were considered. It limits of ARG-Iabeled bands (152) is 5. The
was assumed that the areas on each photograph comparison of this expected number using a
were large enough - at least 3.6 mm x 2.4 mm- Poisson approximation leads to a p value of less
to reduce a: bias in sampling. Three injected rats than lO-12.
were rejected from the study because the labeling These results invalidate the null hypothesis of
was too extensive and diffuse; the observations independence and indicate that the topography
reported below are derived from the six remain- of the olivocerebellar projection is highly cor-
ing animals. The limits ofthe ARG-Iabeled bands related with the pattern of the zebrin 1 PC bands.
18 Marion Wassef et al.

under the control of climbing fibers, which would


explain the good correlation observed between
zebrin 1 immunoreactivity and the topography
of the olivo cerebellar projection. To eliminate
this possibility, in collaboration with Alvarado-
Mallart and Hawkes, we have isolated rat cere-
bellar anlage at E13, before the olivary afferents
have reached the cerebellar plate (unpublished
observations), and grafted them in a hole made
by aspiration in the cerebral cortex of adult
hosts. The grafts were analyzed 2 months later
by double labeling for CaBP and zebrin 1 immu-
noreactivity. As reported previously (Alvarado-
Mallart and Sotelo, 1982; Das, 1973), these cere-
bellar transplants develop a quasinormal lami-
nated cerebellar structure. In the cortical region
of the transplant (Fig. 1.12), PCs are arranged
in alternating zebrin 1+ and zerbin 1- patches,
indicating that the organization of PCs in clusters
differing in their zerbin 1 immunoreactivity does
not necessitate the presence of climbing fibers
and is probably not under their control.

Conclusion
We propose that the development of the cere-
Figure 1.12. E13 cerebellar anlage grafted into a bellum involves an early mechanism of self-
cavity in the cerebral cortex over the choroid plexus organization of its cortical topography. A map
of an adult host and allowed to develop for 2 months. of the cerebellar cortex is progressively built up
The section was double stained for zebrin 1 (DAB) by the migration and spreading of several cate-
and CaBP (FITC) and viewed under a combination gories of biochemically different PCs. Although
of transmitted light and FITC optics. The zebrin 1 +
the proper establishment of the reproducible
(black) and zebrin 1- (white) PCs are arranged in
clusters, indicating that the differential expression of
adult pattern of sagittal zonation of PC subsets
the zebrin 1 epitope by PCs is not under the control certainly depends on cellular interactions during
of the olivary afferents. Bar = 200 j1m. development, the generation of biochemically
different categories of PCs is apparently an
intrinsic program of the neuroepithelium of the
cerebellum.
In addition, they suggest that contiguous sub-
Several observations support the hypothesis
divisions of the inferior olive often project to
that the zonation of the cerebellar cortex defined
nonadjacent cortical areas and that the topo-
by PC heterogeneity is involved in the organiza-
graphy of the olivo cerebellar map is rather
tion of the cortical connections. The PC hetero-
complex.
geneity appears earlier than the ingrowth of
the cerebellar afferents into the cerebellar plate
Is the Pattern of Zebrin 1 during embryonic development; it could provide
Immunoreactivity Under the Direct a complex marking of the cerebellar cortex
through a combinatory mechanism. The study
Control of Climbing Fibers? of the spinocerebellar projection in mutant mice
Because the zebrin 1 epitope is not detected before reveals that their characteristic columnar pattern
P6 (Leclerc et aI., 1988), its expression could be of projection is less sensitive to an early deletion
1. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 19

of theit: target (granule cells) than to a genetic Alvarado-Mallart, R.M., and Sotelo C. (1982): Dif-
PC defect, which suggests a role for PCs in the ferentiation of cerebellar anlage heterotopically
patterning of this projection. In the adult rat transplanted to adult rat brain: A light and electron
cerebellum, there is a close correlation between microscopic study. J. Compo Neurol., 212, 247-
the shifts in zebrin 1 immunoreactivity and the 267.
Armstrong, D.M., Harvey, R.I., and Schild, R.F.
local discontinuities in the olivocerebellar pro-
(1973): The spatial organization of climbing fibre
jection. branching in the cat cerebellum. Exp. Brain Res.,
Several points, however, merit further investi- 18,40-58.
gation: how is PC heterogeneity generated? Is Armstrong, D.M., Harvey, R.J., and Schild, R.F.
PC heterogeneity reflected on their surface? What (1974) Topographical localization in the olivo-
are the timing and pattern of ingrowth of the cerebellar projection: An electrophysiological study
afIerents into the cerebellar plate? How are the in the cat. J. Compo Neurol., 154, 287-302.
large clusters of PCs observed during early Arsenio-Nunes, M.L., and Sotelo, C. (1985): Deve-
development subdivided reproducibly, giving lopment of spinocerebellar system in the postnatal
rise eventually to the adult monolayer and its rat. J. Compo Neurol., 237, 291-306.
characteristic zonation? These questions are Arsenio-Nunes, M.L., Sotelo, c., and Wehrle, R.
(1988): Organization of spinocerebellar projection
currently under investigation.
map in three types of agranular cerebellum:
Purkinje cells vs. granule cells as organizer element.
J. Compo Neurol., 273, 120-136.
Acknowledgments. We wish to thank C. Hill for Azizi, S.A., and Woodward, DJ. (1987): Inferior
the statistical analysis of our measures, R.M. olivary nuclear complex of the rat: Morphology
Alvarado-Mallart for teaching us to the graft and comments on the principles of organization
technique, R. Rouse for critical reading of within the olivocerebellar system. J. Compo Neurol.,
the manuscript, P. Greengard, R. Hawkes, 263, 467-484.
J. Morgan, M. Thomasset, and J.-P. Zanetta for Brown, B.L., and Graybiel, A.M. (1983): Zonal
the gift of antibodies, B. Cholley for technical organization in the cerebellar vermis of the cat.
assistance, and D. Le Cren for photographic Anat. Rec., 205, 25A.
work. Campbell, N.C., and Armstrong, D.M. (1983a): The
olivocerebellar projection in the rat: An autoradio-
graphic study. Brain Res., 275, 215-233.
References Campbell, N.C., and Armstrong, D.M. (1983b):
Topographical localization in the olivocerebellar
Altman, J. (1972a): Postnatal development of the projection in the rat: An auto radiographic study.
cerebellar cortex in the rat. I. The external germinal Brain Res., 275, 235-249.
layer and the transitional molecular layer. J. Compo Chan-Palay, v., Nilaver, G., Palay, S.L., Beinfeld,
Neurol., 145,353-398. M.e., Zimmerman, E.A., Wu, J.Y., and O'Donohue,
Altman, J. (1972b): Postnatal development of the T.L. (1981): Chemical heterogeneity in cerebellar
cerebellar cortex in the rat. II. Phases in the matura- Purkinje cells: Existence and coexistence of
tion of Purkinje cells and the molecular layer. glutamic acid decarboxylase-like and motilin-like
J. Compo Neurol., 145, 399-464. immunoreactivities. Proc. N at/. Acad. Sci. USA, 78,
Altman, J. (1972c): Postnatal development of the 7787-7791.
cerebellar cortex in the rat. III. Maturation of the Chan-Pa1ay, V., Palay, S.L., and Wu, J.Y. (1982):
components of the granular layer. J. Compo N eurol., Sagittal cerebellar microbands of taurine neurons:
145, 465-514. Immunocytochemical demonstration by using anti-
Altman, J., and Bayer, SA (1978): Prenatal devel- bodies against the taurine-synthesizing enzyme
opment of the cerebellar system in the rat. I. Cyto- cystein sulfinic acid decarboxylase. Proc. N at/.
genesis a)1d histogenesis of the deep nuclei and the Acad. Sci. USA, 79, 4421-4225.
cortex of the cerebellum. J. Compo Neurol., 179, Crepel, F. (1971): Maturation of climbing fiber
23-48. responses in the rat. Brain Res., 35, 272-276.
Altman, J., and Winfree, A.R. (1977): Postnatal Crepel, F., Mariani, J., and Delhaye-Bouchaud, N.
development of the cerebellar cortex in the rat. (1976): Evidence for a multiple innervation of
V. Spatial organization of Purkinje cell perikarya. Purkinje cells by climbing fibers in the immature
J. Compo Neurol., 171, 1-16. rat cerebellum. J. Neurobiol., 7, 567-578.
20 Marion Wassef et al.

Crepel, F., Delhaye-Bouchaud, N., and Dupont, J.L. Ingram, V.M., Ogren, M.P., Chatot, e.L., Gossel,
(1981): Fate of the multiple innervation of cere- J.M., and Owens, B.B. (1985): Diversity among
bellar Purkinje cells by climbing fibers in immature Purkinje cells in the monkey cerebellum. Proc. N atl.
control, X-irradiated and hypothyroid rats. Dev. Acad. Sci. USA, 82, 7131-7135.
Brain Res., 1, 59-71. Jande, S.S., Maler, L., and Lawson, D.E.M. (1981):
Das, G.D. (1973): Transplantation of cerebellar tissue Immunocytochemical mapping of vitamin D-
in the cerebellum of neonate rabbits. Brain Res., dependent calcium-binding protein in brain. Nature
50, 170-173. ( Land.), 294, 765-767.
De Camilli, P., Miller, P., Levitt, P., Walter, u., and Landis, S.e. (1973): Ultrastructural changes in the
Greengard, P. (1984): Anatomy of cerebellar Purkinje mitochondria of cerebellar Purkinje cells of the
cells in the rat determined by a specific immuno- "nervous" mutant mice. J. Cell Bioi., 57, 782-
histochemical marker. Neuroscience, 11, 761-817. 797.
Eisenman, L. (1981): Olivocerebellar projections ofthe Lange, W., Unger, J., Pitzl, H., and Weindl, A. (1986):
pyramis and copula pyramidis in the rat: Differential Is motilin a cerebellar peptide? Anat. Embryol., 173,
projections to parasagittal zones. J. Camp. Neural., 371-376.
199,65-76. Langley, O.K., Reeber, A., Vincendon, G., and
Ekerot, e.F., and Larson, B. (1982): Branching of Zanetta, J.P. (1982): Fine structural localization of
olivary axons to innervate pairs of sagittal zones a new Purkinje cell-specific glycoprotein subunit:
in the cerebellar anterior lobe of the cat. Exp. Brain Immunoelectron microscopical study. J. Camp.
Res., 48, 185-198. Neural., 208, 335-344.
Feirabend, H.P.K. (1983): Anatomy and development Larramendi, L.M.H. (1969): Analysis of the synapto-
of longitudinal patterns in the architecture of the genesis in the cerebellum of the mouse. In:
cerebellum of the white Leghorn (Gallus Domesti- Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Develop-
cus). Leiden University, Leiden, Thesis. ment (R. Llinas, ed.). Chicago: American Medical
Gravel, e., Eisenman, L., Sasseville, R., and Hawkes, Association, pp. 803-843.
R. (1987): Parasagittal organization of the rat cere- Leclerc N., Gravel, e., and Hawkes, R. (1988):
bellar cortex: A direct correlation between antigenic Development of para sagittal zonation in the rat
bands revealed by mabQ113 and the topography cerebellar cortex: MabQI13 antigenic bands are
of the olivocerebellar projection. J. Camp. Neural., created postnatally by the suppression of antigen
265, 294-310. expression in a subset of Purkinje cells. J. Camp.
Groenewegen, H.J., and Voogd, J. (1977): The Neural., 273, 399-420.
parasagittal zonation within the olivocerebellar Legrand, e., Thomasset, M., Parkes, e.O., Clavel,
projection. I. Climbing fiber distribution in the M.e., and Rabie, A. (1983): Calcium-binding pro-
vermis of cat cerebellum. J. Camp. Neural., 174, tein in the developing rat cerebellum. An immuno-
417-488. histochemical study. Cell Tissue Res., 233, 389-
Groenewegen, H.J., Voogd J., and Freedman, S.L. 402.
(1979): The parasagittal zonation within the Lohmann, S.M., Walter, u., Miller, P.E., Greengard,
olivocerebellar projection. II. Climbing fiber dis- P., and De Camilli, P. (1981): Immunohistochemi-
tribution in the intermediate and hemispheric parts cal localization of cyclic GMP-dependent protein
of cat cerebellum. J. Camp. Neural., 183, 551-602. kinase in mammalian brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
Hanaway, J. (1967): Formation and differentiation of USA, 78, 653-657.
the external granular layer of the chick cerebellum. Marani, E., and Voogd, J. (1973): Some aspects of
J. Camp. Neural., 131, 1-14. the localization of the enzyme 5'-nucleotidase in the
Hawkes, R., Colonnier, M., and Leclerc, N. (1985): molecular layer of the cerebellum of the mouse.
Monoclonal antibodies reveal saggital banding in Acta Morphol. Neerl. Scand., 11,353-354.
the rodent cerebellar cortex. Brain Res., 333, 359- Marani, E., and Voogd, J. (1977): An acetylcholin-
365. esterase band pattern in the molecular layer of the
Hawkes, R., and Leclerc, N. (1987): Antigenic map cerebellum of the cat. J. Anat., 124, 335-345.
of the rat cerebellar cortex: The distribution of Mariani, J., and Changeux, J.P. (1981): Ontogenesis
parasagittal bands as revealed by a monoclonal of olivocerebellar relationships. I. Studies by
anti-Purkinje cell antibody mabQ113. J. Camp. intracellular recordings of the multiple innervation
Neural., 256, 29-41. ofPurkinje cells by climbing fibers in the developing
Hess, D.H., and Voogd, J. (1986): Chemoarchi- rat cerebellum. J. N eurosci., 1, 696-701.
tectonic zonation of the monkey cerebellum. Brain Meinhardt, H. (1982): Models of Biological Pattern
Res., 369, 383-387. Formation. London: Academic Press.
1. Purkinje Cell Heterogeneity 21

Meinhalidt, H. (1983): Cell determination boundaries mouse: A model to study presynaptic stabilization.
as organizing regions for secondary embryonic Brain Res., 175, 11-36.
fields. Dev. BioI. 96, 375-385. Sotelo, C., Bourrat, F., and Triller, A. (1984) Post-
Miale, I.L., and Sidman, R.L. (1961): An autoradio- natal development of the inferior olivary complex
graphic analysis of histogenesis in the mouse in the rat. II. Topographic organization of the
cerebellum. Exp. Neurol., 4, 277-296. immature olivocerebellar projection. J. Compo
Mullen, R.J., Eicher, E.M., and Sidman, R.L. (1976): Neurol.,222,177-199.
Purkinje cell degeneration, a new neurological Sperry, R.W. (1944): Optic nerve regeneration with
mutation in the mouse. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, return of vision in anurans. J. Neurophysiol., 7,
73, 208-212. 57-69.
Neustadt, A., Frostholm, A., and Rotter, A. (1988): Sperry, R.W. (1963): Chemoaffinity in the orderly
Topographical distribution of muscarinic choliner- growth of nerve fiber patterns and connections.
gic receptors in the cerebellar cortex of the mouse, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, SO, 703-710.
rat, guinea pig and rabbit: A species comparison. Toshio, S., Schoichiro, N., and Kazuo, S. (1976):
J. Compo Neurol., 272, 317-330. Electrophysiological study on the postnatal develop-
Nilaver, G., Defendini, R., Zimmerman, E.A., ment of neuronal mechanisms in the rat cerebellar
Beinfeld, M.e., and O'Donohue, T.L. (1982): cortex. Brain Res., 108, 279-294.
Motilin in the Purkinje cell of the cerebellum. Von der Malsburg, Ch., and Willshaw, DJ. (1977):
Nature, 295, 597-598. How to label nerve cells so that they can inter-
Puro, D.G., and Woodward, DJ. (1977a): Matura- connect in an ordered fashion. Proc. N atl. Acad.
tion of evoked climbing fiber input to rat cerebellar Sci. USA, 74, 5176-5178.
Purkinje cells (I). Exp. Brain Res., 28, 85-100. Wassef, M., and Sotelo, e. (1984): Asynchrony in the
Puro, D.G., and Woodward, DJ. (1977b): Matura- expression of guanosine 3':5' -phosphate-dependent
tion of evoked mossy fiber input to rat cerebellar protein kinase by clusters of Purkinje cells during
Purkinje cell (II). Exp. Brain Res., 28, 427-441. the perinatal development of rat cerebellum.
Reeber, A., Vincendon, G., and Zanetta, J.P. (1981): Neuroscience, 13, 1217-1241.
Isolation and immunohistochemical localization of Wassef, M., Zanetta, J.P., Brehier, A., and Sotelo, e.
a Purkinje cell specific glycoprotein subunit from (1985): Transient biochemical compartmentaliza-
rat cerebellum. Brain Res., 229, 53-65. tion of Purkinje cells during early cerebellar develop-
Scott, T.G. (1964): A unique pattern of localization ment. Dev. BioI., 111, 129-137.
within the cerebellum of the mouse. J. Compo Wassef, M., Simons, J., Tappaz, M.L., and Sotelo, e.
Neurol.,22,1-7. (1986): Non-Purkinje cell Gabaergic innervation in
Shimono, T., Nasaka, S., and Sasaki, K. (1976): Electro- the deep cerebellar nuclei: A quantitative immuno-
physiological study on the postnatal development cytochemical study in C57BL and in Purkinje
of neural mechanisms in the rat cerebellar cortex. cell degeneration mutant mice. Brain Res., 399,
Brain Res., 108, 279-294. 125-135.
Sidman, R.L., and Green, M.C. (1970): "Nervous", Wassef, M., Sotelo, e., Cholley, B., Brehier, A., and
a new mutant mouse with cerebellar disease. Thomasset, M. (1987): Cerebellar mutations affecting
In: Les Mutants Pathologiques Chez l'Animal the postnatal survival of Purkinje cells in the mouse
(M. Sabourdy, ed.) Paris: Centre National de la disclose a longitudinal pattern of differentially sensi-
Recherche Scientifique, pp. 69-79. tivity cells. Dev. BioI. 124, 379-389.
Sotelo, e., and Changeux, J.P. (1974a): Transsynaptic West, MJ., and Del Cerro, M. (1976): Early forma-
degeneration "en cascade" in the cerebellar cortex tion of synapses in the molecular layer of the fetal
of staggerer mutant mice. Brain Res., 67, 519-526. rat cerebellum. J. Compo Neurol., 165, 137-160.
Sotelo, e., and Changeux, J.P. (1974b): Bergmann Ziai, R., Pan, Y.C.E., Hulmes, J.D., Sangameswaran,
fibers and granular cell migration in the cerebellum L., and M-organ, 1I. (1986): Isolation, sequence,
of homozygous weaver mutant mouse. Brain Res., and developmental profile of a brain-specific poly-
77,484-491. peptide, PEP-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 83,
Sotelo, e., and Triller, A. (1979): Fate of presynaptic 8420-8423.
afferents of Purkinje cells in adult nervous mutant
2
Zebrins: Molecular Markers
of Compartmentation in the Cerebellum
Richard Hawkes, Gino Brochu, Louise Dore, Claude Gravel, and Nicole Leclerc

Ordered projections in the brain are established mammalian cerebellar cortex are organized into
in several stages. Initially, the formation of an parasagittal zones. A parasagittal zonation was
afferent pathway depends on white matter inter- shown first for the Purkinje cell efferent projection
actions, such as contact guidance along genetically to the cerebellar nuclei and the lateral vestibular
determined pathways and selective axon fasci- nuclei (e.g., Chambers and Sprague, 1955a, b;
culation, to guide neurites to the correct target Courville and Diakiw, 1976; Haines et aI., 1982;
fields. Subsequently, target cell recognition by Jansen and Brodal, 1940, 1942; Voogd and Bigare,
afferent growth cones and competition between 1980). Subsequently, a similar afferent parcellation
growth cones for targets (and between targets for was shown to be present in some mossy fiber
inputs) serve to eliminate superfluous or incorrect (MF) pathways (Chan-Palay et aI., 1977; Scheibel,
projections and may refine the topography. Many 1977; Voogd, 1969; Yaginuma and Matsushita,
regions, including the neocortex, the superior col- 1986) and in the climbing fiber (CF) projection
liculus, the striatum, and the dorsal column nuclei, from the inferior olives (Armstrong et aI., 1974,
are functionally organized in the form of patches 1982; Beyedet aI., 1982; Brodal, 1976; Brodal et aI.,
or stripes that correspond to the discrete segrega- 1975; Courville, 1975; Courville and Faraco-
tion of the afferent or efferent axons. The same Cantin, 1978; Eisenman, 1981, 1984; Eisenman
appears to be true in the cerebellum. Studies using et aI., 1983; Groenewegen and Voogd, 1977;
the retrograde-anterograde axonal transport of Groenewegen et aI., 1979; Oscarsson, 1969, 1980;
tracers, electro physiological recording, somato- Oscarsson and Sj6lund, 1977; Van Gilder and
topic mapping, and molecular mapping have all O'Leary, 1970; Voogd, 1964, 1967, 1969). The
revealed a parasagittal bandlike topographical sagittal compartmentation of the afferent and
organization of the cerebellar cortex and its afferent efferent connections of the cerebellum have a
and efferent connections. We are using pattern histological equivalent in the myeloarchitecture
formation in the cerebellar cortex as a model to of the white matter. By using the Haggqvist me-
explore the rules that give rise to topographically thod to stain myelin, Voogd (1969) was able to
ordered projections. In this chapter we concen- identify numerous longitudinal axon compart-
trate on molecular markers of cerebellar com part- ments, based largely on differences in the diam-
mentation in the adult, and consider in particular: eters of the fibers. In particular, the large caliber-
Purkinje cell axons are fasciculated together to
1. how many independent topographical maps
form independent clusters, interposed by smaller
art! present in the cerebellum and in what ways
. caliber afferent fibers. For example, in the ferret
are the different maps interrelated?
anterior lobe, five abrupt changes in the axon
2. how are the maps constructed and correlated
diameters were identified in each hemicerebellum,
during development?
thereby defining six parallel compartments: A,
The afferent and efferent connections of the adjacent to the midline, and the B, Cl, C2, C3,

22
2. Zebrins 23

and D compartments disposed laterally. An equi- Intrinsic Markers of


valent compartmentation was identified in the Cerebellar Compartments
same way in the hedgehog, the cat, the tree shrew
(Tupaia), the slow lori (Nycticebus), and the 5' - Nucleotidase
monkey (M acaca).
These data are clearly central to the overall The first evidence that the cerebellar cortex was
problem of cerebellar architecture, but as they nonuniform biochemically came from the studies
have been reviewed extensively in the recent past of Scott (1963, 1964) on the cerebellar distribution
(e.g., Bloedel and Courville, 1981), they will not of the enzyme 5' -nucleotidase (S'N) in the mouse.
be considered further here except to correlate In the cerebellum, S'N is concentrated primarily
patterns of afferent termination with the intrinsic in the molecular layer but it is also found in asso-
cerebellar compartmentation. Instead, the dis- ciation with the Purkinje cell perikarya, Golgi
cussion will concentrate on molecular markers cells, and some axons in the granular layer and
of cerebellar compartmentation. white matter tracts. Scott (1963) found a rostro-

Figure 2.1. Four views of the cerebellar cortex in the P3 +). The patches encompass both the granular layer
rat stained for AChE. The plane of focus is at the level (G L) and the white matter (WM) but not the molecular
of the most superficial synaptic glomeruli and hence layer (ML). C and D show similar patchy staining in
parts of the tissue are always out of focus. A and B the hemispheres, in C corresponding to P6 + of the crus
show the patchy distribution of AChE in the vermis, II, and in D showing the P5+ patch in crus I. Patches
in lobule IV in A and lobule VIII in B. In both cases were identified by reference to the zebrin I distribution
there is a patch of elevated activity that straddles the on adjacent sections (not shown). Scale bar = 200 11m.
midline (P 1+) and two patches to either side (P2 +, (Reprinted with permission from Boegman et aI., 1988.)
24 Richard Hawkes et al.

ca.udal gradient in enzyme activity, with strong activity granular layer of the granular layer
staining in the nodulus, intermediate staining in appears patchy in frontal section, and the dis-
the uvula, pyramis, tuber, and declive, and weak tribution of these patches corresponds to the
staining anteriorly in the culmen, centralis, and arrangement of some antigenic compartments
lingula. Microdensitometry revealed a twofold (Boegman et aI., 1988, and below).
difference in intensity from anterior to posterior
lobe. However, in horizontal and frontal sections Glutamic Acid Decarboxylase
the distribution of the enzyme revealed a wholly
Much literature exists to support the notion that
unexpected pattern of longitudinal bands of acti-
gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) is the princi-
vity. One band was astride the midline of the
pal. neurotransmitter of cerebellar Purkinje cells,
cerebellar cortex and the others were symmetri-
so It was a great surprise when Chan-Palay et al.
cally disposed to either side. The maximum num-
(1981) reported heterogeneity in the distribution
ber of bands was seen in horizontal sections where
of glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) immuno-
the cerebellum was at its widest: in addition to the
reactivity in adult rat, mouse, and monkey. The
~idline band, six others were seen radiating, fan-
GAD+ Purkinje cells had a characteristic mor-
lIke, from the caudal surface in each hemicerebel-
phology, being slightly smaller and with smaller
lum. A more detailed analysis of the 5'N distri-
nuclei. Broad parasagittal zones could be dis-
bution in the adult mouse is presented in Marani
~erned, interposed by unstained compartments;
(1982a, b).
III all, only about half the Purkinje cells were
GAD immunoreactive.
Acetylcholinesterase
Another histochemical demonstration of cere- Moti/in
bellar compartmentation has been made with
In the same article that first described GAD hetero-
acetylcholinesterase (AChE) staining of young
gen~i~y, ~ nonuniform distribution ofthe peptide
and adult cats (Brown and Graybiel, 1983; Marani,
motIlIn III Purkinje cells was reported (Chan-
1982a; Marani and Voogd, 1977; Raman-Moliner,
Palay et aI., 1981). Immunoreactive Purkinje cells
1972). For example, Marani and Voogd (1977)
were most frequent in the flocculus, paraflocculus,
have described a pattern of five positive and six
and the lateral hemispheres. By contrast, stained
interposed negative bands in the molecular layer
profiles were rare in the vermis, appearing either
of young cats. No staining was seen in the white
as scattered individuals or as contributors to three
matter, the Purkinje cell somata, or the granule
very fine para sagittal bands in lobules I to IV. In
cells. No structural basis for the staining was
lobules V and VII additional microzones were
identified. Preliminary evidence suggests that a
identified. However, these data are at variance
similar AChE segregation can be demonstrated
with those of Lange et aI. (1986), who could not
in monkeys (Hess and Voogd, 1986; Ingram et aI.,
stain the cerebellum with seven other antimotilin
1985). The differential staining in the molecular
antisera, and obtained only a rather uniform stain-
layer diminished with age and was not seen beyond
ing with yet another antiserum. It may be that
4 months postnatal. Similarly, in the granular
the region.al heterogeneity in the reported staining
layer and the white matter of the adult rat cere-
pattern ~Ith ~ntimotilin is due to a cross-reacting
?ellar cortex there is a patchy AChE staining that
contamlllant III the polyclonal antiserum, or that
IS associated primarily with the synaptic glomeruli
there are motilinlike molecules in the cerebellum
(Fig. 2.1) (Boegman et aI., 1988). There are obvious
that are recognized selectively by different sera,
differences from region to region within the gra-
and expressed differentially by different cell types
nular layer, associated with the numerical density
(see also Beinfeld and Korchak, 1985).
of strongly immunoreactive glomeruli. Bands are
clear both in the anterior (Fig. 2.1A) and posterior
Cysteine Sulfinic Acid Decarboxylase
lobe vermis (Fig. 2.1 B). Similar prominent patches
of strongly stained glomeruli are seen in crus II Immunocytochemical studies of the distribution
(Fig. 2.1C) and in crus I (Fig. 2.1 D). The AChE ofthe enzyme cysteine sulfinic acid decarboxylase
2. Zebrins 25

(CSADCase)-an enzyme in the taurine synthesis xidase-stained cerebellum, the antigen was named
pathway-revealed that the antigenicity was "zebrin I." Its function is unknown. In sections
confined to a subset of Purkinje cells. Further- of cerebellum immunohistochemically stained
more, autoradiography with eH]-taurine revealed with anti-zebrin I, the immunoreactivity is con-
that many Purkinje cells also accumulate taurine fined exclusively to the Purkinje cells, where it is
(Chan- Palay et aI., 1982). It is not clear that these distributed throughout the somata, the dendrites,
two subsets are identical. The distribution of the and the dendritic spines. The Purkinje cell axons,
CSADCase immunoreactive cells has been ex- axon collaterals, and terminals in the cerebellar
plored in both mice and rabbits (Chan-Palay nuclei are also immunoreactive (Gravel et aI.,
et aI., 1982). Overall, the distributions were similar 1986; Hawkes and Leclerc, 1986,1989). The other
between species. Most imunoreactive profiles were cell types in the cerebellum are immunonegative,
seen in lobules I and X of the vermis, and in the although elsewhere in the central nervous system
paraflocculus and flocculus. In all cases, micro- there is a weak glial immunoreactivity (Plioplys
bands of immunoreactive neurons were disposed and Hawkes, 1986, 1988). Electron microscopy
symmetrically about the midline and interposed shows that the immunoperoxidase reaction pro-
by nonreactive compartments. In the anterior duct is intracellular and cytoplasmic. In regions
lobe, there are seven reactive bands, one at the of the Purkinje cell that are rich in microtubules,
midline and three pairs to either side; in the poster- such as the primary dendrites, the reaction
ior lobe, the bands are less distinct, and the pre- product appears to be microtubule-associated;
sence of bands in the hemispheres is difficult to however, regions of the cytoplasm devoid of
distinguish except in lobules VI and VII, where micro tubules are equally stained so this associ-
at least two are present. Immunocytochemistry ation is probably adventitious. The interesting
with antibodies to taurine itself has complicated feature of zebrin I is that it is expressed only in a
the issue. Several groups have demonstrated tau- subset of Purkinje cells. In the cerebellum as a
rinelike immunoreactivity in Purkinje cells, but whole, about one third of the Purkinje cells are
in these cases all Purkinje cells were immuno- zebrin 1+. The proportion of zebrin I + to zebrin
reactive (Campistron et aI., 1986; Madsen et aI., 1- Purkinje cells varies from lobule to lobule. For
1985; Tomida and Kimura, 1987). More recently, example, in the anterior lobe vermis, only 25% of
Magnussen et ai. (1988) demonstrated bands of the Purkinje cells are zebrin I + whereas c~udal to
taurine-immunoreactive Purkinje cells under light the primary fissure 40% are zebrin 1+, and in the
fixation conditions that, with greater tissue cross- most posterior regions more than 90% of the
linking, tended to disappear. These bands are also profiles in anyone section may be zebrin I + and
CSADCase-immunoreactive. The physical basis there is a tendency for the zebrin I - bands to
of the fixation sensitivity remains unclear. disappear completely.
The immunoreactive Purkinje cells are clustered
together to form a family of parasagittal bands
Zebrin I running roughly rostrocaudally throughout the
In a specific attempt to identify molecules whose cerebellar cortex. Zebrin I + bands alternate with
distributions reflect or encode cerebellar topo- similar bands of zebrin I - cells. The alternation
graphy, an extensive monoclonal antibody library of immunoreactive and unreactive bands is found
was constructed against rat cerebellar polypeptide in both the vermis and in the hemispheres. The
antigens, and was screened by using immuno- expression of the zebrin I epitope seems not to be
cytochemistry for epitope distributions that are all-or-nothing and, in general, bands tend to be-
nonuniform across the cerebellum. One such anti- come weaker and less well defined as they pass
body is anti-zebrin I (mabQl13). Anti-zebrin I into the anterior lobe. The more lateral bands
recognizes an epitope, zebrin I, that appears as a tend to be less strongly stained than the more
single polypeptide band on Western blots of rat medial. Subsequently, four other zebrins have
cerebellar cortex, with an apparent molecular been identified (zebrin II-V).
weight of 120 kDa (Hawkes et aI., 1985). Because The parasagittal zonation revealed by zebrin I
of the striped appearance of the immunopero- is symmetrical about the midline and is repro-
26 Richard Hawkes et al.

du,cible from individual to individual both in the zebrin I - bands called P -. In each hemicerebel-
number and general arrangement of the bands lum, rows of Purkinje cells are linked from lobule
and in much of the finer detail (Fig. 2.2). The to lobule to create a midline band (PI +) and six
bands ofzebrin 1+ cells have been called P+, and lateral p+ bands (P2+ to P7 +) in each hemicere-

a e

d
Figure 2.2. Illustration of the reproducibility of the individual differences can be seen: the variable location
zebrin I staining pattern in the rat. Horizontal sections, of P4 + in lobules VIII and IX of the posterior vermis;
each 50 11m thick, were taken at level interaural4.5 mm the variations in P5 +- P6 + in crus II (e.g., arrowhead
(according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson, 1982) in panel b); the presence of satellite bands. Scale bar =
from eight individuals and immunoperoxidase stained 2.0mm. (Reprinted with permission from Hawkes and
with anti-zebrin I. Bands PI + through P7 + are identi- Leclerc, 1987. Copyright 1987, Wiley-Liss, a Division
fied in panel c. The pattern of immunoreactivity is of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.)
similar in all eight individuals. Three sources of inter-
2. Zebrins 27

Vllb

VIII
CRUS Iia
IXb
CRUS lib
IXa

Figure 2.3. a: Whole mount of the rat cerebellum of organization is clear in the vermis but in the hemisphere
an adult rat immunohistochemically stained with anti- the distribution is also consistent with a checkerboard
zebrin I as seen from the posterior. The bands are of antigenic patches. (Reprinted with permission from
clearly visible in both the vermis and the hemispheres. Hawkes and Leclerc, 1987. Copyright 1987 by Wiley-
b: The distribution of immunopositive elements in the Liss, a Division of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.)
whole mount has been mapped. The parasagittal band
28 Richard Hawkes et al.

be.llum. The bands of zebrin I - cells have been PS + stops at the border oflobule VIII and the
mimed after the immediately medial P + band. uvula. However, an alternative is to consider
The pattern of bands can also be seen in whole- that P4 + and PS + in fact fuse and continue
mount preparations of the cerebellum that have together in lobules IX and X. Like P4 +, PS +
been permeabilized with cycles of freezing and also shows a subdivision within crus II. In this
thawing and then immunostained with anti- case, the minor band is the median PSa + and
zebrin I (Fig. 2.3). the major is PSb + more laterally (also iso-
The seven major p+ bands that we have identi- lating a local PSa -). In more ventral sections,
fied in each hemisphere are as follows: the division of PS + can be followed into the
paramedian lobule. As with P4b +, PSa + does
1. PI + extends the entire length of the vermis not appear to continue into the anterior lobe
from lobules I to X and sits adjacent to the of the hemisphere.
midline (Hawkes and Leclerc, 1987). That is to 6. P6 + runs throughout the hemisphere and is
say, PI + is split by the cerebellar midline as unbranched.
revealed by the olivocerebellar projection 7. P7+ also runs throughout the hemisphere and
(Gravel et aI., 1987) and no P- band separates is unbranched. There is a tendency for the
the PI + bands in left and right hemisphere. immunoreactivity to weaken in the more lateral
The PI + band is the narrowest of the P + family bands so that P7 + is often difficult to trace
and is often represented, in a given section, by with confidence.
only one or two zebrin 1+ Purkinje cells.
2. P2 + is the widest vermal band and extends Most Purkinje cells in the flocculus and para-
throughout all lobules. The band broadens flocculus are zebrin 1+. Differences in staining
gradually as it passes from anterior to post- intensity may reflect some compartmentation but
erior in the vermis. their three-dimensional organization has not been
3. P3 + probably also runs throughout the vermis reconstructed. Likewise, in some animals there is
but the immunoreactivity becomes noticeably a suggestion of an even more lateral P8 + band
weaker in the anterior lobe and it is often diffi- skirting the periphery of the hemisphere but the
cult to follow P3 + into lobules II and I. weakness of the immunoreactivity in the most
4. P4 + runs along the border between the ver- lateral part of the cerebellar cortex has made this
mis and the hemispheres and can be followed impossible to verify.
throughout the cortex in favorable cases. As The above classification of Purkinje cells into
for P3 +, it tends to fade away in the more parasagittal bands is natural in the vermis, but
anterior lobules. Typically, P4 + is split in crus in the hemispheres the matter is less evident, and
IIa and crus lIb into a major median band it may be that the distribution of the zebrin I
(P4a +) and a minor lateral sub-band (P4b +). epitope in the hemispheres is better described as
This bifurcation cannot be followed farther a series of patches. For example, considering the
anteriorly so either P4 + contains a small island PS+ -P6+ region, it may be that PS+ and P6+
of zebrin I - cells (P4a -) or, in accordance with stop at the border of crus II and the paramedian
the tendency of bands to fade toward the ante- lobule, or rather continue as a zebrin I - territory,
rior, the P4b + band extends farther but cannot and that, in terms of natural architecture, the
be identified. Because P4 + follows the vermian anterior extension of P6 + from the copula pyra-
margin in lobules IX and X, the P4 - band midis and the paramedian lobule into crus II
stops caudally in the copula pyramidis. might actually be PS -. The resolution of this
The negative bands in the vermis become question must await studies that correlate the
very narrow in lobule VI, and separate com- distribution ofthe zebrin I epitope with the hodo-
partments are often difficult to resolve. logy of the cerebellar cortex.
S. PS + is confined to the hemispheres where it A frequent source of interindividual differences
is found in all lobules. Inspection suggests an are so-called satellite bands. These are defined as
ambiguity as to the behavior of PS + in the narrow, positively immunoreactive bands of cells,
posterior cortex. Following our classification, often only one cell wide, lying adjacent to one of
2. Zebrins 29

the major bands. In contrast to bands such as that used for the rat cerebellum (Hawkes and
P4b + and P5a +, satellite bands are not constant Leclerc, 1987). Both the number and the position
from individual to individual nor are they neces- of the individual bands is highly reproducible
sarily bilaterally symmetrical. They do, however, from individual to individual. The situation in the
extend substantially in the dorsoventral plane hemispheres is more complex, due probably to
and thus are not simply occasional zebrin 1+ the elaborate lobulation. However, both zebrin
Purkinje cells isolated within zebrin I - territory. I + and zebrin 1- Purkinje cells are present, and
Analogous zebrin 1- satellite bands may also preliminary studies indicate that there are four
exist but would be much harder to detect. lateral bands of P + cells as in the rat. Both zebrin
Zebrin I in the squirrel monkey cerebellum is I + and zebrin 1- Purkinje cells are present in the
confined to the Purkinje cells as in rodents (Leclerc human cerebellum, but clear parasagittal bands
et aI., I 990a). Not all Purkinje cells are immuno- could not be identified (Plioplys et aI., 1985).
reactive, and both in the vermis and the hemi-
spheres stretches of zebrin 1+ Purkinje cells
Zebrin II
alternate with zebrin I - zones. Clusters of anti-
genic Purkinje cells are arranged in parasagittal A second zebrin was identified by a monoclonal
bands running throughout the vermis with a nar- antibody raised against the cerebellum of a weak-
row band straddling the midline (PI +) and two 1y electric fish (Apteronotus). The antigen (zebrin
others running laterally to either side (P2 + and II) is a 36-kDa polypeptide of unknown function.
P3 +). The P + bands are interposed by similar P- It is confined exclusively to a subset of the Purkinje
bands (Fig. 2.4). This numbering scheme follows cells, and has an identical distribution in the rat

Figure 2.4. A frontal section through the cerebellar both zebrin I + and zebrin I - patches are also obvious
cortex ofthe squirrel monkey, immunoperoxidase stain- in the hemispheres, their precise organization is less
ed for zebrin I. Clear zebrin I + and zebrin 1- parasa- clear.
gittal compartments are seen in the vermi5. Although
30 Richard Hawkes et al.

Figure 2.5. A frontal section through the cerebellar Purkinje cells is identical to that revealed by usin!:,
cortex of a young adult rat (P30), immunoperoxidase anti-zebrin I.
stained for zebrin II. The distribution of zebrin 11+

cerebellum to zebrin I (Fig. 2.5) (Brochu et aI., novel heterogeneity among Purkinje cells in the
1990). It differs from zebrin I in its species distri- primate cerebellum. There is a broad pattern of
bution. Whereas zebrin I immunoreactivity is parasagittal bands of Purkinje cells either B 1 + or
confined to some mammals, zebrin II is expressed B 1 -. The epitope differs from the zebrins in being
in numerous species, including fish, birds, and expressed also by various other classes of neurons,
mammals. For example, in Apteronotus the corpus including neurons in the cerebellar nuclei, the
and valvula of the cerebellum are zebrin 11+, motor cortex, and the spinal cord. A nonuniform
whereas the lobus caudalis is zebrin II -. Similarly, distribution has also been found in the rat.
zebrin II reveals parasagittal bands in all mam-
mals so far tested: a representative frontal section Synaptophysin
from the adult opossum is illustrated in Figure 2.6.
Classically, the cerebellar granular layer is con-
We have at least three other monoclonal anti-
sidered to have no architectonic or chemical
bodies that recognize subsets of Purkinje cells
boundaries, and most markers reveal no signs of
(zebrins III- V), but these remain to be character-
compartmentation. However, this appears to be
ized.
an oversimplification. For example, detailed
quantitative studies of Golgi cells in the rat have
Bl
identified significant differences in cell density
A monoclonal antibody to an undefined epitope from region to region, with most found in the
present in rat embryonic forebrain membranes posterior lobe vermis, and fewest in the anterior
(antigen Bl) (Ingram et aI., 1985) has revealed a lobe hemisphere (Lange, 1982). Furthermore, our
2. Zebrins 31

Figure 2.6. A frontal section through the cerebellum only the Purkinje cells are immunoreactive, and zeb-
of an adult opossum (M onodelphis), immunoperoxidase rin II + cells are clustered together to form a family of
stained with anti-zebrin II. As in other species tested, parasagittal compartments.

studies with a monoclonal antibody to the synap- 1990a; Rice and Hawkes, in preparation). Wheth-
tic vesicle antigen synaptophysin reveal clear er regional differences in immunoreactivity are
differences between lobules in the extent of the due to the level of antigen per vesicle, number of
infraganglionic plexus, which is prominent in vesicles per synapse, or synaptic density remains
some lobules and almost absent in others (un- uncertain.
published data). Synaptophysin immunoreactivity
also reveals parasagittal compartments.
Cytochrome Oxidase
By using an antisynaptophysin antibody
(mabQ 155), a pattern of parasagittal bands was A banded distribution of cytochrome oxidase
demonstrated in the mouse cerebellar cortex that (CO) activity is present in the monkey cerebellum
encompasses both the granular and the molecular (Hess and Voogd, 1986; Leclerc et a!., 1990b).
layers (Fig. 2.7)(Hawkes et aI., 1985; Leclerc et aI., Alternating compartments of high and low CO
1990a). Unlike zebrin I, the synaptophysin bands activity are detected in the white matter and gran-
are not as sharply delineated as those of zebrin ular layer of the vermis, but not the hemispheres.
I, and reflect quantitative levels of staining These compartments correspond to those identi-
intensity rather than all-or-none differences. fied by using AChE histochemistry. A similar
Synaptophysin is a polypeptide associated with situation pertains to the rat. Both the molecular
synaptic vesicles, and the mabQ155 epitope has and the granular layers of the rat cerebellar cortex
been demonstrated in all prominent classes of contain CO activity (Leclerc et a!., 1990b). In the
cerebellar and cortical synapse (Leclerc et aI., hemispheres, the activity is distributed uniformly,
32 Richard Hawkes et al.

in the molecular layer. The high CO patches are


joined rostrocaudally to form three parasagittal
bands in each hemivermis.

Are All Molecular


Maps Congruent?
It is crucial to establish exactly how many maps
we are dealing with in the cerebellum. Published
evidence is contentious, with several studies claim-
ing that compartment markers incompletely
overlap whereas others describe complete con-
gruence.
In the rat, we have compared the correlation
between zebrin I bands and other band markers:

1. Zebrin I bands of Purkinje cells are completely


congruent with those revealed by staining for
zebrin II. Preliminary studies suggest that this
is also the case for zebrins III through V.
2. Zebrin 1+ Purkinje cells have been compared
directly with the patches of AChE activity seen
in the granular layer. There is good general
agreement that the AChE + patches underlie
the zebrin I + compartments but the degree of
Figure 2.7. Adult rat cerebellum immunoperoxidase
precision of the match is unclear (Boegman
stained with monoclonal antisynaptophysin. A: Light
micrograph of a 50 lim thick sagittal section through et aI., 1988). In frontal sections of the cerebellar
the cerebellar cortex. Dense punctate deposits corres- vermis, AChE histochemistry reveals a patchy
ponding to the distribution of synapses are seen in the distribution of activity in the granular layer
molecular layer, surrounding the Purkinje cell bodies, and the white matter tracts that corresponds
and in the granular layer. B: Electron micrograph of a in position to the distribution of zebrin 1+/
region of the molecular layer showing labeling of sy- zebrin 1- Purkinje cell compartments. The
naptic vesicles in the presynaptic terminus of a parallel fact that AChE patches are seen only in the
fiber. No reaction product is seen in the postsynaptic vermis indicates that although they correlate
Purkinje cell dendritic spine. C: Horizontal section with zebrin I staining, they are not an obligate
through the cerebellum of an adult mouse showing a
correlate. In the hemispheres, where zebrin I
series of bands in the vermis of the cerebellar cortex.
patches are prominent, for example in crus II,
Banding is seen in both granular and molecular layers.
Scale bars = 20 ~m (A), 250 ~m (B), and 100 pm (C). the AChE activity in the granular layer is dis-
(Reprinted with permission from Hawkes et aI., 1985.) tributed uniformly. The staining pattern in
adult rats is clearly different from that reported
in young cats (Marani and Voogd, 1977;
but in the vermis differences in staining intensity Ramon-Moliner, 1972) and cannot have the
reveal alternating patches oflow and high activity same structural basis. Nevertheless, studies in
that include both granular and molecular layers. both cats and rats concur that bands and
In the molecular layer, the staining is associated patches are only seen in the vermis.
predominantly with the Purkinje cells. The den- 3. Zebrin 1+ Purkinje cell compartments in the
dritic arbors display different levels of reactivity mouse are congruent with the 5'N bands in the
to yield alternating patches. In the granular layer, molecular layer, both in the vermis and in the
the low and high activity patches align with those hemispheres (Eisenman and Hawkes, 1990).
2. Zebrins 33

4. Zebliin 1+ compartments in the vermis have More recently, a monoclonal antibody, P-


been compared directly in rat and monkey PA TH, has been developed that recognizes an
with the compartments revealed by using CO acetylated epitope expressed by a variety of cere-
histochemistry. In the rat, comparison of the bellar gangliosides. When P-PATH is used to
CO distribution with adjacent sections stained stain the rodent cerebellum, a complex pattern of
for zebrin I reveals that the CO-rich bands compartmentation is revealed in the molecular
correspond precisely to the zebrin I - compart- layer which is closely complementary to that re-
ments and the CO-weak bands to zebrin 1+ vealed by using antizebrins (Leclerc et aI., 1990c).
compartments. Thus, in the anterior lobe of P-PATH is thus the first consistent positive
the vermis, we can distinguish three narrow marker for the "zebrin-negative" Purkinje cell
immunoreactive bands, PI + on the midline compartments. However, the demarcation is not
and two more lateral bands (P2 +, P3 +) on quite that clear-cut, as it appears that some com-
each side. The CO compartmentation shares partments, most notably P3 +, are double-labeled
the same structural boundaries as the antigenic both by antizebrin and P-PATH. The details of
zonation, with high CO in Pl- and PT and the relationship are still to be determined.
low CO in PI +, P2+, and P3+. The same These data taken together suggest that there
spatial correlation holds for the posterior ver-are at least two different sorts of compartment
mis and again, high CO levels are associated markers in the adult. On the one hand, there are
with the zebrin I - compartments. The finding antigens such as zebrin 1/11, that are expressed by
that high CO activity is associated with the rata subset of one cell type, in this case Purkinje cells.
zebrin I - compartments is the first report of On the other hand, others are expressed in a more
a positive marker for this set. complex fashion. Thus, the CSADCase bands
The primate CO compartmentation shares involve not only the Purkinje cells, but also basket
the same structural boundaries as the zebrin I and stellate cells in the same compartment
zonation. However, in the squirrel monkey the (Chan-Palay et aI., 1982). In this case, it seems
CO-rich bands correspond to the zebrin 1·+ that something within the compartment environ-
compartments and conversely the CO-weak ment is conducive towards marker expression by
bands underlie the zebrin I - compartments. many cell types. The same situation also applies
Evidently, care is needed when making com- to differences in CO levels between compartments.
parisons about homologous compartments A plausible model to explain this behavior is that
between species, based on their molecular compartmentation is defined fundamentally by
pherrotype. the type of Purkinje cell (i.e., zebrin I + or zebrin
5. Hess and Voogd (1986) show that AChE and I - phenotypes): the differential expression by
CO bands correspond in the vermis of the other cell types in the compartment being secon-
macaque. dary, and dependent on local interactions. Thus,
for example, the presence of bands of 5'N activity
However, in the case of other markers, the in the mouse molecular layer seems to depend on
situation is not so straightforward. Thus, com- the Purkinje cells remaining intact (Hess and
parisons of the heterogeneity revealed by staining Hess, 1986). Alternatively, it is possible that the
for GAD, motilin, and CSADCase suggest that expression of some molecular markers is related
although they overlap, the different markers are to the dynamic metabolic state of the cell rather
not codistributed. Similarly, according to Ingram than to its permanent phenotype, a situation that
et ai. (1985), the B 1 antigen is not codistributed could presage a distinction between topographi-
with the other compartmentation marker they cally defined cerebellar compartments and func-
tested (AChE), and its distribution does not tional units.
resemble those described for motilin, GAD, or
CSADCase. However, our direct comparison of
Bland zebrin I immunoreactivities in adjacent
sections of squirrel monkey vermis showed a
complete correspondence (unpublished data).
34 Richard Hawkes et al.

The Relationship Between respect a common compartmentation: both are


Compartmentation and Connectivity arranged into parasagittally organized bands or
patches in the cerebellar cortex; the two patterns
Climbing Fibers: Complete Correspondence are reproducible; there is a high level of concor-
It is imperative to determine how the intrinsic dance between the cortical localization of transi-
chemistry of the cerebellar cortex relates to the tion zones delineating discrete olivocerebellar
innervation patterns. Anterograde tracer injec- projection territories and the location of transition
tions into the inferior olivary complex generate zones between zebrin I + and zebrin I - cortical
discrete bands of climbing fiber (CF) terminals in regions (Fig. 2.8) (Gravel et aI., 1987). It remains
the cerebellum. This occurs because adjacent to be determined whether every cluster of zebrin
regions in the inferior olives do not necessarily I + and zebrin 1- Purkinje cells receives afferent
project to adjacent regions of the cerebellar input from an individual, specific subgroup of
cortex. (For example, projections to the uvula olivary cells.
and pyramis of rat show that contiguous para-
sagittal bands in the cerebellar cortex can receive
Mossy Fibers: Incomplete Correspondence
afferents from noncontiguous groups of olivary
cells (Eisenman, 1981, 1984; Eisenman and We have concentrated on low thoracic- high lum-
Goracchi, 1983). bar (LTHL) spinocerebellar projections to com-
Direct comparison shows that CF compart- pare mossy fiber (MF) terminal fields to zebrin I
ments and the expression of zebrin I seem to compartments in the rat (Gravel and Hawkes,

Figure 2.8. A comparison of


bands of climbing fibers (A)
with bands of zebrin I in
lobule VIII of the posterior
vermis (8) in the rat as seen
in serial horizontal sections
100 f.lm apart. Labeled CF
terminate both on zebrin 1+
and zebrin 1- Purkinje cells
(A). Contralateral to the
injection site (left), the P2 +
zebrin I + band receives a
discrete WGA-HRP labeled
CF input due to tracer
diffusion across the midline:
the olivocerebellar
projection in the adult rat
is purely ipsilateral. The
arrows in A and 8 point to
the same blood vessel.
Rostral is toward the top
and the midline is indicated
by a dotted line. Scale
bar = 200 f.lm. (Adapted with
permission from Gravel
et aI., 1987. Copyright 1987
by Wiley-Liss, a Division of
John Wiley & Sons, Inc.)
2. Zebrins 35

1990). Several studies in mammals have shown the anterior and posterior lobes, the medial group
that the target area for spinocerebellar terminals ofterminals appears divided in two at the midline,
comprises the anterior lobe (mainly lobules II - V) particularly in lobules V and VIII. This may be
and lobule VIII of the posterior lobe and that because this group consists of two contiguous
within the receiving areas the rosettes are clus- longitudinal bands directly adjacent to the mid-
tered into discrete terminal fields that run sagit- line. The observation that the central area of
tally in the granular layer ofthe vermal and inter- termination for LTHL terminals is split by the
mediate cortex (Arsenio-Nunes and Sotelo, 1985; midline would also be consistent with the my-
Hazlett et ai., 1971; Robertson et ai., 1983; Voogd, eloarchitectonic compartmentation in the cere-
1964, 1969; Yaginuma and Matsushita, 1986). In bellum of the mouse (Marani, 1982a), rat (Voogd
the vermis of the anterior lobe, the number of et ai., 1985), and other mammals (Voogd, 1969),
sagittal L THL terminal fields per hemicerebellum and also with the organization of corticonu-
goes from three to four as they extend caudally clear projections (Armstrong and Schild, 1978;
from lobule II to lobules III, IV, and V. In both Goodman et ai., 1963), olivocerebellar projections

P3 P2 P1 P2 P3
2.0
~ c:J

Figure 2.9. A comparison of Pur-


kinje cell compartments (darkly
shaded rectangles) and mossy fiber 1.5
terminal fields (pale rectangles), after

1
a WGA-HRP anterograde tracer
injection into the LTHL of the spi-
nal cord. Alternate sections were
1.0
processed for anterograde transport
ofWGA-HRP and zebrin I immu-
noreactivity. The vermis of lobule
d
II has been schematically unfolded,
flattened, and viewed from dorsal. 0.5
The region at the tip of the lobule
(around coordinate 0) was not re-
constructed. The dorsal (d) and
ventral (v) aspects are indicated, and o
the scales are in mm, either from
the rostral extent of the cerebellum
(vertical) or from the midline (hori-
zontal). The PI + - P3 + Purkinje cell
0.5
compartments are clear both dor-
sally and ventrally. LTHL terminal

1
fields are differently disposed in the
two faces. For example, dorsally,
there is a parasagittal terminal field 1.0
running beneath P2 +, whereas
ventrally it is displaced medially into
Pl-. Similarly, the LTHL compart-
ment beneath P3 + dorsally, is locat- 1.5 v
ed beneath P2 - ventrally. This has
the effect of subdividing the P + jP-
parasagittal compartments into
short patches with changes in MF 2.0
innervation patterns occurring at L-
____- L______ ~L- ___ --J
----.J
the tip of the lobule. -1.0 0.5 o 0.5 1.0
36 Richard Hawkes et al.

(Campbell and Armstrong, 1983a, b), and zebrin how the ostensibly uniform cerebellar cortex
I territories (Gravel et aI., 1987). In the posterior might encode complex somatotopic maps. The
lobe the two more medial MF clusters on each further subdivision of zebrin compartments by
side are clearly separated by a midline gap, as is MF terminal fields increases the resolution of the
the case in the dorsal face of sublobule VIlla in topography to a point such that anatomical com-
the cat (Yaginuma and Matsushita, 1987). partment widths become compatible with the
A crucial question is, do the MF terminal fields widths of physiological compartments in the
respect the Purkinje cell (PC) compartments? In somatotopic map.
a general sense, since both PC and MF are orga- How longitudinal bands are subdivided to give
nized longitudinally there is inevitably some the patchy mosaic characteristic of the cerebellar
measure of correspondence. However, unlike the somatotopy remains unclear. The obvious means
olivocerebellar projection (OCP), where there is by which longitudinal compartments are sub-
a direct synaptic interaction and precise compart- divided mediolaterally is the natural cerebellar
ment alignment (Gravel et aI., 1987), the mossy foliation. Electrophysiological studies of somato-
fiber-Purkinje cell (MF-PC) interaction is in- sensory projections to the cerebellum have shown
direct (via granule cells), and may be architectu- that each folium and folial complex receives
rally less precise. The problem is compounded inputs from mixtures of peripheral sources and
further by the interlobular and intralobular dif- submodal types, suggesting that each might be a
ferences and by uncertainty as to the trajectories unique functional unit (Joseph et aI., 1978; Kassel
of the granule cell axons. et aI., 1984;Shambeset aI., 1978a, b; Welker et al.,
The limits of the LTHL terminal fields corre- 1987; Welker and Shambes, 1985). For example,
late well with the boundaries of some, but not all, in lobule VIII of Galago, sublobules a, b, and c
zebrin I compartments. For example, in the have receptive fields for the lower neck-upper
anterior lobe the medial LTHL terminal field back region, whereas sublobule d has middle-
occupies the granular layer beneath both PI + back representations (Welker et aI., 1988). The
and the medial third of PI - and so apparently cerebellar lobulation provides a natural means
does not respect the PI +IPI - interface. Likewise, by which afferent inputs can be directed to one
the first lateral pair of LTHL terminal fields sub- or another cortical territory: at the base Qf each
stantially and consistently extend beyond P2 + sulcus, the white matter tracts must bifurcate.
medially into the lateral half of PI - but never Hence, there is a natural transition at the base of
extends across the PI +IP2 - interface laterally, each sulcus by which the granule cells on one side
although in most cases it does abut against either are driven by the MF ventrally and those on the
its lateral or medial edge (e.g., lobule II dorsal) other side by the MF located dorsally. However,
(Fig. 2.9). The organization of LTHL terminal most somatotopic boundaries are not at the base
fields in the anterior lobe results in the gross of the sulci, and for these other mechanisms must
partitioning of the granular layer under PI - into besought.
a central region devoid of labeled terminals, and The MF terminal fields do not only subdivide
medial and lateral receiving areas to either side Purkinje cell compartments sagittally, they also
(except for the ventral part oflobule II where the divide them mediolaterally. An example from
reverse is the case-see below). This is clearly dif- lobule II is shown in Fig. 2.9. Anterograde tracer
ferent from the olivocerebellar projection where has been injected into the lower thoracic-higher
a precise alignment of boundaries was consistently lumbar spinal cord of the rat, and the labeled
found, and zebrin I compartments are never sub- spinocerebellar axons mapped in the cerebellar
divided by afferent terminal fields (Gravel et aI., vermis. In the dorsal surface of lobule II the
1987). LTHL terminal fields are centered more or less
beneath the P + compartments; in contrast, on the
M ediolateral Compartmentation due ventral face they lie in P- compartments, with
to M F Terminal Fields the exception of the midline, where MF and P+
The identification of parasagittal bands of affer- compartments are always congruent. In the
ents and Purkinje cells serves in parr to explain posterior lobe, the LTHL terminal fields in lobule
2. Zebrins 37

Figure 2.10. Anti-zebrin I labels the caudal portion of anti-zebrin 1+ synaptic boutons are seen. The target
the fastigial nuclei preferentially. Three horizontal sec- neurons themselves are not immunoreactive. Scale
tions (A, B, C) through a fastigial nucleus in the rat bar = 200 11m (A-C) and 20 11m (D). (Adapted with per-
immunoperoxidase stained by using anti-zebrin I to mission from Hawkes and Leclerc, 1986. Copyright
show the rostrocaudal gradient of immunoreactivity. 1986 by Wiley-Liss, a Division of John Wiley & Sons,
The dorsolateral protuberance (dp) is always uniformly Inc.)
positive. At higher magnifications (D), the individual
38 Richard Hawkes et al.

VIII show the absence of labeled glomeruli Are There Higher Levels
beneath the P2 + compartment on the dorsal sur- of Organization than
face. As a result, the innervation of the same
compartment can clearly be different between
the Compartment?
dorsal and ventral faces of a lobule. Together with
the natural separation that occurs at the depths
Purkinje cell axons have two modes of termina-
of the sulci, the MF distribution allows parasagit-
tion within the cerebellum. They are the sole
tal compartments to be subdivided mediolater-
efferent projection from the cerebellar cortex to
ally into patches with different patterns of afferent
the deep nuclei, but they also participate in corti-
innervation.
cal local circuits by means of recurrent collaterals
In theory, the differences between the dorsal
that arborize within the cerebellar cortex. There
and ventral aspects of lobule II could be due to
is reason to think that the mediolateral extent of
the differential cortical growth. If the ventral
Purkinje cell axon collaterals is influenced by the
cortex expanded more than the dorsal, and this
parasagittal zonation of the cerebellar cortex. In
expansion did not affect the MF terminal posi-
rodents (Chan-Palay, 1971; O'Leary et aI., 1968;
tions, the result would be as is actually found, the
Palay and Chan-Palay, 1974) and in the cat
relative displacement of the MF terminal fields
(Bishop, 1982) the recurrent axon collaterals of
medially from the p+ to the P- compartments
Purkinje cells spread extensively in the parasagit-
(i.e., from P2 + to PI - and from P3 + to P2 -).
tal plane but have only limited longitudinal spread
However, this is probably not the case. In fact,
within the folium. Bishop (1982) has noted for the
the spacing of the PC compartments does not
cat that the lateral extent of the collateral plexus
differ between dorsal and ventral, as would be
is similar to the dimensions of the sagittal zones
predicted by the differential expansion hypothe-
of the cerebellar cortex as defined by the topo-
sis, but rather it is the MF terminal fields that
graphy of the afferent axon terminals. Given that
come closer together on the ventral face.
neighboring Purkinje cells are a frequent synaptic
target for axon collateral terminals (Chan-Palay,
Corticonuclear Projection
1971), it has been suggested that Purkinje cell
In addition to the cerebellar afferents, the projec- collaterals may define zonal boundaries by func-
tion of the Purkinje cells to the cerebellar nuclei tionally linking topographically related Purkinje
and lateral vestibular nuclei is organized into cells (Bishop, 1982).
parasagittal compartments (reviewed in Haines The strict demarcation of vermal zebrin I + 11-
et aI., 1982), and these compartments correspond para sagittal zones seen at the level of the somata
to those in the olivocerebellar projection. The and dendritic trees is only partly maintained by
topography ofthe corticonuclear projection with the recurrent axon collaterals (Hawkes and Leclerc,
respect to Purkinje cell compartmentation is 1989). Collaterals invade into neighboring terri-
currently under investigation. However, immuno- tory by up to five Purkinje cell widths in the'
cytochemical studies of the fastigial nucleus with anterior lobe, thereby innervating 20% or so of the
anti-zebrin I reveal the presence of discrete ter- width of the P- bands. In the posterior lobe of
minal fields for zebrin I + and zebrin 1- Purkinje the vermis all the P - bands are occupied (Fig. 2.11).
cells (Fig.2.10) (Hawkes and Leclerc, 1986). Hence, there is no simple segregation of collaterals
Because zebrin I is found in the axons and axon within their antigenically homologous bands. On
terminals of zebrin 1+ Pu'rkinje cells, individual the other hand, the density of stained collateral
synaptic boutons are readily identifiable in the profiles does drop dramatically at the P + IP-
cerebellar nuclei (Fig. 2.1OD). For example, zeb- boundaries. If we assume that the density of the
rin I immunocytochemical studies of the projec- zebrin 1+ collaterals behaves in similar fashion,
tion to the ipsilateral fastigial (medial) nucleus then the infraganglionic plexus of lobule IV
have shown that the PI + Purkinje cells terminate changes from more than 90% zebrin 1+ beneath
preferentially in the caudal pole and the Pl- a zebrin 1+ Purkinje cell soma at a p+ IP- bound-
Purkinje cells terminate in the rostral pole (Fig. ary to less than 10% zebrin I + at the neighboring
2.10) (Hawkes and Leclerc, 1986). zebrin I - cell.
2. Zebrins 39

Figure 2.11. Two examples of recurrent collaterals In both examples, immunoreactive collaterals extend
(arrowheads) of zebrin 1+ Purkinje cell axon in lobule deep into the zebrin I - territory of P2 - . Scale bar =
IX of the vermis in the rat (the P2 - compartment, with 50/!m. (Reprinted with permission from Hawkes and
P2 + medially to the left and P3 + laterally to the right). Leclerc, 1989.)
40 Richard Hawkes et al.

The functional significance of the recurrent The Cerebellar Module


axon collaterals of Purkinje cells is obscure. It is
evident that the physiological role of such recur- The above considerations suggest that a modular
rent axon collateral in the integrated action of the description ofthe cerebellum may be appropriate
cerebellum will depend on the target neuron it (Fig. 2.12). There are about 280,000 Purkinje cells
inhibits. The major target for recurrent axon col- in the adult rat cerebellum (Hillman and Chen,
laterals seems to be other Purkinje cells and thus, 1981). If we take the number of para sagittal com-
their role outside the band of origin might be to partments as 32 (as defined by using zebrin im-
inhibit firing in the neighboring medial compart- munocytochemistry: this is an underestimate, in
ment. This could serve to sharpen the physiologi- part because the molecular complexity may be
cal boundaries and limit the spread ofMF- granule higher, and in part because the MF terminal fields
cell activation via the parallel fibers. However, split antigenic compartments), and the number of
several other targets have been identified for the lobules dividing the vermis and hemispheres
recurrent collaterals, including basket cells, Golgi mediolaterally as, on average, 10 (lobules are
cells, and Lugaro cells. With this in mind, it is important because they represent opportunities
clear that the consequences of collateral input will for axon tract bifurcation and thereby for the
depend on which targets are implicated. Taken differential distribution of afferents to different
together with the observation that Golgi cells modules), then we obtain some 320 compart-
receiving Purkinje cell collateral input are much ments per cerebellum. Each subcompartment may
more frequent in the hemispheres than in the be further split by differences in MF terminal
vermis, this suggests the possibility that collaterals
play different physiological roles in different re-
gions of the cerebellum, perhaps even in different aUerents

11
bands.
The observation that zebrin 1+ collaterals from
P2 + oflobule IV preferentially invade P - terri-
tory medially (P 1 -) over laterally (pr), and thus
differentiate medial from lateral band boundaries,
may reflect a still higher level of cerebellar func-
tional organization than the zebrin I compart- cerebellar
COl1eo
ment. On the other hand, the explanation of the
polarized collateral distribution may be embryo-
logical rather than functional. During cerebellar

1
development, Purkinje cells develop earliest in
the vermis, thus creating a mediolateral gradient
of maturity in Purkinje cells ofthe neonate. Thus,
when synapses are formed there could be an excess
of mature targets medially, biasing the mature
cerebellar
nuclei
1m]

1
collateral distribution. This hypothesis cannot be euerents
tested directly as bands of zebrin 1+ Purkinje cells
are not apparent until 15 to 20 days postnatal
(Leclerc et aI., 1988), at which stage the collateral Figure 2.12. The cerebellar module. Left: The rat hemi-
distribution resembles that in the adult. The only cerebellum is schematically drawn. There are eight
zebrin I + and eight zebrin I - para sagittal compart-
previous indication that medial and lateral boun-
ments. In this model, each compartment is divided
daries of parasagittal bands may be different
mediolaterally by natural foliation and by shifts in the
comes from studies of the S'N distribution in MF terminal fields to yield 320 individual modules.
mice that showed that the sagittal bands have Right: One such module is expanded: it comprises
sharp lateral but blurred medial borders (Marani, some 400 individual Purkinje cells that project onto
1982a). 20 target neurons in the terminal field of the cerebellar
nuclei (and lateral vestibular nuclei).
2. Zebrins 41

fields from dorsal to ventral faces, to yield some bellum of the monkey has evidently undergone a
600-odd discrete modules, each averaging 400 to massive expansion in surface area but we see no
500 Purkinje cells. The efferent output from each evidence of large numbers of new alternating
module is via the corticonuclear or corticovesti- zebrin I + and zebrin I - compartments. Rather,
bular projections. Both these pathways are highly the individual zebrin 1+ compartments seem to
convergent. The precise Purkinje cell/nuclear cell have increased in width such that very long conti-
ratio in rat is not known, but taking it to be 26, nuous stretches of zebrin 1+ Purkinje cells are
as in the cat (Palkovits et aI., 1977), then each seen compared to the rat. This idea receives
cerebellar cortex module would project to some support from comparison of the corticonuclear
20 to 40 cells in the cerebellar nuclei. The degree zonation of primates and rodents. In both the rat
of overlap between corticonuclear terminal fields and the squirrel monkey, the vermal A and B zones
is unknown. are both narrow, but in the para vermis and hemi-
The diversity of afferent sources to the cerebellar spheres squirrel monkey of the squirrel monkey,
cortex is easily sufficient to assure that each such there is a significant relative broadening of the
module would receive a unique pattern of inner- Cl, C2, C3, and D zones (Haines et aI., 1982).
vation. Furthermore, even if modules received Although no detailed correlation has been made
identical inputs they might well terminate dif- between corticonuclear zones and antigenic com-
ferently, either overlapping in different combina- partments, this is obviously consistent with what
tions to generate unique composite terminal fields we see here with zebrin I, with relatively narrow
or terminating uniquely on terminal fields with vermal bands in both species and almost all the
different patterns of extracerebellar connectivity. relative expansion in the monkey cerebellar cor-
tex concentrated in the zebrin 1+ compartments
Species Variation Suggests of the paravermis· and hemispheres.
that Compartment Number
Is Conserved Across Phylogeny Events Leading to the Establishment
Cerebellar antigenic compartmentation can be of Cerebellar Connectivity
demonstrated in species as phylogenetically The Development of Intracerebellar
separate as fish and primate. Even within the
Compartments: Perinatal Markers
mammals, the cerebellum of the squirrel monkey
contains many-fold more Purkinje cells than there A selection of immunological markers are now
are in the rat, and the human cerebellum is available to monitor the maturation of Purkinje
that much larger again. How is this expansion cells in rodents. The earliest antigen to be expres-
achieved? Two possibilities can be contrasted: sed is cyclic guanosine 3': 5' phosphate-dependent
either the cortical surface increases by the addition protein kinase (cPK) that appears for the first
of new compartments or it is through the expan- time at embryo day 17 in a subset of Purkinje
sion in volume of a constant set of compartments. cells (Wassef and Sotelo, 1984) (the Purkinje cell
The former mechanism seems to be employed for terminal mitoses are between embryo days 12 and
example in the frontal cortex and striatum, where 15: Altman, 1972). Differential expression by a
modular columns are of roughly similar sizes in subset of Purkinje cells is continued until birth.
many species and expansion occurs by the addi- In the first 5 days postnatally antigen expression
tion offurther modules. However, comparison of gradually extends to include all Purkinje cells,
rat and monkey cerebellar cortices stained for and differential expression is not seen in the adult.
zebrin I suggests that cerebellar expansion occurs Several other perinatal compartment antigens
not by the addition of new compartments but by have also been described. Antisera to vitamin D-
the expansion of the existing ones. At least in the dependent calcium-binding protein (CaBP) and
vermis, the zebrin I compartmentation is rather Purkinje cell-specific glycoprotein (PSG) both
similar in the two species, with clear PI +, P2 +, selectively recognize transient perinatal Purkinje
and P3 + bands identified in both, each set with cell populations (Wassef et aI., 1985). As was the
similar relative sizes. In the hemisphere, the cere- case for cPK expression, the heterogeneity seems
42 Richard Hawkes et al.

A
2. Zebrins 43

to reflect timing differences in the onset of expres- the anterior lobe vermis (Fig. 2.13C) and the hem-
sion, and disappears during the first few postnatal ispheres are unreactive. Expression radiates both
days. anteriorly and laterally so that, about 5 days
after the onset, all Purkinje cells are zebrin 1+
(Fig. 2.13E, F, G). The spread of zebrin expression
Zebrin J/JJ
is not smooth, but rather seems to involve several
Both zebrin I and zebrin II follow an identical clearly defined stages, with sharply defined medio-
developmental timetable in the rat, and can be lateral and rostrocaudal boundaries: for example,
discussed together (zebrins 111- V have not yet the first signs of expression during development,
been investigated). We have identified three stages in the most caudal lobules, stop abruptly midway
in the expression ofthe zebrin I epitope, an initial along lobule VIII. The transient banding pattern
phase in which all Purkinje cells are zebrin 1-, an has disappeared by P12, but differences in stain-
intermediate stage in which all become zebrin 1+, ing intensity indicate the positions of the future
and the final, adult stage of selective expression p+ IP- compartments. The relationship between
by parasagittal bands of cells (Leclerc et aI., 1988). the early and mature compartmentation is un-
The late expression of zebrin I during corticoge- clear. The factors that control the induction and
nesis precludes it from playing a direct role in the spread of zebrin I expression in the Purkinje cell
initial encoding of positional information since population are unknown.
some of the para sagittal zonation ofthe cerebellar In the opossum, the expression of zebrin II
cortex is already established at birth (e.g., Sotelo during development closely resembles that in the
et aI., 1984). A similar developmental profile is rat. M onodelphis is born after only 14 days in
not observed in other brain regions where zebrin utero, and remains in the maternal pouch for
I - immunoreactivity is primarily glial (Plioplys several weeks thereafter. When rat and opossum
and Hawkes, 1986, 1988), and differential staining zebrin II expression timetables are compared, the
is present from the earliest time of expression correspondence is remarkable: the first immuno-
(e.g., the habenular complex: Plioplys and Hawkes, reactivity appears at P20 (equivalent to rat P8),
1987). uniform expression by all Purkinje cells is seen at
Zebrin 1/11 induction begins at the median P28, and zebrin II + III - bands become obvious
posterior vermis at P6. By P7, clusters of im- at about P35. The final maturation of the band
munoreactive Purkinje cells are clearly evident in array appears to be more protracted than in rat,
the posterior lobe vermis (Fig. 2.l3A, B, D), but and the adult appearance is only achieved after

~,-----------------------------------------------------------------
Figure 2.13. The distribution of zebrin I immunore- VIII at P7 to emphasize the presence of a substructure
activity in the rat cerebellar cortex at P7 and P12 seen within the major Purkinje cell compartments. E: At
in horizontal sections. A: Specific Purkinje cell staining P12 the axons can be reliably followed through the
in the vermis oflobule VIII at P7. The immunoreactive internal granular layer (arrowheads). At the midline
cells in this section are organized into three major (dotted line), the corticonuclear projection appears to
clusters and in each case, there is a suggestion of a be exclusively ipsilateral as in the adult, and we never
subcompartmentation within the clusters. B: The poste- see immunoreactive axon profiles projecting to the
rior lobe vermis at P7 contains numerous clusters of contralateral cerebellum. F: Although all Purkinje cells
zebrin I + and zebrin 1- Purkinje cells. The reaction are zebrin I + at P12, in the vermis the putative nascent
product in the white matter is a mixture of specific bands of Purkinje cells that will become zebrin I + in
Purkinje cell axon immunoreactivity and nonspecific the adult are distinguished by their higher levels of
staining. C: In the anterior lobe vermis there is still immunoreactivity (arrowheads). G: Nascent bands are
no specific immunoreactivity at P7. Some nonspecific also apparent in the hemispheres at P12. Shown here
staining, due to the high anti-zebrin I levels used, is is the putative P6 + band (arrowheads) in the immature
seen in the immature granular layer (IGL) and the crus II. Scale bars = 300 /lm (A, B, F) and 100 /lm (C, D,
Bergman glial cell bodies of the Purkinje cell layer E, G). (Modified with permission from Leclerc et a!.,
(PcL). No reaction product is deposited in the external 1988. Copyright 1988 by Wiley-Liss, a Division of
granular layer (EGL). D: A higher power view oflobule John Wiley & Sons, Inc.)
44 Richard Hawkes et al.

P56 (Dore et aI., 1990). In the chicken, the dev- 1971; Puro and Woodward, 1977). A test for this
elopmental timetable has not yet been fully deter- is to lesion selected afferent inputs and then look
mined, but clear zebrin 11+111 - compartments for alterations in the pattern ofzebrin I com part-
are seen in the vermis by 7 days posthatching mentation. Lesion experiments do not support a
(Brochu and Howkes, unpublished observations). role for afferent input in the regulation of the
There remain obvious gaps in our knowledge Purkinje cell zebrin I phenotype. No gross changes
concerning the timetable of Purkinje cell bio- have ever been observed in the adult cerebellar
chemical compartmentation in the rat. Three bands as a result of selective lesioning in the adult.
stages have been identified: perinatal (e.g., cPK Thus, the number of bands, their relative sizes,
immunoreactivity: Wassef and Sotelo, 1984; and their relative positions were unaffected, except
Wassef et aI., 1985), the early zebrin 1+ Izebrin 1- where abnormal lobulation made the latter un-
clusters in the posterior lobe vermis (P6-P9), and avoidable. The range oflesions was not exhaustive,
the mature display of bands and patches (from but we have eliminated CF bilaterally and selec-
PIS). How do these three relate to one another? tive MF bilaterally and unilaterally. Likewise,
Unfortunately, the three patterns never coexist: no changes in the parasagittal zonation of the
the early clusters revealed by using anti-zebrin I cerebellum were found after the elimination of
do not appear until the cPK is distributed uni- hindlimb input bilaterally and vibrissal input
formly, and then they are swamped by the phase unilaterally. This was true both chronically (after
of global zebrin I expression at around P12. A 30 days) and acutely (after 3 days). Hence, there
direct correlation could be made via the topo- is no evidence that tonic afferent input, either
graphy of the olivocerebellar projection or by MF or CF, is required to maintain the selective
using CO histochemistry, but this has not been expression of the zebrin I epitope.
done and it is not even certain that such a correla- It is perhaps not surprising that afferent input
tion is possible. Previous studies of chemical com- does not regulate the zebrin I phenotype of com-
partmentation during perinatal development mitted, differentiated Purkinje cells. It is more
concluded that multiple overlapping families of important that the same holds for lesions in the
Purkinje cells were present, and all markers do afferent cerebellar projections of the newborn rat.
not respect common boundaries. Unilateral olivary lesions, either electrolytically
or by pedunculectomy, unilateral lesion of the
trigeminal and gigantocellular nuclei, bilateral
Afferent Input Does not Regulate
sectioning of the dorsal and ventral spinocerebel-
Zebrin I Expression
lar tracts, and unilateral lesion of the infraorbital
Given the close interrelationships between afferent nerve did not grossly modify the adult zebrin I
compartments and intrinsic cerebellar cortical band distribution. In the case of the vibrissal
markers (above), it is tempting to speculate that input, similar lesions have been shown to result
these are not independent. For example, a topo- in a reorganization of thalamocortical afferents
graphically ordered afferent input, such as the (Killackey and Belford, 1980) and abolition of the
one from the inferior olive, could take control of whisker barrel fields in the mouse somatosensory
the differentiation pathway of the homogeneous cortex (Van der Loos and Woolsey, 1973); thus,
target Purkinje cell field. This is superficially plau- some cortices can respond anatomically to such
sible for three reasons: a) there is a strong correla- peripheral lesions. The general conclusion is clear:
tion between the positions of the cortical bands there is no evidence of plasticity in the cerebellar
in the adult olivocerebellar projection and the map comparable with that in the somatosensory
antigenic bands revealed by anti-zebrin I (Gravel cortex. Furthermore, neonatal lesions had no effect
et aI., 1987), b) the maturation ofthe CF projection on the timing or pattern of the initial induction
parallels the development of differential zebrin I of zebrin I during development.
expression (Crepel, 1971, 1982; Crepel et aI., 1976; Lesion studies in the neonate preclude a role
Mariani and Changeux, 1981), and c) the CF com- for afferent inputs in the regulation of zebrin
partments are present in the white matter before expression postnatally, but a prenatal role in com-
synaptic contact with the Purkinje cells (Crepel, mitment was still possible. To explore this, cere-
2. Zebrins 45

bellar anlagen were dissected from embryos at nates during a narrow developmental window
E12 through E15, that is, before any contact with that opens at 7 weeks and closes by 16 weeks
afferents, and transplanted ectopically into adult postnatally. In the rat, the maturation of the
hosts (Wassef et ai., 1990). In the first series of AChE patches and the bands of zebrin 1+ Izebrin
experiments the grafts were placed into the an- 1- Purkinje cells occurs at much the same time
terior chamber of the eye, and in the second series (Boegman et ai., 1988). In both cases, staining in
into cavities prepared in the neocortex. Grafts the vermis appears in the second postnatal week
were allowed to mature and then were immuno- but, whereas zebrin I immunoreactivity is uni-
peroxidase stained for zebrin I immunoreactivity. formly distributed by P12 and forms bands only
Zebrin I was expressed by grafted Purkinje cells thereafter (Leclerc et ai., 1988), the AChE staining
in cortico and in oculo. Double-labeling experi- of the granular layer is patchy from the first. The
ments confirm that both the zebrin + and the crudely similar postnatal developmental timetables
zebrin - phenotypes are present. Therefore, it ofzebrin I and AChE cannot be taken as evidence
seems probable that afferent input does not play of a close causal link between the two because
a role in the determination of the zebrin phenotype during this period both the afferents and the cere-
of Purkinje cells. bellar neurons and glia undergo major differen-
If afferent regulation is not the source ofPurkinje tiation and adopt their mature appearances.
cell compartmentation, then where does it come Furthermore, both the AChE and the zebrin I
from? One possibility is that bands of zebrin 1+ compartments develop relatively late in cerebel-
and zebrin 1- Purkinje cells arise by clonal ex- lar maturation, and follow well after both Purkinje
pansion from a small number of committed pre- cell and CF compartments appear to be estab-
cursor cells. For example in chimeric mice, a lished (Sotelo et ai., 1984; Wassef and Sotelo, 1984).
fine-grained mosaicism of nonrandom clonal It is interesting that the patchy distribution asso-
compartments has been described in the mature ciated with the granular layer and the white matter
cerebellar cortex (Oster-Granite and Gearhart, is apparent before the formation of synaptic glo-
1982). How these clonal compartments are re- meruli. Given the location of the AChE activity,
lated to the zebrin I compartmentation is still a more likely temporal correlation can be made
unknown, but this line of reasoning could explain with the development offunctional MF input and
the similarities in compartmentation between the maturation of the granular layer. The MF
different cerebellums from chick to primate. If all axons in the cerebellum terminate on granule cell
Purkinje cells arise from a small number of early dendrites in complex synaptic glomeruli that
precursors, each already committed to (but not are generated primarily between PI 0 and P20,
yet expressing) its zebrin I phenotype, then such but the MF growth cones are already present in
a ground plan might well be very stable through- the cerebellum at birth (e.g., Arsenio-Nunes and
out evolution: early embryonic patterns of develop- Sotelo, 1985; Mason and Gregory, 1984) and wait
ment are difficult to change as their ramifications on the arrival of granule cells that migrate from
for later development tend to be wide, and hence their germinal external granular layer. Although
the consequences of change tend to be fatai. How- adult AChE staining is associated with the glo-
ever, differences in the extent of clonal expansion meruli, there is no activity in the external granular
can be accommodated more easily, and would layer where the granule cells are formed, further
result in different species having a common set of suggesting a primary role for the MF afferents in
cerebellar compartments, but of widely differing the creation of AChE patches.
sIzes.
Cytochrome Oxidase
Acetylcholinesterase
In the rat, CO patches are already present at
There are few other studies of chemical band deve- birth. Because zebrin Illl are not expressed before
lopment. As regards AChE, Marani and Voogd P6, a direct comparison between CO and zebrin
(1977) have noted that in cats, bands of elevated I is not possible perinatally. However, from the
activity in the molecular layer are present in neo- general disposition of the clusters, the relationship
46 Richard Hawkes et al.

between zebrin I and CO phenotypes does not


appear to change during maturation. Because
CO can be used as a compartment marker from
.
~ ~.L-'-~.- -'~c. ,Lt
::
t, :
---,I cc

birth, it should be possible to use it in conjunction


1 1 [j 1CN
with zebrin I and/or cPK immunoreactivity to
correlate prenatal with postnatal compartmenta-
tion.

Matching Connections 1
to Compartments
Both the Purkinje cell compartments (Wassef and
Sotelo, 1984; Wassef et aI., 1985) and at least
II
rudimentary MF compartments (Arsenio-Nunes
and Sotelo, 1985) are present in the cerebellar
white matter at birth, before the onset of granule
cell migration, and hence before normal synapto- Figure 2.14. A schematic drawing to contrast two mo-
genesis. The natural conclusion is that nonsynap- dels by which olivocerebellar topography might be
tic interactions in the axon tracts are responsible attained. The adult situation is shown in the upper
box. Purkinje cell modules in the cerebellar cortex
for at least the gross features of axon compart-
(CC) terminate in precise target zones within the cere-
mentation. This is reinforced by the extensive bellar nuclei (CN) and lateral vestibular nuclei. Afferents
studies of myeloan;hitecture (Marani, 1982; Voogd, from discrete regions in the inferior olivary complex
1964, 1969; Voogd et aI., 1985) showing the pre- (IOC) terminate topographically both on the Purkinje
sence of alternating, parasagittal axonal compart- cell compartment and on the CN terminal field. Reci-
ments in the mature white matter that presumably procal connections from the CN are also probably
reflect the migratory histories of the afferent and present. This topography might be achieved in one of
efferent growth cones. two ways. In the first (I), olivocerebellar afferents contact
specific Purkinje cell axons and then send branches to
the cerebellar cortex and the cerebellar nuclei. In the
Climbing Fibers second (II), both Purkinje cell and olivocerebellar sub-
Although compartments are seen in the olivocere- sets terminate independently in the nuclear target zone,
bellar projection from birth and thus predate the and then the climbing fibers follow the Purkinje cell
axons into the cerebellar cortex. The difference bet-
zebrin compartments, the evidence suggests no
ween the two models lies in the locus of the positional
dependence of cortical compartmentation on information: in I, it is in the Purkinje cells; in II, it lies
afferent input either in the adult or during post- in the cerebellar nuclei.
natal development. Rather, it seems that the
maturation ofthe cerebellum requires the precise
matching of topographically organized afferent jection at birth (Crepel, 1982; Sotelo et aI., 1984),
inputs to predefined cerebellar modules. At what several days before the first CF synapses are de-
locus is the matching achieved? Two models can tected (Crepel, 1971; Puro and Woodward, 1977).
be suggested (Fig. 2.14). In the first (e.g., Sotelo, This model is a plausible one, and is consistent
1987), matching occurs directly, by afferent growth with long-established views that the Purkinje cell
cones recognizing markers expressed on the is the prime organizer of cerebellar morphology.
Purkinje cells (Fig. 2.141). Once the appropriate An alternative model is that matching occurs
axon fascicle is identified CF branches run both indirectly because both the Purkinje cells and the
to the cerebellar cortex and to the corticonuclear afferents recognize a common third party (Fig.
target territory. For the CF zonation, recognition 2.1411). One candidate to match cerebellar cortex
would ha ve to be through nonsynaptic mechanisms afferent and efferent topographies are the cere-
since it is clear that bands reminiscent of those in bellar nuclei (Boegman et aI., 1988). There is some
the adult are present in the olivocerebellar pro- evidence that the olivonuclear projection is topo-
2. Zebrins 47

graphically ordered such that an olivary neuron cerebellum and the CF input to the cerebellar
contacts both a Purkinje cell and that cell's target cortex as a secondary collateral path. Such a view
field within the cerebellar nuclei. Thus, Purkinje is consistent with physiological interpretations of
cell efferents and olivary afferents could be brought cerebellar organization, and suggests an alter-
together because they share a common target native view of cerebellar afferent development.
territory in the cerebellar nuclei. Subsequently, The normal description of olivocerebellar organi-
the olivary input to the nucleus could branch to zation is that CF efferents from the inferior olivary
form the much larger projection to the cerebellar complex project topographically to the Purkinje
cortex, perhaps by fasciculation along the Purkinje cell layer and send collateral branches to appro-
cell axons. This view would reverse the usual priate targets in the cerebellar nuclei. This may
nomenclature by treating the olivonuclear pro- have introduced hidden semantic bias into our
jection as the primary afferent pathway to the thinking about cerebellar development. The olivo-

Figure 2.15. A cut-away view of the cerebellar vermis nervate the granular layer dorsally, and those more
to illustrate a possible model by which MF afferents ventral, the granular layer ventrally. The differences in
might differentially innervate dorsal and ventral faces terminal field position arise through chemically or
of a lobule. The two kinds of MF are laminated within physiologically based decision making during synap-
the white matter. Thus, when they come to identify togenesis.
synaptic partners, the more dorsal MFs naturally in-
48 Richard Hawkes et al.

cerebellar projection is already present and orga- different MF afferents to either the dorsal or the
nized into compartments before synaptic contact ventral half of the lobule, and then to identify
with the Purkinje cell targets (Sotelo et aI., 1984). appropriate synaptic partners. For the first part,
Given the relative maturity of the cerebellar nuc- we suggest that a dorsoventral lamination ofaxons
lei at birth, it may be that the primary projection within the white matter leads naturally to a dor-
is olivonuclear rather than olivocortical and that soventral bias in the distribution of their terminals
it is the cortical projection that is the secondary within the granular layer (Fig. 2.15). Extending
collateral. From this perspective the olivocerebel- this segregation into the individual folia could
lar topography would be generated by the olivo- result in individual longitudinal myeloarchitec-
cerebellar and corticonuclear projections both tural compartments that are also subdivided into
first recognizing common nuclear target zones. dorsal and ventral fascicles. The MF growth cones
The same sequence might apply also to the matu- are already situated in the white matter at birth,
ration of the various MF inputs. before the arrival of their granule cell targets. As
the granule cells descend, during the first three
postnatal weeks or so, the MF growth cones
Mossy Fibers invade the granular layer and form the synaptic
The matching ofMF terminal fields to appropriate glomeruli. With an intrinsic lamination in the
Purkinje cell compartments is more complex, be- white matter, the more dorsal axons will naturally
cause there is no direct synaptic interaction bet- innervate the dorsal aspect of the lobule and vice
ween MF and PC in the adult. Once within the versa.
target area, the differences in the terminal field
distributions might arise either through physio- What the Cerebellum Can
logical or molecular mechanisms. For example,
the circuitry could be sculpted by competitive
Tell us About Cartography
interactions. The protracted maturation of the In the nervous system of vertebrates connectivity
MF-GC circuitry during the first postnatal month, between functionally coupled groups of neurons
and probably beyond, could provide an ideal in different brain regions is defined topographi-
substrate for experience/activity-dependent re- cally. This is obvious in the brain stem, for example,
finement of cerebellar circuitry. For example, dif- where neurons are clustered into discrete nuclei,
ferent MF inputs might preferentially couple to but even in ostensibly uniform structures there
Purkinje cells that receive an appropriate olivo- appears to be a compartmental, modular organi-
cerebellar input. During postnatal maturation zation. Well known examples of such organization
synapses with inappropriate physiological pro- would include the cortex, the striatum, and the
perties would be eliminated. cerebellum. This need not be the case a priori: it
Alternatively, molecular cues could operate. would be easy to imagine an organization in
Although regional differences within granule cells which a target would consist of a homogeneous
have not been demonstrated, they may be present mix of two cell classes, each of which would be
either intrinsically or due to induction by the contacted by a different population of inputs.
overlying Purkinje cells. There are also temporary Indeed, this may well often be the case, as exempli-
synaptic interactions between MF growth cones fied by the segregation ofCF inputs to the Purkinje
and Purkinje cells that could serve to aid align- cells and the MF inputs to the granule cells in the
ment. However, any model based on Purkinje cell cerebellum. However important this segregation,
organization has to explain the facts that MF it probably involves different interactions than
terminal fields are frequently spanning zebrin 1+ those involved in the establishment of topogra-
and zebrin r compartments, and often appear phically specific projections.
to be distributed differently on different faces of The fact that spatial segregation is predominant
the same lobule. In this context, how might this probably reflects the restricted repertoire of cell
differential distribution of LTHL terminals bet- recognition molecules, and the need to reuse them
ween dorsal and ventral aspects be achieved? in multiple contexts. What is the significance of
There are two aspects to the problem: to distribute compartmentation? One reason might be embryo-
2. Zebrins 49

logical - to achieve the precise correlation of larger than the target field-an example in the
afferent and efferent projections. By alternating cerebellum is the corticonuclear projection, where
clusters of different chemospecific cells the target there are up to 10 times more Purkinje cells in
presents a set of sharply differentiated internal the cerebellar cortex than there are target neurons
boundaries to the afferent growth cones. For in the cerebellar and lateral vestibular nuclei. In
example, once pathway guidance and axon fasci- a divergent projection, the projection field is smal-
culation have brought the CF to approximately ler than the target field . One example is the olivo-
the correct position, precise terminal fields are cerebellar projection, where each olivary cell
identified thanks to the steplike borders. innervates up to 10 Purkinje cells. From a purely
We have used the cerebellum and its afferent topographical perspective, a divergent projection
projections as a model to explore map formation is merely empty magnification. However, it might
in the central nervous system (Fig. 2.16). In order be the basis for topographically identical input
to characterize the cerebellar maps, it will be fields with dissimilar outputs- that is, branching-
useful to define a number ofterms. To begin, there or it might permit the convergence of different
is the definition of the map itself. The projection combinations of inputs that can be combined
map is the distribution of afferent terminals from functionally. Of course, divergent projections have
a projection field onto a target field . For the other functional advantages; for example, they
present purposes, a map is characterized by a permit signal amplification, improve the signal-
point-to-point correspondence between the pro- to-noise ratio during message transmission, and
jection and target fields. All such projections can allow the same afferent data to be processed in
be characterized in two ways. First, a projection different ways.
may be convergent or divergent (Fig. 2.161). In Second, projections may be overlapping-with
a convergent projection, the projection field is a consequent loss of resolution - or nonover-

A A A A A A

(DJ
A A A A A A
A A A
A B B B B B B
B B
e e
B B B ~ B B B B B B
e e e
e e e e e e
e e e e e e

A A A A A A A A A
AB AB AB
II B B B B B B B B B
Be Be Be
e e e c e c c e c

Figure 2.16. Illustration of different

[]
kinds of maps found in the cerebellum. A A A A A A
In I, the projection field can terminate BBB
either convergently (smaller target: e.g., III B B B B B B eee
corti-conuclear) or divergently (target c e e c e e
larger than projection field, e.g., olivo-
cerebellar). In II, a projection can be
nonoverlapping (e.g., olivocerebellar) or
overlapping (corticonuclea r, MF). In A A A A A A A A A
III, the field is completely or incompletely
filled. In IV, the mapping is continuous
IV B B B B B B e e e
or discontinuous (e.g., olivocerebellar). e e e e e e B B B
50 Richard Hawkes et al.

lapping. For the olivocerebellar projection, com- would present problems if the topography were
partments seem to be discrete and nonoverlapping. continuous, and provides a ready mechanism for
This seems to be equally true in the neonate as error elimination and secondary, perhaps func-
in the adult. For MF and for the corticonuclear tional, refinement.
projections, there is probably some overlap (Fig. The number oftopographically identical parallel
2.1611). pathways within a field-to-field projection can be
A "field" could be defined in a number of ways; considered as a measure of the topographical
for example, as a group of cells that share a redundancy. In the adult animal, this number is
common target field with no functionally signifi- presumably related to the density of information
cant heterogeneity. This definition present prob- passing along the pathway. However, during devel-
lems-fields are defined by their connectivity and opment other considerations come into play. It
so the argument is circular. It does, however, is a characteristic of much of the nervous system
serve to focus attention on the central question development that ordered patterns are sculpted
here: can fields be defined independently of their from initially homogeneous arrays by selective
connectivity? In the case of the cerebellum, the elimination of supernumerary components (e.g.,
answer is clearly, yes. Future target fields can Changeux and Danchin, 1976; Law and Constan-
already be identified in the embryo as early as tine-Paton, 1980; O'Leary et ai., 1986), and several
E 16, well before contact with the afferents. Like- theoretical models of map formation predict the
wise, the differential expression of zebrins by source-specific banding of afferent terminals when
Purkinje cell subpopulations is independent of fibers interact competitively (Constantine-Paton,
afferent input (Leclerc et ai., 1988). 1982; von de Malsburg and Willshaw, 1976).
The map may be completely filled or incomplete One of the better known examples is found in
(Fig. 2.16III). This may be an issue of definition the cerebellum-the elimination of excess CF
only, as fields are defined by their terminals, for synapses during the maturation of the olivocere-
example, patches of granular cells that do not bellar pathway (Crepel et ai., 1976; Mariani and
receive spinocerebellar inputs can simply be treat- Changeux, 1981). These might serve either to
ed as outside the field. The issue is important only ensure topographic mapping despite poor aim
if these gaps are filled at some time during develop- (i.e., improve topographical fidelity) or to ensure
ment: there is no evidence of this either for the complete filling. Elimination occurs both in the
CF or the MF terminals, although it clearly is the CF and the MF projections. In the case of the
case in other brain regions (e.g., the corticocortical olivocerebellar projection, the available data do
projection via the corpus callosum). not support an important role for collateral elim-
A map may be continuous or discontinuous ination in the refinement of topography (Crepel,
(Fig. 2.16IV). If a continuous translation in the 1982; Sotelo et ai., 1984). However, target filling
projection from field to neighboring field maps is clearly a crucial problem for projections such
to a smooth displacement in the target, then the as the olivocerebellar, where the target receives
map is continuous. If the corresponding transla- only one input in the adult; thus, the most plausible
tion in the terminal fields is saltatory, the projec- role for excess CF collaterals is to ensure that
tion is discontinuous. It is striking that both MF each Purkinje cell receives at least one CF input.
and CF projections are discontinuous. The reason As far as the MF are concerned, the matter is
for this is unknown, but discontinuous projections less clear. For example, the circuitry could be
have one evident advantage-sharp internal sculpted by competitive interactions. The pro-
boundaries. From the point of view of target tracted maturation of the mossy fiber-granule
recognition, these can serve to assure that growth cell circuitry during the first postnatal month,
cones that reach approximately the correct target and probably beyond, could provide an ideal
territory by white matter interactions are forced substrate for experience/activity-dependent re-
to terminate within a small, precise area, simply finement of cerebellar circuitry. For example, dif-
because the adjacent areas are incompatible in ferent spinocerebellar inputs might preferentially
some way. This then will prevent the statistical synapse on Purkinje cells that receive an appro-
degradation ("blurring") of the topography that priate olivocerebellar input. During postnatal
2. Zebrins 51

maturation synapses with inappropriate physio- Cerebellum: New Vistas. (S. Palay and V. Chan-
logical properties would be eliminated. Similarly, Palay, eds). Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 195-232.
an exuberance of granule cell dendrites could be Armstrong, D.M., Harvey, R.J., and Schild, R.F. (1974):
an anatomical substrate for the refinement of the Topographical localization in the olivocerebellar pro-
MF pathways (e.g., Morest, 1969; Ramon y Cajal, jection: An electrophysiological study in the cat.
J. Camp. Neural., 154,287-302.
1911 ).
Armstrong, D.M., and Schild, R.F. (1978): An investi-
Developmentally, the question is whether the
gation of the cerebellar corticonuclear projections
projection fields and the target fields are naturally in the rat using an autoradiographic tracing method.
compartmentalized before the establishment of a I. Projections from the vermis. Brain Res., 141,
map. In the cerebellum it appears that they are, 1-19.
and precise topography is achieved by mapping Arsenio-Nunes, M.L., and Sotelo, e. (1985): Develop-
an ordered input onto an ordered target. What ment of the spinocerebellar system in the postnatal
is the advantage of such a matching strategy over rat. J. Camp. Neural., 237, 291-306.
a method involving transynaptic determination? Beinfeld, M.e., and Korchak, D.M. (1985): The regio-
The most obvious gain is that system maturation nal distribution and the chemical, chromatographic
need not proceed linearly. In a neuronal chain in immunologic characterization of motilin brain pep-
which target topography passes from member to tide: The evidence for a difference between brain and
intestinal motilin-immunoreactive material. J. Neu-
member, transynaptic determination requires that
rosci,5, 2502-2509.
development occur serially and proceed in the
Beyerl, B.D., Borges, L.F., Swearingen, B., and Sidman,
descending direction. The converse, where the R.L. (1982): Parasagittal organization of the olivo-
target topography is independently determined, cerebellar projection in the mouse. J. Camp. Neural.,
permits elements of the chain to mature indepen- 209,339-346.
dently. Such a mechanism also avoids a second Bishop, G.A. (1982) The pattern of distribution of the
problem with linear determination-that errors local axonal collaterals of Purkinje cells in the inter-
high in the chain will disrupt the topographic mediate cortex of the anterior lobe and paramedian
organization at all lower levels. Thus, in the lobule of the cat cerebellum. J. Camp. Neural., 210,
cerebellum, the descending efferent pathways can 1-9.
mature earlier than the cerebellum itself without Bloedel, J.R., and Courville, J. (1981): Cerebellar afferent
systems. In: Handbook of Physiology. (l.M. Brookhart,
compromising anatomical topography.
V.B. Mountcastle, and V.B. Brooks, eds). Bethesda,
MD: American Physiology Society, 2, 735-829.
Acknowledgements. Thanks are due to Marc Boegman, R., Parent, A., and Hawkes, R. (1988) Zona-
Colonnier who constructed the various artwork, tion in the rat cerebellar cortex: Patches of high
to Suzanne Bilodeau for typing numerous versions acetylcholinesterase activity in the granular layer
are congruent with Pukinje cell compartments. Brain
of the text, and Mike Lannoo and Andre Parent
Res., 448,237-251.
for their comments. The experimental work was Brochu, G., Maler, L., and Hawkes, R. (1990): Zebrin
dependent on the technical assistance of Line II: A polypeptide antigen expressed selectively by
Thivierge, Jamel Rafrafi, and Rachel Sasseville, Purkinje cells reveals compartments in rat and fish
and was supported by grants from the Medical cerebellum. J. Camp. Neural., 291, 538-552.
Research Council of Canada, the F.R.S.Q., the Brodal, A. (1976): The olivocerebellar projection in the
F.C.A.R., and the N.I.H. cat as studied with the method of retrograde axonal
transport Qf horseradish peroxidase. II. The projec-
tion to the uvula. J. Camp. Neural., 166,417-426.
References
Brodal, A. (1980): Olivocerebellocortical projection in
Altman, 1. (1972): Postnatal development of the cere- the cat as determined with the method of retrograde
bellar cortex in the rat. II. Phases in the maturation axonal transport of horseradish peroxidase 2. Topo-
of Purkirije cells and of the molecular layer. J. Camp. graphical pattern in relation to the longitudinal sub-
Neural., 145, 399-464. division of the cerebellum. In: The Inferior Olivary
Armstrong, D.M., Campbell, N.e., Edgley, SA, Schild, Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology (J. Courville,
R.F., and Trott, 1.R. (1982): Investigations of the e. de Montigny, and Y. Lamarre eds). New York:
olivocerebellar and spino-olivary pathways. In: The Raven Press, pp. 187-205.
52 Richard Hawkes et al.

Brodal, A., Walberg, F., and Hoddevik, G.H. (1975): Constantine-Paton, M. (1982): The retinotectal hookup:
The olivocerebellar projection in the cat as studied the process of neural mapping. In Developmental
with the method of retrograde axonal transport of Order: Its Origin and Regulation (S. Subtelny, ed).
horseradish peroxidase I. The projection to the para- New York: Alan R. Liss Inc, pp. 317-349.
median lobule. J. Compo Neurol., 164,449-470. Courville, 1. (1975): Distribution of olivocerebellar fibers
Brown, B.L., and Graybiel, A.M. (1983): Zonal organi- demonstrated by a radioautographic tracing method.
zation in the cerebellar vermis of the cat. Anat. Rec., Brain Res., 95, 253-263.
205,25A. Courville, 1., and Diakiw, N. (1976): Cerebellar cortico-
Campbell, N.e., and Armstrong, D.M. (1983a): The nuclear projection in the cat. The vermis of anterior
olivocerebellar projection in the rat: An autoradio- and posterior lobes. Brain Res., 110, 1-20.
graphic study. Brain Res., 275, 215-233. Courville, J., and Faraco-Cantin, F. (1978): On the
Campbell, N.C:, and Armstrong, D.M. (1983b): Topo- origin of the climbing fibers of the cerebellum. An
graphical localization in the olivocerebellar projec- experimental study in the cat with an autoradio-
tion in the rat: An autoradiographic study. Brain graphic tracer method. Neuroscience, 3, 797-
Res., 275, 235-249. 809.
Campistron, G., Geffard, M., and Buijs, RM. (1986): Crepel, F. (1971): Maturation of climbing fiber res-
Immunological approach to the detection of taurine ponses in the rat. Brain Res., 35, 272-276.
and immunocytochemical results. J. Neurochem., 46, Crepe!, F. (1982): Regression offunctional synapses in
862-868. the immature mammalian cerebellum. Trends N euro-
Chambers, W.W., and Sprague, 1.M. (1955a): A func- sci., 5, 266-269.
tionallocalization in the cerebellum: I. Organization Crepel, F., Mariani, 1., and De!haye-Bouchard, N. (1976):
in longitudinal corticonuclear zones and their con- Evidence for a multiple innervation of Purkinje cells
tribution to the control of posture, both extrapyra- by climbing fibers in the immature rat cerebellum.
midal and pyramidal. J. Compo Neurol., 103, 105- J. Neurobiol., 7, 567-578.
130. Dore, L., Jacobson, C.D., and Hawkes, R. (1990): The
Chambers, W.W., and Sprague, J.M. (1955b): Func- organization and postnatal development of zebrin
tionallocalization in the cerebellum: II. Somatotopic II antigenic compartmentation in the cerebellar
organization in cortex and nuclei. Arch. N eurol. vermis of the grey opossum, M onodelphis domestica.
Psychiatry, 74, 653-680. J. Compo Neurol., 291, 431-449.
Changeux, J.P., and Danchin, A. (1976): Selective stabi- Eisenman, L. (1981) Olivocerebellar projections to the
lization of developing synapses, a mechanism for the pyramis and copula pyramidis in the rat: Differential
specification of neuronal networks. Nature, 264, projections to parasagittal zones. J. Compo N eurol.,
705-712. 199,65-76.
Chan-Palay, V. (1971): The recurrent collaterals of Eisenman, L.M. (1984): Organization of the olivocere-
Purkinje cell axons: A correlated study of the rat's bellar projection to the uvula in the rat. Brain Behav.
cerebellar cortex with ·electron microscopy and the Evoi., 24, 1-12.
Golgi method. Z. Anat. Entwickl.-Gesch., 134, 200- Eisenman, L.M., and Goracchi, G.P. (1983): A double
234. label retrograde tracing study of the olivocerebel-
Chan-Palay, V., Nilaver, G., Palay, S.L., Beinfeld, M.e., lar projection to the pyramis and uvula in the rat.
Zimmerman, E.A., Wu, J-Y., and O'Donohue, T.L. Neurosci. Lett., 41,15-20.
(1981): Chemical heterogeneity in cerebellar Purkinje Eisenman, L.M., and Hawkes, R (1990): 5'-nucleotidase
cells: Existence and co-existence of glutamic acid and the mabQI13 antigen share a common distri-
decarboxylase-like and motHin-like immunoreac- bution in the cerebellar cortex of the mouse. N euro-
tivities. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci. USA,78, 7787-7791. science, 31, 231-235.
Chan-Palay, V., Palay, S.L., Brown, 1.T., and Van Eisenman, L.M., Sieger, D.D., and Blatt, GJ. (1983):
ItalIie, C. (1977): Sagittal organization of olivocere- The olivocerebellar projection to the uvula in the
bellar and reticulocerebellar projections: Autoradio- mouse. J. Compo Neurol., 221, 53-59.
graphic studies with 35S-methionine. Exp. Brain Res., Goodman, D.C., Hellitt, R.E., and Welch, R.B. (1963):
30,561-576. Patterns of localization in the cerebellar cortico-
Chan-Palay, V., Pala)" S.L., and Wu, J-Y. (1982): nuclear projections of the albino rat. J. Compo N eurol.,
Sagittal cerebellar micro bands of taurine neurons: 121,51-68.
Immunocytochemical demonstration by using anti- Gravel, e., Eisenman, L.M., Sasseville, R, and Hawkes,
bodies against the taurine synthesizing enzyme cys- R., (1987): Parasagittal organization of the rat cere-
teine sulfinic acid decarboxylase. Proc. Natl. Acad. bellar cortex: direct correlation between antigenic
Sci. USA, 79, 4221-4225. Purkinje cell bands revealed by mabQI13 and the
2. Zebrins 53

organization of the olivocerebellar projection. J. cells in the monkey cerebellum. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Camp. Neural., 265, 295-310. Sci. USA, 82, 7131-7135.
Gravel, e., and Hawkes, R. (1990): Parasagittal orga- Jansen, J., and Brodal, A. (1940): Experimental studies
nization of the rat cerebellar cortex: Direct com- on the intrinsic fibers of the cerebellum II. The
parison of Purkinje cell compartments and the corticonuclear projection. J. Camp. Neural., 73,
organization of the spinocerebellar projection. J. 267-321.
Camp. Neural., 291,79-102. Jansen, J., and Brodal, A. (1942): Experimental studies
Gravel, e., Leclerc, N., Plioplys, A., and Hawkes, R. on the intrinsic fibers ofthe cerebellum. The cortico-
(1986): Focal axonal swellings in rat cerebellar Pur- nuclear projection in the rabbit and in the monkey
kinje cells during normal development. Brain Res., (Macacus rhesus). Norske Vid. Akad., Oslo. Avh. [.
363, 325-332. Mat. Naturv. Kl., 3, I-50.
Groenewegen, H.J., and Voogd, J. (1977): The parasa- Joseph, J.W., Shambes, G.M., Gibson, J.M., and Welker,
gittal zonation within the olivocerebellar projec- W. (1978): Tactile projections to granule cells in
tion. 1. Climbing fiber distribution in the vermis of caudal vermis of the rat's cerebellum. Brain Behav.
cat cerebellum. J. Camp. Neural., 174,417-488. Evol., 15, 141-149.
Groenewegen, H.J., Voogd,J., and Freeman, S.L.(1979): Kassel, J., Shambes, G.M., and Welker, W. (1984):
The parasagittal zonation within the olivocerebellar Fractured cutaneous projections to the granule cell
projection. II. Climbing fiber distribution in the inter- layer of the posterior cerebellar hemisphere of the
mediate and hemispheric parts of cat cerebellum. domestic cat. J. Camp. Neural., 225, 458-468.
J. Camp. Neural., 183, 551-602. Killackey, H.P., and Belford, G.R. (1980): Central corre-
Haines, D.E., Patrick, G.W., and Satrulee, P. (1982): lates of peripheral pattern alterations in the trigemi-
Organization of cerebellar corticonuclear fiber sys- nal system of the rat. Brain Res., 183,205-210.
tems. In: The Cerebellum-New Vistas (S.L. Palay Lange, W. (1982): Regional differences in the cytoarchi-
and V. Chan-Palay, eds). Berlin-Heidelburg-New tecture of the cerebellar cortex. In: The Cerebellum-
York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 320-367. New Vistas. (S.L. Palay and V. Chan-Palay, eds).
Hawkes, R., Colonnier, M., and Leclerc, N. (1985): Berlin-Heidelburg-New York: Springer-Verlag, pp.
Monoclonal antibodies reveal sagittal banding in 93-105.
the rodent cerebellar cortex. Brain Res., 333, 359-365. Lange, W., Unger, J., Pitzl, H., and Weindl, A. (1986): Is
Hawkes, R., and Leclerc, N. (1986): Immunocytoche- motilin a cerebellar peptide in the rat? Anat. Embryol.,
mical demonstration of topographic ordering of 173,371-376.
Purkinje cell axon terminals in the fastigial nuclei of Law, M.I., and Constantine-Paton, M. (1980): Right
the rat. J. Camp. Neural., 244, 481-491. and left eye bands in frqgs with unilateral tectal
Hawkes, R., and Leclerc, N. (1987): Antigenic map of ablations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 77, 2314-2318.
the rat cerebellar cortex: the distribution of paras a - Leclerc, N., Beesley, P.W., Colonnier, M., Brown, I.,
gittal bands as revealed by monoclonal anti-Purkinje Gurd, J.W., Paladino, T., and Hawkes, R. (I 990a):
cell antibodymabQl13. J. Camp. Neural., 256,29-41. Synaptophysin expression during synaptogenesis in
Hawkes, R., and Leclerc, N. (1989): Purkinje cell axon the rat cerebellar cortex. J. Camp. Neural., 280, 197-
collateral distributions reflect the chemical compart- 212.
mentation of the rat cerebellar cortex. Brain Res., Leclerc, N., Dore, L., Parent, A., and Hawkes, R.
476, 279-290. (l990b): The compartmentation of the monkey and
Hazlett, J.e., Martin, G.F., and Dom, R. (1971): Spino- rat cerebellar cortex: zebrin I and cytochrome oxi-
cerebellar fibers of the opossum Didelphis marsupialis dase. Brain Res .. 506, 70-78.
virginiana. Brain Res., 33, 257-271. Leclerc, N., Gravel, e., and Hawkes, R. (1988): Deve-
Hess, D.T., and Hess, A. (1986): 5' -nucleotidase of cere- lopment of parasagittal zonation in the rat cerebellar
bellar molecular layer: Reduction in Purkinje cell- cortex: MabQI13 antigenic bands are created post-
deficient mice. Brain Res., 394, 93-100. natally by the suppression of antigen expression in
Hess, D.T., and Voogd, J. (1986): Chemoarchitectonic a subset of Purkinje cells. J. Camp. Neural., 273,
zonation of the monkey cerebellum. Brain Res., 369, 399-420.
383-387. Leclerc, N., Herrup, K., Hawkes, R., Schwarting, G.,
Hillman, D.E., and Chen, S. (1981): Vunerability of and Yamamoto, M. (I 990c): Zebrin II and O-acetyl
cerebellar development in malnutrition. I. Quantita- GD3 divide all Purkinje cells into two distinct com-
tion of layer volume and neuron numbers. Neuro- plementary sets. 20 th Annual Meeting of the Society
science, 6, 1249-1262. of Neuroscience 16, 642.
Ingram, V.I., Ogren, M.P., Chatot, e.L., Gossels, J.M., Madsen, S., Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J.
and Owens, B.B. (1985): Diversity among Purkinje (1985): Immunocytochemical visualization of taurine:
54 Richard Hawkes et al.

Neuronal localization in the rat cerebellum. New'o- Oster-Granite, M.L., and Gearhart, J. (1982): Cell lineage
sci. Lett., 60, 255-260. analyses of Purkinje cells in murine chimeras.
Magnussen, K.R., Madl, J.E., Clements, J.R., Wu, J.- y., In: The Cerebellum-New Vistas (S.L. Palay and
Larson, A.A., and Beitz, A.J. (1988): Colocalization V. Chan-Palay, eds). Berlin-Heidelberg-New York:
of taurine- and cysteine sulfinic acid decarboxylase- Springer-Verlag, pp. 75-92.
like immunoreactivity in the cerebellum of the Palay, S.L., and Chan-Palay, V. (1974): Cerebellar
rat with monoclonal antibodies against taurine. Cortex, Cytology and Organization. New York-
J. Neurosci., 8, 4551-4564. Heidelburg-Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Marani, E. (1982a): Topographic enzyme histochemis- Palkovits, M., Mezey, M., Hamori, J., and Szentagothai,
try of the mammalian cerebellum: 5'-nucleotidase J. (1977): Quantitative histological analysis of the
and acetylcholinesterase. Thesis, University of cerebellar nuclei in the cat. I. Numerical data on cells
Leiden. and on synapses. Exp. Brain. Res., 28,189-209.
Marani, E. (1982b): The ultrastructural localization of Plioplys, A.V., and Hawkes, R. (1986): A survey of
5' -nucleotidase in the molecular layer of the mouse mabQI13 immunoreactivity in the adult rat brain:
cerebellum. In: Neurotransmitter Interaction and Differential staining of the lateral and medial habe-
Compartmentation (H.F. Bradford, ed). New York: nular nuclei. Brain Res., 375, 1-12.
Plenum Publishing Corp, pp. 557-571. Plioplys, A.V., and Hawkes, R. (1987): The development
Marani, E., and Voogd, J. (1977): An acetylcholines- of differential mabQI13 immunoreactivity in the rat
terase band pattern in the molecular layer of the cat habenular complex. Brain Res. Bull., 18,19-24.
cerebellum. J. Anat., 124, 335-345. Plioplys, A.V., and Hawkes, R. (1988): Developmental
Mariani, J., and Changeux, J.P. (1981): Ontogenesis of expression of monoclonal antibody mabQI13 im-
olivocerebellar relationships. I. Studies by intra- munoreactivity in the rat cerebral cortex: Differential
cellular recordings of the multiple innervation of sublayering of layer I and labelling of radial glia.
Purkinje cells by climbing fibers in the developing J. New·osci. Res., 20, 359-375.
rat cerebellum. 1. N eurosci., 1, 696-702. Plioplys, A.V., Thibault, J., and Hawkes, R. (1985):
Mason, C.A., and Gregory, E. (1984): Postnatal matu- Selective staining of a subset of Purkinje cells in
ration of cerebellar mossy and climbing fibers: Transi- the human cerebellum with monoclonal antibody
ent expression of dual features on single axons. mabQI13. J. Neurol. Sci., 70, 245-256.
J. New·osci., 4,1715-1735. Puro, D.G., and Woodward, D.J. (1977): Maturation
Morest, D.K. (1969): The growth of dendrites in the of evoked climbing fiber input to rat Purkinje cells.
mammalian brain. Z. Anat. entwickl.-Gesch., 128, Exp. Brain Res., 28, 85-110.
290-317. Ramon-Moliner, E. (1972): Acetylthiocholinesterase
O'Leary, D.D.M., Fawcett, J.M., and Cowan, W.M. distribution in the brain stem of the cat. Ergeb. Anat.,
(1986): Topographic targeting errors in the retino- 46,1-52.
collicular projection and their elimination by selec- Ramon y Cajal, S. (1911): Histologie du Systeme Nerveux
tive ganglion cell death. J. Neurosci., 6, 3692-3705. de I'Homme et des Vertebres. Paris: Maloine.
O'Leary, J.L., Petty, J., Smith, J.M., O'Leary, M., and Robertson, B., Grant, G., and Bjorkeland, M. (1983):
Inukai, S. (1968): Cerebellar cortex of rat and other Demonstration of spinocerebellar projections in cat
animals. A structural and ultrastructural study. using anterograde WGA-HRP with some observa-
J. Compo Neurol., 134,401-432. tions on spinomesencephalic and spinothalamic pro-
Oscarsson, O. (1969): The sagittal organization of the jections. Exp. Brain Res., 52, 99-104.
cerebellar anterior lobe as revealed by the projection Scheibel, A. (1977): Sagittal organization of mossy fiber
patterns of the climbing fiber system. In: N eurobio- terminal systems in the cerebellum of the rat: A
logy of Cerebellar Organization and Development. Golgi study. Exp. Neurol., 57,1067-1070.
(R. L1imls, ed). Chicago: American Medical Asso- Scott, T.G. (1963): A unique pattern of localization in
ciation, pp. 525-532. the cerebellum. Nature, 200, 793.
Oscarsson, O. (1980): Functional organization of oli- Scott, T.G. (1964): A unique pattern of localization
vary projection to the cerebellar anterior lobe. In: within the cerebellum ofthe mouse. J. Compo Neurol.,
The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology 122,1-8.
(J. Courville, C. de Montigny, and Y. Lamarre, eds). Shambes, G.M., Beerman, D.H., and Welker, W. (1978a):
New York: Raven Press, pp. 279-289. Multiple tactile area in cerebellar cortex: Another
Oscarsson, 0., and Sji:ilund, B. (1977): The ventral patchy cutaneous projection to granule cell columns
spino- olivocerebellar system in the cat 1. Identifica- in rat. Brain Res., 157,123-128.
tion of five paths and their termination in the cere- Shambes, G.M., Gibson,J.M., and Welker, W.(l978b):
bellar anterior lobe. Exp. Brain Res., 28; 469-486. Fractured somatotopy in granule cell tactile areas
2. Zebrins 55

of ra,t cerebellar hemispheres revealed by micro- the cerebellum. A review. In: The Inferior Olivary
mapping. Brain Behav. Evol., 15,94-140. Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology (J. Courville, C.
Sotelo, C (1987): Cerebellar synaptogenesis and the de Montigny, and Y. Lamarre, eds). New York:
organization of afferent projection maps. Pontificae Raven Press, pp. 207-234.
Acad. Scient. Scripta Varia, 59, 65-90. Voogd, 1., Gerrits, N.M., and Marani, E. (1985): Cere-
Sotelo, C, Bourrat, F., and Triller, A. (1984): Postnatal bellum. In: The Rat Nervous System (G. Paxinos,
development of the inferior olivary complex in the ed). New York: Academic Press, 2, 251-291.
rat. II. Topographic organization of the immature Wassef, M., and Sotelo, C (1984): Asynchrony in the
olivocerebellar projection. J. Camp. Neural., 222, expression of guanosine 3':5'-phosphate-dependent
177-199. protein kinase by clusters of Purkinje cells during
Tomida, Y., and Kimura, H. (1987): Immunohisto- the perinatal development of rat cerebellum. Neuro-
chemical and biochemical studies of substances with science, 13, 1217-1241.
taurine-like immunoreactivity in the brain. Acta Wassef, M., Sotelo, C, Thomasset, M., Granholm,
Histochem. Cytochem., 20, 31-40. A-C, Leclerc, N., Rafrafi, R., and Hawkes, R. (1990):
Van der Loos, H., and Woolsey, T.A. (1973): Somato- Expression of compartmentation antigen zebrin I in
sensory cortex: Structural alterations following early cerebellar transplants. J. Camp. Neural., 294, 223-
injury to sense organs. Science, 179, 395-398. 234.
Van Gilder, J.C, and O'Leary, J.L. (1970): Topical Wassef, M., Zanetta, J.P., Brehier, A., and Sotelo, C.
projection of the olivocerebellar system in the cat: (1985): Transient biochemical compartmentalization
an electrophysiological study. J. Camp. Neural., 140, of Purkinje cells during early cerebellar development.
69-80. Dev. Bioi., 111, 129-137.
Von de Malsburg, C, and Willshaw, D.J. (1976): Mecha- Welker, W. (1987): Spatial organization of soma to sen-
nism for producing continuous neural mapping: sory projections to granule cell cerebellar cortex:
Ocularity dominance stripes and ordered retinotec- Functional and connectional implications of frac-
tal projections. Exp. Brain. Res., Suppl. 1, 463-469. tured somatotopy. In: New Concepts in Cerebellar
Voogd, J. (1964): The Cerebellum ofthe Cat. Assen: Van Neurobiology (J.S. Kinged). New York: Alan R. Liss
Gorcum. Inc., pp. 239-280.
Voogd, J. (1967): Comparative aspects of the structure Welker, W., and Shambes, G.M. (1985): Tactile cuta-
and fibre connections of the mammalian cerebellum. neous representation in cerebellar granule cell layer
Prog. Brain Res., 25, 94-135. of the opossum, Didelphis virginiana. Brain Behav.
Voogd, J. (1969): The importance of fiber connections Evol. 27, 57-79.
in the comparative anatomy ofthe mammalian cere- Yaginuma, H., and Matsushita, M. (1986): Spinocere-
bellum. In: Neurobiology ofCerebellar Evolution and bellar projection fields in the horizontal plane of
Development (R. Llinas, ed). Chicago: American lobules of the cerebellar anterior lobe in the cat:
Medical Association, pp. 493-514. An anterograde wheat germ agglutinin-horseradish
Voogd, J., and Bigare, F. (1980): The topographical peroxidase study. Brain Res., 365, 345-349.
distribution of olivary and corticonuclear fibers in
3
Cerebellar Granule Cells and the
Neurobiology of Excitatory Amino Acids
Robert Bala.zs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. J~rgensen

The discovery that the development in the nervous is possible to obtain a preparation from dissociat-
system involves the loss of a significant number ed cerebella ofearly postnatal mice or rats [usually
of nerve cells in many structures has given one of postnatal day (P) 6-8], which predominantly
the deep insights into mechanisms underlying the comprises interneurones (about 95%), primarily
development of neuronal organization and of the granule cells (e.g., Thangnipon et aI., 1983). Ini-
maintenance of nerve cells (for reviews cf. Cowan tially, most of the cells surviving the procedure
et aI., 1984; Hamburger and Oppenheimer, 1982; of tissue dissociation and the transfer to in vitro
Levi-Montalcini, 1982). It seems that during their conditions are immature granule neurones. Many
differentiation nerve cells become dependent on ofthese cells would continue proliferating in vivo,
environmental influences for survival. Most of but in vitro they are unable to do so and are
the evidence concerning specific survival require- prematurely induced to differentiate. This is
ments relates to target-derived trophic factors, characterized by neurite emission soon after plat-
which are available in limited supply; the best ing, the production of a fine network of fibers,
characterized example is nerve growth factor and the migration of neurones along the fibers to
(NGF). In addition, it is believed that afferent form clumps. During cultivation the aggregates
nerve fibers also exert trophic influences on nerve increase progressively in size and the fibers fasci-
cells, but hitherto these have received less atten- culate to form large, more or less interconnecting,
tion than the target-derived effects (Cunningham, bundles (for the appearance of a "mature" culture
1982). Our recent studies on cerebellar granule see, e.g., Figs. 3.1a and 3.2a). Biochemical indices
cells have implicated an afferent system, the mossy show that maturation starts more or less im-
fibers, as a source oftrophic influence at a critical mediately after seeding (Meier et al., 1984) but, in
stage of the maturation of these cells (e.g., Balazs terms of most parameters examined, it is slow in
et aI., 1988a, b; Gallo et aI., 1987a). Here we pre- the first few days followed by a rapid phase of
sent observations indicating that this influence is development, reaching a plateau by 7 to 8 days
mediated through the stimulatioJl of receptors for in vitro (DIV) (e.g., Gallo et aI., 1987a). Matura-
excitatory amino acids (EAA) on the postsynaptic tion is therefore faster in culture than in situ: this
granule cells and put forward evidence suggesting is due to the fact that in vivo cerebellar develop-
that such trophic effects are not unique to the ment is prqtracted because the genesis of nerve
cerebellar granule cells. cells takes place during a relatively long period,
the first three postnatal weeks. Thus, although
Cerebellar Granule Cells certain differences in the state of maturation of
granule cells have been noted (Trenker et aI., 1984),
in Culture the relatively homogeneous compositions of these
We discovered the trophic effect of EAA while cultures can facilitate the detection of transient
studying cerebellar granule cells in culture. With and maturation stage-dependent effects, which
certain manipulations of the culture conditions it could easily be masked in vivo.

56
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 57

Figure 3.1. Survival of granule cells in culture is depen- replaced by calcium agonists: the effect of the
dent on chronic K + -induced depolarization, whose dihydropyridine, (+ )-S-202 791, added to a sister cul-
effect is mediated through a stimulation of Ca2+ in- ture of bin K15 is shown in c and d (1 x 10- 6 and
flux. Cells obtained from P6-8 rat cerebella were grown 5 x 10-8 M, respectively). Other dihydropyridine
in 10% fetal calf serum containing BME supplemented agonists, such as CGP 28392 and BAY K 8644, had
with 2 mM glutamine and 100 Jlgjml gentamicin on comparable effects, BAY K 8644 being the most potent
polylysine-coated Falcon dishes for 8 DIV. Glial cell (lowest effective concentration 1 x 10-8 M). Addition
proliferation was inhibited by adding cytosine to the cultures in K25 of Ca2+ antagonists [lOmM
arabinoside (10 JlM) at about 19 hours after seeding. Mg2 + , 10 JlM D600 or the dihydropyridines nifedipine
When cells were grown in "low" K + -containing and (- )-(R)-202 791 at 10- 7 M] resulted in cell death
medium (5-15 mM) survival was compromised usually as in the "low" K + -containing medium (appearance
by the end of the first week (15 mM K + in b). In con- of cultures similar to that in b, thus not shown
trast, cells survived well in "high" K + -containing separately). Scale bar = 100 Jlm. (Reprinted with
medium (25 mM K + in a). Elevated K + could be permission from Gallo et aI., 1987a.)

Survival Requirements die- usually quite abruptly toward the end of the
of Cerebellar Granule Cells: first week-unless the extracellular K + concen-
tration is elevated (> 20 mM) (Lasher and Zagon,
Chronic Depolarization
1972; Thangnipon et al., 1983). It is noteworthy
It is characteristic of these cultures that in con- that initially the appearance of the cells and their
ventional serum-containing medium nerve cells biochemical maturation are similar in "low" and
58 Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole s. J~rgensen
"high" K +-containing medium (in our studies the trophic effect of the first innervation of granule
5-15mM K +, K5-15, and 25mM K +, K25, res- cells in vivo, which is mediated through Ca 2 +
pectively)(Gallo et aI., 1987a). With respect to the influx linked to the stimulation ofNMDA recep-
possible mechanism underlying the survival- tors (Balazs and Jy)rgensen, 1987; Balazs et aI.,
promoting effect of chronic depolarization, two 1988a, b; Gallo et aI., 1987a).
observations were rather telling. We found that
the dependence of granule cells on K +-induced Survival Requirements
depolarization develops within a narrow window of Cerebellar Granule Cells:
of time, between 2 and 4 DIV. Furthermore, we
obtained evidence that the effect of high K + on
Effect ofNMDA
cell survival is mediated through transmembrane The hypothesis was tested by studying the effect
Ca 2 + influx. This could be demonstrated con- ofNMDA on the survival of granule cells (Balazs
vincingly after the discovery that granule cells are et aI., 1988a, b). It was observed that NMDA treat-
endowed with voltage-sensitive Ca 2 + channels ment can indeed replace high K + in securing the
(VSCC), at which dihydropyridine (DHP) calcium survival of these cells (Fig. 3.2). Quantitative eva-
effectors are as potent as in muscle cells, not only luation is presented in Fig. 3.3, which also shows
in terms of binding, but also in eliciting functional that the effect depends on the concentration of
changes such as stimulation of Ca2+ entry and NMDA. Under the conditions reproduced in
transmitter release (Kingsbury and Balazs, 1987). Fig. 3.3, cell number and DNA content approach-
Significantly, DHP calcium agonists were able to ed, although was usually less than, the value ob-
replace chronic depolarization in promoting cell tained in cultures chronically depolarized with
survival in "low" K + medium, while calcium an- 25 mM K + (K25). Rescue of cells by NMDA was
tagonists blocked the effect of high K + (Fig. 3.1). also influenced by the degree of depolarization,
Assuming that chronic depolarization in vitro within a range at which depolarization on its own
mimics critical physiological events in vivo, the does not substantially improve cell survival: thus,
crucial question relates to developmental events at 5 to 15 mM K T both the efficacy and the po-
that have similar characteristics as those on which tency of NMDA increased as a function of the
granule cell survival depends in culture. As men- K + concentration (Fig. 3.4, see also Fig. 3.3b). In
tioned before, a great proportion of the initially the experiment reproduced in Fig. 3.4 the half-
surviving nerve cells from the dissociated cere- maximal effective concentration of NMDA was
bellum are immature granule cells, which in vitro about 50,uM at 5 mM K + (K5), 40,uM at 10 mM
start to differentiate immediately. In vivo, the K+ (KI0), and 20,uM at 15mM K+ (KI5). The
postmitotic granule cells take about 2 days to latter value is similar to the affinity ofNMDA in
reach the internal granular layer (IGL), while their binding to cerebral membranes (Olverman et aI.,
differentiation has already been in progress from 1984). These characteristics of the survival-
the time they had completed the last division promoting effect of NMDA are consistent with
(Altman, 1982). This timing is therefore similar to the voltage-dependence of the ion conductance
that of the development of the K +-dependence through the NMDA receptor-linked channel
of granule cells in culture. In vivo, at the end of (Mayer and Westbrook, 1987).
migration a unique event takes place in the life Observations displayed in Fig. 3.3b also in-
of granule cells: they receive the first afferent dicated that NMDA primarily rescued nerve cells.
input. This is from the mossy fibers, many of The amounts of proteins that are known to be
which seem to be glutamatergic (Somogyi et aI., greatly enriched in neurones, such as N-CAM,
1986). Granule cells, in turn, are endowed with D3-protein, and synaptin (Bock et aI., 1982;
glutamate (Glu) receptors including the N -methyl- Jy)rgensen, 1983; Nybroe et aI., 1985), showed a
D-aspartate (NMDA)-preferring subtype (Cull- similar dependence both on NMDA concentra-
Candy et aI., 1989), which is linked to an ion tion and on the degree of membrane depolariza-
channel permeable not only to monovalent ions tion, as did cell survival. On the other hand, the
but also to Ca 2 + (Mayer and Westbrook, 1987). amount of glutamine synthetase, which was esti-
We have therefore put forward the hypothesis mated as a marker of astrocytes, the most common
that K +-induced depolarization in vitro mimics nonneuronal cell in the cultures, was very low
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 59

Figure 3.2. Granule cell survival in "low" K + - antagonist D-APV (140.uM; d). Neither APV (e) nor
medium (lOmM K +; KlO) is promoted by NMDA. NMDA (f) (140 .uM) had obvious effects on cultures in
Seven DIV cultures in K25 (a) are well preserved, K25. Scale bar = 100 .urn. (Reprinted with permission;
whereas those in KlO show massive degeneration (b). Bahizs et aI., 1988a. Copyright by Pergamon Press
This was prevented by supplementing KlO with PLC)
NMDA (140.uM;c), whose effect was blocked by the
60 Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. J!6rgensen

DNA CELL NUMBER PROTEIN

24 240 240
0

20 200 200
~

~
E,..

!~
~ 160 160
ii
~ 120 ! 120
! !
.
t t

t i f
Q.

BO eo
8 +
40 40

o ~ 70 140 o ~ 70 140 o ~ 70 140


NMDA(~M)

b
N-CAM D3-PROTEIN SYNAPTIN
120

100

eo

60

40

20

APV 0 35 70 140 APV 0 35 70 140 APV 0 35 70 140


NMDA(~M)

Figure 3.3. Effect of NMDA on the rescue of granule per dish, expressed as a percentage of the estimate in
cells is concentration-dependent. a: Cells were grown K25. As a marker of astroglia the amount of glutamine
in 6-em dishes for 8 days in KlO in the presence of synthetase was also determined: this was very low
NMDA without (0) or with equimolar concentration under all conditions tested and was not significantly
of the antagonist D-APV (e). Controls cultured in affected by NMDA or high K + (not shown). ANOVA
K25 (0). b: NMDA promotes the survival of showed a significant stimulating effect of NMDA for
neurones. This was assessed by measuring proteins, all estimates in a and b (p < .05, except for synaptin
which are known to be markedly concentrated in in KlO). Note that APV not only blocked the effect
nerve cells. Cells were cultured in to-em dishes for of NMDA, but also reduced the limited cell survival
7-8 days in KlO (0) or Kt5 (e). Some cultures in seen in "low" K + cultures. Furthermore, NMDA had
KlO or Kt5 also received 280pM D,L-APV (,6, or no effect on nerve cell survival in K25. (Reprinted with
A) either without NMDA or with t40pM NMDA. permission from Balazs et aI., 1988b. Copyright by
Values represent total amounts ofrespective proteins Pergamon Press PLC.)

and did not show significant changes upon supple- leric acid (APV). The IC so for APV in the presence
mentation of the cultures with NMDA. of 140 JlM NMDA is about 28 JlM, whereas the
The effect of NMDA was blocked by selective most potent inhibitor is MK-801 (IC so about
NMDA receptor antagonists. Figures 3.2 and 3.3 50nM) (Balazs et aI., 1989). MK-801 is a non-
show that the effect of NMDA is completely competitive antagonist, believed to block the
blocked by equimolar concentrations of the com- NMDA receptor-linked ion channel (Wong et aI.,
petitive antagonist D-2-amino-5-ph"sphonova- 1988): this compound is known to have a very
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 61

0.5
a

1. ;
0.4

o 0.3
g';;.

0.2

0.1
140 280 420 560
NHOA (""1

b c
80 40

~
:E
"".....
..
2;
0

60 20

40 J----.L.-----:-'10L---~15
10 15
K+ (1floI) K+ (1floI)

Figure 3.4. Granule cell survival depends both on the survival reached a plateau, while the slope of the
concentration ofNMDA and on the degree of depola- increase in survival depended on the concentration of
rization of granule cells. Cultures grown in the absence K +. This information is displayed in b, giving the
or the presence ofNMDA (at concentrations indicated) maximal survival at the various concentrations of K +
in K5 (A), KI0 (.), K15 (e), and K25 (.). At 7 DlV as a percentage of survival in K25 (efficacy) and in c
the reduction of a tetrazolium salt (MTT) to a colored exhibiting the concentration of NMDA effecting
formazan product, which takes place only in viable half-maximal survival at the various [K +Je (potency).
cells, was measured at 570nm (Manthorpe et aI., 1986). (Reprinted with permission from Balazs et aI., 1988b.
a: Cell survival was a function of the concentration Copyright by Pergamon Press PLC.)
of both NMDA and K +. However, at each [K +Je
62 Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. J\ilrgensen

Table 3.1. Glutamate in the culture medium is res- the limited cell survival that is detectable in the
ponsible for the survival of a proportion of granule absence of NMDA in "low" K + media (Fig. 3.3
cells. and Table 3.1). These findings indicate, therefore,
40/lM Alanine amino- that substances present in granule cell cultures
APV transferase have a trophic influence, which is mediated through
NMDA receptor stimulation. Although the cul-
% of control l00±3 79±2 73±2 ture medium does not contain added Glu, it is
Cerebellar granule cells in "low" K + -medium (K5) in the
known that this amino acid is released by granule
presence of the NMDA antagonist D·APV or 10 U alanine cells into the medium (e.g., Kingsbury et aI., 1988).
aminotransferase + 10 mM pyruvate or in the absence of To test whether Glu was responsible for the limit-
additions (control). At 8 DIV, cell survival was assessed by ed cell survival in "low" K +, the medium was
the reduction of a tetrazolium salt (MTT assay; see legend substituted with alanine aminotransferase and
to Fig. 3.4).
Data from Balazs et aI., 1989.
pyruvate to remove Glu continuously from the
extracellular fluid. This treatment resulted in a
similar loss of cells as was obtained with APV
(Table 3.1).
high affinity for the NMDA receptor ionophore Electrophysiological responses to Glu appear
complex in binding assays using brain membrane to be mediated by at least three classes of pharma-
preparations (Wong et aI., 1986). cologically defined receptors (Watkins and Evans,
In contrast to the selective NMDA receptor 1981). These are named after the agonist that pre-
blockers, other EAA antagonists, including ferentially excites them: NMDA, lX-amino-3-hy-
gamma-D-glutamylaminomethyl sulphonic acids droxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole-propionate (AMPA)
(GAMS) and kynurenic acid (KYN), at a relative- or quisqualate (QA) and kainate (KA). In recent
ly high concentration (200.uM), affected only studies we examined whether receptors other than
slightly, if at all, cell survival promoted by 140 .uM the NMDA preferring subtype are involved in the
NMDA. Findings on KYN are interesting, in trophic effect ofEAA (Balazs et aI., 1990a). It was
view of observations, that this substance, in addi- observed that KA exerts a significant and complex
tion to being a wide spectrum EAA receptor effect in cerebellar cultures (Table 3.2). At low
antagonist, also displaces glycine from an allosteric- concentrations (up to 50.uM) KA promoted cell
stimulating site on the NMDA receptor- survival, resulting in an approximately 50% in-
ionophore complex although its potency is low crease in comparison with cells grown in K5 at
(Asher and Nowak, 1987). However, in recent the maximum, 25 to 50.uM. This is less than
studies we have found that 7-chlorokynurenate obtained with NMDA under optimal conditions
(a more powerful antagonist at this site) is a (see Fig. 3.3) or with 25 mM K + (about two- or
potent blocker of NMDA effects in cultured threefold increase in cell numbers). In contrast to
granule cells (with Annelies Resink and Van der NMDA, the effect of KA, tested at 50.uM, was
Valk, unpublished observations). Thus, consider- not voltage-dependent, the degree of stimulation
ing the pharmacology of the effect, it seems that being similar in a 5 or 10 mM K + -containing
NMDA influences granule cell survival through medium. The selective nature of the effect is in-
a direct stimulation of the NMDA receptor- dicated by reports that claim that KA at the con-
ionophore complex. centration promoting the survival of granule cells
(50 .uM) is toxic to inhibitory interneurones in the
culture (e.g., Novelli et aI., 1987). Elevation ofKA
Involvement of Glutamate concentrations above 50.uM resulted at first in
Receptors Other Than the the cessation of the positive effect on cell survival,
NMDA-Preferring Subtype whereas at relatively high concentrations cells
in the Trophic Effect become vulnerable to KA (Table 3.2). .
It is known that KA can evoke Glu release from
of Excitatory Amino Acids cultured cerebellar granule cells (Gallo et aI.,
NMDA antagonists inhibited not only the 1987b). Thus, the effect of KA on cell survival
trophic effect of NMDA, but also compromised may have been secondary to the stimulation of
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 63

Table 3.2. Granule cell survival is promoted or compromised depending on the


concentration of kainic acid

Cell survival (as a percentage of OD s7o given by the MTT assay in K5)
KA (JIM) 12.5 25 50 100 800

% of control loo±2 120±9 155 ± 7" 154 ± 7" 113±7

Granule cells were grown in K5 in the absence or the presence ofkainic acid (KA) at the concentrations
indicated, for about one week. Cell survival was assessed by means of formazan production by live
cells (MIT assay; see legend to Fig. 3.4). Data were analyzed by ANOVA. The effect of KA was
significant (p < .00001): the significance of the differences compared with control (no added KA) were
assessed using the Newman-Keuls' test.
a p = .01.
b p = .05.

(Data from Balazs et aI., 1990a.)

NMDA receptors by Glu released by KA from effect, this receptor seems to be distinct from the
granule cells. This possibility was examined by conventional ionotropic receptor subtypes. A
determining the influence of KA on cell survival receptor with similar properties has also been
after selectively blocking the NMDA receptors. described in cerebral cortical cultures on the basis
Table 3.3 shows that MK-801, which on its own ofEAA-stimulated 45Ca 2+ influx (Frandsen et aI.,
compromises cell survival, potentiated rather than 1989). Furthermore, evidence consistent with the
counteracted the cell survival promoting effect of existence ofNMDA receptor subtypes in different
KA (the competitive antagonist APV had the parts of the brain has also been obtained recently
same effect)(BaUtzs et aI., 1990a, b). Furthermore, (Monaghan et aI., 1989); based on molecular bio-
whereas the effect of KA was found not to be logical information, it is more than likely that the
voltage-dependent, the potentiation of the KA heterogeneity of ionotropic Glu receptors is much
rescue by NMDA receptor antagonists was
voltage-dependent (Balazs et aI., 1990b).
One of the possible interpretations of these
Table 3.3. NMDA receptors are not involved in the
unexpected observations was that 50 JiM KA not
promotion of cell survival by kainic acid
only rescues cells, but is also responsible for bring-
ing about conditions causing cell death, which is Cell survival (as a percentage of LDH
mediated through an EAA receptor that can be activity in control cultures)
blocked by NMDA antagonists. If so, the addition
of an NMDA agonist should counteract the Kainate + +
potentiation of the effect of KA by a competitive MK-801 + +
NMDA antagonist. Indeed, quinolinic acid, % of control lOO±3 124±3" 84±2" 167±2a ,b
which is a very weak NMDA agonist in the cere-
bellum (Stone and Burton, 1988), counteracted in Granule cells were grown in K5 in the absence or the presence
a concentration-dependent manner the poten- of 5 JIM MK-801 and of 50 JIM kainate. At 8 DIV, cells were
harvested and the activity of lactate dehydrogenase, which
tiation ofKA effect by APV (Balazs et al., 1990b).
was proved to correlate with cell numbers, was determined
On the other hand, NMDA added together with as a marker of cell survival. Activities per 35 mm dish were
KA was never found to be toxic under our experi- expressed as a percentage of the mean value obtained in
mental conditions. Thus, it seems that in the the control (cells grown in K5 without any further additions;
presence ofKA an NMDA-like receptor becomes activity 6,88 ± .19 Jimol lactate formed per hour; n = 6).
One-way ANOVA showed overall significance (p < .001).
manifested, which is activated by Glu in the culture
" Conditions that were significantly different from K5.
medium, and mediates cytotoxicity. Since at this b MK-801 significantly potentiated the effect of kainate
receptor NMDA is not an agonist, while NMDA (Newman-Keuls test, p = .05).
antagonists exert a potent and voltage-dependent Data from Balazs et ai., 1990a.
64 Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. J9Irgensen

greater than indicated by the pharmacological this approach will not similarly facilitate a better
characterization (e.g., Bettler et at, 1990). understanding of afferent-mediated trophic in-
The initial effect of KA is, however, exerted via fluences. However, the hypothesis assigning a tro-
a conventional KA receptor. DNQX (6,7-dini- phic influence to alTerent inputs, mediated through
troquinoxaline-2,3-dione), which is a relatively the stimulation of postsynaptic EAA receptors,
selective KA/AMPA antagonist (Honore et at, awaits substantiation by in vivo studies.
1988), blocked in a concentration-dependent
manner the effect of KA on cell survival. Never- Maturational Stage Dependence
theless, the trophic effect of KA seems to be me- of Effects of Excitatory Amino Acids
diated indirectly: the KA rescue, as well as the
The maturational stage dependence of the re-
potentiation ofthis effect by NMDA antagonists,
sponse of cultured granule cells to NMDA was
are blocked by calcium antagonists, including the
indicated by the observation that the survival
DHP nifedipine (NF), which also counteract the
requirement for NMDA was manifested, as in the
trophic influence of high K +, but not that of
case of chronic depolarization, within the narrow
NMDA (Balazs et at, 1990b). Thus, the stimula-
timespan of 2 and 4 DIV (Balazs et at, 1988b).
tion ofthe KA receptor results, through increased
The question was not addressed in the studies
Na + conductance, in membrane depolarization,
described above whether or not the maturation
which activates VSCC, and it is the increased
of granule cells can reach a stage in culture when
Ca 2 + influx that serves as the intracellular signal
EAA become toxic rather than trophic factors.
for the trophic influence.
However, preliminary studies have indicated that
Finally, our recent investigations have shown
this· is the case: exposure of the cultures to a
that in addition to NMDA and KA, treatment
relatively high concentration of Glu (0.5-1 mM)
with QA or AMPA can also exert a trophic in-
at 2 DIV had no adverse effect, but Glu at much
fluence on cerebellar granule cells (Balazs et at,
lower concentrations killed the cells when added
1991). For some time we have overlooked this
to mature cultures at about 8 DIV (A. Resink and
effect, because it is relatively small (maximal
R. Balazs, in preparation). Furthermore, extensive
stimulation about 25%) and it is detectable only
investigations by Costa and coworkers have
at a very narrow range of concentration (0.5-2 JlM
clearly demonstrated that "mature" granule cells
for QA and 5-10 JlM AMPA). The pharmacology
do degenerate after Glu exposure in culture (e.g.,
of the effect is rather complex. QA/AMPA rescue
Manev et at, 1990). These observations on the
is blocked both by NF and by NMDA antago-
development with maturation of vulnerability to
nists; thus, it is mediated by Ca2+ influx both
Glu are similar to findings on nerve cells other
through VSCC activated by QA/AMP A-induced
than granule cells (e.g., Choi et at, 1987; Frandsen
membrane depolarization and through the open-
and Schousboe, 1987; Rothman, 1983).
ing of NMDA receptor-linked channels by Glu,
The maturational stage dependence is high-
which is known to be released from granule cells
lighted by changes in the responsiveness of granule
by QA/AMPA (Gallo et at, 1987b).
cells to NMDA in the cerebellum during develop-
ment. Detailed investigations of Garthwaite and
colleagues have shown that in cerebellar slices
Conclusions and Implications NMDA can induce granule cell degeneration, but
The observations described in this chapter have that the effect is age-dependent (e.g., Garthwaite
referred to investigations on cultured nerve cells. and Garthwaite, 1986a, b; Garthwaite et at, 1987).
In vitro studies have provided important experi- Combining the information on the toxic action
mental avenues for the elucidation of the role of of NMDA in vivo and in cerebellar slices with
target-derived factors on the survival and dif- the trophic action of NMDA in granule cell cul-
ferentiation of nerve cells (Thoenen, 1991; Varon tures, we propose the following scenario for EAA
et at, 1988) and there is no reason to suppose that receptor-mediated events in the developing cere-
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 65

DEVELOPING MATURE
CEREBELLUM CEREBELLUM
Effect of Effect of
NMDA
a • • • • NMDA

• • • •
EGL ML

~ ~ ~
~ (g)(g)(g)
ML

a
P
(g)(g)(g) P

+ • • • •
0 0 0 ® IGL a
000
o 0 0
0
0 IGL

0 0 0 0 o 0 0 0

Figure 3.5. Scenario of excitatory amino acid receptor- further stage of maturation they become vulnerable
mediated events in the cerebellum. In the immature to NMDA (lightly stippled symbols). In the fully
cerebellum, differentiating granule cells (0), but not mature cerebellum, the vulnerability of granule cells
the replicating or migrating cells (. or .), are to NMDA is low (0). EGL, external granular layer;
responsive to NMDA: cells, which have just completed ML, molecular layer; P, Purkinje cell layer; IGL,
migration and receive the first innervation by mossy internal granular layer. Effect of NMDA: 0, limited;
fibers (heavily stippled symbols), are stabilized in their +, survival promoting; -, toxic.
survival by EAA receptor stimulation, whereas at a

bellum (Fig. 3.5): Replicating and migrating replication has been completed. Most ofthe post-
granule cells are not responsive to NMDA, which mitotic granule cells die soon after their genera-
is toxic only to differentiating granule cells, al- tion, with the exception of those that are in an
though those in the upper part of the IGL are not ectopic position but are innervated by aberrant
adversely affected (Garthwaite and Garthwaite, mossy fibers.
1986a). The latter cells have just completed migra- The stage of maturation at which NMDA has
tion, which takes about the same time as does the a trophic influence on granule cells is superseded
development in culture of granule cell depend- by a stage in which cells are vulnerable to NMDA
ence on either high K + or NMDA (see above). (Garthwaite and Garthwaite, 1986a). It seems
According to our hypothesis, these trophic effects that both the trophic and the toxic effects of
in vitro mimic the influence mediated in vivo NMDA are mediated through the same mech-
through the first innervation that these cells anism, that is, increased Ca 2 + influx (Balazs et aI.,
receive, primarily from the mossy fibers. The 1988b; Balazset aI., 1992; Choi, 1987; Garthwaite
short-term slice experiments of Garthwaite and and Garthwaite, 1986a; Rothman and Olney,
Garthwaite (1986a) cannot provide information 1987). It is an intriguing but still unanswered
on whether mossy fibers exert a trophic influence question as to what is the mechanism(s) that
on this cohort of granule cells via postsynaptic underlies the maturational stage-dependent life-
EAA receptors. The observations of Rakic and or-death response of the cell to the same signal.
Sidman (1973), however, are consistent with such In this respect, differences in the effectiveness of
a role for mossy fibers. In a mutant mouse, the intracellular regulatory mechanisms, which serve
"weaver" (wv), the genetic defect is primarily to maintain free Ca2 + levels within nontoxic limits,
manifested in the cerebellum after granule cell deserve investigation.
66 Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. Jj6rgensen

NMDA TCP

c lH

Figure 3.6. Regional distribution of binding sites detecting either the recognition site or the ion channel
associated with the NMDA receptor recognition site of the NMDA receptor in different structures, except
(left half of brain) and the NMDA receptor-linked ion in the adult cerebellum, where TCP binding was
channel (right half) in the rat brain. The receptor was negligible. However, there is TCP binding in the
labeled with 200 nM eH]Glu in the presence of 1 11M cerebellum, but the affinity is lower than in other brain
QA, whereas the ion channel on adjacent sections with parts. All sections are from the left hemisphere, but
20 nM 3H-N-(1-[2thienyl]cyclohexyl)3,4-piperidine the TCP autoradiograms were reversed. (Reprinted
eH-TCP). Note that throughout the brain there is with permission from Maragos et aI., 1988.)
excellent correlation in the binding of ligands

Finally, with maturation completed, the re- coming that indicate that EAA can also promote
sponsiveness of granule cells to NMDA is greatly the survival of nerve cells other than the cerebellar
reduced (Garthwaite et aI., 1987). It would deserve granule cells. Thus, Brenneman et al. (1990) have
investigating whether or not this is related to the reported that NMDA receptor blockade can,
unusual kinetic properties of ligand binding to depending on concentration, either promote or
the NMDA receptor-linked ion channel in the suppress neuronal survival in dissociated spinal
adult cerebellum (Maragos et al., 1988) (Fig. 3.6). cord cultures. Since these cultures contain a
mixed population of nerve cells, it is possible that
The Survival-Promoting Influence the trophic or toxic effects reflect distinct matura-
of Excitatory Amino Acids, As tional stages of the affected cells. Furthermore,
Ruijter and Baker (1990) have described condi-
Their Toxic Effect, Is Not Restricted
tions that are characterized by NMDA receptor
to the Cerebellar Granule Cells
stimulation exerting either a trophic or a toxic
It has long been known that EAA can exert toxic effect on nerve cells in organ cultures of cerebral
effects on nerve cells throughout the brain (e.g., cortex.
Meldrum and Garthwaite, 1990). Results are forth-
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 67

Trophiic Effects of Excitatory period of development within the kitten visual


Amino Acids Beyond Mere cortex in response to monocular deprivation
Survival of Nerve Cells (Kleinschmidt et aI., 1987; Rauschecker and Hahn,
1987; for review see Collingridge and Singer, 1990)
New observations indicate that EAA can exert
whereas, in the optic tectum of surgically pro-
trophic influences not only in terms of cell sur-
duced three-eyed tadpoles, APV caused desegrega-
vival, but also of advancement of maturation and
tion of the terminals of the retinal ganglion cells
of neurite outgrowth, also affecting nerve cells
(Cline et aI., 1987).
other than the cerebellar granule cells in the de-
The effect of NMDA on nerve cells is known
veloping brain (see various papers in connection
to be voltage-dependent (e.g., Mayer and West-
with both the trophic effe~t of EAA and their
brook, 1987). This property seems to be critical
involvement in plastic changes in the nervous
concerning the role of NMDA receptors both in
system in Balazs, 1990). For example, Pearce et ai.
the plastic changes and in the trophic effects of
(1987) have noted that NMDA receptor stimula-
EAA within the nervous system. This implies that
tion promotes neurite emission from cultured
Ca 2 + conductance through the NMDA receptor-
cerebellar granule neurones, whereas Moran and
linked ion channel, which is instrumental in the
Patel (1989) described an advancement of the
experience-dependent modulation of synaptic
biochemical maturation ofthese cells. Moreover,
connections, is subject to regulation by coopera-
it has also been noted that the trophic effect is
tive interaction with other afferent inputs. Inputs
cell type-specific: in subcortical cultures NMDA
may include afferents operating with EAA, which
appeared to promote the maturation of GABA-
also .stimulate postsynaptic receptors other than
and Glu-ergic, but not the cholinergic cells (Patel
the NMDA-preferring subtype.
et aI., 1990).
The involvement of EAA receptors (including
Glu can also influence the outgrowth of neurites
metabotropic receptors according to recent ob-
of hippocampal pyramidal cells, differentially
servations) in lasting alterations of synaptic trans-
affecting dendrites and axons in a dose-dependent
mission in adult brain structures, exemplified by
manner, and thus modulating the development of
long-term potentiation in the hippocampus or
neuronal shape (Mattson et aI., 1988). However,
the cerebral cortex and long-term depression in
it should be noted that in contrast to cerebellar
the cerebellum, is well documented (for reviews
granule cells, hippocampal neurones respond to
see Collingridge and Singer, 1990; Ito, 1989).
Glu by a reduction of dendritic growth rate and,
Furthermore, NMDA receptors, in particular,
although Ca 2 + seems to serve as the second mes-
seem to play an important role in certain forms
senger for both cell types, the receptors involved
of learning in both immature and adult animals
are the KA/QA preferring subtype. Morphologi-
(Lincoln et aI., 1988; Morris et aI., 1986).
cal maturation of cerebral cortical neurones has
also been claimed to be advanced in culture after Mechanisms Underlying Trophic
exposure to low concentrations of Glu (Aruffo Irifluences of Excitatory Amino Acid
et aI., 1987). on Developing Nerve Cells
There is evidence indicating that the toxic effect
Plastic Changes Induced
of EAA is mediated through an increase in the
by Excitatory Amino Acids
entry of extracellular calcium into granule cells
It seems that processes mediated by EAA are also (Garthwaite and Garthwaite, 1986b). Our obser-
involved in plastic changes in both the developing vations show that Ca 2 + influx also plays a critical
and the adult central nervous system, and that role in the trophic effects' of EAA: NMDA rescue
NMDA receptors again playa critical (although is inhibited by blocking the NMDA receptor-
presumably not exclusive) role. It has been shown linked ion channel, and thus the stimulated cal-
that during development these receptors influ- cium conductance (Balazs et aI., 1989), whereas
ence the fine-tuning of synaptic positions. Thus, the positive influence of non-NMDA receptor
NMDA receptor blockade prevents the ocular agonists (or high K +) on cell survival is com-
dominance shift normally seen during a restricted promised by reducing the availability of extra-
68 Robert Balazs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. J~rgensen

cellular calcium through the depolarization- kinase into the phosphorylated form: this may
activated VSCC (Balazs et al., 1990a, b, 1992; occur after l~ng or repeated bursts of electrical
Gallo et aI., 1987a). It seems, furthermore, that activity simulated by the experimental conditions
the DHP-sensitive L-type of VSCC plays an im- in our studies in which cells are chronically ex-
portant role in cell rescue by high K + or non- posed to high K + or EAA.
NMDA receptor agonists, although DHP block- Treatment of diverse cell types with agents
ade of these channels inhibits only a fraction of influencing growth and differentiation results in
the K + depolarization-induced 45Ca 2 + uptake the rapid and transient expression of a set of early
into granule cells (Carboni and Wojcik, 1988; response genes, which include the proto-oncogene
Kingsbury and Balazs, 1987). These findings imply c-fos (for review see Morgan and Curran, 1991).
that concerning the trophic influence, the site of c-fos is coding for a nuclear protein that, in com-
Ca 2 + entry is also relevant. In this respect, it bination with products of other early response
should be noted that the cell survival promoting genes such as lun, is involved in the regulation
effect of NMDA was resistant to NF, so that of gene expression. Glutamate or NMDA was
Ca2+ entering not only through the DHP-sen- found to induce the expression of c-fos in granule
sitive channels, but also through the NMDA cell cultures (Didier et aI., 1989; Szekely et aI.,
receptor-linked channels, can be effective. 1989). However, at the present time we do not yet
It is now well established that exposure to EAA know whether the EAA-induced expression of
or high K + elicits in granule cells, as in other c-fos is part of the mechanism underlying the
nerve cell types, an increase in the concentration trophic effect or is an epiphenomenon.
of free cytoplasmic Ca 2 +, which may involve, Finally, instead of a summary, the proposition
however, depending on the agonist, quite complex is put forward that processes involving EAA-
interactions (Courtney et aI., 1990). Furthermore, mediated transmission may playa role, besides
our recent observations have extended previous triggering plastic changes in the central nervous
findings (Gallo et aI., 1987a) indicating that after system throughout life, in underpinning the sur-
the elevation of [Ca2+L calmodulin-mediated vival not only of the cerebellar granule cells but
reactions are critically involved in the cascade, also of other types of nerve cells during critical
leading ultimately to cell survival (Balazs et aI., periods in their development. One of the charac-
1992). Ongoing investigations suggest that the teristic features ofthe developing nervous system
next step involves protein phosphorylation me- is an overproduction of nerve cells and their pro-
diated by the Ca 2 + -CaM-dependent protein jections including synaptic contacts, followed by
kinase II (CaM kinase II) (Balazs et aI., 1992). It a regression of the redundant elements. For in-
should be noted here that the toxic effect of EAA stance, pyramidal cells ofthe cerebral cortex make
on granule cells also seems to involve protein abundant transient projections during develop-
phosphorylation, but in this instance the mediator ment, even to areas (that may be quite distant)
is protein kinase C (see, e.g., Manev et aI., 1990). that they do not innervate in the adult (see, e.g.,
CaM kinase has certain properties that can Cowan et aI., 1984). As it is believed that many
facilitate its role in mediating the trophic effect of of the corticofugal projections are glutamatergic
EAA. The activation by Ca 2 + results in the auto- (Monaghan et aI., 1989), such redundant projec-
phosporylation of the kinase, which leads to the tions may fulfill a role by providing, at the right
release of the enzyme from the membrane-cyto- time, a trophic influence ensuring the survival of
skeleton complex and renders CaM kinase II their transynaptic partners.
independent from Ca 2 + (Miller and Kennedy,
1986; Saitoh and Schwartz, 1985). As a conse-
quence, the kinase may reach sites where protein Acknowledgments. R.B. and N.H. acknowledge
phosphorylation may elicit long-term effects and the support provided by ZWO and the Van den
the enzyme can even function under conditions Houten Foundation. O.S.l. received support for
when regulatory mechanisms keep the overall this work from the Danish Medical Research
free Ca2+ levels low. Furthermore, it seems that Council. Weare indebted to Michael Corner for
prolonged increases in [Ca2+1 are required in critically reading the manuscript and to Olga
order to convert a significant proportion of the Pach for typing it.
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 69

References Brenneman, D., Yu, c., and Nelson, P.G. (1990)


Altman, J. (1982): Morphological development of the Multi-determinate regulation of neuronal survival:
rat cerebellum and some of its mechanisms. In: The Neuropeptides, excitatory amino acids and bioelec-
Cerebellum: New Vistas. (S.L. Palay and V. Chan- trical activity. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci., 8, 371-378.
Palay, eds). Berlin: Springer, pp. 8-49. Carboni, E., and Wojcik, W.1. (1988): Dihydropyridine
Aruffo, c., Ferszt, R., Hildebrandt, A.G., and Cerwos- binding sites regulate calcium influx through spe-
Navarro, J. (1987): Low doses of L-monosodium cific voltage-sensitive calcium channels in cerebellar
glutamate promote neuronal growth and differentia- granule cells. J. Neurochem., 50, 1279-1286.
tion in vitro. Dev. Neurosci., 9, 228-239. Choi, D.W. (1987): Glutamate neurotoxicity in cortical
Asher, P., and Nowak, L. (1987): Electrophysiological cell cultures. J. Neurosci., 7, 357-368.
studies of NMDA receptors. Trends Neurosci., 10, Choi, D.W., Malucci-Gedde, M., and Kriegstein, A.R.
284-288. (1987): Glutamate neurotoxicity in cortical cell
Balazs, R. (ed), (1990): Plastic and trophic effects of culture. J. Neurosci., 7,357-368.
amino acid transmitters in the developing nervous Cline, H.T., Debski, E.A., and Constantine-Paton, M.
system. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci., 8(1), 345-503. (1987): N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist
Balazs, R., Hack, N., and J~rgensen, O.S. (1990a): desegregates eye-specific strips. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Selective stimulation of excitatory amino acid Sci. USA, 84, 4342-4345.
receptor subtypes and the survival of cerebellar Collingridge, G.L., and Singer, W. (1990): NMDA
granule cells in culture: Effect of kainic acid. receptors-their role in long-term potentiation.
Neuroscience, 37, 251-258. Pharmacol. Sci., 11, 290-296.
Balazs, R., Hack, N., and Jljilrgensen, O.s. (1990b): Cotman, C.W., and Monaghan, D.T. (1988): Excitatory
Interactive effects involving different classes of ex- amino acid neurotransmission: NMDA receptors
citatory amino acid receptors and the survival of and Hebb-type synaptic plasticity. Annu. Rev.
cerebellar gran,ule cells in culture. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci., 11,6-80.
Neurosci., 8, 347-358. Courtney, M.J., Lambert, J.1., and Nicholls, D.G.
Balazs, R., Hack, N., J~rgensen, O.S., and Cotman, (1990): The interactions between plasma membrane
C.W. (1989): N-methyl-D-aspartate promotes the depolarization and glutamate receptor activation in
survival of cerebellar granule cells: Pharmacological the regulation of cytoplasmic free calcium in
characterization. Neurosci. Lett., 101,241-246. cultured cerebellar granule cells. J. Neurosci., 10,
Balazs, R., Hack, N., Resink, A., Aronica, E., and Van 3873-3879.
der Valk, J.B.F. (1992): Trophic effect of excitatory Cowan, W.M., Fawcett, J.W., O'Leary, D.D.M., and
.amino acids on differentiating granule cells: Involve- Stanfield, B.B. (1984): Regressive events in neuro-
ment of calcium and other second messengers. Mol. genesis. Science, 225, 1258-1265.
N europharmacol. 2. Cull-Candy, S.G., Mathie, A., Symonds, c., and Wyllie,
Balazs, R., and Jljilrgensen, O.S. (1987): Trophic F. (1989): Noise and single channels activated by
function of excitatory transmitter amino acids. excitatory amino acids in rat cerebellar granule
Neuroscience, 22, S41. neurones. J. Physiol., 400, 189-222.
Balazs, R., J~rgensen, O.S., and Hack, N. (1988a): Cunningham, T.J. (1982): Naturally occurring neuron
Stimulation of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor death and its regulation by developing neural
has a trophic effect on differentiating cerebellar pathways. Int. Rev. Cytol., 74, 163-186.
granule cells. Neurosci. Lett., 87, 80-86. Didier, M.,Roux, P., Piechaczky, M., Verrier, B.,
Balazs, R., J~rgensen, O.s., and Hack, N. (1988b): Bockaert, J., and Pin, J.-P. (1989): Cerebellar granule
N-methyl-D-aspartate promotes the survival of cell survival and maturation induced by K + and
cerebellar granule cells in culture. Neuroscience, 27, NMDA correlate with c-fos proto-oncogene expres-
437-451. sion. Neurosci. Lett., 107, 55-62.
Bettler, B., Boulter, J., Hermans-Borgmeyer, I., Frandsen, A., Drejer, 1., and Schousboe, A. (1989):
O'Shea-Greenfeld, A., Deneris, E.S., Moll, C., Glutamate-induced 45Ca2+ uptake into immature
Borgmeyer, V., Hollmann, M., and Heinemann, S. cerebral cortex neurons shows a distinct pharma-
(1990): ·Cloning of a novel glutamate receptor cological profile. J. Neurochem., 53, 1959-
subunit, GluR5: Expression in the nervous system 1962.
during development. Neuron. 5, 583-595. Frandsen, A., and Schousboe, A. (1987): Time and
Bock, E., Divac, I., Norrild, B., Thorn, N.A., Torp- concentration dependency of the toxicity of
Pedersen, c., and Treiman, M. (1982): Synaptic excitatory amino acids on cerebral neurons in
membrane proteins in mammalian brain. Scand. J. primary culture. Neurochem. Int. 10, 583-591.
Immunol., 15, 223-240. Gallo, V., Kingsbury, A., Balazs, R., and J~rgensen,
70 Robert Bahlzs, Nicola Hack, and Ole S. J9Irgensen

O.S. (1987a): The role of depolarization in the activation and olfactory learning. Dev. Brain Res.,
survival and differentiation of cerebellar granule 39, 309-312.
cells in culture. J. Neurosci., 7, 2203-2213. Manev, H., Costa, E., Wroblewski, J.T., and Guidotti,
Gallo, V., Suergiu, R., Giovannini, c., and Levi, G. A. (1990): Abusive stimulation of excitatory amino
(1987b): Glutamate receptor subtypes in cultured acid receptors: A strategy to limit neurotoxicity.
cerebellar neurons: modulation of glutamate and F ASEB J., 4, 2789-2797.
gamma-amino butyric acid release. J. N eurochem., Manthorpe, M., Fagnani, R., Skaper, S.D., and Varon,
49,1801-1809. S. (1986): An automated colorimetric microassay
Garthwaite, G., and Garthwaite, J. (1986a): In vitro for neurotrophic factors. Dev. Brain Res., 25, 191-
neurotoxicity of excitatory amino acid analogues 198.
during cerebellar development. Neuroscience, 17, Maragos, W.F., Penney, J.B., and Young, A.B. (1988):
755-767. Anatomic correlation of NMDA and 3H-TCP-
Garthwaite, G., and Garthwaite, 1. (1986b): Neuro- labeled receptors in rat brain. J. Neurosci., 8,
toxicity of excitatory amino acid receptor agonists 493-501.
in rat cerebellar slice: dependence in calcium con- Mattson, M., Dou, P., and Kater, S.B. (1988): Out-
centration. Neurosci. Lett., 66, 193-198. growth-regulating actions of glutamate in isolated
Garthwaite, G., Yamini, B., Jr., and Garthwaite, J. hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J. Neurosci., 8,
(1987): Selective loss of Purkinje and granule cell 2087-2100.
responsiveness to N-methyl-o-aspartate in rat Mayer, M., and Westbrook, G.L. (1987): The
cerebellum during development. Dev. Brain Res., 36, physiology of excitatory amino acids in the
288-292. vertebrate central nervous system. Prog. N eurobiol.,
Hamburger, V., and Oppenheimer, R.W. (1982): 28, 197-276.
Naturally occurring neuronal death in vertebrates. Meier, E., Regan, C.M., and Balazs, R. (1984): Changes
Neurosci. Commun., 1, 39-55. in the expression of a neuronal surface protein
Honore, T., Davies, S.N., Drejer, 1.,. Fletcher, E.J., during development of cerebellar neurons in vivo
Jacobson, P., Lodge, D., and Nielsen, F.E. (1988): and in culture. J. N eurochem., 43, 1328-1335.
Quinoxalinediones: potent competitive non-NMDA Meldrum, B., and Garthwaite, J. (1990): Excitatory
glutamate receptor antagonists. Science, 241, amino acid neurotoxicity and neurodegenerative
701-703. disease. Trends Pharmacol. Sci., 11, 379-387.
Ito, M. (1989): Long-term depression. Annu. Rev. Miller, S.G., and Kennedy, M.B. (1986): Reglliation
Neurosci., 12, 85-105. of brain type II Ca 2 + jcalmodulin-dependent
J9Irgensen, O.S. (1983): D2-protein and D3-protein as protein kinase by autophosphorylation: A Ca 2 + -
markers for synaptic turnover and concentration. triggered molecular switch. Cell,44, 861-870.
J. Neur. Trans., 18, 245-255. Monaghan, D.T., Bridges, R.S., and Cotman, C.W.
Kingsbury, A., and Balazs, R. (1987): Effect of calcium (1989): The excitatory amino acid receptors: Their
agonists and antagonists on cerebellar granule cells. classes, pharmacology, and distinct properties in the
Eur. J. Pharmacol., 140, 275-283. function of the central nervous system. Annu. Rev.
Kingsbury, A., Gallo, V., and Balazs, R. (1988): Pharmacol. Toxicol., 29, 365-402.
Stimulus-coupled release of amino acids from Moran, J., and Patel, A.J. (1990): Stimulation of
cerebellar granule cells in culture. Brain Res., 448, N-methyl-o-aspartate receptor promotes the bio-
46-52. chemical differentiation of cerebellar granule
Kleinschmidt, A., Bear, M.F., and Singer, W. (1987): neurons and not astrocytes. Brain Res. 486, 215-
Blockade of"NMDA" receptors disrupts experience- 225.
dependent plasticity of kitten striate cortex. Morgan, J.J., and Curran, T. (1991): Stimulus-transmis-
SCience, 238, 355-358. sion coupling in the nervous system: Involvement
Lasher, R.S., and Zagon, I.S. (1972): The effect of of inducible proto-oncogenes fos and jun. Annu.
potassium on neuronal differentiation in cultures of Rev. Neurosci. 14,421-452.
dissociated newborn rat cerebellum. Brain Res., 41, Morris, R.G.M., Anderson, E., Lynch, G.S., and
482-488. Baudry, M. (1986): Selective impairment oflearning
Levi-Montalcini, R. (1982): Developmental neurobio- and blockade of long-term potentiation by an
logy and the natural history of nerve growth factor. N-methyl-o-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5:
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 341-361. Nature, 319, 774-776.
Lincoln, J., Coopersmith, R., Harris, E.W., Cotman, Novelli, A., Nicoletti, F., Wroblewski, J.T., Alho, H.,
C.N., and Leon, M. (1988): NMDA receptor Costa, E., and Guidotti, A. (1987): Excitatory amino
3. Granule Cells and Excitatory Amino Acids 71

acid receptors coupled with guanylate cyclase in autonomous enzyme in Aplysia neurons. J. Cell.
primary cultures of cerebellar granule cells. Bioi., 100, 835-842.
J. Neurosci., 7, 40-47. Somogyi, P., Halasy, K., Somogyi, J., Storm-Mathisen,
Nybroe, 0., Albrechtsen, M., Dahlin, J., Linnemann, S., and Ottersen, O.P. (1986): Quantification of
D., Lyles, J.M., Moller, e.S., and Bock, E. (1985): immunogold labeling reveals enrichment of glu-
Biosynthesis of the neuronal cell adhesion molecule: tamate in mossy and parallel fiber terminals in cat
Characterization of polypeptide e. J. Cell Bioi., 10, cerebellum. N eurosci., 19, 1045-1050.
1-6. Stone, T.W., and Burton, N.R. (1988): NMDA
Olverman, H.J., Jones, A.W., and Watkins, J.e. (1984): receptor and ligands in the vertebrate CNS. Prog.
L-glutamate has higher affinity than other amino Neurobiol., 30, 333-368.
acids for eH]-D-AP5 binding sites in rat brain Szekely, A.M., Barbaccia, M.L., Alho, H., and Costa,
membranes. Nature, 307, 460-462. E. (1989): In primary cultures of cerebellar granule
Patel, A.J., Hunt, A., and Sanfeliu, e. (1990): Cell-type cells the activation of N-methyl-o-aspartate-sensitive
specific effects of N-methyl-o-aspartate on biochemi- glutamate receptors induces c-fos mRNA expression.
cal differentiation of subcortical neurons in culture. Mol. Pharmacol., 35, 401-408.
Int. J. Dev. Neurosci., 8, 379-389. Thangnipon, W., Kingsbury, A., Webb, M., and Balazs,
Pearce, I.A., Cambray-Deakin, M.A., and Burgoyne, R. (1983): Observations on rat cerebellar cells in
R.D. (1987): Glutamate acting on NMDA receptors vitro: Influence of substratum, potassium concentra-
stimulate neurite outgrowth from cerebellar granule tion and relationship between neurones and
cells. FEBS Lett., 223, 143-147. astrocytes. Dev. Brain Res., 11, 177-189.
Rakic, P., and Sidman, R.L. (1973): Organization of Thoenen, H. (1991): The changing scene of neurotro-
the cerebellar cortex secondary to deficit of granule phic factors. Trends N eurosci., 14, 165-170.
cells in weaver mutant mice. J. Compo Neurol., 152, Trenker, E., Smith, D., and Segil, N. (1984): Is cerebellar
133-162. granule cell migration regulated by an internal
Rauschecker, J.P., and Hahn, S. (1987): Ketamine- clock? J. Neurosci., 4, 2850-2855.
xylazine anaesthesia blocks consolidation of ocular Varon, S., Pettman, B., and Manthorpe, M. (1988):
dominance changes in kitten visual cortex. Nature, Humoral and surface-anchored factors in develop-
326, 183-185. ment and repair of the nervous system. Progr. Brain
Rothman, S.M. (1983): Synaptic activity mediates Res., 78, 465-487.
death of hypoxic neurons. Science, 30, 536-537. Watkins, J.e., and Evans, R.H. (1981): Excitatory
Rothman, S.M., and Olney, J.W. (1987): Excitotoxicity amino acid transmitters. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol.
and the NMDA receptor. Trends Neurosci., 10, Toxicol. 21, 165-204.
299-302. Wong, E.H.F., Kemp, J.A., Priestley, T., Knight, A.R.,
Ruijter, J.M., and Baker, R.E. (1990): The effects of Woodruff, G.N., and Iversen, L.L. (1986): The anti-
potassium-induced depolarisation, glutamate recep- convulsant MK-801 is a potent N-methyl-o-aspar-
tor antagonists and N-methyl-o-aspartate on tate antagonist. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 83,
neuronal survival in cultured neocortex explants. 7104-7108.
Int. J. Dev. Neurosci., 8, 361-370. Wong, E.H.F., Knight, A.R., and Woodruff, G.N.
Saitoh, T., and Schwartz, J.H. (1985): Phosphorylation- (1988): [3H]MK-801Iabels a site on the N-methyl-
dependent subcellular translocation of Ca2+- o-aspartate receptor channel complex in rat brain
calmodulin-dependent protein kinase produces an membranes. J. Neurochem., 50, 274-281.
4
Microtubule-Associated Proteins in
Cerebellar Morphogenesis
Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck

Ramon y Cajal's Description growth of a neuritic process? What determines


of Neurogenesis in the Cerebellum whether a process becomes a dendrite or an axon?
Ramon y Cajal invariably refers to an "axis cylin-
In his studies of the cerebellum, Ramon y Cajal der," rather than an axon, to distinguish a mature
(1888, 1960) noted, with characteristic clarity, the process with a smooth outline and regular caliber
major features of morphogenesis in the various from the initial protoplasmic extension, which is
cell types. Many of his observations are now so highly irregular in outline and diameter. How is
familiar as to be commonplace, whereas others this change from one form to another achieved,
are less frequently cited, perhaps because they and what regulates the timing ofthis event? Recent
deal with cellular phenomena whose regulation work has implicated the cytoskeleton in these
is still not properly understood. For example, events, especially the microtubule network. By
Ramon y Cajal was the first to report that the studying the expression and cellular localization
growth ofaxons and dendrites involves several of microtubular proteins during nervous system
distinct steps. He observed that the neuroblast development, the molecular events that underlie
first extends short processes of rather irregular axon and dendrite growth and differentiation are
outline, as, for example, the granule cell does beginning to emerge.
during its initial horizontal, bipolar phase. Only
later do these initial protrusions narrow and
lengthen to form a proper "axis cylinder." Den- The Putative Role of the
drites appear later still. Thus, Purkinje cells have
already produced extensive, well formed axons
Cytoskeleton in Neurogenesis
long before dendrites appear. At this early stage The silver stain revealed to the pioneer light micro-
the Purkinje cell body is decorated with multiple scopists a feature of the internal organization of
short protoplasmic extensions that are subse- the neuron that we now know is crucially in-
quently resorbed to be replaced by the growth of volved in neurogenesis, the neurofibrillar network.
the mature dendritic tree. Neurogenesis thus in- Ramon y Cajal described its appearance in the
volves an initial outgrowth of short, irregular developing Purkinje cell in detail and was aware
processes followed by the formation of a distinct of its continuity from dendrites through the cell
axon and finally by the gradual development of body into the axon. He observed that when the
the dendrites. neurofibrils enter the axon they "are concentrated
A century after these events were first described and welded into a compact and intensely stained
there are still important unresolved questions fibre" (Ramon y Cajal, 1960).
about them. The most fundamental of these relate The neurofibrillar network stained in Ramon
to the cellular mechanisms that regulate these y Cajal's preparations is now known to comprise
morphogenetic events. What promotes the out- neurofilaments, the intermediate filaments of

72
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 73

neurons. Ramon y Cajal does not seem to have indirectly observed by Ramon y Cajal, now re-
entertained any idea of this neurofibrillar network vealed to be microtubules, thus appear to be
performing a cytoskeletal function, but he made essential determinants of the neuritic axis.
an observation that with hindsight strikes one as
extraordinarily prescient. He described distor-
tions ofthe neurofibrillar reticulum ofthe Purkinje The Putative Role of
cell in young mammals subjected to cold, and Microtubule-Associated Proteins
showed that the same phenomenon could be in-
duced experimentally in the embryonic avian cere-
in Axon and Dendrite Growth
bellum by chilling eggs before fixation. Although The above considerations lead readily to the idea
unable to observe them directly, Ramon y Cajal that whatever determines the state of assembly of
had made the first demonstration of the role of microtubules inside developing neurons must play
microtubules within neurons. Microtubules are an important part in determining neuronal mor-
known to disintegrate when exposed to cold, and phology. Recently, much attention has focused
it has recently been reported that the microtubules on the microtubule-associated proteins (MAPs)
found within the dendrites of developing Purkinje because several of these ha:ve been shown to lower
cells are particularly susceptible to low tempera- the critical concentration of tubulin needed for
ture-induced depolymerization (Faivre et aI., microtubule assembly in vitro (Cleveland et aI.,
1985). We can now speculate that the distortions 1977; Murphy and Borisy; see review by Olmsted,
observed by Ramon y Cajal were the result· of 1986). Molecules such as MAP2 and tau, as pro-
altered cellular morphology due to microtubule moters of microtubule assembly in dendrites and
disassembly, or were perhaps a reflection of the axons, are probably of great importance as stabi-
disruption of neurofilament-microtubule cross- lizers of both longitudinal and cross-sectional
bridges. Many decades after Ramon y Cajal's axis. It follows that any variation in their effec-
observations, it was demonstrated that conditions tiveness as promoters of tubulin polymerization
that induce microtubule disassembly, like cold could have a significant effect on neuronal mor-
and specific drug treatments, lead to the inhibition phology. Several findings speak in favor of such
of neurite outgrowth in primary cell cultures and modulatory changes in MAP properties. First, it
the retraction of neurites that had already formed is known that MAP2 and tau from developing
(Black and Greene, 1982; Daniels, 1972; Seeds brain are less effective in promoting tubulin poly-
et aI., 1970; Yamada et aI., 1970). These experi- merization than those from adult brain (Francon
ments demonstrated the importance of micro- et aI., 1982; Mareck et aI., 1980). This correlates
tubules during the initial phases of neuronal with the lesser stability of micro tubules in den-
morphogenesis. drites of Purkinje cells in developing cerebellum
Microtubules also contribute to the develop- compared to those of adult cerebellum (Faivre
ment of the distinct morphologies ofaxons and etaI., 1985). Second, the efficacy of MAP2 and
dendrites. When microtubules are depolymerized tau in promoting tubulin polymerization is pro-
by drugs (Bray et aI., 1978; Matus et aI., 1986) or foundly affected by phosphorylation, and both
other treatments (Anglister et aI., 1982), the neu- molecules are known to be highly phosphorylated
ronal processes not only shrink in length, but they in vivo (Murthy and Flavin, 1983; Tsuyama et aI.,
also lose their cylindrical form: the outline be- 1987).
comes irregular and lateral filopodia appear. In There is now a considerable body of additional
many ways this conversion resembles a reversal evidence indicating that MAPs are involved in
of the transition from initial outgrowth to "axis neuronal morphogenesis. One of the most strik-
cylinder" that Ramon y Cajal remarked on. These ing findings is the temporal correlation between
observations indicate that the micro tubules pro- the expression of different juvenile and adult
vide the basis ofthe structural support for neurite forms of neuronal MAPs and brain maturation
elongation, as well as the constraint that holds in (Matus, 1988). For example, in the developing
check the tendency to lateral spreading in the brain MAP2 is present as a high-molecular weight
neuritic cytoplasm. The cold-sensitive elements form, MAP2b, and a low molecular weight form,
74 Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck

MAP2c. Each form is encoded by a separate correlated with neurite growth, whereas expres-
mRNA derived from the single MAP2 gene sion of "late" MAPs, such as the high molecular
(Garner and Matus, 1988). In the developing brain weight MAP2a, is correlated with neurite stabili-
both MAP2b and MAP2c, with their respective zation (Matus, 1988).
mRNAs, are present, but in the adult brain MAP2c A third feature of the neuronal MAPs that
and its mRNA are reduced to trace levels (Garner implicates them in morphogenesis is their associa-
and Matus, 1988; Riederer and Matus, 1985). In tion with particular aspects of neuronal structure.
rat brain the disappearance of MAP2c, as well as The most striking case of this is the polarization
various other changes in MAP abundance and of high molecular weight MAP2 and tau in neurons
form, occur between post-natal days 10 and 20. throughout the brain, with MAP2 being concen-
It is during this period that axon and dendrite trated in dendrites whereas tau is localized in
growth come to an end and mature neuronal axons (Binder et aI., 1985; Caceres et aI., 1984; De
morphology is attained, suggesting that expres- Camilli et aI., 1984; Matus et aI., 1981; Tucker
sion of the "early" MAPs, such as MAP2c, is et aI., 1988a).

Figure 4.1. The cerebellar cortex


of an adult Xenopus laevis
stained with monoclonal anti-
bodies against MAP2 (A) and
tau (B). MAP2 is dendrite-
specific and tau is enriched in
axons in the amphibian nervous
system, just as they are in the
mammalian brain. gl, granule
cell layer; ml, molecular layer;
P, Purkinje cell body.
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 75

Evolutionary Conservation MAP1 MAP2


of MAP Properties
That these associations of timing and location are
no mere coincidence is shown by recent studies
of MAPs in other species. In the amphibian
Xenopus laevis the disappearance of MAP2c and
the appearance of MAP2a coincide with neuronal
maturation, that is, during metamorphosis, just
as it does in the rat (Viereck et ai., 1988). The
dendritic localization of high molecular weight
MAP2 and the axonal localization of tau have
been demonstrated in birds and amphibia in ad-
dition to mammals (Tucker et ai., 1988a; Viereck
et ai., 1988). The amphibian cerebellum has a
more primitive cytological structure than that of
birds and mammals: its granule cell dendrites do
not form postsynaptic "claws," the Purkinje cell
bodies do not form a monolayer, and their den- Figure 4.2. Adjacent sections through the adult rat
drites are less well organized than the stereotype cerebellum were stained with diluted hybridoma super-
of birds and mammals (Hillman, 1969; Sotelo, natants reactive with MAP! (A) or MAP2 (B). MAP!
1969). Despite the lack of these organizational is concentrated within the dendrites of Purkinje cells
features, high molecular weight MAP2 in Xenopus (P) and Golgi cells (G), whereas MAP2 is most abun-
laevis is exclusively dendritic, and tau is concen- dant within the cell bodies and dendrites of granule
trated within axons, exactly as they are in mam- cells (gl), as well as within varicose processes coursing
mals (Fig. 4.1). vertically through the molecular layer (arrowheads).
Note the absence of anti-MAP2 staining in the white
We have also found that the molecular forms
matter (wm).
of each of the known neuronal MAPs are closely
similar in mammals, birds, and amphibia (Tucker
et ai., 1988a, b; Viereck et ai., 1988). For example, anti-MAP2 staining of Purkinje cell dendrites
MAP2 is represented in all cases by a high mole- contrasted with the absence of staining of granule
cular weight doublet of around 200,000 Da and cell axons (Matus et ai., 1981). Later immuno-
a low molecular weight form (approximately histochemical studies showed that other MAPs
70,000 Da) that is found primarily in the develop- have different patterns of expression in cerebellar
ing nervous system. Thus, three of the fundamen- cells. Thus, MAP3, a 180,000-Da protein, is found
tal properties of neuronal MAPs, their molecular in astroglial cells and neurofilament-rich axons
form, cellular locati~n, and developmental regu- such as basket cell endings (Huber et ai., 1985).
lation, have been conserved throughout vertebrate In contrast, MAPS, a 320,000-Da "early" MAP,
evolution, indicating that these proteins play appears exclusively in neurons where it is present
fundamental roles in neuronal morphogenesis. in both axons and dendrites (Riederer et ai., 1986).
An interesting contrast exists between MAP1
and MAP2 expression in granule and Purkinje
MAP Expression in the Cerebellum cells. MAP1 is highly concentrated in Purkinje
The essential features of MAP expression in the cells, where it is present in both axons and den-
cerebellum have been described in a series of drites, but is much less abundant in granule cells
publications from our laboratory (e.g., Bernhardt (Huber and Matus, 1984). Exactly the opposite is
and Matus, 1982, 1984; Bernhardt et ai., 1985). the case for MAP2, which is much more abundant
The first indication that high molecular weight in granule cells than Purkinje cells. This is most
MAP2 was selectively associated with dendrites readily seen in sections stained with diluted hy-
was found in the cerebellar cortex, where strong bridoma supernatants (Fig. 4.2).
76 Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck

antibodies do not (Fig. 4.4). High molecular weight


MAP2 appears soon after MAP2c, at a time that
corresponds with the formation of dendrites
(Tucker et aI., 1988a, b). MAP1 levels, on the
other hand, remain low until the second week of
life and then slowly increase in step with neuronal
maturation (Riederer and Matus, 1985). The timing
of expression of MAP1 thus shows a much better
correlation with stabilization of neuronal struc-
ture than does that of MAP2.
In developing Purkinje cells of the rat, anti-
MAP2 staining is strong from birth onward, with
particularly high concentrations found in actively
growing domains of the dendritic tree (Fig. 4.5).
Figure 4.3. MAP2 is abundant in Purkinje cells in the Staining of growing Purkinje cell dendrites by
developing (embryonic day 12) quail cerebellum (A), antitubulin is far weaker. This could be because
but cannot be detected in Purkinje cell bodies or their tubulin epitopes in the developing dendrites are
dendrites in the mature (posthatching day 9) cerebellum
masked. However, a simpler explanation is that
(B). The staining within the remainder of the mature
cerebellum is indistinguishable from the rat. gl, granule
MAP2 concentrations rise faster than those of
cell layer; ml, molecular layer; P, Purkinje cell bodies;
wm, white matter.

In a recent study of the quail cerebellum, we


found that MAP2 is entirely absent from mature
Purkinje cell dendrites (Fig. 4.3). Three different
monoclonal antibodies as well as a polyclonal
antiserum failed to stain the quail Purkinje cell
dendrites, although neighboring interneurons
(granule, basket, Golgi, and stellate cells) were all
strongly stained, just as they are in the rat. It
seems that MAP2 is not essential for the mainte-
nance of an extensive dendritic arborization, nor
for the correct organization of microtubules within
these mature processes. MAP2 is, however, ex-
pressed in Purkinje cell dendrites-in the developing
quail cerebellum (Fig. 4.3). This suggests that the
major contribution of MAP2 to neuronal mor-
phology may be to dendritic development rather
than the maintenance of mature form.
A variety of observations support this conclu-
sion. First, there is the contrast between the early
appearance of MAP2 and the late appearance of Figure 4.4. Monoclonal antibodies that recognize the
low molecular weight form of MAP2 in addition to
MAPl. In the rat cerebellum, MAP2 appears as
high molecular weight MAP2 (monoclonal antibody
soon as neurons have finished their final mitosis C) stain the external granule layer of the postnatal day
and begin to differentiate. The form of MAP2 that 7 rat cerebellum (A). In contrast, antibodies specific for
appears first is MAP2c, since antibodies that re- the high molecular weight form of MAP2 (monoclonal
cognize this form ofMAP2 on Western blots will antibody AP14) stain only Purkinje cells and their
stain the external granular layer of the cerebellum, dendrites. P, Purkinje cells; ml, molecular layer; egl,
whereas high molecular weight MAP2-specific external granule layer.
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 77

cytoplasm (Matus et aI., 1986). This would imply


that the role of MAP2 in dendritic development
goes beyond merely promoting microtubule for-
mation.
A similar argument can be made for another
"early" MAP, MAP5. This protein is the earliest
major MAP to be expressed in neurons: it is
already present throughout the external granular
layer (Riederer et aI., 1986), and it appears in the
axons of both retinal ganglion cells and motor
neurons as soon as they form (Tucker and Matus,
1987,1988; Tucker et ai., 1988a). In vitro studies
with PC12 cells also support a role for MAP5 in
neurite outgrowth. When PC12 cells are induced
to grow processes by exposure to nerve growth
factor, MAP5 levels increase more than 10-fold
(Brugg and Matus, 1988). However, this MAP5
is not colocalized with microtubules until several
days after neurite growth. It may therefore initially
have some function other than or in addition to
its involvement with microtubules.

MAP2 Expression
in Cerebellum-Derived Cultures
Culture systems offer the opportunity to study
Figure 4.5. The developing rat cerebellum immuno- morphogenetic events with surrounding tissue
peroxidase stained with a polyelonal antiserum against
influences removed. We have examined two varie-
MAP2. Immunochemical analysis showed that this
ties that provide different circumstances: tissue
antiserum detects only high molecular weight MAP2.
In these sections, taken from cerebella of rats 2-days-
slice cultures (Giihwiler, 1981), where the histo-
old (B), 7-days-old (C), 12-days-old (A), and 21-days- typic organization is initially present but where
old (D), anti-MAP2 staining is very strong in the den- neurons are substantially denervated compared
drites of Purkinje cells bur is absent from surrounding to intact brain, and dispersed cell cultures, where
granule cell axons in the molecular layer (ml in A). individual neurons are completely removed from
Within Purkinje cells the anti-MAP2 staining appears histological influences and are totally denervated.
weaker in cell bodies (P 1- P 4 in A; asterisks in B, C, and Slice cultures were made from sagittal slices of
D) than in more distal areas ofthe dendritic tree (arrow- 19-day rat embryo cerebellum and cultured for
heads in C and D). A and E show cerebellar granular up to 3 weeks. In these cultures the Purkinje cells
layer in which granule cells and their processes (arrow-
are contacted by far fewer granule cell axons, both
heads in E) are particularly strongly stained. Bars = A,
because fat fewer granule cells complete their
30 J1m; D, 20 J1m; E, 15 J1m. egl, external germinal layer;
gel, granule cell layer.
migration and differentiation under these condi-
tions, and also because much of the Purkinje cell
innervation in the intact cerebellum originates
from granule cells outside their own sagittal plane.
tubulin in developing Purkinje cell dendrites In addition, a cerebellar slice lacks the climbing
(Bernhardt and Matus, 1982). This observation fiber input from the inferior olive.
is supported by a study of MAP2 and tubulin in Under these circumstances, Purkinje cells sur-
the dendrites of hippocampal neurons developing vive in reasonable numbers and their placement
in culture where MAP2 is not restricted to micro- within the tissue is close to normal, with the cell
tubules, but is present throughout the dendritic bodies roughly but not exactly in line (Fig. 4.6A).
78 Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck

Figure 4.6. Slice cultures of rat cere-


bellum showing the organization of
MAP2. The same antiserum was used
as in Fig. 4.5 but with immunofluo-
rescence detection. After 10 days in
culture these cerebellar slices have
attained the major histotypic features
of the cerebellar cortex, with Purkinje
cell bodies (P) mostly aligned in a
planar array (A) and unipolar
dendrites having formed (d). Granule
neurons (g) are sparse and scattered.
Bars = A and B, 30 J.lm; C, 20 11m.

As in whole cerebellum, the granule cells migrate nate abruptly (Fig. 4.6A, B). The dendrites stain
to form an inner granular layer but this does not strongly with anti-MAP2, indicating that the
have the strictly sub ganglionic nature found in molecule is abundantly produced despite the lack
intact brain and neither are all the granule cells of innervation and hypotrophy. The cell body is
clustered into groups. Instead, the granule cells also full of MAP2 in contrast to Purkinje cells in
occupy a broad area on both sides of the line of intact brain where there is a tendency for anti-
Purkinje cell bodies (Fig. 4.6A) and the cell bodies MAP2 staining to be more concentrated in the
are scattered rather than clustered. The effect of distal primary and secondary dendrites (Fig. 4.5).
this loss of innervation and his to typic abnormality The morphology of granule cells in slice cultures
has interesting consequences for neuronal mor- is also abnormal. Although readily recognizable
phogenesis and for the organization of the cyto- by their typical small, spherical cell body (Fig. 4.6),
skeleton, at least as far as the distribution of these granule cells do not form the short, claw-
MAP2 is concerned. In slice cultures the Purkinje shaped dendrites characteristic of these cells in
cell dendrites are stunted. Primary dendrites are intact cerebellum, but instead form longer MAP2-
formed but give rise to ~ather few secondary rich processes (see Figs. 4.6B, D, in particular).
branches, which are abnormally short and termi- Despite these abnormalities, the anti-MAP2 stain-
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 79

Figure 4.7. Cerebellar granule cells in dispersed cell with anti-MAP2. n, neuronal cell bodies. Arrowheads
culture showing the distribution ofMAP2 detected by mark the ends of initial MAP2-rich segment of the
immunofluorescence with the same polyc\onal antibody processes seen by phase contrast in B. Bar = 20 /lm.
as in Figs. 4.5 and 4.6. A, Immunofluorescence image

ing in these slice cultures resembles that of intact within the processes ofthese isolated granule cells
brain in two important respects: it is still limited is independent of the distribution oftubulin, anti-
to neurons and appears only in dendrites. This tubulin staining being present throughout the
indicates that whatever the nature of the mech- length of the entire process (Alaimo-Beuret and
anism that targets MAP2 to dendrites, it is not Matus, 1985).
sensitive to loss of innervation even when this has These observations demonstrate that the parti-
significant consequences for the development of tioning of MAP2 into separate microdomains of
neuronal morphology. the neuronal cytoplasm can occur within a single
An interesting extension to these observations process. Interestingly, MAP2 is at first present
can be drawn from dispersed cell cultures from the throughout emerging processes of cultured granule
cerebellum (Fig. 4.7). These consist overwhelm- cells; only later does it become restricted to the
ingly of granule cells and do not contain Purkinje initial segment (Alaimo-Beuret and Matus, 1985;
cells (Burgoyne and Cambray-Deakin, 1988). As see also Burgoyne and Cam bray-Deakin, 1988).
in the slice cultures, the granule cells develop cell Since these cells develop in isolation, it appears
bodies of the characteristic size and spherical that the partitioning of MAP2 in the cytoplasm
form even when denied contact with other cells is under the control of an endogenous genetically
(Fig. 4.7) (see Alaimo-Beuret and Matus, 1985, determined program that is independent of the
and Burgoyne and Cambray-Deakin, 1988). How- actual morphology attained by the cell. Thus,
ever, their processes are abnormal in an interesting although MAP2 may well contribute to the struc-
way. These have a varicose initial portion followed ture of the dendrites in which it normally occurs,
by a long, thin unbranched process (Fig. 4.7B). it alone is not sufficient to induce a separate
This distal domain ofthe process closely resembles dendritic process.
a granule cell axon even to the extent that it
frequently bifurcates at some distance from the
cell body (Fig. 4.7B) in a manner reminiscent of
a parallel fiber. The axonlike nature of this distal
Tubulin Isotypes in the Cerebellum
process is further suggested by the observation Recently, the cerebellum has been used as a model
that it is always devoid of MAP2 (Fig. 4.7A). system for describing the distribution of beta-
Instead, MAP2 is limited to the initial varicose tubulin isotypes. Five distinct beta-tubulin forms
portion o( the process. This partitioning of MAP2 are expressed in mouse brain; they can be dis-
80 Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck

MAP2

MAP1 MAP5

Figure 4.8. Adjacent sagittal sections through the cere- hybridization within the external granular layer (egl)
bellum of a postnatal day 12 rat were hybridized with and granular layer, the white matter (wm) and gray
cDNAs specific for beta-tubulin (A), high molecular matter, and the cerebellum and inferior colliculus (ic).
weight MAP2 (B), MAPI (C), and MAP5 (D) tran- n, deep cerebellar nucleus.
scripts. Note the differences in the relative amount of

tinguished from one another by variable amino These observations also emphasize the impor-
acid sequences at the carboxy terminus. Since tance of MAPs in generating functional hetero-
many MAPs are known to bind to tubulin near geneity within the neuronal microtubule network.
the carboxy terminus, Burgoyne and colleagues Although tubulin may contribute to microtubule
(1988) undertook a comparative immunohisto- function in a way that is not yet understood, the
chemical study of the distribution of MAPs and major role of this protein appears to act as the
beta-tubulin forms in the cerebellum. Although backbone of a filament system that is regulated
they demonstrated that these forms display cellu- and given diverse functions by MAPs.
lar specificity (e.g., isotype 5 is concentrated in
astrocytes, and isotype 6 is found exclusively in
neurons), there was little evidence of compart- MAP mRNA in the
mentalization ofisotypes within any given cell or Developing Cerebellum
colocalization of any isotype with the major brain
MAPs. This observation is in agreement with the We have recently completed an in situ hybridiza-
work of Lewis et al. (1987) and Lopota and tion study of the location of MAP mRNAs in the
Cleveland (1987), who used tissue culture cells to developing rat brain. The results clearly show
demonstrate that beta-tubulin isotypes do not that the developmental regulation of MAP ex-
sort into distinct microtubules. One must con- pression is taking place at the transcriptional
clude from these studies that the compartmentali- level. For example, comparing the density of the
zation of MAPs within neurons is not regulated cDNA hybridization signal between the external
by the distribution of beta-tubulin isotypes. and internal granular layers shows that the mRNA
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 81

for the "early" MAPS is more abundant in the primitive function, possibly associated with tubulin
zone of granule cell proliferation and differentia- binding and the stimulation of polymerization.
tion, whereas the mRNAs for both high molecular However, the amino termini of tau and MAP2
weight MAP2 and MAP 1 are more abundant in differ greatly. This bipartite structure, a common
the internal granular layer, where more mature carboxy terminus and disparate amino termini,
granule cells are found (Fig. 4.8). Another obser- lends itself to speculation concerning functional
vation made within the cerebellum concerns the significance. Certainly the close homology of the
relative abundance of hybridization within gray tubulin-binding domains rules out the possibility
and white matter. The MAP2 eDNA probe does that binding to different classes of microtubules
not hybridize significantly within white matter, is responsible for their partitioning into axons
whereas the MAPI eDNA probe does. This indi- and dendrites. Presumably, the specific functions
cates that MAP 1 is expressed in both neurons of tau in axons and MAP2 in dendrites must be
and oligo den droglial cells, whereas MAP2 is connected with the primary structure ofthe amino
neuron-specific. terminal domains.
The most striking feature to emerge from the
in situ hybridization studies is the clear indication
that the mRNA for MAP2 is found in dendrites Conclusions
(Garner et aI., 1988). This has been most clearly
Our growing knowledge of the microtubule-
demonstrated in the developing cerebral cortex,
associated proteins, their specific localization in
where dendrites are bundled and have a distinctive
axons or dendrites, and the temporal regulation
radial orientation. The autoradiographic signal
of their expression being so closely linked to pro-
with the MAP2 eDNA probe is unequivocally
cess outgrowth or stabilization in many species
associated along the length of these processes,
all indicate a fundamental role for these proteins
and not in the pyramidal cell bodies, where the
in neuronal morphogenesis. The recent introduc-
hybridization with a beta-tubulin eDNA probe
tion of recombinant DNA techniques for study-
produces a punctate pattern. MAP2 mRNA is
ing brain MAPs has provided insights into the
apparently exported so that protein synthesis can
mechanisms that regulate their expression and
occur in the dendrite, whereas for tubulin the
has also begun to reveal the sites at which this
protein is synthesized in the cell body and then
regulation can occur. The power of these techni-
transported. The presence of MAP2 mRNA in
ques promises greater insights, as the recent elu-
dendrites has interesting implications for the po-
cidation of the MAP2 and tau tubulin-binding
tential -regulation of MAP2 synthesis: it seems
domains show. For the same reasons that Ramon
possible that the rate of synthesis may be modu-
y CajaI's attention was turned to the cerebellum
lated immediately and locally within the process,
when he sought to unravel the complexities of
perhaps via cues provided from the afferent path-
brain interconnectivity, that is, its orderly organi-
ways.
zation and development and distinct fields of
axons and dendrites, future studies designed to
Implication of MAP manipulate MAP gene expression experimentally
Chemical Structure will again turn to this system to determine the
precise function of the microtubule network in
The primary sequences of tau and MAP2 have
neuronal morphogenesis.
now been determined (Goedert et aI., 1988; Lee
et aI., 1988; Lewis et aI., 1988), and it transpires
that the tubulin binding sites of MAP2 and tau References
are essentially identical, consisting of 18 amino
Alaimo-Beuret, D., and Matus, A. (1985): Changes in
acid repeats near the carboxy termini ofthe mole-
the cytoplasmic distribution of microtubule-asso-
cules (Lewis et aI., 1988). Indeed, these data show ciated protein 2 during the differentiation of cultured
that the entire carboxy terminal domains of MAP2 cerebellar granule cells. Neuroscience, 14, 1103-1115.
and tau are highly homologous, indicating that Anglister, L., Farber, I.e., Shahar, A., and Grinvald,
these molecules are probably derived from a com- A. (1982): Localization of voltage-sensitive calcium
mon ancestral gene and also suggesting a related channels and developing neurites: Their possible
82 Andrew Matus, Richard P. Tucker, and Christopher Viereck

role in regulating neurite elongation. Dev. Bioi., 94, Faivre, c., Legrand, Ch., and Rabie, A. (1985): The
351-365. microtubule apparatus of cerebellar Purkinje cells
Bernhardt, R., and Matus, A. (1982): Initial phase of during postnatal development of the rat: The density
dendrite growth: Evidence for the involvement of and cold-stability of micro tubules increase with age
high molecular weight microtubule-associated pro- and are sensitive to thyroid hormone deficiency. Int.
teins (HMWPs) before the appearance of tubulin. J. Dev. Neurosci., 3, 559-565. .
J. Cell Bioi., 92, 589-593. Francon, 1., Lennon, A.M., Fellous, A., Marek, A., Pierre,
Bernhardt, R., Huber, G., and Matus, A. (1985): Dif- M., and Nunez, J. (1982): Heterogeneity of micro-
ferences in the developmental patterns of three micro- tubule-associated proteins and brain development.
tubule-associated proteins in the rat cerebellum. Eur. J. Biochem., 129,465-471.
J. Neurosci., 5,977-991. Giihwiler, B.H. (1981): Morphological differentiation
Bernhardt, R., and Matus, A. (1984): Light and electron of nerve cells in thin organotypic cultures derived
microscopic studies of the distribution of microtubule- from rat hippocampus and cerebellum. Proc. R. Soc.
associated protein 2 in rat brain: A difference between B, 211, 287-290.
the dendritic and axonal cytoskeletons. J. Compo Garner, C.C., and Matus, A. (1988): Different forms of
Neurol., 226, 203-221. microtubule-associated protein 2 are encoded by
Binder, L.I., Frankfurter, A., and Rebhun, L.I. (1985): separate mRNA transcripts. J. Cell Bioi., 106, 779-
The distribution of tau in the mammalian nervous 783.
system. J. Cell Bioi., 101,1371-1378. Garner, c.c., Tucker, R.P., and Matus, A. (1988): Specific
Black, M.M., and Greene, L.A. (1982): Changes in the localization of mRNA for the cytoskeletal protein
colchicine susceptibility of microtubules associated MAP2 in dendrites. Nature, 336, 674-677.
with neurite outgrowth: Studies with nerve growth Goedert, M., Wischek, C.M., Crowther, R.A., Walker,
factor responsive PC12 pheochromocytoma cells. J.E., and Klug, A. (1988): Cloning and sequencing of
J. Cell Bioi., 95, 379-386. the cDNA encoding a core protein of the paired
Bray, D., Thomas, c., and Shaw, G. (1978): Growth helical filaments of Alzheimer disease: Identification
cone formation in cultures of sensory neurons. Proc. as the microtubule-associated protein tau. Proc. N atl.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 81, 5626-5629. Acad. Sci. USA, 85, 4051-4055.
Brugg, B., and Matus, A. (1988): PC12 cells express Hillman, D.E. (1969): Neuronal organization of the
juvenile microtubule-associated proteins during nerve cerebellar cortex in amphibia and reptilia. In: N euro-
growth factor-induced neurite outgrowth. J. Cell biology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development
Bioi., 107, 643-650. (R. L1inas, ed). Chicago: AMA-ERF Institute for
Burgoyne, R.D., and Cambray-Deakin, M.A. (1988): Biomedical Research, pp. 279-324.
The cellular neurobiology of neuronal development: Huber, G., Alaimo-Beuret, D., and Matus, A. (1985):
The cerebellar granule cell. Brain Res. Rev., 13, 77- MAP3: Characterization of a novel microtubule-
101. associated protein. J. Cell Bioi., 100,496-507.
Burgoyne, R.D., Cambray-Deakin, M.A., Lewis, S.A., Huber, G., and Matus, A. (1984): Differences in the cel-
Sarkar, S., and Cowan, N.J. (1988): Differential dis- lular distribution ofthe two microtubule-associated
tribution of beta-tubulin isotypes in cerebellum. proteins, MAP 1 and MAP2, in rat brain. J. N eurosci.,
EMBO J., 7, 2311-2319. 4, 151-160.
Caceres, A., Binder, L.I., Payne, M.R., Bender, P., Lee, G., Cowan, N., and Kirschner, M. (1988): The
Rebhun, L., and Steward, O. (1984): Differential sub- primary structure and heterogeneity of tau protein
cellular localization oftubulin and the microtubule- from mouse brain. Science, 239, 285-288.
associated protein MAP2 in brain tissue revealed by Lewis, S.A., Gu, W., and Cowan, N.J. (1987): Free
immunohistochemistry with monoclonal hybridoma intermingling of beta-tubulin isotypes among
antibodies. J. Neurosci., 4,394-410. functionally distinct microtubules. Cell, 49, 539-
Cleveland, D.W., Hwo, S.Y., and Kirschner, M.W. 548.
(1977): Physical and chemical properties of purified Lewis, S.A., Wang, D., and Cowan, N.J. (1988): Micro-
tau-factor and the role of tau in microtubule assem- tubule-associated protein MAP2 shares a micro-
bly. J. Mol. Bioi., 116, 227-247. tubule binding motif with tau protein. Science, 242,
Dariiels, M.P. (1972): Colchicine inhibition of nerve 936-939.
fiber formation in vitro. J. Cell BioI., 53,164-176. Lopota, M.A., and Cleveland, D.W. (1987): In vivo
De Camilli, P., Miller, P.E., Navone, F., Theurkauf, microtubules are co-polymers of available beta-
W.E., and Vallee, R.B. (1984): Distribution of micro- tubulin isotypes: Localization of each of six verte-
tubule-associated protein 2 in the nervous system of brate beta-tubulin isotypes using polyclonal anti-
the rat studied by immunofluorescence. Neuroscience, bodies elicited by synthetic peptide antigens. J. Cell
11,819-846. Bioi., 105,1707-1720.
4. Microtubule-Associated Proteins in Cerebellar Morphogenesis 83

Mareck" A., Fellows, A., Francon, J., and Nunez, J. Seeds, N.W., Gilman, A.G., Amino, T., and Nirenberg,
(1980): Changes in composition and activity of M.N. (1970): Regulation of axon formation by clonal
microtubule-associated proteins during brain lines of a neuronal tumor. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci.
development. Nature, 284, 353-355. USA,66,160-167.
Matus, A. (1988): Microtubule-assoeiated protein: Sotelo, C. (1969) Ultrastructural aspects ofthe cerebellar
Their potential role in determining neuronal mor- cortex of the frog. In: Neurobiology of Cerebellar
phology. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 11, 29-44. Evolution and Development (R. Liinas, ed). Chicago:
Matus, A., Bernhardt, R., and Hugh-Jones, T. (1981): AMA-ERF Institute for Biomedical Research,
High molecular weight microtubule associated pro- pp.327-367.
tein are preferentially associated with dendritic Tsuyama, S., Terayama, Y., and Matsuyama, S. (1987):
microtubules in brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, Numerous phosphates of microtubule-associated
78, 3013-3014. protein 2 in living rat brain. J. BioI. Chem., 262,
Matus, A., Bernhardt, R., Bodmer, R., and Alaimo, D. 10886-10892.
(1986): Microtubule-associated protein 2 and tubulin Tucker, R.P., Binder, L.I., and Matus, A.I. (1988a):
are differentially distributed in the dendrites of Neuronal microtubule-associated proteins in the
developing neurons. Neuroscience, 17, 371-389. embryonic avian spinal cord. J. Compo Neurol., 271,
Murphy, R.A., and Borisy, G.G. (1975): Associated of 44-55.
high molecular weight protein with microtubules Tucker, R.P., Binder, L.I., Viereck, c., Hemmings, B.A.,
and their role in microtubule assembly in vitro. Proc. and Matus, A. (1988b): The sequential appearance
Nat!. Acad. Sci. USA, 72, 2696-2700. of low- and high-molecular weight forms of MAP2
Murthy, A.S., and Flavin, M. (1983): Microtubule in the developing cerebellum. J. N eurosci., 12,4503-
assembly using microtubule-associated protein 4512.
MAP-2 prepared in defined states of phosphorylation Tucker, R.P., and Matus, A.I. (1987): The molecular
with protein kinase and phosphatase. Eur. J. Bio- form and distribution of two deveiopmentally-
chem., 137, 37-46. regulated microtubule proteins (MAP5 and MAP2)
Olmsted, J. (1986): Microtubule-associated proteins. during the morphogenesis ofthe avian retina. Devel-
Annu. Rev. Cell Bioi., 2, 421-457. opment, 101, 535-546.
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1888): Estructura del cerebeio de Tucker, R.P., and Matus, A. (1988): Microtubule-
las aves. Rev. Trim. Histol., No.2. associated proteins characteristic of embryonic brain
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1960): Studies on Vertebrate Neuro- are found in the adult mammalian retina. Dev. Bioi.,
genesis. (Trans. L. Guth). Springfield, IL: Charles C 130, 423-434.
Thomas, 1960. Viereck, c., Tucker, R.P., Binder, L.I., and Matus, A.
Riederer, B., and Matus, A. (1985): Differential expres- (1988): Phylogenetic conservation of brain micro-
sion of distinct microtubule-associated proteins tubule-associated proteins MAP2 and tau. Neuro-
during brain development. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci. science, 26, 893-904.
USA, 82, 6006-6009. Yamada, K.M., Spooner, B.S., and Wessels, M.W.
Riederer, B., Cohen, R., and Matus, A. (1986): MAP5: (1970): Axon growth: Role of microfilaments and
A novel brain microtubule-associated protein under microtubules. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA,66, 1206-
strong developmental regulation. J. Neurocytol., 15, 1212.
763-775.
5
Cerebellar Grafting as a Tool to
Analyze New Aspects of
Cerebellar Development and Plasticity
Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

The cerebellum, especially the cerebellar cortex, growing Purkinje cell dendrites are needed for
is one of the central nervous system regions in the normal acquisition of the tridimensional ar-
which ordered organizational patterns are obvi- rangement of the dendrites (Legrand 1982, 1983;
ous. Its apparent simplicity has attracted many in- Sotelo 1978) and for the stabilization of the pre-
vestigators interested in the mechanisms involved synaptic parallel fibers (Sotelo and Changeux,
in the development of the nervous system. A 1974).
hundred years ago, the study of the cerebellum Neural grafting is a valuable approach to the
of nonmammalian species, and particularly the study of cerebellar development, since it allows
study of its development, allowed Ramon y Cajal one to bring together immature and adult neural
(1888a-c) to obtain the first direct proof of the components. The study of the developmental steps
independence of nerve cells, leading to the form4- of the grafted embryonic neurons will determine
lation of the "Neuron Theory." In the cerebellum, whether or not the age mismatch between inter-
Ramon y Cajal (1890, 1911) also accurately des- acting partners prevents the immature neurons
cribed for the first time the processes of neuronal from proceeding with their developmental pro-
migration and neuronal differentiation as well as gram.
the role of regressive processes during the forma- Neurons are highly differentiated cells that lose
tion of specific neuronal networks. From these their proliferative ability at a distinct step of their
and other studies, it has been concluded that the development; loss of neurons cannot be compen-
precise spatial positioning of the neurons- percei- sated for by proliferation ofthe surviving neurons.
ved as a static arrangement in the adult animal- One experimental way to palliate the loss of neu-
is the result of strict temporal organization during ronal populations in the adult cerebellum is to
development. Neuronal proliferation and migra- replace the missing neurons by grafting homotypic,
tion, together with dendritic differentiation and young postmitotic neurons, taken from isogeneic
axonal outgrowth, proceed according to strictly embryos. In systems organized in a "point-to-
defined timing and kinetics, resulting in the estab- point" manner like the cerebellum (Sotelo and
lishment of complex neuronal networks with a Alvarado-Mallart, 1986), neuronal replacement
high degree of specificity. Furthermore, the pro- is effective only when the grafted neurons succeed
gression of most ifnot all of these sequential steps in reestablishing the anatomical and functional
takes place through cell-to-cell interactions, ap- integrity of the deficient networks by constituting
parently programmed to occur at precise matu- an equivalent synaptic circuit. Such a recon-
rati'onal stages of both neural partners. Indeed, stitution is only possible when the embryonic
absence or even delay in maturation of one of neurons grafted into the adult deficient cere-
these partners results in structural changes of the bellum are able to pursue their developmental
cerebellar circuits. For example, proper inter- program in such a way that the cell-to-cell inter-
actions between developing parallel fibers and actions between immature and adult neurons

84
5. Cerebellar Grafting 85

recapitulate those taking place during normal t2-day-old (E12) mouse embryos (C57BL), and
ontogeny. the hosts were 3- to 4-month-old homozygous
In this chapter, we first report results obtained ped mice with a C57BL genetic background. In
with long-term survivals after cerebellar grafting a few experiments, 22- to 27-day-old ped mice
into the cerebellum of adult mutant mice devoid were used as hosts. Two grafting procedures were
of Purkinje cells, to provide evidence that neuro- performed (Fig. 5.1): either the cerebellar primor-
nal replacement and subsequent synaptic inte- dia were mechanically dissociated in tissue culture
gration does occur. Thereafter, we describe results medium and the cell suspensions used for grafting,
obtained with short-term survivals to analyze the or the primordia were sliced into small pieces,
developmental steps, and their temporal organi- and individual pieces were used in solid trans-
zation, that are followed by the grafted immature plantation. In both instances, the embryonic
Purkinje cells to achieve the neuronal replace- material was injected with small glass pipettes
ment. Finally, we compare the cellular mechanisms at variable depths within the parenchyma of the
underlying this replacement with those governing host cerebellum. The obtained results were almost
the normal ontogeny ofthe cerebellum. This com- identical with the two procedures and, therefore,
parison permits the conclusion that embryonic they have been pooled together.
and adult neurons do interact, according to a In order to repair the cerebellar circuits of the
tempo imposed by the immature grafted Purkinje ped mouse, the Purkinje cell substitution needs
cells, which follow a predetermined pattern of to achieve three essential prerequisites: a) from
maturation, apparently regulated by an internal all the postmitotic neurons and progenitors pre-
clock. sent in the grafts, only neurons of the Purkinje
cell category must leave the implanted cellular
mass and move to the correct position previously
Long-Term Survivals occupied by the missing Purkinje cells. Indeed,
The biological system we have used to determine the restoration of the cortical circuits implies that
the degree of neuronal replacement, and therefore the embryonic Purkinje cells are able to integrate
the degree of structural restoration of the cere- themselves, as the missing link, into the host defi-
bellar circuits resulting from neural grafting, has cient network, b) once they reach the proper loca-
been the cerebellum of adult mice affected by the tions, the grafted neurons must not only follow
Purkinje cell degeneration (ped) mutation. In the their differentiation program and build up their
ped mouse (Landis and Mullen, 1978; Mullen et dendritic trees, but must participate in synapto-
aI., 1976), the cerebellum develops normally until genesis with specific host afferents. These develop-
the end of the second postnatal week. Between mental events would lead to the synaptic integra-
P14 and P45 virtually all Purkinje cells degenerate tion of the grafted Purkinje cells into the deficient
(less than 120 of these neurons remain in 3-month- cortical circuitry, and c) finally, no functional
old mutants, most ofthem located in the nodulus) improvement can be accomplished ifthe disrupted
(Wassef et aI., 1986). Although all Purkinje cells corticonuclear projection is not reestablished. For
degenerate in a relatively short period of time, that, the grafted Purkinje cells need to grow axons
their susceptibility to the pathologic process is that, navigating throughout the adult host cere-
positionally determined (Wassef et aI., 1987). The belllum, reach their appropriate targets in the
cerebellar cortex of mutants aged from 20 to 27 deep nuclei, and synaptically contact the proper
days can be subdivided into compartments con- host nuclear neurons.
taining an almost normal number of Purkinje
cells and apposed compartments where most of Selective Invasion of Grafted Purkinje Cells:
them hav.e disappeared. Thus, at these ages, the
The Role of Competition with
surviving Purkinje cells are arranged in a repro-
ducible pattern of longitudinal zones, which is
Host Purkinje Cells
symmetric relative to the midline. By staining Purkinje cells with immunohisto-
In most of the experiments reported here, donor chemistry [using selective markers: either an anti-
tissue was taken from cerebellar primordia of body against calbindin (Jande et aI., 1981) or
86 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

2
Figure 5.1. Schematic representation of the grafting Figure 5.2. Midsagittal section of the cerebellum of
procedures, indicating the age of the donor cerebellar an adult pcd mouse 3 months after grafting. The material
primordium (E12 mouse) and the two methods of graft- has been immunostained with an antibody against cal-
ing. In the upper part, the arrows point to the various bindin. This light micrograph illustrates the areas of
steps in obtaining cellular suspensions, and their host molecular layer occupied by the grafted Purkinje
implantation into the adult host cerebellum. The cells. Bar = 600 flm .
large arrow in the lower part indicates the direct implan-
tation of solid pieces of cerebellar primordium.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 87

against cyclic guanosine monophosphate-depen- taken from a mouse bearing one allele is trans-
dent protein kinase (De Camilli et ai., 1984)], we planted into the cerebellum of an adult host mouse
have been able to identify grafted Purkinje cells that is of the other allelic form (as has been illus-
in the host cerebella 2 to 5 months after implanta- trated in the hippocampus by Zhou et ai., 1985).
tion. Portions of the grafts evolve into small Since the C57BL strain bearing the pcd muta-
remnants still containing some Purkinje cells. tion is homozygous for Thy-1.2, we have used as
However, the vast majority ofthese neurons have donors 12-day-old embryos of the AKR strain,
moved out and occupy the host molecular layer homozygous for Thy-l.l (Fig. 5.3). Although
(Fig. 5.2), with ectopic cell bodies dispersed C57BL and AKR strains differ in their major histo-
throughout the superficial four fifths of this layer, compatibility antigen haplotypes (Green, 1981),
in several folia in direct contact with the graft the transplants survived for 1 to 2 months in the
remnant. In the largest transplants, the diameter cerebellum of the adult pcd mice. The Thy-l.1
of the spread is about 1.4 mm, suggesting that alloantigenic determinant was visualized immu-
embryonic Purkinje cells can migrate through the nohistochemically by using the OX7 monoclonal
adult cerebellum for distances as long as 0.7 mm. antibody (Mason and Williams, 1980). The graft
One of the most important observations in this remnants comprise Thy-1.1 immunoreactive neu-
study has been to establish that the invasion of rons disposed in a disorganized manner, giving
grafted neurons is not random. Only neurons of them the appearance of dense immunopositive
the category of the missing ones penetrate the masses, surrounded by immunonegative host
host parenchyma, whereas the other grafted neu'- tissue. In the host cortex, within 1.4 mm around
ronal populations remain in the implant remnant. the remnants, Thy-1.1 immunoreactivity is visible
Two lines of evidence have allowed us to reach only in the molecular layer (Fig. 5.4), correspond-
this conclusion. The first derives from the cytolo- ing to zones that, in cresyl violet-stained sections,
gical examination of the cortical areas adjacent can be seen to contain Purkinje cell bodies. In the
to the transplants. In these areas, the only visible most distant regions from the remnants, at the
change in the mutant cortex is the presence, in end of the host territory invaded by grafted cells
the molecular layer, of scattered perikarya of large and where their density is low, it is possible to
neurons exhibiting the ultrastructural features of determine the shape of single Thy-l.1 positive
Purkinje cells, particularly the hypolemmal cistern neurons, and all of them are easily identified as
(Palay and Chan-Palay, 1974). With the exception Purkinje cells because of their peculiar dendritic
of these ectopic Purkinje cells, the appearance of arbors (Fig. 5.5). Immunoreactive cells are absent
the remaining cell classes-basket and stellate from either the granular layer or the deep nuclei.
cells-and their density are similar to those en- The immunohistochemical results indicate the
countered in the mutant molecular layer, at a specificity of the mechanisms involved in the pro-
distance from the graft. cess of invasion of the mutant host cerebellum by
The second line of evidence arises from experi- grafted cells: only Purkinje cells, the cell category
ments designed to visualize specifically all neuro- missing in the host cortex, are able to leave the
nal categories originated from the transplants grafts and to enter the cerebellar parenchyma of
wherever their position in the host. Most neurons the host.
are provided with a cell surface glycoprotein, The question still to be resolved concerns the
Thy-I, that exists in mice in two allelic forms nature of the "mechanisms" involved in the selec-
called Thy-l.l and Thy-1.2 (see refs. in Morris, tive migration of immature Purkinje cells from
1985), and can be distinguished immunologically. the grafts to the molecular layer of the adult host
Although the expression of Thy-1 by granule cell cerebellum. In some previous experiments (Sotelo
bodies has not been clearly demonstrated in vivo, and Alvarado-Mallart, 1985) we have shown that
it is present at high levels in regions containing when pieces of cerebellar primordia are implanted
granule cell processes such as glomeruli and the in the neocortex or the hippocampus, virtually
molecular layer (Fig. 5.3). Thus, immunohisto- all grafted Purkinje cells do not leave the implants,
chemistry allows the distinction between graft and instead form a we\! organized minicerebellum
and host neurons when the cerebellar primordium with a small trilaminated cortex and, occasion-
88 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Figure 5.3. Cerebellar cortex of the vermis of an adult larly parallel fibers, provide the immunoreactivity of
AKR mouse, immunostained with an anti- Thy-l.l the molecular layer. The plasmalemma of Purkinje cell
antibody. The visualization of this cell surface antigen bodies (arrows) are also immunoreactive. In the granu-
permits the recognition of all cerebellar cell classes. In lar layer (GL), the glomeruli are densely stained, and
the molecular layer (ML), Purkinje cell dendrites and the granule cells appear as negative rounded cells
molecular layer interneurons appear as negative struc- surrounded by a very thin circular staining (stars).
tures; the cell membranes of these cellular elements as Bar = 80flm.
well as those of the processes in the neuropil, particu-
5. Cerebellar Grafting 89

ally, a central nuclear zone. Recently, we have the superficial four fifths of the molecular layer.
performed new experiments aimed at disclosing However, this zone of direct invasion extends
whether or not the presence of host Purkinje cells only 100 J1.m from the cannula track. Beyond this
interferes with the invasion of grafted Purkinje distance, the only ectopic cell bodies of grafted
cells into the host molecular layer. Three lines Purkinje cells have a subpial position with short,
of evidence, indicating that the Purkinje cell- inverted dendritic trees that overlap with those
deficient molecular layer exerts a "positive neuro- of host Purkinje cells (Fig. 5.7). As we show below,
tropic" effect (Ramon y Cajal, 1910), have been these subpial Purkinje cells are derived not from
obtained. direct invasion of the molecular layer but rather
by interfolial spread with subsequent penetration
Cerebellar Grafting in Normal Cerebellum into the outer molecular layer. This is far less than
the extensive integration of grafted Purkinje cells
Young adult C57BL mice were implanted with
seen in ped mutant cerebella; in such cerebella,
cerebellar primordia taken from 12-day-old em-
grafted Purkinje cells may spread 700 J1.m from
bryos, and the transplanted cerebella were analyzed
the cannula track (see above).
1 to 3 months later (Figs. 5.6, 5.7). As is often the
case in ped transplanted mice, the graft remnants
Cerebellar Grafting in 20- to
remain squeezed between two folia, lying against
27-Day-Old ped Mice
their pial surfaces. Small numbers of Purkinje
cells do migrate out ofthe remnant, but the grafted As stated above, the cerebellum of these young
and host Purkinje cells are easily distinguished. ped mice still contain abundant Purkinje cells
Host cells have their cell bodies arranged in a arranged in precise longitudinal zones (Wassef
monolayer between molecular and granular layers et aI., 1987). Therefore, cerebellar grafting in these
(Fig. 5.6), whereas the grafted cells have their cell mice offers an optimal control experiment to deter-
bodies ectopically located in the host molecular mine the presumptive role of host Purkinje cells
layer (Figs. 5.6, 5.7). Despite important individual in regulating the extension of host invasion by
variations between mice, the general pattern of grafted neurons in animals of an identical genetic
disposition of grafted Purkinje cells can be sum- background to those used in our long-term sur-
marized as follows: In cortical areas lesioned by vival experiments. Moreover, these experiments
the cannula used for the implantation, there are could disclose whether or not the Purkinje cell
narrow zones devoid of host Purkinje cells. These death results from toxic environmental factors,
areas contain abundant Purkinje cells with ectopic which theoretically may be present in the ped
cell bodies spreading thrc ·;hout the superficial cerebellum during this phase of maximal neuronal
four fifths of the host molecular layer (Fig. 5.6). degeneration. In all these mice, the large majority
In cortical zones juxtaposed to the cannula track, of the implanted tissue remains as an organized
the molecular layer contains overlapping host grafted minicerebellum with an interfolialloca-
and grafted Purkinje cells; the former always tion (Fig. 5.8). Grafted Purkinje cells do invade
at the interface between molecular and granular the host molecular layer but to a much lesser
layer, the latter with ectopic cell bodies occupying extent than in older ped mutants (Figs. 5.8, 5.9).

~----------------------------------------------------------------------
Figure 5.4. Low magnification of an AKR graft in the that illustrated in Fig. 5.4. Thy-l.1 immunoreactivity
cerebellum of an adult pcd mouse 2 months after im- is confined to the host molecular layer. At the deep
plantation, visualized by Thy-l.1 immunoreactivity. part of the fissure, where only a few scattered grafted
The arrow in this sagittal section points to the grafted Purkinje cell bodies were observed in the consecutive
cells that have penetrated the host parenchyma. Note paraffin section stained with cresyl violet, the immuno-
that, with exception of this area of host molecular staining permits the identification of the dendrites
layer, the host cerebellum is immunonegative. Bar = (arrow heads) of these grafted Purkinje cells. No other
600/lm. cellular element is immunoreactive. GL, granular layer.
Figure 5.5. High magnification of an area similar to Bar= 30/lm.
90 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Figure 5.6. Cerebellar grafting in normal mouse cere- Figure 5.7. Same material as for Fig. 5.6 but far from
bellum I month after transplantation. Immunostaining the implantation cannula track. The graft remnant
with anticalbindin. Sagittal section of the vermis, illus- (GR) containing grafted Purkinje cells (white squares)
trating an area in which host Purkinje cells have been lies on the surface of a folium that contains a normal
destroyed by the implantation cannula (arrows) . Host contingent of host Purkinje cells (identified by their
Purkinje cells have cell bodies at the interface between dendritic trees). Note that some of the grafted Purkinje
molecular (ML) and granular layer (GL) (white squares). cells (arrowheads) have penetrated the host cerebellum
Grafted Purkinje cells (white triangles), with ectopic but their cell bodies keep a subpial location. Bar =
cell bodie~ dispersed through the molecular layer, fill 30.um.
the gap created by the lesion. Note that only one
Purkinje cell (double arrow) has invaded the territory
occupied by the host Purkinje cells. Bar = 30.um.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 91

GL

9
Figure 5.8. Calbindin immunostaining of the cerebel- Figure 5.9. Higher magnification of the same material
lum of a pcd mouse grafted at 24 days of age and fixed as that in Fig. 5.8. The grafted Purkinje cells that have
3 months after the implantation. A large graft remnant invaded the host cerebellum have cell bodies located
(GR) organized as a normal trilaminated cerebellar in the superficial half of the host molecular layer and
cortex is visible between two folia of the host cerebel- inverted dendritic trees. Overlying this folium, Purkinje
lum. The arrows indicate areas of host molecular layer cells remaining in the graft remnant (GR) are visible.
containing grafted Purkinje cells. Bar = 500 !tm. GL, granular layer. Bar = 50 !tm.
92 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Furthermore, the cell bodies of the invading Synaptic Integration of Grafted


Purkinje cells remain in the superficial quarter Purkinje Cells into the Circuits of the
of the host molecular layer (Fig. 5.9). Similar Host Cerebellar Cortex
differences in the extent of grafted Purkinje cell
invasion between young and old pcd hosts have Immunohistochemical, ultrastructural, and elec-
been reported (Chang et aI., 1988). The survival of trophysiological studies ofthe grafts demonstrate
large numbers ofPurkinje cells argues against the that, with some limitations, the prerequisites dis-
existence of toxic factors responsible for the degene- cussed above for the repair of deficient circuits
ration of mutant Purkinje cells. can be fulfilled.

Cerebellar Grafting in Nervous Mutant Mice Location and Dendritic Arrangement


of Grafted Purkinje Cells
Nervous (nr) is a neurological mutation also
affecting Purkinje cells (Landis, 1973; Sidman In order to facilitate their synaptic integration,
and Green, 1970). In this mutant cerebellum, the the grafted Purkinje cells must move out of the
degeneration of Purkinje cells is slower and less implant to the location previously occupied by
complete than in the pcd mouse, and about 15% the missing neurons. In our material, the grafted
of them survive. The survival is also positionally neurons do not completely succeed in this task,
related: all Purkinje cells degenerate in the hemi- because their cell bodies remain ectopic (Figs.
spheres, some survive in the flocculus and in the 5.12,5.13). However, since more than 95% of the
intermediate cortex, and half in the vermis, where synaptic inputs to these neurons are dendritic, the
they are grouped into distinct longitudinal stripes distribution and organization of the dendritic
that are symmetric with relation to the midline trees belonging to grafted Purkinje cells become
(Fig. 5.10) (Wassef et aI., 1987). In our experiments, much more important than the location of their
E 12 isogenic cerebellar primordia were implanted cell bodies. These dendritic trees are able to spread
into the intermediate cortex, and the cerebella throughout the host molecular layer (Figs. 5.12,
were studied 2 to 4 months later. By analyzing 5.13), stopping abruptly at the interface with the
the molecular layer ofthe host at its interface with granular layer. In this respect, host and grafted
the graft remnant, we have determined that, in Purkinje cells have identically located dendrites,
general, the invasion of grafted Purkinje cells is spanning the whole depth of the mutant molecular
oriented toward the region devoid of host Purkinje layer.
cells. Furthermore, when the grafted neurons reach The dendrites of grafted Purkinje cells do not
a zone of the nr molecular layer containing the fully acquire the characteristic espalier shape seen
dendritic trees of the host Purkinje cells, they in control animals, and their shapes are related
normally stop their migration. The zones of over- to the position of their cell bodies (Figs. 5.12,
lap between grafted and nr Purkinje cells are 5.13). Those with cell bodies located under the
small; in them the cell bodies belonging to the pial membrane commonly have inverted dendrites,
grafted neurons are almost abruptly excluded whereas those at the center ofthe layer are bipolar
from the deeper half of the host molecular layer and even multipolar. Despite this variety of shapes,
(Fig. 5.11). all these dendrites have acquired the two principal
The results obtained with these three groups features that characterize normal Purkinje cell
of experimental animals point in the same direc- dendrites: a) they comprise a proximal compart-
tion: the extent of the host molecular layer inva- ment of thick, almost smooth branches, and a
sion by grafted Purkinje cells in these mice is distal compartment of spiny branchlets (Fig. 5.12)
much smaller than that obtained when the host and b) they have achieved a flattened arrangement
is completely devoid of Purkinje cells at the with a maximal extension in the sagittal plane
time of transplantation. Thus, the Purkinje cell- (Fig. 5.12), and a minimal one in the transverse
deficient molecular layer seems to exert a "positive plane (Fig. 5.13). They are therefore confined to
neurotropic" effect, of still unknown nature, selec- the plane perpendicular to the bundles of parallel
tive for neurons of the same category as those fibers of the mutant cerebellum (Fig. 5.14). Consi-
missing. dering the features of these dendrites, we can
5. Cerebellar Grafting 93

'10

11
Figure 5.10. Low magnification of an adult nr cerebel- in the host molecular layer between a zone containing
lum cut in the coronal plane and immunostained with host Purkinje cells (arrowheads) and another devoid of
an anticalbindin antibody. Note that the remaining these neurons. Note that the area lacking host Purkinje
Purkinje cells are not randomly located but are loca- cells contains numerous grafted Purkinje cells, with
lized into precise longitudinal compartments, providing somata dispersed throughout the host molecular layer.
mirror images in relation to the midline. Bar = 600 11m. Only a few grafted neurons (white triangles) overlap
Figure 5.11. Cerebellum of an adult nr mouse 3 with the zone containing host Purkinje cells. Immuno-
months after implantation of a cerebellar graft into the staining with anticalbindin. Bar = 45 p.m.
intermediary cortex. The arrows indicate the interface
94 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

13
Figure 5.12. pcd grafted cerebellum 2 months after the normal outward orientation, but exhibits a maximal
implantation. Distal area of the host molecular layer extension in this parasagittal plane of sectioning.
containing grafted Purkinje cells. The arrow points to Immunostaining with an anticyc\ic GMP-dependent
one of these neurons with a dendritic tree resembling protein kinase. Bar = 90 flm.
that of normal Purkinje cells. Note the presence of Figure 5.13. Same material as in Fig. 5.12 but sec-
thick dendritic branches, forming the proximal com- tioned in the coronal plane. Note the flattening of the
partment, as well as distal thin branches of the distal grafted Purkinje cell dendrites in this plane of section-
compartment. The dendritic tree does not have the ing. Bar = 40 flm.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 95

Figure 5.14. Diagrammatic


representation of Purkinje
cell dendritic trees in control
mice and in transplimted
pcd cerebella. Note that the
flattening of the trees in
the coronal plane is more
pronounced in the control
(mono planar) than in the
grafted Purkinje cells.

14
conclude that grafted Purkinje cells have indeed investment qualitatively similar to that of the
succeeded in occupying the host territory where normal cerebellum. Each thick dendritic segment
the lost mutant Purkinje cells had received their bears clusters of stubby spines synaptically con-
whole contingent of synaptic inputs. tacted by climbing fiber varicosities, whereas the
distal branches, from which emerge numerous
Grafted Purkinje Cells and Host Cortical long-necked spines, are the postsynaptic targets
Cerebellar Circuits for parallel fibers. Despite their ectopic location,
The ultrastructural study of grafted Purkinje cells the perikarya receive synaptic inputs from either
within the pcd molecular layer discloses a synaptic stellate cell axons or ascending collaterals of the
96 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

bllsket fibers. These two classes ofaxons, belong- possible restoration of the mutant cerebellar
ing to inhibitory interneurons of the host mole- circuits.
cular layer, also establish synaptic contacts with
the smooth surface of the stem dendrites. These Corticonuclear Reconnection: The Problem
ultrastructural observations clearly indicate that oj Growth and Navigation oj Axons
the synaptic investment of grafted Purkinje cells
Originated Jrom the Grafted
is almost normal, although some few qualitative
Purkinje Cells
(Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, 1987a) and quanti-
tative (Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, 1987b) dif- To obtain functional recovery in pcd mice, it is
ferences have been reported. imperative that the information processed by the
The function of the newly formed synapses repaired cerebellar cortex reach its target neurons,
between the adult host neurons and the grafted located mostly in the deep cerebellar nuclei. Since
Purkinje cells has been investigated, in collabora- the only known cortical output is made by axons
tion with Francis Crepel and colleagues (Gardette of Purkinje cells, it is obvious that the recovery
et aI., 1988), with electro physiologic techniques is dependent on the formation of a new corti-
on pcd cerebellar slices in vitro. The grafted neu- conuclear projection originated from the implan-
rons were impaled with intracellular microelec- ted Purkinje cells. This formation implies a pro-
trodes and their synaptic interactions were analy- cess of oriented axonal growth that, owing to the
zed by electrical stimulation of the white matter nature of the neurons involved and to their cellular
at the base of the folium to activate anterogradely milieu, will share some of the mechanisms under-
climbing and mossy fibers. All grafted Purkinje lying axonal pathfinding in the developing central
cells tested responded to this white matter stimu- nervous system (CNS) as well as axonal regenera-
lation by a typical all-or-none climbing fiber exci- tion in the adult CNS. The similarities and dif-
tatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) (complex ferences between these processes can be summari-
spike), indicating that not only have all grafted zed as follows: a) the distance to be covered by
Purkinje cells formed synapses with axons of the the differentiating axons of grafted Purkinje cells
host inferior olivary neurons, but also that they and the cellular milieu faced by their growth cones
are, as in control animals, monoinnervated by during navigation are those offered by the adult
climbing fibers. Purkinje cell responses, due to host cerebellum. Thus, the obstacles that these
the activation of the mossy fiber-granule cell axons must overcome are those encountered by
pathway, were also elicited in the grafted neurons regenerating axons in the adult CNS, b) the target
when the stimulus intensity was low enough not neurons in cerebellar nuclei are presumably adult
to give rise to any climbing fiber response. The and may not exert the same effects on Purkinje
latencies of the disynaptic responses (ranging axons that occur during development, c) Purkinje
between 2.4 and 5.2 ms) were longer than those cells are grafted either as precursor cells, devoid
resulting from the monosynaptic activation of of neuritic expansions, or as young postmitotic
the climbing fibers. These disynaptic EPSPs were neurons, with neurites remaining within the solid
markedly graded with the stimulus intensity as pieces of implanted cerebellar primordium. Since
in normal cerebellum. Finally, well developed these neurons are not axotomized, the molecular
inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) were signals induced by axotomy are not expressed in
also evoked in grafted neurons, although of the grafting experiments, and d) the immaturity
shorter duration than in control Purkinje cells. of the grafted neurons leads us to suppose that
These electrophysiQlogic experiments confirm that they are capable of greater plasticity than adult
all the excitatory and inhibitory inputs forming neurons, so that they could adapt better to the
the synaptic investment of grafted Purkinje cells existing conditions for their axonal outgrowth.
are functional and have characteristics compar- These considerations suggest that the formation
able to those in control mice. Thus, the grafted of a corticonuclear projection in the mutant cere-
Purkinje cells have been synaptically integrated bellum will need to overcome most ofthedifficul-
into the cortical circuitry of the deficient host ties encountered by central mammalian neurons
cerebellum, fulfilling the second requirement for in their abortive axonal regeneration. It is there-
5. Cerebellar Grafting 97

fore expected that the fulfillment of this pre- grafted deep nuclear neurons. If, conversely, they
requisite will be the most difficult to attain. The are far from the graft remnants and within 600 11m
results obtained in our experiments, using Purkinje from the host deep nuclei, they will direct their
cell immunomarkers to trace their axons, confirm outgrowth toward the latter and, once they enter
this expectation. Only in a low percentage of this territory, differentiate into branched terminal
grafted cerebella is a corticonuclear projection arborizations, bearing numerous varicosities. Our
formed, and the latter only involves a few of the ultrastructural analysis has proven that the vari-
axons emerging from those Purkinje cells that cosities establish specific synaptic connections on
have succeeded in invading the mutant molecular the somata and on the dendrites of host deep
layer. nuclear neurons. Therefore, the third needed pre-
The growth of grafted axons may be limited by requisite is, by far, the most difficult to fulfill. A
two complementary mechanisms. First, the target corticonuclear reconnection is obtained only in
neurons may not present the same influences as a few transplanted cerebella, and the extent of the
occur during development. Second, grafted deep reconnection is always very limited.
cerebellar neurons in the implant remnants may In some of the transplants with solid pieces of
impair contact with host deep cerebellar nuclear cerebellar primordia and in all of those using cell
neurons because they are close to the grafted suspensions (Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, 1986),
Purkinje cells, and may preempt their growing some of the grafted Purkinje cells remain dispersed
axons when the distance between integrated graf- throughout the cannula track and a few within
ted Purkinje cells and host deep nuclei exceeds the host white matter. These cells have stellate
600 11m. Thus, 600ilm seem to be the longest shapes with several main dendritic stems emerging
distance over which the grafted axons can navigate from the cell bodies. The dendrites are short,
and form a new corticonuclear projection. studded with spines, and poorly ramified, suggest-
Normal development depends on interactions ing that they are deprived of most of their normal
between axonal growth cones and local cellular synaptic inputs (Sotelo, 1978). These grafted
elements, provided with specific cues, that stimu- Purkinje cells have been extremely useful in indi-
late (permissive) or inhibit (nonpermissive) the cating that one of the main limiting factors for
oriented axonal outgrowth. In the developing the corticonuclear reconnection lies in the irritial
nervous system, such guidance mechanisms are disorientation of their developing axons within
based on the recognition of cell-surface and extra- the host molecular layer. Indeed, these Purkinje
cellular matrix molecules present in those cells cells outside the gray matter are able to grow axons
signaling the pathway (Dodd and Jessell, 1988). for long distances that follow the longitudinal axis
Whether these clues persist in the mature nervous of the folium, parallel to the bundles of myelina-
system remains unknown; our observations sug- ted fibers present in the white matter (Fig. 5.16).
gest a loss of such clues in the cerebellum of the These observations raise an interesting issue.
adult pcd mouse. Indeed, the majority of the axons In a recent study by Caroni and Schwab (1988),
emerging from grafted Purkinje cells remain within membranes from CNS myelin have been proven
the molecular layer (Fig. 5.15), where they may to be nonpermissive substrata for axonal out-
contact other Purkinje cells or form aberrant growth, and the inhibitory oligodendroglial-
plexuses at the interface between the molecular surface molecules have been identified. In theory,
and the granular layers, as if they are lost in their the immunoblockade of these proteins could per-
search for appropriate target neurons. A small mit the regeneration of central neurons in adult
minority of Purkinje cell axons do leave the mammalian CNS. Our in vivo observations with
molecular layer for unknown reasons and cross the transplants suggest that oriented axonal growth
perpendicularly the granular layer (as normal can occur within the white matter. Although the
Purkinje cells do), reaching the white matter axis distance reached from this growth is rather small,
of the folium. The fate of these axons depends, as it is enough to allow in the mouse an important
already discussed, on their position with respect rate of regeneration of the corticonuclear projec-
to the deep cerebellar nuclei. If they are close to tion. However, even though Purkinje cells have
the graft remnants, they will end by synapsing on a great resistance to axotomy (Ramon y Cajal,
98 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

GL

16
Figure 5.15. Calbindin-immunofluorescent grafted do not cross the granular layer but remain within tQe
Purkinje cells in the molecular layer of the pcd cere- molecular layer, forming a loose bundle of parallel
bellum, cut in the coronal plane 1 month after implan- axons. GL, granular layer; WM, white matter axis of
tation. At the distal border of the host molecular layer the folium. Bar = 60/lm.
containing the grafted neurons, the axons ofthese cells Figure 5.16. Coronal section through the pcd cerebel-
5. Cerebellar Grafting 99

1959; Sotelo, 1978), they are unable to reestab- Short-Term Survivals


lish a corticonuclear projection after cutting their
The results obtained with long-term survivals
axons. The results obtained by Ramon y Cajal
imply that, in the adult mutant cerebellum, the
(1959) in young mammalian cerebellum after
replacement of missing Purkinje cells occurs in a
Purkinje cell axotomy, the so-called Purkinje cells
precise manner, most probably recapitulating the
with arciform axons, suggest the existence of some
processes followed by these neurons during their
axonal regeneration but the inability of these
normal ontogeny. The principal events in cerebel-
axons to find their pathway to the white matter.
lar morphogenesis take place in sequential critical
Thus, as Ramon y Cajal wrote: "A result of the
steps from the cellular proliferation of stem cells
disappearance of the extracollateral portion of
in the primitive cerebellar neuroepithelium to the
the axon and of the compensatory hypertrophy
functional validation and selective elimination of
of the collaterals and initial portion of the axon
synaptic connections (Changeux and Danchin,
is the transformation of the cell of Purkinje into
1976), resulting in the formation of the cerebellar
a neurone with a short axon." All these observa-
cortex and its specific circuitry. These orderly
tions point to an essential question: What prevents
steps (see refs. in Cowan, 1981) concern the follow-
regenerating axons and axons of grafted embryo-
ing processes: a) a phase of cellular proliferation,
nic neurons from finding their ways to their specific
followed by b) an outward migration of young
target neurons? Without this answer, it is difficult
postmitotic Purkinje cells from the subventricular
to imagine how neuronal grafting could succeed
zone to the cortical plate (Goffinet, 1983), c) a
in replacing neurons with the complete synaptic
phase of cytodifferentiation with the formation of
integration needed for the restoration of deficient
neuritic expansions, which will result in the acqui-
systems wired in a "point-to-point" manner.
sition of tridimensionally arranged dendritic trees
(see refs. in Sotelo, 1978) and in the establishment
General Conclusions
of highly organized efferent projections, d) simul-
The results obtained in this series of grafting taneously with the formation of dendritic arbori-
experiments, studied after long-term survival, zations, Purkinje cells begin their synaptogenetic
demonstrate that specific synaptic integration of period, with the formation of redundant connec-
grafted neurons into the deficient cerebellar cir- tions (Crepel et aI., 1976), that will be followed by
cuitry can take place, although with important a secondary period of numerical adjustment, in-
limitations. The replacement of missing Purkinje volving regressive processes resulting in e) the
cells by grafted homologous neurons, that can selective elimination of some synaptic connections
involve 5% to 15% of the volume of the mutant and the f) synaptic stabilization of the remaining
molecular layer (Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, ones. According to the hypothesis of Changeux
1987b), occurs in all the implanted cerebella of and Danchin (1976), these last two processes are
the adult pcd mutant mice. It results in the re- regulated by the function ofthe forming cerebellar
storation of corti co cerebellar circuitry, owing to circuits.
the complete synaptic integration of the grafted In order to determine whether or not grafted
Purkinje cells into the cortex of the mutant cere- Purkinje cells are able to recapitulate their de-
bellum. However, the corticonuclear projection velopmental history within the adult host cere-
is reestablished in only a few percent of grafted bellum, and to disclose the cellular mechanisms
cerebella and involves a low number of grafted underlying the successful neuronal replacement
Purkinje cells. discussed in the preceding section, new grafting

~-----------------------------------------------------------------
lum grafted 2 months before fixation and immunostain- and the granular layer (GL). The left side corresponds
ed with anticalbindin antisera. The upper part of the to the grafted Purkinje cells distributed all along the
micrograph corresponds to the host molecular layer cannula track. Note that the axons of these neurons
containing grafted Purkinje cells with dendrites delinea- have succeeded to grow into the white matter axis
ting the interface (arrowheads) between the molecular (WM) of the folium. Bar = 40 11m.
100 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

GL 19
Figure 5.17. Autoradiogram of a pcd cerebellum graft- The arrows point to grafted Purkinje cell bodies con-
ed I month before fixation. The grafted mouse was taining labeled nuclei. Bar = 40 /lm.
intraperitoneally injected with tritiated thymidine 6 Figure 5.18. Calbindin immunostaining of grafted
and 24 hours after the implantation. The cerebellum Purkinje cells during their tangential migration at the
was cut in the sagittal plane and counterstained with surface of the host molecular layer (ML). Note the
cresyl violet. In this double exposure (bright and dark elongated shapes of the migrating neurons, 5 days after
fields), the auto radiographic silver grains appear white. grafting. Bar = 40 /lm.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 101

experiments in pcd cerebellum have been carried large nucleus provided with a single spherical
out. Most of them have been analyzed in a timed nucleolus. The wide cytoplasm around the nucleus
sequence from 3 up to 21 days after grafting (3- is filled with numerous small and irregular Nissl
21 DAG) in order to compare at a given age the bodies. Neurons of this sort can be encountered
phase of maturation of grafted Purkinje cells with in two locations. Some ofthem are present within
that normally occurring in cerebellar ontogenesis the graft remnant, but the majority is dispersed
(Sotelo et al., 1990). In the following description within the upper three quarters ofthe host mole-
we report the results obtained for each of these cular layer (Fig. 5.17), in folia adjacent to the graft
developmental steps. remnant. In the group of mutant mice treated
with tritiated thymidine 6 and 24 hours after
Proliferation Period implantation, grafted Purkinje cells with auto-
of Grafted Purkinje Cells radiographic labeled nuclei are present in both
locations. About 25% of those Purkinje cells
During the morphogenesis of the mouse cerebel-
integrated in the host molecular layer exhibit
lum, Purkinje cell proliferation starts the embryo-
labeled nuclei (Fig. 5.17). Conversely, in those
nic day 11 and ends 72 hours later (from Ell to
implanted mice injected with the isotope 60 and
E13) (Fujita, 1967; Miale and Sidman, 1961). Since
70 hours after grafting, no labeled Purkinje cells
in our experiments donor tissue is taken from
have been observed.
12-day-old embryos, less than 25% of Purkinje
These results show that the Purkinje cell pro-
cells have already finished proliferation. Thus, the
genitors are able to proliferate when implanted
majority ofthis neuronal population is grafted as
in an adult cerebellum. More importantly, this
neuroblasts, with potential mitotic activity for at
proliferation seems to take place over an interval
least 48 hours. The trauma associated with the
similar to that occurring during normal cerebellar
implantation and the abnormality of the cellular
ontogeny. The incorporation of tritiated thymi-
milieu in which the grafted primordium will under-
dine does not take place 60 hours after grafting,
go its development could influence the prolifera-
a time at which the implanted Purkinje cells have
tion period of grafted Purkinje cell progenitors.
reached an age equivalent to 15 embryonic days,
They could either arrest their proliferative period
the age at which Purkinje cells have ceased their
or prolong it beyond the expected 48 hours. To
proliferative activity in normally developing fetal
study this question we have used the following
mice.
approach: Adult pcd mice were grafted with solid
pieces of E 12 cerebellar primordium. One group
Migratory Routes of Grafted
of recipients was injected intraperitoneally with
tritiated thymidine (5p.Ci/g) 6 and 24 hours after Purkinje Cells
the operation. In another group, the isotope was During the first days after transplantation, the
injected 60 and 70 hours after grafting. Mice of grafts, even when implanted as solid pieces of
both groups were fixed between 45 and 60 days donor cerebellar primordium, modify their shapes
after grafting, their cerebella were embedded in according to physical constraints. The pressure
paraffin, and serial sections were used for the exerted by the parenchyma ofthe host cerebellum
autoradiographic detection of the tritiated thy- forces the grafts to adapt themselves to regions
midine. The sections Were counterstained with of less resistance either created by the cannula
cresyl violet. track or already present in the host cerebellum
In the cresyl violet-stained preparations, graf- like the cerebellar fissures. Thus, 3 to 4 days after
ted Purkinje cells can be identified by their cyto- grafting (3-4 DAG) they have acquired irregular
logical features. They appear as large, ovoid cell shapes consisting of a main elongated mass that
bodies of about 22p.m in diameter, containing a crosses several folia of the host cerebellum, and

~----------------------------------------------------------------------
Figure 5.19. Seven days after grafting, the migrating or oblique direction. Note that the descending proces-
calbindin immunoreactive neurons have changed ses of these grafted neurons (arrows) do not enter into
polarity, crossing the host molecl,llar layer in a radial the host granular layer (GL). Bar = 40 pm.
102 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Figure 5.20. One-Ilm thick section stained with tolui- (arrowheads). The arrows point to the edges of a basal
dine blue, taken from a pcd grafted cerebellum 6 days lamina hole. Note the large processes of grafted
after grafting. The upper part of the micrograph cor- Purkinje cells penetrating the host molecular layer
responds to a lateral swelling of the graft (LSG) lying (star). The asterisks mark grafted Purkinje cells either
on the surface of a folium of the host cerebellum. The in a subpial position or intermingled with the neuropil
graft contains large neurons ofthe Purkinje cell category of the host molecular layer (ML). Bar = 20 tim.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 103

one or several lateral swellings expanding between The second early migratory pathway is formed
the surfaces of adjacent folia, at both sides of the by calbindin-immunoreactive, postmitotic Pur-
main elongated mass. Hence, grafted neural kinje cells that, from the lateral swellings of the
elements are apposed to the host cerebellar graft lying on the surface ofthe host folia, are able
parenchyma in two distinct fashions: a) there is a to pass directly into the host molecular layer
direct apposition (graft-host interface) between through discontinuities in the subpial basal lamina
the trilayered cortex ofthe host folia and the main (Figs. 5.20, 5.21). Glial markers (anti-glial fibrillary
elongated mass, and b) the lateral swellings ofthe acidic protein and antivimentin antibodies), used
graft lie on the pial surface of the host folia, on consecutive sections to those immunostained
separated from the host parenchyma by the pial with anticalbindin, provide a clear picture of the
cells and their basal lamina. size and disposition of the glia limitans below the
Owing to this special disposition ofthe implants, basal lamina. The endfeet of the host Bergmann
grafted Purkinje cells can invade the host paren- fibers facing the discontinuities in the subpial
chyma following two different migratory pathways, basal lamina seem to grow out and invade the
one originating from the graft-host interface and graft, forming an unusual tuft of astrocytic proces-
the other through the pial surface and its basal ses among the grafted neurons (Fig. 5.22). Thus,
membrane. Indeed, 4-5 DAG a migratory stream the holes in the subpial basal lamina provide a
has been formed at the periphery of the host folia direct communication between the graft and the
directly attached to the graft. This stream emerges host molecular layer, with passage of glial fibers
from the graft and moves off, between the subpial (from host to graft) and of Purkinje cells as well
surface and the glial-limiting membrane, in a fun- as glial processes (from graft to host) (Figs. 5.20,
neling manner (at its emergence it consists of a 5.22). We have no precise information about the
five- to seven-cell-deep layer and at its end, about mechanisms underlying the breakage ofthe basal
700 Jlm away, it is formed of a single row of lamina since in our material glial fiber sprouts
elongated neurons), without disrupting the com- and migrating Purkinje cells are simultaneously
pact neuropil of the host cerebellar cortex. seen at the broken patches. However, the known
Six DAG, the tangential migratory stream at- physiology of these two categories of neural cells
tains its maximal extension, about 1.5 mm in lead us to predict that the migrating Purkinje cells
maximum diameter around the graft remnant. could be responsible for this breakage. Indeed,
The cellular composition within this superficial migrating neurons are provided with a leading
stream appears homogeneous. With the excep- process and its terminal growth cone, and it has
tion of some degenerating cells and macro phages been proven that neuronal growth cones are able
close to the host- graft interface, the stream com- to release proteolytic enzymes. One of these re-
prises young undifferentiated bipolar neurons of leased serine proteases is the urokinase-type
about 20 Jlm in diameter, similar to migrating plasminogen activator that, by activation of
Purkinje cells in E18 normal mouse embryos. plasminogen to plasmin, can degradate matrix
The confirmation of the nature of the immature components such as laminin and break the basal
neurons has been obtained with immunohisto- lamina. Similarly, metalloproteinases, also com-
chemical markers since the migrating neurons prised between the cell-associated proteases
express calbindin (Fig. 5.18), as do migrating present in neuritic growth cones, can be involved
Purkinje cells in control fetuses (Wassef et aI., in the breakage of the basal lamina (see refs. in
1985). Monard, 1988).

~----------------------------------------------------------------------
Figure 5.21. Calbindin-immunoreactive cells are immunostained with an antivimentin antiserum. The
tightly packed in a lateral swelling of the graft (LSG) arrowheads mark the external surface of the host mole-
lying on the host cerebellar surface. Note the direct cular layer. Note the fine immunopositive sprouts emerg-
penetration of immunoreactive Purkinje cells (arrows) ing from the host Bergmann fibers penetrating the
into the upper region ofthe host molecular layer (ML) lateral swelling ofthe graft (LSG), and delineating the
6 days after grafting. Bar = 30 tlm. discontinuities of the host subpial basal lamina. Bar =
Figure 5.22. Same material as that in Fig. 5.21, but 30tlm.
104 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

PO

P5
f20 PO

25
Figure 5.23. Calbindin-immunoreactive grafted Pur- bodies. GL, granular layer; ML, molecular layer.
kinje cells in the molecular layer of a pcd mouse 11 days Bar = 40 11m.
after grafting. Note that the neurons have lost their Figure 5.24. Calbindin-immunoreactive grafted Pur-
long smooth processes (compare Figs. 5.19 and 5.23) kinje cells in the molecular layer of a pcd mouse 14
and that they exhibit the morphology of Purkinje cells days after grafting. The arrow points to a distal Purkinje
in the second phase of dendritic development, with cell in its third phase of dendritic development. ML,
short processes emerging in all directions from the cell molecular layer. Bar = 40 11m.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 105

It is important to recall that both types of early phase of the grafted neurons and the consecutive
Purkinje cell migrations commonly coexist, and perikaryal translocation are frozen when the
that both of them are at the origin of the inward leading processes of the migrating neurons reach
migration of the grafted neurons. Indeed, 6-7 the lowest limits of the host molecular layer. This
DAG, grafted Purkinje cells of both origins mas- likely explains why the grafted Purkinje cells do
sively penetrate the host molecular layer. Those not attain their normal location at the interface
arising from the tangential migratory stream do between the molecular and the granular layers,
so by changing polarity and adopting a bipolar and remain ectopic in the long-term survivals
radial or somewhat oblique position. Those (Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, 1986, 1987a).
directly passing from the lateral swellings of the
graft do so traversing the basal lamina and invad- Dendritic Differentiation
ing the host molecular layer. The inward-oriented
of Grafted Purkinje Cells
processes of both groups penetrate the whole
depth ofthe molecular layer, but do not enter the The anticalbindin immunostaining, which pro-
granular layer, as if their permissive microen- vides Golgi-like pictures of developing Purkinje
vironment abruptly stops at the molecular layer- cells, has allowed a complete analysis of the den-
granular layer interface (Fig. 5.19). Occasionally, dritogenesis of the grafted Purkinje cells as well as
the inward migrating Purkinje cells run parallel the evaluation ofthe kind of cellular interactions
to the host Bergmann fibers; we have even ob- taking place between immature and adult neurons
served direct appositions between these categories in the process of building up the dendritic trees
of cells (Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, 1987c; of the former.
Sotelo et aI., 1990). More frequently, in single thin Normal Purkinje cells develop their dendritic
sections there are no apparent cellular interactions trees following three sequential phases (Fig. 5.25),
between the migrating Purkinje cells and the glial called by Ramon y Cajal (1926): a) the phase of
axes (Fig. 5.26). In any case, this later phase of the fusiform cell, b) the phase of the stellate cell
radial migration resembles that occurring during with disoriented dendrons, and c) the phase of
cerebellar ontogeny with one essential difference: orientation and flattening of the dendrites. Re-
it takes place inward instead of outward. Normally, cently, Armengol and Sotelo (1991) have studied
Purkinje cells originate from the ventricular neu- the variety offorms present in the first phase, and
roepithelium and migrate outward to the cerebel- the time sequence as well as the cellular interac-
lar plate, toward the surface of the nascent cere- tions taking place during the passage from the
bellum. first to the second phase. Postmigratory Purkinje
The radially or obliquely oriented Purkinje cells, in 19- and 20-day-old rat embryos, preserve
cells have, as migrating neurons do, a bipolar their bipolar shape, although the long apical den-
shape with asymmetrically long processes emerg- dritic process branches into two asymmetrical
ing from the opposite poles of their elongated segments. These processes are smooth and long
cell bodies (Fig. 5.19). The latter are dispersed and can receive synaptic contacts. During the first
throughout the superficial three quarters of the two postnatal days, the shape of some of these
host molecular layer. Ten DAG, the grafted Pur- neurons evolves into more complicated forms
kinje cells have completely changed the shape and with basilar dendrites. An important change
disposition oftheir processes (see below) but main- occurs during the second through the fourth post-
tain unchanged the position of their cell bodies. natal day when the long dendrites disappear (as
This observation suggests that the migratory well as their transient synaptic contacts), and

~-----------------------------------------------------------------
Figure 5.25. Diagrammatic representation of the deve- phase or "phase of the stellate cell with disoriented
lopment of Purkinje cell dendrites in a control rat. The dendrons." The drawing at P8 illustrates one of these
drawings at E20 and PO illustrate Purkinje cells in neurons in its third phase or the "phase of orientation
their first phase or "phase of the fusiform cells." The and flattening ofthe dendrites." Finally, the last draw-
drawing at P5 illustrates a Purkinje cell in its second ing to the right illustrates an adult Purkinje cell.
106 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-MaIIart

instead the neurons proceed to the formation of rons has completely changed. Despite their ectopic
a great number of perikaryal long filopodia, giv- location, the cells have acquired the typical ap-
ing them the appearance of stellate cells (second pearance of cells in the "phase of the stellate cell
phase). By 3 days postnatally, when the synapto- with disoriented dendrons" (Fig. 5.23). This change
genesis between climbing fibers and Purkinje cells in shape, which occurs when the grafted neurons
starts (Crepel, 1971), most Purkinje cells have attain the biological age of 2 to 3 postnatal days,
finished the resorption or retraction of their pri- coincides with the beginning of the synaptogenesis
mitive long apical and basilar dendrites and have between host climbing fibers and grafted Purkinje
entered the "phase of stellate cells with disoriented cells (see next section), as is the case during cere-
dendrons." The passage from the first to the second bellar ontogeny. It is of interest to note that at
phase seems to be intrinsic to Purkinje cells since this age an important proportion of the grafted
it occurs even in the absence of climbing fibers neurons has also attained the third develop-
(Armengol and Sotelo, 1991). By the end of the mental phase: these cells exhibit multiple, profusely
first postnatal week, the growth cone at the apical branched primary dendrites, like those commonly
end of the stellate cell body has grown out a observed 14-15 DAG.
flattened dendritic tree, with enrichment of termi- In the cerebellum of mutants studied 14-15 DAG
nal spiny branch lets , and \-'1ith the disappearance (biological age for Purkinje cells: 6-7 postnatal
of most of the long somatic filopodia. The cell days), the perikarya of the ectopic neurons have
has entered into the third and final phase of its developed multiple primary dendritic segments
dendritic development. The passage from the (two to six), profusely branching into secondary
second to the third phase requires specific cell- and distal branches (Fig. 5.24). The latter are
to-cell interactions, dealing mostly with the studded with spines and correspond to incipient
synaptogenesis between the developing parallel spiny branchlets. Furthermore, the perikarya have
fibers and the growing dendrites of Purkinje cells lost most of their long filopodia, but shorter and
(see refs. in Sotelo, 1978). thinner processes still remain. A main feature
The dendritic differentiation of grafted Purkinje characterizes the dendritic trees of these grafted
cells follows a process remarkably similar to that Purkinje cells: they span most of the host mole-
just described for normal developing Purkinje cular layer with arbors flattened in the sagittal
cells (Sotelo et aI., 1990). During their migratory plane. This important feature is most evident in
stage, these neurons exhibit a bipolar shape closely those Purkinje cells at the most distal border of
resembling those commonly observed in the "phase the host molecular layer containing a low density
of the fusiform cell" (Fig. 5.19). The essential dif- of grafted neurons: the dendrites of these cells
ference is that the long and asymmetrically branch- have a tangential disposition, with their axis
ed dendritic process has not an apical position, parallel to the cerebellar surface and perpendi-
but instead it is basal and perpendicularly or cular to the bundles of host parallel fibers. Hence,
obliquely oriented toward the molecular layer- despite the anomalies in number and orientation
granular layer interface. Grafted Purkinje cells of the primary branches (due to the ectopic loca-
with bipolar shapes constitute the majority of tion of their perikarya), grafted Purkinje cells at
these neurons observed between 5 and 9 DAG, this stage of differentiation resemble those reach-
corresponding to the biological ages of 17 em- ing the "phase of orientation and flattening of the
bryonic days and 1 postnatal day (the gestation dendrites." This stage coincides with a phase of
in the mouse lasts for 19 days). The main regres- active synaptogenesis between host parallel fibers
sive process that characterizes the formation of and grafted Purkinje cells (see next section), syn-
Purkinje cell dendrites, the retraction of the long aptogenesis that will proceed for at least the whole
and smooth dendrites, and the appearance of next week with an expanding rate. Thus, by 21
long somatic filopodia (the passage from the first DAG, the grafted Purkinje cells are provided with
to the second developmental phase) also occurs dendritic trees qualitatively similar to those re-
in grafted Purkinje cells (compare Figs. 5.19 and ported before in long-term survivals, indicating
5.23). Thus, 10-11 DAG, the shape of these neu- the end of their maturation.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 107

Figure 5.26. Electron micrograph of a grafted Purkinje Figure 5.27. Electron micrograph taken from aped
cell with a subpial location (arrowheads point to the cerebellum 7 days after grafting. This micrograph illus-
basal lamina) penetrating the host molecular layer's trates a large descending process belonging to a grafted
parenchyma. Note the immature appearance of the Purkinje cell (PCD). The arrow points to a synaptic
cytoplasm and the absence of axon terminals synapsing contact between a host axon terminal and the grafted
on the cell body. pcd cerebellum, 7 days after grafting. Purkinje cell process. Bar = 1 /lm.
Bar=2/lm.
108 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Synapse Formation Resulting from the grafted Purkinje cells are directly appos-
in the Synaptic Integration ed to host-beaded axons, some oftheir varicosities
of Grafted Purkinje Cells establishing asymmetric synaptic contacts on the
somatic filopodia (Fig. 5.28). These beaded axons,
In the transplanted cerebella, grafted Purkinje in addition to the typical organelles of climbing
cells during their tangential and radial migration fibers (densely packed rounded agranular vesicles,
(5-9 DAG) exhibit ultrastructural features of im- a few large granulated vesicles, abundant micro-
mature neurons: absence of Nissl bodies and tubules, etc.) can also contain numerous tubular
presence of free polyribosomes suspended in the profiles of the smooth endoplasmic reticulum that
cytoplasmic matrix (Fig. 5.26). In this respect, the resemble those in growth cones, and therefore
grafted neurons resemble Purkinje cells of similar have been considered to be axonal sprouts of the
ages (E17 to PI) in normal perinatal mice. Hence, host climbing fibers.
the large diameter of the cell bodies and processes The number of climbing fibers synapsing on
as well as their immature neuronal appearance somatic filopodia of the grafted Purkinje cells is
provide good markers for the ultrastructural relatively low, and the vast majority of the filo-
identification of the grafted Purkinje cells in the podial and smooth perikaryal membrane is free
adult neuropil of the host molecular layer. of synaptic inputs. Despite this low incidence of
A careful analysis of the synaptic investment synapses, in some rare instances it is possible to
of the migrating Purkinje cells (Sotelo et at, 1990) observe an axon terminal with pleiomorphic vesi-
shows that, despite their proximity to the adult cles establishing a symmetric synaptic contact on
cerebellar neuropil containing abundant axon the soma of a grafted neuron (Fig. 5.29). The axon
terminals, they practically lack synaptic inputs terminals engaged in these rare synapses belong
(Fig. 5.26). Only occasionally, small axon termi- to axons of host molecular layer interneurons,
nals establish a few synaptic junctions on the long stellate, and/or basket cells. Furthermore, the
and smooth dendritic processes of the migrating growing dendrites of the grafted Purkinje cells
neurons (Fig. 5.27). These rare synapses resemble begin to produce a few spines that occasionally
those observed in the cerebellum of perinatal rats, are synaptically contacted by varicosities of the
from E 19 to P2 (West and Del Cerro, 1976). They host parallel fibers. Hence, although the majority
most probably represent, as in the normal cere- of the observed synapses on Purkinje cells at
bellum (Armengol and Sotelo, 1991), transient 10-11 DAG involve host climbing fibers, some
synapses, since they are established on tran- rare synapses belonging to axons of host stellate
sient dendritic branches that regress when the or basket cells as well as to host parallel fibers
Purkinje cells reach the second phase of their den- have also been established at this early stage of
dritic development (see preceding section). The the grafted Purkinje cells' development.
functional meaning of these transient synaptic Between 12 and 14 DAG, synaptogenesis is
contacts is not yet elucidated. very active. Host climbing fibers succeed in form-
Ultimate synaptogenesis between host axons ing complete pericellular nests around the somata
and grafted Purkinje cells starts 10-11 DAG, of the grafted Purkinje cells, and their en passant
when the neurons reach the biological age of 2 to axon terminals establish numerous asymmetric
3 postnatal days. At this developmental stage, the synaptic contacts on the thin somatic filopodia.
neurons have lost their long and smooth dendrites Simultaneously, the frequency of symmetric syn-
and have begun to grow their final dendritic trees aptic complexes between host axon terminals,
("phase of the stellate cell with disoriented den- arising from stellate or basket cell axons as well
drons"). One ofthe main features of this phase is as from axon collaterals of the grafted Purkinje
the production of perikaryal filopodia (Fig. 5.28), cells, and the smooth surface of the bodies of the
which in normal ontogeny of the cerebellum cor- latter rapidly increases. Hence, during this short
respond to the postsynaptic elements of the early phase of development there is a coexistence of
synapses between climbing fibers and Purkinje climbing fibers and stellate or basket cell axons
cells (stage of pericellular nest of Ramon y Cajal, on the surface ofthe grafted Purkinje cell somata.
1911). Some of the somatic processes emerging The dendritic trees of the latter proceed with their
5. Cerebellar Grafting 109

Figure 5.28. This electron micrograph illustrates the Figure 5.29. Same material as that in Fig. 5.28. A
early formation of host climbing fiber-grafted Purkinje host axon terminal (AT), most probably belonging to
cell synapses. Two varicosities of a host climbing fiber an ascending collateral of a basket cell axon, containing
(CF) are partially covered by long somatic filopodia tubular profiles of the smooth endoplasmic reticulum
(asterisks). The arrow points the synaptic contact and pleiomorphic synaptic vesicles, establishes a sym-
between one of these varicosities and a somatic filo- metric synaptic contact (arrow) on the smooth surface
podium. pcd grafted cerebellum 11 days after grafting. of the grafted Purkinje cell body (GPC). Bar = 1 flm.
GPC, grafted Purkinje cell body. Bar = 1 flm .
110 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Figure 5.30. Electron micrograph from a pcd grafted Another varicosity (AT2) has a peridendritic location
cerebellum, 14 days after grafting. This micrograph (GPD, grafted Purkinje cell dendrite) and synapses on
illustrates the translocation of a host climbing fiber. primary dendritic spines (arrowheads). Bar = 1 pm.
One varicosity of this fiber (ATi) has a perisomatic Figure 5.31. Same material as that in Fig. 5.30, but
location around the cell body of a grafted Purkinje cell treated with a postern bedding immunogold method to
(GPC) and synapses on somatic filopodia (asterisks). detect GABA. An axon terminal containing pleiomor-
5. Cerebellar Grafting 111

ultimate. growth, and axonal varicosities of the The most immature synaptic contacts are estab-
host parallel fibers establish abundant asymmetric lished on the distal region of the head of these
synaptic contacts on newly formed dendritic slender spines. They are characterized by their
spines. long synaptic complexes that can reach a length
By 14 DAG, the grafted Purkinje cells have more than twice that of adult parallel fiber-
reached their third phase of dendritic develop- Purkinje cell synapses. More mature synaptic
ment. At this stage, the process of climbing fiber contacts are more numerous and can be identified
translocation, from their somatic to their final easily because they bear synaptic complexes of
dendritic location, has already started. Hence, decreased size (Fig. 5.32), similar to those encoun-
climbing fiber varicosities, although still numer- tered in control adult molecular layer. Hence,
ous at the somatic level, are also present parallel the formation of host parallel fiber- Purkinje cell
to the primary dendritic branches of the grafted synapses follows a sequence of events similar to
neurons, establishing asymmetric synapses on those operating during cerebellar ontogeny of
clusters of stubby spines that emerge from those control mouse, with the same regressive processes
dendritic branches (Fig. 5.30). Similarly, axon ter- ("synaptic adhesion waning" of Larramendi, 1969).
minals belonging to the host stellate or basket By 21 DAG, the synaptic investment of the
cell axons either keep their somatic location or grafted Purkinje cells is qualitatively similar to
begin to synapse on the smooth surface of the that reported in the first part of this chapter in
proximal dendrites. The use of postembedding long-term survivals. Thus, after this period of
gold immunostaining, with an anti-gamma- development, only quantitative changes result
aminobutyric acid (GABA) antibody (as previous- from the continuing synaptogenesis.
ly described by Angaut and Sotelo, 1987), has
revealed the GABA-ergic nature of the axon ter-
minals with pleomorphic vesicles and symmetric General Conclusions
synaptic contacts. Both those synapsing on the The results obtained in the series of short-term
smooth perikaryal surface (Fig. 5.31) and on the survivals indicate that grafted Purkinje cells are
smooth dendritic surface of the grafted Purkinje able to pursue their developmental program in
cell exhibit a positive immunoreaction. the cerebellum of the adult host mutant mouse.
As stated above, by 14 DAG the grafted Purkinje Indeed, the sequential critical steps followed by
cells are provided with sagittally oriented, flatten- Purkinje cells during normal cerebellar morpho-
ed dendrites comprising a proximal and a distal genesis are recapitulated by the grafted cells.
compartment. The latter consists of rapidly form- Cellular proliferation, migration, dendritic devel-
ing spiny branchlets. Synaptogenesis between opment, and synaptogenesis proceed according
host parallel fibers and branchlet spines proceeds to the same time sequence as in the control cere-
at a high rate. This situation is favorable for the bellum.
analysis of the sequential phases in the develop- As far as our results allow us to determine,
ment of host parallel fiber-grafted Purkinje cell Purkinje cell proliferation, which proceeds with
synapses, since newly formed and more mature a time window similar to that characterizing
synapses coexist. The distal dendritic spines, as Purkinje cell precursors in control mouse embryos,
in control mouse cerebellum by the end of the occurs only in the grafted primary neuroepithelium,
first postnatal week (Larramendi, 1969), appear within the transplanted cerebellar mass. What-
as long and slender processes, generally devoid ever the nature of the attracting forces of the host
of postsynaptic differentiation, growing within Purkinje cell-deficient molecular layer, these are
the adult neuropil of the host molecular layer. only exerted on young postmitotic Purkinje cells.
~------~--------------------------------------------------------------
phic synaptic vesicles and immunogold particles is in electron micrograph illustrates synaptic contacts (arrow-
synaptic contact with the smooth surface of a grafted heads) between host parallel fiber varicosities and
Purkinje cell body (GPC). The arrow points to the spines emerging from a distal spiny branchlet of a
symmetric synaptic complex. Bar = 0.5 11m. grafted Purkinje cell (GPD). Bar = 1 Jim.
Figure 5.32. Same material as that in Fig. 5.30. This
112 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

In this respect, the environmental changes implied observations indicate that the cell-to-cell interac-
in the transplantation do not alter the normal tions regulating the molding of these extraordinary
behavior of the Purkinje cell precursors. dendritic arbors in cerebellar ontogeny (see refs.
Purkinje cell migration seems to be the result in Sotelo, 1978) are also operative in the trans-
of the specific attraction of the deficient molecular planted cerebella. The acquisition of the two es-
layer of the host cerebellum. This attraction can- sential features of a Purkinje cell dendritic tree
not be exerted before the physical integration of (the flattening of the dendrites as well as their
the implants into the adult host cerebella, integra- compartmentalization with issuing input segre-
tion that is completed only 3 DAG. The early gation) also occurs in the grafts simultaneously
pathways followed by the young postmitotic with the phase of highest synapse formation,
Purkinje cells exhibit abnormalities with either particularly between host adult parallel fibers
tangential migration over the surface of the neuro- and newly emerging spines from grafted Purkinje
pil of the host molecular layer or direct penetra- cell dendrites. Hence, as already suggested by
tion from the lateral swelling of the graft, after Ramon y Cajal (1960) for normal cerebellar mor-
breaking the subpial basal lamina. Despite these phogenesis, the parallel fibers are the main
abnormalities, these neurons penetrate the host organizing element for the flattened disposition
molecular layer through a radial and/or oblique of the grafted Purkinje cell dendrites and for the
migration, resembling that followed by postmito- induction of distal spiny branchlets. Therefore,
tic Purkinje cells during normal development, the adult parallel fibers are able to proceed accord-
although they follow an inward instead of an ing to the same developmental rules that apply
outward direction. Our study also shows that the during their normal morphogenesis, when they
migratory phase lasts for 4 days and, therefore, are experimentally confronted with specific im-
that the biological age of grafted Purkinje cells mature postsynaptic partners.
at the end of their migratory period is E19, the Despite the apparent similarities in the cellular
age at which the migration of these neurons is mechanisms involved in Purkinje cell dendrito-
also completed in mouse fetuses. The cellular genesis, the timing in the maturation of dendritic
mechanisms involved in normal Purkinje cell trees from grafted Purkinje cells is slightly advanc-
migration are not well established. It has been ed when compared with that observed during
suggested, in view of the radial and/or oblique normal dendritogenesis. This precocity is effec-
trajectory of their migration from the subventri- tive only from the passage from the second to the
cular zone to the cortical plate, that they may use third phase of dendritic maturation. Indeed, the
a radial glial axis, as other cortical neurons do acquisition of the bipolar shape needed for migra-
(Rakic, 1984). The grafted Purkinje cells also tion (first phase) and the regression of the long
exhibit radial and/or oblique trajectories during and smooth dendrites (second phase) seem to be
the last phase of their migration, and some of the consequence of intrinsic mechanisms regulat-
them appear directly apposed to Bergmann fibers ed by the Purkinje cells themselves (Armengol
(Sotelo and Alvarado-Mallart, 1987c). We thus and Sotelo, 1991) and independent from synapto-
can conclude that their migration may follow genesis. Conversely, as stated above, the forma-
cellular mechanisms similar to those used during tion of the ultimate dendritic tree depends on
the normal cerebellar morphogenesis. In any case, synaptogenesis, and the latter is somewhat more
the time window of the migratory period is the precocious in the grafts than in normal cerebellar
same for Purkinje cells in normal cerebellum as ontogeny (see below).
that in implants grafted into adult pcd cerebellum. It is also remarkable to note that synapse for-
At the end of their migration, the grafted mation between grafted Purkinje cells and adult
Purkinje cells have typical bipolar shapes and host presynaptic axons proceeds according to a
begin to build up their ultimate dendritic trees. precise program, recapitulating all of the different
Our morphological analysis has revealed that the steps described during normal cerebellar synap-
three developmental phases described by Ramon togenesis (Larramendi, 1969). These steps can
y Cajal (1926) for Purkinje cell dendritogenesis be summarized as follows: a) early appearance of
are recapitulated by the grafted neurons. These transient synapses on the long and smooth dend-
5. Cerebellar Grafting 113

rites of grafted Purkinje cells during their migra- Purkinje cells there is also a transient stage of
tion, b) beginning of earliest synaptic contacts multiple innervation. This stage lasts only between
between host climbing fiber sprouts and filopodia 10 and 13 DAG and can be detected by the step-
from grafted Purkinje cell perikarya, to establish wise variation in amplitude of the climbing fiber
pericellular nests (Ramon y Cajal, 1890), c) synap- EPSPs recorded during this period. The electro-
togenesis between slender, distal dendritic spines physiological results strongly suggest that the
and host parallel fibers, together with that of ax on cell-to-cell interactions regulating the synapse
terminals from host molecular layer interneurons formation between adult climbing fibers and im-
and the smooth surface of grafted Purkinje cell mature Purkinje cells are of the same nature as
perikarya, and d) the translocation of host climb- those taking place during normal development.
ing fibers from their perisomatic to their final Moreover, since the transient period of multiple
peridendritic location. innervation is shorter and, as discussed above,
Despite the accelerated rhythm ofthe synapto- synaptogenesis between host parallel fibers and
genesis (the above-mentioned sequential steps grafted Purkinje cells also occurs in a shorter time
taking place somewhat faster in the grafts than window than in normal cerebellar morphogene-
in normal development), the timing for its begin- sis, we suggest that in the grafts, the regression of
ning is the same in both situations. In both in- the multiple innervation could be in part due
stances, the starting point seems to be marked by to competition with parallel fiber-Purkinje cell
the maturity of the Purkinje cells, since the synap- synapses.
togenic process begins only when these neurons The most important conclusion from all these
attain the second phase of their dendritic devel- studies on short-term survivals is the striking
opment. These results indicate that the presence similarity in the cellular mechanisms and chrono-
of immature Purkinje cells in the molecular layer logy ofthe sequential steps leading to the matura-
of the deficient mutant cerebellum provokes the tion and synaptic investment of Purkinje cells
sprouting of specific adult host axons with sub- during their normal ontogeny and when grafted
sequent synaptogenesis. The observed differences to the adult pcd cerebellum. This remarkable cor-
in timing may be explained by the fact that, dur- respondence allows us to postulate that matura-
ing reactive synaptogenesis, all presynaptic ele- tion of grafted Purkinje cells follows an internal
ments are available for synaptic contact in the clock that regulates all their developmental pro-
vicinity of grafted Purkinje cells from the beginning grams, independently of environmental signals.
oftheir invasion into the host molecular layer. In Thus, the presence of immature Purkinje cells in
contrast, during normal development most of the the deficient molecular layer of the host would
neurons providing presynaptic axons to the matur- allow adult neurons and glial cells to behave
ing Purkinje cells (granule, stellate, and basket transiently as if they were young postmitotic cells,
cells) do not end their proliferative period before and to interact with the grafted Purkinje cells
the first postnatal week (Fujita, 1967; Miale and according to a tempo imposed by the latter. The
Sidman, 1961). most likely, but not necessarily unique, interpre-
It has been established (Mariani and Changeux, tation ofthis adaptive behavior is that the grafted
1981;Crepelet aI., 1976, 1981)thatoneimportant Purkinje cells themselves regulate gene expression
feature of normal cerebellar synaptogenesis is the of adult neural cells by generating a transient
presence of a phase, lasting about 12 days, of re- permissive microenvironment. The end result of
dundancy of the connections between climbing this plastic process will be the quasinormal devel-
fibers and Purkinje cells. This phase is followed opment of the grafted Purkinje cells and, above
by a regressive process of numerical adjustment, all, their synaptic integration into the deficient
allowing the adult one-to-one relationship. Re- cortical circuitry. The interplay between hosts
cently, Gardette et al. (1990) have performed an and grafts, allowing the specific migration of the
electrophysiological study of the reactive synap- missing Purkinje cells, and the regulatory role
togenesis in grafting experiments and have shown played by the latter in the plastic behavior of adult
that during the establishment of synaptic connec- neurons of the host lead to the restoration of the
tions between adult host climbing fibers and grafted impaired circuitry of the mutant cerebellar cortex.
114 Constantino Sotelo and Rosa-Magda Alvarado-Mallart

Acknowledgments. We thank D·rs. D.M.D. Landis Dodd, J., and Jessell, T.M. (1988): Axon guidance and
and R.V. Rouse for helpful criticism of the manu- the patterning of neuronal projections in vertebrates.
script; Drs. P. Greengard, D.E.M. Lawson, Science, 242, 692-699.
S. Sharma, A.M. Hill, and D. Dahl for gifts, res- Fujita, S. (1967): Quantitative analysis of cell prolifera-
pectively, of anti-GMP cyclic dependent protein tion and differentiation in the cortex ofthe postnatal
mouse cerebellum. J. Cell Bioi., 32, 277-288.
kinase, calbindin, Thy-l.1, vimentin, and GFAP
Gardette, R., Alvarado-Mallart, R.M., Crepel, F., and
antibodies; Dr. J.L. Guenet for a continuous sup- Sotelo, e. (1988): Electrophysiological demonstration
ply of ped mutant mice; and Dr. S. Martinez for of a synaptic integration of transplanted Purkinje
the schematic drawings. cells into the cerebellum of the adult "Purkinje cell
degeneration" mutant mouse. Neuroscience, 24,
References 777-789.
Angaut, P., and Sotelo, e. (1987): The dentato-olivary Gardette, R., Crepel, F., Alvarado-Mallart, R.M., and
projection in the rat as a presumptive GABAergic Sotelo, e. (1990): The fate of grafted Purkinje cells
link in the olivo-cere bello-olivary loop. An ultra- in the cerebellum ofthe adult "Purkinje cell degene-
structural study. Neurosci. Lett., 83,227-231. ration" mutant mouse. II. Development of synaptic
Armengol, J.A., and Sotelo, e. (1991): Early dendritic responses: An "in vitro" study. J. Compo Neurol., 295,
development of Purkinje cells in the rat cerebellum. 188-196.
A light and electron microscopic study using axonal Goffinet, A.M. (1983): The embryonic development of
tracing in "in vitro" slices. Dev. Brain Res. 64, 95-114. the cerebellum in normal and reeler mutant mice.
Caroni, P., and Schwab, M.E. (1988): Antibody against Anat. Embryol., 168, 73-86.
myelin-associated inhibitor of neurite growth neutra- Green, M.e. (1981): Genetic Variants and Strains of the
lizes non permissive substrate properties of CNS Laboratory Mouse. Stuttgart: Gustav Fischer.
white matter. Neuron, 1, 85-96. Jande, S.S., Maler, L., and Lawson, D.E.M. (1981):
Chang, A.C., Triarhou, L.e., Alyea, e.J., Low, W.e., Immunohistochemical mapping of vitamin D-depen-
and Ghetti, B. (1988): Development of wild-type dent calcium binding protein in brain. Nature, 294,
Purkinje cells grafted to pcd mutant cerebellum dur- 765-767.
ing or after the degeneration of host Purkinje cells. Landis, S.C. (1973): Ultrastructural changes in the
Abstracts of the 11 th Annual Meeting of the European mitochondria of cerebellar Purkinje cells in "nervous"
Neuroscience Association. Eur. J. N eurosci., Supp!. mutant mice. J. Cell Bioi., 57, 782-797.
p.309. Landis, S.e., and Mullen, J.R. (1978): The development
Changeux, J.P., and Danchin, A. (1976): Selective stabi- and degeneration of Purkinje cells in "pcd" mutant
lization of developing synapses as a mechanism for mice. J. Compo Neurol., 177,125-143.
the specification of neuronal networks. Nature, 264, Larramendi, L.M.H. (1969): Analysis of the synapto-
705-712. genesis in the cerebellum of the mouse. In: Neuro-
Cowan, W.M. (1981): The development ofthe vertebrate biology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development (R.
central nervous system: An overview. In: Develop- Llinas, ed.) Chicago: American Medical Association,
ment in the Nervous System (D.R. Garrod and J.D. pp. 803-843.
Feldman, eds). Cambridge: Cambridge University Legrand, J. (1982-1983): Hormones thyroidiennes et
Press, pp. 3-33. maturation du systeme nerveux. J. Physiol. (Paris),
Crepel, F. (1971): Maturation of climbing fiber res- 78, 603-652.
ponses in the rat. Brain Res., 35, 272-276. Mariani, J., and Changeux, J.P. (1981): Ontogenesis of
Crepel, F., Delhaye-Bouchaud, N., and Dupont, J.L. olivocerebellar relationships. I. Studies by intra-
(1981): Fate of the multiple innervation of cerebellar cellular recordings of the multiple innervation of
Purkinje cells by climbing fibers in immature control, Purkinje cells by climbing fibers in the developing
X-irradiated and hypothyroid rats. Dev. Brain Res., rat cerebellum. J. Neurosci., 1,696-702.
1,59-71. Mason, D.W., and Williams, A.F. (1980): The kinetics
Crepel, F., Mariani, J., and Delhaye-Bouchaud, N. of antibody binding to membrane antigens in solution
(1976): Evidence for a multiple innervation ofPurkinje and at cell surface. Biochem. J., 187, 1-20.
cells by climbing fibers in the immature rat cere- Miale, I.L., and Sidman, R.L. (1961): An autoradio-
bellum. J. Neurobiol., 7, 567-578. graphic analysis of histogenesis in the mouse cere-
De Camilli, P., Miller, P., Levitt, P., Walter, U, and bellum. Exp. Neurol., 4, 277-296.
Greengard, P. (1984): Anatomy of cerebellar Purkinje Monard, D. (1988): Cell-derived proteases and pro-
cells in the rat determined by a specific immuno- tease inhibitors as regulators of neurite outgrowth.
histochemical marker. Neuroscience, 11, 761-817. Trends Neurosci., 11, 541-544.
5. Cerebellar Grafting 115

Morris, R.I. (1985): Thy-l in developing nervous tissue. Neural Grafting in the Mammalian CNS (A. Bjorklund
Dev. Neurosci., 7, l33-160. and U. Stenevi, eds). Fernstrom Foundation Serie!l
Mullen, R.J., Eicher, E.M., and Sidman, R.L. (1976): Vol. 5, Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 205-215.
Purkinje cell degeneration, a new neurological muta- Sotelo, e., and Alvarado-Mallart, R.M. (1986): Growth
tion in the mouse. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 73, and differentiation of cerebellar suspensions trans-
208-212. planted into the adult cerebellum of mice with
Palay, S.L., and Chan-Palay, V. (1974): Cerebellar Cortex. heredo-degenerative ataxia. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci.
Cytology and Organization. Berlin: Springer. USA,83,1135-1l39.
Rakic, P. (1984): Contact regulation of neuronal migra- Sotelo, e., and Alvarado-Mallart, R.M. (1987a): Re-
tion. In: The Cell in Contact: Adhesions and Junctions construction of the defective cerebellar circuitry in
as Morphogenetic Determinants (G.M. Edelman and adult Purkinje cell degeneration mutant mice by
J.P. Thierry, eds). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Purkinje cell replacement through transplantation
pp.67-91. of solid embryonic implants. Neuroscience, 20,1-22.
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1888a): Nota preventiva sobre la Sotelo, C., and Alvarado-Mallart, R.M. (1987b): Cere-
estructura del cerebelo. Gaceta Medica Catalana, 11, bellar transplantations in adult mice with heredo-
378. degenerative ataxia. Ann. NY Acad. Sci., 495, 242-
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1888b): Estructura de los centros 267.
nerviosos de las aves. Rev. Trim. Histol. Nor. Patol., Sotelo, c., and Alvarado-Mallart, R.M. (1987c):
1,1-10. Embryonic and adult neurons interact to allow
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1888c): Sobre las fibras nerviosas Purkinje cell replacement in mutant cerebellum.
de la capa molecular del cerebelo. Rev. Trim. Histol. Nature, 327, 421-423.
Nor. Patol., 1, 343-353. Sotelo, C., Alvarado-Mallart, R.M., Gardette, R., and
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1890): Sobre ciertos elementos Crepel, F. (1990); Fate of grafted Purkinje cells in
bipolares del cerebelo joven y algunos detalles mas the cerebellum of the adult "Purkinje cell degenera-
acerca del crecimiento y evolucion de las fibras cere- tion" mutant mouse. I. Development of reciprocal
belosas. Gaceta Sanitaria de Barcelona, 2, 208-217; graft-host interactions. J. Compo Neurol. 295, 165-
233-243. 187.
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1910): Algunas observaciones favo- Sotelo, e., and Changeux, J.P. (1974): Trans-synaptic
rabies a la hipotesis neurotropica. Trab. Lab. Invest. degeneration "en cascade" in the cerebellar cortex
Bioi. Univ. Madrid, 8, 63-l35. of the staggerer mutant mice. Brain Res., 67, 519-
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1911): H istologie du Systeme N erveux 526.
de I'H omme et des Vertebres, vol. 2. Paris: A. Maloine, Wassef, M., Simons, J., Tappaz, M.L., and Sotelo, C.
pp.80-106. (1986): Non-Purkinje cell GABAergic innervation
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1926): Sur les fibres moussues et in the deep cerebellar nuclei: A quantitative immuno-
quelques points douteux de la texture de l'ecorce cytochemical study in C57BL and in Purkinje cell
cerebelleuse. Trab. Lab. Invest. Bioi. Univ. Madrid, degeneration mutant mice. Brain Res., 399, 125-
24,215-251. l35.
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1959); Degeneration and Regenera- Wassef, M., Sotelo, e., Cholley, B., Brehier, A., and
tion ofthe Nervous System, vol. 2. New York: Hafner Thomasset, M. (1987): Cerebellar mutations affecting
Publishing Co., pp. 597-630. the postnatal survival of Purkinje cells in the mouse
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1960): Studies on Vertebrate Neuro- disclose a longitudinal pattern of differentially sen-
genesis. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas, p. 316. sitive cells. Dev. Bioi., 124, 379-389.
Sidman, R.L., and Green, M.e. (1970): "Nervous," a Wassef, M., Zanetta, J.P., Brehier, A., and Sotelo, e.
new mutant with cerebellar disease. In: Les Mutants (1985): Transient biochemical compartmentalization
Pathologiques chez I'Animal (M. Sabourdy, ed.). Paris: ofPurkinje cells during early cerebellar development.
Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, pp. 69- Dev. Bioi., 111, 129-l37.
79. West, M.I., and Del Cerro, M. (1976): Early formation
Sotelo, C. (1978): Purkinje cell ontogeny: Formation of synapses in the molecular layer of the fetal rat
and maintenance of spines. In: Maturation of the cerebellum. J. Compo Neurol., 165, l37-160.
Nervous System (M.A. Corner, R.E. Baker, N.E. van Zhou, e.F., Raisman, G., and Morris, R.I. (1985):
den Pol, D.F. Swaab and H.B.M. U ylings, eds). Prog. Specific patterns offibre outgrowth from transplants
Brain Res., 48, 149-168. to host mice hippocampi, shown immunohisto-
Sotelo, e., and Alvarado-Mallart, R.M. (1985): Cere- chemically by the use of allelic forms of Thy-I.
bellar transplants: Immunocytochemical study of Neuroscience, 16, 819-833.
the specificity of Purkinje cell inputs and outputs. In:
6
Light and Electron Microscopic
Immunocytochemistry of Putative
Neurotransmitter Amino Acids in the
Cerebellum with Some Observations on the
Distribution of Glutamine
Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

Ramon y Cajal (1888, 1889) was the first to des- Mathisen et aI., 1983), glycine (Dale et aI., 1986;
cribe accurately the different cell types in the Ottersen et aI., 1986, 1987), taurine (Madsen et aI.,
cerebellum and their interconnections. With the 1985; Ottersen et aI., 1985), glutamate (Ottersen
development ofthe electron microscope, the ultra-and Storm-Mathisen, 1984b; Storm-Mathisen
et aI., 1983), aspartate (Ottersen and Storm-
structural features of the different types of cells
and synapses were soon characterized in great Mathisen, 1985), and glutamine (Laake et aI.,
detail, so that today the synaptology of the cere-
1986). All sera were raised against protein-glutaral-
dehyde conjugates of the respective amino acids
bellar cortex must be regarded as well established
(Mugnaini, 1972; Palay and Chan-Palay, 1974). (Storm-Mathisen et aI., 1983) and were subjected
In contrast, our understanding of the chemical to one or several purification steps in solid or
nature of the cerebellar circuitries is still incom-
liquid phase before satisfactory selectivity was
obtained (for review see Ottersen and Storm-
plete. Early biochemical studies and investigations
based on immunocytochemistry of the gamma- Mathisen, 1987). The observations presented here
amino butyric acid (GABA) synthesizing enzyme, were made in postembedding-labeled sections
glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD), strongly of Durcupan-embedded rat cerebella. For light
suggested that amino acids played major roles as microscopy, semithin (0.5 /lm) sections were etched
transmitters in the cerebellum, as in other partswith sodium ethanolate and subsequently process-
of the central nervous system (CNS) (for reviews ed according to the peroxidase-antiperoxidase
see Mugnaini and Oertel, 1985; Ottersen and procedure as previously described (Ottersen, 1988;
Storm-Mathisen, 1984a). However, it was not Somogyi et aI., 1984). For electron microscopy,
until recently that the neuroactive amino acids ultrathin sections from the same blocks were col-
themselves could be visualized by immunocyto- lected on mesh grids and then treated sequential-
chemistry (Storm-Mathisen et aI., 1983), thus ly with HI0 4 , NaI0 4 (to remove osmium), the
becoming amenable to precise anatomical analysis.primary amino acid antiserum, and a secondary
In this chapter we show how amino acid im- antibody coupled to colloidal gold particles (for
munocytochemistry has provided new insight in details, see Ottersen et aI., 1988a; Somogyi and
the organization of the amino acid transmitter Hodgson, 1985). The density of gold particles in
systems in the cerebellum. the different neuronal profiles could be assessed
by a computer program (Morforel) designed for
this purpose (Blackstad et aI., 1990).
Methods Most of the material presented is from rats that
Work in our laboratory has led to the production were perfusion fixed through the heart with a
and characterization of antisera against GAB A mixture of 1%paraformaldehyde and 25% gluta-
(Ottersen and Storm-Mathisen, 1984b; Storm- raldehyde. Alternatively, in order to study the

116
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 117

synaptiG: handling of the amino acids, some cere- matograms were sprayed with glutaraldehyde
bella were rapidly removed from anesthetized and poly-L-Iysine and then processed exactly as
rats, cut into sagittal slices, and kept in artificial for immunocytochemistry (Ottersen, 1988). The
cerebrospinal fluid in vitro before immersion in taurine antiserum, for instance, only labeled one
the same fixative as above. Some ofthe slices were band in this system, the position of which corres-
exposed to 5 mmoljI K + (= physiological concen- ponded to that of authentic taurine (see Fig. 6.3D).
tration) throughout, whereas others were exposed The glutamate antiserum labeled a different band
to 55 mmoljl K + (after an initial period of stabi- that had comigrated with authentic glutamate.
lization in physiological K +) to evoke synaptic Similar results were obtained with the other anti-
release. A similar strategy was previously used sera tested. This test system should in principle
by Storm-Mathisen et ai. (1986a, b) to study the pick up any unknown cross-reacting small mole-
synaptic handling of amino acid transmitters in cule, provided it occurs in a sufficiently high con-
the hippocampus. The calcium dependency of centration.
the release process was examined in the presence The closest one can come to proving specificity
of 0.1 mmoljI Ca2+ and 1Ommoljl Mg2+. is to show that the immunoreactivity disappears
Specificity of the antisera was tested by screen- when the proposed antigen is selectively removed
ing them against more than 40 different small from the tissue. This can be achieved with taurine,
molecular compounds that had been coupled to but only in the species that are totally dependent
rat brain protein by means of glutaraldehyde and on dietary taurine due to lack of a synthetic
subsequently spotted on Millipore filters for in- machinery. Cats fall into the latter category
cubation together with Vibratome or frozen tissue (Sturman et aI., 1985). Cerebellar sections ob-
sections (Ottersen and Storm-Mathisen, 1984b), tained from 8-week-old kittens deprived of dietary
or embedded in resin for incubation together taurine both prenatally and postnatally displayed
with plastic sections (Ottersen, 1987). A series of an almost total lack of taurinelike immunoreac-
such test conjugates was routinely included in the tivity (taurine-LI), supporting the specific nature
immunocytochemical experiments for continuous of our antiserum (Madsen et aI., 1990).
monitoring of specificity (see Figs. 6.3, 6.8, 6.10,
6.11). The latter test conjugates were prepared
from the six most abundant amino acids in the Results and Discussion
brain and were incorporated in a sandwich that
GABA and Glycine
could be processed in the same drops of sera as
the tissue sections. This strategy insured identical Of all neuroactive amino acids in the cerebellum,
conditions for testing and immunocytochemistry GABA is the one that has been studied in greatest
on the light microscopical as well as on the elec- detail. Early biochemical and immunocytoche-
tron microscopical levels. With the electron micro- mical studies revealed the presence of GAD in
scope it was possible to quantify the immuno- basket, stellate, and Golgi cells (Fonnum et aI.,
labeling of the different amino acid conjugates by 1970; McLaughlin et al., 1974; Oertel et aI., 1981;
assessment of the density of gold particles over Ribak et aI., 1978; Saito et aI., 1974), and the
them (Ottersen, 1987). All antisera produced a same cell types have also been found to contain
highly selective labeling of the conjugate pre- GABA-LI (Gabbott et aI., 1986; Ottersen et aI.,
pared from the amino acid used for immuniza- 1988a; Ottersen and Storm-Mathisen, 1984b;
tion (see Figs. 6.3,6.8,6.10,6.11). No significant Seguela et aI., 1985; Somogyi et aI., 1985). Both
affinity could be detected toward glutaraldehyde- markers also occur in the axons and axon ter-
treated brain macromolecules, indicating that minals of the Purkinje cells (cf. Fonnum and
amino acids incorporated in proteins were not Walberg, 1973), whereas results differ somewhat
recognized by the specific antibodies. concerning the level of each of these markers in
The antisera were also tested against the small the Purkinje cell bodies (see, e.g., Gottlieb et aI.,
molecular fraction of a crude rat brain homo- 1986; Mugnaini and Oertel, 1985; Wu et aI., 1986).
genate. This was done after separation of the frac- Our rat material consistently shows a low level
tion by thin layer chromatography. The chro- of GABA-LI in the Purkinje cell bodies, at the
118 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

'GLY
'-

Figure 6.1. Adjacent semi thin sections through perfu- calize GABA and glycine. The vast majority of the cell
sion-fixed rat cerebellar cortex, showing the distribution bodies in the molecular layer are labeled for GABA,
of GABA-LI (A) and glycine-LI (B). The majority of but unlabeled for glycine (double arrowhead). Asterisks,
the Golgi cells (long arrows) contain both immunore- Purkinje cells; short arrows, glycine immunoreactive
activities, whereas others are single-labeled for fibers in the molecular layer. MO, molecular layer; GC,
glycine (crossed arrow) or GABA (not shown). Most of granule cell layer; WM, white matter. Bar = 50 pm.
the glomeruli (single arrowheads) also appear to colo- (Modified with permission from Ottersen et ai., 1988a.)

light as well as at the electron microscopic levels for GABA as well as glycine (Fig. 6.1); (Ottersen
(Figs. 6.1, 6.2).a The Purkinje cells differ from the et aI., 1988a). The remaining Golgi neurons were
other types of putative GABA-ergic neurons in labeled for either GABA-LI or glycine-LI. Colo-
the cerebellum by the length of their axons, and calization of the two amino acids was also de-
the low level of GABA in the Purkinje cell bodies monstrated at the level of the Golgi cell terminals
may simply reflect a rapid transfer of newly formed by means of the postembedding immunogold
GAD into the axons, allowing little time for GABA technique (Ottersen et aI., 1988a). In contrast,
synthesis in the cell bodies. the stellate and basket cells appeared to contain
Glycine-LI occurs in a sizeable subpopulation GABA, but, with few exceptions, little or no glycine
of the Golgi neurons (Fig. 6.1). Analysis of conse- (Fig. 6.1). The modest number of glycine immu-
cutive semi thin sections revealed that about 70% nopositive terminals that were encountered in
of the Golgi cells contained immunoreactivities the molecular layer displayed the ultrastructural
characteristics of stellate cell terminals and were
found to be double-labeled for GABA whenever
a Purkinje cells of chicken (Matute and Streit, 1986) this was tested (Ottersen et aI., 1988a).
and pigeon (Domenici et ai., 1988) have been reported
to contain a higher level of GABA-LI than Purkinje The biological significance of a colocalization
cells of rats. In the pigeon the Purkinje cells were of GAB A and glycine in the same neurons remains
particularly strongly labeled in folium X. to be explored. The Golgi cell terminals are
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 119

Figure 6.2. Electron micrographs showing the distri- (P) are weakly immunolabeled. Gr, granule cell. Inset
bution of GABA-LI at the transition between the shows part of a glomerulus. The Golgi cell boutons
Purkinje cell and granule cell layers (main picture), and (b) display a high concentration of gold particles,
in the deep part of the granule cell layer (inset). From whereas the granule cell dendrites (d) and the mossy
same specimen as Fig. 6.1. Note strong immunolabeling fiber boutons (Mf) display gold particle concentrations
of basket cell terminals (B) and of Golgi cell bodies similar to that over empty resin. Bars: main picture =
(Go). High densities of gold particles are also found 0.7 flm; inset = 0·4 flm.
over basket cell axons (asterisks). Purkinje cell bodies

depleted of both immunoreactivities on stimulation from purified glomerulus fractions (Morales and
by high K+(Ottersen et al., 1990a}, showing that Tapia, 1987). Both amino acids were reported to
both amino acids belong to a releasable pool. show a Ca 2 +-dependent release, although the
This is in agreement with data on biochemically glycine release appeared to be more strictly Ca 2 +
recorded eH] GABA and eH] glycine release dependent than the GABA release (Morales and
120 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

Figure 6.3. Photomicrographs showing distribution of LI in the nucleus interpositus anterior. Note strong
taurine-LI in a sagittal section of perfusion-fixed rat immunolabeling ofaxons (arrows) and nerve terminal-
cerebellar cortex (posterior vermis). A: Taurine-LI like puncta, some of which (arrowheads) appear to
occurs in Purkinje cell bodies (large asterisk), Purkinje contact unlabeled cell bodies (asterisks). D: Specificity
cell dendrites (small arrowheads), and in Purkinje cell testing. The soluble fraction of a total brain extract (E)
axons (crossed arrow), but is not detectable in stellate was applied to a cellulose gel together with free gluta-
and basket cell bodies (large arrowheads), Bergmann mate (G) or free taurine (T). After separation in n-
glial processes (small arrows), and in the granule cell butanol: acetic acid: H2 0 (4: 1: 1), the chromatograms
layer (GC). Small asterisks: Pial surface. M 0, molecular were sprayed with glutaraldehyde and poly-L-lysine,
layer. B: Enlarged detail from A, showing that taurine- and then processed exactly as for immunocytochemis-
LI extends into the Purkinje cell dendritic spines (arrow- try. The taurine antiserum labeled a single spot that
heads). Inset in B: Test section containing brain protein- had comigrated with authentic taurine (right half of
glutaraldehyde-amino acid conjugates prepared from figure), whereas the glutamate antiserum labeled a
the six most abundant amino acids in the brain. Code: separate band that had comigrated with authentic glu-
1. GABA, 2. glutamate, 3. taurine, 4. glycine, 5. none tamate (left half of figure). E: Tissue section and test
(i.e., glutaraldehyde-treated brain protein), 6. aspartate, section (inset) treated with a taurine antiserum that
and 7. glutamine. Note selective labeling of the taurine had been preadsorbed with glutaraldehyde-taurine com-
conjugates. The test section was incubated together plexes (200 liM with respect to the amino acid). Bar =
with the tissue section shown in A and B. C: Taurine- 25 lim. (From Ottersen, 1988).
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 121

Tapia, 1987). However, the issue is complicated


by the fact that Triller et al. (1987), using im-
munocytochemistry, found glycine receptors to
be localized postsynaptic to GAD-negative Golgi
cell terminals but not to GAD-positive ones. b
Thus, the possibility should be considered that
the released glycine partly acts on strychnine-
i~sensitive glycine receptors that are not recog-
nIzed by the receptor antibodies used.
Our finding of glycine-LI in Golgi cells receives
s~pport from an immunocytochemical investiga-
tIon based on a glycine antiserum different from
that used here (Campistron et aI., 1986a), and
from autoradiographic data showing that Golgi
cell boutons are endowed with high affinity uptake
mechanisms for [3H] glycine (Wilkin et aI., 1981).

Taurine
Taurine has attracted a great deal of interest as
a possible transmitter in the cerebellum following Figure 6.4. Electron micrograph of ultrathin section
through the molecular layer, treated with a taurine
the work of Yarbrough et ai. (1981) and Okamoto
antiserum. From the same specimen as in Fig. 6.3. The
et al. (1983a-d). These groups showed in the rat
Purkinje cell dendritic spines (S) are greatly enriched
and guinea pig, respectively, that the synaptically in taurine-LI, whereas the parallel fiber terminals (PI)
evoked inhibition of the Purkinje cells produced are moderately labeled (see Fig. 6.6). Bar = O.4jlm.
by electrical stimulation of the superficial part of
the molecular layer was blocked or substantially
reduced by 6-aminomethyl-3-methyl-4H, 1,2,4-
hibit the morphological features of stellate cell
benzothiadiazine-l,l-dioxide (TAG). The same
terminals and that can be found to contain GABA
compound also blocked the action of microin-
in adjacent sections (Fig. 6.6) (Ottersen et aI.,
jected taurine, but not that of GABA, on the
1988b). The electrophysiological studies described
Purkinje cells. These and additional results sug-
above and the immunocytochemical data from
gested that the actual transmitter in the stellate
this and other laboratories (Campistron et aI.,
cell synapses with the Purkinje cells was taurine.
1?86b; Ida et aI., 1987; Yoshida et al., 1986) are
It was therefore surprising to find, in our initial
dIfficult to reconcile; possible explanations have
study oftaurine in the cerebellum, that only a very
been discussed previously (Ottersen, 1988; Ottersen
small proportion ( < 2%) of the stellate neurons
et aI., 1988b). Importantly, the stellate cell termi-
were labeled with our taurine antiserum (Madsen
nals are depleted ofGABA-LI in slices exposed to
et aI., 1985). In contrast, taurine-LI occurred in
high K + in vitro (Ottersen et aI., 1990a), showing
high concentrations in the Purkinje cells. These
that the GABA that they contain belongs to a
results have since been confirmed and extended by
releasable pool. This effect is largely Ca 2 + de-
postembedding immunocytochemistry (Figs. 6.3,
pendent.
6.4, 6.5) (also see Ottersen, 1988). Quantitative
The enzyme cysteine sulphinic acid decarboxy-
electron microscopic analysis of ultrathin sections
lase (CSAD) has been proposed to be of major
treated with the taurine antiserum shows a modest
importance in the synthesis oftaurine in the CNS
density of gold particles over terminals that ex-
(for review see Wright et aI., 1986). Immunocyto-
~hemical studies have suggested that this enzyme

b Araki et al. (1988) and van den Pol and Gorcs (1988) IS concentrated in a subpopulation of Purkinje
~ound no or very low glycine receptor immunoreactivity cells that form sagittally oriented bands through-
III the granule cell layer of the cerebellum. out the cerebellar cortex (Chan-Palay et aI., 1982).
122 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

Figure 6.5. Electron micrographs


showing colocalization of taurine-LI
(A) and GABA-LI (B) in a Purkinje cell
terminal (PT), which establishes a sym-
metric contact (arrowheads) with a
small dendrite (D). From nucleus inter-
positus anterior (same specimen as in
Fig. 6.3C). Strong immunolabeling is
also found in a large proportion of the
myelinated axons (AX). E, endothelial
cell; V, vessel lumen. Bar = 0.4 11m.

Within these bands, CSAD-LI also occurred in sitometry carried out on the same sections revealed
other cell types, including stellate and granule modest variations in the level oftaurine-LI among
cells. Similar bands could not be detected by the Purkinje cells (Fig. 6.7). The small variations
visual inspection of transverse cerebellar sections observed did not conform to any zonal or banding
treated with our taurine antiserum. Specifically, pattern.
a careful analysis of consecutive semi thin sections That taurine and CSAD are differentially dis-
through the vermis and intermediate zone (where tributed does not necessarily mean that the
the CSAD-positive bands are reported to be very taurine synthesis is independent of CSAD. Thus,
conspicuous) showed that all cells that could be the very low turnover rate of taurine (5.5 days;
identified as Purkinje cells were taurine immuno- Huxtable, 1981) may allow for a considerable
reactive (Ottersen, 1988). Moreover, optical den- redistribution of this amino acid after its forma-
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 123

TAURINE - LI IN CEREBELLUM
180

160
N
E 140
::1.
.....
UI 120
Q)

- 100
u
.Cl..
a. 80
"0
0 60
Cl

40

20~
UI ..:

.....
E E
..
"0
...
E Q)
E I: I:
..: ...
E
...
E .;
...
--
0
a.
.c
0 Q)
E Q)

..
Q)

- E
..
"0 "0 Q) Q) UI

- -
UI UI
I: ci.
·c .I:
Q) Q)
~ .!!.
I:
Q)

I:
Q)
Q)
"0 .c 0
UI
Q)

Cl Q)
u Q)

Cl :>.

... ... ... ...:::J C C


..
Cl
~ UI
..:
...Cl
~ ~

...Cl
~ ~
UI Cl UI
Q) Q)
:::J :::J :::J 0 Cl Cl 0
a.. a.. a.. a.. (!) (!) a.. (!) (J) m (!) (J) ::E

Figure 6.6. Quantitative assessment of the distribution are enriched in taurine-LI compared to the other parts
oftaurine-LI in perfusion-fixed rat cerebellum (immu- of the Purkinje cells. Hatched zone indicates back-
nogold technique). The densities of gold particles over ground labeling (gold particle density over empty resin).
the different profiles were calculated by means of a From dorsal part of dentate nucleus with overlying
computer program (Morforei). Bars indicate standard cortex. Based on data from Table 1 in Ottersen, 1988
errors ofthe mean. Note that the Purkinje cell terminals which should be consulted for statistical analysis.

100
r---------------------------------------------------------
6 90
(/)
(/)

~
(/)
z
: 60

~ l~t~--------------------------------------------------------
- TOWARDS MIDLINE

Figure 6.7. Level of taurine-LI in 110 consecutive tions that could be interpreted as zonal labeling. Straight
Purkinje cell bodies, as assessed by optical densitometry line indicates the mean labeling intensity of the stellate/
in a transverse 0.5-Jlm section through posterior vermis basket cell bodies (n = 30). 100% transmission is defined
(same staining procedure as in Fig. 6.3). Each dot re- as value obtained by measurement through the slide
presents one Purkinje cell body. Note absence of varia- outside tissue section.
124 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

tion, due to active or passive release and uptake clearly indicate that CSAD is unsuitable as a
processes. It should be pointed out that taurine marker of taurine-containing neurons.
has been suggested to participate in osmoregula- By virtue of the immunogold technique, it was
tion in the CNS (Solis et aI., 1988; Wade et aI., possible to study the intracellular distribution of
1988), as previously established for other tissues taurine in the Purkinje cells in a quantitative
(Fugelli and Thoroed, 1986), in which case one manner (Fig. 6.6). Whereas there were no signifi-
must envisage that taurine can pass between dif- cant differences in the levels of immunoreactivity
ferent cellular compartments. It should also be between perikaryon, dendrites, and dendritic
recalled that alternative routes for taurine syn- spines, the Purkinje cell terminals in the deep
thesis have been described (Ca vallini et aI., 1976), cerebellar nuclei seemed to be enriched in taurine-
and that a substantial amount of taurine may be LI relative to other parts of the neuron. Coloca-
derived from the diet (Sturman et aI., 1985). Ir- lization of taurine-LI and GABA-LI could be
respective of the mechanisms involved, the dif- demonstrated by analysis of adjacent ultrathin
ferential distributions of taurine and CSAD sections through Purkinje cell terminals (Fig. 6.5).

Figure 6.8. Distribution of glutamate-LI in cerebellar Bergmann glial cell bodies (crossed arrows) and pro-
slices incubated in artificial cerebrospinal fluid contain- cesses (small arrows). The diffuse labeling in the granule
ing 5 mmolil K + (A) or 55 mmoljl K + (B). Increasing cell layer in B probably also resides in glial elements.
the K + concentration leads to a loss of glutamate-LI Arrowheads, stellate/basket cell bodies; asterisks,
from structures resembling mossy fiber terminals Purkinje cell bodies. MO, molecular layer; GC, granule
(arrows) and from the interstices between the Purkinje cell layer. Inset in B: Test section incubated together
cell dendrites in the molecular layer (corresponding to with the tissue sections shows selective labeling of
the situation of the parallel fiber terminals). These glutamate conjugates (code, see Fig. 6.3). Bar = 50 11m.
changes are associated with an enhanced labeling of
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 125

At the organelle level, taurine-LI was enriched in microscopic analyses ofthe same material confirm
mitochondria, but also over clusters of synaptic that the labeling resides in parallel fiber terminals
vesicles (Fig. 6.5) (also see Otters en, 1988). A pos- (not illustrated). In the granular layer, glutamate-
sible role of taurine, if released from the Purkinje LI is concentrated in profiles with the light and
cell terminals, could be to interact with the post- electron microscopic characteristics of mossy fiber
synaptic GABA receptors to modulate the res- terminals (Figs. 6.8, 6.9). Glial cells are weakly
ponse to GAB A (Horikoshi et ai., 1988). labeled. This pattern changes dramatically after
exposing the slice to 55 mmol/l K + to induce
Glutamate and Aspartate synaptic release (Ottersen et ai., 1990b). In such
Glutamate has long been considered to be the preparations, the labeling of the nerve terminals
neurotransmitter released by the majority of the is strongly depressed, whereas that of the glial
excitatory synapses in the CNS (for review see cells is increased (Figs. 6.8, 6.9). These changes
Fonnum, 1984; Ottersen and Storm-Mathisen, are largely Ca2+ dependent and similar to those
1984a). Immunocytochemical studies of gluta- found in hippocampal slices (Storm-Mathisen
mate are complicated by the fact that a large et ai., 1986a, b). The findings are compatible with
fraction of the glutamate in the brain participates biochemical studies, suggesting that a substantial
in various metabolic pathways rather than in proportion of the released glutamate is taken up
synaptic mechanisms (Fonnum, 1984). In agree- into glial cells (for review see Fonnum, 1984). In
ment, in perfusion-fixed brains, glutamate-LI is conclusion, the slice experiments show that the
found in all neuronal compartments, although mossy and parallel fiber terminals not only are
quantitative electron microscopic studies have enriched in glutamate, but are also endowed with
pointed to a differential distribution with highest the ability to release glutamate on depolarization.
levels in terminals of proposed glutamatergic Taken together with electron microscopic analyses
pathways (Ji et ai., 1991; Ottersen, 1987, 1989; of the distribution of glutamate-LI in perfusion
Otters en and Bramham, 1988; Somogyi et ai., fixed material (Ottersen, 1987, 1989; Ottersen and
1986; also see Rinvik and Ottersen, 1988). The Bramham, 1988; Somogyi et ai., 1986; Hamori
differences between putative glutamatergic ter- et ai., 1990), the present results from slices favor
minals and other cell compartments are accen- the idea that glutamate is a transmitter in both
tuated in brain tissue that is immersion fixed after of these fiber systems. However, particularly in
incubation in vitro, rather than fixed by perfusion the case of the mossy fiber system, it is likely that
(Fig. 6.8) (also see Storm-Mathisen et ai., 1983, some fibers release other compounds, in addition
1986a, b). This accentuation may reflect a high to, or instead of, glutamate (see, e.g., Morales and
synthetic capacity in the terminals, which provides Tapia, 1987; discussion in Beitz et ai., 1986).
a significant build-up of the transmitter during The third main type of excitatory fibers in the
the in vitro incubation (for a discussion of other cerebellum is the climbing fiber system. Several
mechanisms, see Storm-Mathisen and Ottersen, lines of evidence suggest that these fibers (or a
1988a, b). Another advantage with the slice pre- subpopulation) may use L-aspartate as a trans-
paration is that it permits us to decide whether mitter. Thus, D-[3H] aspartate is taken up by
the glutamate can be subject to synaptic release. climbing fibers and subsequently transported re-
In slices that have been incubated under physi- trogradely to the parent cell bodies in the inferior
ological K + concentrations, glutamate-LI is con- olivary complex (Matute et ai., 1987b; Wiklund
centrated in the interstices between the Purkinje et ai., 1982, 1984). Further, destruction of the
cell dendrites in the molecular layer (Fig. 6.8A). inferior olivary complex by 3-acetylpyridine leads
The labeling has a granular appearance, suggest- to a decreased calcium-dependent release of as-
ing a localization in nerve terminals. Electron
C partate from the cerebellar cortex (Cuenod et ai.,
1989; Streit et ai., 1988; Toggenburger et ai.,
1983).
C A similar pattern of glutamate-LI has been obtained
in perfusion-fixed material by means of a sensitive Despite extensive search with the light and
postembedding staining technique (Liu et aI., 1989; electron microscope in all parts of the cerebellum
Matute et aI., 1987a; Streit et aI., 1988): in several different species, we have never suc-
126 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

Figure 6.9. Electron micrographs from the same speci- of glutamate-LI in glial cell processes (G 1 in B). Gr,
men as in Fig. 6.8. From granule cell layer (A, 5 mM K +; granule cell body; b, unlabeled bouton, probably belong-
C, 55 mM K +) and molecular layer (B, 55 mM K + ). ing to a Golgi cell. Asterisks in A, swollen profiles of
Confirming the light microscopical observations, the uncertain identity; asterisks in B, parallel fibers; asterisk
immunogold technique shows that depolarization with in C, granule cell dendrite. Arrow in C points to a gap
high K + is followed by a depletion of glutamate-LI junction between glial lamellae. Bar = 0.4 j1m.
from mossy fiber terminals (Mf) and an accumulation

,,
Figure 6.10. Photomicrographs showing the distribu- arrows) are unlabeled. Depolarization with high K +
tion of aspartate-LI in the cerebellar cortex of a slice abolishes labeling of climbing fibers, but not that of
that had been immersion-fixed after incubation in arti- the Bergmann cell bodies (arrowheads). Mo, molecular
ficial cerebrospinal fluid containing 5 mM K + (A) or layer; GC granule cell layer. Inset in B: Test section
55 mM K + (B). (The same slice is represented in Figs. 6.8, incubated together with the tissue section. Only the
6.9). Note strong labeling of beaded fibers interpreted aspartate conjugates are labeled (code, see Fig. 6.3).
as climbing fibers (arrows). The Purkinje cell dendrites Bar = 50j1m.
(small arrows) and stellate/basket cell bodies (crossed
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 127

ceeded ,in displaying a significant concentration in vitro incubation. (Our failure to find a signifi-
ofL-aspartate-LI in climbing fibers after perfusion cant concentration of aspartate-LI in these fibers
fixation (however, see Campistron et aI., 1986c). in perfusion-fixed material may simply indicate
Even quantitative studies with the immunogold that the transmitter pool of the climbing fibers is
technique have failed to associate climbing fibers very small under physiological conditions in vivo.)
with endogenous aspartate (Zhang et aI., 1990). The aspartate-LI in the climbing fibers can be
In the slice preparation, however, aspartate-LI is abolished by in vitro depolarization with high
sometimes selectively concentrated in structures K + (Fig. 6.10).
that are reminiscent of climbing fibers (Fig. 6.10). It should be emphasized that the in vitro find-
Analogous to the situation for glutamate discus- ings are open to an alternative explanation. Thus,
sed above, one interpretation ofthis finding is that it is clear from our electron microscopical studies
the climbing fibers synthesize aspartate rapidly of perfusion-fixed cerebellum (Zhang et aI., 1990)
enough to cause a net accumulation during the that aspartate occurs rather diffusely in many cell

Figure 6.11. Electron micrographs of test section simi- the figure. Note selective labeling of the glutamine
lar to those represented in Figs. 6.3, 6.8, and 6.10, incu- conjugate. Spacer sections are from rat hippocampus.
bated with an antiserum against glutamine. Represent- Bar = 2/lm.
ative conjugate clumps are enlarged in right part of
128 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

<;ompartments, probably reflecting a diversity of be determined whether aspartate uptake could


metabolic roles (also see Madl et aI., 1987; explain or at least contribute to the enrichment
Ottersen and Storm-Mathisen, 1985). During the of aspartate-LI in the climbing fibers in vitro.
in vitro incubation, part of this metabolic aspar- Even when taking into account our in vitro data,
tate could leak into the medium and be subject to we feel that the sum of immunocytochemical
uptake by the climbing fibers. Thus, it remains to evidence speaks against a transmitter role of

Figure 6.12. Electron micrographs showing the distri- (G in upper panel). The test results (see Fig. 6.11) suggest
bution of glutamine-LI in the Purkinje cell layer (A) that the gold particles represent the presence of gluta-
and granule cell layer (B) of perfusion-fixed rat cerebel- mine (and not unspecific labeling), even at sites with
lar cortex. Gold particles occur over all cell profiles, very low gold particle concentrations. P, Purkinje cell
including basket cell and mossy fiber terminals (B and body; G, in lower panel, granule cell body; d, granule
Mf), but are particularly concentrated over glial cells cell dendritic digits. Bar = 0.4 Jim.
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 129

aspartate in the climbing fibers (see discussion in and subsequent conversion of these amino acids
Zhang et aI., 1990). to glutamine by glutamine synthetase.
As is the case for the mossy fiber system, climb-
ing fibers appear to contain several different neuro-
active substances. It has been claimed that the
Conclusion
climbing fibers that are capable of releasing aspar- All amino acids with proposed transmitter roles
tate are preferentially distributed to the cerebellar in the cerebellum can now be studied by direct
hemispheres (Cuenod et aI., 1989; Streit et aI., immunocytochemical methods. The postembed-
1988). Other substances that may be released ding immunogold technique permits a quanti-
from climbing fibers include homocysteate and tative assessment ofthe levels of immunoreactivity
adenosine (Cuenod et aI., 1989; Do et aI., 1986). and offers a resolution that is high enough to
Recently, corticotropin-releasing factor has attract- allow for analysis of the amino acid distribution
ed interest as an additional neuromodulator in at the organelle level. Having established the
this fiber system (Cummings et aI., 1988; Palkovits pattern of amino acid distribution in perfusion
et aI., 1987; Powers et aI., 1987). The nature of fixed tissue (reflecting the in vivo distribution),
the interaction between the different neuroactive the stage is set for exploiting immunocytochem-
compounds present in the climbing fibers remains istry for more dynamic studies. In this chapter
to be explored. we have shown that the technique can be used to
demonstrate synaptic release and glial uptake of
amino acids. We have also used our antisera to
Glutamine
analyze the redistribution of amino acids that
Glutamine is regarded as an important precursor occurs in different models of experimental epilepsy
of transmitter glutamate and has also been as- (Meldrum et aI., 1987). Future applications of
signed a role in GABA synthesis (Bradford et aI., this technique will include its use as a tool to
1978; Hamberger et aI., 1979; Kvamme, 1983). study the synthesis and metabolism of the amino
Biochemical studies have suggested that gluta- acids in the different cell compartments, in normal
mine is synthesized in the glial cells before being as well as in pathological tissue.
transferred to the neurons for subsequent con-
version to glutamate by means of phosphate-
activated glutaminase (Benjamin and Quastel, Acknowledgments. The expert technical assistance
1972; Van den Bergh et aI., 1975). In agreement of A.T. Bore (preparation of sera), K. H~genhaven,
with this theory, immunocytochemical studies J. Knutsen, and B. Riber (immunocytochemistry)
indicate that the major glutamine synthesizing is gratefully acknowledged. We also wish to thank
enzyme, glutamine synthetase, is preferentially C. Ingebrigtsen and G. Lothe for photographi-
localized in glial cells (Norenberg and Martinez- cal assistance, T. Eliassen for typing and editing
Hernandez, 1979). the manuscript, and Drs. J. Storm-Mathisen and
The results obtained with our glutamine anti- F. Walberg for constructive criticism ofthe manus-
serum (Laake et aI., 1986), which shows no de- cript. Supported by the Norwegian Council for
tectable cross-reactivity with glutamate or other Science and the Humanities, the Norwegian
amino acids tested (Fig. 6.11), are consistent with Council on Cardiovascular Disease, the Nor-
the above data. The highest level of glutamine-LI wegian Society for Fighting Cancer, and Odd
is found in glial cells (both astrocytes and oligo- Fellows Research Fund.
dendrocytes), but modest labeling also occurs
in putative glutamatergic as well as in putative
GABA-ergic terminals (Fig. 6.12) (also see Ji et aI.,
References
1991; Zhang et aI., 1991) compatible with the
Araki, T., Yamano, M., Murakami, T., Wanaka, A.,
proposed precursor role of glutamine. The glial Betz, H., and Tohyama, M. (1988): Localization of
labeling is strongly enhanced in slices exposed to glycine receptors in the rat central nervous system:
high K + (not shown). This probably reflects an An immunocytochemical analysis using monoclonal
increased glial uptake of glutamate (and GABA) antibody. Neuroscience, 25, 613-624.
130 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

B,?itz, AJ., Larson, A.A., Monaghan, P., Altschuler, sulfur-containing amino acids from various rat
R.A., Mullett, M.M., and Madl, J.E. (1986): Immuno- brain regions. J. Neurochem., 46, 779-786.
histochemical localization of glutamate, glutamine Domenici, L., Waldvogel, HJ., Matute, c., and Streit,
and aspartate aminotransferase in neurons of the P. (1988): Distribution of GAB A-like immuno-
pontine nuclei of the rat. Neuroscience, 17, 741-753. reactivity in the pigeon brain. Neuroscience, 25,
Benjamin, A.M., and Quastel, J.H. (1972): Locations 931-950.
of amino acids in brain slices from the rat. Tetro- Fonnum, F. (1984): Glutamate: A transmitter in mam-
dotoxin-sensitive release of amino acids. Biochem. malian brain. J. Neurochem., 42,1-11.
J., 128, 631-646. Fonnum, F., Storm-Mathisen, J., and Walberg, F.
Blackstad, T.W., Karagiille, T., and Ottersen, O.P. (1970): Glutamate decarboxylase in inhibitory
(1990): MORFOREL, a computer program for two- neurons. A study of the enzyme in Purkinje cell
dimensional analysis of micrographs of biological axons and boutons in the cat. Brain Res., 20,
specimens, with emphasis on immunogold pre- 259-275.
parations. Comput. BioI. Med., 20, 15-34. Fonnum, F., and Walberg, F. (1973): An estimation
Bradford, H.F., Ward, H.K., and Thomas, A.J. (1978): of the concentration of y-aminobutyric acid and
Glutamine-a major substrate for nerve endings. glutamate decarboxylase in the inhibitory Purkinje
J. Neurochem., 30, 1453-1459. axon terminals of the cat. Brain Res., 54, 115-
Campistron, G., Buijs, R.M., and Geffard, M. (1986a): 127.
Glycine neurons in the brain and spinal cord. Fugelli, K., and Thoroed, S.M. (1986): Taurine
Antibody production and immunocytochemical transport associated with cell volume regulation in
localization. Brain Res., 376, 400-405. flounder erythrocytes under anisosmotic conditions.
Campistron, G., Geffard, M., and Buijs, R.M. (1986b): J. Physiol., 374, 245-261.
Immunological approach to the detection of taurine Gabbott, P.L.A., Somogyi, J., Stewart, M.G., and
and immunocytochemical results. J. Neurochem., Hamori, J. (1986): GABA-immunoreactive neurons
46,862-868. in the rat cerebellum: a light and electron micro-
Campistron, G., Buijs, R.M., and Geffard, M. (1986c): scopic study. J. Compo Neurol., 251, 474-490.
Specific antibodies against aspartate and their Gottlieb, D.I., Chang, y.-c., and Schwob, J.E. (1986):
immunocytochemical application in the rat brain. Monoclonal antibodies to glutamic acid decar-
Brain Res., 365, 179-184. boxylase. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 83, 8808-
Cavallini, D., Scandurra, R., Dupre, S., Santoro, L., 8812.
and Barra, D. (1976): A new pathway of taurine Hamberger, A.C., Chiang, G.H., Nylen, E.S., Scheff,
biosynthesis. Physiol. Chern. Physics, 8, 157-160. S.W., and Cotman, C.W. (1979): Glutamate as a
Chan-Palay, V., Palay, S.L., and Wu, J.-Y. (1982): CNS transmitter. I. Evaluation of glucose and gluta-
Sagittal cerebellar micro bands of taurine neurons: mine as precursors for the synthesis of preferentially
Immunocytochemical demonstration by using anti- released glutamate. Brain Res., 168, 513-530.
bodies against the taurine-synthesizing enzyme Hamori, 1., Takacs, J., and Petrusz, P. (1990): Im-
cysteine sulfinic acid decarboxylase. Proc. N atl. Acad. munogold electron microscopic demonstration of
Sci., 79, 4221-4225. glutamate and GABA in normal and deafferented
Cuenod, M., Do, K.Q., Vollenweider, F., Klein, A., cerebellar cortex: Correlation between transmitter
and Streit, P. (1989): The puzzle of the transmit- content and synaptic vesicle size. J. Histochem.
ters in the climbing fibers. In: Experimental Brain Cytochem., 38, 1767-1777.
Research Series (Symposium on The Olivocere- Horikoshi, T., Asanuma, A., Yanagisawa, K., Anzai,
bellar System in Motor Control, Turin). Berlin: K., and Goto, S. (1988): Taurine and p-alanine act
Springer. on both GABA and glycine receptors in Xenopus
Cummings, S., Sharp, 8., and Elde, R. (1988): Cor- oocyte injected with mouse brain messenger RNA.
ticotropin-releasing factor in cerebellar afferent Mol. Brain Res., 4, 97-105.
systems: A combined immunocytochemistry and Huxtable, R.J. (1981) Sources and turnover rates of
retrograde transport study. J. N eurosci., 8,543-554. taurine in nursing and weaned rat pups. J. Nutr.,
Dale, N., Ottersen, O.P., Roberts, A., and Storm- 11,1275-1286.
Mathisen, 1. (1986): Inhibitory neurones of a motor Ida, S., Kuriyama, K., Tomida, Y., and Kimura, H.
pattern generator in Xenopus revealed by antibodies (1987): Antisera against taurine: Quantitative char~
to glycine. Nature (Lond.), 324, 255-257. acterization of the antibody specificity and its ap-
Do, K.Q., Mattenberger, M., Streit, P., and Cuenod, plication to immunohistochemical study in the rat
M. (1986): In vitro release of endogenous excitatory brain. J. Neurosci. Res., 18,626-631.
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 131

Ji, Z., Aas, J.-E., Laake, J., Walberg, F., and Ottersen, with antisera against conjugated GABA, glutamate,
O.P. (1991): An electron microscopic analysis of and aspartate. Neuroscience, 22, 17-27.
glutamate and glutamine in terminals of rat spino- Morales, E., and Tapia, R. (1987): Neurotransmitters
cerebellar fibers.J. Compo Neurol., 307, 1-15. of the cerebellar glomeruli: Uptake and release of
Kvamme, E. (1983): Glutamine. In: Handbook of labeled y-aminobutyric acid, glycine, serotonin and
Neurochemistry, Vol. 3, 2nd ed (A. Lajtha, ed). New choline in a purified glomerulus fraction and in
York: Plenum, pp. 405-422. granular layer slices. Brain Res., 420, 11-21.
Laake, J.H., Gundersen, V., Nordblll, G., Ottersen, Mugnaini, E. (1972): The histology and cytology of
O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J. (1986): An antiserum the cerebellar cortex. In: The Comparative Anatomy
against glutamine,. In: Excitatory Amino Acids and Histology of the Cerebellum. The Human
(P.J. Roberts, J. Storm-Mathisen, and H.F. Cerebellum, Cerebellar Connections and Cerebellar
Bradford, eds). London: Macmillan, pp. 448- Cortex (0. Larsell, and J. Jansen, eds). Minneapolis:
450. University of Minnesota Press, pp. 201-264.
Liu, C.J., Grandes, P., Matute, c., Cuenod, M., and Mugnaini, E., and Oertel, W.H. (1985): An atlas ofthe
Streit, P. (1989): Glutamate-like immunoreactivity distribution of GABAergic neurons and terminals
revealed in rat olfactory bulb, hippocampus and in the rat CNS as revealed by GAD immuno-
cerebellum by monoclonal antibody and sensitive cytochemistry. In: Handbook of Chemical Neuro-
staining method. Histochemistry 90, 427-445. anatomy, Vol. 4. (A)Jjorklund, and T. Hokfelt, eds).
Madl, J.E., Beitz, AJ., Johnson, R.L., and Larson, A.A. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 436-608.
(1987): Monoclonal antibodies specific for fixative- Norenberg, M.D., Martinez-Hernandez, A. (1979):
modified aspartate: Immunocytochemical localiza- Fine structural localization of glutamine synthetase
tion in the rat CNS. J. Neurosci., 7, 2639-2650. in astrocytes of rat brain. Brain Res., 161, 303-310.
Madsen, S., Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J. Oertel, W.H., Schmechel, D.E., Mugnaini, E., Tappaz,
(1985): Immunocytochemical visualization of taurine: M.L., and Kopin, U. (1981): Immunocytochemical
Neuronal localization in the rat cerebellum. localization of glutamate decarboxylase in rat
Neurosci. Lett., 60, 255-260. cerebellum with a new antiserum. Neuroscience, 6,
Madsen, S., Ottersen, O.P., Storm-Mathisen, J., and 2715-2735.
Sturman, IA. (1990): Immunocytochemical locali- Okamoto, K., Kimura, H., and Sakai, Y. (1983a):
zation of taurine: Methodological aspects. In: Taurine: Evidence for taurine as an inhibitory neurotrans-
Functional Neurochemistry, Physiology, and Cardio- mitter in cerebellar stellate interneurons: Selective
logy (H. Pasantes-Morales, et aI., eds). New York: antagonism by TAG (6-aminomethyl-3-methyl-
Wiley-Liss, pp. 37-44. 4 H, 1,2,4-benzothiadiazine-1, 1-dioxide). Brain Res.,
Matute, c., Liu, CJ., Grandes, P., Cuenod, M., Streit, 265, 163-168.
P. (1987a): Glutamate-like immunoreactivity re- Okamoto, K., Kimura, H., and Sakai, Y. (1983b):
vealed in rat brain by monoclonal antibody and Antagonistic action of 6-aminomethyl-3-methyl-
sensitive staining method. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 13, 4H,1,2,4-benzothiadiazine-1,1-dioxide (TAG) and
1562. evidence for a transmitter role of taurine in stellate
Matute, C., and Streit, P. (1986): Monoclonal anti- interneurons in the cerebellum. In: Sulfur Amino
bodies demonstrating GABA-like immunoreac- Acids: Biochemical and Clinical Aspects. (K.
tivity. Histochemistry, 86, 147-157. Kuriyama, R. Huxtable, and H. Iwata, eds). New
Matute, C., Wiklund, L., Streit, P., and Cuenod, M. York: Alan R. Liss, pp. 151-160.
(1987b): Selective retrograde labeling wih D-eH]- Okamoto, K., Kimura, H., and Sakai, Y. (1983c):
aspartate in the monkey olivocerebellar projection. Taurine-induced increase of the Cl-conductance of
Exp. Brain Res., 66, 445-447. cerebellar Purkinje dendrites in vitro. Brain Res.,
McLaughlin, B.J., Wood, IG., Saito, K., Barber, R., 259, 319-323.
Vaughn, J.E., Roberts, E., and Wu, I-Y. (1974): The Okamoto, K., Kimura, H., and Sakai, Y. (1983d): Ionic
fine structural localization of glutamate decarboxy- mechanisms of the action of taurine on cerebellar
lase in synaptic terminals of rodent cerebellum. Purkinje cell dendrites in vitro: Intradendritic study.
Brain Res., 76, 377-391. Brain Res., 260, 261-269.
Meldrum, B.S., Swan, J.H., Ottersen, O.P., and Ottersen, O.P. (1987): Postembedding light- and elec-
Storm-Mathisen, J. (1987): Redistribution of trans- tron microscopic immunocytochemistry of amino
mitter amino acids in rat hippocampus and cere- acids: Description of a new model system allowing
bellum during seizures induced by L-allylglycine identical conditions for specificity testing and tissue
and bicuculline: An immunocytochemical study processing: Exp. Brain Res., 69, 167-174.
132 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

Qttersen, O.P. (1988): Quantitative assessment of Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J., Madsen, S.,
taurine-like immunoreactivity in different cell types Skumlien, S., and Str9lmhaug, J. (1986): Evaluation
and processes in rat cerebellum: An electro micro- of the immunocytochemical method for amino
scopic study based on a postembedding immunogold acids. Med. BioI., 64, 147-158.
labelling procedure. Anat. Embryol., 178,407-421. Ottersen, O.P., Storm-Mathisen, J., and Somogyi, P.
Ottersen, O.P. (1989): Postembedding immunogold (1988a): Co localization of glycine-like and GABA-
labelling of fixed glutamate: An electron micro- like immunoreactivities in Golgi cell terminals in
scopic analysis of the relationship between gold the rat cerebellum: a postembedding light and elec-
particle density and antigen concentration. J. Chem. tron microscopic study. Brain Res., 450, 342-353.
Neuroanat., 2, 57-66. Ottersen, O.P., Storm-Mathisen, J., and Laake, J.H.
Ottersen, O.P., and Bramham, C.R. (1988): Quantita- (1990a): Cellular and subcellular localization of
tive electron microscopic immunocytochemistry of glycine studied by quantitative electron microscopic
excitatory amino acids. In: Frontiers in Excitatory immunocytochemistry. In: Glycine Neurotransmis-
Amino Acid Research, (E.A. Cavalheiro, J. Lehmann, sion. (O.P. Ottersen and J. Storm-Mathisen, eds).
and L. Turski, eds). New York: Alan R. Liss, pp. Chichester: Wiley, pp. 303-328.
93-100. Ottersen, O.P., Laake, J.H., and Storm-Mathisen, J.
Ottersen, O.P., Davanger, S., and Storm-Mathisen, J. (1990b): Demonstration of a releasable pool of
(1987): Glycine-like immunoreactivity in the cere- glutamate in cerebellar mossy and parallel fibre
bellum of rat and Senega:ese baboon, Papio papio: terminals by means oflight and electron microscopic
A comparison with the distribution of GABA-like immunocytochemistry. Arch. Ital. BioI., 128, 111-
immunoreactivity and with [3H]glycine and [3H]- 125.
GABA uptake. Exp. Brain Res., 66, 211-221. Palay, S.L., and Chan-Palay, V. (1974): Cerebellar
Ottersen, O.P., Madsen, S., Meldrum, B.S., and Storm- Cortex. Berlin-Heidelberg-New York: Springer.
Mathisen, J. (1985): Taurine in the hippocampal Palkovits, M., Uninth, c., Gorcs, T., and Young, W.S.
formation of the Senegalese baboon Papio papio: III (1987): Corticotropin-releasing factor in the
An immunocytochemical study with an antiserum olivocerebellar tract of rats: demonstration by
against conjugated taurine. Exp. Brain Res., 59, light- and electron-microscopic immunohistochem-
457-462. istry and in situ hybridization histochemistry. Proc.
Ottersen, O.P., Madsen, S., Storm-Mathisen, J., Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 84, 3911-3915.
Somogyi, P., Scopsi, L., and Larsson, L.-1. (1988b): Powers, R.E., DeSouza, E.B., Walker, L.c., Price,
Immunocytochemical evidence suggests that taurine D.L., Vale, W.W., and Young, W.S. III (1987):
is colocalized with GABA in the Purkinje cell Corticotropin-releasing factor as a transmitter in
terminals, but that the stellate cell terminals pre- the human olivocerebellar pathway. Brain Res., 415,
dominantly contain GABA: A light- and electron 347-352.
microscopic study of the rat cerebellum. Exp. Brain Ramon y Cajal, S. (1888): Sobre las fibras nervi os as
Res., 72, 407-416. de la capa molecular del cerebelo. Rev. Trimestr.
Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, 1. (1984a): Neurons Histol., 2, 33-41.
containing or accumulating transmitter amino Ramon y Cajal, S. (1889): Sobre las fibras nerviosas
acids. In: Handbook oj Chemical Neuroanatomy, de la capa granulosa del cerebelo. Rev. Trimestr.
Vol. 3. (A. Bjorklund, T. Hokfelt, and M.J. Kuhar, Histol., 4, 107-118.
eds). Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 141-246. Ribak, C.E., Vaughn, J.E., and Saito, K. (1978):
Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J. (1984b): Glu- Immunocytochemical localization of glutamic acid
tamate- and GABA-containing neurons in the decarboxylase in neuronal somata following colchi-
mouse and rat brain as demonstrated with a new cine inhibition of axonal transport. Brain Res., 140,
immunocytochemical technique. J. Compo Neurol., 315-332.
229, 374-392. Rinvik, E., and Ottersen, O.P. (1988): Demonstration
Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J. (1985): Differ- of GABA and glutamate in the nucleus reticularis
ent neuronal localization of aspartate-like and glu- thalami: A postembedding immunogold-Iabelling
tamate-like immunoreactivities in the hippocam- investigation in the cat and baboon. In: Cellular
pus of rat, guinea pig, and Senegalese baboon (Papio Thalamic Mechanisms. (M. Bentivoglio, and R.
papio), with a note on the distribution of GABA. Spreafico, eds). Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 321-337.
Neuroscience, 16, 589-606. Saito, K., Barber, R., Wu, J.-Y., Matsuda, T., Roberts,
Ottersen, O.P., and Storm-Mathisen, J. (1987): Local- E., and Vaughn, J.E. (1974): Immunohistochemical
ization of amino acid neurotransmitters by immuno- localization of glutamate decarboxylase in rat
cytochemistry. Trends Neurosci., 10, 250-255. cerebellum. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci. USA, 71, 269-273.
6. Immunocytochemistry of Putative Neurotransmitter Amino Acids 133

Seguela, P., Gamrani, H., Geffard, M., Calas, A., and Localization and release of excitatory amino acids.
Le Moal, M. (1985): Ultrastructural immunocyto- In: Frontiers in Excitatory Amino Acid Research
chemistry of y-aminobutyrate in the cerebral and (E. Cavalheiro, J. Lehmann, and L. Turski, eds).
cerebellar cortex of the rat. Neuroscience, 16, New York: Alan R. Liss, pp. 101-108.
865-874. Sturman, J.A., Moretz, R.C., French, 1.H., and
Solis, J.M., Herranz, A.S., Herreras, 0., Lerma, J. and Wisniewski, H.M. (1985): Taurine deficiency in the
del Rio, R.M. (1988): Does taurine act as an osmo- developing cat: Persistence of the cerebellar external
regulatory substance in the rat brain? Neurosci. granule cell layer. J. Neurosci. Res., 13, 405-
Lett., 91, 53-58. 416.
Somogyi, P., Halasy, K., Somogyi, J., Storm-Mathisen, Toggenburger, G., Wiklund, L., Henke, H., and Cuenod,
J., and Ottersen, O.P. (1986): Quantification of im- M. (1983): Release of endogenous and accumulated
munogold labelling reveals enrichment of gluta- amino acids from slices of normal and climbing
mate in mossy and parallel fibre terminals in cat fiber-deprived rat cerebellar slices. J. Neurochem.,
cerebellum. Neuroscience, 19, 1045-1050. 41,1606-1613.
Somogyi, P., and Hodgson, A.J. (1985): Antiserum to Triller, A., Cluzeaud, F., and Korn, H. (1987): GABA-
y-aminobutyric acid. III Demonstration of GABA containing terminals can be apposed to glycine
in Golgi-impregnated neurons and in conventional receptors at central synapses. J. Cell. Bioi., 104,
electron microscopic sections of cat striate cortex. 947-956.
J. Histochem. Cytochem., 33, 249-257. Van den Bergh, C.1., Matheson, D.F., Ronda, G.,
Somogyi, P., Hodgson, A.J., Chubb, I.W., Penke, B., Reijnierse, G.L.A., Blokhuis, G.G.D., Kroon, M.C.,
and Erdei, A. (1985): Antisera to y-aminobutyric Clarke, D.O., and Garfinkel, D. (1975): A model of
acid. II Immunocytochemical application to the glutamate metabolism in brain: A biochemical
central nervous system. J. Histochem. Cytochem., analysis of a heterogeneous structure. In: Metabolic
33,240-248. Compartmentation and N eurotransmission. Relation
Somogyi, P., Hodgson, A.J., Smith, A.D., Nunzi, M.G., to Brain Structure and Function (S. Ber!, D.O.
Gorio, A., and Wu, 1.-Y. (1984): Different popula- Clarke, and D. Schneider, eds). New York: Plenum
tions of GABAergic neurons in the visual cortex Press, pp. 515-543.
and hippocampus of cat contain somatostatin- van den Pol, A.N., and Gorcs, T. (1988): Glycine and
or cholecystokinin-immunoreactive material. J. glycine receptor immunoreactivity in brain and
Neurosci., 4, 2590-2603. spinal cord. J. Neurosci., 8, 472-492.
Storm-Mathisen, 1., Leknes, A.K., Bore, A.T., Vaaland, Wade, J.V., Olson, J.P., Samson, F.E., Nelson, S.R.,
J.L., Edminson, P., Haug, F.M.S., and Ottersen, and Pazdernik, T.L. (1988): A possible role for
O.P. (1983): First visualization of glutamate and taurine in osmoregulation within the brain. J.
GABA in neurones by immunocytochemistry. Neurochem., 51, 740-745.
Nature (Lond.), 301,517-520. Wiklund, L., Toggenburger, G., Cuenod, M. (1982):
Storm-Mathisen, 1., and Ottersen, O.P. (1988a): Aspartate: Possible neurotransmitter in cerebellar
Localization of excitatory amino acid transmitters. climbing fibres. Science, 216, 78-79.
In: Excitatory Amino Acids in Health and Disease Wiklund, L., Toggenburger, G., and Cuenod, M. (1984):
(D. Lodge, ed). Chichester: John Wiley, pp. 107-141. Selective retrograde labelling of the rat olivocerebellar
Storm-Mathisen, J., and Ottersen, O.P. (1988b): climbing fiber system with D-[3H]-aspartate. N euro-
Anatomy of putative glutamatergic neurons. In: science, 13, 441-468.
Neurotransmitters and Cortical Function. (M. Avoli, Wilkin, G.P., Csillag, A., Balazs, R., Kingsbury, A.E.,
T.A. Reader, R.W. Dykes, and P. Gloor, eds). New Wilson, J.E., and Johnson, A.L. (1981): Localization
York: Plenum, pp. 39-70. of high affinity [3H]glycine transport sites in the
Storm-Mathisen, J., Ottersen, O.P., and Fu-Long, T. cerebellar cortex. Brain Res., 216, 11-33.
(1986a): Antibodies for the localization of excitatory Wright, C.E., Tallan, H.H., and Lin, Y.Y. (1986):
amino acids. In: Excitatory Amino Acids. (P.J. Taurine: Biological update. Annu. Rev. Biochem., 55,
Roberts, J. Storm-Mathisen, and H.F. Bradford, 427-453.
eds). London: Macmillan, pp. 101-116. Wu, J.-Y., Denner, L.A., Wei, S.c., Lin, c.-T., Song,
Storm-Mathisen, J., Ottersen, O.P., Fu-Iong, T., G.-X., Xu, Y.F., Liu, J.W., and Lin, H.8. (1986):
Gundersen, V., Laake, J.H., and Nordb~, G. (1986b): Production and characterization of polyclonal and
Metabolism and transport of amino acids studied monoclonal antibodies to rat brain L-glutamate
by immunocytochemistry. Med. Bioi., 64, 127-132. decarboxylase. Brain Res., 373, 1-14.
Streit, P., Do, K.Q., Vollenweider, F., Grandes, P., Liu, Yarbrough, G.G., Singh, O.K., and Taylor, D.A.
C.J., Matute, C., and Cuenod, M. (1988): (1981): Neuropharmacological characterization of
134 Ole P. Ottersen and Jon H. Laake

a taurine antagonist. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther.,219, the issue of metabolic compartmentation. Anat.
604-613. Embryol., 184,213-223.
Yoshida, M. Karasawa, N., Ito, M., Sakai, M., and Zhang, N., Walberg, F., Laake, lH., Meldrum, B.S., and
Nagatsu, I. (1986): Demonstration oftaurineimmuno- Ottersen, O.P. (1990): Aspartate-like and glutamate-
reactive structures in the rat brain. Neurosci. Res., like immunoreactivities in the inferior olive and
3,356-363. climbing fibre system: A light microscopic and
Zhang, N., Laake, J., Nagelhus, E., Storm-Mathisen, semiquantitative electron microscopic study in rat
J., and Ottersen, O.P. (1991): Distribution of and baboon (PAPIO ANUBIS). Neuroscience, 38,
glutamine-like immunoreactivity in the cerebellum 61-80.
of rat and baboon (Papio anubis) with reference to
7
The Expanding Role of the Basilar Pontine Nuclei
as a Source of Cerebellar Afferents
G.A. Mihailoff, R.J. Kosinski, S.A. Azizi, H.S. Lee, and B.G. Border

Among those studying the structural and func- lular elements involved in these circuits, one might
tional organization of the cerebellum, one might assume that such activities represent the domi-
expect that there would be almost universal agree- nant functional operation of the BPN. However,
ment that information transmitted to the cere- recent observations regarding the connectivity
bellum from the cerebral cortex plays an impor- and intrinsic cytologic and synaptic organiza-
tant role in cerebellar function. In fact, the lateral tion of the BPN suggest that this brain stem cell
cerebellar hemispheres are often referred to as the group is involved in a more diverse array of pre-
neocerebellum since this portion of the cerebellar cerebellar activity than has previously been recog-
cortex is thought to derive the majority of its nized. First, it has been shown that the dorsal
input from neocortex. Furthermore, although this column nuclei (and spinal cord neurons) project
has not been analyzed or quantified in a rigorous to the BPN (Swenson et aI., 1984) and ultra-
manner, it is obvious that as the volume of the structural studies suggest further that two types
neocortical mantle increases across phylogeny, of dorsal column nuclei (DorCN) cells might be
there is a parallel increase in the volume of the involved in this projection (Kosinski et aI., 1986).
cerebellar hemispheres that presumably reflects This finding, in conjunction with the observations
an increase in cerebral cortical input. It is perpaps of Shambes et ai. (1978), which describe multi-
unexpected then that there are no direct projec- synaptic, peripheral cutaneous inputs to granular
tions extending from the cerebral cortex to the cells ofthe cerebellar cortex suggest that the BPN
cerebellum except for rather sparse transient con- might possibly represent one cell group that
nections present during a relatively brief period conveys these signals to the cerebellar cortex.
early in the postnatal development of the central Second, the presence of gamma-aminobutyric acid
nervous system (Distel and Hollander, 1980; (GABA)-ergic neurons was reported in the BPN
Tolbert and Panneton, 1983, 1984). Thus, corti- of the rat (Border and MihailofT, 1985) and more
cal signals to the cerebellum are transmitted recently in the cat and monkey (Brodal et aI.,
indirectly via cortical projections to various pre- 1988; Thier and Koehler, 1987). These findings
cerebellar nuclei, which then provide access to the correlate with the observations of Ramon y Cajal
cerebellar nuclei and cortex. Based on numbers (1911), who illustrated a heavily spine-laden cell
of fibers (Allen and Tsukahara, 1974), it appears within the BPN that gives rise to an axon that
that the principal precerebellar cell group involved ramifies extensively within the dendritic domain
in the linkage of the cerebral and cerebellar cor- of the parent neuron and presumably represents
tices is the basilar pontine nuclei (BPN). a local-circuit type neuron. In addition, previous
In line with these considerations, the BPN are Golgi and electron microscopic studies have
often viewed from a narrow functional perspective demonstrated cellular and synaptic features that
as faithful transmitters of cerebrocerebellar infor- also suggest the possible existence oflocal-circuit
mation. Again, because of the vast number of cel- type neurons in the BPN (Cooper and Beal, 1978;

135
136 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

(ooper and Fox, 1976; Mihailoff and McArdle, basilar pons is shown in Figure 7.1 along with
1981; Mihailoff and King, 1975; Mihailoff et aI., illustrations of two representative WGA-HRP
1981a). The implication then is that the BPN injection sites in the BPN.
might function in more of an integrative capacity Much to our surprise, more than 50 cell groups
than has previously been considered. were identified in these experiments as having a
In this chapter we review a series of observ- projection to the BPN. Their distribution is plotted
ations that can be divided into four major cate- in Figures 7.3 and 7.4. Among these afferent sys-
gories: a) a survey of noncortical BPN afferent tems are a number of newly identified inputs such
systems, b) a demonstration that some BPN af- as the central amygdaloid nucleus, the nucleus
ferent projections contain GABA-ergic compo- basalis of Meynert, and the horizontal nucleus of
nents, c) an illustration of certain BPN cytological the diagonal band of Broca. Many BPN afferent
and synaptic features, and d) a review of recently cells were also newly identified in the midbrain;
obtained findings regarding the functional pro- these included somata adjacent to the red nucleus
perties of BPN neurons. These observations pro- (perirubral cells), the medial and lateral terminal
vide additional support for the concept that the nuclei, nuclei of the posterior commissure,
BPN, in addition to being involved in the trans- Edinger-Westphal nucleus, nucleus of Darksche-
mission of cerebrocerebellar information, also witsch, periaqueductal gray, substantia nigra pars
receive a substantial variety of other afferent in- lateralis and reticulata, peri peduncular nucleus,
formation that is processed and integrated in and portions of the mesencephalic reticular for-
some manner before eventually being forwarded mation. Among the newly identified inputs from
to the cerebellar cortex. Consequently, the BPN medullary and pontine levels of the brain stem
appear to playa broader role as a source of were the trigeminal chief sensory nucleus, nucleus
cerebellar afferent information than has common- X of the vestibular complex, the pedunculopon-
ly been recognized in the past. tine tegmental and dorsal tegmental nuclei, numer-
ous components of the medullary reticular form-
ation, and certain raphe nuclei (dorsalis, obscurus,
Results magnus, pallidus). Also newly identified as BPN
afferents were several cell groups known to have
Survey of Noncortical BPN Afferent Systems
direct projections to the cerebellum such as the
The retrograde transport of wheat germ agglutinin lateral reticular nucleus, nucleus prepositus, the nu-
conjugated to horseradish peroxidase (WGA- cleus of Roller, the parabrachial nuclei, and the locus
HRP) was used in rats to identify the cell bodies coeruleus and subcoeruleus. In addition to each of
of origin for all afferent projections to the BPN these newly identified inputs, all cell groups reported
with the exception of the cerebral cortex (Mihailoff in previous studies to give rise to BPN afferents were
et aI., 1988b). With a parapharyngeal surgical confirmed. Included in this category are neurons in
approach, the injection micropipette was placed several hypothalamic nuclei, the zona incerta, various
into the basilar pons under visual control. Although pretectal nuclei and the nucleus of the optic tract,
technically difficult, this approach was particu- superior colliculus, ventral lateral geniculate nu-
larly advantageous because it avoided the comp- cleus, cerebellar nuclei, spinal trigeminal nucleus,
lication of false-positive labeling that might have dorsal column nuclei, external cuneate nucleus,
resulted from a dorsal approach to the BPN and and nucleus dorsalis at lumbar levels of the spinal
the possible transection and injury-filling ofaxo- cord.
nal systems coursing longitudinally through the Because a number of the labeled cell groups
brain stem immediately dorsal to the BPN. Cases in these studies were known to have direct projec-
were discarded if histological sections through tions to the cerebellum, there was a concern that
the injection site illustrated any tissue disruption some or all of such neurons might have been
by the micropipette outside the BPN or if there labeled by virtue of the fact that their axon passed
appeared to be an extension ofthe injected tracer through the BPN to enter the cerebellum via the
outside the BPN. For orientation, a Nissl-stained middle cerebellar peduncle (brachium pontis).
transverse section through the midportion of the Consequently, such axons might have been
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 137

Figure 7.1. A: Nissl-stained transverse brain stem sec- illustrate medial (Med), ventral (Vent), and lateral (Lat)
tion illustrates the location of the basilar pontine nuclei subdivisions of BPN. C, D: Representative lateral (C)
(BPN), cerebral peduncle (CP), medial lemniscus (ML), and medial (D) WGA-HRP injection sites in the BPN
and nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis (NRTP). B: performed using a ventral, parapharyngeal surgical
Same section as in A but at higher magnification to approach.

transected and injury-filled at the BPN injection preceded by a brachium pontis lesion, each ofthe
site. To assess this possibility, two series of rats cerebellar-projecting cell groups in question ex-
were prepared. In both series, 12 to 16 placements hibited labeled somata as anticipated. In the series
(0.08-0.12 pI per site) of WGA-HRP were distri- in which the WGA-HRP injections followed the
buted throughout the cerebellar hemispheres and bilateral brachium pontis lesions, the nuclei in
vermis (Fig. 7.2C). In addition, in one of these question continued to exhibit labeled somata in
series, the WGA-HRP injections were initiated approximately the same number as the nonlesion
30 to 60 minutes after bilateral transection of the cases. These findings were interpreted to suggest
brachium pontis lateral and dorsal to the BPN that the cell groups in question project to both
in a region where this fiber bundle was about to the BPN and the cerebellum, but their axons
enter the cerebellum (Fig. 7.2A, B). In the series do not enter the cerebellum via the brachium
in which the WGA-HRP injections were not pontis.
138 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

using immunoperoxidase methods to visualize


glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD), it seemed
unlikely that the many labeled axons and axon
terminals observed might originate solely from
the relatively sparse number of immunoreactive
somata in the rat BPN. To determine if GABA
neurons extrinsic to the pontine gray provided
afferent projections to the BPN, a double-labelling
protocol involving the retrograde transport of
WGA-HRP was employed in combination with
GAD immunocytochemistry. Tissue sections col-
lected throughout the brain of animals that
received ventral BPN injections of WGA-HRP
as part of the preceding study of BPN afferents
were reacted according to an HRP stabilization
procedure (Rye et aI., 1984) and subsequently in-
cubated with GAD antiserum (Oertel et aI., 1981)
and processed according to routine immunoper-
oxidase procedures (Border et aI., 1986).
As indicated in Figures 7.3 and 7.4, double-
labeled somata (asterisks) were observed in the
zona incerta, perirubral area, anterior pretectal
nucleus, lateral cerebellar nucleus, and in various
portions of the medullary reticular formation.
Collectively, the largest accumulation of double-
labeled somata was exhibited by the medullary
reticular formation, followed in descending suc-
cession by the perirubral area, zona incerta, and
anterior pretectal nucleus. The fewest number of
double-labeled cells were observed in the lateral
cerebellar nucleus.
The double-labeled somata exhibited a wide
range in size and morphology; a few examples are
Figure 7.2. A: Nissl-stained transverse brain stem sec- shown in Figure 7.5. The largest of the double-
tion illustrating the position of the brachium pontis labeled cells exhibited a spheroidal-shaped or
(BP). B: Transverse section taken at approximately the multipolar-shaped soma and were most frequently
same rostrocaudallevel as A. Note the bilateral electro- located in the reticular formation. However, other
lytic lesions that involve the BP. C: Transverse brain double-labeled reticular formation somata were
stem demonstrating multiple injections ofWGA-HRP relatively small and spindle-shaped. In the peri-
that essentially involve nearly the entire cerebellar cor- rubral area and zona incerta, most double-labeled
tex. (A and B reprinted with permission from Mihailoff somata were small and multipolar or spindle-
et aI., 1988b.)
shaped whereas in the lateral cerebellar and
anterior pretectal nuclei, a mixture of small and
medium somata was observed.
Some BP N AfJerents Contain In several cases, the injection site in the BPN
extended dorsal to the medial lemniscus and cere-
GABA-Ergic Components
bral peduncle and in such cases double-labeled
In the course of light microscopic studies (Border cells were observed only in the lateral cerebellar
and Mihailoff, 1985) designed to demonstrate the nucleus. Although not specifically llsed in this
presence of GABA-ergic neurons in the rat BPN study, such cases in essence served as control
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 139

• HAP
*HAP& GAO

Figure 7.3. The composite distribution of neuronal gradely labeled somata observed in cases in which the
somata labeled by the retrograde transport of WGA- HRP injection site was restricted to the BPN as indi-
HRP (-), as well as those somata double-labeled with cated in section 9. (The illustrated sections are taken
WGA-HRP and immunoperoxidase staining for GAD from the atlas of Paxinos and Watson, 1986.)
(*). This is a composite diagram depicting all retro-
140 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

• HRP
*HRPS. GAO

Figure 7.4. The composite distribution of neuronal grade\y labeled somata observed in cases in which the
somata labeled by the retrograde transport of WGA- HRP injection site was restricted to the BPN, as indi-
HRP (e), as well as those somata double-labeled with cated in section 9 of Fig. 7.3. (The illustrated sections
WGA-HRP and immunoperoxidase staining for GAD are taken from the atlas of Paxinos and Watson, 1986.)
(*). This is a composite diagram depicting all retro-

experiments, which ruled out the possibility that lar nucleus in the latter experiments is apparently
the double-labeled cells were HRP-positive as a due either to the interruption and injury-filling of
result of the inadvertent spread of the injected GABA-ergic cerebello-olivary axons that are
tracer into the tegmentum dorsal to the BPN. known to course through the ventral portion
The double-labeling of cells in the lateral cerebel- of the tegmentum, or to retrograde transport in
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 141

B
Figure 7.5. Two examples of somata double-labeled to the substantia nigra (SN) and red nucleus (Rn). The
by the retrograde transport of WGA-HRP (from a midline (ml) is to the reader's right. The cell body
BPN injection) and immunoperoxidase staining for indicated in B is located within the ventromedial portion
GAD. The cell in A is from the perirubral are a adjacent of nucleus gigantocellularis.

axons of cerebellar nuclear neurons that project system and immediately followed first by 1 liter
to nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis. of chilled 1%- 1% fixative and then 1.5 liters of
chilled 2%-2% fixative. At the conclusion of
Synaptic Features in the Monkey BPN perfusion, the animal was left undisturbed for 30
Suggest the Presence of minutes, whereupon the entire brain was removed
from the cranial cavity and immersed in 300 ml
Local-Circuit Neurons
of the 2%- 2% fixative. The brain was refrigerated
Recently, five cynomolgous monkeys became overnight and on the following morning, the entire
available for ultrastructural and immunocyto- basilar pons was isolated from the brain stem and
chemical (ICC) studies. Each animal was first subdivided under a dissecting microscope into
immobilized with ketamine, intubated, and then small tissue blocks containing pontine regions
deeply anesthetized with Nembutal. While an- located medial, ventral, or lateral to the cerebral
esthesia was being achieved, 2.5 ml of the vasodi- peduncle. The tissue was then osmicated, de-
lating agent Diamox (acetazolomide, 1 mljkg) was hydrated, and embedded in plastic according to
infused intravenously and 15 minutes later a tho- previously described (Mihailoff and McArdle,
racotomy was initiated while the animal was arti- 1981) routine methods for studies of the rat BPN.
ficially ventilated. The heart was immediately Before osmication, tissue to be processed for
freed from the pericardial sac and a blunt 10- immunoperoxidase studies (70-l1m vibratome sec-
gauge needle introduced into the left ventricle. tions) was imlJlersed in 2% normal goat serum in
This needle was connected via polyethylene tubing Tris buffer and then incubated for 24 hours at
and a peristaltic pump to three flasks, one con- 4°C in rabbit anti-GABA (Maley and Newton,
taining physiological saline, the other a mixture 1985) diluted 1: 2000 in 2% normal goat serum in
of 1% paraformaldehyde- l % glutaraldehyde in Tris buffer. After immersion in goat anti-rabbit
0.1 M sodium phosphate buffer, and the third immunoglobulin G (IgG) (1 : 200) for 1 to 2 hours,
contained 2% paraformaldehyde-2% glutaral- the free-floating sections were incubated for
dehyde in 0·1 M sodium phosphate buffer. 1 hour with rabbit peroxidase antiperoxidase
After incising the right atrium, 300 ml of warm (PAP) diluted 1:400. The tissue was then reacted
saline was rapidly flushed through the vascular with 0.05% diaminobenzidene (DAB) and 0.01,!~
142 G.A. MihailofT et al.

Figure 7.6. Two representative examples oflarge bou- with some dense core vesicles and both form only
tons are shown in A and B. A: One variety exhibits a asymmetrical synaptic junctions. C: Transversely sec-
fairly regular oval shape, contacts several dendritic tioned dendritic shaft contains three clusters of pleo-
appendages on its perimeter, and is partially enclosed morphic vesicles located near symmetrical synaptic
by glial lamellae. B: The second variety exhibits an active sites. (Fig. 7.6c reprinted with permission from
irregular shape and is rarely surrounded by glial pro- MihailofT and Border, 1990.)
cesses. Both types primarily contain round clear vesicles
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 143

Figure 7.7. A: An example ofthe type of bouton at the ofthe most commonly observed type of bouton, in this
small end of the size spectrum. It contains round vesicles instance contacting the shaft of an intermediate-sized
with an asymmetric active site. B: Dendritic appendage dendrite. D: Bouton forming a symmetrical active site
postsynaptic to a bouton containing small round vesi- and containing a mixture of flattened, discoid, and
cles as well as several dense core vesicles. C: An example spheroidal vesicles as well as a dense core vesicle.
144 G.A. MihailofT et al.

hydrogen peroxide in Tris buffer (pH 7.4) for 3 to disposed electron-dense material. In addition, a
6 minutes. Subsequently, the tissue was lightly curious group of dendritic profiles was distin-
osmicated and prepared for electron microscopy guished by their electron-dense cytoplasmic matrix
in a routine manner as previously described. (Fig. 7.8A-C). Occasionally a similar dense matrix
In addition to the above procedure, the post- was clearly present within synaptic boutons
embedding immunogold method was used to (Fig. 7.8D). In this chapter, however, we focus on
identify GABA-immunoreactive profiles within two other specific categories of synaptic profiles,
the BPN. Briefly after collecting ultrathin sections each of which involves apparent synaptic interac-
on nickel grids, the tissue was exposed to 1% per- tion between two processes, both of which contain
iodic acid (4 minutes), 1% sodium metaperiodate synaptic vesicles.
(4 minutes), and 2% normal goat serum (NGS; In the first example (Fig. 7.9), a glial-invested
45 minutes) with gentle washing in distilled water glomerular synaptic complex consisting of a
between each step. The tissue was then incubated central axon terminal surrounded by several small
in rabbit anti-GABA diluted 1: 1000 with 1% NGS dendritic profiles also includes on its periphery a
containing 0.3% Triton X-100 for 1.5 hours. After vesicle-containing profile. In all examples of this
3 washes (5 minutes each) in a solution comprising type observed to date, the peripheral vesicle-
10 ml 0.05 M Tris containing 5 mg polyethylene containing profile was postsynaptic to the central
glycol, the tissue was incubated in goat anti- axon terminal. Only in rare instances, however,
rabbit IgG conjugated with 15 nm colloidal gold did the peripheral bouton form a distinct synaptic
particles (Janssen; 1:20 dilution) for 1.5 hours. active site in the same thin section (Fig. 7.11A),
The sections were then gently rinsed with four and in this circumstance the postsynaptic ele-
changes (5 minutes each) of distilled water and ment was a neighboring small dendritic profile.
stained with uranyl acetate (20 minutes) and lead Whereas the central axon terminal contained
citrate (20 minutes). Although there was no at- round clear synaptic vesicles and formed asym-
tempt to quantify rigorously the number of gold metric active sites, the peripheral boutons con-
particles overlying a given profile, we routinely tained pleomorphic (mostly spherical) vesicles
assessed the extent of potentially nonspecific label- embedded in a pale or lucent matrix and formed
ing by noting the number of gold particles present symmetrical active sites.
over the lumen of blood vessels and the compact The second category of synaptic arrangement
portion of axonal myelin. Such nonspecific or typically consisted of two vesicle-containing pro-
background labeling was extremely sparse. files and a dendritic element that were distributed
Careful examination of the neuropil in tissue throughout the neuropil without glial encapsula-
sections from control monkey brains revealed an tion and for that reason are referred to as non-
abundance of presynaptic profiles exhibiting a glomerular synaptic complexes (Fig. 7.10). As in
wide variety of sizes and synaptic vesicle and the first category, the pale bouton with pleomor-
active site morphology. Included were several phic vesicles was always postsynaptic to a bouton
varieties of round vesicle boutons (Figs. 7.6 and that contained uniformly round vesicles and was
7.7 A-C) that formed asymmetric active sites with itself only rarely presynaptic to a dendritic profile
dendritic shafts and spines. Boutons containing in the same plane of section (Fig. 7.10B, C). Those
a population of pleomorphic (various sizes and profiles that were presynaptic to the pale boutons
shapes) vesicles (Fig. 7.7D) formed symmetrical always contained round clear vesicles and formed
active sites whereas boutons populated only with asymmetric active sites. However, the round
large, dense-core vesicles (not illustrated) were vesicle boutons were quite variable in size (always
also observed. In some instances, profiles identi- smaller than glomerular central boutons) and in
fied as dendrites by their content of ribosomes several instances, the round vesicle bouton exhi-
and microtubules (Fig. 7.6C) contained synaptic bited a dense background matrix (Fig. 7.10D)
vesicles that varied in size and in shape from that might perhaps be characteristic of a parti-
spheroidal to discoidal (pleomorphic). These cular class of neuron in the B PN whose somatic,
vesicles were often aggregated near membrane dendritic, and axonal profiles exhibit an elevated
specializations that consisted of symmetrically cytoplasmic matrix density. On the other hand,
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 145

Figure 7.8. A: Certain dendrites exhibited an unusually such a dense dendrite is shown in B, whereas a similar
dense cytoplasmic matrix (solid block arrow) that easily dense dendrite and a spine1ike appendage are illustrated
distinguished them from other dendritic elements (open in C. Occasionally, synaptic boutons as indicated in D
block arrow) in the vicinity. An oblique section through exhibited a similar dense matrix.
146 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

Figure 7.9. Examples of glomerular synaptic com- peripheral vesicle-containing profile (*) characteristi-
plexes are shown in A-D. A: Central axon terminal cally exhibits a pale or lucent background. Arrows
surrounded by dendritic appendages and synaptic indicate polarity of synapse. (Fig. 7.9A reprinted with
boutons that are always postsynaptic to the central permission from Mihailoff and Border, 1990.)
axon. B-D: Other examples of glomeruli in which the
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 147

Figure 7.10. Examples of nonglomerular arrangements are involved, the pale profile (*) is postsynaptic to
that include synaptic contact between two vesicle- another bouton and presynaptic to a dendritic element.
containing profiles. A: A simple variety shown here D: Dense matrix bouton is presynaptic to a pale
involves only two profiles with the pale or lucent bouton bouton (*).
(*) in a postsynaptic position. B, C: When three profiles
148 G.A. MihailofT et al.

Figure 7.11. Examples ofGABA-immunogold labeled in which GABA-Ir pale bouton (*) is postsynaptic to
pale boutons. A: In glomeruli, only the small peripheral a small axon terminal and presynaptic to a nearby
boutons are GABA-Ir. Here a triadic arrangement is dendritic appendage. C, 0: In some instances, the GABA-
evident as the GABA-Ir bouton (*) is postsynaptic to Ir pale boutons (*) are linked by thin strands. Note
the central axon and presynaptic to an adjacent den- also in 0 the transversely sectioned GABA-Ir dendrite
dritic appendage, which i~ itself postsynaptic to the (Dd). (Reprinted with permission from MihailofT and
central axon terminal. B: Nonglomerular arrangement Border, 1990.)
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 149

those dendrites that were postsynaptic to pale the central bouton. In their nonglomerular loca-
profiles in these serial synaptic arrangements tions, the pale GABA-Ir boutons were involved
were not distinctive in any way other than their in serial (but not triadic) synapses in which again
relatively small size, which suggests that they they were always postsynaptic to another vesicle-
probably represent more distal portions of the containing profile and presynaptic to a dendri-
dendritic tree or a type of dendritic spine or pro- tic element (Figs.7.11B and 7.12B). Also, the
trusion. No dense matrix dendrites have yet been pale GABA-Ir boutons participated in relatively
observed to participate as postsynaptic elements simple, nonserial synapses with dendritic profiles
in a serial synapse. (Fig. 7.12A, C). Often these pale GABA-Ir bou-
In the primate material processed for immuno- tons appeared to be joined to one another by thin
cytochemistry, the frequency with which GABA- strands (Figs. 7.11 C, D and 7. 12C). Occasionally,
immunoreactive (GABA-Ir) somata and dendrites a GABA-Ir pale bouton formed synaptic contact
(Fig. 7.11A) have been observed throughout the with a GABA-Ir dendrite (Fig. 7.13C). In addi-
neuropil was noticeably greater than in the rat tion, transversely (Fig. 7.12D) or longitudinally
BPN. This is to be expected, based on light micro- (Fig. 7.13A) sectioned dendrites containing small
scopic studies (Brodal et aI., 1988), which revealed aggregates of clear synaptic vesicles and an oc-
a much greater density ofGABA-Ir cell bodies in casional dense-core vesicle (Fig. 7.13B) also ex-
the monkey BPN compared to the rat. In addi- hibited immunogold labeling.
tion, as previously observed in the rat (Border The second general category of GABA-Ir
and Mihailoff, 1990; Mihailoff etal., 1988a), pre- bouton characteristically contained a large accu-
synaptic profiles in the monkey BPN were clearly mulation of pleomorphic synaptic vesicles (most
GABA-positive (Mihailoff and Border, 1990). being spheroidal of varying size) and formed
Although both immunoperoxidase and immu- symmetrical active sites (Fig. 7.14A-C). Most of
nogold material provided qualitatively the same these profiles were relatively small ( < 1.0 Jim dia-
observations, in this chapter we focus primarily meter), rounded boutons (Fig. 7.14B), although
on the gold-labeled material since it was possible others were larger and more irregularly shaped
to distinguish readily the intracellular contents of (Fig. 7.14A). In addition, this type of GABA-Ir
the labeled profiles. GABA-immunoreactivity was bouton participated in serial synapses in which
observed within BPN somata, dendrites, and a they were presynaptic to the pale type GABA-Ir
variety of profiles that contained synaptic vesicles. bouton (Fig. 7.14C), which in turn was presyn-
In no instance was gold labeling observed in aptic to an adjacent dendritic spine.
association with those profiles that exhibited the
dense cytoplasmic matrix, although, such struc- Evidence that Inhibition and Afferent
tures were clearly preserved in the immunogold
Convergence are Part of the Functional
material.
Operations in the Rat BPN
Synaptic boutons immunopositive for GABA
could generally be divided into two categories: In an effort to correlate the morphological fea-
those that exhibited a distinctly pale or lucent tures of cytology and synaptic organization in
matrix with a sparse complement of synaptic the BPN with an electrophysiologically derived
vesicles and those that contained a relatively large view of certain functional properties of BPN
accumulation of synaptic vesicles that gave them neurons, single-unit recording experiments in the
a darker but more typical appearance compared BPN were initiated. The basic protocol for these
to surrounding elements in the neuropil. The pale studies is illustrated schematically in Figure 7.1SA.
GABA-Ir boutons participated in both glom- The animal was initially anesthetized through
erular and nonglomerular arrangements. In the the inhalation of 3% to 4% halothane and then
glomerular complexes (Fig. 7.11A), the GABA-Ir maintained on a lower concentration (2-3%) with
boutons were involved in a characteristic triadic the use of an endotracheal tube. For orthodromic
serial synapse in which they were postsynaptic to activation of corticopontine neurons, an indi-
the c~ntral bouton and presynaptic to an adjacent vidually moveable concentric bipolar electrode
dendritic profile that was itself postsynaptic to (Rhodes SNE-100; outer diameter, 0.2 mm; center
150 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

Figure 7.12. Not all pale


GABA-Ir boutons are
involved in serial synapses.
A: Pale GABA-Ir bouton
(arrow) receives an en passage
synaptic contact. B: Pale
GABA-Ir bouton forms
synaptic contact with two
adjacent dendritic profiles.
C: Pale GABA-Ir bouton
forms a synapse with an
adjacent dendrite (thin
arrow) and is continuous
with a slender stalk (paired
arrows). D: Commonly
observed type of gold-labeled
profile that does not exhibit
any synaptic vesicles or
membrane specializations.
May represent a plane of
section through a pale bouton
such as that shown in C or
might be a transversely
sectioned dendritic shaft
from a GABA-Ir neuron.

exposure 0.25 mm) was held in a plexiglass holder the deep cerebellar nuclei. For antidromic activ-
and inserted perpendicular to the cortical surface. ation ofBPN neurons, an array ofthree monopolar
Electrodes were initially positioned on the basis stimulating electrodes (approximately 500-750.um
of stimulation or recording in the sensorimotor apart) was inserted either into a contralateral
cortex but later were located using the stereo- cerebellar lobule at a depth of 350.um or was
taxic (somatotopic) map developed by Hall and stereotaxically positioned within the brachium
Lindholm (1974). Additional stimulating elec- pontis contralateral to the BPN recording site.
trodes were positioned simultaneously within a After securing each electrode or electrode array
variety of other cortical regions, and in some cases, in position, the animal was inverted carefully to
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 151

Figure 7.13. A: Longitudinally sectioned GABA-Ir Ir dendrite receiving synaptic contact from a pale GABA-
dendrite (thick arrows) within the BPN. B: Enlargement Ir bouton. Note the extensive subsynaptic specializa-
of portion of dendrite in A (thin arrows) to demonstrate tion. (arrow). (Fig. 7.130 reprinted with permission
the presence of clear and dense core vesicles. C: GABA- from Mihailoff and Border, 1990.)

the supine position and maintained in the stereo- cathodal pulses of 0.2 ms duration at constant
taxic instrument while the BPN was exposed current intensities ranging from 40 to 200 jJ.A at
through a ventral, parapharyngeal surgical ap- frequencies ranging from 1 to 10 Hz. A weak
proach. Finally, small bipolar-stimulating elec- anodal pulse (20 f..lA, 2 ms) followed the cathodal
trodes were placed bilaterally under the skin of pulse in an attempt to minimize the potential
the forepaws, hind paws, and vibrissae pad areas. deleterious effects of excessive current delivered
Anesthesia was continued with halothane but the through the stimulating electrode. Peripheral
level was reduced to 0.75%-\.0% (of total oxygen) cutaneous sites were electrically stimulated with
for the remainder of the recording session. up to double the intensity of cortical sites. For
Single BrN neuron activity was recorded with each BPN single-unit (neuron) recorded, the
glass microelectrodes filled with 2 M NaCI and threshold response was determined by identify-
fast green (impedance of 3- 12 Mil) and conven- ing the lowest stimulation current from a given
tional apparatus was used for neuron pulse de- cortical site that was sufficient to evoke a response
tection and histogram computation. Brain elec- in that cell. In the case of peripheral stimulation,
trical stimulation consisted of single or double when threshold values were determined, natural
152 G .A. Mihailoff et al.

Figure 7.14. A: Large variety ofGABA-Ir bouton that involving a dark GABA-Ir bouton that is presynaptic
is densely populated with synaptic vesicles giving it to a gold-labeled pale bouton, which is in turn pre-
a dark appearance compared to the pale GABA-Ir synaptic to a small dendritic profile. (Reprinted with
boutons. B: Small dark GABA-Ir boutons are also permission from Mihailoff and Border, 1990.)
present throughout the neuropil. C: Serial synapse

stimulation was applied (gentle brushing of glab- of 300 Hz with constant latency) delivered through
rous and hairy skin, light squeezing and tapping, the brachium pontis.
joint manipulation) to determine the sensory At the conclusion of the recording session, each
modality responsible for elicitation of that re- animal was administered an overdose of Nem-
sponse. Finally, all BPN neurons that were tested butal and perfused transcardially with 10% buf-
exhibited the appropriate response to antidromic fered formalin. The brain was removed, sectioned
stimulation (followed high frequency stimulation transversely at 50 pm on a freezing microtome,
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 153

pg3847 pg3848

FlPer BIN " 2MS


FLSCx BIN = 2 MS

0.0 0.2 0.0 0.2


TIME (SEC) TIME (SEC)

Figure 7.15. A: Protocol employed in the initial single- of two different BPN single units to stimulation of
unit recording experiments. Three stimulation sites the forepaw (FLPer, B) and the contralateral forelimb
included cerebral cortex (Ctx), paramedian lobule somatosensory cortex (FLSCx, C). Small thin arrows
(PML) of the cerebellar cortex, and the forepaw contra- indicate excitatory responses whereas small thick arrows
lateral to the Ctx stimulation site. B, C: Poststimulus indicate period of suppressed electrical activity (in-
time histograms (100 sweeps) illustrating the response hibitory trough). Asterisk indicates stimulus artifact.

and the sections mounted, stained with cresyl occurred at similar latencies of 4 to 6 ms after
violet, coverslipped, and examined to determine stimulation at threshold intensity. Threshold sti-
the location of stimulating and recording elect- mulation in motor and sensory areas of the face
rodes. Because of the relatively large size of the representation produced slightly more rapid re-
BPN and the fact that it was approached ven- sponses at 2 to 5 ms latencies. Quantitatively,
trally under visual control, positioning of the re- threshold stimulation produced a broad range of
cording electrode within the BPN was not a spike amplitudes in responsive neurons, whereas
problem. qualitatively the responses were consistent in that
spikes were followed by a decrease in cellular
electrical activity for periods ranging from 50 to
Cerebral Cortical and Peripheral Stimulation
80 ms (Fig. 7.16A, B). This quiet period, which
Shown in Figure 7.16 are several representative is referred to as the inhibitory trough, was also
poststimulus histograms that illustrate the spike apparent, albeit less prominently, after stimula-
activity in single BPN units in response to cere- tion at an intensity below (subthreshold) that
bral cortical stimulation. Excitatory responses which was required to elicit an excitatory response
to forelimb motor and forelimb sensory cortex in the BPN (Fig. 7.16C). The inhibitory trough
154 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

A B
250 / 250 I
pg42112 I pg42111
/
200 200

150 150
FLMCx FLSCx BIN = lMS
BIN = 1 MS
100 100

50 50

0 ~ I I" "'II"",.,~I;'W~M ••~Ol : l o . .l...,.........,,-,..oII,MII~_.......~O I: 1


0.0 0 .1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.0 0 .1 0.2 0.3 0 .4
TIME (SEC) TIME (SEC)

C / * 0 E
pg3512 / *
300
4961
250
50
200 /
Subthresh. BIN '" 1 MS
150
Cx
100 les.
50 ...
0 01 : 1 0 01 : 2
0.0 0. 1 0.2 0.3 0.0 0. 1 0.2 0.3
TIME (SEC) TIME (SEC)

Figure 7.16. A, B: Representative poststimulus time inhibitory trough is present. D: Following ablation of
histograms illustrating responses of two BPN single gray matter comprising a large portion of sensorimotor
units to stimulation of the forelimb motor cortex cortex and stimulation of the underlying white matter
(FLMCx, A) or forelimb sensory cortex (FLSCx, B). or the medullary pyramid, the inhibitory trough can
Thin arrow, excitatory response; thick arrow, inhibitory still be observed. E: Oscilloscope traces depicting the
trough; asterisk, stimulus artifact. C: Poststimulus time responses of two BPN units to antidromic activation
histogram illustrating the activity of a single BPN unit after stimulation of the white matter of the cerebellar
following subthreshold stimulation of the sensorimotor paramedian lobule.
cortex. No excitatory response is apparent while the

persisted when a large expanse of the sensori- characteristic responses to cortical stimulation
motor gray matter was carefully removed by suc- described above were also responsive to anti-
tion and the subjacent white matter stimulated dromic activation (Fig. 7.16E). The antidromic
(Fig. 7.l6D). Occasionally, single units responded response appeared at a constant latency of 1 to
to stimulation of more than one cortical site with 2 ms and was capable of following repetitive
the most frequent combination being forelimb stimulation up to 300 Hz.
sensory and vibrissae sensory areas. In contrast to the unitary excitatory responses
In several but not all cases, stimulating elec- to cortical stimulation, the response of single BPN
trodes were positioned in a cerebellar folium or neurons to peripheral electrical stimulation of
the brachium pontis to test for antidromic activa- the forepaw consisted of two excitatory peaks
tion of BPN units. Neurons that exhibited the characterized by a short burst of activity at 4 to
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 155

WGA - HAP

BPN
Figure 7.17. Experimental protocol employed to de- Boutons of the two afferent systems were labeled either
monstrate convergence of afferents originating in the by the orthograde transport of WGA-HRP or by de-
dorsal column nuclei (DorCN) and forelimb cerebral generation after a lesion involving the DorCN. BPN-
cortex (FLSCtx) on a single BPN neuron that projects PML projection neurons were labeled by the retrograde
to the paramedian lobule (PML) of the cerebellum. transport of WGA-HRP.

6 ms, followed by a slightly longer burst beginning combination with cortical and natural peripheral
at 12 ms and lasting for 10 to 30 ms (Fig. 7.15B). stimulation, those units responsive to sensory
Interestingly, both peaks persisted in experiments cortex stimulation were activated by light tactile
that involved ablation of sensorimotor (forelimb) stimuli whereas those responsive to motor cortex
cortex. Responses to electrical stimulation at vari- were activated by squeezing or tapping of the
ous points on the face occurred more rapidly forepaw and were unresponsive to light tactile
with latencies of2 to 5 ms. All neurons responsive cutaneous stimulation.
to peripheral electrical activation were also tested Based on these findings, which demonstrate a
using natural stimulation applied with a cotton convergence of cortical and peripheral inputs
swab, blunt probe, forceps, or joint manipulation. on single BPN neurons, two questions arise. The
Approximately 81% of the cells tested responded first concerns the identification ofthe anatomical
to light tactile stimulation of glabrous or hairy substrate for this convergence and the second is
skin, whereas 16% responded to deep pressure or where in the cerebellar cortex do such BPN neu-
tapping and only 3% responded to joint manipu- rons project. In response to the first question,
lation. since it had been recently demonstrated that the
Also, as can be seen in Figure 7.15C, many dorsal column nuclei (DorCN) project to the BPN
BPN units responded to stimulation of both the (Kosinski et aI., 1986; Swenson et aI., 1984) a series
periphery and the corresponding region of sen- of experiments employing the protocol illustra-
sorimotor cortex, in this case the forepaw peri- ted in Figure 7.17 was implemented. First, a large
pherally (electrical and light tactile brushing), and ablative lesion was performed so as to involve the
forelimb sensory cortex. Of 110 units recorded, entire extent of the dorsal column nuclei on the
42 (38%) responded to both peripheral and corti- right side of the medulla. Three to 5 days later,
cal stimulation whereas 48 (44%) responded only WGA-HRP was injected into the left sensorimo-
to cortical stimulation and 20 (18%) responded tor cortex and the right paramedian lobule of the
only to peripheral stimulation. cerebellum. After another 36- to 48-hour survival
Other BPN units responded to a combination period, the animal was sacrificed, the BPN sec-
offorelimb peripheral and forelimb motor cortex tioned on a vibratome, and the tissue reacted to
stimulation. Moreover, when cells were tested in visualize HRP. After stabilizing the HRP reaction
156 G.A. MihailofT et al.

Figure 7.18. Electron micrograph illustrating the re- synapse from a degenerative DoreN axon (large arrows)
sults of the experiment depicted in Fig. 7.17. In the and a cortical bouton (small arrows) labeled by the
center of the field, a dendrite of a BPN-PML projection orthograde transport of WGA-HRP. (Reprinted with
neuron is identified by the presence of retrogradely permission from Kosinski et aI., 1988.)
transported WGA-HRP. This dendrite receives a

product, the tissue was prepared for electron DorCN boutons tend to contact primarily the
microscopy (Kosinski et ai., 1988). The rationale proximal and intermediate portions of the den-
was to establish that an HRP-labeled pontopara- dritic tree. Experiments to control for the possibi-
median neuron received both cortical and DorCN lity of spurious results due to the transneuronal
synaptic boutons and thus, the PML would be movement ofWGA-HRP included (a) injection of
identified as a target for the convergent inputs to WGA-HRP into only one ofthe three areas under
BPN neurons. study in a single animal, and (b) a PML injection
Upon electron microscopic examination ofthe of WGA-HRP combined with a lesion in either
BPN in such an experiment, numerous HRP- cortex or DorCN. No evidence for the possibility
labeled dendrites of pontoparamedian neurons of either retrograde or anterograde transneuronal
were observed in synaptic contact with HRP- movement of HRP was obtained at either the
labeled boutons or degenerative boutons. Al- light or electron microscopic level.
though a few examples were observed, it was
extremely rare to see both cortical and DorCN
Stimulation of Cerebellopontine Afferents
boutons in contact with the same pontopara-
median neuron dendrite (Fig. 7.18). This was to In addition to cortical and peripheral stimulation,
be expected, however, since corticopontine syn- single BPN units were tested for responsiveness
apses are known to be formed with distal por- to stimulation of the cerebellar nuclei (DCN).
tions of BPN neuron dendritic systems whereas After electrical stimulation of the lateral cerebel-
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 157

A B
200 " PG4534 200 pg4422

150 150

100 DeN 100 DeN


"
50
., B IN : 2 MSEC
50

.. ,,\y1lliM "'1"*4W~ BIN : 2MSEC


0 ~~~~ ~~ ~ ., ~ .•
.~ ~,.......... 0-
0.0 0.2 0,4 0.0 0.2 0.4
C
200 / SEC o SEC
PG4533 200 4751
",

150
DeN 150 BCtx
FLMCx FLMCx
100 100

50 50
-I '1 J
'* BIN : 2MSEC 111 BIN : l msec
0
0.0 0.2
~\, t' .. ~ ~~,.. .... ,w.1,
0.4
.,wII!II'''''''''''0.' ' 1"' ' ~0.2
'I'II• 0.3 .~''II~~''.
0.4 0.5
SEC SEC

Figure 7.19. Representative poststimulus time histo- only with inhibition. C: BPN single unit activity follow-
grams illustrating the response of BPN single units to ing combined stimulation of forelimb motor cortex
stimulation of the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN). A: (FLMCz) and DCN. Compared to A, the inhibitory
BPN unit showing an initial excitatory response (thin trough is enhanced. D: Following transection of the
arrow) followed by inhibition (thick arrow). Asterisk brachium conjunctivum (BCtx) and stimulation of the
indicates stimulus artifact. B: BPN unit responding FLMCz, the inhibitory trough persists.

lar nucleus, single neurons in medial or ventral thus produce antidromic spikes in BPN neurons
BPN regions either exhibited spike activity that might be confused with the short latency of
(Fig. 7.19A) at very short latencies (2.5 ms or less) orthodromic activation. To test for this possibi-
or their activity was suppressed (Fig. 7.19B) with- lity, if a BPN unit exhibited short latency spike
out evidence of any preceding excitation. Since it activity to DeN activation, the stimulus fre-
was not possible to move the stimulation site in quency was increased in a stepwise fashion. If the
the DeN once the recording electrode had been spike activity followed stimulus frequencies above
positioned in the BPN, whether or not a given 150 Hz, the response was interpreted as anti-
BPN neuron that was initially excited by DeN dromic. In fact, none of the recorded DeN-re-
stimulation might subsequently be inhibited (or sponsive units exhibited such following capability
vice versa), could not be ascertained. and thus we are confident that the increased spike
Although it has been shown that pontocerebel- activity recorded was the result of a short latency
lar axons provide a very small number of colla- excitatory input from the DeN.
terals to the DeN (Dietrichs and Walberg, 1987), DeN-responsive units in the BPN were also
there are pontocerebellar axons coursing through tested to determine if they received cortical or
the white matter adjacent to the DeN that could peripheral inputs. Although none received peri-
be incidentally activated by the current pulse pheral somatosensory input, some were clearly
delivered through the stimulating electrode and responsive to stimulation at various cortical sites,
158 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

with the combination of DeN and forelimb motor made it clear that the basilar pontine nuclei rep-
cortex being particularly effective. Interestingly, resent one of the most (if not the single most)
when the cortical and cerebellar nuclear activa- important cell group involved in the transmission
tion of BPN neurons was paired such that cere- of cortical information to the cerebellum. There-
bellar stimulation followed the cortical stimula- fore, it should be understood that the observa-
tion by 2.5 ms, the effect of the convergent inputs tions described in this chapter must not be
was to enhance the inhibitory trough (Fig. 7.19, interpreted as an attempt to minimize the im-
compare A and C). In addition, in two cases, the portance of the transfer of cerebral cortical
brachium conjunctivum (BC) was transected- information to the cerebellum as a fundamental
midway in the recording session after BPN units functional operation of the BPN. Rather, it is
that were responsive to combined DeN-motor intended that this chapter should point out addi-
cortex stimulation had been identified. After the tional functional attributes of the BPN by focus-
transection, BPN units were not responsive to ing attention on a) the newly identified BPN
DeN stimulation alone but continued to exhibit afferent systems and b) the recently described
spike activity in response to cortical stimulation. cellular characteristics and synaptic circuitry in-
The inhibitory trough observed after cortical trinsic to the BPN. Taken together, these two
stimulation was still present after Be transection features strongly suggest that the BPN function
but in an attenuated form. No BPN units have with much more diversity in their role as a source
yet been recorded that exhibited evidence of re- of cerebellar afferent signals than has previously
ceiving convergent peripheral and DeN input. been recognized.
The convergence of cerebellar and cortical in-
puts on single BPN neurons has also been con-
N oncortical BP N AfJerents
firmed with anatomical methods (Lee et aI., 1990).
Seven days after a series of 4 unilateral electrolytic
In survey studies of noncortical BPN afferent
lesions were made in the cerebellar nuclei, WGA- projections using the retrograde transport of
HRP was injected at 3 to 5 sites within the con- WGA-HRP, more than 50 subcortical cell groups
tralateral sensorimotor cortex. After an addi- were identified (Mihailoff et aI., 1988b). Because
tional 48-hour survival period, the animals were of this rather large number, one might question
perfused and processed for the stabilized HRP whether false-positive labeling due to injury-filling
reaction and electron microscopy, as described in ofaxons might account for some of the labeled
the studies of cortical and peripheral somato- cell groups. We suggest that this is unlikely be-
sensory convergence. These studies revealed the cause all injections were made by penetrating
presence of HRP-Iabeled cortical boutons and from the ventral surface ofthe BPN and no cases
degenerative cerebellar nuclear terminals in syn- were used if there was any evidence that the
aptic contact with a single BPN neuron dendritic micropipette extended into or dorsal to the fiber
shaft. systems (medial lemniscus, cerebral peduncle, late-
rallemniscus) forming the dorsal boundary of the
basilar pontine gray. Furthermore, although the
Discussion zone of effective HRP uptake cannot be defined
Over the past several years, a rather substantial accurately, it has been demonstrated that there
amount of information has been amassed regard- is little likelihood that WGA-HRP can be taken
ing the anatomical connectivity of the cortico- up and transported in sufficient quantity by un-
pontocerebellar system in the rat, cat, and monkey damaged fibers to produce false-positive somatic
(for review see Brodal 1982, 1987). These studies labeling (Brodal et aI., 1983). Thus, it seemed un-
have made it possible to appreciate better the likely that any of the axonal systems coursing
precision and complexity that characterizes this through the tegmentum adjacent to the BPN had
system. But perhaps of equal importance is the inadvertently been labeled.
fact that collectively, such studies have reempha- Another complication was considered, how-
sized the enormity of the cerebral cortical projec- ever, when it was noticed that several cell groups
tions to the cerebellum and, at the same time, labeled after BPN injections were known to have
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 159

direct projections to the cerebellum. For several and pars lateralis, along with the pedunculopon-
of these cell groups, such as the locus coeruleus tine tegmental nucleus and the spinal vestibular
or the lateral reticular nucleus, it was known that nucleus. In addition, there are the afferent cell
at least some of their axons entered the cerebel- groups that were shown in subsequent studies to
lum by way of the superior or inferior cerebellar contain GABA-ergic components; these included
peduncle but for certain other cell groups, the the lateral cerebellar nucleus, zona incerta, peri-
route of cerebellar entry was unknown. Thus, the rubral cells, anterior pretectal nucleus, and cells
possibility was entertained that some of these in the medullary reticular formation. Many BPN
cells were labeled because their axons entered the projecting cell groups involved in visual system
cerebellum through the middle cerebellar peduncle circuitry are also of interest, as are the numerous
(brachium pontis) and thus were unknowingly cell groups that, in addition to their BPN con-
damaged and injury-filled at the injection site nections, also project directly to the cerebellum.
within the BPN as they coursed toward the Obviously, many ofthese connections are worthy
brachium pontis. This possibility was negated, of additional investigation and their existence
however, by experiments that involved a multiple serves to emphasize the diversity of afferent sys-
array of WGA-HRP injections in the cerebellar tems reaching the BPN.
cortex which, in some animals, were preceded by
large bilateral lesions of the brachium pontis.
Do BPN Local-Circuit Neurons Exist?
The pattern and number of retrogradely labeled
somata were essentially the same under the two There is as yet no unequivocal answer to this
conditions, and thus it was reasoned that the cell question, although the existing evidence suggests
groups in question did project to both the cere- an affirmative response. Several years ago, Golgi
bellar cortex and the BPN, and that such cells and electron microscopic (EM) studies in the
did not use the brachium pontis to enter the monkey (Cooper and Beal, 1978; Cooper and Fox,
cerebellum, at least not in appreciable quantity. 1976) and rat (Mihailoff et aI., 1981a; Mihailoff
Of the 50-odd afferent cell groups identified in and McArdle, 1981) illustrated BPN neurons with
these studies, 22 had previously been demonstrat- complex dendritic appendages, multiple, locally
ed in a variety of orthograde tracing experiments. ramifying axonlike processes, flat and pleomor-
Thus, approximately 28 of these cell groups re- phic vesicle boutons, synapses between vesicle-
present previously unrecognized BPN afferents. containing profiles, and evidence for the possible
In some instances, the number of labeled cells in existence of presynaptic dendrites. Several years
a nucleus was quite sparse (Le., the central nucleus earlier, studies in the opossum had produced
of the amygdala and horizontal nucleus of the similar observations (Mihailoff and King, 1975)
diagonal band), and one might question whether whereas quite a number of years previous to the
such connections are truly functional or might opossum report, Ramon y Cajal (1911) had illus-
represent a vestige of a developmental anomaly. trated a BPN neuron with a locally ramifying
This question remains for future investigation, axon. Although this collection of evidence was
although the presence of such connections in a quite suggestive, the question was by no means
number of animals suggests that they are not the resolved since there was no way to be certain with
remnants of truly aberrant development. the Golgi (and EM) method that the entire extent
The number of labeled somata observed in of the presumptive locally ramifying axon was
certain other cell groups was numerically more being visualized. Moreover, existing electrophy-
substantial, but the implications regarding their siological studies of BPN neuronal activity were
functional relationship to the BPN and cerebellum contradictory. Single-unit recording by Sasaki
are unclear. In this category are included the et al. (1970) in the cat provided strong evidence
midbrainperipeduncular nucleus, the dorsal teg- for the existence of cortical and recurrent colla-
mental nucleus, and the periaqueductal gray. Per- teral activated inhibition in the BPN. Conversely,
haps somewhat less mysterious because of their the intracellular studies of Allen et aI. (1977) indi-
involvement with sensorimotor circuitry are the cated the presence of weak inhibitory postsynaptic
labeled cells in substantia nigra pars reticulata potentials (IPSPs) that were only rarely observed,
160 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

and thus these authors essentially denied the lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat (Famiglietti
existence of a significant number of BPN local- and Peters, 1972) and mouse (Rafols and Valverde,
circuit neurons. 1973), where it was demonstrated that the pale
More recently, ICC studies have demonstrated bouton with pleomorphic vesicles was actually a
the presence of GAD- and GABA-immunoreactive presynaptic dendritic appendage emanating from
somata and axon terminals in the BPN ofthe rat a local-circuit neuron. An earlier EM study of
(Border and Mihailoff, 1985), cat (Brodal et aI., the primate BPN also illustrated a similar pale
1988), and primate (Brodal et aI., 1988; Thier and presynaptic profile (Cooper and Beal, 1978) and
Koehler, 1987). Although it is clear, at least in the further, the presence of presynaptic dendrites in
rat, that some of the GABA-ergic terminals are our own material (Mihailoff and Border, 1990)
derived from cell groups extrinsic to the BPN strongly supports the notion that local circuit
(Border et aI., 1986) one might presume that the neurons are present in the BPN.
population of GABA-ergic neurons within the The nonglomerular complex also includes a
BPN might also give rise to a portion of the pale GABA-Ir synaptic profile; however, the
GABA-ergic axon terminals. So it is that ultra- round vesicle bouton that is in a presynaptic
structural ICC studies in the rat (Border and position is often a much smaller profile than the
Mihailoff, 1990) indicate the presence of two central axon terminal of the glomerular synapse.
populations ofGABA-ergic boutons in the BPN. Also, the arrangement of the nonglomerular com-
In the monkey, observations from the present plex is such that the pale profile is presynaptic to
study are also somewhat suggestive of the pres- a dendritic element that is not postsynaptic to the
ence of two populations of GABA boutons. If, participating round vesicle bouton. Usually no
eventually, only one population of GABA bou- more than three profiles are involved in the non-
tons is shown to be present in the primate or glomerular complex and there is no glial invest-
alternatively two types with one variety in great ment of this group of boutons as there is with the
abundance, one might speculate that the tremen- glomerular variety.
dous increase in the number of BPN GABA Although still in a preliminary stage, our
neurons is linked in some way to a decrease in, monkey ICC material also illustrates GABA-Ir
or an elimination of, some or all the extrinsic boutons that typically contain many synaptic
GABA-ergic BPN afferent systems. This course vesicles giving the bouton a darker appearance.
of events could explain why one segment of the These boutons form relatively isolated one-to-
GAB A bouton pool (the largest GAB A boutons) one synapses with essentially all somatic and
appears to be relatively sparse in the monkey. dendritic compartments but mostly small dendrit-
The present study also demonstrated in control ic structures. They also form synaptic complexes
neuropil the existence of two types of synaptic with GABA-Ir pale boutons. At present, we inter-
complexes (glomerular and nonglomerular) that pret these observations to suggest that at least
included GABA-Ir boutons. Each of these com- some ofthe dark GABA-Ir boutons are of extrinsic
plexes exhibited serial synaptic contacts involving origin and further that these afferents interact
at least two vesicle-containing profiles and one with the intrinsic GABA neurons represented by
or several dendritic elements. In the glomerular the pale GABA-Ir boutons. An important question
type synapse, a large central axon terminal is to be resolved is whether the local-circuit neurons
contacted on its perimeter by several dendritic have axons and if so, how can the synaptic boutons
structures that are postsynaptic to the central of such axons be identified. It is clear that most
bouton. In addition, one or more of the perimeter X-type interneurons in the lateral geniculate nu-
profiles often contains pleomorphic synaptic cleus of the cat exhibit conventional action poten-
vesicles and appears to be presynaptic to an ad- tials (Ramos et aI., 1985; Sherman and Friedlander,
jacent dendritic profile and postsynaptic to the 1988), but there is at least a suspicion that Y-type
central bouton. The triadic arrangement of these interneurons might not give rise to an axon.
structures, and the pale appearance of the pleo- Once again it seems that the accumulated EM
morphic vesicle bouton, are very similar to the and ICC observations in the monkey are not
glomerular complexes described in the dorsal absolutely definitive but argue strongly for the
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 161

existence of a BPN local-circuit neuron. Although input mediated by the DorCN, or the combina-
the possibility that the BPN GABA-ergic neurons tion of cortical and cerebellar nuclear afferents.
actually project to the cerebellum should be con- Although previous studies have reported that
sidered, on the basis of double-labeling studies in BPN neurons receive convergent input in various
the cat (Brodal et aI., 1988) and preliminary experi- combinations from a variety of different cortical
ments in our laboratory, no such projections have regions (Boyd and Aitkin, 1976; Oka et ai., 1975;
yet been identified. It remains, however, that such Potter et aI., 1978; Ruegg and Weisendanger,
projections might be quite sparse in number and/ 1975; Ruegg et aI., 1977; Sasaki et aI., 1975), the
or restricted in distribution so that their detection work of others tends to emphasize the selectivity,
would require a systematic and comprehensive restriction, or segregation of the various types of
search. It seems that what is required is for future cortical input to BPN neurons (Baker et aI., 1976).
studies to employ immunocytochemistry along In our own experiments, some BPN neurons
with intracellular recording and HRP-filling to clearly received convergent input from different
allow visualization of the entire axon and per- portions of sensorimotor cortex such as forelimb
haps in this way be able to definitively resolve and face sensory cortices. However, it was our
this question. intent to search for the potential convergence of
cortical and peripheral somatosensory input and
it was not at all uncommon to encounter single
Current Observations on BPN BPN units that received peripheral somatosensory
input from the face region or the forepaw in
Functional Properties
combination with an input from the correspond-
Two particularly relevant observations can be ing region of sensorimotor cortex. Similarly, BPN
gleaned from the preliminary electro physiological neurons were encountered that, for example, re-
studies of the corticopontine system reported in ceived convergent input from the lateral cere-
this chapter. First, in nearly every test situation a bellar nucleus and forelimb motor cortex. Curi-
BPN unit that responded to cortical stimula- ously, in both types of convergent input the ex-
tion exhibited first an excitatory response (spike citatory or spike-generating capacity of the two
activity) that was then followed by an inhibition inputs did not seem to summate but rather the
or suppression of activity that lasted for a variable depth and duration of the inhibitory trough was
period oftime (50-80 ms). This inhibitory trough, enhanced, apparently as a function of the conver-
as it is referred to, was present even when cortical gent inputs. In fact, stimulation of the lateral
stimulation intensity was lowered below the thresh- cerebellar nucleus alone sometimes produced only
old value required to produce an excitatory re- inhibition in the BPN. This might prove to be an
sponse in the BPN neuron, and was also observed interesting correlation with the observation in rat
after cortical gray matter ablation and subse- that a portion of the pool of lateral cerebellar
quent stimulation ofthe underlying cortical white neurons that project to the BPN are GABA-ergic.
matter. The latter two observations suggest that Equally curious is the finding that ifthe brachium
a) the inhibition is an active process and not the conjunctivum is transected, the inhibitory trough
result of cortical disfacilitation and b) the inhibi- is still present after cortical cerebral stimulation.
tion is not a function of intracortical circuitry. It seems. clear, however, that activation of some
Whether an inhibitory pontine local circuit is cerebellopontine neurons leads to inhibition or
solely responsible for the inhibitory trough re- suppression ofBPN neuron activity. Exactly what
mains for further investigation since in the rat, it role is played by such an inhibitory input must
is possible that subcortical GABA-ergic afferent remain unanswered for the moment, although
projections might be activated by cortical stimu- such observations suggest that inhibitory opera-
lation, thus resulting in inhibition within the BPN tions may exist at several levels in the BPN. The
with a similar latency. presence of both excitatory and inhibitory BPN
A second relevant observation concerns the inputs from the DCN also correlates with previous
convergent input to BPN neurons that involves EM studies that suggested that the cerebellopont-
cortical afferents and peripheral somatosensory ine system consisted of more than one type of
162 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

neuron (Mihailoff et aI., 1981 b; Watt and Mihailoff, Border, B.G., and Mihailoff, G.A. (1985): GAD-immu-
1'983). These findings are consistent with double- noreactive neural elements in the basilar pontine
label experiments involving the combination of nuclei and nucleus reticularis tegementi pontis of the
ICC and retrograde transport of WGA-HRP, rat. I. Light microscopic studies. Exp. Brain Res., 59,
which indicated that GABA-ergic (Border et aI., 600-614.
Border, B.G., and Mihailoff, G.A. (1990): GABA-ergic
1986) and glutaminergic (Border and Mihailoff,
neural elements in the basilar pontine nuclei of the
1991) neurons of the cerebellar nuclei project to
rat: Electron microscopic immunochemistry. J. Camp.
the BPN. Also, recent fluorescent double-label Neural. 295, 123-135.
studies have revealed that the cerebellopontine Border, B.G., and Mihailoff, G. (1991): Glutamate im-
system includes axon collaterals of neurons that munoreactivity in the rat basilar pons: Light and
project rostrally to the thalamus as well as colla- electron microscopy reveals labeled boutons and
terals arising from axons that project to the inferior cells of origin of afferent projections. N eurosci. 45:
olive (Lee et aI., 1989). Thus, the existing evidence 47-61.
seems to indicate that the cerebellar projection Boyd, J., and Aitkin, L. (1976): Responses of single units
to the BPN is complex and consists of both exci- in the pontine nuclei of the cat to acoustic stimulation.
tatory and inhibitory inputs that appear to arise Neurosci. Lett., 3, 259-263.
Brodal, P. (1982): The cerebropontocerebellar pathway:
from at least two different populations of neurons.
salient features of its organization. In: The Cerebel-
In summary, the anatomical and electrophys-
lum-New Vistas (V. Chan-Palay, and S. Palay, eds).
iological observations that comprise this chapter Exp. Brain Res., Suppl. 6, 108-132, Heidelberg:
indicate that as a whole, BPN neurons receive an Springer.
unusually diverse set of afferent projections, that Brodal, P. (1987): Organization of cerebropontocere-
some BPN neurons in fact receive convergent bellar connections as studied with anterograde and
inputs from different afferent sources that are retrograde transport of HRP-WGA in the cat. In:
then transmitted to a specific locus in the cerebel- New Concepts in Cerebellar Neurobiology (S. King,
lar cortex, and that the functional activity within ed). New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc. pp. 151-182.
the BPN includes inhibitory operations that might Brodal, P., Dietrichs, E., Bjaalie, J., Nordby, T., and
be accomplished at least in part by local-circuit Walbert, F. (1983): Is lectin-coupled HRP taken up
neurons. Taken together, these findings suggest and transported by undamaged as well as damaged
fibers in the central nervous system? Brain Res., 278,
a more expansive, diverse, and integrative role for
1-9.
the BPN than has previously been anticipated. Brodal, P., Mihailoff, G., Border, B., Ottersen, O.P.,
Much additional experimentation will be required and Storm-Mathisen, J. (1988): GABA-containing
to address several of the new questions that have neurons in the pontine nuclei of rat, cat and monkey.
emerged from the present studies. An immunocytochemical study. Neuroscience, 25,
27-45.
Cooper, M.H., and Beal, J.A. (1978): The neurons and
References synaptic endings in the primate basilar pontine gray.
J. Camp. Neural., 180,17-42.
Allen, G.I., Oshima, T., and Toyama, K. (1977): The Cooper, M.H., and Fox, c.A. (1976): The basilar pontine
mode of synaptic linkage in the cerebro-ponto- gray in the adult monkey (Macaca mulatta): A Golgi
cerebellar pathway investigated with intracellular study. J. Camp. Neurol., 168, 145-174.
recording from pontine nuclei cells of the cat. Exp. Dietrichs, E., and Walberg, F. (1987): Cerebellar nuclear
Brain Res., 29, 123-136. afferents-where do they originate? Are-evaluation
Allen, G.I., and Tsukahara, N. (1974): Cerebrocerebellar of the projections from some lower brain stem nuclei.
communication systems. Physiol. Rev., 54, 957-1006. Anat. Embryol, 177,165-172.
Baker, J.A., Gibson, A., Glickstein, M., and Stein, J. Distel, H., and Hollander, H. (1980): Autoradiographic
(1976): Visual cells in the pontine nuclei of the cat. tracing of developing subcortical projections of the
J. Physiol. (Land.), 255, 415-433. occipital region in fetal rabbits. J. Compo Neural.,
Border, B.G., Kosinski, R.J., Azizi, S.A., and Mihailoff, 192,505-518.
G.A. (1986): Certain basilar pontine afferent systems Famiglietti, E.B., and Peters, A. (1972): The synaptic
are GABA-ergic: combined HRP and immunocyto- glomerulus and the intrinsic neuron in the dorsal
chemical studies in the rat. Brain Res. Bull., 17, lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J. Camp. N eurol.,
169-179. 144,255-334.
7. The Basilar Pontine Nuclei 163

Hall, R.D., and Lindholm, E.P. (1974): Organization of suggesting that both the corti cop on tine and cere-
motor and somatosensory neocortex in the albino bellopontine systems are each composed of two
rat. Brain Res., 66, 23-38. separate neuronal populations: An electron micro-
Hamos, J.E., Van Horn, S.c., Raczkowski, D., Uhlrich, scopic and horseradish peroxidase study in the rat.
D.J., and Sherman, S.M. (1985): Synaptic connectivity J. Comp. Neurol., 195,221-242.
of a local circuit neurone in the lateral geniculate Ok a, H., Sasaki, K., Matsuda, Y., Yasuda, T., and
nucleus of the cat. Nature, 317, 618-621. Mizuno, N. (1975): Responses of pontocerebellar
Kosinski, R.I., Azizi, S.A., Border, B.G., and Mihailoff, neurons to stimulation of the parietal association
G.A. (1986): Origin and ultrastructural identification and the frontal motor cortices. Brain Res., 93,
of dorsal column nuclear synaptic terminals in the 399- 407.
basilar pontine gray of rats. J. Comp. Neurol., 253, Oertel, W., Schmechel, D., Tappaz, M., and Kopin,
92-104. I. (1981): Production of a specific antiserum to
Kosinski, R.I., Azizi, S.A., and Mihailoff, G.A. (1988): rat brain glutamine acid decarboxylase by injec-
Convergence of cortico- and cuneo pontine projec- tion of antigen-antibody complex. Neuroscience,
tions onto components of the pontocerebellar system 6, 2689- 2700.
in the rat: An anatomical and electrophysiological Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (1986): The Rat Brain in
study. Exp. Brain Res., 71, 541-556. Stereotaxic Coordinates. New York: Academic Press.
Lee, H.S., and Mihailoff, G.A. (1990): Convergence of Potter, R., Ruegg, D., and Wei send anger, M. (1978):
cortical and cerebellar projections on single basilar Responses of neurones of the pontine nuclei to stimu-
pontine neurons: A light and electron microscopic lation of the sensorimotor, visual and auditory cortex
study in the rat. Neurosci, 39,.561-577. of rats. Brain Res. Bull., 3, 15-19.
Lee, H., Kosinski, R., and Mihailoff, G. (1989): Collate- Rafols, lA., and Valverde, F. (1973): The structure of
ral branches of cerebellopontine axons reach the the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the mouse.
thalamus, superior colliculus, or inferior olive: A A Golgi and electron microscopic study. J. Comp.
double-fh,lOrescence and combined fluorescence- Neurol., 150, 303-332.
horseradish peroxidase study in the rat. Neuroscience, Ramon y Cajal (1911): Histologie du Systeme Nerveux
28, 725-734. de L'Homme et des Vertebres, Vol. I. Paris: Maloine,
Maley, B., and Newton, B. (1985): Immunohistochemis- pp. 960-978.
try of gamma-amino butyric acid in the cat nucleus Ruegg, D.G., and Weisendanger, M. (1975): Cortico-
tractus solitarius. Brain Res., 330, 364-368. fugal effects from sensorimotor area I and somato-
Mihailoff, G.A., and Border, B.G. (1990): Evidence for sensory area II on neurons of the pontine nuclei in
the presence of presynaptic dendrites and GABA- the cat. J. Physiol. Lond., 247, 745-7S7.
immunogold labeled synaptic boutons in the monkey Ruegg, D., Sequin, J., and Weisendanger, M. (1977):
basilar pontine nuclei. Brain Res., 516, 141-146. Effects of electrical stimulation of somatosensory
Mihailoff, G.A., and King, lS. (1975): The basilar pon- and motor areas of the cerebral cortex on neurones
tine gray of the opossum: A correlated light and of the pontine nuclei in squirrel monkeys. Neuro-
electron microscopic analysis. J. Comp. N eurol., 159, science, 2, 923-927.
521-552. Rye, D., Saper, c., and Wainer, B. (1984): Stabilization
Mihailoff, G., Kosinski, R., Azizi, S., and Border, B. of the tetramethylbenzidine (TMB) reaction product:
(1988b): A survey of non cortical afferent projections Application for retrograde and anterograde tracing
to the basilar pontine nuclei. A retrograde tracing and combination with immunocytochemistry. J.
study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol., 282, 617-643. Histochem. Cytochem., 32,1145-1153.
Mihailoff, G., Kosinski, R., Border, B., and Lee, H.S. Sasaki, K., Kawaguchi, S., Shimono, T., and Prelevic,
(1988a): A review of recent observations concerning S. (1970): Electrophysiological studies of the pontine
the synaptic organization of the basilar pontine gray. Brain Res., 20, 425-438.
nuclei. J. Elec. Mic. Tech., to, 229-246. Sasaki, K., Oka, H., Matsuda, Y., Shimono, T., and
Mihailoff, G., and McArdle, C. (1981): The cytoarchi- Mizuno, N. (1975): Electrophysiological studies of
tecture, cytology and synaptic organization of the the projections from the parietal association area to
basilar pontine nuclei in the rat. II. Electron micro- the cerebellar cortex. Exp. Brain Res., 23, 91-102.
scopic studies. J. Comp. Neurol., 195,203-219. Shambes, G., Gibson, J., and Welker, W. (1978): Frac-
Mihailoff, G., McArdle, c., and Adams, C. (1981a): The tured somatotopy in granule cell tactile areas of rat
cytoarchitecture, cytology and synaptic organization cerebellar hemispheres revealed by micromapping.
of the basilar pontine nuclei in the rat. I. Nissl and Brain Behav. Evol., 15,94-140.
Golgi studies. J. Comp. Neurol., 195,181-201. Sherman, S.M., and Friedlander, M.J. (1988): Identifi-
Mihailoff, G., Watt, c., and Burne, R. (1981 b): Evidence cation of X versus Y properties for interneurons in
164 G.A. Mihailoff et al.

the A-laminae of the eat's lateral geniculate nucleus. Tolbert, D., and Panneton, W. (1984): The transience
Exp. Brain Res., 73,384-392. of cerebrocerebellar projections is due to selective
Swenson, R., Kosinski, R., and Castro, A. (1984): Topo- elimination of axon collaterals and not neuronal
graphy of spinal, dorsal column nuclear, and spinal death. Dev. Brain Res., 16, 301-306.
trigeminal projections to the pontine gray in rats. J. Watt, c., and Mihailoff, G. (1983): The cerebellopontine
Compo Neurol., 222, 301-311. system in the rat. II. Electron microscopic studies.
Thier, P., and Koehler, W. (1987): Morphology, number J. Compo Neurol., 216, 429-437.
and distribution of putative GABA-ergic neurons in Welker, W. (1987): Spatial organization of somato-
the basilar pontine gray of the monkey. J. Compo sensory projections to granule cell cerebellar cortex:
N eurol., 265, 311-322. functional and connectional implications of fractured
Tolbert, D., and Panneton, W. (1983): Transient cere- somatotopy (Summary of Wisconsin Studies) In:
brocerebellar projections in kittens: Postnatal de- New Concepts ill Cerebellar Neurobiology
velopment and topography. J. Compo Neurol., 221, (l.S. King, ed.). New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc.,
216-228. pp.239-280.
Part 2
Electrophysiology of Purkinje
and Inferior Olivary Neurons

The first chapter in this part is an up-to-date review of the electrophysiologi-


cal properties of single Purkinje cells. It begins with a review of the many
experimental approaches which were brought to bear to dissect the wide
range of ionic conductances found in these neurons and to reveal the
distribution of the various channels over the somadendritic membrane.
This section of the chapter ends with the story of the "P" (for Purkinje)
channel. The second section, beginning with consideration of Purkinje cell
ligand-gated responsiveness, speaks to the question of the functional signi-
ficance of the wealth of conductances enjoyed by these cells.
The second chapter brings the glass closer and considers the electrical
properties of single channels in the soma of Purkinje cells in organotypic
culture. Patch clamp recordings using both cell-attached and outside-out
configurations were used to study voltage-gated sodium and potassium
currents; calcium currents were not identified. The sodium current had
two components (transient and sustained). The authors believe that one
channel may underlie this based on the elemental properties of the channels
as determined by fluctuation analysis and single-channel measurements.
Two types of K channel were identified using single-channel recordings;
one (small conductance channel) with properties similar to the delayed
rectifier, the other (large conductance) probably corresponding to the
gK.Ca' Purkinje cells do not respond to NMDA, but large inward currents
are elicited by non-NMD A glutamate agonists, and they respond to GAB A
with a large inward current. Soma apparently have a high density of
GABAergic channels and a low density of glutaminergic channels.
The last chapter in this section is concerned with the oscillatory properties
of 10 neurons and introduces the use of a computer-brain slice hybrid to
study this problem.

165
8
The Electrophysiology of the
Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Revisited
Rodolfo R. Llimis and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

The electro physiological properties of mamma- aptic response to climbing fiber activity, the com-
lian Purkinje cells has been a subject of study plex spike, is observed at all levels of cerebellar
for more than 30 years (Eccles et aI., 1967) and phylogeny (Llinas and Hillman, 1969).
has produced a wealth of information relating to
the ionic conductances in mammalian central
neurons (cf. Llimis, 1988). Yet it is clear from the
Purkinje Cell Electroresponsiveness
many examples of intracellular recordings ob- That the electrophysiology of Purkinje cells is as
tained in vivo that the Purkinje cell is capable of baroque as their morphology (Fig. 8.1, see color
many functional states. Such complexity is the Plate 1) was established long ago when it was
expected product of the rich intrinsic electrical discovered that these cells generate complex pat-
properties of these cellular elements and of the terns of dendritic spiking (Llinas et aI., 1968;
wealth of synaptic connectivity they receive. Llinas and Nicholson, 1971). These action poten-
Indeed, if we consider that the tertiary spiny tials were found to be generated by different
branch lets of mammalian Purkinje cells are con- branches such that as many as six to eight different
tacted by many as 200,000 parallel fibers (Fox dendritic spike-generating zones were revealed
and Barnard, 1957; Fox et aI., 1967) and that, by the presence of several all-or-none components
unlike other central nervous system (CNS) spines in each action potential. Later it was discovered
(Peters et aI., 1976), only one parallel fiber synapse that these action potentials are calcium dependent
is made onto each spine head (Paley and Chan- (Llinas and Hess, 1976; Llinas et aI., 1977; Llinas
Paley, 1974), a large variation of dendritic-input and Sugimori, 1980a).
patterns and thus, an enormous variability in The detailed study of the ionic mechanisms
the activation states, may be expected. Equally underlying Purkinje cell electro responsiveness
surprising is the other main synaptic input, the began with in vitro intracellular recordings in
climbing-fiber system. In most adult cells only cerebellar slices (Llinas and Sugimori, 1980a, b).
one climbing-fiber afferent contacts a given The results presented in those papers suggested
Purkinje cell. The contacts are made on groups that, in addition to dendritic-calcium electrores-
of postsynaptic spines arising from the smooth ponsiveness, somatic electroresponsiveness may
surface of the primary and secondary dendrites be characterized by the presence of at least two
(Larramendi and Victor, 1967). Clusters of six to types of voltage-gated sodium conductances.
eight spines are often organized in longitudinal
rows along the dendrite and are contacted by en Ionic Conductances:
passant enlargements of the climbing fiber as it
progresses over the surface of the dendrites. As
General Description
many as 300 spines are contacted by a single Although current-clamp studies showed that the
afferent fiber (Llimis et aI., 1969). Given this struc- sodium and calcium conductances in Purkinje
ture, it is not surprising that a stereotyped syn- cells were largely confined to the somatic and
167
168 Rodolfo R. Llimis and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

dendritic membranes, respectively (Llinas and by a full set of sodium-dependent action potentials
Sugimori, 1980a, b), a more direct demonstration at the somatic level, which generated clear in-
of the distribution of these ionic conductances ward all-or-none action currents in the immediate
was required in order to understand the complex vicinity of the soma, with amplitudes on the order
firing properties of these cells. Two types of experi- of hundreds of pico amps (Fig. 8.2B, middle trace).
ments were designed toward this end. The first The negative deflection (inward current) indi-
consisted of com paring the properties of the spike cated that the sodium-dependent spikes were gen-
waveform, as recorded intracellularly with the erated at the somatic level (Fig. 8.2C). With a
extracellular field potentials simultaneously gen- further increase in the depolarizing current in
erated by the same neuron. To implement this addition to the fast sodium spikes, slower calcium-
paradigm, Purkinje cells were recorded intracel- dependent spikes were elicited (dots). These latter
lularly at soma while the extracellular current flow spikes were seen by the somatic patch electrode
was monitored using a currentometric amplifier (middle trace) as positive deflections (outward
configuration and a patch pipette (Fig. 8.2A). currents) (dots in Fig. 8.2C), indicating a spike
Depolarization to threshold was accompanied generation site remote from the soma. That the

~18OOpA
11111 J 1
<--_ _

o E F

1.1 ' - ~.
I
CI 20mV
.6OOpA
2M

100m...,

Figure 8.2. Simultaneous ' intracellular and extracel- while the high-threshold calcium spikes (dots) generated
lular recording from a Purkinje cell. In A, intracel- extracellular positive deflections (outward current flow).
lular recordings were obtained at the somatic level The calcium spikes are indicated by dots (the intracel-
while a patch electrode filled with normal Ringer solu- lular and extracellular records). In D , the extracellular
tion recorded current transmembrane at the somatic electrodes moved from the somatic to the dendritic
level. In 8, the upper trace is the intracellular somatic level of the same cell. Experiments similar to those in
potential trace. The second trace is the transmem- 8 and C were performed. In E, the direct stimulation
brane current. Note that every spike in the intracel- of cells at the somatic level produced clear positive
lular record is accompanied-by a sharp extracellular potentials. Extracellularly (outward current) following
negativity (inward current). The third trace figure indi- a stimulus increase (F), calcium spikes (dots) are seen
cates the size and duration of direct current injections as negative current fields (inward currents) by the cur-
that activated the cell. C: Similar recording as 8 , but rent metric amplifier (Llimis and Sugimori, unpublished
a higher current-injection pulse. The fast spikes were obvervations).
accompanied by simultaneous extracellular negativity
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 169

direction of current flow at the somatic level barium was then iontophoresed into the bath
was the opposite for the sodium- and calcium- and full spikes were observed as soon as either
dependent action potentials indicates that these of these divalent cation species reached the surface
two distinct types of spikes were generated at of the dendritic tree of the impaled cell. The res-
different sites in the neuron. ponse to BaCl 2 iontophoresis is shown in Figures
The patch electrode was then gently moved to 8.3C and 8.3D. Indeed, current injection of in-
the dendritic level while the intracellular electrode creasing amplitude produced subthreshold
was kept in place (Fig. 8.2D). In this configuration, responses at low levels and prolonged barium-
the patch electrode recorded extracellular cur- dependent all-or-none depolarization at high cur-
rents with precisely the opposite polarity ofthose rent levels. The middle traces in Figure 8.3C illus-
recorded when the electrode was near the soma. trate the amplitude and duration of the trans-
That is, when fast action potentials were activated, membrane current injected, and the lower trace
the patch electrode near the dendrites recorded shows the onset and duration of the iontophoretic
outward current transients (Fig. 8.2E), but when BaCl 2 injections. Direct observation at the ion-
the slower calcium spikes were generated, clear tophoretic electrode made it possible to direct the
inward current flow was detected at the dendritic barium solution to different regions of the Purkinje
level (Fig. 8.2F). cell dendritic tree. The traces in Figure 8.3D are
In a second type of experiment, designed to shown at a slow sweep speed to demonstrate the
test further the hypothesis that voltage-gated prolonged spikes typically recorded following
channels are segregated over the plasmalemma, barium superfusion over the dendritic tree. When
intrasomatic recordings were made in the pres- the barium electrode was placed immediately next
ence of tetrodotoxin (TTX) and the absence of to the soma (Fig. 8.3E), no obvious electro-
extracellular calcium (Fig. 8.3A). Calcium or responsiveness was observed following direct

c E

80"
n~="",,:

B o Co", TTl F

20mV
2n.o.
I

=========
20mV
I 2n.o.

-----12n.o. 800n.o.
'j(j"$

Figure 8.3. Intrasomatic recording from a Purkinje tions the soma is directly depolarized and a large
cell in the presence of TTX and zero extracellular response is obtained. This response is seen at slower
calcium. In A, the intracellular recording obtained at sweep speed in D, where six stimuli similar to those
the somatic level following outward transmembrane shown in C are presented at short intervals. In E, the
current pulses before electrophoretic barium applica- barium electrode is moved from the dendritic region
tion to the dendritic tree. Three levels of transmem- to the immediate vicinity of the soma and the experi-
brane current injection are shown in mid-trace. In C, ment was repeated. Under these conditions no barium
barium is electrophoretically applied (note lowest trace) action potential was observed (F) (Llinas and Sugimori,
at the dendritic level. During the iontophoretic injec- unpublished observations).
170 Rodolfo R. Llinas and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

depolarization of the soma (Fig. 8.3F). This was Dendritic Conductances


the case although the barium injection was the
Because Purkinje cell smooth dendrites in mam-
same and the voltage steps were higher than those
mals, as well as in some lower vertebrates, are
used in Figures 8.3C and 8.30. These experiments
large enough to allow intradendritic penetration
indicate once again that the somatic membrane
without cell injury, a direct study of the electro-
of the Purkinje cell has a very low density of
physiology of Purkinje cell dendrites has been
voltage-gated calcium channels.
possible in several species. Intradendritic record-
In addition to the electrophysiological investi-
ings were first made in reptilian Purkinje cells,
gation ofthe properties of the Purkinje cell soma,
where it was discovered that dendrites are capa-
a supplementary set of experiments was designed
ble of voltage-dependent electro responsiveness
using fluorescent TTX (Sugimori et aI., 1986) to
(Llimis et aI., 1968; Llinas and Nicholson, 1971).
localize the sodium channels over the Purkinje
In a latter set of studies on avian Purkinje cells
cell (Fig. 8.4, see color Plate 2). The fluorescent
it was further demonstrated by intradendritic re-
derivative ofTTX (7-diethylaminocoumarin-TTX)
cording that these processes can support calcium-
used in these studies (Angelides, 1981) was ex-
dependent electroresponsiveness (Llinas and
cited at 380 nm and had fluorescent emission at
Hess, 1976). Both of these studies also showed
480 nm. Aliquots of fluorescently labeled TTX
that dendritic spikes are quite different from
(10-100mM) were added to the bath at a final
axonic action potentials in that they are not con-
concentration of 10 to l00nM. The slices were
ducted continuously. In vitro recordings from
then observed via a 6, 25, or 50 x objective, using
Purkinje cells in guinea-pig cerebellar slices
a silicone-intensified television camera (MTI
(Llinas et aI., 1977; Llinas and Sugimori, 1980b)
Model 65) fiber-coupled to a double-microchannel
revealed that Purkinje cell dendrites are charac-
plate (I. T.T). The images obtained by this method
terized by two distinct types of electroresponsive-
were stored and digitized using a polyproces-
ness: so-called plateau potentials and compound
sor (Hamamatsu system) that allowed us to
dendritic spikes. The difference between these
quantify the amount of fluorescence omitted from
two types of dendritic activation, in our opinion,
the tissue. Slices were photographed in pseu-
reflects the distribution of calcium channels over
docolor using an image processing system
the dendritic arbor rather than the presence of
(Oeanza Inc.).
different species of voltage-gated conductances.
At low magnification (Fig. 8.4A) TTX binding
Also associated with dendritic electroresponsive-
sites were clearly visible in the axonal and somatic
ness was the activation of voltage- and calcium-
portion of the Purkinje cells but were not seen in
activated potassium channels (Llinas et aI., 1968;
the dendritic tree, except for the very basal portion
Llinas and Nicholson, 1971) and calcium- activated
of the main arbor. Note that whereas the labeling
chloride channels (Llano et aI., 1991).
was most prominent in the white matter and in
the granular and Purkinje cell layers, it was faint
in the molecular layer, probably because the
parallel fibers were transversely cut. At a higher
Plateau Potentials
magnification (Fig. 8.4B) the somata of Purkinje The plateau potentials of Purkinje cells are char-
cells could be clearly seen as fluorescent, whereas acterized by prolonged all-or-none events of
the dendritic tree, which was clearly seen in white rather low amplitude that may last for tens
light, did not show any TTX fluorescence. The or even hundreds of milliseconds (Llinas and
presence of nonspecific binding was investigated Sugimori, 1980a, b). This is shown in Figure 8.5,
by adding unlabeled TTX, which reduced the where the impaled dendrite was directly activated
fluorescence to close to autofluorescent levels, via the recording electrode. The plateau responses
indicating that the fluorescence of the labeled generated by injected current pulses (lower traces)
TTX was specifically linked to TTX binding sites. were totally separable from the somatic fast spike
Blocking of sodium action potentials following and the dendritic spike potentials. Indeed, as illus-
application of labeled TTX was demonstrated trated in Figure 8.5, a small dendritic depolariza-
by intransomatic recording. tion may produce different amplitude plateau
Plate 1

Figure 8.1. Intracellular fluorescence image of a fixation. Note that the diameter of the primary, sec-
Purkinje cell injected with Lucifer yellow in an iso- ondary, and even the tertiary dendrites is large
lated cerebellar slice from the adult guinea pig. This enough to be impaled intracellularly (somatic diame-
image indicates the size and distribution of the differ- ter 45 lIm).
ent portions of the Purkinje cell in the absence of
Plate 2

Figure 8.4. Fluorescence microscopy demonstrating the pre!,ent at the Purkinje cell dendritic tree. In B, high
distribution of TTX binding sites in cerebellar slices. magnification of this fluorescence demonstrates the
Note that the fluorescent-label Trx in A is clearly somatic distribution of TTX binding sites in Purkinje
present in the cerebellar white matter as well as at the cells (Sugimori, Lliruis and Angelides, unpublished ob-
granular layer and the Purkinje cell layer, but is not servations) .

Figure 8.7. lliustration of different levels and locations tial. D-H: Fluctuations of fluorescence in the dendritic
of [Ca2 +]j during spontaneous dendritic spiking in a tree during activity. E-G: Full dendritic spikes occurred
Purkinje cell. The body of the Purkinje cell has been and were accompanied by increased [Ca2 +]j in the main
drawn to mark its location with respect to the fura-2 dendrites. H: Intermediate time between spikes. I: Re-
signal. A: Fluorescent image of the neuron after remov- turn of [Ca2 +]j to the baseline level after termination of
ing the intracellular electrode. B: Subtraction of back- dendritic spike burst. In this and other figures, increases
ground fluorescence from continuous recording show- in [Ca2 +]j are indicated by the color bar on the right:
ing no increase in [Ca2 +]j at the resting potential. C: Green indicates low and red indicates high [Ca2 +]j
[Ca2 +1i at the beginning of the burst before dendritic (modified from Sugimori and LIinas, 1990b).
action potential generation, the so-called plateau poten-
Plate 3


.,
I
() 0

,.
I
.,
CI
0

LL

•·
0 CI 0
Q

" a
Q

CO I
••
.,=·
0

.,"
Plate 4

Figure 8.8. Distribution of [Ca2+]j after iontophoretic with respect to the fura-2 signal.) During the plateau
application of glutamate to the dendrite of a Purkin- potential, a distinct increase of [Ca2+]j was observed
je cell following fura-2 loading. A: The fluorescent in the absence of a plateau potential. C: The plateau
cell (arrowhead marks the position of the ion- potential was followed by full dendritic spiking. Note
tophoretic electrode). B: Iontophoresis producing a that the [Ca2+]j increase, although widespread, was
plateau potential (left-pointing arrowhead) after the highest in the three lower main dendrites . Calibration
initial ligand-dependent depolarization. (The den- was 30 m V, 5 ms (modified from Sugimori and
dritic tree was drawn in white to mark its location Llimis, 1990b).

Figure 8.9. Distribution of intracellular


calcium in Purkinje cells due to ligand
activation of a metabotropic glutamate
receptor. A: Purkinje cell filled with
fura-2. B: Fura-2 response following fir-
ing of the Purkinje cell. C: The response
of the cell after removing extracellular
calcium. D: Response of Purkinje cell to
glutamic acid injection at the somatic
level to demonstrate the release of calci-
um from intracellular stores following
activation of metabotropic glutamic acid
receptor at the soma and dendrites of the
Purkinje cells (Llinas and Sugimori, un-
published observations).
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 171

plateaus represents an equilibrium state in which


the inward calcium current is balanced by an
outward potassium current flowing across the
dendritic membrane. The dendritic calcium cur-
rent has recently been described at the single
channel level using patch clamping of Purkinje
cell dendrites (U sowicz et aI., 1991).
A detailed description of the voltage-gated
calcium conductances was obtained following

.~.--------------------
20mv
2 nA iontophoretic application of glutamic acid onto
1 the dendrites. This produced well defined ligand-
50msec
dependent potentials in the dendrites on which
Figure 8.5. Intradendritic recording from a Purkinje voltage-gated events were observed (Fig. 8.6A).
cell in the presence of TTX. Direct depolarization at Thus, glutamic acid iontophoresis produced a
this site generated initially a passive response. At an depolarization whose amplitude was related to
intermediate level calcium-dependent plateau poten- the amplitude of the iontophoretic current
tials were observed. At a higher level of depolarization (Fig. 8.6A). The traces in Figure 8.6A show the
dendritic spikes were generated (Llinas and Sugimori, responses to six stimuli of increasing amplitude
unpublished observations). under control conditions. A similar set of stimuli
were delivered after calcium removal and in
the presence of TTX (Fig. 8.6B). Note that
potentials, some of which may be large enough to in Figure 8.6A the response consisted of three
trigger a family of dendritic calcium spikes. distinct components. The lowest iontophoretic
Plateau potentials were abolished by calcium injection elicited a smooth triangular depolariza-
channel blockers or by the removal of extracel- tion that reached a p~ak at the end of the injection.
lular calcium (Llimls and Sugimori, 1980a, b). A slightly larger injection generated a longer de-
Furthermore, in the presence of potassium chan- polarization and a plateau potential that out-
nel blockers such as tetraethylammonium, the lasted the iontophoretic pulse. Note that the
plateau voltage often overshot the resting poten- plateau potential began after the termination of
tial. This suggests that the amplitude of these the iontophoretic injection, had a distinct peak,

A Normal B
Ringer Ca' free

-~ 20 mV

100 nA

100 msec
Figure 8.6. Intradendritic recording following direct larization generated fast small spikes, indicating somatic
application of glutamic acid in the immediate vicinity firing ofthe Purkinje cell and, finally, longer and more
of the recording site. In A, iontophoretic glutamate prolonged dendritic calcium spikes. In B, a similar set
pulses of different amplitudes (A) generated prolonged of glutamic iontophoretic injections generate smooth
responses consisting of slow depolarizations followed potentials in the presence ofTTX, and no extracellular
by calcium-dependent plateau responses. At higher calcium (Llinas and Sugimori, unpublished obser-
levels of glutamate injection the trans dendritic depo- vations).
172 Rodolfo R. Llimis and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

and a very slow falling phase. This is particularly background fluorescence (obtained during an
clear when compared to the pure ligand-dependent electrically silent period just before activation).
potential shown in Figure 8.6B. With further in- Entry of calcium during the plateau potential
creases in iontophoretic pulse amplitude, the firing that preceded dendritic calcium spike activation
level for the soma was reached producing small was most prominent in the periphery of the den-
(3-4 mV), fast sodium-dependent spikes that were dritic tree (Fig. 8.7C), whereas increased [Ca 2 +]j
electrotonically conducted to the dendritic record- was not seen in the main dendrites until the initial
ing site. Note also the prolonged plateau potential peak of a calcium spike (Fig. 8.70). Subsequent
that followed this initial response. Finally, the action potentials occurred in different branches
largest two injections, in addition to plateau poten- of the Purkinje cell as shown in Figure 8.7E
tials and somatic sodium spikes, generated large through 8.7G. The image in Figure 8.7H was
dendritic calcium spikes (20-30 m Vat the record- taken at a time of intermediate activity between
ing site). Figure 8.6B illustrates the responses dendritic spikes. In Figure 8.71, dendritic spiking
elicited after addition of TTX and removal of abruptly ceased and [Ca2+]j returned to control
calcium from the bath. Note the smooth character levels. Note that the increase in [Ca 2 +]j occurred
of these potentials, indicating that at even the initially at the fine dendritic branches and that
lowest level of glutamic acid injection a voltage- during repetitive activity some dendritic branches
gated calcium current adds to the ligand-dependent showed larger signals than others (Sugimori and
polarization. These results also demonstrate the Llinas, 1990b).
clear difference between plateau potentials and
dendritic calcium spikes. Dendritic Glutamate Iontophoresis:
In an attempt to understand better the mech- Voltage-Dependent and
anism underlying the dendritic plateau potentials,
M etabotropic Responses
calcium entry was visualized directly and meas-
ured using the dye, fura-2. In earlier Ca-sensitive The distribution of [Ca2+]j following iontopho-
dye studies the intracellular calcium concentra- retic application of glutamate to the Purkinje cell
tion changes were visualized using the ratio method dendrites is shown in Figure 8.8 (Sugimori and
for fura-2 (Tank et aI., 1988), with activation at Llinas, 1990a, b, see color Plate 4). The arrowhead
340 and 380 nm (Tsien and Poeni, 1986), or using in Figure 8.8A marks the position of the ionto-
arsenazo II (Ross and Werman, 1987). Both sets phoretic electrode with respect to the fura-2-
of measurements demonstrated a clear increase loaded cell. As shown in Figure 8.8B, iontophoresis
in calcium concentration in the dendritic tree of glutamate elicited a plateau potential (left-
during the generation of calcium spikes. During pointing arrow) during which distinct calcium
the plateau phase of the potential the dendritic entry was observed at the level of the dendritic
branchlets were the first structures to demonstrate branchlets (right-pointing arrow). Whenever the
calcium entry while full action potentials occurred ligand-dependent depolarization did not generate
concomitantly with an increase in calcium entry a plateau potential (e.g., waveform indicated by
into the main dendritic branches (Tank et aI., the dots in Fig. 8.8B), no calcium entry was ob-
1988). These results supported the view that cal- served. When the plateau potential elicited full
cium influx occurs at the dendritic branchlets and dendritic spiking (Fig. 8.8C), widespread calcium
is recorded at the main dendrites and at the soma entry was particularly prominent in the main
as plateau potentials or spikes, depending on the dendrites (yellow and green).
number of dendritic branches that are activated. These results indicate that glutamate ionto-
As shown in Figure 8.7 (see color Plate 3), phoresis activates the spiny branch lets that gene-
Purkinje cells injected with fura-2 could be imaged rate maximal amplitude responses via the opening
with good spatial resolution with excitation at of voltage-gated calcium channels in these fine
380nm (Sugimori and Llinas, 1990b). Figure 8.7B processes (Sugimori and Llinas, 1990a, b). It is
represents the image obtained after on-line sub- these responses that were observed at the somatic
traction of the fluorescence of the injected Purkinje level as plateau potentials. When a sufficient
cell (Fig. 8.7A) from the averaged (n = 16 frames) number of branchlets were depolarized to bring
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 173

the plateau potential to the threshold for spike in Purkinje cells. Indeed, since the phosphoinosi-
initiation, the secondary dendritic branches gen- tide-derived second messenger IP3 has been shown
erate full spikes. These spikes propagated ortho- to be a potent mobilizer of internally stored calcium
dromically toward the soma and antidromically in numerous tissues, including the CNS, (Berridge
into other distal dendritic branchlets producing and Irvine, 1989; Ferris et ai., 1989), we expected
an image such as that shown in Figure 8.8C. that the increase in free intracellular calcium may
The results further suggest that, in vivo, once be mediated by IP3. Since the activation of the
enough spines are activated through the parallel phosphoinositide cycle is in response to stimula-
fiber activation, the branchlets respond in a close- tion with neurotransmitters, hormones and growth
to-unitary manner and that at the junction between factors have been extensively characterized in a
the spiny branch lets and the main dendrite, acti- number of tissues (Berridge and Irvine, 1984).
vation of the voltage-dependent calcium and then Following binding of the agonist to the cell surface
the potassium conductance and chloride curtails receptor protein, an as yet unidentified G protein
the high amplitude of these responses. Further- is activated, which in tum activates a phospho-
more, it must be remembered that dendritic inositide-specific phospholipase C. The phospholi-
inhibition, which is extraordinarily effective in pase C hydrolyzes phosphatidylinositol-4,5-bis-
blocking both plateau potentials and dendritic phosphate to yield two putative second messengers,
action potentials, acts directly on the smooth IP3 and diaccylglycerol (DG). Inositol triphosc
portion of the dendritic tree (Llinas et ai., 1968) phate has been shown to mobilize calcium from
and serves as a remote inhibitory mechanism internal endoplasmic reticulum stores (Berridge
capable of regulating the transmission of dendritic and Irvine, 1984), whereas DG is thought to acti-
activity down to the soma. vate a phospholipid- and calcium-sensitive protein
Besides the activation of voltage-gated calcium kinase (Nishizuka, 1984). There is also evidence
channels, iontophoretic injection of glutamic acid to suggest that a metabolic product of IP3,
can activate intracellular calcium release in the inositol-l,3,4,5-tetrakisphosphate (IP4), may play
absence of extracellular calcium and in the pres- a role in maintaining elevated calcium concentra-
ence of ethylene glycol tetraacetic acid (EGT A) tions (Berridge and Irvine, 1984).
(1 mM). This metabotropic response, which seems More specifically, phosphoinositide turnover
to induce calcium release from intracellular stores, and the generation of second messengers in the
is demonstrated in Figure 8.9 (see color Plate 4.) CNS results from the actions of a number of
following fura-2 injection into a Purkinje cell. neurotransmitters (Nahorski et ai., 1986), includ-
Note that the cell, which was loaded with fura ing glutamic acid. Blackstone et ai. (1989) dem-
in the somatic and dendritic compartments onstrated that glutamate and its analogues,
(Fig. 8.9A), demonstrated a clear fura-2 response quisqualate and ibotenate, were able to induce
at the dendritic arbor during spike activation accumulation of inositol phosphates in rat cere-
(Fig. 8.9B). Following removal of extracellular bellar slices. The effect of phosphoinositides was
calcium and addition of EGT A to the bath, acti- not secondary to release of other neurotransmit-
vation of the cell did not trigger a fura-2 response ters or to calcium influx. In animals with Purkinje
(Fig. 8.9C). By contrast, iontophoretic washing cell degeneration, glutamate was ineffective. The
of glutamic acid onto the body and dendrites calcium-transporting properties ofthe IP3 receptor
following a clear increase in [Ca 2 +1 was seen. In protein purified from cerebellum (Supattapone
this case, the intracellular response was not limited et ai., 1988) have been demonstrated in reconsti-
to the dendrites but was also strongly visible at tuted lipid vesicles (Ferris et ai., 1989) and the
the somatic level (Fig. 8.9D), which would be protein has been localized to the Purkinje cell
expected where Ca is released from intracellular endoplasmic reticulum (Ross et ai., 1989). Al-
stores. though stimulation of phospho in osit ide turnover
These results have suggested to us that a by glutamate and the mobilization of radioactive
mechanism that, regulates intracellular calcium calcium by IP3 have been studied in these cells,
concentration such as inositol triphosphate (IP3) mobilization of calcium by glutamate has never
may be activated by the metabotropic receptors been characterized.
174 Rodolfo R. Llim'ts and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

In short, then, two types of calcium-dependent of one another during parallel fiber activity. As
siow conductances can be observed at the den- with the plateau potentials, dendritic action poten-
dritic level by glutamate iontophoresis: one is tials are totally abolished by pharmacological
voltage-dependent and the other is metabotropic. manipulations that block calcium channels or by
the removal of extracellular calcium. In contrast,
in the presence of extracellular BaH powerful
Dendritic Action Potentials dendritic responses may be generated that seem
The second eiectroresponsive property of Purkinje to obliterate the differences between dendritic
cell dendrites to be studied was the ability ofthese spikes and plateau potentials. In short, the charac-
processes to generate full action potentials. Per- teristics of Purkinje cell dendritic excitability lead
haps the most intriguing characteristic of their us to conclude that different parts of the dendritic
initial description was the fact that dendritic tree are independently electro responsive and that
action potentials comprise several distinct all-or- the two categories of response, the plateau poten-
none components (Llinas and Nicholson, 1971; tials and dendritic spikes, differ only in that they
Llinas and Hess, 1976). Thus, rather than the are generated by different compartments of the
simple waveforms observed, for example, in a Purkinje cell dendrites that, in addition, have
motoneuron, where one soma dendritic com- different complements of potassium channel types.
ponent may be observed (Terzuolo and Araki,
1961; Eccles et aI., 1966a), Purkinje cell spikes
may have as many as six all-or-none components,
The P Channel
as initially demonstrated in reptiles (Llinas and Beyond concluding that a single class of calcium
Nicholson, 1971). This implies that the action channel underlies dendritic electro responsiveness,
potentials are probably generated in the main we concluded further that this calcium channel,
branches of the Purkinje cells. Specific dendritic present in Purkinje cells, is not among those de-
branches may, in fact, be activated independently scribed by Nowycky et aI., in dorsal root ganglion

c o
FTX+TTX TTX

r--->_____________

B FTX E
TTX +FTX

120 mV

______________ 120 mV
s---!~

11 nA
2 nA
100 ms 20 ms

Figure 8.10. A: Intracellular recording from a Purkinje blocks the fast action potentials and the plateau poten-
cell shows fast Na + -dependent spikes and Ca 2 + -de- tials shown in B. D: Ca 2 + -dependent slow action poten-
pendent action potentials (arrows). B: Five minutes tial is generated by direct stimulation of the Purkinje
after application of FTX, Ca 2 + spiking disappeared, cell in the presence of TTX. E: Addition to FTX to the
and a prolonged Na + -dependent plateau potential is bath produces a blockage of the voltage-dependent
observed that far outlasts the duration of the stimu- Ca2+ electroresponsiveness (Llinas and Sugimori, un-
lation (lower trace). C: Addition of TTX to the bath published observations).
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 175

cells (1985). That is, it is not an L channel on the calcium channels studied in dorsal-root ganglion
grounds that it is not blocked by dihydropyri- and other cells (see Miller, 1987).
dines; not an N channel in that it is not blocked More reccently, histochemical studies, based
by il-conotoxin and does not inactivate; and not on the development of a polyclonal antibody
a T channel in that it is activated at - 45 m V and against the P channel, yielded a rather compre-
does not inactivate. Recently, we have demon- hensive picture of the spatial distribution of the
strated that calcium-dependent responses in P channels in the CNS (Hillman et a!., 1991).
Purkinje cells are blocked by funnel-web spider Moreover, the distribution of antibody binding
toxin (FTX). The effect of FTX on the electrore- sites agrees well with the electro physiological
sponsive properties of Purkinje cells is illustrated findings relating to the distribution of P calcium
in Figures 8.10A and 8.10B. channels. Indeed, a dense reaction in Purkinje
In Figure to.8A, a Purkinje cell demonstrates cell dendrites indicates antigen localization dis-
oscillatory behavior upon direct intracellular sti- tally on the dendritic arbor, especially at bifurca-
mulation. The arrows indicate the calcium spikes. tion points. Ultrastructural studies of the immuno-
After bath application of FTX (Fig. 8.l0B), the response further demonstrated membrane reaction
oscillation was blocked and a prolonged activation occurring as patches on the main dendrites, spiny
of the persistent sodium conductance (Llinas and branches, and spines (Hillman et a!., 1991). The
Sugimori, 1980a; Sugimori et a!., 1986; Gahwiler latter, perhaps the most surprising finding, de-
and Llano, 1989) produced a prolonged plateau monstrates that the head, and occasionally the
potential. This plateau was blocked by the addition neck, of the spine have clear immunoreactive
of TTX to the bath (Fig. 8.10C). The opposite sites at the plasmalemma, indicating that spines
experiment, where the sodium conductance was themselves may be capable of electro responsive-
initially blocked by TTX and a voltage-gated ness.
calcium spike was elicited by direct stimulation,
is illustrated in Fig. 8.l0D. Following the addition
of FTX to the bath, the prolonged calcium spike
Functional Properties of the
was blocked (Fig. 8.10E). Funnel-web spider toxin, Synaptic Input
which has been shown to be a polyamine (Cherksey
The Ascending Axons of the Granular
et aI., 1989), was effective in the micromolar range
Cells and Fractured Somatotopy
and was capable of blocking both plateau poten-
tials and-dendritic action potentials, supporting It has long been established that the mossy fiber
the view that one type of channel underlies these system can elicit, via the granule cell relay, well-
two responses. defined regions of Purkinje cell activity that are
In addition, the characteristics of single chan- restricted to the immediate vicinity ofthe granule
nels isolated from guinea-pig cerebellum have fiber terminals and that do not correlate with the
been determined after their incorporation into horizontal distribution of the parallel fibers over
lipid bilayers (Cherksey et a!., 1989; Llinas et aI., the molecular layer (Eccles et al., 1966b; Oscarrson
1989). The current-voltage relation is similar to and Sjolund, 1977a, b; Shambes et a!., 1978). This
that of the macroscopic current observed in finding, which was recently confirmed using im-
Purkinje cells with patch clamp (Llinas et aI., aging probes linked to c-fos and with 2-deoxy-
1989) and has the same sensitivity to FTX as glucose (Sharp et a!., 1989), is at odds with the
Purkinje cells studied in brain slices (Llinas et aI., view that parallel fiber stimulation can generate
1989). The average open time, which is voltage- activity in Purkinje cells located along the whole
dependent, varies from 1 to 3 ms. The single- length of the parallel fiber system. A possible
channel conductance, measured using an extra- solution for this problem was proposed some
cellular divalent anion concentration of 80 JlM, years ago with suggestion that the ascending
was on the order of 8 to lOpS for barium and 6 portion ofthe granular cell axon was responsible
to 8 pS when calcium was the divalent anion. for this behavior (Llinas, 1982).
This characterization of the P calcium channel Following this proposal, intracellular studies
clearly distinguishes it from the three types of in this cerebellar slices (100-150 Jl) (Fig. 8.11 A)
176 Rodolfo R. Llimis and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

A C 18
14 ---~ ==
12 'v-
10

8
r
Jl
nA

B 2

o
-2 __ --'1'-..___ I 60 mV
1 20 mV
50 msec
50 msec

Figure 8.11. Recording of intracellular potentials in shown in Band C. The gradation of these graded
Purkinje cells by stimulation of the ascending axons synaptic events occurs in distinct steps as indicated by
of granule cells. A: In vitro preparation consisting of a the distinct components of the rising phase. In C, the
very thin (approximately 100 11m) cerebellar slice that responses are shown at lower gain to demonstrate their
is stimulated at the granular cell level while the Purkinje synaptic nature. When the cell is hyperpolarized by
cell, located immediately above the stimulation site, is injection of - 2 nA to the recording electrode, all action-
intracellularly recorded. In B, stimulation to the gran- potential activity ceases. As the cell is depolarized, the
ular layer generates distinct steps of synaptic activa- amplitude of the synaptic potential decreases and finally
tion with monosynaptic latency. The synaptic poten- reverses at an injection level of 10 nA, demonstrating
tial may be large enough to reach threshold for spike the chemical nature of this potential (Llimis and
initiation and to generate full-size action potentials as Sugimori, unpublished observations).

have demonstrated that the activation of granule potential increased with membrane hyperpolari-
cells can produce, even in this very restricted zation. These results demonstrate the chemical
slice, clear synaptic potentials and full activation nature of this response.
of the Purkinje cells (Figs. 8.11 B, C). The sche- The possible functional effect of the ascending
matic diagram in Figure 11 A illustrates that with axon has been questioned on quantitative ana-
very thin slices, much of the surviving granule cell tomical grounds. Indeed, it was demonstrated
axon is represented by its ascending portion. The that whereas Purkinje cells in the rat may receive
graded nature ofthis synaptic response to granule as many as 150,000 parallel fibers, only a small
cell activation is shown in Figures 8.11 Band 8.11 C percentage (3%) are actually generated by the
(0 m V) as a set of synaptic potentials of increasing ascending axons (Napper and Harvey, 1988). It
amplitudes. The nature of this response is de- was further estimated that one ascending axon
monstrated in Figure 8.11 C, where a diminution may contact a particular Purkinje cell some 17
and reversal of this synaptic potential was ob- times and that the total number of synapses
tained with membrane depolarization, while the between a Purkinje cell and the granule cells
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 177

lying immediately under its dendritic arbor (274 inhibition produced by the release of gamma-
on the average) would provide no more than aminobutyric acid (GABA) from these terminals
4700 synapses on the Purkinje cell (Napper and is capable of blocking spike initiation at the axon
Harvey, 1988). However, this may be quite a hillock (Andersen et aI., 1964). This is especially
large number when interpreted from a functional powerful since a Purkinje cell may receive as
viewpoint. Indeed, since a climbing fiber requires many as 10 to 20 such terminals (Paley and Chan-
about 300 junctions to generate a "most power- Paley, 1974). Less well known, on the other hand,
ful activation of the Purkinje cells" (LIiil<ls et aI., is the fact that the basket cells also have ascend-
1969), the important variable may not be the ing axon collaterals that ramify on the Purkinje
total number of synapses contacting a structure. cell arbor to produce powerful dendritic inhibi-
Indeed, 4700 contacts may easily represent the tion capable of modifying dendritic spiking
synaptic connectivity of 10 or so climbing fibers. (Llinas et aI., 1968). In addition, the stellate cells,
Another issue that Napper and Harvey (1988) did which are located in the molecular layer, send
not seem to consider is that the timing of synaptic fine presynaptic terminals that cover most of the
activation is very important in determining syn- Purkinje cell dendritic tree. These cells are an
aptic effectiveness. Indeed, activation of the mossy- order of magnitude more numerous than the
fiber input to a patch of granular cells produces basket cells and the fields of their terminal axonic
well-defined activity within that patch producing arborizations are smaller than the dendritic span
synchronous and repetitive activation of a parti- of any given Purkinje cell (Paley and Chan-Paley,
cular set of granular cell axons that converge and 1974). They clearly play an important role in
synapse on a Purkinje cell (more than 4000) on dendritic integration within a single Purkinje cell.
their way toward their final bifurcation in the Iontophoretic injection of GABA at the den-
molecular layer. dritic level produces clear membrane hyperpo-
From a functional point of view the implica- larizations that can be reversed by membrane
tions of the numbers given above are quite signi- hyperpolarization. The sensitivity of these inhibi-
ficant, making the ascending axons of the granule tory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) to bicculine
cells an important component in the activation (Sugimori and LIinas, 1985) indicates an under-
of Purkinje cells via the mossy fiber-granular cell lying GABA type-A receptor. Furthermore, in
input. It may be important to remember that in addition to decreasing the excitability of the soma
addition to the powerful excitability observed and dendrite by the activation of a chloride con-
under anesthetized conditions (Eccles et aI., 1966b), ductance, GABA has a direct effect on the calcium
the parallel fibers may produce a modulating conductances at the dendritic level. This is illus-
effect where even small excitatory inputs may be trated in Figure 8.12, where iontophoresis of
sufficient to modulate the firing rate of a Purkinje glutamate (Fig. 8.l2A) elicited sodium- and cal-
cell that is close to its firing level. cium-dependent action potentials in the soma
(Fig. 8.12B). Under similar conditions, applica-
GABAergic Responses tion of GAB A to the dendritic tree was accom-
panied by a reduction of calcium electrorespon-
and Dendritic Inhibition siveness (Fig. 8.l2C). Thus, as shown in Figure
A more complete view of the many functional 8.12C, following the iontophoresis of GABA at
states that each cell may have can be envisioned the dendritic level, the rapid firing of sodium
if one considers that at least two types of inhibi- spikes was unchanged, whereas the powerful cal-
tion play directly on the soma and dendrites of cium response seen before the GABA injection
this neuron. Because the descending axons of the was abolished. Equally interesting is the fact that
basket cells wrap around the somata and come even when GABA receptors are blocked pharma-
together over the axon to form the so-called cologically by picrotoxin, application of GABA
Pinseau Terminal (Ramon y Cajal, 1904), the still reduces the calcium response (not shown)
178 Rodolfo R. L1inas and M utsuyuki Sugimori

2n'"
20mv
150n'"
200mlK

Figure 8.12. A: Intracellular recording from soma of oscillatory events. In C, application of glutamic acid
a Purkinje cell and dentritic iontophoretic application at the dendritic level leads to a total reduction of
of glutamic acid and GABA. In B, the glutamate ion- calcium spikes without affecting the somatic firing
tophoretic injection produces full sodium-dependent of neurons (L1inas and Sugimori, unpublished obser-
action potentials and calcium spikes as observed by vations).

(Llimls and Sugimori, unpublished results). These soma, sodium channels predominate. In addition,
findings suggest that GABA may have a direct the firing of a Purkinje cell in response to parallel-
effect on calcium channels that is independent of fiber or climbing-fiber activation is dictated by
its action on GABA A receptors. Inhibitory syn- the types and distributions of synaptic input that
apses would then have three mechanisms of action: the cell receives, as well as to their intrinsic elec-
through membrane hyperpolarization, by shunt- trophysiology.
ing active somatic and dendritic, and by reducing
Parallel-Fiber Activation
the excitability of the dendritic tree by acting
directly on calcium channels. Parallel fibers activate primarily the spiny branch-
lets and do so either directly through the verti-
calor ascending portion of the granular cell axon
Summary and through the horizontal compartment of the
In summary, the main characteristic of mam- parallel fibers. A Purkinje cell receives as many
malian Purkinje cells is their electrophysiological as 200,000 parallel fibers, making this quantita-
versatility, which is due largely to the quite specific tively the richest synaptic input. The postsynaptic
corifiguration of channels in its some-dendritic depolarization is restricted to the fine terminals
membrane. Indeed, at the dendritic level, calcium of the dendritic branches and has been demon-
channels seem to be the main voltage-gated con- strated to activate the so-called simple spikes,
ductance, and these are distributed differently which fire at a frequency of 10 to 20 Hz (cf. Llimls
in the fine and large dendrites, whereas at the and Simpson, 1981). This response begins as a
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 179

depolarjzation of the spiny branchlets and is fast and powerful IPSPs at the postsynaptic target
boosted by local calcium plateau potentials, which cell terminals, as demonstrated in the isolated
may begin as early as the postsynaptic spine cerebellum brain stem preparation (Llim'is and
(Hillman et aI., 1991). The dendritic plateau poten- M iihlethaler, 1988).
tials in turn activate a low-threshold, sodium In short, then, the electro responsive properties
plateau potential in the soma that serves to trigger of the Purkinje cell to activation by each of its
the full sodium spikes that travel down the axon afferent systems seem to be related closely to its
to release transmitter at the Purkinje cell presyn- integrative properties. The mossy fiber-parallel
aptic axon terminals, in the deep cerebellar nuclei. fiber system produces integration that may modu-
The presence of two plateau potentials between late cerebellar nuclear cells for a long time. This
the synaptic input and somatic firing allows for is in contrast to the climbing-fiber system, which
an extraordinarily subtle modulation of firing elicits well-defined, short-lasting, powerful inhi-
since the plateau potentials may be regulated by bitory potentials in deep nuclear cells. The ulti-
anyone or a combination of three mechanisms: mate relation of these two forms of Purkinje cell
a) voltage- and calcium-gated potassium conduct- activation to cerebellar function has been the
ances that have very different time courses, b) the subject of a great deal of research. Our own view
activation of GABAergic receptors that increase is that climbing- and parallel-fiber systems time-
the conductance to chloride, which may reduce share the Purkinje cell but are not completely
calcium flow at the dendrites, and c) shunting independent of one another functionally.
somatic currents to block both the plateau poten-
tial and the full action potential. The latter is
known to be the case, as a large shunt is observed References
at the somatic level during the activation of basket
Andersen, P., Eccles, J.e., and Voorhoeve, P.E. (1964):
cells and dendritic-level stellate cells. Thus, paral-
Postsynaptic inhibition of cerebellar Purkinje cells.
lel fiber- Purkinje cell synapses and the associated J. N europhysiol., 27, 1138-1153.
interneurons represent the circuits that ultimately Angelides, K. (1981): Fluorescent and photoactivatable
modulate Purkinje cell simple spike firing. fluorescent derivatives of tetrodotoxin to probe the
sodium channel of excitable membranes. Biochem-
Climbing-Fiber Activation istry, 20, 4107-4118.
As first shown in mammals, activation of climbing Berridge, M.J., and Irvine, R.F. (1984): Inositol tri-
fibers produces a burst response known as a phosphate, a novel second messenger in cellular
"complex spike" (Eccles et aI., 1966a). This com- signal transduction. Nature, 312, 315-321.
plex spike produces different patterns of action Berridge, M.J., and Irvine, R.F. (1989): Inositol phos-
phates and cell signalling. Nature, 341, 197-205.
potentials in the Purkinje cell axon, depending
Blackstone, e.D., Supattapone, S., and Snyder, S.H.
on the membrane potential (Eccles et aI., 1966a),
(1989): Inositol phospholipid-linked glutamate re-
and may also produce powerful calcium-de- ceptors mediate cerebellar parallel-fiber- Purkinje-
pendent plateau potentials (Llimls and Sugimori, cell synaptic transmission. Proc. Nat!. Acad. Sci.
1980b). The climbing fibers activate both dendritic USA, 86, 4316-4320.
spikes and plateau potentials with large levels of Cherksey, B., Lin, J.-W., Sugimori, M., and Llimis, R.
current flowing longitudinally into the cell soma. (1989): Further studies on the P type calcium channel
In contrast, this is prevented during parallel fiber in lipid bilayers and on the nature of FTX calcium
activation by the large load imposed by the channel blocker. Soc. Neurosci. Abst., 14, 652.
Purkinje cell dendritic tree. However, since climb- Eccles, J.e., Ito, M., and Szentgothai, J. (1967): The
ing fibers cover primarily the smooth portion of Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
the dendritic tree, which they depolarize in a
Eccles, J.e., Llinas, R., and Sasaki, K. (1966a): The
more or less synchronous manner, the loss of excitatory synaptic action of climbing fibers on the
current in the dendrites is minimal and thus the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. J. Physiol., 182,
Purkinje cell soma and axon can fire action poten- 268-296.
tials at a high frequency (Llimls and Sasaki, 1989). Eccles, le., Llinas, R., and Sasaki, K. (1966b): Parallel
Such firing may be as high as 400 Hz, producing fibre stimulation and responses induced thereby in
180 Rodolfo R. Llinas and Mutsuyuki Sugimori

the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Exp. Brain Res. Llinas, R., Nicholson, C, Freeman, J.A., and Hillman,
7,17-39. D.E. (1968): Dendritic spikes and their inhibition in
Ferris, CD., Huganir, R.L., Supattapone, S., and alligator Purkinje cells. Science, 160, 1132-1135.
Snyder, S.H. (1989): Purified inositol 1,4,5-trisphos- Llinas, R., and Sasaki, K. (1989): The functional or-
phate receptor mediates calcium flux in reconstituted ganization of the olivo-cerebellar system as examined
lipid vesicles. Nature, 342, 87-89. by multiple Purkinje cell recordings. Eur. J. New·o-
Fox, CA., and Barnard, J.W. (1957): A quantitative sci., 1,587-602.
study of the Purkinje cell dendritic branchlets and Llinas, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980a): Electrophysio-
their relationship to afferent fibers. J. Anat., 91, logical properties of in vitro Purkinje cell somata in
299-313. mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. (Lond.),
Fox, CA., Hillman, D., and Siegesmund, K.A. (1967): 305,171-195.
The primate cerebellar cortex: A Golgi and electron Llinas, R., and Sugimori, M. (l980b): Electrophysio-
microscopical study. Prog. Brain Res., 25, 174-225. logical properties of in vitro Purkinje cell dendrites
Gahwiler, B.H., and Llano, I. (1989): Sodium and in mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol. (Lond.),
potassium conductances in somatic membranes of 305, 197-213.
rat Purkinje cells from organotypic cerebellar cul- Llinas, R., Sugimori, M., Lin, J-W., and Cherksey, B.
tures. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 417,105-12. (1989): Blocking and isolation of a calcium channel
Hillman, D., Chen, S., Aung, T.T., Cherksey, B., from neurons in mammals and cephalopods utiliz-
Sugimori, M., and Llinas, R.R. (1991): Localization ing a toxin fraction (FTX) from funnel-web spider
of P-type calcium channels in the central nervous poison. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 86, 1689-
system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 88, 7076-7080. 1693.
Larramendi, L.M.H., and Victor, T. (1967): Synapses Llinas, R., Sugimori, M., and Walton, K. (1977):
on spines of the Purkinje cell of the mouse. An Calcium dendritic spikes in the mammalian Purkinje
electron microscopic study. Brain Res., 5, 15-30. cells. Soc. Neurosci. Abst., 3, 58.
Llano, I., Leresche, N., and Marty, A. (1991): Calcium Llinas, R.R., and Simpson, J.I. (1981): Cerebellar
entry increases the sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje control of movement. In A.L. Towe, and E.S. Luschei,
cells to applied GABA and decreases inhibitory (eds.): Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. New
synaptic currents. Neuron, 6, 565-574. York: Plenum Publishing Corporation, pp. 231-
Llinas, R. (1982): General discussion. Radial connecti- 302.
vity in the cerebellar cortex: A novel view regarding Miller, R.J. (1987): Multiple calcium channels and
the functional organization of the molecular layer. neuronal function. Science, 235, 46-52.
Exp. Brain Res. Suppl., 6, 189-194. Nahorski, S.R., Kendall, D.A., and Batty, I. (1986):
Llinas, R. (1988): The intrinsic electro physiological Receptors and phosphoinositide metabolism in the
properties of mammalian neurons: Insights into central nervous system. Biochem. Pharmacol., 35,
central nervous system function. Science, 242,1654- 2447-2453.
1664. Napper, R.M., and Harvey, R.I. (1988): Number of
Llinas, R., Bloedel, J.R., and Hillman, D.E. (1969): parallel fiber synapses on an individual Purkinje cell
Functional characterization of neuronal circuitry of in the cerebellum of the rat. J. Compo Neurol., 274,
frog cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 32, 847-870. 168-177.
Llinas, R., and Hess, R. (1976): Tetrodotoxin-resistant Nishizuka, Y. (1984): The role of protein kinase C in
dendritic spikes in avian Purkinje cells. Proc. Natl. cell surface signal transduction and tumour pro-
Acad. Sci., 73, 2520-2523. motion. Nature, 308, 693-698.
Llinas, R., and Hillman, D.E. (1969): Physiological and Nowycky, M.C, Fox, A.P., and Tsien, R.W. (1985):
morphological organization of the cerebellar circuits Three types of neuronal calcium channel with dif-
in various vertebrates. In Llinas, R. (ed.): Neuro- ferent calcium agonist sensitivity. Nature, 316, 440-
biology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development. 443.
Chicago: American Medical Association Education Oscarrson, 0., and Sjolund, B. (1977a): The ventral
and Research Foundation, pp. 43-73. spino-cerebellar system in the cat. I. Identification
Llinas, R., and Miihlethaler, M. (1988): Electrophysio- of five paths and their termination in the cerebellar
logical of guinea pig cerebellar nuclear cells in the anterior lobe. Exp. Brain Res., 28, 469-486.
in vitro, brainstem-cerebellar preparation. J. Physiol. Oscarrson, 0., and Sjolund, B. (1977b): The ventral
(Lond), 404, 241-258. spino-cerebellar system in the cat. III. Functional
Llinas, R., and Nicholson, C (1971): Electrophysio- characterization of the five paths. Exp. Brain Res.,
logical properties of dendrites and somata in alligator 28, 505-520.
Purkinje cells. J. Neurophysiol., 34, 532-551. Paley, S.L., and Chan-Paley, V. (1974): Cerebellar
8. Electrophysiology of the Purkinje Cell 181

Cortex, Cytology and Organization. New York: of intracellular calcium transients in Purkinje cells
Springer-Verlag. following glutamate iontophoresis. Soc. Neurosci.
Peters, A., Paley, S.L., and Webster, H.D.F. (1976): The Abst., 16, 894.
Fine Structure of the Nervous System. Philadelphia: Sugimori, M., and L1inas, R.R. (1990b): Real-time
WB Saunders. imaging of calcium influx in mammalian cerebellar
Ramon y Cajal, S. (1904): Histologie du Systeme Nerveux Purkinje cells in vitro. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci., 87,
de L'Homme et des Vertebrates. Madrid: Instituto 5084-5088.
Ramon y Cajal. Sugimori, M., Llinas, R., and Angelides, K. (1986):
Ross, CA., Meldolesi, J., Milner, T.A., Satoh, T., Fluorescence localization of tetrodotoxin receptors
Supattapone, S., and Snyder, S.H. (1989): Inositol in mammalian cerebellar cortex in vitro. Soc. N euro-
1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor localized to endoplas- Sci. Abst., 12, 463.
mic reticulum in cerebellar Purkinje neurons. Nature, Supattapone, S., Worley, P.F., Baraban, J.M., and
339,468-470. Snyder, S.H. (1988): Solubilization, purification, and
Ross, W.N., and Werman, R. (1987): Mapping calcium characterization of an inositol trisphosphate recep-
transients in the dendritis of Purkinje cells from the tor. J. Bioi. Chern., 263,1530-1534.
guinea-pig cerebellum in vitro. J. Physiol. (Lond.), Tank, D.W., Sugimori, M., Connor, J.A., and L1inas,
389,319-336. R.R. (1988): Spatially resolved calcium dynamics of
Shambes, G.M., Gibson, J.M., and Welker, W. (1978): mammalian Purkinje cells in cerebellar slice. Science,
Fractured somatopy in granule cell tactile areas of 242, 773-777.
rat cerebellar hemispheres revealed by micromap- Terzuolo, CA., and Araki, T. (1961): An analysis of
ping. Brain Behav. Evol., 15,94-140. intraverses extracellular potential changes asso-
Sharp, F.R., Gonzalez, M.F., Sharp, J.W., and Sagar, ciated with activity in single spinal motoneurons.
S.M. (1989): c-fos expression and (14C) 2-deoxy- Ann N. Y. Acad. Sci., 94, 547-558.
glucose uptake in the caudal cerebellum of the rat Tsien, R.Y., and Poeni, M. (1986): Fluorescence ratio
during motor/sensory cortex stimulation. J. Compo imaging: A new window into intracellular ionic
N eurol., 284, 621-636. signalling. TI BS, 11, 450-455.
Sugimori, M., and L1imis, R. (1985): Direct demonstra- Usowicz, M.M., Sugimori, M., and Llinas, R.R. (1991):
tion of dendritic inhibition in cerebellar purkinje Elementary properties of calcium channels in
cells in-vitro. Soc. Neurosci. Abst., 11,689. cerebellar Purkinje cells. Soc. Neurosci. Abst., 17,
Sugimori, M., and Llinas, R. (1990a): Fura-2 imaging 342.
9
V oltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels
in Purkinje Cells from
Organotypic Cerebellar Cultures
Isabel Llano, Beat H. Gahwiler, and Alain Marty

The Use of Advanced Biophysical dissociated adult neurons or with cultures of dis-
Techniques to Study the Membrane sociated neurons of neonatal or fetal origin.
Properties of Central Whereas the former approach yields a prepara-
tion of nerve cells devoid of any synaptic contacts
Nervous System Neurones with other neurons, nerve cells in dissociated cul-
The Technical Challenge tures usually suffer from insufficient cellular dif-
ferentiation and organotypic organization. As a
Classical membrane biophysics has been devel- result, individual neurons cannot be identified
oped in carefully selected preparations that bear easily in the living state on the basis of their
no direct relation to the mammalian central ner- characteristic cytoarchitecture or cellular loca-
vous system (CNS). This is easily understand- tion.
able. Tissue derived from mammalian CNS is One way to overcome some of these difficulties
not only difficult to maintain and manipulate, is to turn to slice preparations, which contain the
but it also contains nerve cells with intricate den- normal anatomical neuronal relationships of the
dritic processes that render the use of voltage- tissue of origin. Furthermore, many brain areas
clamp techniques hazardous. Furthermore, their are organized in a laminated pattern, so that
cellular surface is not easily accessible to experi- appropriately oriented slices will preserve many
mental manipulations. All these sources of diffi- ofthe synaptic connections. Recently, a procedure
culty have been recognized for a long time. As a has been described allowing to "clean" the sur-
result, new technical developments such as the face of neuronal cells in acute brain slices and
patch-clamp technique (Hamill et ai., 1981) have thus making them suitable for patch-clamping
received relatively few applications in the CNS, (Edwards, et ai., 1989). This new technical devel-
in contrast to the wealth of information gathered opment opens the way for detailed studies of
with this experimental approach in peripheral identified neuronal preparations without the use
tissues. Much ofthe CNS work has concentrated of culturing procedures.
on preparations such as the neuroendocrine cells We report here an alternative approach that
of the pituitary gland, which can be isolated and involves cultivation of slices derived from young
studied in vitro relatively easily. These cells have animals. Despite thinning down to monolayer
a simple shape that is suited to voltage-clamp thickness, organotypic slice cultures (Giihwiler,
studies and they can be identified before recording 1981 a, 1984) retain the basic structural and con-
by using the noninvasive hemolytic plaque assay nective organization of the tissue of origin. Easy
(e.g. DeRiemer and Sakmann, 1986). access to nerve cells not only facilitates the use of
standard electro physiological techniques, but also
The Search for a Suitable CNS Preparation makes these cultures amenable to patch-clamp
Up to this date, most patch-clamp studies of CNS methods without the necessity of cleaning the
tissue have been carried out either with acutely surface of the tissue. In the following sections we
182
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 183

describe the morphological and physiological The overall cytoarchitecture of cerebellar tissue
properties of the preparation that has been used with cortical and deep cerebellar nuclei can still
in our studies, "organotypic" cultures of rat cere- be recognized after several weeks in vitro. Because
bellum, and compare the functional results ob- of continued neuronal migration, Purkinje cells
tained with those that have been gathered using are no longer aligned in single rows as in situ, but
similar electrophysiological approaches in other rather clustered in folialike structures. In contrast,
preparations of mammalian cerebellum. deep cerebellar neurones form distinct nuclei that
are located near the base ofthe cerebellar cultures.
Living Purkinje cells can be identified by their
Cerebellar Organotypic Cultures large size, their characteristic dark cytoplasm
(viewed with phase contrast microscopy), and
Preparation of Cultures their relative location within the slices. Recently,
In the present studies, cerebellar cultures were a more stringent identification of Purkinje cells
prepared from newborn (0 to I-day-old) rats. has been carried out using antibodies to the 28-
Early postnatal age is a good compromise since KDa Ca-binding protein, calbindin. Figure 9.lA
at least part of the cytoarchitecture is already illustrates the distribution of calbindin-positive
present and a high degree of neuronal survival Purkinje cells in cerebellar cultures. Whereas
can be achieved. Cerebella are dissected ascepti- some calbindin-positive Purkinje cells were ap-
cally and quickly cut with a McIlwain chopper parently scattered within the slices, others formed
into 400-.um thick sagittal slices. The slices are organized folialike structures. Purkinje cell axons,
placed individually on glass coverslips and em- again identified by calbindin immunohisto-
bedded in a plasma clot formed by chicken plasma chemistry, are observed to myelinate during the
and thrombin. To optimize aeration and feeding second week in vitro. They grow toward and ter-
of the cultures, the coverslips bearing the cultures minate within the deep cerebellar nuclei (Fig. 9.1A).
are inserted into plastic test tubes to which 0.5 ml Interestingly, the shape of dendritic arborizations
of medium are added, placed in a roller drum that greatly depends on the relative location of Purkinje
is kept in a dry incubator at 36°C, and slowly cells within the slices. Whereas Purkinje cells that
rotated at 10 revolutions per hour. Cultures are have migrated away from the laminae display
fed at weekly intervals with a medium consisting relatively short, randomly oriented multipolar
of horse serum (25%), basal medium (Eagle, 50%) dendrites, Purkinje cells within laminae have
and Hanks' balanced salt solution (25%), sup- dendrites that are clearly oriented within the
plemented with glucose to a final concentration molecular layer (Fig. 9.1 B). All primary and
of 6.5 gil. To reduce the number of nonneuronal secondary dendrites are densely covered by spiny
cells, the fresh ex plants are subjected to moderate processes.
doses of ionizing irradiation (x ray, 250 kV, 600- It is evident that the dendritic trees of Purkinje
1200 rads), and the cultures are treated during cells never achieve the full arborization reminis-
one day with the antimitotic agent cytosine cent of their adult in situ conterparts, but resem-
arabinoside. The slices are used for electrophysio- ble, as far as multipolar dendrites are concerned,
logical experiments within 2 to 5 weeks after those observed in the experimentally, degranu-
explanations. lated cerebellum (Altman and Anderson et aI.,
1972; Crepel et aI., 1980; Herndon et aI., 1971;
Sotelo, 1975). Well oriented Purkinje cells located
Cellular and Tissue Differentiation
within laminae have dendritic arbors resembling
After about 10 to 15 days in vitro, the majority those of Purkinje cells in approximately 2- to
of cells and fibers damaged during the slicing 3-week-old rats (Hendelman and Aggerwal, 1980).
procedure have degenerated; as a result, the cere- The reasons for the failure of Purkinje cells to
bellar cultures have flattened from the original grow a fully arborized dendritic tree are not com-
400.um into a monolayer preparation. This pletely understood, but it is likely that the out-
spreading greatly facilitates visualization of indi- of-phase course of parallel fibers in these two-
vidual living neurones with contrast-enhancing dimensional cultures plays a decisive role (Blank
optics. and Seil, 1982).
184 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

Figure 9.1. A: Center of a cerebellar culture after the culture presented in A, viewed with interference
30 days in vitro. The culture has been stained with contrast optics after staining with antibodies against
aI)tibodies raised against the 28 KDa Ca-binding calbindin. Bar = 500/lm for A and 50/lm for B.
protein calbindin. B: Laminae of Purkinje cells from
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 185

In newborn rats, only Purkinje cells with small logical studies using extracellular (Gahwiler,
filapodia and a short axon can be visualized 1978) and intracellular (Mariani et aI., 1991)
(Gahwiler, 1981 b). The intricate dendritic and recording techniques have clearly shown that
axonal arborizations of Purkinje cells in cerebel- olivary stimulation results in Purkinje cells in the
lar slice cultures derived from newborn rats clearly generation of complex responses that closely
demonstrate that maturation continues in the resemble the characteristic climbing fiber re-
in vitro environment after the tissue has been sponses as observed in situ and in acute cere-
isolated. The electrophysiological data to be dis- bellar slices.
cussed in the next section will show that the same The co culture approach offers not only unique
holds true for the functional properties of cultured opportunities for studying the functional pro-
Purkinje cells. The observation of morphological perties of cerebellar afferents, but may also allow
and physiological maturation in vitro is particu- characterization of the transmitter used by climb-
larly important in view of recent reports sug- ing fibers. Moreover, such a preparation may be
gesting that differentiation may stop at the time instrumental in elucidating mechanisms involved
of explantation if the tissue is to be derived from in target search and recognition and may provide
older animals. In those cultures (Jaeger et aI., important information about the role played by
1988; Kapoor et aI., 1988), survival of cultured trophic substances in the establishment of syn-
cerebellar neurones from 8 to II-day-old rats aptic connections.
apparently did not differ appreciably from those
taken at earlier stages. Differentiation and matu-
ration ofneurones, however, varied considerably. Ionic Conductances ofPurkinje Cells
These studies (Jaeger et aI., 1988) in fact showed
that only maturation attained in vivo could be Voltage-Gated Conductances
maintained in the in vitro environment, and that of Purkinje Cell Somata
little morphological or physiological maturation
Intracellular recordings from Purkinje cells in
occurred after the tissue had been isolated. It will
cerebellar mammalian slices (Llimls and Sugimori,
have to be determined whether these results are
1980a) have yielded a detailed description of the
solely due to the age of the donor animals or to
various firing patterns that characterize the elec-
the particular culture conditions used.
troresponsiveness of these neurones and have
suggested the existence of several membrane con-
Cerebellar-Olivary Cocultures
ductances as underlying their electrical behavior.
In cultured slices, afferent fibers are severed during Specifically, these studies have demonstrated that
the slicing procedure, rapidly degenerate, and the Na conductance plays a role not only in the
can therefore no longer be stimulated. Cocultures generation of the fast potentials, but also in the
derived from anatomically remote brain areas generation of prolonged or "plateau" potentials,
provide an interesting alternative approach for which are prominent after the blockade of voltage-
studying functional properties of neuronal pro- dependent Ca conductances. The effects of intra-
jections in an in vitro preparation. To achieve cellular injections of the K channel blocker tetra-
this goal, two slices are embedded side by side on thylammonium (TEA) on the level of the Na-
a glass coverslip at a distance of about 1 mm dependent plateau depolarization as well as on
and then cultured for several weeks. During this the duration of action potentials have further
period, fibers originating in cells of the origin of indicated that K conductances contribute to
the projection grow toward and into the neigh- shaping the electrical signals. Finally, the com-
boring slice and form functional synaptic connec- parison of somatic recordings with direct den-
tions with the target neurones. dritic intracellular recordings (Llimls and Sugimori,
We have made use of this approach to establish 1980b) has led to the suggestion that most of the
an in vitro analog of the olivocerebellar projec- voltage-dependent Ca conductance of Purkinje
tion. Within the first 2 weeks in vitro, olivary cells is localized at the dendritic arborization of
fibers bridge the gap separating the two explants these cells.
and grow into the adjacent cerebellar slice to As in explant cultures (Gruol, 1983; Gruol and
innervate cerebellar Purkinje cells. Electrophysio- Franklin, 1987) and in dissociated cerebellar cul-
186 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

10 mV I

100 ms

B c

12.5 nA
20 ms
10 ms

Figure 9.2. A: Recording of the membrane potential 7.2). B: Whole-cell membrane currents recorded under
obtained under whole-cell current-clamp conditions voltage-clamp conditions in response to a depolarizing
from a Purkinje cell perfused with KCl saline (con- step to - 20 m V. Holding potential: - 70 m V. The cell
taining, in mM: 130 KCl, lOEGTA-K, 10 Hepes-K; was perfused with KCl saline. The record has not been
pH: 7.2). The cell resting membrane potential was - 56 corrected for either leak current or capacitive currents.
mY: spontaneous synaptic potentials as well as over- C: Whole-cell membrane currents elicited by a depolari-
shooting actions potentials are observed. The horizontal zation to - 20 m V from a holding potential of - 90 m V,
line marks the OmV membrane potential level. Unless in a cell perfused with CsCI (in mM: 120 CsCl, 10 Cs-
otherwise noted, data shown in this and subsequent EGTA, 10 Cs-Hepes; pH: 7.2). The external solution
figures have been obtained from slices bathed in an contained 500 nM tetrodotoxin (TTX) and 2 mM tetra-
external saline containing (in mM): 140 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, ethylammonium (TEA). The record has been corrected
1 CaCI 2 , 1 MgCI 2 , 10 Na-Hepes, and 5.5 glucose (pH: for leak current and capacitive transients.

tures (Hirano and Ohmori, 1986) prepared from replacement of K + with Cs + in the recording
fetal rat cerebellum, Purkinje cells in organotypic pipette, several nA of inward currents can be
cultures exhibit in current-clamp whole-cell re- obtained upon step depolarizations; these cur-
cording conditions spontaneous synaptic poten- rents present a transient as well as a sustained
tials and overshooting action potentials (Fig. 9.2A), component (Fig. 9.2C). These three types of cur-
indicating that they have developed in vitro a rents can be ascribed to the activation of Na, K,
set of membrane conductances. When examined and Ca conductances, respectively.
under voltage-clamp, depolarizing steps to K + However, the use of the whole-cell recording
perfused neurones elicit large inward currents technique for the characterization of voltage-
with fast activation and inactivation time course, dependent conductances is hindered in Purkinje
which are followed by prominent outward cur- neurones by the complicated morphology of these
rents (Fig. 9.2B). When the Na and K conduc- cells, which renders it impossible to achieve a
tances are blocked by the addition of tetrodotoxin spatially homogeneous control of membrane
(TTX) and TEA to the external solution, and by potential. In fact, lack of control of the membrane
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 187

potential was evident in whole-cell recordings attached and cell-free membrane patches that can
from various anomalies in the current-voltage be obtained using patch-damp techniques offer
relations and in the time course of the voltage- alternative approaches. Owing to its noninvasive
activated currents. Thus, the identification of the nature, the cell-attached recording configuration
specific ionic channels that underlie the voltage- can be especially useful for an initial screening
dependent conductance changes and the descrip- of the various types of membrane conductances
tion of their biophysical properties must be carried present, whereas the cell-free configurations, which
out using other experimental techniques. The cell- allow a strict control of membrane potential and

Test pulse
(mV)
30

50

70

Figure 9.3. Cell-attached record-


ings from the soma of Purkinje cells.
Each set of traces corresponds to
single sweeps obtained on voltage
steps, the amplitude of which is
indicated at the left. The pipette
holding potential was set at 30 m V,
which brings the resting patch poten-
tial to approximately -90mV. The
recording pipette was filled with the
standard ex ternal saline. In this and
other figures concerning voltage-
activated currents, individual re-
cords have been corrected for leak
and capacitive transients. Filter
cut-off frequency (fc): 3.5 kHz. In
this and subsequent figures upward
arrows mark the onset of the voltage
pulse, downward arrows the return
to holding potential.
188 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

of the solutions to which the patch of membrane time course and are followed, during depolarizing
is 'exposed, are best suited to carry out a formal steps of 70 m V or higher, by outward current
description of these conductances. In the following fluctuations that last throughout the duration
sections we summarize the properties of the of the pulse. The addition of 5 mM TEA to the
somatic voltage-gated conductances of Purkinje recording pipette eliminates the outward compo-
cells that we have observed using the cell-attached nent of the current (indicating that it is due to the
and the outside-out configurations of the patch- activation of K conductances), and allows us to
clamp technique. study the inward current fluctuations. As illustra-
ted in Figure 9.4A for a depolarizing step of 50 m V,
Cell-Attached Recordings the voltage-dependent inward currents activate
rapidly upon depolarization (the time to peak of
In cell-attached recordings of Purkinje cells ob- the averaged current, last trace, being 0.65 ms)
tained with pipettes filled with a standard external and partially inactivate during the ensuing 10 ms.
saline, depolarizing voltage steps reveal inward Inward currents are observed for depolarizing
current fluctuations that are rare for depolariza- pulses higher than 30mV, the maximum current
tions of 30 mV and become prominent at higher being obtained for pulses of70mV. Under these
membrane potentials (Fig. ,}.3). These current fluc- recording conditions, all cell-attached patches
tuations are characterized by a fast activation tested displayed inward currents with similar

Figure 9.4. A: Cell-attached recordings


from Purkinje cell somata, obtained with
a pipette filled with standard external
saline to which 5 mM TEA had been
added. The three upper traces correspond
to single sweeps ofNa current fluctuations
elicited by 20-ms depolarizations of 50 mV;
the last trace shows the average current

B ~5PA calculated from 16 pulses. The patch


membrane potential was hyperpolarized
5 ms by 30 mV relative to its resting potential.
.fc = 3.5 kHz. B: Cell-attached recordings
from Purkinje cell soma, obtained with
a pipette containing 55 mM BaCl 2, 85 mM
TEA-Cl, 10mM TEA-Hepes (pH: 7.2).
The three upper traces correspond to
single sweeps elicited by 20-ms depolar-
izations of 50 m V; the last trace shows the
average current calculated from 20 pulses.
The patch membrane potential was hyper-
polarized by 30 m V relative to its resting
potential. fc = 3.5 kHz.
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 189

kinetics and voltage dependence. As will be dis- at the dendritic arborization, as is the case for
cussed below, these properties correspond well Purkinje cells in acute slices of adult mammalian
to those that we have described for the voltage- cerebellum (Llimls and Sugimori, 1980b). Our
dependent Na channels in this preparation using results contrast clearly with those of Hirano and
outside-out patches from Purkinje cell somata Hagiwara, who find that, although a number of
(Gahwiler and Llano, 1989). somatic patches had little Ca channel activity, a
Hirano and Hagiwara (1989) have reported third of the patches displayed Ca currents with a
that in Purkinje cells from dissociated cultures of peak current of 50 pA or more. Also in contrast
fetal rat cerebellum, two types of Ca channels with our results, these authors report that many
(steady and transient) are present in Purkinje cell patches did not show any Na channel activity. In
somata. In contrast, we find that the somata of our studies, Na channel activity has been systema-
Purkinje cells in organotypic cultures do not tically observed on both the cell-attached and the
present any channel activity that corresponds to outside-out configuration (see below), provided
that expected of voltage-gated Ca channels. Thus, that the membrane holding potential is held suf-
in--conditions that optimize the recording of Ca ficiently hyperpolarized to prevent inactivation.
channel activity (Ba- and TEA-containing solu- These results presumably have to be ascribed to
tions in the recording pipette, Fig. 9.4B), there the fact that culture conditions can determine the
was no measurable channel activity for depolar- functional expression and/or distribution of specific
izing pulses ranging from 30 to 120 mV in any of channel types within a neuronal cell. In fact, stu-
the patches studied. These results indicate that, dies carried out on dissociated cerebellar cultures
in organotypic cultures, most of the voltage- from new-born rat cerebellum have shown that
dependent Ca conductance, which is evident from the electro responsiveness of Purkinje cells in cul-
the capacity of these cells to produce Ca action ture depends drastically on the type of culture
potentials, and voltage-activated Ca currents in medium used (Bossu et aI., 1989).
whole-cell recording conditions, must be located

Figure 9.5. A: Na currents recorded


from an outside-out patch in response
to 300-ms step depolarizations
to -40mV. Holding potential:
-90mV. The two upper traces cor- ____--J110 pA
respond to single sweeps, the lowest 50 ms
trace to the average current calcu-
lated from 21 depolarizations, applied I r '

at intervals of 4 s. The peak of the


mean current is 24.2 pA and the
sustained mean current is 0.4 pA. fc =
2.5 kHz. The external bath was sup- B
plemented with 1 mM TEA, and the
pipette solution contained, in mM:
120CsF, lOCsCI, 1 NaCI, 10 Cs-
EGTA, and lO Cs-Hepes. B: Bursts
of single-channel activity selected
from the final 200 ms of six 300-ms
step depolarizations to -40mV,
from the same patch as illustrated in
A. Holding potential: - 90 mV, fc =
2.5 kHz. (Adapted with permission
from Giihwiler and Llano 1989.) 5 ms
190 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

Outside-Out Patches current value of 2 pA and a mean open time of


0.37 ms for depolarizations to -40mV. The sus-
tained bursts of channel openings can be observed
The N a Channels only in membrane patches that have peak inward
The studies of Llimls and Sugimori (1980a) have currents of relatively large amplitudes (greater
shown that Purkinje cells exposed to Co 2 + gen- than 10 pA at - 40 mV), and in which the detec-
erate both fast Na-dependent spikes and a slow tion of single-channel events during the early
depolarization, which tends to maintain the phase of the current is accordingly not possible.
membrane potential near - 20 mV. These plateau We have thus used fluctuation analysis to estimate
potentials are sensitive to TTX and are absent the single-channel current and the number of
when extracellular Na + ions are removed from active channels in those patches that had "macro-
the external solution. These observations have scopic" Na currents. The result of such analysis
been interpreted by the authors as indicating the has provided estimates of the elementary current
existence of two distinct Na conductances: a fast that are very close to those obtained for the
Na conductance and a slow, non-inactivating channels active in the late phase ofthe Na current.
Na conductance. Our studies have led us to pro- In membrane patches containing a low number
pose that a single type ofNa channel can underlie of channels, individuals channel openings can be
these two types of electrical phenomena. clearly discerned during the early component of
The first indication to suggest the latter inter- the current. These channels open with very brief
pretation comes from the observation that the latency upon depolarizing steps (Fig. 9.6A) and
inward current elicited by long depolarizing steps have an elementary current amplitude at - 40 mV
in outside-out patches obtained with CsF-filled of 2 pA (Fig. 9.6B), which is in accord with both
pipettes displays two distinct components. As the estimates from fluctuation analysis in patches
illustrated in Figure 9.5A for a depolarization to containing many channels and with the analysis
- 40 m V, the initial response to the change in of single-channel parameters of the channels res-
membrane potential is characterized by a surge of ponsible for the late bursts of channel openings.
inward current fluctuations, which reach their Also in accord with the latter are the measures of
peak amplitude within 0.6 ms and subsequently mean open time for the early channels (0.3-0.5 ms,
decline. After this initial surge of current, bursts depending on the patch studied).
of single-channel openings interspersed with The comparison of single-channel properties
periods of silence are observed throughout the during both phases of the Na current indicates
duration of the depolarizing step. This type of that a single type ofNa channel is responsible for
channel activity gives. rise to a macroscopic cur- the totality of the Na current observed upon
rent that presents a maintained component (see sustained depolarization. Can this channel ac-
Fig. 9.5A, lowest trace). Since both early and late count for the plateau depolarizations reported by
components of the inward current are blocked by Llinas and Sugimori? The main requirement for
the specific channel blocker TTX, they can be this is that the channels present a voltage depend-
identified as being the result of the activity of Na ence such that they would give rise to a sustained
channels. activity for a membrane potential near - 20 m V.
The question as to whether both components In fact, the sustained component is clearly voltage
of the Na current are due to one single type of dependent; it is observed only for membrane
Na channel or whether two distinct channel types potentials between - 60 and - 20 mV, its peak
need to be invoked was answered by comparing amplitude being reached at -40mV. The exis-
the elementary properties of the channels that tence of this component is reflected by a remark-
underlie both phases. As is seen in Figure 9.5B, able feature of the steady-state properties of the
wh'ere one of the bursts of channel openings is macroscopic Na current: there is a significant
shown at an expanded time scale, single-channel overlap between the activation and inactivation
events can be clearly detected during the bursts curves between - 60 and - 40 m V, which implies
of activity. The analysis of their amplitude and that the Na channels do not inactivate completely
open time distributions yields a single-channel in this voltage range. This type of behavior has
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 191

A B
120
t
-
I/J
. 5 80
0
a..
as
1;j
c 40

o 1 2 3 4
Amplitude (pA)

~2PA
0.5 <'
5 ms
-
a.
CD
"0
1.5 :E
a.
• E
• • • c:(

2.5
Figure 9.6. A: Single Na channel events activated by gaussian distribution. The value of single-channel cur-
20-ms steps to -40mV. Holding potential: -90mV. rent obtained from the fit is - 2.06 pA (standard devi-
The lowest trace corresponds to the averaged current ation: 0.27). Events near 4 pA correspond to double
calculated from all the sweeps that had active channels openings. Lower panel: Single-channel current-voltage
within a set of 25 depolarizations. fc = 2.5 kHz. External relation for the patch shown in A. Elementary current
and internal solutions were the same as in Fig. 9.5. values were obtained from fits of the elementary current
B: Upper panel: Histogram of elementary current ampli- histograms to a gaussian distribution, as shown in the
tudes for the patch shown in A. The solid line corre- upper panel. (Reprinted with permission from Giihwiler
sponds to the best fit of the data values to a single and Llano, 1989.)

also been reported for the rat brain sodium chan- Purkinje cell soma. In somatic patches from
nel type II cDNA clone expressed in oocytes Purkinje cells in organotypic cultures, the K
(Stiihmer et aI., 1987). As will be described below, conductance is quite prominent, giving rise to
the K conductance of Purkinje cell somata has outward currents as large as 100 pA (for depolar-
an activation threshold of - 20 mV. The combined izations to 20 m V) in some of the patches tested.
effect of the maintained Na current and of the Figure 9.7 presents an example ofthe macroscopic
absence of K current in the voltage range com- K currents recorded from an outside-out patch
prised between - 60 and - 30 m V will be to clamp with a pipette filled with KCl. The K conductance
the membrane potential near -30 or -20mV, activates in a voltage-dependent manner the time
as observed by Llinas and Sugimori (1980a). to peak of the current decreasing from 4 to 1.4 ms
between - 20 m V (the activation threshold for
the outward current) and 50 m V. The current
The K Channels
dacays during the pulse, reaching values of 0.2 to
Under physiological conditions, the interplay 0.1 of its peak amplitude, with time constants of
between the Na channels and the K channels the order of6 ms, the value of which is not voltage
should determine the firing properties of the dependent. The macroscopic K currents are quite
192 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

+50 mY

+30 mY
Figure 9.7. K currents obtained from
an outside-out patch maintained at
a holding potential of - 90 mV. The
o mY outward current amplitude increases
as the membrane is stepped to test
potentials of - 40, - 20, 0, 30, and
-20 mY SOmV, as indicated to the right of
each trace. Each trace is the average
of 10 depolarizing steps of 40 ms
duration. fc = I.S kHz. The solid lines
J -40 mY
on each record correspond to single
exponential fits of the decay phase of
~25 pA the current. The external bath con-
tained 200 nM TTX and the pipette
10 ms KCI.

variable from patch to patch, especially with res- the K conductance. Single-channel recordings
pect to their time course, the extent of the current corresponding to the "transient" K conductance
decay during depolarizing pulses, and their sensi- increase have not been reported.
tivity to the value of the holding potential. We have identified, using single-channel re-
The variability of the macroscopic features of cordings, two types of K channels that differ in
the K current arises most likely from the existence their elementary conductance and their kinetic
of various types of K channels, with a contribu- properties (Giihwiler and Llano, 1989). Activity
tion to the total current that varies from patch corresponding to these channels is illustrated in
to patch. Whole-cell recordings (Bossu et aI., Figure 9.8A, which corresponds to a steady-state
1988) and cell-attached recordings (Hirano and depolarization to 30 m V. Two discrete current
Hagiwara, 1989) of the macroscopic K currents levels can be discerned, with elementary currents
of cultured Purkinje cells have already shown of 2.6 pA and 10.4 pA, respectively (Fig. 9.8B).
that the K conductance ofthese neurones has two Both ofthese K channels are blocked by low con-
components, a rapidly inactivating, "transient," centrations of TEA (2 mM). The channel with
component and a "sustained" component. The lower elementary current presents a linear single-
features of the "transient" component, especially channel current-voltage relation (elementary
its sensitivity to the holding potential (Bossu et aI., chord conductance of 28 pS), activates with brief
1988; Hirano and Hagiwara, 1989) and to 4- voltage-dependent delays upon depolarizing steps,
aminopyridine (Bossu et aI., 1988), have led both and is subjected to a slow inactivation process.
groups to consider this "transient" component of Its properties are similar to those of the delayed
the K conductance as an A current. At the single- rectifier K channel, which has been described for
channel level, Bossu et al. have identified a non- skeletal muscle (Standen et aI., 1985) and for the
inactivating, TEA-sensitive K channel with a 22 giant squid axon (Llano et aI., 1988b). This channel,
pS elementary conductance as the main channel which is probably identical to the 22 pS channel
type contributing to the maintained portion of of Bossu and collaborators, contributes a large
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 193

B ~10PA
20 ms
60

U)

'E
Q)
Figure 9.8. A: Selected portions of K single- ~
channel events recorded from an outside-out
patch during a steady-state depolarization to
30 m V. fc = 1.5 kHz. Same external and internal
solutions as in Fig. 9.7. B: Histogram of event 30
amplitudes for the single-channel events recorded
during a maintained depolarization to 30 mV.
Only single step transitions were included in the
histogram. The data were fitted by the sum of
two gaussian distributions, with single-channel

o +,L..D...r.L_:a...c~AL:~';1.
current amplitudes of 2.66 pA (standard devi-
ation: 0.20) and 10.38 pA (standard deviation: .. ,
104), respectively. (Reprinted with permission from 1
Giihwiler and Llano, 1989.)
o 8
Amplitude (pA)
16

fraction of the macroscopic K current recorded Transmitter-Activated Conductances


upon step depolarizations in Purkinje cell somata.
The large conductance (90 pS) K channel is A formal identification of the neurotransmitter
most likely the same as the 70 to 100 pS channel mediating the synapse between climbing fibers and
reported for Purkinje cells in explant cultures Purkinje cells is still lacking; however, the situation
(Yool et aI., 1988). This channel probably cor- is clearer for the input arriving to Purkinje cells
responds to a Ca-dependent K channel, called from both the parallel fibers of the granule cells
BK channel, as described in various neuronal and and the axons of basket and stellate cells. Several
nonneuronal preparations (reviewed by Latorre lines of evidence suggest that glutamate mediates
et aI., 1989). The existence of Ca-dependent K the excitatory action of granule cells on Purkinje
channels in Purkinje cells has already been sug- cells, whereas gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA)
gested by intracellular recordings, which show a mediates the inhibitory action of basket and stel-
prominent after hyperpolarization following Ca late cells on Purkinje cells. Our studies have led
spikes (Uimis and Sugimori, 1980a). us to describe some properties ofthe specific ionic
194 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

channels that are activated by glutamatergic tergic agonists was observed systematically in all
agonists and by GABA in Purkinje cells, and have Purkinje cells that we have studied, even though
provided further support for the spatial segrega- the experimental conditions (absence of extracel-
tion of functional receptors on these cells (Llano lular Mg2 + and presence of glycine in concentra-
et aI., 1988a). tions which have been shown to potentiate the
NMDA response; Johnson and Ascher, 1987) were
Differential Sensitivity of Purkinje Cells designed to optimize the activation of NMDA
receptors. These results are in accord with pre-
to Neuroactive Amino Acids
vious studies that have shown that adult Purkinje
Figure 9.9A presents whole-cell recordings of the cells in acute slices have a low sensitivity ofNMDA
responses of a Purkinje cell to local applications (Crepel et aI., 1982). It must be pointed out that
of GAB A and of the three main types of glutama- under similar experimental conditions, pyramidal
tergic agonists, N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), cells from organotypic hippocampal cultures res-
quisqualate, and kainate. The cell has been bathed pond with large conductance increases to NMDA
in Mg2 + -free external saline to prevent the voltage- (Llano et aI., 1988a). We thus consider that the
dependent block of NMDA channels by Mg2 + insensitivity of Purkinje cells to NMDA does not
ions (Nowak etaI., 1984); TTX and Cd2+ were reflect nonspecific effects of culture conditions on
added in order to inhibit synaptic transmission the development and/or maintenance ofNMDA
and decrease the possibility of synaptic inter- receptors, but rather an intrinsic property of
actions. Under these conditions, the Purkinje Purkinje cells, which is expressed in organotypic
cell responds with large inward currents to the cultures as it is in vivo.
two non-NMDA agonists, but fails to respond to The response to GABA in this particular cell
NMDA. This differential sensitivity to glutama- is relatively small (Fig. 9.9A), most likely due

A
100llM NMDA 10 IJM Kai 2pM Quis 5 IJM GABA
10 pM Gly

1nA

B
2IJM GABA 2).lM GABA 2 IJM GABA
25fJM Bic
r, i Iflr i r
"J 11'

Figure 9.9. A: Whole-cell current responses to gluta- et aI., 1988a.) B: Whole-cell current responses of a
matergic agonists and GABA. Holding potential, - 60 Purkinje cell to short applications of GABA in control
m V. Agonists were applied through a fast perfusion solution (left trace), in the presence ofbicuculline metho-
system during the time indicated by the bars above chloride (center trace), and after washing ofbicuculline
each record. The slice was bathed in Mg2 + -free saline, (right trace). Holding potential, - 60 m V. The external
which was supplemented with 200 nM TTX and 250 J1M bath contained 200 nM TTX. Internal solution was
Cd 2 +. Internal solution, KCI. (Adapted from Liane KCI.
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 195

to the f~ct that external Cd2+ partially inhibits region of the cell that was not under accurate
GABA receptors. In the absence of Cd 2+ , 2 pM potential control. In accord with this possibility,
GABA elicits large inward currents, which can be comparisons of the responses to GABA and to
reversibly decreased by the GABA-A receptor glutamatergic agonists in the whole-cell and the
antagonist bicuculline (Fig. 9.9B). outside-out recording configuration have indi-
In the course of characterizing the whole-cell cated that Purkinje cell somata have a large den-
responses to neuroactive substances, it was system- sity GABA-ergic channels but a very low density
atically observed that although GABA responses of glutamatergic channels, the large whole-cell
reversed at the membrane potential expected from currents observed in response to excitatory amino
the equilibrium potential of Cl- ions (the charge acids being most likely of dendritic origin (Llano
carrier through the GABA channel under our et aI., 1988a). These results are of interest since
recording conditions), the responses to both q uis- they demonstrate that Purkinje cells in organo-
qualate and kainate reversed at membrane poten- typic cultures attain the spatial segregation of
tials 20 to 40 m V more positive than that expected functional amino acid receptors, which is expected
for cation-selective channels (0 m V). These obser- of their adult in vivo counterparts from binding
vations suggested that the whole-cell currents for assays and electrophysiological studies (e.g., Chan-
the glutamatergic agonists were arising from a Palay, 1978; Crepel et aI., 1982).

10 11M Quis

50

.,'"
E
.,>
......
o
ci
Z

25

0~--~~--------,- ______3 -____- ,


o 1.25 2.5

2 PAL 10 ms
Amplitude (pA)

Figure 9.10: Single-channel activity elicited by quis- solution, CsC!. Right panel: Corresponds to the dis-
qualate in a somatic outside-out patch held at a mem- tribution of elementary currents for the ensemble of all
brane potential of - 90 m V. The traces have been selec- openings elicited during a long agonist application in
ted to illustrate the various current levels that are found the same patch. The data were fitted by the sum of
upon agonist application. fc = 1.5 kHz. The bath con- three gaussian distributions (continuous curve) with
tained Mg2 + -free saline, with 200 nM TTX; pipette single-channel current values of 0.86, 1.37, and 1.65 pA.
196 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

Properties of Amino Acid-Activated The latter hypothesis has been put forward by
Channels Jahr and Stevens (1987) and by Cull-Candy and
Usowicz (1987), based on their observations that
Glutamatergic Channels in outside-out patches obtained from dissociated
On the basis of their differential sensitivity to hippocampal neurones and from cultured cere-
pharmacological agents, three types of glutamate bellar neurones, NMDA and non-NMDA agonists
receptors have been described for the vertebrate produce channel openings to the same subcon-
nervous system (see Mayer and Westbrook, 1987 ductance states and that transitions between the
for review). These receptors are preferentially acti- various states occur. In contrast to this view,
vated by the glutamatergic agonists NMDA, quis- Ascher and Nowak (1988) advocate that the ability
qualate, and kainate, respectively. Whether the of non-NMDA agonists to elicit NMDA-like
different agonists act on individual receptor-ionic openings arises from a weak agonist action of
channel complexes or whether one single type of these compounds on NMDA receptors and favor
ionic channel coupled to three different receptor the hypothesis according to which NMDA and
subtypes mediates all the glutamatergic actions non-NMDA agonists activate distinct receptor-
has been the matter of considerable debate. ionic channel complexes. Because Purkinje cells

50 J.lM Kai

100 ms

1
....>.
.....
til
t::
Q)
q
-....
t':S
1-4
0
Q)
0.1
Figure 9.11. Current fluctua-
tions recorded from a somatic
p.. outside-out patch upon the
en application of kainate, starting
0.01
about 70 ms after the beginning
of the trace. Holding potential,
- 60 m V. External and internal
solutions as in Fig. 9.10. Lower
0.001 panel: The power spectrum
for the kainate-induced current
noise was fitted with a single
Lorentzian function (solid line),
I I I I II I I I '" I I I I I I II
which yielded a time constant of
10 100 1000 1.04 ms. (Adapted from Llano
Frequency (Hz) et ai., 1988a.)
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 197

are insensitive to NMDA, they provide an oppor- cation of quisqualate to an outside-out patch
tunity to test the responses to non-NMD A agonists from the soma of a Purkinje cell. The amplitude
in a preparation that is devoid of NMDA recep- distribution of the quisqualate-activated events
tors. Figure 9.10 presents typical examples ofthe shows three peaks at current values of 0.9,
single-channel events that are activated by appli- 1.4, and 1.7 pA, corresponding to elementary

A 10 pM GABA

J 0.55
50pA

B
21lMGABA

~2PA
20 ms

3.0 1.5 o
Amplitude (PA)

Figure 9.12. A: Response of a somatic outside-out patch of the current events elicited by GABA in another
to the application of GAB A, during the time indicated outside-out patch. Holding potential: -80mV. The
by the bar above the record. The membrane potential right panel presents the distribution of elementary cur-
was maintained at - 60 m V.Ie = 1.5 kHz. External and rent amplitudes for the GABA-activated channels in
internal solutions as in the two previous figures. The this patch. The solid line corresponds to the fit of the
solid line corresponds to the fit of the decay phase of data to a single gaussian function, with a single-channel
the current to a double exponential function plus a current of - 1.6 pA. Bins of amplitude lower than
baseline component. Fit parameters: 446 ms, - 57 pA; -1.1 pA were excluded from the fit.
80 ms, - 51 pA; baseline: - 4.5 pA. B: Selected portions
198 Isabel Liano, Beat H. Gahwiler, and Alain Marty

conductances of 9.5, 15, and 18 pS, respectively. In a few outside-out patches that happened to
Absent from such records were the large conduct- have a small number of active channels, an analy-
ance (50 pS) events that characterize NMDA- sis of single-channel events was carried out. An
induced openings. example of the channel activity induced by 2 JlM
NMDA-like openings were also absent upon GABA is presented in Figure 9.12B. The most
application of kainate in all outside-out patches frequent conductance state ofthese channels cor-
studied, an example of which is shown in Figure responds to 28 pS (see histogram fit), but openings
9.11. The spectrum of the current fluctuations to a level of 20 pS are observed as well. Multiple
induced by kainate is described by a single Lorent- conductance states are characteristic of GABA
zian with a time constant of 1 ms (Fig. 9.11, lower channels, and have been studied in detail in mouse
panel). Analysis of the relation between the mean spinal cord neurones in culture (Bormann et aI.,
current and the variance yields a value of 2.5 pS 1987). The most frequent conductance state in
for the elementary conductance. those neurones is 30 pS, a value close to the 28 pS
The results show that responses to non-NMDA level we report for Purkinje cells.
agonists differ in Purkinje cells, which are totally
insensitive to NMDA, and in cultured CNS neu-
rones, which are sensitive to all three types of Conclusions
glutamatergic agonists. Our finding that NMDA-
like openings are totally absent from the former The Promises of a Patch-Clamp
responses gives support to the hypothesis of Approach to the CNS
Ascher and Nowak and casts serious doubts on
The long resistance to introduce patch-clamp
the single-channel type hypothesis.
methodology to the CNS is in part due to techni-
cal difficulties. As we are witnessing now, some
GABA-ergic Channels of these difficulties can actually be overcome.
There is, however, another more fundamental
As mentioned earlier, outside-out patches from objection against such an approach. This line of
the soma of Purkinje cells have a large density of thought is advocated by people who underscore
GABA-sensitive channels. A typical example of the gap existing between the properties of single
the currents obtained upon activation of these molecules and the behavior of complex neuronal
channels is presented in Fig. 9.12A. Application networks. The number and diversity of single ele-
of 10 JlM GABA leads to an inward current with ments (ion channels, or even individual synapses)
a peak value close to 100 pA, which declines close is such that it can appear as a delusion to try to
to baseline values even though agonist application explain "interesting" properties of the CNS by
is maintained. The time course of the current combining such elements. Therefore, it could
decay is described by two exponential components appear more informative to concentrate on com-
with time constants of 80 and 446 ms. Desensiti- paratively integrated information (e.g., by study-
zation of GABA-activated currents takes place in ing current-clamp responses at the cellular level),
both spinal cord neurones (Hamill et aI., 1983) at least up to the point where major neuronal
and chromaffin cells (Bormann and Clapham, types will be characterized from that point of
1985), and it is described as well by two exponen- view.
tials. However, the time course of desensitization There are undoubtedly good reasons for caution
we find for Purkinje cell GABA-ergic channels is in these criticisms. Thus, it is certainly necessary
much faster than that of peripheral cells. Time to integrate single-channel results at the cellular
constants of2.8 and 20.8 s have been reported for level as one goes on. This is particularly difficult
the decay time of the response to 20 JlM GABA with neurones because of somatodendritic differ-
in chromaffin cells. A complete study of the desen- entiation and because of the lack of spatial voltag!!
sitization process of the GABA channel from control in whole-cell recordings. In Purkinje
Purkinje cells will be needed to determine whether cells, we have tried to circumvent this difficulty by
these results reflect a basic molecular difference recording "macroscopic" somatic currents from
in the receptor types involved. large outside-out patches, but other, better suited
9. Voltage- and Transmitter-Gated Channels 199

techniques will have to be designed to study den- Crepel, F., Dhanjal, S.S., and Sears, T.A. (1982): Effect
dritic conductances as well. In general, however, of glutamate, aspartate and related derivatives on
we feel that the analytical approach represented cerebellar Purkinje cell dendrites in the rat: an in
by the patch-clamp technique offers a promising vitro study. J. Physiol., 329, 297-317.
lead to the study of the CNS. The study of syn- Chan-Palay, V. (1978): Autoradiographic localization
of gamma-amino butyric acid receptors in the rat
aptic transmission should greatly benefit from the
central nervous system by using 3H-muscimol. Proc.
identfication of the specific neurotransmitters and Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 75,1024-1028.
ion channels involved. The study of voltage- Cull-Candy, S.G. and Usowicz, M.M. (1987): Multiple
dependent ion channels should help in the under- conductance channels activated by excitatory amino
standing of the origin of the spiking properties of acids in cerebellar neurons. Nature, 325,527-528.
individual neurones. Generally, we are of the DeReimer, S.A., Sakmann, B. (1986): Two calcium cur-
opinion that a detailed analysis of ion channels rents in normal rat anterior pituitary cells identified
can simplify the interpretation of cell properties. by a plaque assay. In: Calcium Electrogenesis and
Finally, one decisive advantage ofthe patch-clamp Neuronal Functioning (U. Heinemann, M. Klee, E.
technique is to gain access to the cell interior. This E. Neher, and W. Singer, eds). Berlin: Springer-
will permit the study of synaptic transmission Verlag, pp. 138-154.
Edwards, F.A., Konnerth, A., Takahashi, T., and Sak-
involving intracellular messages, an area of enor-
mann, 0. (1989): A thin slice preparation for patch-
mous potential that has been left practically un- clamp recordings from neurones of the mammalian
touched in the CNS. central nervous system. Pjliigers Arch., 414, 600-612.
Giihwiler, B.H. (1978): Mixed cultures of cerebellum
References and inferior olive: Generation of complex spikes in
Purkinje cells. Brain Res., 145, 168-172.
Altman, J., and Anderson, W. (1972): Experimental Giihwiler, o.H. (1981a): Organotypic monolayer cul-
reorganization of the cerebellar cortex. I. Morpho- tures of nervous tissue. J. Neurosci. Meth., 4, 329-
logical effects of elimination of all micro neurons 342.
with prolonged X-irradiation started at birth. Giihwiler, o.H. (1981b): Morphological differentiation
J. Camp. Neural., 146, 355-406. of nerve cells in thin organotypic cultures derived
Ascher, P., and Nowak, L. (1988): Quisqualate- and from rat hippocampus and cerebellum. Proc. R. Soc.
kainate-activated channels in mouse central neu- Land. B, 211, 287-290.
rones in culture. J. Physiol., 399, 227-245. Giihwiler, o.H. (1984): Slice cultures of cerebellar, hip-
Blank, N.K., and Seil, F.J. (1982): Mature Purkinje cells pocampal and hypothalamic tissue. Experientia, 40,
in cerebellar tissue cultures. An ultra-structural study. 235-243.
J. Camp. Neural., 208,169-176. Giihwiler, o.H., and Llano, I. (1989): Sodium and
Bormann, J., Hamill, O.P., and Sakmann, 0. (1987): potassium conductances in somatic membranes of
Mechanism of anion permeation through channels rat Purkinje cells from organotypic cerebellar
gated by glycine and y-aminobutyric acid in mouse cultures. J. Physiol., 417,105-122.
cultured spinal neurones. J. Physiol., 385, 243-286. Gruol, D.L. (1983): Cultured cerebellar neurons: Endo-
Bormann, J., and Clapham, D.E. (1985): y-Aminobuty- genous and exogenous components of Purkinje cell
ric acid receptor channels in adrenal chromaffin activity and membrane response to putative trans-
cells: A patch-clamp study. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. mitters. Brain Res., 263, 223-241.
USA, 82, 2168-2172. Gruol, D.L., and Franklin, c.L. (1987): Morphological
Bossu, J.-L., Dupont, J.-L., and Feltz, A (1988): Potas- and physiological differentiation of Purkinje neurons
sium currents in rat cerebellar Purkinje neurones in cultures of rat cerebellum. J. Neurosci., 7, 1271-
maintained in LIs (Leibovitz) medium. Neurosci. 1293
Lett., 89, 55-62. Hamill, O.P., Bormann, J., and Sakmann, B. (1983):
Bossu,J.-L., Dupont,J.-L., and Feltz, A. (1989): Calcium Activation of multiple conductance state chloride
currents, in rat cerebellar Purkinje cells maintained channels in spinal neurones by glycine and GABA.
in culture. Neuroscience, 30, 605-617. Natur, 305,805-808.
Crepel, F., Delhaye-Bouchaud, N., Dupont, J.L., and Hamill, O.P., Marty, A, Neher, E., Sakmann, B., and
Sotelo, C. (1980): Dendritic and axonic fields of Sigworth, F.J. (1981): Improved patch-clamp tech-
Purkinje cells in developing X-irradiated cerebellum. niques for high-resolution current recording from
A comparative study using intracellular staining with cells and cell-free membrane patches. Pjliigers Arch.,
horse radish peroxidase. Neuroscience, 5, 333-347. 393,254-261.
200 Isabel Llano, Beat H. Giihwiler, and Alain Marty

l;Iendeiman, W.J., and Aggerwal, A.S. (1980): The Llinas, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980a): Electrophysiologi-
Purkinje neuron: I. A Golgi study of its development cal properties of in vitro Purkinje cell somata in
in the mouse and in culture. J. Compo Neurol., 193, mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol., 305, 171-
1063-1079. 195.
Herndon, R.M., Margolis, G., and Kilham, L. (1971): Llinas, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980b): Electrophysiologi-
The synaptic organization of the malformed cerebel- cal properties of in vitro Purkinje cell dendrites in
lum induced by perinatal infection with the feline mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol., 305, 197-
panleucopenia virus (PL V). J. N europathol. Exper. 213.
Neurol.,30,557-570. Mariani, J., Knopfel, T., and Gahwiler, B.H. (1991):
Hirano, T., and Ohmori, H. (1986): Voltage-gated and Coculture of inferior olive and cerebellum: Electro-
synaptic currents in rat Purkinje cells in dissociated physiological evidence for multiple innervation of
cell cultures. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 83, 1945- Purkinje cells by olivary axons. J. Neurobiol., 22,
1949. 865-872.
Hirano, T., and Hagiwara, S. (1989): Kinetics and dis- Mayer, M.L., and Westbrook, G.L. (1987): The physio-
tribution of voltage-gated Ca, Na and K channels logy of excitatory amino acids in the vertebrate
on the somata of rat cerebellar Purkinje cells. Pflugers nervous system. Prog. Neurobiol., 28,197-276.
Arch., 413, 463-469. Nowak, L., Bregestovski, P., Ascher, P., Herbert, A.,
Jaeger, c.B., Kapoor, R., and Llinas, R. (1988): Cytology and Prochiantz, A. (1984): Magnesium gates gluta-
and organization of rat cerebellar organ cultures. mate-activated channels in mouse central neurones.
Neuroscience, 26, 509-538. Nature, 307, 462-465.
Jahr, C.E., and Stevens, C.F. (1987): Glutamate activates Sotelo, C. (1975): Anatomical, physiological and bio-
multiple single channel conductances in hippocam- chemical studies ofthe cerebellum from mutant mice.
pal neurones. Nature, 325,522-525. II. Morphological study of cerebellar cotrical neu-
Johnson, J.W., and Ascher, P. (1987): Glycine poten- rons and circuits in the weaver mouse. Brain Res.,
tiates the NMDA response in cultured mouse brain 94,19-44.
neurones. Nature, 325,529-531. Standen, N.B., Stanfield, N.B., and Ward, T.A. (1985):
Kapoor, R., Jaeger, c.B., and Llinas, R. (1988): Electro- Properties of single potassium channels in vesicles
physiology of the mammalian cerebellar cortex in formed from the sarcolemma of frog skeletal muscle.
organ culture. Neuroscience, 26, 493-507. J. Physiol., 364, 339-358.
Latorre, R., Oberhauser, A., Labarca, P., and Alvarez, Stlihmer, W., Methfessel, c., Sakmann, B., and Numa,
O. (1989): Varieties of calcium-activated potassium S. (1987): Patch clamp characterization of sodium
channels. Annu. Rev. Physiol., 51,385-399. channels expressed from rat brain cDNA. Eur. Bio-
Llano, I., Marty, A., Johnson, J.W., Ascher, P., and phys. J., 14, 131-138.
Giihwiler, B.H. (1988a): Patch-clamp recording of Yool, AJ., Dionne, V.E., and Gruol, D.L. (1988): Dev-
amino acid-activated responses in "organotypic" slice elopmental changes in K + selective channel activity
cultures. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 85, 3221-3225. during differentiation of the Purkinje neuron in cul-
Llano, I., Webb, c.K., and Bezanilla, F. (l988b): Potas- ture. J. Neurosci., 8,1971-1980.
sium conductance of the squid giant axon. Single-
channel studies. J. Gen. Physiol., 92,179-196.
10
Electroneuronal Hybridization:
A Novel Approach to Investigate
Rhythmogenesis in the Interior Olivary Nucleus
YosefYarom

Detailed studies of the structure and function Electroresponsive Properties


of neurons of the inferior olivary nucleus carried of Olivary Neurons
out over the last two decades have pointed out some
unique features ofthese cells. These neurons have In the last decade, with the development of intra-
an immensely dense dendritic tree (Scheibel and cellular techniques permitting stable, reliable re-
Scheible, 1955; Scheibel et aI., 1956) and in addi- cording from neurons of the mammalian central
tion to multiple spinelike appendages (de Zeeuw nervous system, it has become evident that the
et aI., 1989a), they have a unique synaptic organi- type and density of ionic channels and their
zation in which excitatory, inhibitory, and electri- distribution over the somadendritic membrane
cal synapses are meaningfully organized along determine the electroresponsive properties of
the dendritic tree (Sotelo et aI., 1974; de Zeeuw central neurons (Llinas, 1988). The uniqueness of
et aI., 1989b). olivary neurons arises from the diversity, rela-
Physiological studies have also indicated several tively high density, and almost complete segrega-
unique prQperties. The exceptionally low firing tion of their ionic channels. The different ways in
rate (Armstrong et at, 1968; Armstrong and which these channels can coactivate or interact
Rawson, 1979; Bell and Grimm, 1969; Leonard endow the olivary neurons with the capability to
et aI., 1988; Llim'ts and Sasaki, 1989; Sasaki et aI., generate a variety of patterns of electrical activ-
1989), the electrical coupling (Llinas et aI., 1974; ity ranging from a single robust action potential
Llinas and Yarom, 1981a), the olivary reflexes through a repetitive pacemakerlike activity to
(Eccles et aI., 1966), and the powerful synaptic subthreshold synchronized membrane potential
connection between these neurons and the cere- fluctuations (Llinas and Yarom, 1981a, b, 1986a;
bellar Purkinje cell (Eccles et aI., 1966; Llinas and Yarom, 1989; Yarom and Llinas, 1987).
Sugimori, 1980) are just a few of the properties The intracellular records, which were obtained
that distinguish these neurons from others in the through the use of the brain slice technique
central nervous system. (Fig. lO.1), typify the various electrical activities
This chapter focuses on the electroresponsive detectable in olivary neurons. In Figure lO.1A a
properties of individual olivary neurons and the supra threshold positive current pulse delivered at
emergent electrical properties of the nucleus as resting potential level elicited a complex neuronal
revealed by in vitro experiments combining the response characterized by a broad potential, a
conventional intracellular technique with a new prolonged afterhyperpolarizing potential, and a
experimental approach based on use of a hybrid prominent rebound response. In previous work
system: the neuron in the slice preparation to- (Llinas and Yarom, 1981 a, b) it was demonstrated
gether with an analog model of the nucleus. that the broad action potential results from the

201
202 Yosef Yarom

A B

J---
JvA--
120mv~
'"----
~25mv
..-r-l1--_--===!lnA
100msec 0.5 sec
C D

IlnA
---1.----------'
---I 2 sec 400msec
Figure 10.1. The different types of electrical activities response is due to the presence of TTX (10 - 5 M) in the
encountered in intracellular recordings from olivary physiological solution. C: Prolonged bursts of activity
neurons. A: At resting potential level, olivary neurons after DC hyperpolarization of the membrane. Upper
respond to a positive current pulse with a complex trace obtained at lower current and demonstrates shorter
neuronal response that reflects the sequential activation train of activity. The current intensity is shown at the
of various types of ionic channels. B: The repetitive lowest trace. The duration of the burst of activity varies
activation of the low-threshold Ca spike by negative in different preparations and probably represents dif-
current pulses of different intensities (top to bottom: ferent neuronal populations size within the particular
increase in current intensity) reveal the oscillatory slice (Lampel and Yarom, unpublished observations,
nature of this conductance. The absence ofNa-dependent see also Llinas and Yarom, 1981, 1986).

sequential activation of Na channels located at is completely inactive at resting potential and


the cell body level and Ca channels that are likely since it has a distinct threshold that is close to or
to be distributed over the dendritic membrane. at the resting level, it effectively governs the
The activation ofCa-dependent K channels, which electrical behavior of neurons (Gutnick and
are probably distributed all over the somaden- Yarom, 1989; Llimis, 1988).
dritic membrane, generate the prolonged after- As shown in Figure lO.1B, a negative current
hy~rpolarizing potential. The rebound response, pulse delivered at resting potential level evoked
on the other hand, reflects the activation of a a repetitive activation of this conductance even
low-threshold Ca conductance located mainly at under conditions where Na conductance was
the cell body and characterized by its low voltage blocked by tetrodotoxin (TTX). An increase in
of activation and relatively fast voltage-dependent the current intensity (top to bottom) resulted
inactivation. Since the low-threshold conductance in an increase in the duration of the repetitive
10. Electroneuronal Hybridization 203

activation. The repetitive activation of the low- frequency, with controllable gain in its feedback
threshold conductance results from the relatively loop. Such a unit will generate sustained oscilla-
slow inactivation process and the afterhyperpo- tions at the given frequency, provided that the
larization that follows each individual response. closed loop gain is equal to or greater than 1. Each
Thus, the low-threshold Ca conductance has an unit receives its input from a point that sums the
intrinsic tendency to oscillate. activity in all the other oscillating units (I: 1-4)
Under normal conditions, a much longer burst as well as from a triggering source. The activity
of activity could be occasionally recorded. As in this system is determined by the "unit gain,"
shown in Figure 10.1 C, these bursts of activity which determines the feedback loop gain of each
are triggered by the release from a prolonged unit, and the "coupling gain," which determines
negative current that held the membrane at a the extent ofthe coupling between the units. Trigger
hyperpolarizing level. The burst duration depends signals to the electrical units were generated by
on the preceding hyperpolarizing level and always a rate-controllable trigger source that distributes
terminates with a short series of low-threshold a constant trigger signal either randomly to all
Ca spikes (LTS) resembling those obtained in the the units or at a constant rate specifically to any
presence ofTTX (Fig. 10.1 B). These observations given unit. A set of switches (Sj, St, So) controls
have led us to hypothesize that olivary neurons the internal connections between the units. In this
can be regarded as limit cycle oscillators (Llinas system sustained oscillations are generated if the
and Yarom, 1986a). Indeed, in about 10% of the equivalent overall gain (the feedback loop-gain
experiments the membrane potential of olivary in each unit plus the contributions to the loop
neurons undergoes sinusoidal-like oscillations. from the other units) exceeds a certain level ( + 1).
The examples shown in Figure 10.1 D demonstrate Figure 10.2 also demonstrates the connection
the waveforms of these oscillations and their of the olivary neuron to the simulator. The out-
voltage dependence. The most important features puts of all four units were summed, translated
revealed by this experiment are that the frequency into current, and injected into the neuron through
ofthe oscillations is independent ofthe membrane the intracellular microelectrode. The neuronal
potential whereas the amplitude decreases as the response (E), as recorded by the microelectrode,
membrane is hyperpolarized (top to bottom). was introduced into the summing points of each
In an attempt to explore the mechanisms under- of the oscillating units. With this arrangement,
lying the oscillatory activity of inferior olivary the connections to and from the olivary neuron
neuronal ensembles, a novel experimental ap- are similar to those of any of the oscillating units.
proach was developed. According to the ratio-
nale underlying this approach, if an analog model
Properties of the Simulator
(constructed according to the working hypothesis
assumptions) successfully replicates the phenom- The properties of the simulator are shown in
ena observed in the neuronal system, and if the Figure 10.3. The increased oscillations induced
analog system can be merged into the neuronal by increasing the unit gain in an isolated oscillating
system and the hybrid system can still produce unit are demonstrated in Figure 10.3A. Oscillations
the same phenomena, the working hypothesis is were evoked by a single triggering pulse (the first
then strongly reinforced. A brief description of negative deflection in each trace) delivered to the
the approach and the main results obtained same unit at four different levels of unit gain. As
through its use is given in the following sections. the gain increases (top to bottom), the decay time
of the oscillations increases; as a result, the ampli-
tude and duration of the oscillations increase.
The Simulator The effect of increasing the coupling level
The simulator consists offour identical intercon- between the simulator units is demonstrated in
nected oscillating units; a schematic illustration Figure 1O.3B, C. Simultaneous recordings of the
ofthe system is given in Figure 10.2. The heart of electrical activity in all the units, at two levels of
each oscillating unit (OSC 1-4) is an operational coupling, are presented. The three coupled units
amplifier with a notch filter, tuned to a given (three lower traces) were mutually connected,
204 YosefYarom

RATE ~~.-__~T~R~IG~GpE~R~S~O~U~R~C~E______~
I I

GAIN

COUPLING

OUTPUT

VOLT AGE TO CURRENT

OLIVARV NEURON

Figure 10.2. Schematic presentation of the simulator units. The couping gain (coupling) that is determined
illustrates the organization of the hybrid system. Trig- after the summing point is also simultaneously con-
ger source, A rate-controlled trigger source that distri- trolled. So. A toggle switch connecting the output of any
butes a constant trigger signal either randomly to all given unit to the summing points of all the other units.
the units or specifically to any given unit. St, A toggle The outputs from all the units are summed, translated
switch connecting any given unit to the trigger source. into current, and injected into an olivary neuron whose
OSC 1 to 4, The four oscillating units, whose gain is output (E) is fed back into the summing points of each
simultaneously controlled. Sj, A toggle switch connect- of the units. (Adapted with permission from Yarom,
ing the input into the oscillating units. S 1 to 4, Summing 1989.)
points preceding the inputs of each of the oscillating

whereas the input to unit 1, which received the Generation of Sustained Oscillations
triggering signal, was unconnected; thus, unit 1
serves as the source but does not receive the By interconnecting the entire system and deliver-
feedback signal. As the coupling between the units ing a trigger signal to one of the units, sustained
increases, the oscillations in the coupled units oscillations could be generated, provided that the
increase both in amplitude and duration. It should unit gain and the coupling level were properly set.
be mentioned that in this system the load effect, This behavior is demonstrated in Figure 1O.3D,
expected in a coupled system, does not exist. which shows the activity induced by a triggering
Therefore, even when the coupling was increased, signal delivered to unit 1 (upper trace) and re-
the source, unit 1, remained unchanged. Further- corded simultaneously in all four units. The build-
more, since each of the coupled units responds up of sustained oscillations is characterized by an
actively to the input signal and since all of them initial phase during which the amplitude of the
are interconnected, at the high coupling level response in unit 1 decreases, while the amplitude
(Fig. lO.3C), the oscillations in the coupled units of the responses in the coupled units increases
outlast the response in unit 1. until the activities in all the units are locked in
10. Electroneuronal Hybridization 205

A B C of one or more of the units from the system. The


_Af'-~ __ ... - - - --A.rv--_. -. ability to produce sustained oscillations depends
on the unit gain and the coupling level in such a
way that a system with a high coupling level and
low gain behaves similarly to a system with a low
coupling level and high unit gain. In fact, once a
400 msec
set of "threshold" parameters (a level of gain
D settings where a single trigger signal to unit 1
evoked sustained oscillations) was selected, they
could be modified in a reciprocal way and the
system would still maintain the same properties.
Therefore, a system comprising coupled oscil-
lating units, each of which can generate damped
oscillations and responds actively to input signals,
can generate sustained oscillations. In such a
Figure 10.3. Properties of the simulator. A: Increased
system in-phase activity of the oscillating units
oscillations induced by an increase in unit gain. The is a necessary condition for the development of
activity of unit 1 during progressive increases in the sustained oscillations. The ability to reach this
unit gain. In each trace the unit gain was set to a different condition, which is an intrinsic property of the
level, increasing from top to bottom. Note that the unit system, depends on the extent of the coupling
gain affects the duration of the oscillations more than between the electrical units. On the other hand,
their amplitude. B-C: The activity in all the units is maintenance of the oscillations depends on the
shown at two different levels of coupling gain. Only integrity of the system.
unit 1 (upper traces) was triggered. All the units were
mutually connected except for the input to unit 1
(Switch Sj of unit 1 was open; see Fig. 10.2). Note
that increased coupling level did not affect the signal
The Hybrid System
in unit 1 and that, at the highest coupling gain (C), the
signals in the coupled units outlast that of unit 1. D: In order to determine to what extent the mech-
Sustained oscillations generated by the electrical simu- anism underlying the generation of oscillations
lator with a threshold setting of the unit and coupling in the olivary nucleus behaves similarly to the
gains. The activity recorded in all four units after a analog simulator, a hybrid system, consisting of
triggering signal to unit 1. Note that the sustained the analog simulator and an olivary neuron in a
oscillations develop simultaneously in all units and slice preparation, was constructed. If such a hybrid
that the amplitude is larger than that induced by the system were to retain the properties of the analog
triggering signal. (Adapted with permission from Yarom, simulator by itself, and if an inferior olivary neuron
1989).
behaved similarly to each of the electrical units
and contributed actively to the generation of
phase and amplitude. From this time on, a pro- rhythmic activity within such a system, then the
gressive increase in amplitude in all the units is olivary neuron would also appear to act as a
evident. The process then develops into its final generator of damped oscillations. Moreover, it
form, and sustained oscillations of constant ampli- would not be unreasonable to suggest that the
tude are generated. As shown in this figure, accu- oscillating inferior olivary nucleus itself operates
rate synchronization of the units seems to be a similarly to the analog model.
prerequisite for the generation of sustained oscil- Sustained oscillations induced in the hybrid
lations. system by a single triggering pulse are demons-
Once sustained oscillations are generated they trated in Figure 1O.4A. The top two traces are the
will be maintained if no interference is introduced; activity in units 1 and 3 (units 2 and 4 were also
interference is defined either as a reduction in connected but are not illustrated). The third trace
coupling level or unit gain or as a disconnection is the activity in the olivary neuron as recorded
206 YosefYarom

unit

olivary
neuron

O.5sec
B
unit #1

unit #3

olivary
neuron \20IllV
/V"'J''J'.r.,."....,---
~------------------~~~________====~IO.5nA
i O.5sec
Figure 10.4. The activation and termination of sus- and 3 and the olivary neuron are shown. The lowest
tained oscillation in the hybrid system. Simultaneous trace indicates the changes in the DC holding current.
recording from units 1 and 3 (two upper traces) and Oscillations were invoked by releasing -0.3 nA DC
from an olivary neuron (lower trace). Units 2 and 4 are holding current in the neuron (first arrow). Oscillations
also connected but are not shown. A: A continuous were terminated by hyperpolarizing the neuron (second
recording illustrates the development of the oscillations arrow). Note that the release of the holding current was
after a single triggering signal delivered to unit 1 (arrows followed by a train of LTSs, and that the last LTS that
denote the triggering time). Note the phase shift of the occurred in phase with the electrical oscillations (de-
response ofthe neuron in relation to the electrical units noted by *) seems to be the immediate trigger for the
during the first three waves and the accelerated rising development ofthe sustained oscillations. The neuron
phase of the waves recorded from the neuron just was treated with harmaline (5 mg/ml) and CsCI (5 mM)
before the development of full size oscillations. B: As in order to reduce various rectifications.
in A, continuous recordings of the activity in units 1

by the intracellular microelectrode. A trigger sig- oscillations in the olivary neuron followed the
nal was delivered to unit 1, recorded as a fast frequency dictated by the electrical system, the
negative deflection (arrow). As with the simulator waveform was deformed and did not follow the
by itself, in the hybrid system the sustained oscil- sine wave generated by the electrical system.
lation evoked by the triggering signal is also A careful examination of the initial phase re-
characterized by an initial phase, during which a veals that immediately after the triggering signal,
slow build-up of the oscillation was observed. a prominent phase shift exists between the wave
During the sustained oscillation, the amplitude in the olivary neuron and the electrical units. TQis
of the olivary wave was in the range of25 to 30 mV phase lag is actually expected whenever a sinusoi-
and, as a result, the peak of the oscillatory wave dal current is irttroduced into an electrical circuit
occasionally reached threshold and action poten- composed of a resistor and a capacitor (RC
tials were generated. Although the sustained circuit) such as provided by the neuron. During
10. Electroneuronal Hybridization 207

the initial phase, a progressive decrease in the are shown. The neuron was held at a hyper-
phase shift of the olivary oscillations was obser- polarizing level by - 0.3 nA DC current injected
ved. This decrease in phase lag was accompanied through the microlectrode. Releasing the holding
by increasing deformation of the waveform in the current (arrow) generated an LTS that was large
olivary neuron. The deformation was character- enough to trigger a sodium-dependent spike and
ized by an accelerated rising phase, indicating the to activate the electrical oscillators. Since the
presence of an active process. Once a phase-locked frequency generated by the olivary neuron was
condition was achieved, a state of sustained oscil- in the order of 5 Hz (while the tuned frequency of
lations was quickly reached in the entire system. the electrical system was 9 Hz), the electrical acti-
Therefore, as in the simulator by itself, synchron- vity during the first second after release from
ization between the different elements of the hyperpolarization appears chaotic and the ampli-
hybrid system appears to be a necessary pre- tude of the oscillation varied from wave to wave.
requisite for the generation of sustained oscil- This situation persisted until the rhythm gener-
lations. ated by the olivary neuron subsided and the fifth
According to the above description, a regen- LTS (marked by *) coincided with the sine wave
erative response in the olivary neuron contri- in the electrical units. From that time on, a state
buted to the reduction of phase lag, thereby of sustained oscillation was quickly reached.
insuring complete synchronization between the Reintroducing the -0.3 DC current into the
olivary neuron and the electrical units. The pro- olivary neuron (second arrow) induced, in addition
perty of olivary neurons that most likely explains to the drop in membrane potential, a dramatic
the phase advance leading to synchronization is reduction in the amplitude of the olivary neuron
the low-threshold Ca conductance. Since both oscillations. As a result, the oscillations in the
the activation and inactivation of this con- coupled electrical units slowly declined. Thus, it
ductance occurs within a rather small potential is clear that most of the oscillatory response in
range around the resting level, it generates graded the olivary neuron is generated by a voltage-
regenerative responses as the result of relatively dependent conductance, most likely the low-
small depolarizations, provided that they are pre- threshold Ca conductance, which appears to be
ceded by hyperpolarization (Llinas and Yarom, essential for the generation of sustained oscilla-
1981a). Thus, the hybrid system, which oscillates tions.
about the resting potential, provides favorable
conditions for the activation of this low-thresh- Preferred Frequencies of Oscillations
old Ca conductance. Even if the hybrid system does not accurately
The contribution of the low-threshold Ca re- simulate the conditions in the inferior olivary
sponse to the generation of sustained oscillations nucleus, it provides a new tool with which the
was examined by shifting the membrane potential complex neuronal properties postulated to exist
to either more negative or less negative values in olivary neurons can be investigated. The follow-
(relative to the resting level). Since the LTS has a ing example demonstrates the use of the hybrid
distinct threshold and since it is completely inac- system to explore the behavior of olivary neurons
tive at potentials less negative than the resting in the frequency domain.
level, a DC voltage shift in either direction will Since, as already demonstrated, the LTS of the
prevent the generation of the LTS. The effect inferior olive neuron actively participates in the
of DC hyperpolarization on the sustained oscilla- generation of sustained oscillations by the hybrid
tion of the hybrid system is demonstrated in system, it is expected that the ability of the hybrid
Figure lOAR. The experiments were performed system to generate sustained oscillations will be
in the presence of harmaline (5 mg/ml) and CsCI frequency dependent. The frequencies where the
(5 mM) which, by blocking several types of recti- low-threshold Ca conductance is most readily
fication, induce linear current-voltage relations activated will be the frequencies "preferred" by
(Yarom and Llinas, 1987). As in Figure 10AA, the olivary neuron. At these preferred frequencies,
only the olivary activity (lower trace) and the minimum "gains" should be needed to evoke sus-
activity in two of the coupled units (upper traces) tained oscillations.
208 YosefYarom

COl •• •
..........
CJ
Q)4
.........
III

d
....0
~2
s..
:=;j
'0

0
2 4 6 B
frequency

20mV

0.5 sec
Figure 10.5. Frequency-duration curve of the hybrid the presence of TTX (10 - 5 M). All the responses were
system reveals that the ability to generate sustained elicited by a single trigger pulse delivered to unit 1. The
oscillation is frequency dependent.l nsets: Intracellular duration was measured from the first negative deflection
recordings from the olivary neuron illustrating the to the point where the amplitude was reduced to twice
responses of the hybrid system at four different fre- the size of the noise. Note the discontinuity of the
quencies. The simulator gain settings were constant duration axis (the uppermost data points represent
throughout the experiment, which was carried out in sustained oscillation).

In order to determine the preferred frequencies, triggered at low (3 Hz) and high (8.7 Hz) frequen-
the gains of the system were set to given values, cies appear to comprise two components: the
usually just below threshold for oscillations at the passive response to the sinusoidal input current
lowest frequency. Then the duration of the oscilla- and the regenerative low-threshold spikes. At
tions induced by a single triggering pulse (given these frequencies the L TSs were activated only
to one of the electrical units) at different frequen- by the first one or two waves. In fact, a failure to
cies (set at the electrical units) was measured and generate an LTS was immediately followed by
plotted as a function of the frequency. The results fast decay of the passive oscillatory response,
of such an experiment are shown in Figure 10.5. reflecting the decay of the current input. The
The four recorded traces, which were obtained at full-size oscillations observed at 6.4 Hz, however,
the frequencies indicated by the arrows, represent lasted 2 s but displayed a continuous decrease in
the activity ofthe olivary neuron within the hybrid amplitude. This decrease eventually led to an
system after a single triggering pulse (first negative abrupt failure to generate an LTS, which was
deflection) delivered to one of the electrical units. followed by a rapid decline of the oscillations. At
These records clearly demonstrate that the ability intermediate frequencies (5.1 Hz), the same trig-
of the hybrid system to generate sustained oscilla- gering signal evoked sustained oscillations with
tions, at a given set of gains, is frequency depen- constant amplitude that persisted as long as the
dent. The rapidly decaying oscillatory responses system remained intact and the gains were un-
10. Electroneuronal Hybridization 209

changed. The results of these experiments are same neurons in different constellations, thereby
summarized in the "tuning curve" shown, which increasing the versatility of the system.
describes the duration ofthe oscillation as a func- It is important to determine if this oscillation
tion ofthe frequency. Only at three close frequen- is the source of the motor rhythm or just its ref-
cies (4.7, 5.l, and 5.7) did sustained oscillations lection. The difference between these two possibi-
develop. At other frequencies the oscillations lities reflects a fundamental difference in our
lasted only a few seconds. Although this apparent interpretation of olivary function. If the olive is
high sensitivity (narrow-band tuning curve) could the source (Lamarre, 1979, 1984; Llimis, 1984),
result from the method of quantification, it reveals its role is to determine the basic rhythm of muscle
that the olivary neuron preferred to operate at activity that underlies all motor performance
certain frequencies. In other experiments the pre- (Llinas, 1984). If, on the other hand, it is just a
ferred frequency ranged between 4 and 6 Hz, with reflection of motor rhythm, it might serve as a
an average of 5.2 (n = 7). correcting relay station (or "phasic motor control
It should be mentioned that in a series of control system"; see Llinas, 1970) and the subthreshold
experiments, including the examination of the activity could then be a device that ensures the
responses ofthe same neuron with the same setting most efficient correcting procedures (Schoner and
of unit and coupling gains both with and without Kelso, 1988).
octanol (0.01%), it has been verified that the fre- If the olive is the drive for motor activity, how
quency dependence of the hybrid system reflects can subthreshold oscillation, which by definition
the properties of the neuron and not those of the is an internal phenomenon, determine the rhythm
electrical units. of muscle activity? It is possible that in order to
maintain a rhythm only one or a few olivary
neurons within the defined (coupled) group have
Discussion to be active at any given time. Thus, the firing of
Functional Role of Olivary Subthreshold an olivary neuron, which under normal physio-
logical conditions appears to be unrelated to any
Oscillations
behavioral parameter (with the exception of the
Even though the present study did not address the neurons ofthe dorsal cap), is in fact following an
question of the functional role of the subthresh- accurate timing schedule (Sasaki et aI., 1989).
old oscillations, it is clear, however, from the very According to this scheme, when movement is
essence of their nature that the oscillations can disturbed or sensory inputs report an unusual
serve as an internal time reference (Llimis, 1989). circumstance, a massive, synchronous activation
According to this concept, the activity in a group of the group of coupled neurons will reset the
of olivary neurons whose size is determined by motor rhythm. If this is true, measuring the acti vity
the extent of the coupling is associated with a of a single neuron might lead to a wrong conclu-
rhythmic activity that takes place in the motor sion regarding its functional role.
system. It is not unlikely that this rhythm is in The basic assumption underlying the second
fact the source of the physiological tremor that possibility is that the subthreshold oscillations
occurs in a muscle or in a group of muscle units reflect motor rhythm rather than drive it. Accord-
during motor behavior (Llimis, 1984). ing to this view, the subthreshold rhythm in an
The ability of the olivocerebellar system to olivary neuron will function only during the
determine not only the size of the group of coupled performance of a routine motor task. As in the
olivary neurons (through the inhibitory pathway, previous possibility, the olivocerebellar system
originating- in the deep cerebellar nucleus and determines the size of the group of coupled neu-
terminating next to the olivary gap junction; rons, relative to the number of muscles involved
Llimis and Sasaki, 1989; Sotelo et aI., 1986) but in that motor task, as well as the functional state
also its functional state (quiescent or oscillating) of the group relative to the onset of the movement.
offers a unique mechanism to associate a variable The difference is that in this case the subthreshold
number of muscle units with a variable number oscillation will sum with any synaptic input to
of olivary neurons. It is also possible to use the elicit an output at a precise time as determined
210 Yosef Yarom

by the phase of the oscillations. It is not unlikely in size (due to many elements each having several
that such precise timing would be needed for the subelements), the number of poorly defined pa-
system to respond in the most effective way to rameters increases, producing conditions where
incoming signals (Llinas, 1984; Marshall and practically any phenomenon could be simulated.
Walsh, 1956). In the approach presented here, a working
Although there is no single compelling reason hypothesis is first formulated and a "simulator,"
to reject either of these possibilities, there are which is a hardware representation of this hypo-
several observations and considerations that thesis, is then constructed. The ability of this
argue against each of these hypotheses. If olivary hardware representation to interact with the bio-
subthreshold oscillations are the source of motor logical system is then examined. The controllable
rhythm, all cells should be active all the time, as part in such a "hybrid system" serves as a unique
is the physiological tremor. The precise control tool to examine the behavior of the biological
of the group size seems to be less important. system under conditions that were postulated to
Furthermore, if only a few neurons within a group exist by the working hypothesis. The biological
of coupled neurons fire at any given time, the part of the hybrid system, on the other hand,
convergence of olivary neurons onto a single neu- limits the number of assumptions and thereby
ronal structure has to be postulated. Alternatively, the degrees offreedom; both an excessive number
the group of coupled olivary neurons might drive of assumptions and degrees of freedom tend to
another less efficient oscillator. reduce the usefulness oftheoretical models. There-
On the other hand, the observation that physio- fore, this approach tends to fill the gap between
logical tremor disappears after either a lesion pure theoretical modeling and conventional
of the inferior olivary nucleus or in the presence electrophysiological experiments.
of alcohol (Sinton et al., 1989), a suppressor of the The working hypothesis in the present study
low-threshold calcium conductance (Llinas and was formulated to account for the subthreshold
Yarom, 1986b), is a strong indication that the membrane potential oscillation Qf olivary neurons.
inferior olive is the source of motor rhythm. According to this hypothesis, each olivary neuron
is a generator of damped oscillations. Since these
oscillators are electrotonically coupled, b)! inter-
Electroneuronal Hybridization acting they can generate synchronized sustained
oscillations. The simulator in this case comprises
A new experimental approach to investigate the
an interconnected set of oscillating units, each
complex electrical behavior of neuronal assembly
capable of generating damped oscillations; when
was introduced. This approach was used to point
interacting with each other, they can generate
at a possible mechanism that underlies the rhyth-
sustained oscillations. The hybrid system com-
mic activity of inferior olivary neurons.
prises the simulator and an intracellularly impaled
Most of the electrophysiological phenomena
olivary neuron in a slice preparation. It has been
that have been observed in biological systems,
demonstrated that the activity in such a hybrid
such as the mammalian central nervous system
system is similar to that observed in the olivary
(CNS), were attributed to complex interactions
nucleus.
between several elements (e.g., neurons) or sub-
The three most important conclusions derived
elements (e.g., channels). When these phenomena
from the behavior of the hybrid system are:
were investigated, the following approach was
commonly used: the subelements were functional- 1. In-phase activity of the neurons is essential in
ly isolated, their specific properties characterized, order for such a system to generate sustained
a comprehensive theoretical model was then con- oscillations.
structed, and its predictions were compared to 2. The L TS serves as a phase advance mechanism
the actual physiological phenomenon. Failure to that compensates for the phase lag introduced
predict accurately the behavior of the biological by the RC circuit of the neurons.
system indicates that a subelement or one of its 3. Olivary neurons exhibit a preferred frequency
properties was inadequately presented in (or even of oscillation that reflects the interplay between
missing from) the model. As these models increase several ionic channels.
10. Electroneuronal Hybridization 211

If a mechanism similar to that found in the Eccles, le., L1inas, R., and Sasaki, K. (1966): The
hybrid system operates in the inferior olivary excitatory synaptic action of climbing fibres on the
nucleus, several conclusions can be drawn that Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. J. Physiol., 182,
provide a substantial contribution to our under- 268-296.
standing of olivary activity. For example, from Gutnick, MJ., and Yarom, Y. (1989): Low threshold
calcium spikes, intrinsic neuronal oscillation and
the behavior of the hybrid system it is clear that
rhythm generation in the CNS. J. Neurosci. Meth.,
a sufficiently large group of olivary neurons has 28,93-99.
to be coupled in order to generate sustained oscil- Lamarre, Y. (1979): Cerebro-cerebellar mechanisms
lations. On the other hand, the frequency of these involved in experimental tremor. In: Developments
oscillations is independent of the group size. in Neuroscience (J. Massion, and K. Sasaki, eds.).
Thus, the coupling between olivary neurons Amsterdam, New York: ElsevierjNorth Holland,
should be considered not just as a device that pp.249-259.
synchronizes spikes but as the main control Lamarre, Y. (1984): Animal models of physiological,
that dictates the functional state of the internal essential and parkinsonian-like tremors. In:
clock mechanisms. Accordingly, the dense gamma- Movement Disorders: Tremor (L.J. Findley, and
amino butyric acid (GABA)-ergic innervation of LJ. Capildeo. eds.). New York: Macmillan, pp. 183-
194.
the olivary nucleus (Sotelo et aI., 1986), which
Leonard, e.S., Simpson, ll., and Graf, W. (1988):
is postulated to modulate the coupling between Spatial organization of visual messages of rabbit's
these neurons (Llimis, 1974; Llinas et aI., 1974; cerebellar flocculus. I. Typology of inferior olive
Llinas and Sasaki, 1989; Llinas and Yarom, 1981a), neurons of the dorsal cap of Kooy. J. Neurophysiol.,
determines not only how many neurons will be 60,2073-2090.
synchronously active, but also whether the under- L1imis, R. (1970): Neuronal operations in cerebellar
lying timing device will be functional. transactions. In: The Neurosciences: Second Study
Program (V.M., Brooks, ed.). New York: Rockefeller
Acknowledgement. This work was supported by University Press, pp. 409-426.
a grant from the United States-Israel Binational L1imis, R (1974): 18th Bowditch lecture: Motor aspects
Foundation. of cerebellar control. Physiologist, 17, 19-46.
Llinas, R. (1984): Rebound excitation as the physiologi-
cal basis for tremor: A biophysical study of the
References oscillatory properties of mammalian central neu-
Armstrong, D.M., Eccles, J.e., Harvey, RJ., and rones in vitro. In: Movement Disorders: Tremor
Matthews, P.B.S. (1968): Responses in the dorsal (LJ. Findley, and LJ. Capildeo, eds.). New York:
accessory olive of the cat to stimulation of hind limb Macmillan, pp. 165-181.
afferents. J. Physiol., 194, 125-145. L1inas, R (1988): The intrinsic electrophysiological pro-
Armstrong, D.M., and Rawson, J.A. (1979): Activity perties of mammalian neurons: Insights into central
pattern of cerebellar cortical neurons and climbing nervous system function. Science, 242, 1654-1664.
fibre afferents in the awake cat. J. Physiol., 289, L1inas, R (1989): Electrophysiological properties of the
425-448. olivocerebellar system. Exp. Brain Res., 17,201-208.
Bell, e.e., and Grimm, R.J. (1969): Discharge properties L1inas, R., Baker, R., and Sotelo, e. (1974): Electrotonic
of cerebellar Purkinje cells recorded with single and coupling between neurons in cat inferior olive.
double microelectrodes. J. Neurophysiol., 32,1044- J. Neurophysiol., 37, 560-571.
1055. L1inas, R, and Sasaki, K. (1989): The functional organi-
de Zeeuw, e.1., Holstege, le., Ruigrok, T.J.H., and zation of the olivo-cerebellar system as examined by
Voogd, J. (1989a): An ultrastructural study of multiple Purkinje cell recordings. Eur. J. Neurosci.,
GABA-ergic, the cerebellar and mesodiencephalic 1,587-602.
innervation of the cat medial accessory olive: L1inas, R, and Sugimori, M. (1980): Electrophysiologi-
Anterograde tracing combined with immunocyto- cal properties of in vitro Purkinje cell somata in
chemistry. J. Compo Neurol., 284,12-35. mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol., 305, 171-
de Zeeuw, C.I., Holstege, J.C., Ruigrok, TJ.H., and 195.
Voogd, J. (1989b): The GABA-ergic, cerebellar and L1inas, R., and Yarom, Y. (1981a): Electrophysiology
mesodiencephalic innervation of the glomeruli in the of mammalian inferior olivary neurons in vitro. Dif-
cat inferior olive. A comparison at the ultrastructural ferent types of voltage dependent ionic conductances.
level. Exp. Brain Res., 17, 111-117. J. Physiol., 315,549-567.
212 YosefYarom

LJimis, R., and Yarom, Y. (1981b): Properties and dis- Schoner, G., and Kelso, 1.A.S. (1988): Dynamic pattern
. tribution of ionic conductances generating electro- generation in behavioral and neural system. Science,
responsiveness of mammalian inferior olivary neu- 239,1513-1520.
rones in vitro. J. Physiol., 315, 569-584. Sinton, C.M., Krosser, B.I., Walton, K.D., and Llinas,
Llimis, R., and Yarom, Y. (1986a): Oscillatory proper- R. (1989): The effectiveness of different isomers of
ties of guinea-pig inferior olivary neurones and their octanol as blockers of harmaline-induced tremor.
pharmacological modulation: An in vitro study. Pflugers Arch., 414, 31-36.
J. Physiol., 376,163-182. Sotelo, c., Llinas, R., and Baker, R. (1974): Structural
Llimis, R., and Yarom, Y. (1986b): Specific blockage of study of the inferior olivary nucleus of the cat:
low threshold calcium channels by high molecular Morphological correlates of electrotonic coupling.
weight alcohols. Neurosci. Abst., 12, 174. J. Neurophysiol., 37, 541-549.
Marshall, 1., and Walsh, E.G. (1956): Physiological Sotelo, c., Gotow, T., and Wassef, M. (1986): Localiza-
tremor. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat, 19,260. tion of glutamic-acid-decarboxylase-immunoreactive
Sasaki, K., Bower, 1.M., and Llinas, R. (1989): MUltiple axon terminals in the inferior olive of the rat, with
Purkinje cell recording in rodent cerebellar cortex. special emphasis on anatomical relations between
Eur. J. Neurosci., 1, 572-586. GABA-ergic synapses and dendrodentritic gap
Scheibel, M.E., and Scheible, A.B. (1955): The in- junctions. J. Compo Neurol., 252, 32-50.
ferior olive. A Golgi study. J. Compo Neurol., 102, Yarom, Y. (1989): Oscillatory behavior of olivary neu-
77-132. rons. Exp. Brain Res., 17, 209-221.
Scheibel, M.E., Scheible, A.B., Walberg, F., and Brodal, Yarom, Y., and Llinas, R. (1987): Long-term modifiabi-
A. (1956): Areal distribution of axonal and dendritic lity of anomalous and delayed rectification in guinea
patterns in inferior olive. J. Compo Neurol., 106, pig inferior olivary neurons. J. Neurosci., 74, 1166-
21-50. 1177.
Part 3
Electrophysiology of Movement

The first chapter in this part documents a series of experiments (1) describing
the main characteristics of the spontaneous saccades in the rat in light and
darkness .(saccades have relatively high-peak velocity and large ability
while gaze holding ability is poor) and (2) the effect oflesioning the 10 and
two cerebellar areas known to be involved in the saccadic performance
(lobules VI, VII, VIII of the dorsal vermis-no significant change, and the
flocculus and paraflocculus-same as the monkey in that there is a
reduction in the time constant of the neural integrator, but different in that
fast postsaccadic drift is larger than in the monkey; 40% compared to 15%).
In the rat, the flocculus/paraflocculus are important to improve poor gaze
holding ability of the oculomotor system. 10 is essential for maintaining
leakiness of the neural integrator within physiological range and for gain
of the pulse to step transformation at the proper value. Effect is quanti-
tatively similar as that following floccular/parafloccular lesion. The condition
is that the 10 controls ocular motility through action on floccular/parafloc-
cular Purkinje cells.
In the third chapter, the authors ask whether there is meaning in the
geometry inherent in the CF zones and the orthogonally directed parallel
fibers. Answer: probably. AChE staining (delineates white matter compart-
ments with presumptive projections to different parts of the vestibular
nucleus complex), which provides an independent delineation of floccular
zones, invited these experiments to combine anatomical and electrophysio-
logical techniques to determine the patterns of eye movements evoked by
stimulation of each compartment. Results are considered in conjunction
with effects of floccular stimulation on the six VOR pathways. The major,
short latency components of the eye movements evoked here are in good
agreement with predictions from combining stimulation ofVOR paths with
measurements of pulling directions of individual muscles. Consequently the
classes of eye movements found in the present study differ in some ways
from those found earlier. For example: abduction of ipsi eye corresponds
to horizontal class in the old description. This difference can be explained
by known influence of the flocculus on the horizontal canal pathway to
lateral and medial recti muscle.
A possible connection between the mossy and climbing fiber system at
precerebellar level is explored in the next chapter. Extracellular recordings

213
214 Electrophysiology of Movement

-in the decerebrate cat and awake Rhesus monkey were taken. In the cat,
it was found that the CF system can be modulated phasically and tonically
in response to passive movements of the extremity. Short and rapid
movements are most effective, but the authors conclude that CF is able
to transmit all parameters of the movement, especially at which position
the movement has been performed. The results for the monkey are similar
to the cat, but during volitional movement, motor commands seem to
have preference upon the 10 with respect to the sensory feedback produced
by the passive movement. There is a relationship between SS and CS. The
inverse relationship has not necessarily to be produced by the effect of
the CS on the SS. Both inverse and parallel relationships were observed.
The fourth chapter is a review of recent work on the CF's heterosynaptic
action on PC, and the authors present the hypothesis that the CF are most
critical for real time operations performed by the cerebellum during motor
execution. They demonstrate a specific short-term action of CF on SS
responses in passive paradigms as well as in multiple, sagittally organized
PC during perturbed locomotion. At least part of the action of the CF
system is related to its capacity to produce a short term enhancement of
the PC's SS responsiveness to the MF-GC-PP input.
The authors in the fifth chapter use single-unit recording and transient
multicell inactivation in conscious trained animals to answer questions
concerning the relation between the cerebellar circuit and function as seen
by the research scientist and neurologist. They test two hypotheses; (l)
within the cerebellum there are multiple maps (one for each major division
and thus deep nucleus) of the body and multiple modes of motor control;
(2) there is a common function across maps and modes of movement that
is consistent with the physiological experiments and the anatomical circuitry.
That function is the coordination of movement (as originally proposed by
Fluorens in 1824).
In the concluding chapter, recent work points to a need to reappraise
the role of the cerebellum in behavior (e.g., the cerebellum regulates both
voluntary and involuntary movements). Once this is recognized, we can
reevaluate why voluntary movements are so susceptible to cerebellar
damage. "One can conclude that the purpose ofthe cerebellum is to ensure
that motor nuclei respond with strength proportional to the response
evoking properties of the eliciting stimuli as determined by their associative
strength, intensity and biological significance" (see p. 332).
11
Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements
in the Pigmented Rat
Piergiorgio Strata, Leonardo Chelazzi, Filippo Tempia,
Ferdinando Rossi, and Mirella Ghirardi

As for most body movements, the cerebellum behavior, since saccadic parameters can be easily
is not necessary for the generation of ocular and accurately measured. In addition, the neural
saccades, but it is important for their correct mechanisms that contribute to their performance
performance. Lesion, stimulation, and recording are rather well known (Carpenter, 1988; Fuchs
experiments show that different regions of the et aI., 1985; Leigh and Zee, 1983).
cerebellum contribute to this performance (see
Carpenter, 1988; Leigh and Zee, 1983, for reviews).
By contrast, little is known about the importance
of the inferior olive in saccadic control. Such
The Saccadic System
knowledge is important for a better understanding Saccades are fast eye movements that shift the
of the role of the cerebellum in motor perfor- sight line. They are generated by a short-lasting
mance. By comparing the different effects of the high frequency discharge of the ocular moto-
lesion of the inferior olive and of localized areas neurons impinging on the extraocular muscles.
of the cerebellum on saccadic activity, it is also Such a burst has been defined as pulse of inner-
possible to contribute to a better understanding vation and is responsible for rotating the eyeball
of the role of the olivocerebellar system in cere- to a new position in the orbit. This phasic moto-
bellar operation. neuron activity is followed by a prolonged tonic
We present here a review of recent data ob- discharge at a lower frequency, which has been
tained in our laboratory aimed at describing the defined as step of innervation. The latter is respon-
main characteristics ofthe spontaneous saccades sible for holding the eyeball in the new acquired
in the pigmented rat in light and in darkness position against the viscoelastic forces that pull
(Chelazzi et aI., 1989). In addition, we describe the eye toward the center ofthe orbit (Fuchs et aI.,
the effects of lesioning the inferior olive and two 1985; Robinson, 1975). An adequate matching
cerebellar areas that are known to be involved in of the pulse and of the step is therefore necessary
the saccadic performance: lobules VI, VII, and for this purpose. It is maintained that the phasic
VIII of the dorsal vermis and the flocculus and discharge is generated by a burst of synchronized
paraflocculus (Chelazzi et aI., 1990; Strata et aI., activity of neurons located in the paramedian
1990). pontine reticular formation (pulse generator) and
The use ofthis experimental paradigm has two transmitted to the ocular motoneurons to pro-
advantages. On one hand, in the rat it is possible duce the saccadic gaze shift (GS) (see Fuchs et aI.,
to destroy almost entirely and selectively the in- 1985, for references). It is assumed that the pulse
ferior olive by means of an intraperitoneal injec- generator conveys its signals also to a neural
tion of 3-acetylpyridine (Balaban, 1985; Desc1in integrator that transforms the phasic activity in
and Escubi, 1974). On the other hand, the saccadic a tonic one. Also, this tonic activity is then trans-
system provides a suitable model to study motor mitted to the motoneurons and is responsible

215
216 Piergiorgio Strata et al.

for holding the eye position reached by the GS Effect of Inferior Olive Lesion
(Robinson, 1975, 1989).
The tonic activity tends to decrease with time In the intact rat, the slow postsaccadic drift re-
and, therefore, in the darkness the eye presents a corded in the dark (Fig. 11.1 E) has an exponential-
slow postsaccadic drift toward the central position like time course that is better defined for more
in the orbit. This drift is attributed to an intrinsic eccentric saccades. To calculate the time constant
leakiness of the neural integrator (Robinson, of such a drift in the dark, we followed two dif-
1974). By measuring the time constant of this ferent methods: a) We averaged the time constant
slow drift it is possible to determine the character- values determined on single randomly selected
istics of the integrator. In the light, the eye position saccades of different amplitudes and eccentricities.
reached by the GS is maintained because the step In this case the time constant was 1567 ms (± 829
is kept constant by the superposition of an opto- SD); b) We averaged 20 spontaneous saccades
kinetic reflex. In other words, the leakiness of the starting from the midline and ending at 10° of
neural integrator is compensated by the visual eccentricity. With this method the value was
information elicited by the image slip on the retina, 4228 ms. These values are considerably shorter
when the eye tends to deviate from the position than that of more than 20 s determined in higher
reached by the GS. mammals including man and monkey (Becker
Saccades performed in the light in the absence and Klein, 1973; Robinson, 1974; Zee et aI., 1981).
of a lesion often show a fast postsaccadic drift, This fact indicates that in the dark the rat has a
which usually is of less than 10. It may appear poor ability to hold the eye in a lateral position.
either as a forward or backward postsaccadic Following inferior olive lesion, the small, fast,
drift and is due to a nonperfect matching of the postsaccadic drift becomes pronounced and is
pulse and the step of innervation (Bahill et aI., always in the backward direction. In the dark it
1975a; Chelazzi et aI., 1989; Optican and is followed by the slow drift (Fig. 11.1 F) (Hess
Robinson, 1980). Significant alterations of such et aI., 1988; Tempia et aI., 1989). The time constant
a matching are reflected by a corresponding in- of the slow drift calculated with the first method
crease of postsaccadic drift amplitude. Thus, the showed a significantly shorter value of 561.8
measure of the drift, particularly in pathological (± 211.4 SD) (Student's t test, p < .001). Also the
conditions, may be taken as an index of the under- value calculated with the second method was
lying neural abnormalities. remarkably reduced, to 895 ms. From these data
Since most of the saccadic parameters in the we may conclude that the integrity of the inferior
rat have not been described in the literature, we olive is essential to maintain the time constant
have first characterized the spontaneous saccades of the neural integrator within its physiological
in seven intact rats (Chelazzi et aI., 1989). Control range.
values are reported below as a reference, together Saccades recorded in the light from intact rats
with those obtained in the rats with different types usually show a good postsaccadic stability of the
of lesion. In five rats the inferior olive was des- gaze (Fig. 11.1 A). Fast exponential postsaccadic
troyed by means of 3-acetylpyridine (68-75 mg/ drifts, when present, are of a very small amplitude
kg IP) (Balaban, 1985; Desclin and Escubi, 1974). and directed either onward or backward. Their
In two other groups of five rats each, the floccu- time constant had an average value of 113 ms
lus-paraflocculus or the posterior vermis (lobuli (± 33.6 SD). Following inferior olive lesion, on
VI, VII, and VIII) was ablated by suction. Record- the contrary, every saccade presented a very large
ing of spontaneous ocular movements has been fast postsaccadic drift, always in the backward
performed in the head-restrained condition 4 weeks direction (Fig. 11.1 B), as previously reported at a
to 6 months after the inferior olive lesion and 1 qualitative level by Hess et ai. (1988). Its time
week to 6 months after cerebellar lesions. Eye constant was 101.3 ms (± 40.7 SD) and it was not
position has been recorded by means of a phase- significantly different from that of the intact rat.
detection, search coil system (Kasper et aI., The fact that following inferior olive lesion the
1987). fast postsaccadic drift was in the backward direc-
11. Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements 217

LIGHT DARKNESS
A E

Control

10 lesion

C G

V lesion

D H

F-PF lesion
_t----

~200
5s
Figure 11.1. Eye position records in the light (left side) vermis (C and G) or of the flocculus-paraflocculus (D
and in the dark (right side) from a normal rat (A and and H). In F and H, the initial fast postsaccadic drift
E), from a rat with inferior olive lesion (B and F), and and the following slow centripetal drift are easily re-
from animals with a lesion of the dorsal cerebellar cognizable.

tion indicates that there is a pulse-step mismatch, with large eccentricities. However, such a mech-
where the pulse is larger relative to the step. The anism cannot account for the entire phenomenon,
increased leakiness of the neural integrator, by since a large postsaccadic drift was also present
shortening the time constant of the slow postsac- when the saccades were centripetally directed.
cadic drift, may also contribute to the amplitude In order to ascertain whether the mismatch is
of the fast drift, particularly for centrifugal saccades due to a larger pulse or to a smaller step, we have
218 Piergiorgio Strata et al.

compared the average amplitude ofthe GS of the Therefore, the integrity of the inferior olive is
intact rats with that of the rats with lesion of the important for the pulse-step matching of the rat.
inferior olive, both in the light and in the dark. The next step was to study the quantitative
In the intact rats the average values were, res- relationship between the pulse and the step of
pectively, 13.2° (± 2.2 SD) and 9.20 (± 2.0 SD). innervation in the rats with lesion of the inferior
In the animals with the lesion of the inferior olive olive for saccades of different amplitudes and
such values were, respectively, 14.4° (± 2.5 SD) eccentricities. The GS amplitude represents fairly
and 11.8° (± 3.3 SD). Statistical analysis revealed well the pulse, whereas more difficult is the deter-
no significant difference between the two groups. mination of the amplitude of the step, which is
The gaze position held in the light relative to represented by the initial eye position value ofthe
the position before the saccade may be defined as slow component of the postsaccadic drift in the
gaze-sustained deviation (GSD). In the intact rats dark. However, such a position is difficult to
the average value was 13.4° (± 1.6SD) and it was determine in the rat with lesion of the inferior
significantly different (p < .001) from that of 8.l ° olive because of the presence of the fast post-
(± 1.9 SD) measured after the lesion of the inferior saccadic drift of large amplitude, which is well
olive (Fig. 11.2). The steady position after a sac- evident for more than 200 ms. Therefore, in order
cade, which is maintained by visual feedback sig- to ascertain the step amplitude we have used
nals, depends on the step amplitude and on the different approaches.
leakage of the neural integrator. When the sac- 1. In the first experiment, we averaged 10 sac-
cades end near the midline, however, such a posi- cades recorded in the dark starting from the mid-
tion does not depend on the leakage. Since these line and ending at 10° of eccentricity. Since the
saccades also presented a large postsaccadic drift time constant of the fast postsaccadic drift is
in the backward direction, the step amplitude about lOOms, its contribution to the postsaccadic
must be smaller relative to the pulse amplitude. drift is over after 400ms. We then extrapolated

'""'
till
Q) 20 I:=:J GSD ~ GS
"0
'-'
I.Ll
0
:J 15
t:
....J
Q..
10
~
z
<C
I.Ll 5
:E

ROL FLO
PARAF
I F RJOR POSTERI R
IV V RMI
Figure 11.2. Mean GS and GSD amplitudes of sac- amplitude is significantly reduced after lesion of the
cades recorded in the light from control rats (control) inferior olive and of the flocculus- paraflocculus (p <
and after lesion of the inferior olive, of the flocculus- .001). (Reprinted with permission of Oxford University
paraflocculus, and of the posterior vermis. The GSD Press from Chelazzi et aI., 1990.)
11. Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements 219

,/

/
25 /.

/ •

./
...
/
.
::: ..... .
Figure 11.3. Relationship between the GSD /'
15 •• •
....:: :......
:. .. .
and the saccadic GS for one rat with lesion /'
of the inferior olive. The dashed line repre-
sents the ideal relationship with an angular
coefficient of 1; sacca des of normal rats, not
/.~:
~...
••
/
..• • ••
. •

I •. . ••
displayed, are well fitted by this line. The 5 /.: .::-:
solid regression line which is the best fitting •••

for this animal, crosses the abscissa at
a positive value and has a slope of 0.72.
(Reprinted with permission of Oxford 5 15 25
University Press from Strata et aI., 1990.) G (deg

the entire time course of the slow postsaccadic coefficient of 0.72 and crossed the abscissa at a
drift from that part of the postsaccadic drift start- positive value. These results show that a linear
ing at 400 ms. From this extrapolation, we found correlation between these two parameters is main-
that the eye position from which the slow post- tained after the lesion. However, since the more
saccadic drift would start at the end of the GS eccentric saccades are also those with a higher
was 7.6°. Therefore, we may conclude that for amplitude, it is not clear how much of the post-
these saccades of 10° of amplitude and eccentri- saccadic drift depends on the amplitude and how
city, the gain of the pulse to step transformation much on the eccentricity.
is 0.76 and therefore the contribution of the mis- Therefore, we studied a) the relationship be-
match to the postsaccadic drift is 2.4°. We also tween the postsaccadic drift and the saccadic
averaged similar saccades performed in the light amplitude for classes of saccades with similar
and the GSD was 5.2°, with a consequent ampli- eccentricities, and b) the relationship between the
tude of the postsaccadic drift of 4.8°. Therefore, postsaccadic drift and the eccentricity for classes
the contribution of the leakage of the neural inte- of saccades of similar amplitudes.
grator to the postsaccadic drift is also 2.4°. In the first case there was a linear relationship
2. A second experiment was aimed at deter- between the drift amplitude and the GS amplitude
mining the gain of the pulse to step transforma- (Fig. 11.4A-E). Since in each plot the eccentricity
tion and the relative contribution of the leakage is constant, the average slope of all the regression
of the neural integrator to the postsaccadic drift lines represents how much the postsaccadic drift
in the light as a function of the amplitude and the increases with the increase of the GS amplitude.
eccentricity of the saccades. Such a slope had an average value of 0.21 and
Figure 11.3 illustrates the relationship between therefore the gain of the pulse to step transfor-
the GSD and the GS for saccades of different mation was 0.79. This value is almost the same
amplitudes and eccentricities in one rat. Their as that obtained in the previous experiment.
ratio indicates the gain of the postsaccadic posi- Figure 11.4A-E shows another interesting
tion in the light (Strata et ai., 1990). In the intact result. When the saccadic amplitude tends to
rats, this gain is near 1, since the postsaccadic zero, the component of the postsaccadic drift due
drift amplitude is very small; it is represented by to the pulse-step mismatch also tends to zero.
the interrupted line having an angular coefficient Therefore, the Y intercepts provide an indication
of 1 and crossing the zero of the cartesian coordi- ofthe amount ofthe postsaccadic drift that is due
nates. Following inferior olive lesion, the regres- to the leakage of the neural integrator. The values
sion line (continuous line) presented an angular of the Y intercepts increased linearly with the
220 Piergiorgio Strata et aI.

A B C
ECC 1-3 ECC 4-6 ECC 7-9
8: .6 b: .26 r: .93 8: 1.78 b: .2 r: .95 8: 1.89 b: .21 r: .87
15 15 15

10 10 10

5 5 5

0 0
10 20 30 0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30

0 E F
ECC 10-12 ECC 13-15
r: .77 3.07 b: .2 r: .79 6
8: 3.16 b: .16 8:

15 .- 5
15
~

10 -°
.-

c
(/)
c:J
4

-
(/) 10 °0 3 " "
a..
III
C
2
5 5 (/)
a.. 1
"
0 0 0
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30 0 5 10 15 20
GS (0) Eccentricity (0)
Figure 11.4 A-E: Postsaccadic drift (PSD) in the light the Y intercept "a," and the correlation coefficient "r,"
as a function of GS amplitude for different classes of together with the eccentricity range, are reported on
saccades with similar eccentricities after inferior olive each plot. Note the increase of the Y intercept (extra-
lesion. Each class includes all the saccades within 3° of polated PSD for hypothetical saccades of 0° of GS)
eccentricity (ECC). The best fitting regression lines along with the increase of eccentricity as plotted in F.
have been calculated for the plotted mean drift ampli- (Reprinted with permission of Oxford University Press
tude values. Each triangle represents the mean (± 1 SD) from Strata et aI., 1990.)
of at least three saccades. The angular coefficient "b,"

increase of the eccentricity, with a slope of 0.21 tion of the mismatch to the postsaccadic drift is
(Fig. 11.4F). This means that the amount of post- the same for all the saccades of the same class.
saccadic drift due to the leakage of the neural Thus, the increase of the postsaccadic drift with
integrator increases 0.21 for each degree of eccen-
0
the increase of the eccentricity is due to the leakage
tricity. This value also coincides with that ob- of the neural integrator. Such a contribution was
tained in the previous experiment. also linearly related to the eccentricity with an
Figure 11.5A-G shows that there was a linear average slope of 0.17. This means that for each
correlation between the postsaccadic drift ampli- degree of eccentricity, there is 0.17 of drift due 0

tude and the saccadic eccentricity for classes of to the leakage.


saccades of defined amplitudes. Since in each plot From Figure 11.5 it is clear that the Y intercepts
the saccadic amplitude is constant, the contribu- provide an indication of the value of the post-
11. Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements 221

A B c
GS 1-3 GS 4-6 GS 7-9
8:.5 b: .34 r:.9 8: 1.93 b: .11 r: .74 8: 2.41 b: .18 r: .97
15 15 15

10 10 10

5 5

o~----~---------- o~----~----------~
o 10 20 30 o 10 20 30

D E F
GS 10-12 GS 13-15 GS 16-18
8: 3.58 b: .14 r: .78 8: 4.18 b: .08 r:.4 8: 3.88 b: .21 r: .72
15 15 15

10 10 10

5 5

o~----~----~--~ o~----~----------~ O+-----~----~----


o 10 20 30 o 10 20 30 o 10 20 30

G H

---
GS 19-21
6

-...
8: 5.18 b: .14 r: .5
°

/
15 >- 5

-
'u
4

---°
c: A/
10 CD
()
()
W
3 /
-
Q
UJ 'b 2
a.. 5
as
Q 1
UJ
0 a.. o~--~----~--~--~
0 10 20 30 o 5 15 20
Eccentricity (0)

Figure IISA-G: Postsaccadic drift (PSD) amplitude regression lines have the angular coefficient "b," the Y
in the light as a function of eccentricity for different intercept "a," and the correlation coefficient "r" re-
classes of saccades with similar amplitudes after in- ported above each graph, together with the GS range.
ferior olive lesion. Each class includes all the saccades H: Extrapolated PSD values of saccades of 0 of eccen-
0

within 3 ofGS. Each triangle represents the mean drift


0
tricity as a function of GS. (Reprinted with permission
amplitude (± 1 SD) of at least three saccades. The of Oxford University Press from Strata et aI., 1990.)
222 Piergiorgio Strata et al.

saccadic drift due to the mismatch. Since the rats with lesion of the inferior olive. Also the
values of the Y intercepts increased linearly with amplitudes of the GS in the dark were not signifi-
the increase of the amplitude with a slope of 0.24 cantly different in the four groups of rats.
(Fig. l1.5H), we may conclude that the contribu- As far as the time constant of the slow postsac-
tion of the pulse-step mismatch to the postsac- cadic drift is concerned, its value in the animals
cadic drift is 0.24° for each degree of amplitude. with vermal lesions was 1787 ms (± 1246 SD)
In other words, the gain ofthe pulse to step trans- when determined on randomly selected saccades,
formation is 0.76 at all saccadic amplitudes. This whereas when calculated on 20 averaged saccades
value also fits with the values calculated in the of 10° of amplitude and eccentricity, its value was
two previous experiments. 4188 ms. These values are not significantly dif-
The above series of experiments shows that ferent from those of the intact rats. In contrast,
following inferior olive lesion the saccades per- following the lesion to the flocculus-paraflocculus
formed by the rat have a post saccadic drift with the value obtained with the first procedure was
an amplitude that depends both on a reduced gain 675.7ms (±208.8SD) and significantly reduced
(about 0.77) of the pulse to step transformation (p <.001). Also, the value obtained with the second
and on an increased leakiness of the neural inte- method was remarkably reduced, to 888 ms.
grator. The latter contributes linearly, with a slope However, there was no significant difference be-
of 0.21 to 0.24. tween the time constant measured in the rats with
Finally, we studied the peak velocity and the lesion of the inferior olive and of the flocculus-
duration of saccades as a function of the GS am- paraflocculus.
plitude. Such functions, called the main sequence In the rats with flocculus-paraflocculus lesion
(Bahill et ai., 1975b), were not affected by the we also attempted to define the relative contri-
inferior olive lesion either in the light or the dark. bution of the pulse-step mismatch and of the
leakage of the neural integrator to the postsac-
cadic drift. To this aim, we performed a similar
Effect of the Lesion of the series of experiments; the results may be sum-
Posterior Vermis and of the marized as follows: The gain of the pulse to step
Flocculus-Paraflocculus transformation was 0.79 at all saccadic ampli-
tudes. In addition, the contribution ofthe leakage
Following lesion of the posterior vermis, there of the neural integrator to the postsaccadic drift
were no significant changes in the parameters that was 0.13 0 to 0.23° for each degree of eccentricity.
we investigated (Fig. 11.1 C, G). By contrast, fol- These findings are also very similar to those ob-
lowing lesion of the flocculus-paraflocculus tained following inferior olive lesion.
(Fig. I1.1D, H), there were remarkable abnorma- Finally, we found no significant alterations in
lities in the spontaneous saccades, which are strik- the saccadic main sequence both in the light and
ingly similar to those observed after inferior olive in the dark following flocculus-paraflocculus
lesion. lesion.
In fact, in the rats with lesion of the flocculus-
paraflocculus the spontaneous saccades performed
in the light showed a fast post saccadic drift in the
Discussion
backward direction (Fig. 11.1 D). Following this Our experiments show, first of all, that the intact
type of lesion, the average amplitude of the GS head-restrained pigmented rat has a remarkable
in the light (12S, ± 4.0 SD) is not significantly spontaneous saccadic activity both in the light
different from that obtained in the other three and in the dark. The saccades are characterized
experimental conditions, whereas the amplitude by relatively high peak velocities and large ampli-
of the GSD (7.4°, ± 2.9 SD) was significantly re- tudes, whereas the gaze-holding ability in the
duced with respect to that of the intact rats and dark is rather poor (Chelazzi et ai., 1989).
of the rats with dorsal vermis lesion (p < .001 in Concerning the cerebellar influence on the sac-
both cases; Fig. 11.2). However, the GSD was not cadic performance, we have shown that the lesion
significantly different from that calculated in the of the vermis does not induce any significant
11. Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements 223

alteration. That saccadic peak velocity and dura- and on the GS amplitude. It is likely that in the
tion are not affected by this lesion is not unexpec- monkey the leakage contribution is rather modest,
ted since it has already been shown in the monkey since the time constant of the neural integrator
that these parameters are unchanged even after is relatively longer with respect to the rat, even
total cerebellectomy (Optican and Robinson, after the flocculus-paraflocculus lesion. This fact,
1980) or cooling of the medial cerebellar nuclei however, does not explain a gain higher than 1
(Vilis and Hore, 1981). However, a saccadic dys- of the postsaccadic position in the monkey.
metria has been described. When in the monkey From our experiments we may draw the con-
the intracerebellar nuclei are spared, the dysmetria clusion that the flocculus-paraflocculus area is
depends on the saccadic direction (Ritchie, 1976), responsible for enhancing the time constant of
whereas when the lesion includes the nuclei, the the neural integrator and the gain of the pulse to
monkey shows a saccadic hypermetria (Optican step transformation. Therefore, at least in the rat,
and Robinson, 1980). The lack of a significant the flocculus-paraflocculus region is important
variation in the average amplitude of the GS in to improve the poor gaze-holding capabilities of
our experiments may be due to different experi- the oculomotor system.
mental conditions, since we have analyzed sponta- Our experiments have also shown that the inte-
neous saccades made to explore the environment, grity of the inferior olive is essential for main-
whereas in the quoted studies target-directed sac- taining the leakiness of the neural integrator
cades were recorded from trained monkeys. How- within the physiological range and the gain ofthe
ever, it is also possible that in the rat accurate pulse to step transformation at its proper value.
saccades are not important and therefore they The effects of the inferior olive lesion are also
are not corrected by the cerebellum. It is known similar to those of the flocculus-paraflocculus
that the rat has no fovea or any other high acuity lesion from a quantitative point of view. There-
retinal area. fore, it is likely that the inferior olive controls the
Our results obtained following flocculus- ocular motility through the action exerted on the
paraflocculus lesion are in part similar to those flocculo-parafloccular Purkinje cells. The simi-
reported in the monkey, where a considerable larity between the results of inferior olive and
reduction of the time constant of the neural inte- flocculus-paraflocculus lesions is unexpected and
grator has been reported (Zee et aI., 1981). How- is worth further discussion.
ever, they are different as far as the postsaccadic One simple explanation would be to assume
drift is concerned. In the monkey, this drift is of that following inferior olive lesion, the inhibition
a small amplitude, up to a maximum of 15% of exerted by the Purkinje cells on their target cells
the GS amplitude (Zee et aI., 1981), whereas in is completely abolished. An almost complete loss
our experiments it averages around 40% of the of inhibitory action of these cells has been reported
GS. In addition, in the monkey the drift is both (Ito et aI., 1979) and has been attributed to a loss
in the onward and in the backward direction with of a trophic factor released by the climbing fibers
a predominance of the former one, whereas in our (Ito, 1984) or to an irreversible damage of the
experiments it is always in the backward direction. Purkinje cells (Rossi et aI., 1987) due to the pro-
A possible explanation for this discrepancy may longed hyperactivity that follows the inferior
be that in monkey experiments the extent of the olive lesion for more than 1 month (Benedetti
flocculus-paraflocculus lesion was variable in dif- et aI., 1984; Montarolo et aI., 1982). Such a loss,
ferent animals and not complete, whereas in our however, is only partial (Karachot et aI., 1987;
experiments in the rats it was possible to ablate Montarolo et aI., 1981) and it seems unlikely that
this cerebellar area totally and consistently. such a partial loss is equivalent to a total Purkinje
Another major feature of our experiments is that cell removal.
in the rat the postsaccadic drift in the light is due A second possibility is that the action of the
in part to the reduced gain of the pulse to step inferior olive is not mediated by the olivocere-
transformation and in part to the increased leak- bellar fibers, but by their collaterals to the brain
age of the neural integrator, the relative contri- stem. Such collaterals would degenerate as a con-
bution depending on the saccadic eccentricity sequence of the retrograde degeneration of the
224 Piergiorgio Strata et al.

olivary cells, which follows flocculus-parafloccu- Acknowledgments. This work has been supported
Ius removal. This hypothesis should be tested by by grants from C.N.R. and M.P.1.. We are in-
lesioning the Purkinje cells by means of kainic debted to Mr. M. Orlandini and Ms. G. Milano
acid, which prevents the inferior olive degener- for their assistance.
ation (Ito et aI., 1980) and allows the survival of
the climbing fiber terminal arborizations (Rossi References
et aI., 1991).
Previous experiments have demonstrated that Bahill, A.T., Clark, M.R., and Stark, L. (1975a): Glis-
an acute reversible inactivation of this nucleus sades-eye movements generated by mismatched
leads to increased firing frequency ofthe Purkinje components of the saccadic motoneuronal con-
trol signal. Math. Biosci., 26,303-318.
cells (Montarolo et al., 1982) and decreased firing
Bahill, A.T., Clark, M.R., and Stark, L. (1975b): The
of their target neurons in the intracerebellar and main sequence, a tool for studying human eye move-
vestibular nuclei (Benedetti et aI., 1983; De'Sperati ments. Math. Biosci., 24,191-204.
et aI., 1985). On the basis of these data, it has been Balaban, e.D. (1985): Central neurotoxic effects of
proposed that the inferior olive is able to change intra peritoneally administered 3-acetylpyridine,
the gain of any system under cerebellar control harmaline and niacinamide in Sprague-Dawley and
(Strata and Montarolo, 1982). Evidence for this Long-Evans rats: A critical review of central 3-
assumption has been provided (De'Sperati et aI., acetyl pyridine neurotoxicity. Brain Res. Rev., 9,
1985; see Strata, 1987; Strata et aI., 1989). One 21-42.
month after the inferior olive lesion, however, Becker, W., and Klein, H.M. (1973): Accuracy of sac-
the firing frequency of the Purkinje cells and of cadic eye movements and maintenance of eccentric
eye position in the dark. Vision Res., 13, 1021-1034.
their target neurons is again at the control level
Benedetti, F., Montarolo, P.G., and Rabacchi, S. (1984):
(Benedetti et aI., 1984; Lopiano and Savio, 1986).
Inferior olive lesion induces long-lasting functional
Since in our experiments the ocular movements modifications in the Purkinje cells. Exp. Brain Res.,
have been recorded at least 1 month after the 55,368-371.
olivary lesion, the ocular deficits we have de- Benedetti, F., Montarolo, P.G., Strata, P., and Tempia,
scribed are likely not due to such a change of F. (1983): Inferior olive inactivation decreases the
spontaneous firing. Instead, they may be due to excitability of the intracerebellar and lateral vesti-
a direct active involvement of the inferior olive bular nuclei in the rat. 1. Physiol. (Lond.), 340,
in controlling the leakiness of the neural inte- 195-208.
grator and the gain of the pulse to step transfor- Carpenter, R.H.S. (1988): Movements of the Eyes.
mation. London: Pion.
We suggest that visual information carried to Chelazzi, L., Rossi, F., Tempia, F" Ghirardi, M., and
Strata, P. (1989): Saccadic eye movements and gaze
the flocculus-paraflocculus through the dorsal holding in the head-restrained pigmented rat. Eur.
cap of the inferior olive plays a specific role in 1. Neurosci., 1,639-646.
contributing to the maintenance of proper eye Che1azzi L., Ghirardi, M., Rossi, F., Strata P., and
position after the GS and therefore stabilizes the Tempia, F. (1990): Spontaneous saccades and gaze
gaze. Such a role may be carried out in two ways. holding ability in the pigmented rat: II. Effect of
First, the inferior olive may exert an on-line effect localized cerebellar lesions. Eur. 1. N eurosci., 2,
on the gaze-holding ability, and second, it may 1085-1094.
be essential to maintain the system in a calibrated Desclin, J.e., and Escubi, J. (1974): Effects of 3-acetyl-
condition. In fact, the flocculus seems essential pyridine on the central nervous system of the rat as
fQr the adaptive control of the postsaccadic drift demonstrated by silver methods. Brain Res., 77,
in primates (Optican et aI., 1986). In addition, 349-364.
De'Sperati, e., Montarolo, P.G., and Strata, P. (1985):
recent experiments have shown that a consist- Inferior olive control of the dynamic responses of
ent mismatch appears when vision is suppressed the medial vestibular neurones to labyrinthine stiml.l-
(Viirre et aI., 1987) or when a visual slip is intro- lation in the rat. 1. Physiol. (Lond.), 369, 48p.
duced artificially at the end of a saccade (Kapoula Fuchs, A.F., Kaneko, e.R.S., and Scudder, e.A. (1985):
et aI., 1989; Optican and Miles 1985). Brainstem control of saccadic eye movements. Annu.
Rev. Neurosci., 8, 307-337.
11. Cerebellar Control of Saccadic Eye Movements 225

Hess, BtJ.M., Savio, T., and Strata, P. (1988): Dynamic the eat's vestibulo-ocular integrator. Brain Res., 71,
characteristics of optokinetically controlled eye 195-207.
movements following inferior olive lesions in the Robinson, D.A. (1975): Oculomotor control signals.
brown rat. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 397, 349-370. In: Basic Mechanisms oj Ocular Motility and
Ito, M. (1984): The Cerebellum and Neural Control. Their Clinical Implications (G. Lennerstrand and
New York: Raven Press. P. Bach-y-Rita, eds). Oxford: Pergamon Press,
Ito, M., Jastreboff, P.J., and Miyashita, Y. (1980): pp. 337-374.
Retrograde influence of surgical and chemical floc- Robinson, D.A. (1989): Integrating with neurons. Annu.
culectomy upon dorsal cap neurons of the inferior Rev. Neurosci. 12,33-45.
olive. Neurosci. Lett., 20,45-48. Rossi, F., Cantino, D., and Strata, P. (1987): Morpho-
Ito, M., Nisimaru, N., and Shibuki, K. (1979): Destruc- logy of Purkinje cell axon terminals in intra-
tion of inferior olive induces rapid depression in cerebellar nuclei following inferior olive lesion.
synaptic action of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature, Neuroscience, 22, 99-112.
277,568-569. Rossi, F., Vaudano, E., Borsello, T., Bravin, M., and
Kapoula, Z., Optican, L.M., and Robinson, D.A. (1989): Strata, P. (1991): Target-deprived climbing fibres
Visually induced plasticity of postsaccadic ocular are maintained for a long time in the cerebellar
drift in normal humans. J. Neurophysiol., 61, 879- cortex of the adult rat. Eur. J. Neurosci., Suppl. 4,
891. 165.
Karachot, L., Ito, M., and Kanai, Y. (1987): Long-term Strata, P. (1987): Inferior olive and motor control. In:
effects of 3-acetylpyridine-induced destruction of Cerebellum and Neuronal Plasticity (M. Glickstein,
cerebellar climbing fibers on Purkinje cells inhibition J. Stein, and C. Yeo, eds). New York-London:
of vestibulospinal tract cells of the rat. Exp. Brain Plenum, pp. 209-223.
Res., 66, 229-246. Strata, P., Chelazzi, L., Ghirardi, M., Rossi, F., and
Kasper, H.I., Hess, B.I.M., and Dieringer, N. (1987): A Tempia, F. (1990): Spontaneous saccades and gaze
precise and inexpensive magnetic field search coil holding ability in the pigmented rat: I. Effects of
system for measuring eye and head movements in inferior olive lesion. Eur. J. N eurosci., 2, 1074-1084.
small laboratory animals. J. Neurosci. Meth., 19, Strata, P., and Montarolo, P.G. (1982): Functional
115-124. aspects of the inferior olive. Arch. Ital. Bioi., 120,
Leigh, R.I., and Zee, D.S. (1983): The Neurology oJEye 321-329.
Movements .. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis. Strata, P., Montarolo, P.G., and Hess, B.1.M. (1989):
Lopiano, L., and Savio, T. (1986): Inferior olive lesion Role of inferior olive in the control of eye move-
induces long-term modifications of cerebellar inhi- ments. In: The Olivocerebellar System in Motor
bition on Deiters nuclei. N eurosci. Res., 4, 51-61. Control (P. Strata, ed). Exp. Brain Res. Series 17.
Montarolo, P.G., Palestini, M., and Strata, P. (1982): Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, London, Paris,
The inhibitory effect of the olivocerebellar input on Tokyo: Springer-Verlag, pp. 281-293.
the cerebellar Purkinje cells in the rat. J. Physiol. Tempia, F., Chelazzi, L., Rossi, F., Ghirardi, M., and
(Lond.),332,187-202. Strata, P. (1989): Spontaneous saccades in the pig-
Montarolo, P.G., Raschi, F., and Strata, P. (1981): Are mented rat after inferior olive lesion. In: The Olivo-
the climbing fibres essential for the Purkinje cell cerebellar System in Motor Control (P. Strata, ed).
inhibitory action? Exp. Brain Res., 42, 215-218. Exp. Brain Res. Series 17. Berlin, Heidelberg, New
Optican, L.M., and Miles, F.A. (1985): Visually induced York, London, Paris, Tokyo: Springer-Verlag,
adaptive changes in primate saccadic oculomotor pp.294-298.
control signals, J. Neurophysiol., 54, 940-958. Viirre, E., Cadera, W., and Vilis, T. (1987): The pattern
Optican, L.M., and Robinson, D.A. (1980): Cerebellar of changes produced in the saccadic system and
dependent adaptive control of the primate saccadic vestibuloocular reflex by visually patching one eye.
system. J. Neurophysiol., 44,1058-1076. J. Neurophysiol., 57, 92-103.
Optican, L.M., Zee, D.S., and Miles, F.A. (1986): Floc- Vilis, T., and Hore, 1. (1981): Characteristics of saccadic
cular lesions abolish adaptive control of post-sac- dysmetria in monkeys during reversible lesions of
cadic ocular drift in primates. Exp. Brain Res., 64, medial cerebellar nuclei. J. Neurophysiol., 46, 828-
596-598. 838.
Ritchie, L. (1976): Effects of cerebellar lesions on sac- Zee, D.s., Yamazaki, A., Butler, P.H., and GUcer, G.
cadic eye movements. J. Neurophysiol., 39, 1246- (1981): Effects of ablation of flocculus and parafloc-
1256. culus on eye movements in primate. J. Neurophy-
Robinson, D.A. (1974): The effect of cerebellectomy on siol., 46, 878-899.
12
A Possible Connection Between the
Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems
at Precere bellar Level
Francisco J. Rubia

This chapter addresses the possibility that there is that the CF system, as revealed by the recording
a precerebellar connection between the two main of the CS of the PC during passive displacement
inputs to the cerebellum, the mossy (MF) and the of the forelimb, is able to transmit very precise
climbing fiber (CF) system. The idea that both information about all the parameters of the
afferent systems could be interconnected before movement.
their entrance into the cerebellum came to us as With respect to the question of whether the CF
we analyzed the results of extracellular recordings system is conveying to the cerebellum more tonic
of Purkinje cells (PC), especially the behavior of or phasic information, there are also differences
their two different spikes, the complex spike (CS) in the literature. Most authors agree that the CF
and the simple spike (SS), and their relationship system is more likely to transmit phasic informa-
in two different preparations: the decerebrate cat tion. However, there are reports of the system's
and the awake Rhesus monkey. capability to signal tonic events also. For example,
Our intent in recording PCs from the decere- Ferin et al. (1971) reported a tonic increase of the
brate cat was to analyze the capability of the CF CS activity of the PC during or after the end of
system to signal peripheral events. Several authors caloric and galvanic stimulation of the labyrinth,
have reported that the CF system is able to trans- and in the frog cerebellum Llimls et al. (1971)
mit to the cerebellum information from the pe- observed tonic increase in the CS responses during
riphery: so did Eccles et al. (1972a-d) and Leicht horizontal angular acceleration. Tonic modula-
et al. (1973), with regard to activation of cuta- tion of the CS activity was also observed during
neous, muscle, or mechanoreceptors of the fore- different sleep states by Marchesi and Strata
limb and hindlimb. Similar results were obtained (1970), Mano (1970), and Hobson and McCarley
by Ishikawa et al. (1972a, b) with muscle stretch (1972). Marini et al. (1976) showed tonic modula-
and by Maekawa and Simpson (1972) and Simpson tion of the CS in the PCs of the nodulus in
and Alley (1974) with teleceptive stimulation. response to rotation of the animal around its
Rushmer et al. (1976) also reported that the CF longitudinal axis.
system was activated with very small displace-
ments of the forelimb. These last authors recorded Results Obtained
the CS responses of the PC and observed that
they appeared to be independent of the quality,
in the Decerebrate Cat
amplitude, and direction ofthe movement. Thus, Using the decerebrate cat, we were able to show
they concluded that the CF system acted as an that the CF system can be modulated phasically
"event detector," signaling only the touching of and tonically in response to passive movements
the limb to the ground or its lifting during loco- of the extremity.
motion. The experimental procedure has been described
In our own studies, however, we have found elsewhere (Rubia and Kolb, 1978). The passive

226
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 227

movement was elicited by a DC motor attached The extracellular recording of the PC was per-
to the forelimb, so that the foot pad moved around formed only within the CI zone of lobule V, the
the wrist joint, the rest of the arm being fixed. The representation area of the forelimb in the anterior
movement followed in most cases a ramp function lobe of the cerebellum. The two spikes of the PC
beginning at a starting position and proceeding were reliably separated with an electronic device
to an end position with constant velocity. The and peristimulus time histograms (PSTH) were
movement was reproduced exactly in order to constructed.
average a sufficient number of sweeps and in this An example of the capability of the CF system
way collect as many CS responses as possible to transmit peripheral information is shown in
from the Pc. Figure 12.1. In this figure (Fig. 12.1B), the CS of

c
::J J \w=t.oo/s

A
]L..::.: +20
-IS

z -IS
'PH iii -10
"-
o· VI
~
Z
:l
0
u
'PL .10
+ 5

B
i i
o 2.5 55
-20·
] ~w=t.oo/S D
-20. J ~
COUNTS/BIN

~] n=80

i
o 25 55 o -10 -20
'P /degl
Figure 12.1. Response of a PC to passive movement The number of sweeps is n = 80. C: The response of
of the cat's forepaw. A: A DC motor-driven device another PC to displacements of 5° at different starting
moves the forepaw around the wrist joint from a low positions. D: The mean probability of CS discharge
(L) to a high (H) position and back. 0° is the horizontal with 95% confidence limits for upper (H) or lower (L)
plane. B: response of the PC with the CS is illustrated. positions. (From Kolb and Rubia, 1980, with permis-
A PSTH shows the CS response to a ramp movement sion.)
(from - 20° to + 20°) with constant velocity (40°Is).
228 Francisco 1. Rubia

o
-..t
II
C

o
If)
II
'IS o
NIS/SINnO:J
. III
If)
:

_l
~ .0 : • J _ . OJ 00 O.'J o J LI1
II N
C

-"~.::1 -" ..... J-. "oJ ".:.. J ·0-: J


.. o 0

o
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 229

the rec(i)rded PC signals the end position ( + 20°, CF system can inform the cerebellum about the
0° being the horizontal plane) with a tonic increase limb position at which the movement has oc-
in the firing frequency, the positive acceleration curred.
of the upward movement, the velocity, and the This observation can be seen better in Figure
direction of the movement. The mean frequency 12.2. Here, another PC responded with the CS to
of the tonic discharge increased about five times the same movement performed at different limb
when the upper position was changed from + 5° positions. At the highest positions, the discharge
to + 20°, In Figure 12.1 C we can see that the frequency is also higher, and at lower positions
response of another PC during the upward move- the dispersion of the CS responses, as shown in
ment is different depending on the position at the dot raster in Figure 12.2A, is greater,
which the movement has been performed, Al- Therefore, the static parameter position can
though the displacement (5°) and the velocity be conveyed to the cerebellum by the CF system
(40° Is) are the same, the best responses are ob- in two ways: directly by a tonic increase of the
tained at the highest positions, Thus, besides the discharge frequency and indirectly in connec-
signalization of the movement parameters, the tion with a dynamic parameter, informing the

A B c
O·f~ __ /~/
______ . w =40 /s
-20. --1/
-40. --' n=45
-----
------- 0

n = 25
'(t: CD
rl.
"l
~~
'.'
IS!
10
5

' 10·
t.
I,,'
oJ
o -
• UP
~;~ ® o DOWN
:~,
" 0· 0,3
',\1, z
,I):':
.~ ',1 ..
......
iii
"'-
Vl
I-
! ·20·
0.2
.
O·T
Z
@

~j
, ::::>

f. ...... +.. .
0
::., ' U 0,1
.
. . '". ..........
"

.......... ·30· o
10 20 30 40
® 6 If> (degl

~
. :~' . ,
.,1.
,'., .
..,
.,' ..
..
.,
'.~
~
, , '0·
0 2,5 55
Figure 12.3. CS discharge of a PC in response to dis- discharge during upward and downward movement
placements of the forepaw of different amplitudes. plotted agaihst the range covered by the movement.
A: Dot raster. B: PSTH. C: Mean probability ofthe CS (From Kolb and Rubia, 1980, with permission.)

~----------------------------------------------------------------------
Figure 12.2. CS discharge of a PC in response to a and downward (md) movements. The shaded zones
displacement of 5° at different paw positions. A: Each represent the 95% confidence limits (vertical direction)
of the dots represents a CS discharge. B: PSTH of the and movement amplitude (horizontal direction). (From
CS discharge. C: Stack plot of the PSTH. D: Mean Kolb and Rubia, 1980, with permission.)
probability of the CS discharge during upward (mu)
230 Francisco J. Rubia

c;erebellum at which position a certain movement the amplitude of the movement, the CS ofthe PC
has been performed. responded changing also the discharge frequency.
The amplitude of the movement (displacement) This can be observed in Figure 12.4. By increasing
can also be transmitted to the cerebellum by the velocity, the mean probability of the unit dis-
CF system. In Figure 12.3, a PC is responding charge also increases. The conclusion from these
with the CS to a movement of constant velocity results is that the higher the velocity, the higher
(400/s) but different amplitude. We observe that also the probability of the discharge of the PC
the shorter the movement, the probability of the with its CS.
discharge is higher and the response more precise Summarizing the results shown in the last two
(Fig. 12.3A, B, specimens 1 and 2). It is interesting figures, we can say that short and rapid move-
to note that in all cases the movement covered ments are the most adequate for transmission
the displacement in specimen 1 and 2. From this through the CF system. If we observe the PSTH
type of experiment we concluded that the shorter shown in Figure 12.4B during the movements
the displacement, the better the response of the with the lowest velocities (12 and 20 /s), there is
0 0

PC with its CS. an increase in the firing rate of the CS at the


By changing the dynamic parameters we also beginning and at the end of the movement. This
obtained consistent changes in the responses of can mean that the cell is responding to both
the PC with its CS. For example, if we changed acceleration signals (positive and negative accel-
the velocity from 12° to 80° Is, by keeping constant eration) of the movement. If this were the case,

A B c

t oO \. "t, •
n: 60 w Ideg/sl
. 'i, :.'1 .
. ....:.:\ " .
'.
,
-:"'.\.;....
~12
' - - --" ''--- --'

.:- P1o.s1 !%1


.. 'oO •

,' , .: r
.) . ,-
~20 'L,- 10'
0.4
'H··
~ ';'i
.' ·r ..
t

...
10
'----' '---' • : mu •
..
o mel
'i: .
· t.:
t,...I; 40
0.2
L.....I L......J

60
'-' L.J
10 20 40 60 80
w Ideg/sl
80

I
o
I I
2.5 5s o 2.5 5s
Figure 12.4. CS discharge of a PC in response to dis- during upward and downward movement plotted against
placements of 20° with different velocities. A: Dot raster. velocity. (From Kolb and Rubia, 1980, with permission.)
B: PSTH. C: Mean probability of the CS discharge
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 231

obviously during short and rapid movements celeration signal of the movement can be trans-
both acceleration signals can be better summed, mitted well to the cerebellum via the CF system.
producing a higher CS discharge of the Pc. From these types of experiments we can con-
To test this hypothesis, we used a movement clude that the CF system is able to transmit to
with a constant amplitude of So and a logarithmic the cerebellum all the parameters of the move-
increase and decrease of the frequency (Fig. 12.S). ment and especially also at which position the
By calculating the Pearson correlation coefficient movement has been performed. The parameter
between the PSTH (Fig. 12.SA) and the move- acceleration is one of the best transmitted by this
ment function, its first, second, and third deri- system and, therefore, small and rapid movements
vatives and certain sections of the movement produce the best response in the CS of the Pc.
(Fig. 12.SB), we concluded that the best correla-
tion obtained was between the averaged discharge Results Obtained
rate of the CS and the negative acceleration. The
continuous line in the histogram of Figure 12.5A
in the Awake Monkey
represents the absolute value ofthe negative accel- It can be argued that these results could be ob-
eration. It can be observed that it fits nicely with tained only because the preparation was the de-
the response of the PC with its CS. Thus, the ac- cerebrate cat and not the intact animal. However,

A B

IS' ]
fIt) r
10°
1200 c.p (t) 0.31
pos. half wave of c.p(I) 0.32
30
n =40 neg. half wave of 'P(t) 0.25
N
z III
~ (I) - 0,06
CD "-
01
"-
Vl -0
Qj + tP (I) 0,10
t- - tp (t) - 0,20
z
::l
0
u If> It) - 0,58
0 0 + If> (I) - 0,08
,,
"' - If> (I) - 0,80
, , ~, ,, :
" ,, ,, '" ,, if> (t) - 0.D1
, ,, ,, :
, ,
.
•.", , " ,,
+ cp (t )
- if> (I)
0,22
- 0,20
.~ ~ ~

, ,
o 5
Figure 12:5. CS discharge of a PC during a sinusoidal in A and different functions (f(t)): qJ (t), sinusoidal move-
movement of 5° and logarithmically increasing and ment function; cp (t), velocity; ip (t), acceleration; (jJ (t),
decreasing frequency. A: PSTH and dot raster of the third derivative of (t). The best correlation is found
discharge. The solid line in the PSTH represents the between the averaged discharge rate and the negative
absolute value of negative acceleration. B: Pearson acceleration-ip (t). (From Kolb and Rubia, 1980, with
correlation coefficients (r) evaluated between the PSTH permission.)
232 Francisco J. Rubia

s~milar results have been obtained in the awake The possibility of comparison between passive
monkey using a similar movement around the and active movements showed us that in many
wrist joint. Furthermore, this preparation allowed cases the active movement was able to suppress
us to compare between the movement passively strongly or even cancel the response produced in
produced and a similar movement actively in- the PC by the passive movement. This can be
duced by the monkey. observed in Figure 12.7, where the CS response of
The monkeys were trained to move a lever by one PC during passive extensions of the monkey's
flexing and extending the hand in order to pursue hand was completely suppressed during similar
a light signal. The coincidence of a visual target and active extensions. The discharge rate seems to be
the light signal connected to the lever triggered increased before the movement onset, but this
the reward. Additionally, the lever could also be phenomenon can be observed better in the next
connected to a DC motor for the performance of figure.
similar passive flexions and extensions. In Figure 12.8 the response of the CS of one
The passive movements produced similar res- PC to passive extensions of the monkey's hand
ponses in the CS of the PC as those observed in is also suppressed during active extensions, but
the decerebrate animal. The acceleration signal the cell started to have a significant increase in
was also best transmitted to the cerebellum via the CS activity as early as 360 ms before the
the CF system. In Figure 12.6 a PC is responding movement onset.
with its CS to passive movements with different Thus, during volitional movements the motor
velocities and accelerations. Figure 12.6B shows commands seem to have preference upon the
the relationship between the discharge rate ofthe olivary cells with respect to the sensory feedback
CS and the acceleration values. Here again, the produced by the passive movements.
higher the acceleration, the higher also the dis-
charge rate of the cell with CS.

A B

n=38
A CS
imp/s
10" ] p
6

0"'5
V
60"'5 ]

']
z CS
iii
.....
(/) 2
~
Z
:l
0
U
0
0 200 'llO 600 800 deg/s2
-300 0 +500ms acceleration

Figure 12.6. CS response ofa Rhesus monkey's PC to discharge rate during the first lOOms after movement
passive movements ofthe hand ofthree different mean onset and the 200 ms of the preceding resting period
accelerations. A: Averaged position curve (P) of 38 (in impulsesjs) plotted against mean values of acceiera"
similar movements. Velocity curves (V), whereby the tion of passive movements for the unit in A and three
downward slopes provide a measure for the accelera- other PCs. (From Bauswein et aI., 1984, with permis-
tion: 298°/s (left), 661°/s (middle), and 845°/s (right). sion.)
PSTH ofthe CS activity. B: Difference between the CS
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 233

A B C

PASSIVE FLEX . PASSIVE EXT. ACTIVE EXT.

n = 23 n =20 n = 20

] ] o
,'"

I
.. \
CS
:- '1.
. i
.. I

-
I
~ 10
en
U)
I-
Z CS
~
:::J
0
u 0 ~

- 0.5 0 +0.55 - 0.5 0 +0.55 - 0.5 0 +0.55


Figure 12.7. CS response of a monkey's PC during The shaded areas represent the resting period before
passive and active movements. A: Mechanogram of 23 the movement lasting 250 (A), 300 (B), and 500 (C) ms,
superimposed hand movements, dot raster, and PSTH respectively. Note the responses during passive exten-
of the CS discharge during passive flexion. B, C: The sion and its suppression during active extension.
same during passive and active extension, respectively. (Reprinted with permission from Bauswein et aI., 1983.)

Relationship Between the Complex Figure 12.9 illustrates two cells showing this
type of relationship. The first cell shows an in-
and the Simple Spike of the PC crease in the firing rate of the CS during upward
Since the CS represents only one type of discharge movement and end position together with a de-
of the PC, it is interesting to observe what kind ctease in the discharge frequency ofthe SS during
of interaction occurs between this type of spike the same time (Fig. 12.9B, C). The second cell
and the SS in the same cell. shows an increase in the CS discharge frequency
From the beginning of our recordings we ob- during both dynamic phases of the movement
served that many cells in the decerebrate cat and a corresponding decrease of its SS during the
(approximately 47%) showed what we called an same periods (Fig. 12.9D, E).
inverse relationship between the discharges of By changing the parameters of the movement,
both types of spike, that is, an increase in the this inverse relationship between the two spikes
firing rate of the PC with the CS was accompanied remains relatively constant. As we can see in
by a decrease of the discharge frequency of the Figure 12.10, the change in the amplitude of the
cell with the SS. movement changed also the response of the PC
234 Francisco 1. Rubia

A B

PASSIVE EXT. ACTIVE EXT.

20· ]
J/--~ o
n=60 n=60
z 500 J _ _ _
ro ~r-- J~F

" EMG Figure 12.8. CS and SS

§1~ ~
(f)
discharges of a monkey's
E PC during passive and
_ _ _ _ _-' active movements of the
hand. The minimal resting
, " ~, .'
periods before the
movement onset (200 ms
PC in A and 800 ms in B)
.'
•.1.. ~ " are represented by
shaded zones. In this
figure the electro-
myogram of flexors (F)
CS and extensors (E) are also
z shown. Note an increase
ro in the CS activity before
"z
(f)
I-
the movement during
active extension, which is

§1] ~
significant at - 360 ms.

~~ss
In the dot raster small
dots are for SS and large
dots for CS. (Reprinted
with permission from
I Bauswein et aI., 1983.)
o
i i r, ------~----~I------'I

-0.5 +0.5 -1.0 0 +055

with CS and a corresponding change in the SS increase in the firing rate of the PC with the CS
can be observed. The cell in this figure shows the accompanied by a tonic decrease in the SS.
above-mentioned phenomenon, namely, that the These results can suggest that both afferent
best response of the PC with the CS was to small systems could have a kind of connection between
movements and that if the displacement is suf- each other. However, it can be argued that the
ficiently large, both acceleration signals can be decrease in the firing rate of the SS could be due
observed separately, each producing an increase to the effect produced at the PC by the CS. It is
in the discharge of the PC with the CS. The well known that after the occurrence of a CS, a
decrease in the firing rate of the cell with the SS pause in the firing of the PC with the SS is pro-
accompanies the increase every time in the CS, duced. Therefore, the decrease in the SS could be
producing an almost perfect mirror image. This the sum of all the pauses produced by the CS.
can also be seen in another PC of the same type However, the inverse relationship between both
by applying a movement that followed a multiple types of spike of the PC is not the only possible
ramp function (Fig. 12.10B). Notice that at the relation. A parallel relationship can also occur.
final standing position of + 20° there is a tonic In the decerebrate cat, approximately 15% of the
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 235

'PH] 1//,
I II I
111 I
<fl -.J '
i1 : tu :
n m
COUNTS/BIN
CS
B
~ ~ 00 I

~
H
'fll ~ -30 0 n ~ 60

C
SS 100
1 F
15J
I
I

5~r G CS
o ~

]
CS n~100
H SS1~]~
D H
10ms
'PH ~ +10 0

'fll ~ 0 0 n ~100
SS 60
E 30l 0
I I I
0 2.5 5s
Figure 12.9. Relationship between CS and SS of two G and H show an expanded part of the PSTH of B
different PCs (B, C and D, E) during a passive move- and C for better illustration of the time relationship
ment of the eat's forepaw. Note the inverse relationship between CS and SS. (From Rubia and Kolb, 1978, with
between both types of spike in both cells. The dynamic permission.)
sections are represented in the PSTH by shaded areas.

PC recorded showed this last type of relationship, ward movement of the ramp function (Fig. 12.12B).
that is, an increase in the firing rate of the CS was It is interesting to note that the increase in the SS
accompanied by an increase in the SS discharge firing occurs with or without the occurrence of
ofthe same cell. The rest comprised mixed types. the CS and that when the CS occurs, there is
always a pause in the SS firing, although sometimes
An example of this parallel behavior of both very short.
types of spike of the PC can be seen in Figure Fromthese observations we can conclude that
12.11. Here two different PCs (G, H and I, K) the inverse relationship has not necessarily to be
show during the upward movement of the ramp produced by the effect of the CS on the SS firing
function an increase in both types of spike. of the Purkinje cell.
In Figure 12.12 we can see two different PCs In the awake monkey, both inverse and parallel
showing the two relationships mentioned above: relationships could be observed. The only dif-
the first with an inverse relationship between the ference with the decerebrate cat was that in the
two types of spike (Fig. 12.12A) of almost mirror awake monkey approximately 50% of the cells
image character, and the second with a parallel showed the parallel behavior compared with 15%
behavior of both spikes especially during the up- in the decerebrate animal.
236 Francisco 1. Rubia
A B

-J
w=L.Oo/s
20
+ °1
-20·
COUNTS/BIN COUNTS/BIN
n=20
CS ':] CD CS 10]

:~
SS40J
20

i ,
o
i
2.5 5s
CS':]

SS4~ C
20
PIO.51/%)

0.8
..
w=60·/s
' mu

O"y
' i-
CS':]
md
0.6

0.4
"~;;;;: '.:.:~~::!Yo- . '
5540j ..•y/.-: ...•

0.2
20 -30·
0
0 -20 !O .20
'P Ideg)
0 2.5 5s
Figure 12.10. CS and SS discharges of a PC in response a multiple ramp function . C: Mean probability of the
to passive movements of different amplitudes of the CS activity of the cell in B during the dynamic sections
cat's forepaw (A). B: PSTH of another PC of the same of the movement. (From Kolb and Rubia, 1980, with
type (inverse relationship between CS and SS) during permisson.)

In the experiments using the awake monkey ments there was the previously mentioned shift
we could also observe that by comparing passive of the cell discharge, firing the cell several hundred
with active movements, if both types of spike of milliseconds before the movement onset, but the
the PC recorded had a parallel relationship during parallel relationship remains. This can be ob-
passive movements, during similar active move- served in Figure 12.13. The cell shown in this
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 237


A
-30
I: I : W =l.3°/s
oo~
0
I
I tu
I
I
I
I
I
I
td I
I
F - 30 J W= l.Oo/s

I n m N 1l n m TIL y
COUNTS/BIN
CS CS n=100
30] n =100
70 ]
B 15
G 35

0 0
55 55 180

C
3001 120
H
150
60
of
0
CS 20
n =50 n=100

D 10
CS
30 ]
15

1
0 0
55 100 55
IDO l
E K 50
80
of 0

i i I j
0 2.5 5 0 2.5 5$
Figure 12.11. Relationship between CS and SS dis- of spike can be observed in the cells shown in G, H
charges offour different PCs during passive movements and I, K, respectively. (From Rubia and Kolb, 1978,
of the cat's forepaw. A parallel increase in both types with permission.)

figure had a parallel relationship between its spikes, zone, and recorded only PC lying in the very
and this relationship remains after changing from superficial layer of lobules Va, b, and c in the
passive to active movements. decerebrate cat.
By using the same passive movement of the
Spatial Distribution forepaw we recorded PCs in parasagittal or
mediolateral direction (parallel fiber direction),
of the PC Responses analyzing also the relationship between their
Considering these differences in the behavior of spikes.
both types.of PC spikes, it was interesting to find The results show that with respect to the CS
out if there was a certain spatial distribution of of the PC, the response of all the cells recorded,
the different types within the cerebellar cortex. no matter where the cells were located, was quali-
For this purpose we designed an experiment res- tatively very similar. The response consists of
tricting our recordings to a parasagittal strip of increases in the average discharge rate during the
the CF projection to the anterior lobe, the Cl transitional phases ofthe movement, particularly
238 Francisco J. Rubia

A B

[
+200

-2)
cs
Z
III
.......
(/)
] ~.rn","=lc.n°r",,-~. ]

100
n=1.0

]
I-
Z
::J
0
SS u

.;

" .

I I
o 5 o 5s
Figure 12.12. CS and SS discharges of two different that there is an increase in SS discharge in spite of the
PCs during passive movements of the cat's forepaw. CS occurrence. (Reprinted with permission from
Note the inverse (A) and parallel (8) relationships Marini et aI., 1982.)
between both types of spike. The dot raster in 8 shows

at the end of the upward movement (negative ac- inverse and a parallel behavior, as shown in
celeration). This response remains fairly constant Figure 12.15C. Nevertheless, as a rule, by record-
in the sagittal direction but even moreso in the ing along the mediolateral direction, every 250
mediolateral direction. to 300!lm the same relationship can be again
This can be seen in Figure 12.14. In this figure recorded (Fig. 12.16).
the CS responses of the 157 PCs recorded in para- It is difficult to interpret these results in a
sagittal and mediolateral (every 50 !lm) direction physiological sense, but if we consider the output
are shown (Fig. 12.14A). The responses are quali- of the PC it seems obvious to assume that in the
tatively so similar that by correlating the CS case of a parallel relationship between both types
response of every cell with the average CS res- of spike, a strong inhibition should result on the
ponses of all the recorded 157 cells in all ex- target cells, whereas in the case of an inverse
periments, the correlation was always positive relationship one can assume that the inhibition
(Fig. 12.15B). exerted on the target cells by one type of spike
A completely different picture arises if we ob- can be counterbalanced by the decrease of inhi-
serve the SS activity of the recorded PCs. Some bition through the other type. If so, PCs showing
of them show an inverse relationship between the an inverse relationship between their spikes
CSand SS and others a parallel relationship with should not be effective in inhibiting their target
all the intermediate types. Thus, the Pearson cor- cells. Since they are the majority of the cells re-
relation between the SS of a PC and the average corded, at least in the decerebrate cat, we can
CS responses of all the recorded PCs can be posi- suppose that the cerebellum is receiving more
tive or negative, respectively. Surprisingly, even information signals than those leaving the cere-
at a distance of only 50!lm two PCs show an bellar cortex. The comparison between the number
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 239

A B C D
PASSIVE EXT ACTIVE EXT. PASSIVE FLEX . ACTIVE FLEX .

E~

PC

~,----~,r_--_" ,.-----,.---_., ~,----~jr----., r,----~Ir_--_.i

-0.5 0 .0.5 -0.5 0 .0.5 -0.5 0 .0.5 -0.5 0 .0.55


Figure 12.13. CS and SS activity of a PC of an awake (B and 0), as shown by shaded areas. Note the shift of
monkey during passive and active extensions and cell discharge in o. (Reprinted with permission from
flexions of the hand. The minimal resting periods before Bauswein et aI., 1983.)
movement onset were 250 ms (A and C) and 500 ms

of cells with a parallel behavior in the decerebrate Motor Cortex Effect


cat and in the awake monkey can be interpreted on the Relationship Between
in the sense that in the awake animal the cere-
bellum is more active than in the decerebrate
the PC's Spikes
preparation. In a series of experiments we stimulated the motor
The different types of relationship observed cortex electrically in order to observe its effect on
between the two types of spike of the PC let us the sensory feedback signals impinging on the PC
presume that somewhere a functional link between during passive movements of the forepaw. The
both afferent systems may exist. It is difficult to electrical stimulation was applied to the repre-
assume that this link can be established at the sentation area of the forepaw in the motor cortex
level of the PC itself and, therefore, we surmise of the cat, trying to produce the same type of
that this link should exist at a precerebellar movements as those passively induced by the
level. DC motor. As soon as the desired movement was
The results obtained with electrical stimulation produced, the stimulus strength was reduced until
of the motor cortex point also to this conclusion. the movement was no longer visible. Then, the
240 Francisco J. Rubia

B
1 2

z 200
~
z 50,.
ro "
(,/) ./
"~
(,/)

100
I-
Z
5 100
:::> u
o
u

o 5,12 s o 5,12 s

200
z
ro
"
(,/)
I-
Z 100
A :::>
o
u

o 5,12 s

z
ro
"Z
I-
(,/)

:::>
o
u
o 5,12 s

Figure 12.14. CS responses of several PCs recorded in tween two penetrations in the mediolateral direction was
three different cerebellar folia to passive movements of 50 f.lm . B: Stack plots of the different PSTH. Binwidth:
the cat's forepaw. A: Diagram of the cerebellar anterior 640 ms. Note the qualitative similarity of the responses.
lobe showing the c1 zone (shaded) and the different (From Rubia and Tandler, 1981, with permission.)
penetrations of the microelectrode. The distance be-
!'-'
-
s::o
'<
'"'"
$>0
::s
0-
o

0'

(JQ

cs "l"j
ss 5'
o
"1
+1
CIl
'<
z ;!;.
Q t t o
:]1 ~ t--
«
fTff tf!1fT Uff! H a
..J '"
W
1000 cr f 11
z 15 0
to
"- l 1
III
t--
z
:::> 50 11m
! t
0
u f
j
50 11m
A 0
Jt"~L~57_~ 5.~2 s B
ij -1 C

Figure 12.15. Pearson correlations between CS and SS of PCs recorded within a mediolateralline and the average CS response
in an experiments. A: Average CS response of an 157 recorded Pc. Binwidth: 5 ms. B: Values of the correlation between CS of 21 PCs
within a representative recording line and the average response of A. The presence of a blood vessel prevented the recording of
three PCs on the right side of the correlation diagram. The decreasing positive values on the right are due to decreasing responsiveness
of PCs at the border of the cl zone. The bars at each value represent the 99% confidence interval of the correlations. C: Values
of the correlation between SS of eight different PCs and the average CS response. (From Rubia and Tandler, 1981, with permission.)

~
-
242 Francisco J. Rubia

A B
CS/5S
+1
z CS-A
0
I-
«
f CD ~-
. -
-l CD
W 55
a: Q)
a:
0 0
u
z
0
'"«a: 1 o 5,125 o 5.125
w
a..
CD
-1 50J.lm
Figure 12.16. A: Pearson correlations between SS of correlation was calculated only for upward movement
11 different PCs and the average CS response of and upper position (shaded zone). Binwidth: 160 ms.
Fig. 12.15A. B: PSTH of CS and SS of four different (From Rubia and Tandler, 1981, with permission.)
cells whose correlation values are shown in A. The

motor cortex was stimulated during different tion of the motor cortex was able to disrupt the
phases of the passive movement, recording both relationship between the spikes, as can be ob-
types of PC spikes. served in Figure 12.18. In the PC shown in this
From the observations of the results obtained, figure, the relationship between the spikes was of
two phenomena should be emphasized. Electrical the inverse type and the electrical stimulation of
stimulation during the dynamic phases of the the motor cortex during the upward movement
passive movement can result in increasing or de- produced a decrease in the CS response, but the
creasing the sensory feedback signals recorded at corresponding suppression of the SS was greater
the PC as CS and SS discharges, but without instead of smaller. The same holds true for the
changing the relationship between the two types downward movement.
of cell spikes. This can be seen in Figure 12.17. In The conclusions we have inferred from these
this figure, two different PCs are shown. Figure types of experiments is that a functional link
12.17 A illustrates the result of the electrical sti- should exist at the precerebellar level and that the
mulation of the motor cortex during the upward motor cortex can influence this link in two ways:
and downward movements of the forelimb of the either affecting the sensory feedback signals at a
ketamine anesthetized cat. In both cases we ob- previous level to the link and, therefore, keeping
tained in this PC of the inverse type (increase in the functional relationship constant (Fig. 12.17),
the CS response and decrease in the SS response) or impinging on the link, disrupting in this way
a reduction of the response, the relationship re- the functional relationship between the two
maining approximately constant. afferent systems (Fig. 12.18). Although this
In the second PC shown in Figure 12.17B, we hypothesis appears rather speculative, it seems
have a cell of the parallel type (increase in both to us interesting enough to be analyzed further.
CS and SS responses). Stimulation of the motor With respect to the forepaw, important infor-
cortex during the upward and downward move- mation from this part of the body comes as MFs
ments results in an increase in the responses of to the cerebellum through the cuneo cerebellar
the cell with the CS and SS in both cases. Here, pathway. On the other hand, it is well known that
also, the relationship between the two different there are connections between the main cuneate
spikes remains constant. nucleus (MCN) neurons and the inferior olive
However, in other cells the electrical stimula- (10). Thus, one of the candidates for the site at
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 243

A B

.20°] .20 0
J w-40i's

:j
-20° ~o

n-20 n-20

CS
I I :~
110 j 110j
55

C5 ]
l~j
5S

C5
2]0 ----..

55

j
] 301JA
~

15

o 5 o 55
Figure 12.17. CS and SS discharges of two different PC showing a parallel type of relationship. Note in A
PCs to passive movements of the cat's forepaw with the reduction ofthe response affecting both spikes and
and without electrical stimulation of the motor cortex. in B an increase in the response of the cell with both
The electrical stimulation of the cortex is marked by spikes. (Reprinted with permission from Marini et a!.,
horizontal bars. A: PSTH of a PC showing an inverse 1982.)
relationship between CS and SS. B: PSTH of another

which the presumed functional link can be is the to wheat germ agglutinin (WGA- HRP) and Dia-
cuneate nucleus. For this reason we questioned midino Yellow Dihydrochloride.
whether there were MCN neurons projecting Injection ofWGA- HRP into the 10 results in
simultaneously to both the cerebellum and the retrogradely labeled neurons in the caudal, middle,
10 and rostral subdivisions of the contralateral MCN
(Fig. 12.19). In the caudal and middle MCN the
neurons were concentrated ventrally. They were
N euroanatomical Experiments
predominantly multipolar-, triangular-, and fusi-
To approach this issue we examined the cuneo- form-shaped.
cerebellar and cuneo olivary projections in the After WGA-HRP injection into the cerebellar
cat using two tracer combinations: Fast Blue anterior lobe, especially lobules Vb and Vc, the
(FB) and Diamidino Yellow Dihydrochloride retrogradely labeled neurons were found ipsil-
(DY.2HCI) or horseradish peroxidase conjugated aterally and predominantly concentrated in the
244 Francisco J. Rubia

A B C

+200[ ~
-200
[ ~s [~ n: 40

CS ; :; JOt
ID
U)
0 t t
SS \< 501 7sf IJti
§ 25 ~ 50
" ............ 30IJA
75J
50
'C..
1~ ~30uA
I I i I
0 5s
Figure 12.18. CS and SS response of a PC to passive Note the different behavior of both spikes as compared
movements ofthe eat's forepaw with and without elec- to the cell shown in Figure 12.17 A. (Reprinted with
trical stimulation of the motor cortex (horizontal bars). permission from Marini et aI., 1982.)

rostral subdivision of the MeN, bordering the this hypothesis, WGA- HRP injections into the
external cuneate nucleus (Fig. 12.20). subdivisions of the MeN were performed.
Although in the rostral MeN the neurons pro- The injections of WGA-HRP into the sub-
jecting to the 10 were most numerous medially divisions of the MeN without affecting other
and ventrally and those projecting to the cere- nuclei showed retrogradely labeled neurons locat-
bellum were located laterally, there was consider- ed within the MeN, as can be observed in Figure
able overlapping ofthe two neuronal populations, 12.23A. The neurons are grouped in the ventral
especially in the ventral region. zone of the nucleus, although some of them were
A quantitative distribution of the labeled neu- also found in the dorsal rim of the MeN.
rons after injection of WGA-HRP into the 10 The quantification of the distribution of the
and the anterior lobe of the cerebellum is shown labeled neurons indicates, however, that the
in Figure 12.21. It can be observed that the majority number of neurons projecting in an ascending
of the MeN cells labeled after 10 injection are direction within the MeN is greater than that of
located caudally to the obex (Fig. 12.21A, B), a descending direction (Fig. 12.23B). These dif-
whereas after injection into the cerebellum the ferences could be appreciated in all the cases
labeled neurons are concentrated rostrally to the studied, from which a summary can be observed
obex (Fig. 21,21 C). However, there is a region in Figure 12.23C.
around the obex rostral to it, where both neuronal These results hint at the possibility that MeN
populations overlap. neurons have projections within the nucleus. The
By using double-labeling techniques in the com- localization of these neurons in the ventral zone
binations mentioned above, the results indicated of the MeN is similar to that of the cuneoolivary
that MeN neurons projecting to the cerebellum neurons. However, using the double-labeling tech-
represent a different population of neurons from nique, by injecting Diamidino Yellow into the 10
those projecting to the 10. Thus, the hypothesis and WGA-HRP into the rostral subdivision of
that the same MeN neuron can project simul- the MeN, we were able to show that the neurons
taneously to the cerebellum and to the 10 was with a presumed intrinsic projection and the cuneo-
discarded (Fig. 12.22). olivary projection neurons are two different popu-
However, the possibility remains of a connection lations. The proximity of the two neuronal groups
between the neurons of origin of the cuneocere- is remarkable, although within the ventral zone
bellar and cuneoolivary paths within the MeN the first group was located more dorsally than
either directly or through interneurons. To analyze the group neurons projecting to the fO (Fig. 12.24).
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 245

Figure 12.19. Drawings of transverse sections through mm from the obex. ECN, external cuneate nucleus;
the MCN showing the distribution of retrogradely GR, gracile nucleus; c, d, v, vi, vm, central, dorsal,
labeled neurons after WGA-HRP injections into the ventral, ventrolateral, and ventromedial parts of the
10. Each dot represents a labeled neuron. Numbers at MCN, respectively. (Reprinted with permission from
the lower left of each section indicate the distance in Alonso et aI., 1986.)
246 Francisco J. Rubia

Figure 12.20. The same as in Fig. 12.19 but after injec- (Reprinted with permission from Alonso et aI., 1986.)
tion of WGA~HRP into the cerebellar anterior lobe.
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 247

A 80
- contralateral
c:: 70 ••.• ipsi lateral
..;:::;0 60
&l 50
-...
en
.!!!
Qj 40
u
-c 30
~
Qj 20
.0
.!!! 10

+1 -1 -2 -3 -4 -5 -6 -7
OBEX
B 80
c:: 70
..;:::;0 60
&len 50
'Ui
Qj 40
u
-c 30
~
Qj 20
.0
.!!! 10

+2 +1 -1 -2 -3-4 -5 -6 -7 -8mm
OBEX
ROSTRAL I MEDIAL CAUDAL

C 40
c::
..;:::;0 30
u
-...5l
.!!!
Qj 20
u
-c
~
Qj 10
.,
.0
.!!!
... A ...

+2 +i -1 -2 -3 -4 -5 -6 -7 -8 mm
OBEX
Figure 12.21. Distribution of retrogradely labeled different animals) and the cerebellar anterior lobe (C).
neurons in the contralateral and ipsilateral MCN after o in the abscissa represents the obex. (Reprinted with
WGA-HRP injections into the 10 (A and B, two permission from Alonso et ai., 1986.)
248 Francisco J. Rubia

Figure 12.22. Fluorescent photomicrography of the inferior olive) but not double-labeled neurons. The
rostral MCN showing labeled cells either with Fast location of one DY. 2HCliabeled cell is indicated by an
Blue (injected into the cerebellar anterior lobe) or arrow. (Reprinted with permission from Alonso et aI.,
Diamidino Yellow Dihydrochloride (injected into the 1986.)

In those experiments in which the lectin PH A- interpretation of the results and make the hy-
Land WGA- HRP were used as anterograde pothesis of the intrinsic connectivity within the
tracers, we could also observe labeled terminals MeN more plausible. In any case, the evidence
within the MeN even in those cases where the of an intrinsic connection within the nucleus can
injections were restricted to a small group of be obtained by means of intracellular injections.
neurons (Fig. 12.25). The results obtained in ant- It remains also to be elucidated if the presumed
erograde direction with both tracers were similar. connection is exclusively intrinsic or not.
The labeled terminals within the rostral subdivision The results obtained so far indicate that in the
of the MeN were preferentially localized in a MeN they are several anatomically independent
transitional zone between the MeN and the ex- populations of neurons since each one projects
ternal cuneate nucleus. In some cases in which to a different target. Several authors have found
the cuneo cerebellar neurons were labeled with similar results by trying to identify neurons pro-
Diamidino Yellow, injections ofWGA-HRP into jecting simultaneously to the thalamus and tectum
the medial and caudal subdivisions of the MeN (Berkley et aI., 1980; Bull and Berkley, 1984;
showed labeled terminals around the cuneocere- Wiberg and Blomqvist, 1984) or to the spinal
bellar neurons. A precise localization of these cord and thalamus (Bromberg et aI., 1981).
terminals can probably be obtained only with the However, electrophysiological studies show that
electron microscope. in some cases the same MeN neuron has a func-
Nevertheless, the technique required for the tional relationship with two different structures
use of the PHA-L, namely, the iontophoretic in- (Haring et aI., 1984; Johnson et aI., 1968; Gordon
jection, as well as some of the characteristics of and Seed, 1961). Therefore, considering the anato-
this tracer, for example the uptake by the dendrites mical independency ofthe various populations of
exclusively and the anterograde transport, reduce neurons within the MeN, the hypothesis of the
considerably the probability of errors in the existence of interneurons connecting functionally
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 249

B 90 100
c: 90
.g 80 c:
80

I
0
~ 70 70
60
";;r
~ 50
60
u a; 50
al 40 u
=
1l
30
"0 40
.!1 30
!!
20 :8 20
10 ~
10

c DIIIECTlOtt 0' THI! INTIIINSIC CONNECTIONS

....
Figure 12.23. Intrinsic connections within the MCN. intrinsic connections based on the results obtained
A: Drawings of transverse sections through the MCN after injections into the rostral, medial, and caudal
showing retrogradely labeled neurons after injections parts of the MCN. The size of each arrow is related to
ofWGA- HRP into the rostral and caudal parts of the the mean number of neurons obtained from different
MCN. B: Quantification ofthe labeled neurons showed injections in every part of the nucleus. (From
in A. The abscissa represents the distance from the M.J. Blanco, Doctoral dissertation, 1988, with per-
obex in mm. C: Diagram showing the direction of the mission.)
250 Francisco J . Rubia

Figure 12.24. Drawings of transverse sections through stars represent the cells of origin of the cuneoolivary
the MeN showing retrogradely labeled neurons (dots) projection. (From MJ. Blanco, Doctoral dissertation,
after injection into the rostral part of the nucleus. The 1988, with permission.)

the different neuronal populations appears more of Cooke et al. (1971a, b) also shows that the
attractive. information from the periphery to the cerebellum
Rustioni et aL (1984) have described small is transmitted polysynaptically, suggesting the
gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-ergic inter- existence of interneurons within the MCN. Al-
neurons in the medial subdivision of the MCN, though this has not yet been established, our
probably involved in local circuitries. The work results with retrogradely labeling techniques could
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 251

Figure 12.25. Fluorescent photomicrography of the nucleus and DY.2HCllabeled neurons after injection
caudal part of the MCN showing WGA-HRP (asteriks) into the 10 (arrows). (From M.l Blanco, Doctoral dis-
labeled cells after injection into the rostral part of the sertation, 1988, with permission.)

be interpreted in the sense that they are the effect sumably projecting within the MeN is also an
of interrupting interneuronal pathways from the independent population. The functional meaning
MeN to the cerebellum. This would explain why of these neurons could be the establishment of a
after injections of WGA-HRP into rostral parts connection between different neuronal populations
of the nucleus we found labeled neurons at any ofthe MeN, which could put together the results
level caudally to the injection but definitely not obtained with anatomical and electrophysiological
the presence of labeled neurons rostrally to the experiments.
injections into the caudal parts of the nucleus. Summarizing these results, we can conclude
The experiments with double-labeling tech- that the existence of an anatomical connection
niques indicate that the neuronal population pre- between the cuneocerebellar and cuneo olivary
252 Francisco 1. Rubia

Figure 12.26. Anterogradely lab-


A eled terminals in the rostral part
of the MeN (A) after injection of
PHA-L into the caudal part of the
nucleus (8). (From c.L. Paino,
Doctoral dissertation, 1988, with
permission.)

:.\-.h. r{
,
BI
I

..
" -.

\ \
1
r
' ,
,.
~

neurons is possible. This hypothesis is supported WGA-HRP or PHA-L labeled terminals around
by the following findings: the close vicinity between the cuneo cerebellar neurons. All these facts speak
the group of cuneo olivary neurons and that of in favor of a possible connection between the
presumed intrinsic connection, the greater number cuneoolivary neurons and those neurons project-
of ascending projections, and the visualization of ing to the cerebellum.
12. Mossy and Climbing Fiber Systems 253

Finally, it is interesting to note that the cerebral Eccles, Ie., Sabah, N.H., Schmidt, R.F. and
cortex projects preferentially to the ventral zone Taborikova, H. (1972c): Cutaneous mechanorecep-
of the MeN, making possible a cortical influence tors influencing impulse discharges in cerebellar
on this hypothetical anatomical link between the cortex. III. In Purkinje cells by climbing fiber input.
two afferent systems to the cerebellum. Exp. Brain Res., 15,484-497.
Eccles, Ie., Sabah, N.H., Schmidt, RF., and Taborikova,
H. (1972d): Integration by Purkinje cells of mossy
and climbing fiber inputs from cutaneous mechano-
References receptors. Exp. Brain Res., 15,498-520.
Alonso, A., Blanco, M.J., Paino, C.L., and Rubia, F.I Gordon, G., and Seed, W.A. (1961): An investigation
(1986): Distribution of neurons in the main cuneate of the nucleus gracilis of the cat by antidromic
nucleus projecting to the inferior olive in the cat. stimulation. J. Physiol., 155, 589-60l.
Evidence that they differ from those directly project- Haring, J.H., Rowinski, MJ., and Pubols, B.H. (1984):
ing to the cerebellum. Neuroscience, 18, 671-683. Electrophysiology of raccoon cuneocerebellar neu-
Bauswein, E., Kolb, F.P., and Rubia, FJ. (1984): Cere- rons Somatosens. Res., 1,247-264.
bellar feedback signals of a passive hand movement Hobson, J.A., and McCarley, RW. (1972): Spontaneous
in the awake monkey. PflUgers Arch., 402, 292-299. discharge rates of cat cerebellar Purkinje cells in
Bauswein, E., Kolb, F.P., Leimbeck, B., and Rubia, F.J. sleep and waking. Electroenceph. CUn. Neurophysiol.,
(1983): Simple and complex spike activity of cere- 33,457-469.
bellar Purkinje cells during active and passive Ishikawa, K., Kawaguchi, S., and Rowe, M.J. (1972a):
movements in the awake monkey. J. Physiol., 339, Actions of afferent impulses from muscle receptors
379-394. on cerebellar Purkinje cells. I. Response to muscle
Berkley, K.J., Blomqvist, A., Pelt, A., and Flink, R. vibrations. Exp. Brain Res., 15, 177-193.
(1980): Differences in the collateralization of neuronal Ishikawa, K., Kawaguchi, S., and Rowe, M.J. (1972b):
projections from the dorsal column nuclei and lateral Actions of afferent impulses from muscle receptors
cervical nucleus to the thalamus and tectum in the on cerebellar Purkinje cells. II. Responses to muscle
cat: An anatomical study using two different double- contraction: Effects mediated via the climbing fiber
labeling techniques. Brain Res., 202, 273-290. pathway. Exp. Brain Res., 16, 104-114.
Blanco, MJ. (1988): Proyeccion cuneo-cerebelosa y Johnson, J.I., Welker, W.I., and Pubols, B.H. (1968):
cuneo-olivo-cerebelosa. Estudio de la posible inter- Somatotopic organization of raccoon dorsal column
accion antre sus celulas de origen en el NCI. nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol., 132,1-44.
Doctoral dissertation, Universidad Complutense, Kolb, F.P., and Rubia, FJ. (1980): Information about
Madrid. peripheral events conveyed to the cerebellum via the
Bromberg, M.B., Burnham, J.A., and Towe, A.L. (1981): climbing fiber system in the decerebrate cat. Exp.
Doubly projecting neurons of the dorsal column Brain Res., 38, 363-373.
nuclei. Neurosci. Lett., 25, 215-220. Leicht, R., Rowe, M.J., and Schmidt, R.F. (1973):
Bull, M.S., and Berkley, KJ. (1984): Differences in the Cutaneous convergence onto the climbing fiber input
neurons that project from the dorsal column nuclei to cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Physiol., 228, 610-618.
to the diencephalon, pretectum and tectum in the Llinas, R, Precht, W., and Clarke, M. (1971): Cerebellar
cat. Somatosen. Res., 1, 282-300. Purkinje cells responses to physiological stimula-
Cooke, J.D., Larson, B., Oscarsson, 0., and Sjolund, tion of vestibular system in the frog. Exp. Brain Res.,
B. (1971a): Origin and termination of cuneocerebellar 13,408-43l.
tract. Exp. Brain Res., 13, 339-358. Maekawa, K., and Simpson, 1.1. (1972): Climbing fiber
Cooke, J.D., Larson, B., Oscarsson, 0., and Sjolund, B. activation of Purkinje cells in the flocculus by im-
(1971b): Organization of afferent connections to pulses transferred through the visual pathway. Brain
cuneocerebellar tract. Exp. Brain Res., 13, 359-377. Res., 39, 245-25l.
Eccles, J.e., Sabah, N.H., Schmidt, RF., and Taborikova, Mano, N. (1970): Changes of simple and complex spike
H. (1972a): Cutaneous mechanoreceptors influencing activity of cerebellar Purkinje cells with sleep and
impulse discharges in cerebellar cortex. I. In mossy waking. Science, 170, 1325-1327.
fibers. Exp. Brain Res., 15, 245-260. Marchesi, G.F., and Strata, P. (1970): Climbing fibers of
Eccles, J.e., Sabah, N.H., Schmidt, RF., and Taborikova, cat cerebellum: Modulation of activity during sleep.
H. (1972b): Cutaneous mechanoreceptors influencing Brain Res., 17, 145-148.
impulse discharges in cerebellar cortex. II. In Purkinje Marini, G., Provini, L., and Rosina, A. (1976): Gravity
cells by mossy fiber input. Exp. Brain Res., 15, 261- responses of Purkinje cells in the nodulus. Exp. Brain
277. Res., 24, 311-323.
254 FraIfcisco J. Rubia

Marini, R., Rubia, FJ., Kolb, F.P., and Bauswein, E. to passive movements of the cat forepaw. Brain Res.,
(1982): Cortical influence upon cerebellar Purkinje 106,1-20.
cells responding to natural, peripheral stimulation in Rustioni, A., Schmeche~ D.E., Cheema, S., and Fitzpatrik,
the cat. Neuroscience Letters, 33,55-59. D. (1984): Glutamic acid decarboxylase containing
Paino, c.L. (1988): Conexiones efferentes del nucleus neurons in the dorsal column nuclei of the cat.
cuneatus en e1 gato. Doctoral dissertation, Uni- Somatosen. Res., 1, 329-357.
versidad Complutense, Madrid. Simpson, J.I., and Alley, E. (1974): Visual climbing
Rubia, F.J., and Kolb, F.P. (1978): Responses of cerebel- fiber input to rabbit vestibulo-cerebellum: A source
lar units to a passive movement in the decerebrate of direction-specific information. Brain Res., 82,
cat. Exp. Brain Res., 31, 387-401. 302-308.
Rubia, F.J., and Tandler, R. (1981): Spatial distribution Wiberg, M., and Blomqvist, A. (1984): The projection
of afferent information to the anterior lobe ofthe eat's to the mesencephalon from the dorsal column nuclei:
cerebellum. Exp. Brain Res., 42, 249-259. An anatomical study in the cat. Brain Res., 311,
Rushmer, D.S., Roberts, WJ., and Augter, G.K. (1976): 225-244.
Climbing fiber responses of cerebellar Purkinje cells
13
Eye Movements and the Zonal Structure
of the Rabbit Flocculus
John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

The flocculus is part of the compensatory eye 1969). Oscarsson (1969,1979) has suggested that
movement control system that can stabilize gaze each zone constitutes a functional computing
during head rotations about any axis in space. unit receiving information related to a different
Therefore, understanding signal processing in the motor control mechanism, but the nature of this
flocculus requires knowledge of how the multi- computation is still unknown. Even so, elabora-
dimensionality of these eye movements is repre- tion of Oscarsson's proposal in a specific, be-
sented. Studies of the messages carried by the haviorally tractable context has been possible
visual climbing fibers (CFs) as well as studies of through studies of CF signals and eye move-
the eye movements and eye muscle activity elicited ments in relation to the zonal structure of the
by electrical stimulation suggest that the basis of flocculus.
this representation is the zonal structure of the In both the anesthetized and alert rabbit, floc-
flocculus. cular CF activity is modulated in relation to the
The zonation of the flocculus, like that of the speed and direction of movement of large, tex-
rest of the cerebellum, is rooted in the anatomical tured visual patterns (Leonard, 1986; Simpson
organization of the CF inputs and the Purkinje and Alley, 1974). Recordings from the dorsal
cell outputs. The inferior olive can be anato- cap of the inferior olive and from floccular
mically subdivided such that each subdivision's Purkinje cells (Graf et aI., 1988; Leonard et aI.,
terminal field in the cerebellar cortex has the 1988; Simpson et aI., 1981) have shown that the
form of a zone or strip oriented approximately visual CF modulation can be divided into three
orthogonal to the long axis of the folia (e.g., classes according to the axis about which rota-
Brodal and Kawamura, 1980; Gerrits and Voogd, tion of the visual world evokes the greatest modu-
1982; Groenewegen and Voogd, 1977; Voogd, lation. For one class of CFs the preferred axis is
1969; Voogd and Bigare, 1980). A similar zonal vertical; for the other two classes the preferred
arrangement is also apparent in the pattern ofCF axes lie close to the horizontal plane at about 45°
activation of Purkinje cells produced by electri- or 135° to the midsagittal plane. The CFs domi-
cal stimulation of peripheral nerves (e.g., Oscars- nated by the ipsilateral eye have preferred axes
son, 1969; Andersson and Oscarsson 1978a, b, that are either vertical or close to 135° aximuth;
Oscarsson and SjOlund, 1977). These CF termi- the CFs dominated by the contralateral eye have
nation zones necessarily define corresponding preferred axes close to 45° azimuth. These findings
Purkinje cell zones because each Purkinje cell is support the idea that retinal image movement is
contacted by only one CF. Anatomical and phys- represented by visual CF activity in a reference
iological studies have shown that Purkinje cells frame whose axes have spatial orientations quite
of a given zone terminate in the same cerebellar like those of the best-response axes of the ves-
or vestibular nucleus (Brodal and Kawamura, tibular semicircular canals (Simpson and Hess
1980; Oscarsson and Andersson, 1978a, b; Voogd, 1977; Simpson et aI., 1979). Furthermore, the

255
256 John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

similarity of the orientation of both of these sets erior canal VOR pathways is coupled with the
of axes to the rotation axes of the extraocular fact that an eye movement about the roll axis can-
muscles (Graf and Simpson, 1981; Simpson and not be produced by inhibition of only the path-
Graf, 1981) suggests a relatively simple relation- ways from the ipsilateral anterior canal. Clearly,
ship between the preferred axis of each class of CFs the relation between eye movements and the
and the axis of eye rotation produced by activa- zonal structure of the flocculus needs to be
tion of the Purkinje cells upon which that class addressed further.
of CFs synapses. These geometrical considera- Recently, a zonation of the cerebellum was also
tions point to the zonal structure of the flocculus demonstrated using acetylcholinesterase (AChE)
as the structural correlate of a reference frame histochemistry (Hess and Voogd, 1986; Voogd
representing eye movements. et aI., 1987). With this technique, five compart-
A zonation of the flocculus has also been pro- ments separated by raphes and running oliquely
posed on the basis of the topographical distri- from caudomedial to rostrolateral can be identi-
bution of the sites where microstimulation elicited fied within the white matter ofthe rabbit flocculus
different classes of eye movements or influenced (Tan et aI., 1989; Van der Steen et aI., 1989). The
different vestibular ocular reflex (VOR) pathways most lateral compartment is part of the white
(Dufosse et aI., 1977; Ito et aI., 1982; Nagao matter of zone C2 (of Voogd); the four other
et aI., 1985; Yamamoto, 1979). However, the re- compartments are labeled 1 through 4, counting
ported localization and extent of these zones is from lateral to medial. The majority of the white
not consistent in these studies. Presumably, the matter is made up of the serially adjacent com-
difference arose in part because no independent, partments 1, 2, and 3. The C2 compartment is
anatomical delineation of the zones was concomi- small and present in only the most caudoventral
tantly available for comparison with the physio- part of the flocculus, and compartment 4, which
logy and in part because two different measures is quite thin, does not reach the cortex until the
of the influence of floccular stimulation on eye most rostral part of the flocculus. AChE staining
muscles were used, namely, eye movements and of the rabbit flocculus reveals a possible anatom-
electromyography. ical substrate for different classes of eye move-
In previous experiments, three classes of eye ments since it delineates white matter compart-
movements (horizontal, rotatory, and vertical) ments that presumably have projections to
were evoked by electrical stimulation in the different parts of the vestibular nucleus complex.
rabbit's flocculus (Dufosse et aI., 1977; Nagao The ability of the AChE method to provide an
et aI., 1985). The question naturally arises as to independent delineation of floccular zones invited
whether a one-to-one correspondence exists experiments in which physiological and anatomi-
between the three preferred axes of the visual cal techniques are combined in the same animal
CFs and these three classes of eye movements. to determine the patterns of eye movement evoked
Whereas the eye rotation axes of the horizontal by stimulation of each compartment.
and rotatory classes do correspond to the pre-
ferred axes of the visual CFs, the vertical eye
movement class comprises rotations about the
Methods
roll (nasal-occipital) axis, which is not a preferred The eye movements evoked by electrical micro-
axis of the visual CFs. Furthermore, the vertical stimulation in the flocculus were recorded in 26
eye movement class is not predicted by combining awake, pigmented rabbits. During the experiment
the results of electromyographic measures of the the animals were comfortably restrained and the
effects of electrical stimulation on the various head held with the nasal bone at 57° to the hori-
VOR pathways (Ito et aI., 1973, 1977, 1982) with zontal. The movements of both eyes were meas-
measurements of the kinematic action of indivi- ured simultaneously in three dimensions using
dual eye muscles in the rabbit (Graf and Simpson, two orthogonal coils attached to each eye (Van
1981; Simpson and Graf, 1981, 1985). This incon- der Steen and Collewijn, 1984). At least 5 days
sistency is made apparent when the finding that before the experiment, two search coils, one hori-
the flocculus does not directly influence the post- zontal on top of the superior rectus muscle and
13. Eye Movements 257

Figure 13.1. The orthogonal VA


coordinate system used to
measure the angular dis-
~L
placements of the eyes. See
text for explanation.
1350 CCW

'~ ......
. .'.--.*---
............. ............
,,- " ...
/
/

one vertical following the limbus, were implanted the vertical coil using the phase-angle detection
under the conjunctiva of each eye with the rabbit technique (Collewijn, 1977), whereas the rotation
under general anesthesia. The orthogonal co- components about the 45° and 135° axes were
ordinate system used to measure the angular dis- measured with the horizontal coil using the
placements consisted of a vertical axis and two amplitude detection technique (Robinson, 1963;
horizontal axes oriented at azimuths of 45° and Van der Steen and Collewijn, 1984). The vertical
135° (Fig. 13.1). The zero reference was rostral in coils had absolute calibration, whereas the hori-
the midsagittal plane and the azimuthal coordi- zontal coils were calibrated before implantation.
nate was taken to increase to each side of the The three eye position components for each eye
reference direction. The components of the evoked were charted on a pen recorder and stored on
eye movements were named after their respective magnetic tape. In the off-line computer analysis,
axis (vertical axis, 45° axis and 135° axis) referenced each position component was digitized at 125 Hz
to the ipsilateral (left) or the contralateral (right) with a resolution of 10 s of arc.
eye. The sense of the rotation components about One to 3 days before the experiment, the rabbit
the horizontal axes was defined as clockwise (CW) was reanesthetized, the bone above the para-
or counter-clockwise (CCW) according to how median lobe of the cerebellum on the left side
they would be seen by an observer looking along was removed, and the opening was surrounded
each axis towards the rabbit's eye. With these by a chamber made of dental acrylic. The dura
conventions, the 135° axis of one eye is parallel was covered with an antibiotic gel and a thin
to the 45° ,axis ofthe other eye and a CW rotation sheet of silicone rubber and then the chamber
of one eye is conjugate to a CCW rotation of the was filled with bone wax and paraffin. On the day
other eye. The sense of the rotation components of the experiment, the dura was opened under
about the vertical axis was described for each eye local anesthesia. The flocculus was identified by
as leftward or rightward. The vertical axis (hori- recording the modulation of Purkinje cell CF
zontal) component of rotation was measured with activity in response to movement of handheld
258 John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

visual patterns. With the awake rabbit in dark- at least one of the six recorded axes. The amplitude
ness, the glass or metal recording microelectrode of these slow eye movements was typically 1° to
was then used to deliver mono polar electrical 5°, but occasionally amplitudes of up to 20° were
stimuli at 200-jlm intervals during withdrawal observed. Eye movements were more readily
through the flocculus. The standard stimulus was produced by stimulation in the deep granular
a I-s train ofO.2-msec pulses at 200Hz and -20 layer and white matter than from the Purkinje
jlA. Some ofthe more effective locations for evok- cell or molecular layers. The onset of the major
ing eye movements were marked by electrolytic components of the eye movements usually oc-
lesions. After termination of the experiment, the curred between 8 and 48 ms after the stimulus
animals were deeply anesthetized and perfused. onset. The peak speed (maximum 20 °/sec) oc-
In serial sections reacted for AChE the lesion curred 100 to 200 msec after movement onset. In
sites and electrode tracks were located with re- most instances the eye returned to the initial posi-
spect to the anatomically distinguishable com- tion within 4 to lOs after the end of the stimulus.
partments. The eye movements were classified according to
the largest recorded component of the response.
Of the 12 possible classes (two eyes x three axes
Results x two senses of rotation), two of them alone
Classification of the Evoked accounted for more than 75% of the responses.
For 59% (82/140) of the effective stimulation
Eye Movements
sites, the largest component was a CCW rotation
An effective site of electrical stimulation was de- of the ipsilateral (left) eye about its 135° axis
fined as a site at which the standard stimulus (a (Fig. 13.2). In about half of these cases the re-
I-s train ofO.2-msec pulses at 200 Hz and - 20 jlA) sponse was conjugate in that the largest com-
evoked an eye movement of at least 0.5° about ponent of the contralateral (right) eye was a CW

Figure 13.2. Example ofthe class of evoked


-
Stimulus
- - eye movements for which the largest com-
ponent was a CCW rotation of the ipsi-
Right (Contra) Eye lateral (left) eye about its 135° axis. The
largest component of the contralateral
VA]: (right) eye movement was in this case a
conjugate rotation about its 45 0 axis. In

~
this and the subsequent figures the three
jCCW
45 traces for each eye show the angular dis-
CW placement about the indicated axes (L, left-
T N ward; R, rightward; CW, clockwise; CCW,
135~CCW counterclockwise). The periods of electrical
CW stimulation of the left flocculus are indic-
ated at the top. The schematic drawings on
Left (Ipsi) Eye the righ~ show how the eye movements
would appear if a cross had been placed

VA]: on the cornea. The dashed cross indicates


the initial position and the solid cross

<i@
shows the position at the end ofthe stimu-
lation (T, temporal; N, nasal). Note that
135JCW neither eye movement was "vertical." The
CCW movement of the ipsilateral eye is due:;
N T largely to the actions of the vertical recti,
45JCW whereas the movement of the contralateral
CCW ,--, eye is due largely to the actions of the
4 sec obliques.
13. Eye Movements 259

rotation about its 45° axis. However, in some longer than the latency of the CCW component
instances the component of conjugacy of the about the ipsilateral 135° axis.
contralateral eye was essentially absent. The azi- For 19% (26/140) of the effective stimulation
muthal orientation of the average rotation axis, sites, the largest response component was abduc-
which lay close to the horizontal plane, was esti- tion of the ipsilateral eye; that is, a leftward rota-
mated for each eye by "vector" summation of the tion of the left eye about its vertical axis (Fig. 13.3).
45° and 135° components. Although angular The vertical axis component of the accompanying
displacements are not vector components, com- movement of the right eye was nearly always
pounding the small displacements involved pro- conjugate; that is, leftward (adducting). The ex-
vides a reasonable indication of the orientation ample shown in Figure 13.3 had nearly only verti-
of the rotation axis. For the ipsilateral (left) eye cal axis components, but not infrequently a modest
the axis of rotation, projected into the horizontal CCW component about the ipsilateral (left) 135°
plane, was located at 141.0° azimuth. For those axis was also present.
cases in which the component of conjugacy was Although the above two classes accounted for
the largest component of the accompanying move- most of the responses, several additional types of
ment ofthe contralateral (right) eye, the similarly responses were recorded. Of these responses, one
computed rotation axis was located at 32.0° azi- that was often found was a CCW rotation about
muth. On some occasions when the largest re- the contralateral (right) 135° axis that was larger
sponse component was a CCW rotation about than or comparable to an accompanying CW
the ipsilateral 135° axis, disjunctive rotations of rotation about the contralateral (right) 45° axis.
both eyes about the vertical axis were also evoked. In the latter case, the net result of the two compo-
In those cases the latency of the adducting com- nents about the 45° and 135° axes of the contra-
ponent of the ipsilateral eye was about lOOms lateral eye was essentially an upward rotation

Stimulus
• • •
Right (Contra) Eye

Left (Ipsi) Eye

VA]:
135~
<0
CW

CCW
Figure 13.3. Example ofthe class of evok-
~
CW N T
ed eye movements for which the largest
component was abduction of the ipsi- 45
lateral (left) eye. The format is the same as CCW
that of Figure. 13.2. ~o
260 John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

alJout the roll (nasal- occipital) axis (shown in the accompanying contralateral eye movement,
Fig. 13.5 as nearly equal, but opposite rotations the largest component was a smaller, varying
of the contralateral eye about its 45° and 135° amount of adduction about the vertical axis. When
axes). However, the component about the 135° either of the two neighboring compartments (zones
axis typically had a longer latency than the com- 1 and 3) was stimulated, the largest component
ponent about the 45° axis. Even when the re- usually was a CCW rotation of the ipsilateral
sponse of the contralateral eye was "vertically" (left) eye about its 135° axis (Figs. 13.5,13.6). In
upward, the response of the ipsilateral eye was about half of these cases, the largest component
not a conjugate "vertically" downward move- of the contralateral (right) eye movement was a
ment, but instead it was essentially a CCW rota- CW rotation about its 45° axis. In addition,
tion about the 135° axis. stimulation of these two compartments some-
times resulted in a smaller, varying amount of
Eye Movement Classes in Relation horizontal convergence resulting from adduction
of the ipsilateral and contralateral eyes (Fig. 13.5).
to the Anatomical Compartments
Because the most lateral compartment (C2) and
When the middle of the three largest compart- the most medial compartment (zone 4) are com-
ments (zone 2) was stimulated, the largest re- paratively small, the eye movements,. if any,
sponse component was abduction of the ipsi- evoked by stimulating only the fibers within each
lateral eye about the vertical axis (Fig. 13.4). For of these two zones have not been determined.

B Stimulus

Right (Contral Eye

VAJ:
A
45JCCW
CW @
T N
135JCCW-
CW

-e
Left (lpsi) Eye

VAr~
135JCW
CCW N T

45J CW ------------------
CCW
' 4 sec '
Figure 13.4. Eye movements evoked by stimulation of indicated by large dots, medium dots, and fine dots,
the middle compartment (zone 2). The location of the respectively. Compartment 4 is indicated by the clear
electrolytic marking lesion is indicated on a frontal area between compartment 3 and the brachium pontis.
section ·through the left flocculus by the arrow in A. The abducting response of the ipsilateral eye is shown
From lateral to medial, compartments 1, 2, and 3 are in B. Stimulus strength 10 /lA.
13. Eye Movements 261

B Stimulus

VAJ:
Righi (Contra) Eye

~--------------
A
451ccw____________

135
JCw
Ccw____--____
JCW --~r----
~
T N

Left Opsi) Eye

VAJ:~
135JCW \------.
CCW - ~
<@
N T
4S]CW ____-------------
CCW r,
'4 sec ' LSo
Figure 13.5. Eye movements evoked by stimulation of B that the largest response component was a CCW
the medial compartment (zone 3). The location of the rotation of the ipsilateral (left) eye about its 135° axis.
marking lesion is indicated by the arrow in A. Note in Stimulus strength 2 f1A.

The predominant components of the evoked raises the question of whether axons originating
eye movements closely reflect the action of indi- from physiologically different parts of the dorsal
vidual pairs of eye muscles (Graf and Simpson, cap of Kooy and its ventrolateral outgrowth re-
1981; Graf et aI., 1988; Simpson and Graf, 1981). spect the boundaries of the AChE compartments.
The movement of the ipsilateral eye evoked by This point has been investigated by Tan, Voogd,
stimulation of the middle compartment (zone 2) and Gerrits (personal communication) using
was due predominantly to the horizontal recti anterograde horseradish peroxidase (HRP) tech-
(lateral rectus contracting). The movement of the niques. Injections into the rostral part of the
ipsilateral eye evoked by stimulation of the two dorsal cap and the adjacent ventrolateral out-
bordering compartments (zones 1 and 3) was due growth, whose cells are maximally sensitive to
predominantly to the vertical recti (inferior rectus visual world rotation about axes close to 135°
contracting); in about half the cases the accom- contralateral azimuth or 45° ipsilateral azimuth
panying contralateral eye movement was pro- (Leonard et aI., 1988; Simpson et aI., 1981), re-
duced largely by the obliques (superior oblique sulted in labeled fibers in the contralateral floc-
contracting). culus in white matter areas corresponding to the
anatomically identified compartments 1 and 3.
HRP injections into the caudal part of the dorsal
Relation between CF Afferents cap, whose cells are maximally sensitive to rota-
tion of the visual world about the vertical axis,
and the Anatomical Compartments
resulted in labeled fibers in that part of the floc-
The association between different classes of eye cular white matter corresponding to the anatomi-
movements and the anatomical compartments cally defined compartment 2.
262 John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

B Stimulus

Right (Contra) Eye

VAJ:~
~
A
"lCCW______________
Jcw T N

13J ccw____________----__----
Jcw
Left (ipsi) Eye

4 5] CW ~
CCW [50
'4 sec '
Figure 13.6. Eye movements evoked by stimulation of B that the largest response component was a CCW
the lateral compartment (zone 1). The location of the rotation of the ipsilateral (left) eye about its 135° axis.
marking lesion is indicated by the arrow in A. Note in Stimulus strength 5/lA.

Discussion three are excitatory. Two of them influence the


pathways from the horizontal canal to the ipsi-
Classes of Evoked Eye Movements-
lateral medial and lateral recti. Increased activity
Past and Present
of the related Purkinje cells results in increased
The present and previous findings (Dufosse et aI., activity ofthe lateral rectus muscle and decreased
1977; Nagao et aI., 1985) of the eye movements activity of the medial rectus muscle. The other
produced by electrical stimulation in the rabbit four pathways under direct inhibitory control are
flocculus should be considered in conjunction the anterior canal pathways, which excite the ipsi-
with the effects of flocculus stimulation on the lateral superior rectus and contralateral inferior
various VOR pathways. Purkinje cells ofthe floc- oblique muscles and inhibit their antagonists, the
culus monosynaptically inhibit some of the vesti- ipsilateral inferior rectus and contralateral supe-
bular nuclei neurons in the three-neuron arc of rior oblique muscles.
the VOR (Baker et aI., 1972; Fukuda et aI., 1972; The major, short latency components of the
Highstein, 1973; Ito et aI., 1970; Kawaguchi, 1985). eye movements evoked in the present study are
The selectivity of this inhibition for particular in good agreement with those predicted by com-
VOR pathways was shown in the rabbit by record- bining the effects of flocculus stimulation on the
ing the influence of flocculus stimulation on eye various VOR pathways (Ito et aI., 1973, 1977,
muscle activity produced by vestibular nerve stim- 1982) with measurements ofthe pulling directions
ulation (Ito et aI., 1973, 1977, 1982). Of the six of the individual extraocular muscles (Ezure and
pathways influenced, three are inhibitory and Graf, 1984; Graf and Simpson, 1981; Simpson,
13. Eye Movements 263

1983; Simpson and Graf, 1981, 1985). A conse- panying ipsilateral eye movement was not "verti-
quence of this result is that the classes of eye cal," but rather it was predominantly a CCW
movements found in the present study differ in rotation about the 135° axis. In the previous
some respects from those found earlier. For one studies, the ipsilateral "vertical" eye movements
of the two predominant classes of eye movements are considered to be due to the influence of the
found in the present study, abduction of the ipsi- flocculus on the pathways from the ipsilateral
lateral eye was the largest component. This class, anterior canal to the ipsilateral vertical recti.
which corresponds to the horizontal class found However, this conclusion is at variance with ana-
before, can be explained by the known influence tomical and physiological measurements of the
ofthe flocculus on the horizontal canal pathways actions of these muscles (Ezure and Graf, 1984;
to the lateral and medial recti. For the most GrafandSimpson, 1981; Simpson, 1983; Simpson
common class of eye movements encountered in and Graf, 1981). From those measurements it can
the present study, the largest component was a be concluded that the "rotatory" movements of
CCW rotation ofthe ipsilateral eye about its 135° the ipsilateral eye are due to the action of the
axis. The largest component of the accompanying vertical recti and that a combined action of the
movement of the contralateral (right) eye was obliques and vertical recti is required to produce
often a CW rotation about its 45° axis. This class "vertical" movements. Therefore, the presence of
of eye movement is similar to the rotatory class "vertical" eye movements indicates that the floc-
reported before, although there are some notable culus stimulation influenced both anterior and
differences with respect to the frequency with posterior canal pathways. In our study, the laten-
which this class was encountered and with respect cies of the contralateral 135° axis component
to the relative amplitudes of the ipsilateral and were longer than those of the contralateral 45°
contralateral movements. In contrast to the pre- axis component. This difference implies that the
sent findings, the rotatory class described before effect on the posterior canal pathways was less
was relatively rarely found and the movement direct and possibly not the result of stimulation
of the contralateral eye was larger than that of of Purkinje cells.
the ipsilateral eye. The rotatory movements of the The eye rotation axes of the horizontal and
contralateral eye were considered to be due to the rotatory classes, as defined in both the present
inhibitory influence of the flocculus on the path- and previous experiments, are consistent with the
ways from the ipsilateral anterior canal to the preferred axes of the visual CFs referenced to the
contralateral oblique muscles, but an equally dominant eye (Graf et aI., 1988; Simpson et aI.,
satisfactory explanation was not given for the 1981). That is, the abduction component of the
rotatory movements of the ipsilateral eye. In the response of the ipsilateral eye to stimulation of
present study, the axis of rotation ofthe ipsilateral zone 2 is a rotation about the preferred axis of
eye movements in the rotatory class was located the vertical axis CFs, whereas the predominant
at 141 ° azimuth, which is very close to the location ipsilateral and contralateral components evoked
(148° azimuth) of the rotation axis of the paired by stimulation in zones 1 and 3 are spatially
vertical recti (Graf et aI., 1988). This similarity closely associated with the ipsilateral 135° axis
indicates that the ipsilateral "rotatory" eye move- CFs and the contralateral 45° axis CFs, respec-
ments are essentially the result of floccular inhibi- tively. For all three relations the evoked eye move-
tion of the pathways from the ipsilateral anterior ment would have resulted in excitation of the
canal to the ipsilateral vertical recti. respective CFs if the eye had been viewing a
The vertical eye movement class composed of stationary visual world. It should be noted that
rotations of both eyes about the roll axis (Dufosse the absence of a vertical class of eye movements
et al., 1977; Nagao et aI., 1985) was not found in for the ipsilateral eye substantially improves the
the present study. Whereas the relatively small correspondence between the classes of visual CFs
contralateral "vertical" eye movements found by and the classes of evoked eye movements since
us are apparently the same as the contralateral the roll (nasal-occipital) axis is not a preferred
eye movements in the vertical class of Dufosse axis of the CFs.
et al. (1977) and Nagao et aI. (1985), the accom-
264 John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

Topographical Organization ofthe Flocculus flocculus remains to be determined. Nevertheless,


the shared organization of the preferred axes of
The organization of the floccular white matter the visual CFs and the rotation axes of the pre-
into compartments revealed by AChE histochem- dominant classes of eye movements evoked by
istry can be directly related to particular classes stimulation in anatomically distinguishable floc-
of eye movements and, thus, to particular VOR cular compartments provides a framework-in
pathways. Of the three largest compartments, the a literal sense-for investigating the meaning of
middle one (zone 2) is related to the horizontal the geometry inherent in the CF zones and the
canal pathways since the major component of the orthogonally directed parallel fibers.
eye movements evoked by stimulation in this
compartment is abduction of the ipsilateral eye.
The two bordering compartments (zones 1 and Acknowledgments. Supported by National Insti-
3) are both related to the anterior canal pathways tutes of Health Grant NS-13742, Medigon Grant
since the major components of the evoked eye 900-550-092, and the Netherlands Organization
movements are the consequence of decreasing the for Scientific Research (NWO).
activity of the ipsilateral superior rectus and the
contralateral inferior oblique muscles ·and/or in- References
creasing the activity of the ipsilateral inferior
rectus and the contralateral superior oblique Andersson, G., and Oscarsson, O. (1978a): Projections
muscles. to lateral vestibular nucleus from cerebellar c1im bing
In this study we were unable to distinguish fiber zones. Exp. Brain Res., 32, 549-564.
Andersson, G., and Oscarsson, O. (1978b): Climbing
zone 1 from zone 3 on the basis of differences in
fiber microzones in cerebellar vermis and their pro-
the patterns of evoked eye movements. Stimula- jection to different groups of cells in the lateral vesti-
tion of both of these zones typically resulted in bular nucleus. Exp. Brain Res., 32, 565-579.
eye movements whose largest component was a Baker, R.G., Precht, W., Llinas, R. (1972): Cerebellar
CCW rotation of the ipsilateral (left) eye about modulatory action on the vestibulo-trochlear path-
its 135° axis. Furthermore, the range of amplitudes way in the cat. Exp. Brain Res., IS, 364-385.
ofthe accompanying conjugate component about Brodal, A., and Kawamura, K. (1980): Olivocerebellar
the contralateral 45° axis was also similar for projection: A review. Adv. Anat. Embryol. Cell Bioi.,
stimulation of zone 1 and zone 3. Although, these 64,1-140.
findings suggest that both zones receive ipsilateral Collewijn, H. (1977): Eye- and head-movements in
freely moving rabbits. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 266,
135° axis CFs and contralateral 45° axis CFs, they
471- 498.
do not preclude the possibility of a differential
Dufosse, M., Ito, M., and Miyashita, Y. (1977): Func-
distribution within each zone of these two CF tionallocalization in the rabbit's cerebellar flocculus
classes, which arise from different parts of the determined in relationship with eye movements.
rostral dorsal cap and the adjacent ventrolateral Neurosci. Lett., 5, 273-277.
outgrowth. From an autoradiographic study of Ezure, K., and Graf, W. (1984): A quantitative analysis
the CF projections to the cat flocculus, Gerrits of the spatial organization of the vestibulo-ocular
and Voogd (1982) concluded that the rostral dor- reflexes in lateral and frontal eyed animals. I. Orien-
sal cap and the ventrolateral outgrowth each tation of semicircular canals and extraocular muscles.
project differentially to both zones 1 and 3. Since Neuroscience, 12,85-93.
there is no anatomically precise border between Fukuda, J., Highstein, S.M., and Ito, M. (1972): Cere-
the rostral dorsal cap and the ventrolateral out- bellar inhibitory control ofthe vestibulo-ocular reflex
investigated in rabbit IIIrd nucleus. Exp. Brain Res.,
growth, a first approximation association may
14,511-526.
be made between the rostral dorsal cap and the Gerrits, N.M., and Voogd, J. (1982): The climbing fiber
45° axis CFs and between the ventrolateral out- projection to the flocculus and adjacent paraflocculus
growth and the 135° axis CFs (Leonard et aI., in the cat. Neuroscience, 7, 2971-2991.
1988; Simpson et al., 1981). The extent to which Grar, W., and Simpson, J.1. (1981): The relations between
these two physiologically distinguishable classes the semicircular canals, the optic axis, and the extra-
of CFs project to separable areas in the rabbit ocular muscles in lateral-eyed and frontal-eyed ani-
13. Eye Movements 265

mals. In: Progress in Oculomotor Research Dev. N euro- cerebellar anterior lobe as revealed by the projection
sci. (A. Fuchs, and W. Becker, eds). Amsterdam: Else- patterns of the climbing fiber system. In: Neuro-
vier, pp. 411-420. biology of Cerebellar Evolution and Development
Graf, W., Simpson, J.I., and Leonard, C.S. (1988): The (R. Llinas, ed.). Chicago, IL: American Medical Asso-
spatial organization of visual messages in the floc- ciation, pp. 525-537.
culus of the rabbit's cerebellum. II. Complex and Oscarsson, O. (1979): Functional units of the cere-
simple spike responses of Purkinje cells. J. Neuro- bellum-sagittal zones and microzones. Trends
physiol., 60, 2091-2121. N eurosci., 2, 143-145.
Groenewegen, H.J., and Voogd, J. (1977): The para- Oscarsson, 0., and Sjolund, B. (1977): The ventrospino-
sagittal zonation within the olivocerebellar projec- cerebellar system in the cat. I. Identification of five
tion. I. Climbing fiber distribution to the vermis of paths and their termination in the cerebellar anterior
cat cerebellum. J. Compo Neurol., 174,417-488. lobe. Exp. Brain Res., 28, 469-486.
Hess, D.T., and Voogd, J. (1986): Chemoarchitectonic Robinson, D.A. (1963): A method of measuring eye
organization of the monkey cerebellum. Brain Res., movement using a scleral search coil in a magnetic
369, 383-387. field. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron., 10,137-145.
Highstein, S.M. (1973): Synaptic linkage in the vesti- Simpson, 1.1. (1983): Transformation of coordinates
bulo-ocular and cerebello-vestibular pathways to intrinsic to the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Soc. N eurosci.
the VIth nucleus in the rabbit. Exp. Brain Res., 17, Abstr., 9, 315.
301-314. Simpson, J.I., and Alley, K.E. (1974): Visual climbing
Ito, M., Highstein, S.M., and Fukuda, J. (1970): Cere- fiber input to rabbit vestibulo-cerebellum: a source
bellar inhibition of the vestibulo-ocular reflex in of direction-specific information. Brain Res., 82,
rabbit and cat and its blockage by picrotoxin. Brain 302-308.
Res., 17,524-526. Simpson, 1.1., and Graf, W. (1981): Eye muscle geometry
Ito M., Nisimaru, N., Yamamoto, M. (1973): Specific and compensatory eye movements in lateral-eyed
neural connections for the cerebellar control of vesti- and frontal-eyed animals. Ann. NY Acad Sci., 374,
bulo-ocular reflexes. Brain Res., 60, 238-243. 20-30.
Ito, M., Nisimaru, N., and Yamamoto, M. (1977): Speci- Simpson, J.I., and Graf, W. (1985): The selection of
fic patterns of neuronal connetions involved in the reference frames by nature and its investigators. In:
control of rabbit's vestibulo-ocular reflexes by the Adaptive Mechanisms in Gaze Control (A. Bethoz,
cerebellar flocculus. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 265, 833- and M.G. Jones, eds). Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 3-16.
854. Simpson, J.I., Graf, W., and Leonard, C. (1981): The
Ito, M., OrJov, I., and Yamamoto, M. (1982): Topogra- coordinate system of visual climbing fibers to the
phical representation of vestibulo-ocular reflexes in flocculus. In: Progress in Oculomotor Research. De-
rabbit cerebellar flocculus. Neuroscience, 7, 1657- velopments in Neuroscience (A. Fuchs, and W. Becker,
1664. eds). Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 475-484.
Kawaguchi, Y. (1985): Two groups of secondary vesti- Simpson, J.I., and Hess, R. (1977): Complex and simple
bular neurons mediating horizontal canal signals, visual messages in the flocculus. In: Control of Gaze
probably to the ipsilateral medial rectus muscle, by Brain Stem Neurons(R. Baker, and A. Berthoz, eds).
under inhibitory influences from the cerebellar floc- Amsterdam: ElsevierjNorth Holland, pp. 351-360.
culus in rabbit. Neurosci. Res., 2, 434-446. Simpson, 1.1., Soodak, R.E., and Hess, R. (1979): The
Leonard, C.S. (1986): Signal characteristics of cerebellar accessory optic system and its relation to the vesti-
Purkinje cells in the rabbit flocculus during compen- bulocerebellum. In: Reflex Control of Posture and
satory eye movements. Doctoral dissertation, New Movement (R. Granit, and O. Pompeiano eds).
York University. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 715-725.
Leonard, C.S., Simpson, 1.1., and Graf, W. (1988): The Tan, J., Eisenman, L.M., Gerrits, N.M., Hawkes, R.,
spatial organization of visual messages in the floc- and Voogd, J. (1989): Compartmentalization within
culus of the rabbit's cerebellum. I. Typology of in- the rabbit flocculus. A mabQ 113 immunohisto-
ferior olive neurons of the dorsal cap of Kooy. J. chemical, acetylcholinesterase and anatomical trac-
Neurophysiol., 60, 2073-2090. ing study. Eur. J. Neurosci., 2, (Suppl) 33.
Nagao, S., Ito, M., Karachot, L. (1985): Eye field in the Van der Steen, J., and Collewijn, H. (1984): Ocular
cerebellar flocculus of pigmented rabbits determined stability in the horizontal, frontal and sagittal planes
with local electrical stimulation. N eurosci. Res., 3, in the rabbit. Exp. Brain Res., 56, 263-274.
39-51. Van der Steen, J., Simpson, 1.1., and Tan, J. (1989): Eye
Oscarsson, O. (1969): The sagittal organization of the movements evoked from histochemically distin-
266 John I. Simpson, Johannes Van der Steen, and Joep Tan

guished compartments of the rabbit flocculus. Soc. Voogd, J., Gerrits, N.M., and Hess, D.T. (1987): Para-
Neurosci. Abstr., 15, 613. sagittal zonation of the cerebellum in macaques:
Voogd, J. (1969): The importance of fiber connections An analysis based on acetylcholinesterase histo-
in the comparative anatomy ofthe mammalian cere- chemistry. In: Cerebellum and Neuronal Plasticity
bellum. In: Neurobiology ofCerebellar Evolution and (M. Glickstein, C. Yeo, and J. Stein, eds). New York:
Development (R. Llinas, ed.). Chicago, IL: American Plenum, pp. 15-39.
Medical Association, pp. 493-514. Yamamoto, M. (1979): Vestibulo-ocular reflex path-
Voogd, J., and Bigare, F. (1980): Topographical distri- ways of rabbits and their representation in the cere-
bution of olivary and cortico nuclear fibers in the bellar flocculus. In: Progress in Brain Research.
cerebellum: A review. In: The Inferior Olivary Nucleus Reflex Control of Posture and Movement (R. Granit,
(J. Courville, C. De Montigny, and Y. LaMarre eds). and O. Pompeiano, eds). Amsterdam: Elsevier/
New York: Raven Press, pp. 207-234. North Holland, pp. 451-457.
14
The Dynamic Selection Hypothesis:
A Proposed Function for
Cerebellar Sagittal Zones
James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

This chapter reviews our recent studies of the Purkinje cells to nuclear neurons. This projection
climbing fiber's heterosynaptic action on individ- was divided into three large sagittal zones, each
ual Purkinje cells and presents a new hypothesis, corresponding to a region of the cerebellar cortex
the "dynamic selection" hypothesis, regarding the and a nonoverlapping region of termination
functional basis for the sagittal zones of the cere- within the deep cerebellar nuclei.
bellum. This hypothesis is developed through a The historic observations of Voogd (1969)
discussion of our findings as well as those from based on the characteristics of fiber systems found
other laboratories in the context of two theories of within the white matter underlying the cerebellar
climbing fiber function: one proposing that these cortex revealed that the cerebellum may be divis-
afferents perform a major role in establishing en- ible into more than the three sagittal zones des-
grams in the cerebellar cortex required for motor cribed by Brodal and Jansen (1940). Subsequent
learning and the other arguing that climbing fibers studies clearly showed that the multiple sagittal
are most critical for real-time operations per- bands reported by Voogd also describe the sagittal
formed by the cerebellum during motor execution. distribution of Purkinje cells projecting to discrete
Our experiments support the latter view by dem- nonoverlapping regions in the cerebellar nuclei
onstrating a specific short-term action of climb- (Courville et aI., 1973; Courville and Diakiw, 1976;
ing fibers on simple spike responses in passive Haines et aI., 1982). The careful anatomical experi-
paradigms as well as in multiple, sagittally organ- ments of Dietrichs and colleagues (Dietrichs, 1981;
ized Purkinje cells during perturbed locomotion. Dietrichs and Walberg, 1980) further characterized
Other studies examining the effect of cerebellar the discrete, nonoverlapping projections of cortico-
ablation on the classically conditioned nictitating nuclear fibers to regions of the cerebellar nuclei by
membrane/eyeblink response in rabbits substan- illustrating the wormlike progression ofPurkinje
tiate our previously held contention that the cere- cell terminals from progressively more medially
bellum is neither sufficient nor necessary for this or laterally located sagittal bands of Purkinje
type of conditioned behavior currently being used cells.
as models for motor learning. Parallel anatomical experiments in other labo-
The sagittal organization ofthe cerebellum has ratories revealed that the termination of climbing
been well recognized since its original description fibers in the cerebellar cortex also was localized
by Jansen and Brodal (1940). Since that time, this to sagittal bands (Courville, 1975). Several studies
principle has been progressively refined with il- in many species have now substantiated that
lustrations of its organizational details, its specific cells located in specific regions of the inferior
fiber systems, and its modifications occurring olive terminate in the contralateral cerebellar
throughout phylogeny. The original description cortex in a specific distribution of sagittal bands
of the sagittal zones focused on the organization (Brodal and Kawamura, 1980). Intriguingly, the
ofthe cotticonuclear projection, the projection of dimensions of these sagittal bands are similar to

267
268 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

those describing the organization of the cor- in tum impinge on cerebellar nuclear neurons,
ticonuclear projection (Voogd and Bigr, 1980; providing an input to the olivary site from which
Groenewegen et al., 1979; Groenewegen and the climbing fiber projection originates.
Voogd, 1977). The correspondence between the Also important to the hypothesis developed
sagittal bands describing the termination patterns in this chapter is the observation that the soma-
of olivocerebellar fibers and those characterizing totopic organization of mossy fiber inputs to the
the origin of corticonuclear fibers is critical to the cerebellum does not appear to be organized in
hypothesis developed in the discussion below. sagittal strips similar to those characterizing the
In addition to the olivocerebellar and cortico- olivocerebellar and corticonuclear projections.
nuclear fibers, major components of the nucleo- The studies of Shambes et ai., (1978) and Bower
cortical projection, the recurrent collaterals of and Woolston (1983) provide strong evidence
cerebellar nuclear neurons coursing to the granu- that stimuli applied to specific regions of the
lar layer of the cerebellar cortex as mossy fibers body surface activate mossy fiber projections to
(Tolbert et ai., 1976), also appear to be organized widespread regions of the cerebellar cortex in
sagittally (Tolbert et ai., 1978). Although excep- a distribution that has been termed a "patchy
tions exist, nucleocortical fibers originating from mosaic." Each body region is represented in
specific sites within a cerebellar nucleus project multiple patches, and the patches are organized
to relatively nonoverlapping, sagittally distrib- in a complex map of noncontiguous cutaneous
uted regions of the cerebellar cortex (Dietrichs, regions. Consequently, mossy fiber inputs acti-
1981; Dietrichs and Walberg, 1980). vated from any specific site on the body surface
The electrophysiological experiments of actually are distributed to multiple sagittal zones.
Robertson and. Laxer (1981) provide evidence As a result, the afferent projection conveying most
that the sagittal organization of the climbing fiber of the information pertaining to time- and ampli-
system also characterizes its somatotopy. Based tude-dependent modulation of peripheral inputs
on their findings, complex spike responses of and activity in central pathways does not appear
Purkinje cells evoked by discrete stimuli applied to obey the most pervasive anatomical organiza-
to specific regions of the body surface occur in tional feature of the cerebellum: the sagittal zones.
neurons organized sagittally within the cerebellar
cortex. This electrophysiological observation fits
well with the combined morphological and elec- Operation Performed by the
trophysiological studies of Gellman et ai., (1983, Climbing Fibers
1985), illustrating that neurons at least within
the dorsal accessory subnucleus of the inferior
in the Cerebellar Cortex
olive are somatotopically organized. Consequently, Despite the fact that the electrophysiological
olivary neurons at anyone site not only would events evoked by the climbing fibers on the
respond to stimulation of specific regions of the dendritic tree of Purkinje cells is now reasonably
body surface but also would project to a specific well understood due largely to the studies of
sagittally distributed termination site. This pattern Eccles et ai., (1966) and the subsequent work of
of termination also characterizes the sagittal dis- Llinas and colleagues (Llinas, 1985; Llinas and
tribution of collaterals from individual olivary Nicholson, 1976; Llinas and Sugimori, 1982;
neurons (Rosina and Provini, 1983). Pellionisz and Llinas, 1977; Tank et ai., 1988), the
Last, the elegant anatomical studies of Houk aspect of motor function to which this unique
and Gibson (1987) illustrate that specific compo- afferent system contributes remains a subject of
nents of the corticonuclear and olivocerebellar extensive experimentation and discussion. Al-
projections comprise subcircuits of cerebellar though there are currently several specific hypo-
afferent and efferent pathways that relate specific theses regarding the precise function of this system,
olivary and cerebellar cortical regions with a most proposals fall into two categories. One view
specific population of cerebellar nuclear neurons. argues that the climbing fiber system plays a
These studies show that olivocerebellar fibers critical role in establishing engrams important
project to discrete groups of Purkinje cells that for motor learning within the cerebellar cortex.
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 269

This argument has been supported principally In general, the experiments were designed specifi-
by two avenues of experimentation. The first ex- cally to eliminate the need for electrically sti-
amined the effectiveness of conjointly stimulating mulating the climbing fiber input to a recorded
climbing and mossy fiber inputs to Purkinje cells Purkinje cell, consequently minimizing any effects
in producing a long-term modification in Purkinje due to abnormal recruitment patterns or activa-
cell responsiveness (Ekerot and Kano, 1985; Ito tion rates of the climbing fiber inputs. Two types
et aI., 1982; Ito and Kano, 1982; Kano and Kato, of paradigms were developed. One used spon-
1987). A second group of experiments has focused taneously occurring complex spikes to trigger
on providing evidence that sites within the cere- peripheral stimuli activating mossy fiber inputs
bellum are both sufficient and essential for the at specified times after the occurrence of the cell's
establishment of certain types of motor learning. climbing fiber input (Ebner and Bloedel, 1981,
Most of these experiments have employed the 1984). The second approach used a trial-sorting
classical conditioning of the nictitating mem- technique comparing the simple spike respon-
brane eyeblink reflex in the rabbit as a model siveness of Purkinje cells in trials in which the
(McCormick and Thompson, 1984a, b; Thompson, climbing fiber input was activated with that in
1986; Yeo et al., 1984, 1985a, b). These studies trials in which complex spikes were not evoked by
have used both electrophysiological recording the peripheral stimulus (Ebner et al., 1983).
within cerebellar nuclei as well as ablation experi- In studies examining the effects of sponta-
ments to support this view. The data indicate that neously occurring climbing fiber inputs on the
lesions in a specific region ofthe interposed nuclei responsiveness of Purkinje cells to their mossy
(McCormick and Thompson, 1984a, b) or lobule fiber-granule cell-parallel fiber input, the complex
HVI of the cerebellar cortex (Yeo et aI., 1984, spikes were electronically discriminated and used
1985a, b) eliminate the capacity of a conditioned to trigger the application of either natural peri-
animal to execute the conditioned reflex but do pheral stimuli (Ebner and Bloedel, 1981) or
not produce significant effects on the performance electrical stimuli applied to the surface of the
of the unconditioned reflex. Furthermore, the cerebellar cortex to activate a parallel fiber beam
climbing fiber system has been strongly implicated (Ebner and Bloedel, 1984). With these paradigms
in the process responsible for establishing the it was possible to determine any changes in the
proposed memory traces within the cerebellum simple spike responsiveness of Purkinje cells at
(McCormick et aI., 1985; Thompson, 1986). various intervals after their climbing fiber input.
Alternate views of climbing fiber function have Furthermore, possible "conditioning" effects of
focused on the operational role this system may repeatedly coupling the activation of climbing
exercise during ongoing motor behavior (Bloedel fiber and parallel fiber inputs to the same Purkinje
and Ebner, 1985; Llimis, 1985). An extended series cell could also be tested.
of experiments in our laboratory supports this In brief, these experiments demonstrated that
view (for review see Bloedel and Ebner, 1985; the climbing fiber input to a Purkinje cell modified
Bloedel and Zou, 1987; Bloedel and Zuo, 1989) its responsiveness to the activation of mossy
and was was the basis for proposing the gain fibers. However, in contrast to the studies em-
change hypothesis (Bloedel et aI., 1983; Bloedel ploying the conjunction stimulation paradigm
and Ebner, 1985). A general overview of the initial (Ekerot and Kano, 1985; Ito and Kano, 1982; Ito
findings leading to the formulation of the gain et aI., 1982), the responsiveness of Purkinje cells
change hypothesis was presented previously increased rather than decreased. This increase in
(Bloedel and Ebner, 1985; Bloedel and Lou, 1987) responsiveness was reflected by changes in ampli-
and therefore will be reiterated in only enough tude of both excitatory as well as inhibitory re-
detail to provide the background necessary for sponse components. Furthermore, the effect of
the argument to be presented below. climbing fiber inputs on the responsiveness of
The initial series of experiments done together Purkinje cells was short lasting, persisting for
with Ebner (Bloedel et aI., 1983; Ebner and Bloedel, only 200 msec. Successively pairing climbing fiber
1981, 1984; Ebner et aI., 1983) employed passive and parallel fiber inputs to the same Purkinje cell
paradigms in decerebrate, unanesthetized animals. did not produce a prolonged modification in its
270 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

A responsiveness. Consequently, when a peripherally


activated mossy fiber input or a directly activated
Non-CF Trial
parallel fiber input was no longer coupled with
SS-PSTH the cell's climbing fiber input, the Purkinje cell's
responsiveness returned to control levels within
an extremely short period of time.
An example of the data obtained with the
sorting paradigm is shown in Figure 14.1. With
CF Trial SS-PSTH
this technique, two histograms were constructed,
one a nonclimbing fiber trial simple spike histo-
gram and the other a climbing fiber trial simple
spike histogram. The nonclimbing fiber trials
were those in which a tap to the forepaw of the
All Trial CF-PSTH acutely decerebrate cat did not evoke a complex
spike within the response window indicated by
the arrows below the abscissa of each histogram .

• o n = 200
In contrast, the climbing fiber trials were those
in which the Purkinje cell responded to the tap
with a complex spike within the same response
All Trial SS-PSTH window. The amplitudes of the histograms were
then normalized to the number of trials (n). The
standard all-trial histogram combining the non-
,... climbing fiber and climbing fiber trials is shown
~ . 20 n = 46 in Figure 14.lD.
>-1-
CF-Trial Aligned ~ :E Critical to the hypothesis to be developed from
~~ these data is the fact that the simple spike re-
SS-PSTH ~: sponsiveness of Purkinje cells was greater in the
Q:~
~
climbing fiber trials (B). Note that the amplitudes
msec 600 of both the excitatory (E) as well as the inhibitory
Tap (I) response components were increased. These
2.5 mm
modifications in response amplitude were quanti-
Figure 14.1. Increase in the amplitude of both an exci- fied by calculating the gain change ratio for each
tatory as well as an inhibitory component in a Purkinje component. In this example, the excitatory re-
cell's response to a tap applied to the dorsum of the
sponse component increased by a factor of 3.63
forepaw. The all-trial histogram (D) illustrates the re-
(RE = 363%), and the inhibitory component in-
sponse of this Purkinje cell to 200 successive stimuli
applied with the time course shown by the tap trace creased in amplitude by a factor of 1.86 (R) =
below the histograms. The arrows indicate the response 186%). These data typify the statistically verified
window in which the occurrence of complex spikes was finding in these experiments: the occurrence of a
considered a response evoked by the peripheral stimu- climbing fiber input resulted in an increased
lus and consequently was used as the basis for sorting amplitude of both excitatory as well as inhibitory
the 200 trials into two groups. One group, consisting responses evoked by the mossy fiber- granule cell-
of 154 trials, was used to construct the non climbing parallel fiber input to the same cell.
fiber trial SS-PSTH (A); the other group of 46 was used These data, together with a multiunit study in
to construct the climbing fiber trial SS-PSTH (B). A which the responses of up to three Purkinje cells
histogram from these trials aligned on the evoked
were recorded simultaneously (Bloedel et aI., 1983),
complex responses is shown in E. The excitatory and
inhibitory response components are demarcated in
were used as a basis for proposing the gain change
both A and B. A comparison of the climbing fiber trial ~----------------------------------
(B) and nonclimbing fiber trial (A) simple pike histo- the gain change ratios resulting from this calculation is
grams was done by calculating the gain change ratios shown in B. (Reprinted with permission from Ebner
for each of the two response components. The value of et aI., 1983.)
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 271

hypoth~sis. This operational view contends that ing fiber activation during the conditioning phase
the action of the climbing fiber demonstrated in should persist after acquisition, even though the
the experiments just reviewed reflects a mech- climbing fiber inputs may diminish or cease. In
anism by which these afferents produce a short- other words, if the climbing fiber inputs were
lasting increase in a Purkinje cell's responsiveness acting as a "teacher," the modification in Purkinje
that is critical to the processing of information cell responsiveness should persist even after these
conveyed by the mossy fiber inputs to the cerebel- afferents are no longer activated. In contrast, the
lar cortex. It should be emphasized that the find- data demonstrated that the modulation of simple
ings on which this hypothesis is based are very and complex spike activity occurred in parallel:
different in two respects from those resulting from increases or decreases in climbing fiber activation
the studies employing the conjunction paradigm. were statistically associated with parallel changes
First, the data from our experiments indicate that (i.e., increases or decreases, respectively, in the
the responsiveness of Purkinje cells after climbing depth of modulation of simple spike activity
fiber inputs increases rather than decreases due throughout the period of observation). No data
to the heterosynaptic action of climbing fibers. indicated that a persistent effect in the simple
Second, the effect observed in our studies is spike responses of Purkinje cells was induced by
shortlasting. A persistent change in Purkinje cell the action of the climbing fiber system. Conse-
responsiveness could not be produced by re- quently, the findings in this study are appreciably
peatedly coupling a cell's spontaneous climbing more consistent with the gain change hypothesis
fiber input with the electrically or naturally evoked than the learning hypothesis.
mossy fiber-granule cell-parallel fiber input to Also inconsistent with the learning theory were
its dendritic tree. the results of studies examining the effects of
Despite the findings supporting the existence chronically removing the entire ipsilateral cere-
of a mechanism by which climbing fibers can bellar cortex and nuclei on the capacity of decere-
exert a functionally important short-term effect brate ferrets to acquire this task (Bloedel et aI.,
on Purkinje cell responses, discussions continue 1987; Bloedel and Zuo, 1989). Although a motor
regarding an alternate or even an additional role deficit was apparent in both the unperturbed as
for climbing fibers in motor learning (Kano and well as the perturbed step cycles of these animals,
Kato, 1987; McCormick et aI., 1985; Thompson, they were capable of learning to step over the
1986). Our laboratory has approached this gen- bar despite the fact that massive regions of the
eral issue using two different preparations. The cerebellum had been removed. Clearly, the cere-
first employed a perturbed locomotion paradigm bellum was not necessary or essential for the
in which an acutely decerebrate locomoting cat manifestation of this type of conditioned behavior.
or ferret encountered a bar placed in the trajectory To clarify whether or not the classical condi-
of one of its forelimbs during swing phase on each tioning of the nictitating membrane response of
successive trial (Lou and Bloedel, 1986). After an the rabbit occurs in the absence of the cerebellum,
amazingly few (5-30) trials, these animals were experiments were performed in acutely decerebrate
conditioned to step over the bar (Lou and Bloedel, rabbits with functionally total cerebellar abla-
1987a). In one of the experiments examining the tion. In these preparations all the cerebellum was
role of the cerebellum in this process (Lou and removed, including the nuclei, except a small
Bloedel, 1987b), up to five Purkinje cells were portion of the lateral-most tips of the hemi-
simultaneously recorded in regions of the cere- spheres. Decerebrate rabbits were employed
bellar anterior lobe receiving climbing fiber inputs because of our interest in recording from single
activated in response to this type of perturbation. neurons throughout the entire time course of the
The relationship between simple and complex acquisition and performance of the conditioned
spikes was assessed throughout the acquisition, nictitating membrane/eye blink reflex in subse-
performance, and extinction of this conditioned quent experiments. These preparations made it
behavior. It was hypothesized that if the learning possible to apply enough stimuli successively to
theory were correct, any change in simple spike condition the reflex while recording from a single
activity associated with the Purkinje cell's climb- neuron.
272 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

:\/,
A CR Failure
C Tone Only

\/
t
+T +8
t
+T
B D Tone Only-Extinction
CR-UCR

t
+T
~.
+8

--
-.-
>-
.::1
lIIe

a:: t
0
+T
,
msec
~ ,
600

Figure 14.2. Individual trials obtained from an experi- reflex, A showing a trial in which there was a failure of
ment in which the eyeblink reflex was classically condi- the conditioned response and B a trial in which a
tioned in an acute decerebrate-decerebellate rabbit, conditioned response was evoked. After conditioning
The times at which either a tone stimulus (T) or a a conditioned response could be evoked in trials in
periorbital electrical stimulus were applied are shown which only the tone stimulus was applied (C) until
below the time axis of each record. The trials in A and extinction occurred (D).
B were obtained during the conditioning of the eyeblink

The recordings in Figure 14.2 illustrate single the decerebrate animal is very similar to that
trials obtained from a series of experimental ob- observed in intact rabbits (Gormezano et aI.,
servations in one rabbit. In these experiments a 1983). Our studies have also demonstrated that
500-Hz tone was used as a conditioned stimulus, the conditioned response in decerebrate rabbits
which was terminated coincidentally with the is temporally related to the interstimulus interval.
unconditioned stimulus, a single periorbital Changing the interval between the conditioned
electrical pulse. The nictitating membranejeye- stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus also
blink reflex was monitored using an emitter- modifies the interval between the conditioned
transducer system that measured changes in the stimulus and the conditioned response. Conse-
reflection of an infrared beam of light off the quently, the temporal relationship between the
conjunctiva of the eye. In Figure 14.2A the con- onset of the conditioned response and the un-
ditioned and unconditioned stimuli were both conditioned stimulus remained relatively fixed,
applied but no conditioned response was evoked, as expected in this type of associative pheno-
a so-called failure. Figure 14.2B illustrates a trial menon (Gormezano et aI., 1983).
in which both a conditioned and an unconditioned Although there always remains some debate
response were evoked. Once the conditioning as to whether the mechanisms of associative
was complete, the reflex was activated by the ap- conditioning in this task are comparable in de-
plication of only the tone stimulus (Figure 14.2C). cerebrate and intact animals, the data clearly
After extinction, no response was evoked by the reveal that the cerebellum is not essential or
tone (Fig. 14.2D). Similar data in five animals necessary for this type of conditioned behavior
have been completely analyzed systematically, to occur. These findings also imply that the climb-
and controls for sensitization have been perform- ing fiber system is not establishing engrams within
ed (see Kelly et aI., 1990). Plots of the acquisition the cerebellar cortex critical for this type of motor
and extinction of this conditioned behavior illus- learning, as previously proposed (Thompson,
trate that the time course of the conditioning in 1986).
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 273

In our: view, there are two feasible explanationsanterior lobe. Similar to the motor learning
for the difference between the findings in our experiments, the forelimb of cats and ferrets was
experiments and those of Thompson's laboratory perturbed by a bar interposed into the swing
(McCormick et aI., 1984a, b; Thompson, 1986) phase during locomotion. In the experiments
and Yeo and colleagues (Yeo et aI., 1984, 1985a, b).reported in this section, the perturbation was
First, cerebellar ablation can have potent, long- applied intermittently, on the average of every 7
lasting effects on the excitability of brain stem to 9 step cycles.
nuclei (see Gilman et aI., 1981, for review). In a For the purpose of examining the relationship
recent preliminary report on adaptation of the between the activation of the cells' climbing fiber
VOR in goldfish (Baker et aI., 1988; Weiser et aI., inputs and their simple spike responsiveness to
1988), the prolonged tonic effects produced by the perturbation, a new multiunit data processing
cerebellar ablation were found to last for as long algorithm was developed. This method, referred
as 1 month. These data not only reiterate the to as the RTPR (real time postsynaptic response)
importance of the cerebellum's tonic action on (Lou and Bloedel, 1986), quantitatively assesses
extracerebellar nuclei, but they also demonstrate the amplitude of the population's response to a
these effects in the context of a specific type of peripheral event on a trial-by-trial basis. This
motor learning. analysis has been described extensively elsewhere
Second, cerebellar ablation can produce a defi- (Lou and Bloedel, 1986). In brief, the algorithm
cit in motor performance that may be responsible is based on the assumption that neighboring,
in part for the negative findings in some ablation sagittally oriented Purkinje cells localized to a
studies. A recent report by Welsh and Harvey surface folium have components oftheir terminal
(1989) showed that cerebellar ablation decreases fields that contact the same nuclear neuron. This
the force associated with the movement of the assumption has substantial anatomical support.
nictitating membrane over the conjunctiva. Conse- Sagittally oriented Purkinje cells have been shown
quently, a system requiring a specific force to to project to the same discrete region within the
move a lever arm attached to a low-friction cerebellar nuclei (Bishop et aI., 1979; Dietrichs,
potentiometer in order to measure the amplitude 1981; Dietrichs and Walberg, 1980; Houk and
of the nictitating membrane reflex may have too Gibson, 1987). Second, anatomical studies de-
much inertia to detect adequately the reflex after monstrated that the termination field of indivi-
cerebellar ablation. The infrared-emitting diode dual Purkinje cell axons within a region of the
detec.tor used in our paradigm measured the re- cerebellar nucleus is fairly extensive, likely con-
traction of the eyeball associated with the reflex tacting dendrites of several neighboring nuclear
rather than the nictitating membrane's movement neurons (Bishop et al., 1979; see also Palkovits
across the eyeball. Consequently, a reflex weak- et aI., 1977). For the purposes of the analysis, it
ened by cerebellar ablation may be detectable was assumed that each of the recorded neurons
with the infrared detector system while being impinges on a simple lumped-circuit linear model
immeasurable with a transducer system requiring of a nuclear neuron and that the unitary post-
the displacement of a mechanical load. synaptic potential (PSP) evoked by each of the
Purkinje cells is comparable, an argument that
also has some support in the literature (Fanardjian
and Sarkissian, 1980).
Response Properties of Sagittal Analytically, each simple spike generated by
Strips of Purkinje Cells during each Purkinje cell is converted to an analog signal
Perturbed Locomotion with a time course comparable to that of the
inhibitory PSP (IPSP) evoked in nuclear neurons
The perturbed locomotion paradigm introduced by the action of Purkinje cells (Ito et aI., 1964,
above has been actively employed in our labora- 1970). The simulated PSPs are then summed
tory to investigate the response properties of linearly in each trial using the lumped-circuit
sagittally oriented strips of 3 to 6 Purkinje cells model of the nuclear neuron. This method pro-
localized within various sagittal zones within the vides an analog response characterizing the
274 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

,activity of all the recorded neurons. Further- spike activity are shown in A-E and J-N, re-
more, the amplitude ofthis analog representation spectively. The time of occurrence of complex
of the population's output can be integrated bet- spikes in each cell is indicated by symbols located
ween windows demarcating the beginning and beneath the time axis of each digitized spike train.
end of the response period. Different symbols are used to represent the
The data shown in Figure 14.3 characterize the climbing fiber activity of each cell. The time of
initial findings from this set of experiments. This occcurrence of all the cell's complex spikes during
figure shows data from two selected trials obtained the entire 1600 ms of recording is shown in F and
while recording responses from a set of five sagit- O. The RTPR calculated from the spike train of
tally oriented Purkinje cells in the paravermal each neuron is shown in G and P for the unper-
region of the anterior lobe of an acute decerebrate turbed and perturbed trials, respectively. The time
ferret. Records obtained from a trial in which the course of the protraction and retraction of the
locomotor cycle was not perturbed are shown in ipsilateral forelimb is shown in Hand Q. These
A-I, and records from a trial in which the forelimb records represent the movement of a low-friction
ipsilateral to the recording site was perturbed potentiometer ·arm attached by a string to the
during swing phase are shown in J-R. For each forelimb of the ambulating anima,l. The time
trial the digitized spike trains of each cell's simple course of the perturbation bar's movement is

Unperturbed Perturbed

Cell 1 Cell 1

A L I I
i!i
"
I 1111 I Complex Spikes
,
J l'l;;,,__.uIl1llIlIUII---lLJ..JIIlJ..I__J Complex Spikes

Cell 2 F J " " Cell 2 o J~


Q ~+

B L I " Ig II lIP I RTPR


j 'if' "," ,,,,,,,,.,
GI¥ "1' 'H' I '
Ktl I I III II 1111 II I
'" '"
Cell 3 Cell 3

C lU!HL-~~__~I I~I~I I~1 Right Leg L 141____ , ~I!I~I LI- -l . - '


Cell 4
~ Cell 4

t
H I I

o I I 1111 I " I 11111 Perturbation ML II II III 1111 I II


+
III
Perturbation

CeliS
~;:::::=:; R~
E Lilli III III
I .1:=1

11111111
x
'-----'-__-'-__-'---', co
II II III ..
o
,
1600

9430,DEO

Figure 14.3. Analysis of the responses of five simul- F and 0, with each cell's climbing fiber responses
taneously recorded Purkinje cells during an unper- shown on a separate line. G and P: The RTPR cal-
turbed (A-I) and perturbed (J-R) locomotor cycle. culated from the unperturbed and perturbed trials. H
A- E and J - N: Digitized simple spike activity recorded and Q: The protraction and retraction of the ipsilateral
from each Purkinje cell during the unperturbed and forelimb. I and R: Time course ofthe movement ofthe
perturbed trials. The time of occurrence of each cell's perturbation bar. SS, simple spikes; CS, complex spikes;
complex spike activity is shown below each trace with RTPR, real-time postsynaptic response. (Reprinted
different symbols. The time of occurrence of these with permission from Bloedel and Lou, 1987.)
complex spikes is shown in the composite diagrams in
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 275

shown i:Q. R. The arrow in Q illustrates the time synchronous volley of climbing fiber inputs
the ferret's forelimb contacted the perturbation induces an increase in the responsiveness of the
bar. The arrowheads above the axis in P and G Purkinje neurons to inputs from the mossy fiber-
indicate the visually determined response window granular cell-parallel fiber input activated by the
used to calculate the response amplitude and the same behavioral conditions.
synchrony of the climbing fiber inputs to the
recorded neurons.
Two marked differences between the responses The Dynamic Selection Hypothesis:
observed in the unperturbed and perturbed trials An Initial Proposal Regarding
are clearly evident in this figure. First, the pertur- the Functional Importance
bation evoked synchronous climbing fiber inputs
to the simultaneously recorded Purkinje cells
of Cerebellar Sagittal Zones
(compare F and 0). Second, this synchronous Based on these initial findings and some of the
activation of climbing fiber inputs was also as- distinctive morphological features of the organ-
sociated with a much more dramatic modulation ization of both climbing and mossy fiber inputs
ofthecombined simple spike activity ofthe recor- to the cerebellar cortex reviewed above, we are
ded neurons (compare G and P). formulating a hypothesis regarding a mechanism
These two response characteristics, the syn- by which the sagittal zonation of the cerebellar
chronous activation of climbing fiber inputs and cortex could meaningfully contribute to the in-
the amplitude of the RTPR, were analyzed syste- formation processing required for the cerebellum's
matically to determine quantitatively the differences role in regulating coordinated motor behavior.
between the observations in the perturbed and The hypothesis focuses on providing a mechanism
unperturbed trials. These analyses revealed (see that functionally implements the refined topo-
Lou and Bloedel, 1986, for further details) that graphy characterizing the corticonuclear and
the occurrence of the perturbation resulted in a olivocerebellar systems as well as the output pro-
significant increase in the synchronous activation jections from the deep cerebellar nuclei (see Houk
of climbing fiber inputs during the phase of loco- and Gibson, 1987), together with the unique short-
motion demarcated by the response windows lasting heterosynaptic action of climbing fibers
across all sets of neurons analyzed. Furthermore, described above.
there was a statistically significant correlation As emphasized earlier, functionally there are
between the extent of the synchrony and the interesting dichotomies in the topographic and
amplitude of the RTPR. The gain change ratio somatotopic organization of cerebellar afferent
was also determined. This quantity depicts the and efferent systems. The relatively comparable
percent increase in response amplitude of the distribution of zones describing the olivocere-
RTPR when the climbing fiber inputs to at least bellar and corticonuclear projections provides an
50% of the recorded neurons were activated. elegant example of a precise spatial relationship
These data, together with the results from the between an afferent projection and the population
other analyses, strongly imply that there is a of output neurons on which they impinge. Intri-
relationship between the synchronous activation guingly, the input system in this circuit does not
of the climbing fiber inputs to the simultaneously contain the afferents carrying critically important
recorded neurons and their simple spike modula- quantitative information regarding ongoing
tion in response to the perturbation. changes in the periphery or activity in descending
In addition to showing that climbing fiber pathways. This information is conveyed by the
inputs to sagittally distributed Purkinje cells are mossy fiber system, an afferent system consisting
synchronously activated under appropriate be- of many different projections activated by a spec-
havioral conditions, these findings also strongly trum of peripheral receptor systems, spinal path-
support the gain change hypothesis reviewed ways, and descending projections (Bloedel and
above. The correlation between the response Courville, 1981).
amplitude and the synchrony index as well as the Furthermore, the mossy fiber system is a much
increased gain change ratio implies that this more distributed afferent system than the climbing
276 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

Perturbed Passive Perturbed


Locomotion Tap Volitional
Movement

Figure 14.4. Distribution of mossy and climbing fiber conditions, hypothetically resulting from the activation
inputs associated with three different conditions, each of afferents projecting from the forepaw (see text for
resulting in a perturbation applied to the dorsum of qualifications). The distribution of climbing fiber inputs
the animal's forepaw. The distribution of mossy fiber is shown as crosshatched strips.
inputs (stippled patches) is the same in each of the three

fiber projection (see Bloedel and Courville, 1981, populations of Purkinje cells that will be most
for review). Although the mossy fiber input pro- highly modulated by mossy fibers activated under
jects to the cerebellar cortex in a well-defined the same behavioral conditions. Functionally
organizational pattern characterized as a patchy relevant patterns of afferent convergence in the
mosaic (Llinas, 1982; Bower and Woolston, 1983; inferior olive would selectively activate climbing
Shambes, et aI., 1978), these mosaic patterns con- fibers projecting to a specific distribution of sagit-
sist of multiple representations of noncontiguous tally distributed bands of Purkinje cells within
body regions extending over rather substantial the cerebellar cortex (Robertson and Laxer, 1981).
regions of the cerebellar surface. As a consequence, These termination patterns would specify compo-
punctate stimuli are likely to activate Purkinje nents of the corticonuclear system that in turn
cells in several sagittal zones (Bloedel and Burton, project to discrete populations of cerebellar nu-
1970; Yu et aI., 1985). This dispersion ofmodula- clear neurons. Critical to this hypothesis is that
ted Purkinje and nuclear cell activity presumably these specified populations of Purkinje cells would
would minimize the contribution of the topogra- be much more dramatically modulated by ac-
phy characterizing the corticonuclear and nuclear tivated mossy fiber inputs than Purkinje cells in
efferent projections to the spatial distribution of other sagittal zones due to the heterosynaptic
inputs to extracerebellar sites. action of climbing fibers on the simple spike re-
Based on the premise that a mechanism must sponsiveness of these neurons.
exist to produce a spatially focused modulation To provide a better qualitative understanding
of Purkinje and nuclear cell activity appropriate of this view, three conditions are compared in
to a set of existing physiological conditions, we Figure 14.4. This example assumes that a stimulus
are proposing the "dynamic selection hypothesis." applied to the forepaw activates the same mossy
According to this view, the distribution of climb- fiber input under three different behavioral condi-
ing fiber inputs activated by specific patterns of tions: a passive tap of the forepaw dorsum, a tap
convergence in the inferior olive determines those of the forepaw resulting from a perturbation of an
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 277

"automlltic" movement such as locomotion, and The objective of the experiment is to record
a tap of the forepaw dorsum resulting from a simultaneously from two zones receiving mossy
perturbation applied during a volitional goal- fiber inputs evoked by the perturbation of the
directed movement. The distribution of this input forelimb, with only one zone receiving climbing
is shown as stippled patches in each of the three fiber inputs activated by the same stimulus. For
panels. It is acknowledged that this is a simplifi- the dynamic selection hypothesis to be tenable,
cation. Certainly, integration occurring in pre- there should be a substantially greater modula-
cerebellar nuclei could modify the distribution of tion of simple spike activity in the zone receiving
mossy fiber inputs responding to the peripheral the synchronously activated climbing fiber input.
stimulus. However, this possibility is not pertinent Figure 14.5 illustrates the data from one of the
to the inferences to be drawn from the example. sets of neurons recorded in the course of this
It is also acknowledged that mossy fiber inputs study. The format of this figure is similar to that
in addition to those responding to the forepaw of Figure 14.3. The cells were recorded with both
would be activated in each ofthe three conditions. a medial (array 2, K-Q) and a lateral (array 1,
However, this example focuses only on the mossy A-J) array during a single trial in which the step
fiber input activated as a consequence of the tap cycle of the cat was perturbed with the bar inter-
on the dorsum ofthe forepaw. Consequently, the posed into the trajectory during swing phase. The
climbing fiber and parallel fiber inputs both con- digitized simple spike activity of the six cells
verge on different populations of Purkinje cells in recorded with the lateral array and the three cells
each circumstance. Based on the gain change hy- recorded with the medial array is shown in B-G
pothesis, it is assumed that these patterns of con- and L-N, respectively. The time of occurrence of
vergence "select" or specify the components of each cell's complex spike is shown beneath each
the corticonuclear system to be most highly modu- record using a different symbol. A and K show
lated by the mossy fiber-granule cell-parallel the temporal relationship of the complex spikes
fiber inputs under each behavioral condition. evoked in all of the neurons on both arrays, with
Because of the topography of the corticonuclear the complex spike activity of each Purkinje cell
projection, this interaction will also specify the shown on separate lines. The protraction and
cerebellar output neurons to be most affected by retraction of the perturbed forelimb are shown in
cerebellar cortical interactions. identical records in I and P; and the contact of
the bar by the limb is shown in J and Q, also in
Preliminary Experimental identical records. Last, the RTPR calculated from
Evaluation of the Dynamic the responses of each subgroup of neurons is
shown in Hand O.
Selection Hypothesis Notice that four of the six Purkinje cells record-
Our laboratory is currently engaged in a series of ed with the lateral-most array received synchro-
experiments designed to test aspects of the dy- nously-activated climbing fiber inputs in response
namic selection hypothesis. These experiments are to the perturbation (A). A comparable synchrony
being performed in locomoting, acutely decere- was clearly absent in the more medial zone (K).
brate cats in which two sagittal strips of Purkinje Similarly the simple spike activity of the neurons
cells are recorded simultaneously using two arrays in the lateral zone was much more highly modu-
of five electrodes each. One set of electrodes is lated, as revealed by a comparison of the RTPRs
placed in a more medial sagittal zone, usually in Hand O. The responses recorded from this
within the A or B zone, and the more lateral array particular set of neurons have been analyzed for
is localized on the same folium in either the C 1 , both the synchrony index as well as the RTPR
C z, or C 3. zone. The precise zone in which the amplitude and gain change ratio. The synchrony
recording array is located is assessed based on its index for the lateral and medial groups of Purkinje
distance from midline (Yu et aI., 1985) as well as cells was 00405 and 0.114, respectively. The gain
the criterion of Oscarsson (1979) regarding the change ratio for the lateral cells was 204.65%.
somatotopic organization of climbing fiber inputs The value of this quantity for the medial group
to each of the sagittal zones. could not be calculated because of the low syn-
278 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

ARRAY 1 ARRAY 2 chrony index for these neurons.


(C1 Zone) (A Zone) Clearly, in this experiment there is a dramatic
difference between the responses of the cells re-
.. ..
CS

A ·• K
. .. corded with the two arrays. Consistent with the
dynamic selection hypothesis, the simple spike
modulation was much more dramatic in the strip
of cells also receiving a synchronous volley of
Cell 1 climbing fiber inputs. More studies are required

B I II I
Cell 2
II II
Cell 7
in order to evaluate this question further. How-
ever, these initial findings provide strong en-
C I I I111 I 111111 L .
1111111 III III 1111 I couragement for continuing to test the idea.
Cell 3 Cell 8
D 111111111 111111 III M ""~II",,illll~~---::-....,....,~ Conclusion and Comment
Cell 4 Cell 9
E JI N 111111110111 The data employing the perturbed locomotion
IllillUlllIlIllllW II
paradigm support the notion that at least a part
CeliS
F I11II1 111111 W H ofthe action of the climbing fiber system is related
to its capacity to produce a short-lasting enhance-
Cell 6 ment of the Purkinje cell's simple spike respon-
G 1111
• siveness to the mossy fiber-granule cell-parallel
fiber input. There continues to be little evidence
in any of our experiments for the view that the
o~ climbing fibers produce a long-term change in
the responsiveness of Purkinje cells. Not only
p ,-j_/'~~---!l have we failed to find any long-lasting changes,
but the changes we have observed consist of an
Pb
enhancement rather than a depression of the cell's
J IlIl responsiveness as required by several of the learn-
o msec 1600
ing theories. Furthermore, behavioral data from
other laboratories (Kim et aI., 1987; Mano et aI.,
Figure 14.5. Analysis of responses recorded simulta1 1986) have provided additional evidence that the
neously from electrodes in two different arrays, one
climbing fiber system is activated in relationship
located more medially in the A zone (K-Q) and one
to volitional arm movements in a manner con-
located laterally in the C, zone (A-J). Cells 1 to 6 were
recorded with array 1, whereas cells 7 to 9 were recorded
sistent with an operational role of the climbing
with array 2. All records were obtained during the fiber system in regulating the output of the cere-
same perturbation trial in an ambulating, acutely bellar cortex (Andersson and Armstrong, 1987;
decerebrate cat. B-G and L-N: Digitized simple spike Armstrong and Edgley, 1984; Bloedel and Ebner,
activity of each recorded Purkinje cell. The time of 1985; Llimls, 1985; Pellionisz, 1985) rather than
occurrence of each cell's complex spikes is indicated a role in establishing engrams in the cerebellum
beneath each record. These complex spikes are shown required for motor learning (Thompson, 1986).
in a composite diagram for each electrode array in A Furthermore, the lesion studies in our laboratory,
and K, with the complex spikes from each cell being (Bloedel et aI., 1987; Bloedel and Zuo, 1989; Kelly
shown on a separate line. Hand Q: RTPR calculated
et aI., 1990) continue to cast considerable doubt
from the cells' responses recorded with the lateral and
on the premise that the cerebellum is essential
medial electrode arrays, respectively. I and P: The
protraction and retraction of the forelimb recorded and sufficient for at least two types of conditioned
with a potentiometer attached by string to the ipsilat- reflex behaviour: the nictitating membrane/
eral forelimb. J and Q: The sensor output illustrating eyeblink reflex in the rabbit and the avoidance
the time of contact of the forelimb with the perturbation conditioning of the forelimb movement in the
bar. ferret.
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 279

The dynamic selection hypothesis is intended elegantly systematized the fundamental charac-
to provide an initial proposal regarding the teristics of the cerebellar circuitry.
functional role of the cerebellar sagittal zones.
Although at present this view is highly qualitative
and very speculative, the initial studies designed Acknowledgements. We wish to thank Mr. Joel
to test the suppositions on which it is based have McAlduff for his help in the experiments and
been supportive. One of its most appealing Ms. Deirdre Janus for her help in preparing the
features is its consistency with most ofthe organi- manuscript. This research was supported by NIH
zational and physiological features of the olivo- grant NS 21958 and NIH grant NS 07309.
cerebellar system. For example, this view can easily
incorporate the electrotonic coupling of inferior References
olivary neurons (Llinas et at, 1974, Llinas and Andersson, G. and Armstrong, D.M. (1987): Complex
Yarom, 1981) as well as the data from the multi- spikes in Purkinje cells in the Lateral vermis (b Zone)
unit recording studies of Llinas and colleagues ofthe cat cerebellum during locomotion. J. Physiol.,
illustrating the inherent rhythmicity in the olivo- 385, 107-134.
cerebellar system and the expression of this Armstrong, D.M., and Edgley, S.A. (1984): Discharges
rhythmicity in the activation of sagittal strips of of Purkinje cells in the paravermal part of the cere-
Purkinje cells (Bower and Llinas, 1983; Sasaki bellar anterior lobe during locomotion in the cat.
et at, 1989). In addition, the new electrophysio- J. Physiol., 352, 403-424.
logical findings regarding the dynamic properties Baker, R., Weiser, M., and McElligott, J.G. (1988):
Adaptive gain control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex
of the dendritic tree of the Purkinje cells presented
in goldfish. I. Hemicerebellectomy. Soc. Neurosci.
in this symposium (see presentations by Llinas Abstr., 14, 169.
and Sugimori in this volume as well as Llinas and Bishop, G.A., McCrea, R.A., Lighthall, J.W., and Kitai,
Sugimori, 1980a; Tank et at, 1988) provide direct S.T. (1979): An HRP and autoradiographic study of
evidence for the existence of active modifications the projection from the cerebellar cortex to the nu-
in dendritic characteristics that could underlie cleus interpositus anterior and nucleus interpositus
the change in responsiveness of Purkinje cells. posterior of the cat. J. Compo Neurol., 185,735-756.
In closing, it should be emphasized that the Bloedel, J.R., and Burton, J.E. (1970): Electrophysiolo-
hypothesis presented here addresses only one gical evidence for a mossy fiber input to the cerebellar
feature of the operation performed by the climbing cortex activated indirectly by collaterals of spino-
cerebellar pathways. J. Neurophysiol., 33, 308-320.
fiber input to the cerebellum. Specifically, this
Bloedel, J.R., and Courville, J. (1981): A review of
view addresses its action on the responsiveness
cerebellar afferent systems. In: Handbook of Physio-
of Purkinje cells and the spatial integration that logy, Vol. II. Motor Control. (V.B. Brooks, ed.).
may result due to the overlapping relationship of Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, pp. 735-830.
the olivocerebellar and corticonuclear projections. Bloedel, J.R., and Ebner, T.J. (1985): Climbing fiber
The precise contribution of the climbing fiber function: Regulation of Purkinje cell responsiveness.
system to motor execution and the way in which In: Cerebellar Functions. (J.R. Bloedel, J. Dichgans,
this system is integrated into the control systems and W. Precht, eds). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York:
responsible for regulating ongoing movements Springer-Verlag, pp. 247-259.
are not addressed. However, the consistency of Bloedel, J.R., Ebner, TJ., and Yu, Q.-X. (1983): In-
these data with other electro physiological and creased responsiveness of Purkinje cells associated
anatomical findings from several laboratories is with climbing fiber inputs to neighboring neurons.
J. Neurophysiol., 50, 220-239.
contributing useful insights into the functional
Bloedel, J.R., and Lou, J.8. (1987): The relation between
characteristics of a neurobiological system whose Purkinje cell simple spike responses and the action
precise function has been an elusive subject for of the climbing fiber system in unconditioned and
investigators. It is appropriate that these increased conditioned responses offorelimb to perturbed loco-
insights into this system's operational character- motion. In: Cerebellum and Neuronal Plasticity.
istics are occurring at a symposium held to honor (M. Glickstein,J. Stein, and C. Yeo, eds). New York:
the contributions of Ramon y Cajal, who so Plenum, pp. 261-276.
280 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

Bloedel, JR., and Zuo, e.-e. (1989): The heterosynaptic Ebner, T.J., Yu, Q.-x., and Bloedel, JR. (1983): Increase
action of climbing fibers in the cerebellar cortex. in Purkinje cell gain associated with naturally ac-
Exp. Brain Res. Series 17, 246-264. tivated climbing fiber inputs. 1. Neurophysiol., 50,
Bloedel, J.R., Zuo, e.-e., Ferguson, R., and Lou, J.-S. 205-219.
(1987): Modifications in the character of a condi- Eccles, J.e., Llinas, R., and Sasaki, K. (1966): The
tioned response produced by extensive cerebellar excitatory synaptic action of climbing fibres on the
lesions in the decerebrate ambulating ferret. Soc. Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. 1. Physiol., 182,
N eurosci. Abstr., 13, 232. 268-296.
Bower, J, and Llinas, R. (1983): Simultaneous sampling Ekerot, e.-F., and Kano, M. (1985): Long-term depres-
of the responses of closely adjacent Purkinje cells sion of parallel fibre synapses following stimulation
responding to climbing fiber activation. Soc. Neuro- of climbing fibres. Brain Res., 342,357-360.
sci. Abstr., 9, 607. Fanardjian, B.V., and Sarkissian, V.A. (1980): Spatial
Bower, J.M., and Woolston, D.e. (1983): Congruence organization of the cerebellar corticovestibular pro-
of spatial organization of tactile projections to gra- jection in the cat. Neuroscience,S, 551-558.
nule cell and Purkinje cell layers of cerebellar hemi- Gellman, R., Gibson, A.R., and Houk, J.e. (1985):
spheres of the albino rat: Vertical organization of Inferior olivary neurons in the awake cat: Detection
cerebellar cortex. 1. Neurophysiol., 49, 745-766. of contact and passive body movement. 1. Neuro-
Brodal, A., and Kawamura, K. (1980): Olivocerebellar physiol., 54, 40-60.
projection: A review. Advances in Anat. Embryol. and Gellman, R., Houk, J.e., and Gibson, A.R. (1983):
Cell BioI., 64,1-140. Somatosensory properties of the inferior olive ofthe
Courville, J. (1975): Distribution of olivo-cerebellar cat. 1. Compo Neurol., 215, 228-243.
fibers demonstrated by a radio-autographic tracing Gilman, S., Bloedel, J.R., and Lechtenberg, R. (1981):
method. Brain Res., 95, 253-263. Disorders ofthe Cerebellum. Philadelphia: Davis Co.
Courville, J., and Diakiw, N. (1976): Cerebellar cortico- Gormezano, I., Kehoe, E.J., and Marshall, B.S. (1983):
nuclear projection in the cat. The vermis of the Twenty years of classical conditioning research
anterior and posterior lobes. Brain Res., 110, 1-20. with the rabbit. In: Progress in Psychobiology and
Courville, J, Diakiw, N., and Brodal, A. (1973): Cere- Physiological Psychology, Vol: 10. (J. Sprague, and
bellar corticonuclear projection in the cat. The para- A.N. Epstein, eds). New York: Academic Press,
median lobule: An experimental study with silver pp. 197-275.
methods. Brain Res., 50, 25-45. Groenewegen, H.J., and Voogd, J. (1977): The parasa-
Courville, J., and Faraco-Cantin, F. (1980): Topography gittal zonation within the olivocerebellar projection.
of the olivocerebellar projection: An experimental I. Climbing fiber distribution in the vermis of the cat
study in the cat with an auto radiographic tracing cerebellum. 1. Compo Neurol., 174,417-488.
method. In: The Inferior Olivary Nucleus: Anatomy Groenewegen, H.J., Voogd, J., and Freedman, S.L.
and Physiology. (J. Courville, e. DeMontigny, (1979): The parasagittal zonation within the olivo-
and Y. Lamarre, eds). New York: Raven Press, cerebellar projection. II. Climbing fiber distribution
pp. 235- 277. in the intermediate and hemispheric parts of cat
Dietrichs, E. (1981): The cerebellar corticonuclear and cerebellum. 1. Compo Neurol., 183, 551-601.
nucleocortical projections in the cat as studied with Haines, D.E., Patrick, G.W., and Satrulee, P. (1982):
anterograde and retrograde transport of horseradish Organization of cerebellar corticonuclear fiber sys-
peroxidase. III. The anterior lobe; Anat. Embryol., tems. In: The Cerebellum-New Vistas. (S.L. Palay, and
162,223-247. V. Chan-Palay, eds). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-
Dietrichs, E., and Walberg, F. (1980): The cerebellar Verlag, pp. 320-371.
corticonuclear and nucleocortical projections in Houk, J.e., and Gibson, A.R. (1986): Sensorimotor
the cat as studied with anterograde and retrograde processing through the cerebellum. In: New Con-
transport of horseradish peroxidase. II. Lobulus cepts in Cerebellar Neurobiology. (J.S. King, and
simplex, Crus I and Crus II. Anat. Embryol., 161, J. Courville, eds). New York: Alan R. Liss, pp. 387-
83-103. 416.
Ebner, T.J., and Bloedel, J.R. (1981): Role of climbing Ito, M., and Kano, M. (1982): Long-lasting depression
fiber afferent input in determining the responsiveness of parallel fiber-Purkinje cell transmission induced
of Purkinje cells to mossy fiber inputs. 1. Neuro- by conjunctive stimulation of parallel fibers and
physiol., 45, 962-971. climbing fibers in the cerebellar cortex. Neurosci.
Ebner, T.J., and Bloedel, J.R. (1984): Climbing fiber Lett., 33, 253-258.
actiorr on the responsiveness of Purkinje cells to Ito, M., Sakurai, M., and Tongroach, P. (1982): Climb-
parallel fiber inputs. Brain Res., 309, 182-186. ing fibre-induced depression of both mossy fibre
14. Dynamic Selection Hypothesis 281

responl'iveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebel- Different types of voltage-dependent ionic conduc-
lar Purkinje cells. J. Physiol., 324, 113-134. tances. J. Physiol., 315,549-567.
Ito, M., Yoshida, M., and Obata, K. (1964): Monosyn- Lou, 1.-S., and Bloedel, J.R. (1986): The responses of
aptic inhibition ofthe intracerebellar nuclei induced simultaneously recorded Purkinje cells to perturba-
from the cerebellar cortex. Experientia, 20, 575- tions of the step cycle in the walking ferret: A study
576. using a new analytical method-the real time post-
Ito, M., Yoshida, M., Obata, K., Kawai, N., and Udo, synaptic response (RTPR). Brain Res., .365, 340-
M. (1970): Inhibitory control of intracerebellar nuclei 344.
by the Purkinje cell axons. Exp. Brain. Res., 10, Lou, J.-S., and Bloedel, J.R. (1988a): A new condition-
64-80. ing paradigm: Conditioned limb movements in loco-
Jansen, J., and Brodal, A. (1940): Experimental studies moting deerebrate ferrets. Neurosci. Lett., 84, 185-
on the intrinsic fibers of the cerebellum. II. The cor- 190.
ticonuclear projections. J. Compo Neurol., 73, Lou, J.-S., and Bloedel, J.R. (1988b): A study of cere-
267-321. bellar cortical involvement in motor learning using
Kano, M., and Kato, M. (1987): Quisqualate receptors a new avoidance conditioning paradigm involving
are specifically involved in cerebellar synaptic plas- limb movement. Brain Res., 445, 171-174.
ticity. Nature, 325, 276-278. Mano, N.I., Kanazawa, I., and Yamamoto, K.I. (1986):
Kelly, T.M., Zuo, c.-c., and Bloedel, J.R. (1990): Complex-spike activity of cerebellar Purkinje cells
Classical conditioning of the eye blink reflex in the related to wrist tracking movement in monkey. J.
decerebrate-decerebellate rabbit. Behav. Brain Res., Neurophysiol.,56,137-158.
38,7-18. McCormick, D.A., Stienmetz, J.E., and Thompson,
Kim, 1.H., Wang, J.-J., and Ebner, TJ. (1987): Climbing R.F. (1985): Lesions of the inferior olivary complex
fiber afferent modulation during treadmi1llocomo- cause extinction of the classically conditioned eye-
tion in the cat. J. Neurophysiol., 57, 787-802. blink response. Brain Res., 359, 120-130.
Llinas, R. (1982): Radial connectivity in the cerebellar McCormick, D.A., and Thompson, R.F. (1984a): Cere-
cortex: A novel view regarding the functional organi- bellum: Essential involvement in the classically con-
zation of the molecular layer. In: The Cerebellum- ditioned eyelid response. Science, 223, 296-299.
New Vistas. (S.L. Palay, and V. Chan-Palay, eds.) McCormick, D.A., and Thompson, R.F. (1984b):
Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 189-194. Neuronal responses of the rabbit cerebellum during
Llinas, R. (1985): Functional significance of the basic acquisition and performance of a classically con-
cerebellar circuit in motor coordination. In: Cere- ditioned nictitating membrane-eyelid response. J.
bellar Functions. (J.R. Bloedel, J. Dichgans, and Neurosci., 4, 2811-2822.
W. Precht, eds.) Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Oscarsson, O. (1979): Functional units of the cerebellum-
Springer-Verlag, pp. 170-185. sagittal zones and microzones. Trends N eurosci., 2,
Llinits, R., Baker, R., and Sotelo, C. (1974): Electrotonic 143-145.
coupling between neurons in cat inferior olive. J. Palkovits, M., Mesey, E., Harmori, J., and Szentago-
Neurophysiol., 37, 560-571. thai, 1. (1977): Quantitative histological analysis of
Llinas, R., and Nicholson, C. (1976): Reversal properties the cerebellar nuclei in the cat. I. Numerical data on
of climbing fiber potential in cat Purkinje cells: An cells and on synapses. Exp. Brain Res., 28, 189-
example of a distributed synapse. J. Neurophysiol., 209.
39,311-323. Pellionisz, AJ. (1985): Tensorial brain theory in cere-
Llinas, R., and Sasaki, K. (1989): The functional organi- bellar modelling. In: Cerebellar Functions: (J.R.
zation of the olivocerebellar system as examined by Bloedel, J. Dichgans, and W. Precht, eds.) Berlin,
multiple Purkinje cell recordings. Eur. J. Neurosci., Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer-Verlag,
1,587-602. pp.201-299.
Llinas, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980a): Electrophysio- Pellionisz, A., and Llinas, R. (1977): A computer model
logical properties of in vitro Purkinje cell dendrites of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neuroscience, 2,37-48.
in mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol., 305, Robertson, L.T., and Laxer, H.D. (1981): Localization
197-213.. of cutaneously elicited climbing fiber responses in
Llinas, R., and Sugimori, M. (1980b): Electrophysio- lobule V of the monkey cerebellum. Brain Behav.
logical properties of in vitro Purkinje cell somata in Evol., 18, 157-168.
mammalian cerebellar slices. J. Physiol., 305, 171- Robertson, L.T., Laxer, H.D., and Rushmer, D.s. (1982):
195. Organization of climbing fiber input from mechano-
Llinas, R., and Yarom, Y. (1981): Electrophysiology receptors to lobule V vermal cortex in the cat. Exp.
of mammalian inferior olivary neurons in vitro. Brain Res., 46, 281-291.
282 James R. Bloedel and Thomas M. Kelly

Rosina, A., and Provini, L. (1983): Somatotopy of climb- Voogd, 1., and Bigare, F. (1980): Topographical dis-
ing fiber branching to the cerebellar cortex in the tribution of olivary and corticonuclear fibers in
cat. Brain Res., 289, 45-63. the cerebellum: A review. In: The Inferior Olivary
Sasaki, K., Bower, J.M., and Llinas, R. (1989): Multiple Nucleus: Anatomy and Physiology. (J. Courville,
Purkinje cell recording in rodent cerebellar cortex. C. de Montigny, and Y. Lamarre, eds.) New York:
Euro. J. Neurosci., 1, 572-586. Raven Press, pp. 207-234.
Shambes, G.M., Gibson, J.M., and Welker, W. (1978): Weiser, M., McElligott, J.R., and Baker, R. (1988):
Fractured somatotopy in granule cell tactile areas Adaptive gain control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex
of rat cerebellar hemispheres revealed by micro- in goldfish. II. Total cerebellectomy. Soc. Neurosci.
mapping. Brain Behav. Evol., 15,94-140. Abstr., 14, 169.
Tank, D.W., Sugimori, M., Connor, J.A., and Llinas, Welsh, J.P., and Harvey, J.A. (1989): Cerebellar lesions
R.R. (1988): Spatially resolved calcium dynamics of and the nictitating membrane reflex: Performance
mammalian Purkinje cells in cerebellar slice. Science, deficits of the conditioned and unconditioned re-
242,773-777. sponse. J. Neurosci., 9, 299-311.
Thompson, R.F. (1986): The neurobiology oflearning Yeo, C.H., Hardiman, M.J., and Glickstein, M. (1984):
and memory. Science, 223, 941-947. Discrete lesions of the cerebellar cortex abolish the
Tolbert, D.L., Bantli, H., and Bloedel, J.R. (1976): classically conditioned nictitating membrane re-
Anatomical and physiological evidence for a cere- sponse ofthe rabbit. Behav. Brain Res., 13, 261-266.
bellar nucleocortical projection in the cat. N euro- Yeo, C.H., Hardiman, M.J., and Glickstein, M. (1985a):
science, 1, 205-217. Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane
Tolbert, D.L., Bantli, H., and Bloedel, J.R. (1978): response of the rabbit. I. Lesions of the cerebellar
Organizational features of the cat and monkey cere- nuclei. Exp. Brain Res., 60, 87-98.
bellar nucleocortical projection. J. Compo Neurol., Yeo, c.H., Hardiman, M.J., and Glickstein, M. (1985b):
182,39-56. Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane
Voogd, J. (1969): The importance of fiber connections response of the rabbit. II. Lesions of the cerebellar
in the comparative anatomy of the mammalian cere- cortex. Exp. Brain Res., 60, 99-113.
bellum. In: Neurobiology of Cerebellar Evolution and Yu, Q-X, Ebner, T.J., and Bloedel, J.R. (1985): Electro-
Development. (R. Llinas, ed.) Chicago: American physiological study of the corticonuclear projection
Medical Association, pp. 493-541. in the cat cerebellum. Brain Res., 237,121-134.
15
Cerebellar Output: Multiple Maps and
Modes of Control in Movement Coordination
W. Thomas Thach, S.A. Kane, J.W. Mink, and H.P. Goodkin

For the scientist, one of the chief goals in cerebellar fined to a single joint, the wrist. The second battery
research has been to find physiological meaning of tasks consisted of fiye untrained "natural"
in the function of the anatomical circuitry. Ramon movements that involved multiple joints.
y Cajal, in providing many of the anatomical These studies test two hypotheses. One is the
details of the circuitry, did not hesitate to specu- hypothesis, developed from prior work (Asanuma
late on its function. Since then, elucidation of.the et al., 1983; Thach, 1970, 1978; Thach et aI., 1982),
synaptic actions of the different neurons forming that within the cerebellum there are multiple
those circuits has brought a functional explana- maps of the body and multiple modes of motor
tion closer to hand (Eccles et aI., 1967; Llinas, control. Each deep cerebellar nucleus is proposed
1981). Yet there has remained the technical pro- to contain a separate somatotopic representation
blem of relating the actions of the neural cir- of the body (Fig. 15.1), and to have a unique
cuitry to the sphere of activity that it undoubtedly control function over the body: the fastigius is
principally controls, bodily posture and move- proposed to assist stance and gait; interpositus,
ment. to speed initiation of movements triggered by
For the neurologist, a chief goal has been to sensory cues that guide the response, to stabilize
recognize, from some abnormality in behavior, holds, and possibly both to damp and to promote
when the cerebellum is injured, and whether dif- oscillation; and dentate to speed initiation of
ferent parts of the cerebellum give different mani- movements triggered by mental or arbitrary sen-
festations of injury. From the classic ablation sory cues, and to control multijointed movements
studies of Fluorens and Luciani in animals through more than single-jointed movements. The second
to the elegant clinical studies of Babinski and hypothesis is that there is a common function
especially Holmes in man, it has been clear that across maps and modes of movement that is con-
the behavioral deficits are principally if not ex- sistent with the physiological experiments and the
clusively of posture and movement. Yet there has anatomical circuitry. That function, as originally
been disagreement on whether or not function is proposed by Fluorens in 1824, is coordination of
localized and if so how, and what the one over- movement.
riding function of the circuitry really is.
The following studies attempt to answer some
of these questions through use of two relatively Methods
new techqiques, single-unit recording and transient Two Complementary Methods
multicell inactivation in awake trained animals to Define Control Signals
(Evarts, 1966; Hikosaka and Wurtz, 1983; Hubel,
from Cerebellar Nuclei
1960; Ricci et aI., 1957). Two types of behavior
were examined. One battery of tasks consisted of Two methods were used to determine the nature
five overtrained instrumented movements con- of the control signal that is generated by the

283
tv
00
.!>-
Motor Cortex
<J VL Thalamus 0

Interneurons (Cord), Inferior Olive, OUTPUT


Other brain stem nuclei <J Red Nucleus 0

Motor ond Inter - neurons(Cord + Brain Stem) <J Vestibular Nuc o

~~
~~

INPUT
<J "Association" Cerebral Cortex
Pontine Nuclei

<J------ Somatosensory Receptors(Limbs), Motor Cortex


Mossy Fibers (Input) Spinocerebella rand
~
Nuclear Cell Flbers(Output) ....,
Pontine Nuclei p-
Excitatory o C> <J~---------------- Somatosensory Receptors(Trunk), Aud.- Vis. Info. o
Spinocerebellar Nuclei (Aud. and S
I>'
Vis. pathways unknown)
Purkinje Cell Fibers Labyrinths '"....,
o p-
Inhibitory • Vestibular Nuclei I>'
(')
p-
Figure 15.1. Diagram of the body representation with each of the deep cerebellar nuclei. (Adapted with permission from Asanuma et aI., 1983. Copyright by 2
Elsevier Science Publishers.) ~
15. Cerebellar Output 285

cerebellar nuclei. One method was the extracel- cient for proposing that the signal controls the
lular recording with metal microelectrodes of the behavior in question.
discharge of single cerebellar nuclear cells during
motor performance. Since this discharge is in fact Battery of Five Instrument-Trained
the cerebellar output signal, modulation of neural Multijoint Movements
discharge in relation to time of onset of motor
Figure 15.2 shows the monkey seated in a modified
performance and to some aspects and not others
Forenger primate chair with its head, trunk, and
provides one of the strongest clues we have as to
elbow restrained. The wrist (and eyes) alone are
what aspects of behavior the cerebellar signals
allowed to move freely. The hand is inserted,
are capable of controlling. A second method is
fingers extended, into a glove-shaped manipulan-
the temporary or permanent inactivation of pools
dum that is moved in a horizontal plane by flexing
of neurons with muscimol (Hikosaka and Wurtz,
or extending the wrist. This involves chiefly the
1983) or kainic acid (McGeer and McGeer, 1978),
long flexors and extensors of the fingers (Schieber
respectively. A Hamilton syringe with a 26-gauge
and Thach, 1985). Electromyography (EMG) is
needle was inserted into the microdrive system
recorded periodically with electrodes pasted onto
and 1 JLI of muscimol or kainic acid injected
the skin over the muscles of many parts of the
stereotaxically where task-related unit activity
body in addition to the operant arm. A poten-
had been recorded. Judging from the type of
tiometer monitors angular wrist position; a tacho-
deficit produced as a function of the site of the
meter, angular velocity; velocity is differentiated
injection, and of the volume of necrosis after
to give acceleration. A visual target consists of a
kainic acid injections, the sphere of inactivated
vertical band or "window" on an oscilloscope
neurons was about 1 mm in diameter. More than
screen in front of the animal. The target moves
30 muscimol injections were made to map the
right and left under the control of a Rockwell
nuclei without permanent injury, the effects typi-
AIM-65 microcomputer. The manipulandum is
cally wearing off in 7 to 10 hours. This removes
coupled to a cursor, which appears as a vertical
the cerebellar output signal from the downstream
line on the oscilloscope. The job of the animal is
circuits that more directly control the behavior.
to line up the cursor within the target window,
The type of deficit that this causes in behavior
and to maintain the alignment despite movements
should be complementary to the type of correla-
of the window.
tion that the neural discharge has with behavior.
Theoretically, the neural discharge would be the The Tests. Animals perfomed five tasks designed
positive image of the negative deficit in behavior to dissociate the functions of the longitudinal cere-
caused by cerebellar ablation. It is important to bellar zones (Eager, 1966; Jansen and Brodal, 1940)
emphasize the complementary nature of the two by flexing and extending the wrist (Fig. 15.2). To
methods of evaluation, since the restricted use of test the dentate nucleus, subjects performed a) jerk
just the one or the other can lead to logical errors (no stop on target): a prompt visual-triggered
of circuit analysis. These errors are different and move (Fig. 15.2, top). The subject aligned his cur-
particular to the method employed. Seeing that sor on the band by holding the wrist 35° in flexion
a recorded neural signal correlates with the be- or extension. At an interval randomly varied be-
havior does not necessarily mean that the signal tween 2 and 4 s, the band shifted instantaneously
caused the behavior or that the behavior caused 70° in the opposite direction to a new position.
the signal. This is the error of false or indirect The subject was required to begin to move within
correlation. Seeing a deficit in behavior produced the allowed reaction time and to extend (or flex)
by ablation may suggest that a particular neural the wrist the full 70° through and past the target.
control signal has been removed, but is not suffi- This tests the hypothetical function of the dentate
cient to prove that the signal really ever existed. nucleus. Neural discharge should change before
This is the error of deduction from insufficient EMG change and movement onset. A functional
evidence. But the recording of a neural signal that deficit would show itself as a delayed onset time;
is the complement of the neurological deficit caused b) jump (stop on target): a prompt visual-trig-
by its removal are together necessary and suffi- gered move identical to that above, except that
286 W. Thomas Thach et al.

FLEX TIME
I
EXTEND ·
5 I~RWD
,
I 70.
: : load: Torque
-----,----', :
I

Jerk
~ RT 'MT

Jump
---~~ RT :MT'
____~-;;Rwii--
--------~I ~'----------
Pert : : Load: Torqve
-- - - - - ______1 I

Torque RT ~WD

5
/
Ramp

RAM

Figure 15.2. Two batteries of behavioral tasks. Trained jointed movements are on the right. RT, reaction time;
wrist movements are on the left. Nontrained multi- MT, movement time; RWD, reward.

the subject was required to move and stop within EMG and movement. A deficit in dentate function
the visual target and hold there for at least 0.9 s would show itself as a prolonged reaction time.
(Fig. 15.2, second from top). This tests the hypo- Interpositus signals should change before termi-
thetical functions of the dentate and interpositus. nation of movement and movement terminating
Dentate signals should change before onset of EMG changes, and correlate with terminal tremor.
15. Cerebellar Output 287

A deficit in interpositus function would show it- modulate in relation to RAM; discharge from
self as a delay in ending the movement, and a other nuclei should not. A deficit in interpositus
pronounced endpoint tremor in attempting to function would show itself as a slowing of the
hold the final position; to test the interposed frequency of the alternating movements and a
nucleus further, subjects performed c) pert: return loss of regularity of period.
to central hold position after perturbation from
it by torque step (Fig. 15.2, third from top). For Battery of Five Natural Nontrained
this task, the target band was centered in the Postures and Movements
middle of the oscilloscope screen. As for the above
tasks, the subject aligned his cursor on the band, A second stereotyped battery of natural move-
in this task by holding the wrist in a "central" ments was also studied before and after nuclear
position of 10° of flexion. At an interval randomly inactivation (Fig. 15.2, right column). This group
varied between 2 and 4 s, the torque load main- of movements consisted of standing, sitting,
tained against flexion (or extension) suddenly walking, reaching for pieces of fruit from the
switched to oppose extension (or flexion), per- examiner's hand, and using thumb and forefinger
turbing the hold and displacing the hand from to pick pieces of fruit from deep, narrow food
the central position. The subject then had to react wells. Stance, sitting, and walking were evaluated
within the allowed reaction time to return the immediately after the animal was released from
hand to the central position. This tests the hypo- the primate chair. Reaching involved coordinated
thetical function of the interposed nucleus. A movement of shoulder and elbow; reaching was
performed while sitting and standing on the floor
deficit in interpositus function would show itself
as a delay in the return ofthe hand after perturba- and while sitting in the primate chair. The chair
reduced the requirements for active postural ad-
tion ofthe central hold position, and an increased
terminal oscillation around that position; d) rainp: justments of trunk and other limbs and permitted
ramp tracking of visual target against viscous easier assessment of the relative contributions
and negative viscous loads (Fig. 15.2, fourth from of shoulder and elbow. The precision pinch task
top). As for the above tasks, the subject aligned required coordinated movements of the hand and
his cursor within the target band on the oscillo- fingers, including the pinch per se of thumb and
scope by holding the wrist 35° in flexion or exten- forefinger, and "tea-cup" posturings of the other
sion. At an interval randomly varied between 2 digits to keep them out of the way ofthe edges of
and 4 s, the target band began to move at a the food well. These movements were movie-filmed
velocity of 28° per s. The subject had to begin to or video-taped from front, back, and side views,
move before the target band left his cursor behind and analyzed frame by frame. Drawings were
to track the target band remaining within it~ made of individual frames or evenly spaced frame
sequences. Distances were measured for these
boundaries for an excursion of 70° until it stopped,
and to maintain the hold for an additional 0.9 s. natural movements by comparison with known
lengths of the examiner's hand, the monkey's
This tests the hypothetical function of the inter-
posed nucleus and its cerebellar cortical input. body, and the grid pattern of floor tiles. Eye move-
ments were also visually examined after drug
Interposed neural signals should change in rela-
tion to physiological tremor. A deficit in inter- injections.
Penetration tracks, injection tracks, and lesion
positus function would show itself as an increase
of the amplitude of physiological tremor from sites were verified using standard histologic pro-
less than a degree to as much as 10 to 15° of wrist cedures.
arc, and a slowing of tremor frequency from the
normal 8 to 12/s to 3 to 6/s; e) RAM: self-paced Results
rapidly alternating movements (Fig. 15.2, fifth
Unit Recording
from top). The subject was instructed to flex and
extend as rapidly as possible. This tests the func- Single-unit activity was recorded across tasks
tion of the interposed nucleus and its cerebellar performed in standard order and across dent-
cortical input. Interposed neural signals should ate, interposed, and fastigial nuclei. Change in
288 w. Thomas Thach et al.

discharge frequency was systematically compared movements, although the movements were dif-
with a number of different movement variables. ferent: jump required a termination on target;
These variables included the times of onset and jerk, none. The discharge time-aligns better
termination of movement and the direction, velo- on the start of movement than on the stop. For
city, acceleration, and force of movement. During pert, there is a prompt response of the unit at
the steady-state hold position before and after about 14ms, which is at about the same time or
movement, unit discharge was compared to the a ms earlier than the earliest EMG change (not
hold position per se, the pattern of muscle activity shown; cf. Thach, 1978). This persists through
used to hold the position (systematically varied and after the stop. Oscillations in unit discharge
by varying the torque load), the intended direction at 12 Hz correlate with oscillations in the wrist as
of next movement, the oscillatory error in moving it settles into the final hold position. For ramp,
and holding, if any (tremor), and the type of the discharge increases before the onset of move-
stimulus situation eliciting the movement (per- ment and persists throughout the movement.
turbation of the visual target or somatosensory Small oscillations in the unit discharge relate to
perturbation of the limb to be moved, or self- and follow tremor of the wrist at around 6 Hz
paced). (Elble et ai., 1984). For RAM, unit discharge is
Figure 15.3 shows some typical units, one in time-aligned on the extreme offlexion and is seen
each of the three cerebellar nuclei, as they fire to modulate as greatly as in the above tasks with
across the trained wrist movement tasks. Dis- flexion (peak) and extension (trough).
charge displays are in the raster format, where The unit in dentate (Fig. 15.3D, right column
each dot represents an action potential, and of raster displays) also changed its discharge
each row of dots, the discharge during a trial. The frequency, but only in relation to some of the
trials are aligned on the onset of movement, re- tasks-especially jump and jerk. For jerk, the
presented by the vertical line. Within each hori- discharge increased about lOOms before move-
zontal row, the stimulus onset and the movement ment (cf. also Thach, 1970, 1975). For jump, the
stop are indicated by short vertical bars. The first discharge increased even earlier-about 130 ms
trial is at the bottom of each raster, and each before onset of movement (cf. also Thach, 1978).
subsequent trial is the next row up. A moving For pert, the increase in discharge was not as
average of the discharge frequency across trials great or consistent trial-to-trial as for interpositus,
is given above the raster rows. Units on the and began about 50 ms after the perturbation.
abscissa are ms before (left) and after (right) the Neither were there any oscillations in unit dis-
onset of movement; on the ordinate, discharge charge that correlate with the oscillations at the
frequency is in impulses/so wrist as it settled into position. For ramps, there
The unit located in the fastigius nucleus (Fig. was hardly any change in unit discharge, there
15.3F, left column of raster displays), although being a slight decrease. For RAM, there was no
located in a region presumed to be concerned with change in discharge frequency of this unit, al-
forelimb movement, did not change its discharge though there was for others.
frequency in relation to any of the trained wrist
movements-jerk, jump, pert, ramp, or RAM.
This was typical ofthe units in this area. No part Inactivation
of fastigius was found to contain units related to Inactivation deficits were evaluated by comparing
these movements. the pre- and postinjection movement traces and
The unit located in interpositus (Fig. 15.31, by movie film and video analysis. Several variables
middle column of raster displays) did change were examined, including onset and termination
markedly, and in relation to all of the movements. times, movement time, velocity, amplitude, and
For both jerk and jump, the unit commenced to EMG patterns.
fire about 40 ms before the onset of movement, For fastigius (Fig. 15.4F, left column), inactiva-
which is about 10ms before the onset of first tion with muscimol or kainic acid caused no
EMG change (not shown; cf. Thach, 1970, 1978). detectable abnormality in the performance of any
The discharge pattern was similar for these two of the five trained wrist movements. The animal
15. Cerebellar Output 289

I D

RAM

"
'"

, , \
. ', I .
, "

"
• I _ \

"
Figure 15.3. Discharge of single fastigius (F), interposed rasters are aligned on the perturbation onset (center
(I), and dentate (D) neurons across the five trained wrist vertical line), The first tic mark represents the onset of
movements. For jerk,jump, and ramp, rasters of single- return from displacement; the second tic mark repre-
unit discharge are aligned on movement onset (center sents onset of reward. For RAM, rasters are aligned on
vertical line). Tic marks in the raster before the center peak flexion (or extension), and the first and second tic
vertical line represent the onset of the visual stimulus; marks represent peak extension (or flexion), Above the
tic marks after represent the onset of reward. For pert, rasters is the average frequency summed across trials.
290 w. Thomas Thach et al.
D
Bt:fore ft r

fo
rt
r+
r+ rtr+r1lr+ r-
= Before

I
~ - fter

Time I'

~ .... ~

Figure 15.4. Major deficits produced by microinjection (left) and after (right) interpositus injection of muscimol.
ofmuscimol and kainic across the tasks. F, top, repre- Under each graph is the power frequency spectrum
sents stance after each of two fastigial muscimol injec- of the tremor. Below is a figure using three successive
tions, an anterior one causing adduction of hindlimb video frames to show tremor. D, top, bar graph show-
(left), and a posterior one causing adduction offorelimb ing reaction times before (white) and after (hatched)
(right) . The figures below represent sequential video dentate injection of muscimol for each of four direction/
frames in the course of falling that was caused by an load combinations (extension/extensor, extension/
ipsilateral muscimol injection. I , top, represents wrist flexor, flexion/extensor, and flexion/flexor) and the
position during successive trials of extension before totals. Bottom, deficits in reaching and pinching.

did not appear to be any different in its behavior stance, and gait. In the extreme case, it imme-
than when performing during a training session. diately fell to the ground (to the side of the injec-
Jerks, jumps, and perts were performed with the tion), and could not rise to a standing position.
same reaction times and accuracy. Ramps were There was no apparent weakness of the limbs
performed without tremor, unless the injection distally or proximally, and the animal made fre-
was made near the fastigial border with inter- quent vigorous efforts to attain the upright stance.
positus (see below). RAMs were performed at After 5 or so minutes of trying, the animal
the same frequency. We at first suspected that could often stand upright, only to fall again when
the injection had missed the nucleus and was it attempted to walk. An example of one of these
in the white matter. Nevertheless, immediately falls is shown in Fig. 15.4F. Video frames at fixed
upon getting down from its primate chair, the intervals are shown as the animal falls to the right
monkey showed severe difficulties with sitting, (side ofthe muscimol injection). The cause for the
15. Cerebellar Output 291

falls was not apparent from any abnormality in paired. Jerks and jumps were performed with
trunk or limb strength or position. When standing normal reaction times. Sitting, standing, and
and walking, there was a tendency to place the walking were normal, except for the very occa-
limbs on the affected side in a more adducted sional manifestation of tremor in certain posi-
position. This by itself did not appear sufficient tions, and the tendency to drag the dorsum of the
to cause a fall. But during a fall, the efforts that foot of the affected limb. Picking food bits out of
the animal made to prevent itself from falling the foodwell was normal, except as affected by
were not sufficiently abducted to succeed. tremor at shoulder, elbow, and wrist in certain
Sitting was similarly affected, with falls back- positions.
ward and to the side of the lesion. The animal For dentate (Fig. 15.4D, right column), the
would approach the examiner and lean against principle deficits were noted in reaching and in
his leg for support, only to fall when the examiner picking food bits out of food wells. In reaching,
moved away. With some injections the tendency the animal would overshoot the target by as much
was not so severe and the animal would sit-quietly, as 6 cm, bumping its hand against that of the
only to fall when it attempted to reach for a food examiner's hand that held the food bit. This oc-
bit. curred in the absence of any tremor. The over-
There was a tendency to somatotopic locali- shoot resulted from a combined overextension of
zation. Most injections into the fastigius caused the elbow and overflexion of the shoulder by as
falling that appeared due to defective control of much as 30°. This joint angle error was not seen
arm and leg. Two anterior injections caused pref- in the single-joint movements of the wrist in jumps
erential adduction of the hind limb sparing the and perts, which landed on target with little or no
forelimb, with falls precipitated from the hind- position error. Although interpositus injections
quarters. One posterior injection caused just the resulted in tremor that sometimes affected reach
reverse, with preferential adduction of the fore- at the endpoint, the position errors were never as
limb, and falls precipitated from the forequarter. great or as sustained as those after dentate injec-
The eye movements appeared normal; there tions.
was no nystagmus. The animal appeared to have In the food well task, a variety of errors were
no vertigo, and not to be ill at ease. There was no seen that also pointed to a coordinative problem
retching or vomiting. It reached for raisins or across joints. After dentate injections, the monkey
apple with either hand, without tremor, and fed did not employ the precision pinch with the
itself avidly, while lying on the floor, unable to accuracy and frequency as it did previously.
stand. The animals could pick food bits out of a Instead, it resorted to "single finger" strategies.
deep food well using a precision pinch without These consisted of poking the raisin at the bottom
apparent deficit. of the food well and either impaling it on the
For interpositus (Fig. 15.41, middle column), forefinger'S nail or raking it up the side of the well
deficits were apparent in both the trained move- into the palm or against the side of the thumb.
ments at the wrist and in the "natural" multijoint Frequently the adjacent digits 3, 4, and 5 would
movements. The most prominent feature was a get caught over the side of the food well, instead
large amplitude, low frequency (3 Hz) tremor. This of being held up and out of the way or folded
appeared as a terminal tremor of the wrist in the together so as to allow the whole hand into the
trained single-joint movements at the end of food well. The relative inability to coordinate the
jumps and perts and during ramps, and at the adjacent digits and to use digits 1 and 2 in a
shoulder and elbow in the natural multijoint precision pinch appeared out of proportion to
movements during reaching. With several injec- the fairly good use of the single index finger.
tions the hind limb was involved, manifesting as Not all dentate injections produced a deficit,
a tremor while sitting. and when one did, the deficit could either be one
There was occasionally a slight delay in the of reaching or pincning or both. The worst reach-
reaction time on perts (1O-20ms) and a loss of ing deficit was produced by an injection into the
regularity and rapidity of rapid alternating move- most lateral part of dentate, and the worst
ments. Otherwise, movements appeared unim- pinching deficit by an injection into the medial.
292 w. Thomas Thach et al.

For these two injections, the reach deficit was body representations are not at all contradictory,
accompanied by a normal pinch, and the pinch but instead represent real differences in the topo-
deficit by a normal reach. Injections between the graphic organization as it exists at the levels of
two gave deficits in both reaching and pinching. sensory input, cerebellar cortex, and cerebellar
This fits with the body mapping scheme depicted nuclear output, and as it is revealed by different
it Fig. 15.1, with the forelimb mapped onto the methods of study. Recent anatomical studies have
coronal dimension of the nuclei, with proximal given a rather clear and relatively simple view
limb lateral and distal limb medial. of how the body is represented within the cere-
bellar nuclei (Allen et aI., 1977; Allen et al., 1978);
Asanuma et al., 1983; Kalil, 1982; Stanton, 1980).
Discussion These studies support a pattern of multiple repre-
Coordination of the movement of the many sentation of the body within the nuclei: in the last
muscles and joints ofthe body is one ofthe oldest ofthese, there is a complete representation within
ideas on the fundamental function of the cerebel- each nucleus, with the tail anterior and the
lum (Fluorens, 1824). This idea is based on the head posterior, limbs medial and trunk lateral
impression that, after cerebellar damage, move- (Asanuma et aI., 1983).
ments at a single joint are less impaired than are The present nuclear mapping study using the
movements involving multiple joints. The idea two methods of single-unit recording and repeated
implies that within the cerebellar architecture, muscimol injections across the two batteries of
there are circuit functions that are specialized to tasks provides physiological support for the cere-
span and coordinate the activities of multiple bellar nuclear output scheme first described by
joints. This would in turn imply a precise relation anatomical studies above.
ofthat architecture to the many joints and muscles
of the body.
Yet even at the present time there are appar- Fastigius
ently conflicting views as to how the body and its Previous studies of the deficits following ablation
various parts are functionally represented within of the fastigial nuclei have reported abnormalities
the cerebellum. The question has been studied of upright stance and gait (Botterell and Fulton,
repeatedly, using ablation, electrical stimulation, 1938a; Sprague and Chambers, 1953). Antziferova
and single-neuron activity during movement et aI. (1980) found unit activity in fastigius that
within different regions of the cerebellum. These was well related to fictive scratching and walking,
studies have resulted in the following different whereas Arshavsky et aI. (1980) found little or no
schemata: a) no localization (Holmes, 1917; Manni such activity in inpterposed and dentate.
et aI., 1964) b) medial cerebellum controlling One study (Brown, 1949) and most clinical
trunkal musculature and lateral cerebellum con- neurologists have interpreted the stance deficit
trolling distal musculature (Adrian, 1943; Brown, of midline cerebellar lesions as being due to an
1949; Goldberger and Growden, 1973; Hampson, abnormality of the function of proximal trunk
1949; Hampson et aI., 1952; Sasaki et aI., 1976; muscles. This may in part be the result of Snider's
Snider and Eldred, 1952; Snider and Stowell, 1944; sensory input maps in cerebellar cortex (Snider
Thach, 1967; Victor et aI., 1959), c) lateral cere- and Eldred, 1952; Snider and Stowell, 1944), which
bellum controlling proximal musculature to pro- show trunk in the midline and limbs laterally. But
vide a postural base upon which fine, distal move- iffastigial inactivation affects only the trunk and
ments are performed under intermediate cerebellar the hip and shoulder girdle musculature, then the
control (Massi on, 1979), and d) medial cerebellum animal should a) have abnormalities in reaching
controlling whole body postural mechanisms and with arm or leg, which require these muscles, yet
lateral cerebellum controlling whole body voli- b) still be able to support itself upright by exten-
tional movements (Botterell and Fulton, 1938a, b; sion at elbow and knee. In our study, neither con-
Chambers and Sprague, 1955a, b; Dow and dition was observed. When the monkey's trunk
Moruzzi, 1958; Sprague and Chambers, 1953). was supported in a sitting position in the primate
It may well be that these different schemata for chair, the animals had no difficulty in reaching,
15. Cerebellar Output 293

and they performed this task as if fully normal. nuclear ablation deficits is allowed by the finer
By contrast, stance and gait were often so severely spatial resolution of our two methods (unit re-
affected that the animal could not prevent itself cording and chemical inactivation), neither of
from falling, even though elbow and knee exten- which involves fibers of passage.
sion appeared of normal force and coordination Our results confirm that there are movement
in reaching while seated in the chair. In sum, the task-specific differences between interposed and
abnormality appeared not one of weakness of dentate nuclei. This is true for both methods of
anyone muscle group, but rather of the inability study: the correlation of single-neuronal dis-
to place and hold the limb properly to serve as a charge with behavior, and the behavioral deficits
support while standing. after inactivation. Moreover, the neuron-behavior
A probable clinical correlation is the syndrome correlations correspond to the ablation-behavior
of hypertensive intracerebellar hemorrhage (Fisher deficits. Thus, interpositus neurons signal wrist
et aI., 1965). The presenting complaint is an in- oscillation, and interpositus inactivation increases
ability to stand and walk. This usually occurs the amplitude and slows the frequency of the
initially in the absence of incoordination of willed oscillation. Interpositus neurons fire at short
movements, or signs of brain stem damage. The latency to wrist perturbation, and interpositus
hemorrhage is just to one side of the midline deep inactivation delays the response time to this
within the cerebellum. From the similarity of the stimulus. Interpositus neurons fire in relation to
clinical presentation and the findings of this study, self-paced rapid wrist alternation, and interposi-
the syndrome would appear to be caused purely tus inactivation decreases the regularity of these
by damage ofthe fastigial nuclei. movements.
What is the function of interpositus? If inter-
I nterpositus positus reponds at critically short latencies to
perturbation from a holding position, and if in-
Electroanatomical studies have shown that inter- activation of interpositus delays the movement
positus receives receptor-specific information at response to return to the hold position, then it
short latency directly and indirectly (via cortex) follows that interpositus may playa role in that
from the large fiber, rapidly conducting spino- response. This is the conclusion of some previous
cerebellar pathways (Allen et aI., 1977; Burton unit-recording studies (Strick, 1983; Thach, 1978),
and Onoda, 1977, 1978; MacKay and Murphy, which point out the similarity to the segmental
1974; Soechting et aI., 1978; Thach, 1978). Single and especially the "long loop" stretch reflex. The
interpositus neurons of awake moving monkeys question remains as to the specificity of the re-
have been shown to carry a signal proportionate sult. This question is raised by the inconstancy
to physiological tremor that is similar to the and rather meager magnitude of the delay. If the
tremor signal also carried by Ia spindle afferents "functional stretch reflex" per se were fully routed
(Elble et aI., 1984; Schieber and Thach, 1985). through interpositus, then one might have ex-
There has so far been no such sharp distinction pected a more robust impairment of its perfor-
between the motor deficits that follow interposed mance by interpositus inactivation. This result
and dentate ablation; the ablation of either is not having been obtained, one wonders if the
supposed to include limb ataxia and action tremor more important and global function is something
(Botterell and Fulton, 1938b; Goldberger and bigger but yet beneath the surface.
Growden, 1973). Nor has cooling of the inter- Perhaps a more important clue is the relatively
posed and dentate shown striking differences dramatic correlation of neural discharge and of
(Brooks et aI., 1973; Uno et aI., 1973). Indeed, the inactivation deficit with oscillation. We have
above cited ablation and cooling experiments elsewhere entertained the idea (Elble et aI., 1984;
and the cooling experiments of Cooke and Thach et aI., 1986), as have others (Glaser and
Thomas (1976) and Vilis and Hore (1977, 1980) Higgins, 1966; Henatsch, 1967; Wiener, 1948), that,
attribute cerebellar tremor specifically to involve- since oscillation is a problem inherent in the mech-
ment of the dentate. We believe that the distinc- anical reflex design of the motor system, the cere-
tion that we make between interposed and dentate bellum may have been included to damp the
294 w. Thomas Thach et al.

oscillation. Mechanical systems have their dash- However, previous studies of dentate ablation
pots and electronic systems have their resistors, had not shown a conspicuous difficulty in initiat-
and it is possible that a similar solution is also ing voluntary movement (Botterell and Fulton,
taken by the nervous system. If the cerebellum 1938; Goldberger and Growden, 1973; Luciani,
were to damp oscillation actively, then the ques- 1911; Chambers and Sprague, 1955b). The reac-
tion becomes whether this is at fhe level of seg- tion time delay of some 200 ms after human cere-
mental stretch reflexes or of long loops, or both. bellar injury, first reported by Holmes (1917), has
This question also has been discussed in the above been confirmed by subsequent ablations of den-
and other references. tate (vide supra; Thach, 1975; Trouche and
Does the cerebellum also promote oscillation? Beaubaton, 1980), but the delay has shrunken to
It is of interest that two of the most reliable one as small as 25 to 50ms. The large and phy-
empirical tests for cerebellar injury include both logenetically increasing size of the dentate would
the presence of unwanted oscillation and the in- appear to argue for some more important func-
ability to produce oscillation. The observation tional contribution.
that interpositus neurons may best signal, and It can be argued that small reaction time dif-
interpositus inactivation most impair self-paced ferences may be very critical in certain rapid and
rapid alternating movements fits with the idea of skilled motor performances. But another inter-
Walshe (1977) that the most rapid of oscillations pretation is that the timing delay may be a clue
may be produced by undamping stretch reflex to some larger and more important function
mechanisms. lurking just beyond our present knowledge. In
The phenomenon of the interpositus link to this vein, one piece of work in human cerebellar
oscillation is in keeping with the work of Gilman patients has suggested that dentate functions may
and colleagues (Gilman, 1969; Gilman et al., 1976) be perceptual or mental. The finding was that
and of Smith and colleagues (Frysinger et aI., patients with lateral cerebellar lesions are unable
1984; Smith and Bourbonnais, 1981; Wetts et aI., to distinguish small duration differences in tone
1985) on agonist-antagonist muscle control by pips presented serially (Ivry and Keele, 1988).
the cerebellum. The idea would be that this part Other speculative articles have suggested even
of the cerebellum may adjust and coordinate in more abstract functions, such as the coordination
time and across muscles the bias and/or gain of of thought and mental skills (Leiner et aI., 1986;
stretch reflexes of agonist and antagonist muscles. Leiner et aI., 1987).
This could be for a variety of purposes other than Our experiments of recording and inactivating
the prevention of oscillation. Oscillation could neural discharge within the dentate nucleus con-
simply be the by-product of maladjustment. firmed and extended previous results on initiation
of volitional movement. Dentate neurons changed
Dentate firing well before movement onset, especially the
The parallel phylogenetic development of the visually triggered jerks and jumps, and it was
cerebral neocortex and lateral cerebellum, to- these movements that were delayed by dentate
gether with the connections between them, have inactivation. On perts, dentate discharge lagged
long suggested that the former must work along that of interposit us, and inactivation had no effect
with the latter. In trained monkeys, dentate neural on the reaction time. Dentate neurons carried no
discharge was reported to lead the discharge of tremor signal, and dentate inactivation had no
motor cortex neurons and movement (Lamarre effect on tremor. In some animals, inactivatiQn of
et aI., 1983; Thach, 1975, 1978), whereas dentate dentate causes tremor of hand tracking during
inactivation delayed both the discharge of motor the ramp task only. This movement is usually a
cortex neurons and movement (Meyer-Lohman compound one of eyes and wrist though some
et al., 1977; Spidalieri et aI., 1983). This supported animals learn to complete the ramp-track without
the idea that the dentate was involved in initiating looking apparently from memory. This may ex-
volitional movement from motor cortex (Allen plain the inconstancy of the result across animals
and Tsukahara, 1974; Brooks and Thach, 1981; and the results of Vilis and Hore (1977, 1980).
Eccles, 1967; Evarts and Thach, 1969). Dentate neurons (like interpositus neurons) fre-
15. Cerebellar Output 295

quently discharged in relation to RAMs, but den- stretch reflex), appear to be more critically con-
tate inactivation did not impair the alternations trolled by interpositus than dentate. Do these
beyond a slight slowing. observations then reveal the full identity of dentate
These results are consistent with some previous control?
interpretations as to the differences between den- The "room behavior" as casually observed after
tate and interpositus (Allen and Tsukahara, 1974; dentate inactivation did not prepare us for what
Botterell and Fulton, 1938a, b; Brooks and Thach, we observed under systematic testing (Kane et aI.,
1981; Chambers and Sprague, 1955a, b; Eccles, 1989). To be sure, there was a "clumsiness and
1967; Evarts and Thach, 1969). Dentate discharges awkwardness" of the hand in picking up items or
earliest in relation to stimuli that give no informa- warding off threats, and a tendency to lift the
tion as to the nature of the movement that is to affected foot a little higher than its fellows in
follow. These stimuli do not ordinarily cause a walking, both of which were described by Botterell
movement, and the monkey has to make the and Fulton (1938b) after large hemispheric abla-
association through operant conditioning. Once tions. But when the animal reached into deep
learned, the monkey will not respond to the narrow food wells in which raisins, bits of apple,
movement unless it is motivated to do so. These or M&M candies were placed, the effects of den-
criteria can be used to create an operational defi- tate inactivation became dramatic. The animals
nition of "volitional movement." It seems appro- suffered an impaired ability to pinch in a precision
priate that it is the part of the cerebellum that is grip. This normally involves the coordinated
best connected to the cerebral association cortex opposition of thumb and index. When specifically
that is most critically concerned with these tasks. looked for, the animals had also lost this ability
Tasks in which the stimulus is delivered to the in retrieving food bits from the examiner's hand:
part to be moved, in which the stimulus direction the deficit did not depend on the food well alone.
determines the movement direction (as in the The animals retained the ability to move one digit

Figure 15.5. Model of


granule cell parallel fiber
control of muscular co-
ordination.

--i6-4-.....I(H-----tc:::J.-1.--kl-+---KH---....-I- - - -
-.J-~+---tc::H--<K}-+--t<J-+-t:J_-._-----

--+--I--~-+---f-+--+--tr-+__+--,-- - - - - - -
296 w. Thomas Thach et al.

at a time, and made use of this to get food out of Parallel Fibers, Purkinje Celis,
the food well. This resulted in "poking," "stabbing" M yotomes in the Coronal Dimension,
with the fingernail, and "raking" up the side to and Coordination
get the food bit out of the well. The other fingers
would move independently, lacking the previous Does cerebellar circuitry provide a mechanism
synchrony with each other and with the thumb for multijoint coordination? These results are
and index. interpreted in context of the prior anatomical
The deficits were not confined to the hand (Asanuma et aI., 1983; Kalil, 1982; Stanton, 1980)
alone, but also affected shoulder and elbow. This and physiological (Thach et al., 1982) findings of
was best seen in standing and reaching for profer- somatotopic representation of the body within
red food bits, and especially while seated in the each of the three cerebellar nuclei. In each repre-
primate chair. The principle visible error was an sentation, the mapping is of the caudorostral
overshoot ofthe hand past the target. This resulted dimension of the body onto the sagittal dimension
from an overflexion at the shoulder and overex- of the nucleus. Thus, the hindlimbs are represented
tension at the elbow, compounding to give the anteriorly, the forelimbs more toward the middle,
endpoint error. In a single joint, one might have and the head posteriorly in the nuclei. This orient-
attributed the overshoot purely to a delay in ation suggests that the myotomes, running ortho-
stopping the movement. But the time delay as gonal to the long axis of the body, run in the
measured at the wrist was small, and the position coronal dimension and parallel to the trajectory
overshoot scarcely detectable (the animal easily of the parallel fibers. Since the parallel fibers are
landed within the 10-15° target window). For the connected to the nuclear cells by Purkinje cells, a
two joints, the error had to have resulted more coronal "beam" of parallel fibers would control
from excessive angle than from late timing, and, though inhibitory modulation the nuclear cells
judging from the negligible position error at the influencing the synergistic muscles in the myotome.
wrist, to be uniquely a deficit of multijointed The parallel fiber in this way would be a single
movements. neural element spanning and coordinating the
Mackel (1987) lesioned portions of the deep activities of multiple synergic muscles and joints.
nuclei with kainic acid and studied a variety of Fig. 15.5 shows a coronal beam of parallel
movements, especially movements of the digits. fibers running parallel to the myotomes in the
His lesions involved both interpositus and dentate, forelimb representation within the deep nuclei.
and he did not dissociate tremor from the other At one level of organization, the parallel fiber
deficits. He did remark upon a striking difficulty spans the synergic muscles within the myotome,
of the monkeys in making independent digit move- and the strength of the contact on the Purkinje
ments, which he likened to the corticospinal deficits cell determines the strength of activity of the
described by Lawrence and Kuypers (1968). These muscle within the limb. It is clear that, as drawn,
observations, along with the earlier ones of the action of one parallel fiber will have an effect
Botterell and Fulton (1938b), support our obser- very different from that of another. The top
vations of abnormalities of finger movements after parallel fiber, acting through the Purkinje cell and
dentate lesions. Nevertheless, it seems to us that the nuclear cell, will be seen to inhibit distal
they missed an essential point. That is, that move- musculature relative to proximal, whereas the re-
ments of individual joints, including independent verse is true for the second parallel fiber from the
finger movements, are relatively normal. It is the top. The assumptions are that a parallel fiber may
movement of two or more joints, as well exem- have different effects on the successive Purkinje
plified by the thumb and index in precision pinch, cells it passes. At a second level of organization,
and the shoulder and elbow in reaching, that are the parallel fibers that span nuclei will perforce
qualitatively more severely affected by dentate have a coordinating effect on the outputs of the
lesions. This is in keeping with the rather mild different nuclei (e.g., of stance, of stretch reflex
deficits seen in single-joint movement after dentate gains, and of voluntary movements). It is clear that
ablations. motor activities are coordinated at both these
levels, that of synergist muscles, and that of different
15. Cerebellar Output 297

modes of control. The question remains, "What Summary


is the coordinating circuitry?" Exactly like the
The deep cerebellar nuclei were mapped in awake
modern circuit designer's labeled logic line, the
Rhesus monkeys using a) the behavioral corre-
parallel fiber obviously makes contact with both
lation of recorded single-unit discharge and b) the
elements of organization. This anatomical fact
behavioral deficits produced by muscimol and
suggests that it can perform the function we tenta-
kainic acid microinjection across two task bat-
tively ascribe to it. Future work will be needed to
teries employing five trained wrist movements and
prove its sufficiency in this role.
five nontrained multijointed movements. The
It is just as clear that the cerebellum is not the
following conclusions were made.
only mechanism that coordinates multijointed
movement. Coordination is built into antagonist 1. There is a separate somatotopic representation
muscles and alternating stepping in the spinal within each nucleus, with caudal parts anterior,
cord, and into antigravity stance and head-limb rostral parts posterior, trunk lateral, and limbs
synergies in the brain stem. It may be that move- medial. Each nucleus controls a different type
ment of single digits in combinations and se- or "mode" of movement.
quences is achieved at the level of motor or pre- 2. The medial fastigial nucleus controls muscles
motor cortex, just as visual cortex may build up only in the modes of sitting, standing, and
complex receptive fields from simple fields. These walking: its neurons did not fire in relation to
synergies, relatively simple and hardwired, may the trained wrist movements, and inactivation
need modification if variety and change of combi- produced no deficit in these movements. In-
nations and sequences ofthe moving components activation did produce severe deficits in sitting,
is required. standing, and walking, with inability to main-
We have pointed out the favorability of the tain an upright position, or frequent falls to
parallel fiber and its connections with the Purkinje the ipsilateral side. There was a tendency for
cells through to coronal beams of nuclear cells somatotopic organization, with hind limb ante-
and thence ultimately to synergic muscles and rior and forelimb posterior in the nucleus.
modes of control as a logical candidate for the 3. The intermediate interposed nucleus controls
coordinating link. Given such a link, the amount stretch and other somatosensory reflexes and
of variation that the mechanism permits should may help damp the inherent mechanical reflex
go up with the number of such coordinating links. oscillation of body parts: it fires in relation to
Adjustment and adaptation would also appear mechanical reflex tremor and to prompt move-
to be desirable attributes of the coordinating ment initiated by perturbation of the part to
principle, and at the level of the contact of the be moved (the so-called long loop reflex); its
linking fiber its linked cells. The cerebellar cortical inactivation increases the amplitude and slows
circuitry appears to have all these desirable the frequency ofthe tremor, and may prolong
features. the reaction time of an attempted long loop
Verification of this mechanism will require reflex. Interpositus may also preferentially con-
a) showing that, after cerebellar cortical injury, trol self-paced rapid alternating movements: it
multijointed movements are sufficiently more fires in relation to them, and its inactivation
impaired than single-jointed movements such that slows their frequency and impairs their regu-
the sum of the abnormalities at the single joints larity.
cannot account for the magnitude of the abnor- 4. The lateral dentate nucleus helps initiate (via
malities in the compound movement, b) recording thalamus and motor cortex) those movements
from Purkinje cells to show that they are differen- that are triggered by mental or arbitrary sen-
tially cQntrolled in single versus multijointed sory cues and therefore called "volitional": it
movement, and c) that the parallel fiber is the fires before such movements; its inactivation
sole exclusive agent of this control. delays onset ofthese movements. Inactivation
affected multijointed movements much more
than single-jointed movements. The dissoci-
ation was sufficiently extreme to warrant re-
298 W. Thomas Thach et al.

consideration of the old idea that a cardinal Burton, I.E., and Onoda, N. (1978): Dependence of the
purpose of cerebellar circuitry is the coordi- activity of interpositus and red nucleus neurons on
nation of multijointed movements. A model sensory input data generated by movement. Brain
whereby this might be achieved is presented. Res., 152,41-63.
Chambers, W.W., and Sprague, I.M. (l955a): Func-
References tionallocalization in the cerebellum. I. Organization
in longitudinal corticonuclear zones and their contri-
Adrian, E.D. (1943): Afferent areas in the cerebellum bution to the control of posture, both extrapyramidal
connected with the limbs. Brain, 66, 289-315. and pyramidal. J. Compo Neurol., 103, 105-129.
Allen, G.I., Gilbert, P.F.e., Marini, R., Schultz, W., Chambers, W.W., and Sprague, I.M. (1955b): Func-
and Yin, T.e.T. (1977): Integration of cerebral and tional localization in the cerebellum. II. Somatic
peripheral inputs by interpositus neurons in monkey. organization in cortex and nuclei. Arch. N eurol.
Exp. Brain Res., 27, 81-99. Psychiatry, 74, 653-680.
Allen, G.I., Gilbert, P.F.C., and Yin, T.e.T. (1978): Cooke, I.D., and Thomas, e.S. (1976): Forearm oscil-
Convergence of cerebral inputs onto dentate neurons lations during cooling of the dentate nucleus in the
in monkey. Exp. Brain Res., 32, 151-170. monkey. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol., 54, 430-436.
Allen, G.I., and Tsukahara, N. (1974): Cerebrocerebellar Dow, R.S., and Moruzzi, G. (1958): The Physiology and
communication systems. Physiol. Rev., 54, 957-1006. Pathology of the Cerebellum. Minneapolis: Univ. of
Antziferova, L.I., Arshavsky, Yu, I., Orlovsky, G.N. Minnesota Press.
and Pavlova, G.A. (1980): Activity of neurons of Eager, R.P. (1966): Patterns and mode of termination
cerebellar nuclei during fictitious scratch reflex in of cerebellar corticonuclear pathways in the monkey
the cat. I. Fastigial nucleus. Brain Res., 200, 239-248. (Macaca mulatta). J. Compo Neurol., 126, 551-566.
Arshavsky, Yu.I., Orlovsky, G.N., Pavlova, G.A., and Eccles, I.e. (1967): Circuits in the cerebellar control of
Perret, e. (1980): Activity of neurons of cerebellar movement. Proc. N atl. Acad. Sci. USA, 58, 336-343.
nuclei during fictitious scratch relfex in the cat. II. Eccles, I.e., Ito, M., and Szentagothai, I. (1967): The
The interpositus and lateral nuclei. Brain Res., 200, Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine, New York:
249-258. Springer-Verlag, Inc.
Asanuma, e., Thach, W.T., and lones, E.G. (1983): Elble, R.I., Schieber, -M.H., and Thach, W.T. (1984):
Anatomical evidence for segregated focal groupings Activity of spindle afferents and neurons of motor
of efferent cells and their terminal ramifications in cortex and cerebellar nuclei during action tremor.
the cerebellothalamic pathway of the monkey. Brain Brain Res., 323,330-334.
Res. Rev., 5, 267-297. Evarts, E.V. (1966): Methods for recording activity of
Botterell, E.H., and Fulton, I.F. (1938a): Functional individual neurons in IT. :)Ving animals. In: Methods
localization in the cerebellum of primates. II. Lesions in Medical Research. Chicago, IL: Year Book Medical
of midline structures (vermis) and deep nuclei. J. Publishers, pp. 241-250.
Compo Neurol., 69, 47-62. Evarts, E.V., and Thach, W.T. (1969): Motor mecha-
Botterell, E.H., and Fulton, I.F. (1938b): Functional nisms of the CNS: Cerebrocerebellar inter-relations.
localization in the cerebellum of primates. III. Annu. Rev. Physiol., 31, 451-498.
Lesions of hemispheres (neocerebellum). J. Compo Fisher, e.M., Picard, E.H., Polak, A., Mayman, H.,
Neurol., 69, 63-87. and Ojeman, R. (1965): Acute hypertensive cerebel-
Brooks, V.B., Kozlovskaya, I.B., Atkin, A., Horvath, F.E., lar hemorrhage: Diagnosis and surgical treatment.
and Uno, M. (1973): Effects of cooling dentate nucleus J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 140, 38.
on tracking-task performance in monkeys. J. N euro- Fluorens, P. (1824): Recherches Experimentales sur les
physiol., 36, 974-995. Proprietes et les Fonctions du Systeme Nerveux, dans
Brooks, Y.B., and Thach, W.T. (1981): Cerebellar con- les Animaux Vertebres. Paris: Crevot.
trol of posture and movement. In: Handbook of Frysinger, R.t., Bourbonnais, D., Kalaska, I.F., and
Phyiology, The Nervous System, Section 1, Vol. II. Smith, A.M. (1984): Cerebellar cortical activity during
(V.B. Brooks, ed.). pp. 877-946. Bethesda: American antagonist cocontraction and reciprocal inhibition
Physiological Society. offorearm muscles. J. Neurophysiol., 51,32-49.
Brown, I.R. (1949): Localizing cerebellar syndromes. Gilman, S. (1969): The mechanism of cerebellar hypo-
JAMA, 141, 518-521. tonia. Brain, 92, 621-638.
Burton, I.E., and Onoda, N. (1977): Interpositus neu- Gilman, S., Carr, D., and Hollenberg, I. (1976): Kine-
rons discharge in relation to a voluntary movement. matic effects of deafferentation and cerebellar ablation.
Brain Res., 121, 167-172. Brain, 99, 311-330.
15. Cerebellar Output 299

Glaser, G.H., and Higgins, D.C. (1966): Motor stability, Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., and Dow, R.S. (1987): Cere-
stretch responses, and the cerebellum. In: Muscular brocerebellar learning loops in apes and humans. I tal.
AJferents and Motor Control. Proc. Nobel Symp. I. J. Neurol. Sci., 8, 425-436.
(R. Granit, ed). Stockholm: Almquist & Wiksell, Llimis, R. (1981): Electrophysiology of cerebellar net-
pp. 121-138. works. In: Handbook ofPhysiology, Section 1, Vol. II,
Goldberger, M.E., and Growden, J.M. (1973): Pattern Part 2 (V.B. Brooks, ed.). pp.831-876. Bethesda:
of recovery following cerebellar deep nuclear lesions American Physiological Society.
in monkeys. Exp. Neurol., 39, 307-322. Luciani, L. (1911-1924): Human Physiology. Trans.
Hampson, J.L. (1949): Relationship between cat cere- by F.A. Welby. London: MacMillan & Co., Ltd.
bral and cerebellar cortices. J. Neurophysiol., 12, MacKay, W.A., and Murphy, J.T. (1974): Response
37-50. of interpositus neurons to passive muscle stretch.
Hampson, J.L., Harrison, C.R., and Woolsey, C.N. J. Neurophysiol., 27,1410-1423.
(1952): Cerebro-cerebellar projections and the soma- Mackel, R. (1987): The role of the monkey sensory
totopic localization of motor function in the cere- cortex in the recovery from cerebellar injury. Exp.
bellum. Res. Publ. Ass. N erv. M ent. Dis. 30, 299-316. Brain Res., 66, 638-652.
Henatsch, H.D. (1967): Instability ofthe proprioceptive Manni, E., Henatsch, H.D., Henatsch, E.-M., and Dow,
length servo: Its possible role in tremor phenomena. R.S. (1964): Localization offaci1itatory and inhibitory
In: Neurophysiological Basis of Normal and Abnormal sites in and around the cerebellar nuclei affecting
Motor Activities (M.D. Yahr and D.P. Purpura, eds). limb posture, alpha and gamma motoneurons, J.
New York: Raven Press, pp. 75-89. Neurophysiol., 27, 210-228.
Hikosaka, 0., and Wurtz, R.H. (1983): Visual and Massion, J. (1979): Role of motor cortex in postural
oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra adjustments associated with movement. In: Inte-
pars reticulata. IV. Relation of substantia nigra to gration in the Nervous System (H. Asanuma and
superiorcolliculus. J. Neurophysiol.,49, 1285-1301. V.J. Wilson, eds). Tokyo: Igaku Shoin, pp. 239-
Holmes, G. (1917): The symptoms of acute cerebellar 259.
injuries due to gunshot injuries. Brain, 40, 461-535. McGeer, E.G., and McGeer, P.L. (1978): Kainic Acid
Hubel, D.H. (1960): Single unit activity in lateral geni- asa Tool in Neurobiology. New York: Raven Press.
culate body and optic tract of unrestrained cats..J. Meyer-Lohman, J., Hore, J., and Brooks, V.B. (1977):
Physiol. (Lond.), 156, 91-104. Cerebellar participation in generation of prompt
Ivry, R.B., Keele, S.W. (1989): Timing functions of the arm movements. J. Neurophysiol., 40,1038-1050.
cerebellum. J. Cognitive Neurosci. 1, 136-152. Ricci, G., Doane, B., and Jasper, J.H. (1957): Microelec-
Jansen, J., and Brodal, A. (1940): Experimental studies trode studies in conditioning: technique and pre-
on the intrinsic fibers of the cerebellum. II. The cor- liminary results. In: Premier Congress Internationale
ticonuclear projection. J. Compo Neurol., 73, 267- des Sciences Neurologiques. Bruxelles: Reunions
321. Plenieres (Bruxelles: Editions: Acta Medica Belgica,
Kalil, K. (1982): Projections ofthe cerebellar and dorsal 1957) pp. 401-415.
column nuclei upon the thalamus of the Rhesus Sasaki, K.S., Kawaguchi, S., Oka, H., Saki, M., and
monkey. J. Compo Neurol., 195, 25-50. Mizuno, N. (1976): Electrophysiological studies on
Kane, S.A., Goodkin, H.P., Keating, J.G., and Thach, the cerebellocerebral projections in monkeys. Exp.
W.T. (1989): Inc~ordination in attempted reaching Brain Res., 24, 495-507.
and pinching after inactivation of cerebellar dentate Schieber, M.H., and Thach, W.T. (1985): Trained slow
nucleus. Abst. Soc. Neurosci., 15, 52. tracking. II. Bidirectional discharge patterns of cere-
Lamarre, Y., Spidalieri, G., and Chapman, C.E. (1983): bellar nuclear, motor cortex, and spindle afferent
A comparison of neuronal discharge recorded in the neurons. J. Neurophysiol., 55,1228-1270.
sensorimotor cortex, parietal cortex, and dentate Smith, A.M., and 'Bourbonnais, D. (1981): Neuronal
nucleus of the monkey during arm movements trig- activity in cerebellar cortex related to control of pre-
gered by light, sound or somesthetic stimuli. Exp. hensile force. J. Neurophysiol., 45, 286-303.
Brain Res .• Suppl. 7, 140-156. Snider, R.S., and Eldred, E. (1952): Cerebro-cerebellar
Lawrence, D.G., and Kuypers, H.G.J.M. (1968): The relationships in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol., 15,
functional organization of the motor system in the 27-40.
monkey. I. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Snider, R.S., and Stowell, A. (1944): Receiving areas of
Brain, 91, 1-14. the tactile, auditory, and visual systems in the cere-
Leiner, H.C., Leiner, A.L., and Dow, R.S. (1986): Does bellum. J. Neurophysiol., 7, 331-357.
the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behav. Soechting, J.F., Burton, J.E., and Onoda, N. (1978):
Neurosci., 100,443-453. Relationships between sensory input, motor output
300 W. Thomas Thach et al.

and unit activity in interpositus and red nuclei dur- Thach, W.T., Perry, J.G., and Schieber, M.H. (1982):
ing intentional movement. Brain Res., 152, 65-79. Cerebellar output: Body maps and muscles spindles.
Spidalieri, H.J., Busby, L., and Lamarre, Y. (1983): Fast In: The Cerebellum-New Vistas (S.L. Palay and
ballistic arm movements triggered by visual, auditory, V. Chan-Palay, eds). New York: Springer-Verlag,
and somesthetic stimuli in the monkey. II. Effects of pp. 440-454.
unilateral dentate lesion on discharge of precentral Thach, W.T., Schieber, M.H., Mink, J.W., Kane, S.A.,
cortical neurons and reaction. J. Neurophysiol., 50, and Horne, M.K. (1986): Cerebellar relation to mus-
1359-1379. cle spindles in hand tracking. Prog. Brain Res., 64,
Sprague, J.M., and Chambers, W.W. (1953): Regulation 217-224.
of posture in intact and decerebrate cat. I. Cerebellum, Tilney, F. (1923): Genesis of cerebellar functions. Arch.
reticular formation, and vestibular nuclei. J. N euro- Neurol. Psychiatry, 9,137-169.
physiol., 16,451-463. Trouche, E., and Beaubaton, D. (1980): Initiation of a
Stanton, G.B. (1980): Topographical organization of goal-directed movement in the monkey. Exp. Brain.
ascending cerebellar projections from the dentate Res., 40, 311-321.
and interposed nuclei in macaca mulatta: An antero- Uno, M., Kozlovskaya, I.B., and Brooks, V.B. (1973):
grade degeneration study. J. Comp. Neurol., 190, Effects of cooling interposed nuclei on tracking-
699-731. task performance in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol., 36,
Strick, P.L. (1983): The influence of motor preparation 996-1003.
on the response of cerebellar neurons to limb dis- Victor, M., Adams, R.D., and Mancall, E.L. (1959): A
placements. J. Neurosci., 3, 2007-2020. restricted form of cerebellar cortical degeneration
Thach, W.T. (1967): Somatosensory receptive fields of occurring in alcoholic patients. Arch. N eurol., 1,
single units in cat cerebellar cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 579-688.
30, 675-696. Vilis, T., and Hore, J. (1977): Effects of changes in mech-
Thach, W.T. (1968): Discharge of Purkinje and cere- anical state of limb on cerebellar intention tremor.
bellar nuclear neurons during rapidly alternating J. Neurophysiol., 43, 279-291.
arm movements in the monkey. J. N europhysiol., 31, Vilis, T., and Hore, 1. (1980): Central neuronal mech-
785-797. anisms contributing to cerebellar tremor produced
Thach, W.T. (1970): Discharge of cerebellar neurons by limb perturbations. J. Neurophysiol., 43, 279-291.
related to two maintained postures and two prompt Walshe, E.G. (1977): Persistence of stretch reflexes fol-
movements. I. Nuclear cell output. J. N europhysiol., lowing cerebellar ablation, and a resonance theory
33,527-536. of cerebellar function. In: Physiological Aspects of
Thach, W.T. (1975): Timing of activity in the cerebel- Clinical Neurology (F.e. Rose, ed.). Oxford: Blackwell
lar dentate nucleus and cerebral motor cortex during Scientific Publications, pp. 215-224.
prompt volitional movement. Brain Res., 169, 168- Wetts, R., Kalaska, J.F., and Smith, A.M. (1985): Cere-
172. bellar nuclear cell activity during antagonist cocon-
Thach, W.T. (1978): Correlation of neural discharge traction and reciprocal inhibition of forearm muscles.
with pattern and force of muscular activity, joint J. Neurophysiol., 54, 231-244.
position, and direction of intended next movement Wiener, N. (1948): Cybernetics: Control and Communi-
in motor cortex and cerebellum. J. N europhysiol., 41, cations in the Animal and in the Machine. New York:
654-676. John Wiley, pp. 113-136.
16
The Role of the Cerebellum in
Voluntary and Reflexive Movements:
History and Current Status
John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

Because important scientific questions are usually ter. Whereas the occurrence of such voluntary
examined by a number of research groups that acts can be increased in frequency by association
may differ from each other in both methodology with environmental events, the relationship be-
and theoretical orientation, it is not unusual to tween stimulus and response remains probabilistic.
have opposing interpretations of a research out- It has been concluded that the consequences of a
come. Moreover, the interpretation of any single voluntary act must in some manner determine its
research group may be powerfully influenced and frequency of occurrence. For this reason, such
rewarded by the culture, politics, and historical behavior is also referred to as instrumental in that
precedents to which it has been exposed. There- it is assumed to be instrumental in obtaining
fore, the validity of a scientific interpretation some reward. Those who prefer to eliminate any
and its recognition as "truth" by the scientific teleological inferences prefer to use the term
community is greatly enhanced when that truth "operant" to indicate simply that the behavior
is arrived at by independent research groups operates on the environment.
differing in their intellectual backgrounds and The second group, located predominantly in
coming from altogether different historical frame- the USSR, derived its interest in cerebellar function
works. Such has been the case with the study of from the perspective of reflexology as propounded
cerebellar function. Thus, one purpose of this by Sechenov and later Pavlov. Students of Pavlov
chapter is to review how similar conceptions of began to examine the role of the cerebellum in
cerebellar function were arrived at by two different the regulation of both unlearned (i.e., uncondi-
and to a large extent noninteractive groups of tioned) reflexes and in learned (i.e., conditioned)
scientists: the first located in the "West," by which reflexes. The focus was on behavior that occurs
is meant western Europe and the western hemi- in a relatively stereotyped manner in response to
sphere, and the second in the USSR. a identifiable antecedent events. In contrast to in-
Scientists in the West focused their attention strumental conditioning, which requires that the
on the role of the cerebellum in voluntary be- experimenter wait for an animal to make a re-
havior-behavior that occurs without any perceiv- sponse before providing a reward, Pavlovian con-
able external cause and is therefore considered to ditioning requires the experimenter to elicit the
be self-generated. Causality for voluntary acts desired response. It is not difficult to see the
can only be inferred by what the act accomplishes, relationship between the historical and political
thereby giving such behavior a purposive charac- context within which these two groups operated
and their views concerning the important deter-
minants of behavior.
a Unless otherwise noted, citations to historical works
were derived from the following references: Holmes Instrumental behavior and conditioned reflexes
1917; Luciani, 1915; Pollock and Davis, 1927; Popov, have in common the fact that they represent learn-
1929a; Rawson, 1932. ed responses. Indeed, Pavlov, along with many

301
302 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

learning theorists in the West, believed that condi- behaved in a manner suggesting that their inten-
tioned reflexes formed the basis of instrumental tion or will to perform various motor acts had
behaviors. As we shall see, the early consensus of not been affected. For instance, subjects with cere-
scientists in the West and USSR was that the bellar lesions would attempt to feed or avoid
cerebral cortex was involved in the acquisition of obstacles even though their ataxia led to repeated
instrumental behaviors and conditioned reflexes failures. Because such behaviors were attempted
whereas the cerebellum was necessary for their despite cerebellar dysfunction, Rolando con-
optimal performance. However, the vulnera- cluded that the cardinal function of the cerebellum
bility ofthese two learned behaviors to cerebellar was to reinforce the motor impulse for voluntary
damage has led to frequent speculations that the movement. Since Rolando observed complete
cerebellum was somehow involved not only in paresis immediately after cerebellectomy, he con-
the expression of learned behavior, but with the cluded that the etiology of ataxia following partial
learning process itself(see Rawson, 1932). Recently, cerebellar damage was an alteration in the bal-
a strong claim has been made that the cerebellum anced regulation of motor impulses emanating
is not only essential but actually the center for from the cerebral cortex. This resulted in the para-
learning and memory of some reflexive motor lysis of a limited set of muscles that were nor-
acts. In this view, the plastic events that underlie mally regulated by the damaged cerebellar tissue.
motor learning are located within the cerebellum. Rolando's observations on the effects of cerebellar
Thus, a second purpose ofthis chapter is to review damage were in marked contrast with the effects
and evaluate the most recent controversies con- of damage to the cerebrum. Lesions ofthe cerebral
cerning the involvement of the cerebellum in cortex appeared to disrupt an animals' will, voli-
learning that have emerged from the use of tion, or desire to perform various motor acts. The
Pavlovian conditioned reflexes, especially the implications of this dichotomy were clear: the
nictitating membrane reflex of the rabbit, and to cerebrum was the source of volition and voluntary
place this controversy within the historical con- behavior and the cerebellum ensured coordinated
text of previous cerebellar research carried out behavior by reinforcing the cerebral impulses that
in the West and in the USSR. initiated voluntary movements. This view of cere-
bral and cerebellar function determined, for nearly
200 years, the direction of research on the role of
The Western Tradition: Cerebellar the cerebellum in regulating voluntary move-
Regulation of Voluntary Movement ment.
By the early 1900s a consensus was being de-
veloped in western Europe and the western Hemi- Fluorens
sphere that the cerebellum was intimately involved
in the regulation of voluntary motor behavior. The notion of a dominant role for the cerebellum
This consensus was reached after more than 200 in voluntary behavior was also supported by the
years of research dealing with the effects of cere- work of Flourens (1824-1842), but he had a dif-
bellar removal on sensory and motor function in ferent interpretation for the etiology of the motor
a variety of vertebrate species. deficits following cerebellar damage. In Flourens'
view, the function of the cerebellum was coordi-
nation per se, and the deficits following such
Rolando
damage were specifically due to the withdrawal of
The conception that cerebellar function is par- the brain center for motor coordination. Flourens
ticularly related to the regulation of voluntary studied the effects of varying amounts of cerebellar
motor behavior can be traced to the conclusions damage in the same animal. During the incremen-
of Rolando (1809-1823). In agreement with earlier tal removal of successively deeper layers of the
findings of Lorry (1760), Rolando noted that cere- cerebellum, Flourens observed progressive deficits
bellar damage profoundly disrupted the ability in behavior. Mter complete cerebellectomy, a bird
to walk. However, Rolando further noted that would lose the ability to walk, stand, fly, and right
animals with cerebellar lesions, although ataxic, itself but would still attempt to escape from a
16. Role of the Cerebellum 303

threatening stimulus. However, such escape be- the reflexive use of proprioceptive information
ha viors were not expressed in an organized fashion from the muscles, tendons, joints, and vestibular
but were chaotic and uncoordinated. Flourens labyrinths.
reinterpreted Rolando's description of paresis to The attempt by Flourens to interpret motor
be muscle weakness, but further noted that the deficits produced by cerebellar damage as result-
power of movements could be retained after cere- ing from damage to a center for the coordination
bellar damage. However " ... the co-ordination of of voluntary movements was soon questioned by
movement, the ability for controlled and determined a number of investigators. In a review of 18 human
movement was lost ..." (Flourens, 1824 as cited in cases of cerebellar deficiency dating from 1839,
Rawson, 1932; emphasis added). Thus, Flourens Dickenson (1865) concluded that the main deficit
believed that deficits of voluntary movement following cerebellar pathology was the failure of
were not due to a lack of determination (i.e., voluntary muscle power. Deficits in coordination
volition) but rather due to an inability to control per se were not believed to be the etiology of the
or coordinate such movement. motor syndrome evident in the clinical cases.
Flourens' view of the cerebellum as the primary Rather, the cause of irregular and unbalanced
coordinating center of the brain was also held by movements was thought to result from asym-
Lussana (1862) and later by Lewandowsky (1903, metric cerebellar damage that in turn produced
both cited in Luciani, 1915). These investigators an uneven distribution of power in the volun-
conceived of the cerebellum as a center for muscle tary musculature. These views were essentially
sense and attributed deficits in coordination of a restatement of Rolando's earlier observations.
voluntary movement to impairments in the ability Dickenson obtained further support for his views
to use proprioceptive information. In this light, from ablation studies in a variety of animals in-
Lussana recognized that " ... the muscular sense cluding snakes, toads, salamanders, tortoises, fish,
serves in animals to make known the resistance pigeons, and guinea pigs. He observed that,
met with, and in voluntary movements to regulate throughout phylogeny, the addition of the cere-
the forces of contraction by which the muscle is bellum in the otherwise decerebrate animal pro-
able to overcome it" (Lussana, 1862, cited in vided power to movements and allowed them to
Luciani, 1915). Lewandowsky took as proof for occur more naturally. Dickenson's views were to
his position the fact that the elimination of move- be strongly supported by Luciani.
ment-induced feedback that occurs in tabes dor-
salis or resection of the dorsal roots of the spinal
Luciani
cord produces the classic signs of cerebellar dys-
function: dysmetria of voluntary movement and Prefacing the results of7 years' work on the prob-
"hen's gait," a form of dysmetria in which flexion lem of cerebellar function (1884-1891), Luciani
and extension are exaggerated during walking. declared that " ... whatever the extent or degree
The views of Sherrington (1906) were consonant of the cerebellar lesion, ... the resulting symp-
with those of Lussana and Lewandowsky. Sher- toms are disturbances of voluntary movements"
ring ton conceived of coordinated movement as a (Luciani, 1915). Luciani provided a highly devel-
chain of brain stem and spinal reflexes occurring oped framework in which to view the primary
in an organized sequence. Voluntary motor acts etiologies of cerebellar damage. This framework
occurred when the cerebral cortex engaged such remains valuable to the present day. Employing
a pattern of spinal and bulbar reflex arcs. The freely moving dogs and apes, Luciani identified
fluidity of any voluntary movement depended on three stages of motor behavior following cere-
the reflexive regulation of muscle tone by pro- bellar lesions. The first stage was the period of
prioceptors of antagonistic muscle groups- "dynamic phenomena," in which the irritative
Sherrington's reciprocal innervation. Sherrington effects of the lesion on the remainder of the brain
envisioned the cerebellum as having an essential served as the source for the consequent behavioral
role in this process, namely to act as the " ... head deficits. Symptoms of this period following uni-
ganglion ofthe proprioceptive system ..." to ensure lateral cerebellectomy were unrest and agitation,
that normal muscle tone was maintained through ipsilateral extensor rigidity, pleurothotonus, nys-
304 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

tagmus, and forced rotation along the long axis nomena, Luciani observed ataxic animals behav-
of the bod y away from the side of the lesion. These ing in ways that he interpreted to demonstrate the
symptoms subsided within 10 days after surgery. animals' inteo,tion to avert the consequences of
Luciani asserted that previous expetimental atonia, asthenia, and astasia. For instance, exag-
work on animals had been confined to the first gerated abduction of the limbs during walking
stage, the early postoperative period, and had (the "drunken sailor's gate") was viewed as a
therefore failed to observe the true deficits in behavioral compensation for the adverse effects
behavior resulting from withdrawal of cerebellar of falling due to limb dysfunction. Exaggerated
function. These occurred in the second stage, the abduction of the limbs, by widening the base
stage of cerebellar deficiency. The summation of of support and lowering the center of gravity,
the motor deficits in this period constituted the minimized the probability and severity of falling.
global behavioral deficit recognized as ataxia. Another compensatory behavior exhibited by cere-
Luciani devoted the majority of his attention to bellectomized dogs was crossing of the forelimbs
this stage. Finally, the third s.tage, or stage of during walking. Luciani believed that this
"compensatory phenomena," resulted from either behavior was intentionally performed to main-
the recovery of function of the damaged tissue tain equilibrium and thereby compensate for the
(organic compensation) or from changes in the horizontal oscillations resulting from atonia and
function of the cerebrum and/or remaining cere- asthenia of the vertebral muscles.
bellar tissue (functional compensation). Functional In addition to the importance that Luciani
compensation permitted compensatory behaviors assigned to the cerebellum for maintaining muscle
to counter the negative consequences of cerebellar tone, strength, and stasis, he also recognized tro-
deficiency. phic influences of the cerebellum on the brain and
Luciani described three fundamental deficits the periphery. Direct trophic influences of the
during the period of cerebellar deficiency: asthenia, cerebellum on central nervous structures to which
a loss of muscle strength; atonia, a loss of muscle it was connected ensured normal function of sen-
tone; and astasia, tremor and rhythmic oscilla- sory and motor systems. Indirect trophic influ-
tion. These three primary symptoms were pre- ences on peripheral organs were recognized when
sumed by Luciani to be the underlying etiology of subjects with cerebellar lesions exhibited muscle
all movement disorders resulting from the with- atrophy, changes in cutaneous elements, and
drawal of cerebellar function. Noting that dogs diminished resistance to injury.
with cerebellar lesions were unable to support
their weight on land but could swim virtually as
well as normal dogs, Luciani suggested that co- Holmes
ordination per se was not abolished by removal An important development in our understanding
of the cerebellum but rather that the symptoms of cerebellar function was initiated by Holmes
that appeared as deficits in coordination were (1917), who studied the motor deficits exhibited
the consequence of deficits in tone (atonia) and by human subjects with gunshot injuries to the
strength (asthenia) required for movement against back of the head. These studies were notable for
resistive forces. One component of astasia that their greater attention to quantitative measures.
Luciani observed in his experimental animals was Holmes shared with Rolando, Flourens, and
similar to the clinical observation of titubation: Luciani a strong interest in the performance defi-
the hesitancy to initiate a voluntary movement cits associated with voluntary movements. Con-
or the inability to execute voluntary movements sistent with Luciani's description of asthenia,
in a smooth and stable manner without tremor. Holmes' quantification of the forces of grasping,
In ,agreement with Flourens, Luciani did not elbow flexion and extension, supination, prona-
ascribe these deficits to a delay in the development tion, and ankle extension revealed up to 50%
of the voluntary impulse but, rather, to the in- deficits on the side ipsilateral to a unilateral cere-
complete summation of neural impulses necessary bellar injury relative to the unaffected side. Patients
for the smooth performance of voluntary acts. with unilateral cerebellar lesions were described
During the third stage of compensatory phe- as reluctant to move the limbs ipsilateral to the
16. Role of the Cerebellum 305

Figure 16.1. Effect of a gunshot wound to the right hand, respectively. Upper markings represent time in
half of the cerebellum on voluntary movement in a tenths of seconds calibrated by a tuning fork at 128 Hz.
man. The kymographic record is taken from Holmes Note that the right hand (A') showed an approximately
(1917). The patient was instructed to grasp calibrated 200-ms increase in response latency, a longer rise time
springs with each of his hands simultaneously. The to peak amplitude and a lower amplitude as compared
vertical bars (marked I and l' for the left and right with the unaffected left hand (A). Also, note that the
hands, respectively) indicate the time at which the latency to initiate ·and achieve relaxation was longer
command to grasp was given. The left (A) and right (A') for the affected right hand (B') as compared with the
grasping responses were followed by a command to normal left hand (B). Reprinted with permission from
relax at the vertical lines marked 2 and 2'. Relaxing Holmes, 1917.
responses are bibeled Band B', for the left and right

lesion, and on command these patients moved Holmes subdivided the cerebellar ataxic syn-
the limbs ipsilateral to the lesion with less vigor, drome into five components: decomposition of
force, and speed than those contralateral to the movement, asynergia, dysmetria, tremor, and
lesion. The grasp of the hand ipsilateral to a deviations from the line of movement. Decompo-
unilateral cerebellar lesion was demonstrated to sition of movement described the break up of
be delayed in initiation by 200 ms, reduced by continuous movements into a series of discrete
50% in its peak force, and doubled in its time to movements separated by pauses. Asynergia re-
peak force relative to the unaffected hand (see ferred to the absence of synergistic actions of an-
Fig. 16.1). The delay in response onset was mir- tagonistic muscles required for graceful motion.
rored by an equivalent delay in the initiation of Dysmetria simply referred to alterations in the
relaxation, indicating that response onset and range and force of movements. Deviations from
offset were equally affected (see Fig. 16.1). Holmes the line of movement described movements that
noted the uniqueness of this clinical presentation did not take the normal course of direction to
because, in contradistinction to lesions of the ensure the least expenditure of energy. Holmes
motor cortex, cerebellar lesions affected many conceived ofthe cerebellum as a motor reinforcing
groups of muscles equally while not imparting organ that did not in and of itself initiate move-
rigidity, affected all voluntary movements while ments, but instead regulated the motor system so
not limiting them in range, and left the primary that a ". .. response to a volitional stimulus is
reflexes relatively intact. immediate, effective and proportional to the in-
306 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

tensity of the cerebral impulse" (Holmes, 1917). of the cerebellum in motor movement are worth
Cerebellar regulation of voluntary movement was noting:
thought to be effected by continuous regulation
of postural tone as well as by phasic regulation It is an organ which has evolved on the afferent rather
in the alterations of tone required for the synchro- than on the motor side of the central nervous system.
nous actions of muscles necessary for coordinated But it receives and integrates proprioceptive impulses
from all parts of the body, and by virtue of these it
movement. Later, Holmes added that, in addition
keeps the motor mechanisms in such a state of tone
to the musculature, the tone ofthe cerebral motor that they can react promptly and efficiently to voluntary
cortex and the subcortical motor nuclei were regu- impulses, and thus assures the correct co-operation of
lated by the cerebellum. Accordingly, the cere- the separate motor centers that are concerned in indi-
bellum assured that the higher centers mediating vidual acts. (p. 528)
movement responded expeditiously and with nor-
mal magnitude to volitional impulses (Holmes, Although Holmes. believed that a change in
1939). postural tone was the etiology of the deficits in
Although cerebellar lesions appeared to have both involuntary and voluntary movements, the
their greatest effects on the performance of vol un- implication of these findings with regard to the
tary movements, Holmes made the important long-standing notion that the cerebellum was
observation that involuntary (reflexive) move- specifically involved in regulating voluntary motor
ments were also impaired after cerebellar damage. acts was not realized. In fact, the data provided
For example, even the disynaptic knee jerk elicited by Holmes strongly suggested that the deficits in
by a tap on the patellar tendon was weaker, less performance were global in nature, affecting both
brisk, and harder to elicit on the side ipsilateral voluntary and involuntary movements. As we
to a unilateral cerebellar lesion as compared to will see, this observation had great implications
the contralateral side. During the acute phase for subsequent studies dealing with a possible role
following cerebellar injury the patellar reflex could of the cerebellum in learning.
be entirely absent. With longer recovery time, the
ipsilateral leg would exhibit a normal amplitude
The Consensus of 1927
response but would continue to swing like a pen-
dulum at times when the normal leg would come The Combined Meeting of the Section of Neuro-
to rest. Ankle reflexes and nociceptive reflexes to logy of the Royal Society of Medicine and the
pin pricks were found to be less brisk in initiation American Neurological Association held in
and maximal amplitude. As noted by Holmes London in 1927 firmly reinforced Rolando's original
(1917, p. 471), "Even if his two hands are pricked notion of a specific role for the cerebellum in
by a pin the patient withdraws the affected limb voluntary movement. In summarizing the pro-
less readily and less briskly than the normal." ceedings, Walshe authoritatively proclaimed that
Holmes was aware of similar reports by Luciani "It is solely voluntary movement, therefore, which
(1915), who found that nociceptive reflexes in is dependent upon cerebellar activity" (original
experimental animals were also reduced by cere- emphasis of Walshe, 1927). Feeling constrained
bellar lesions. However, Holmes (1917, p. 511) by the purely descriptive methodology of Luciani,
noted that in humans the decrease in nociceptive investigators at this meeting capitalized on the
reflexes occurred with " ... no alteration in the findings of Sherrington and Magnus, which de-
threshold of, or diminution in the acuity to, tactile monstrated that postural reflexes, presumed to be
and painful stimuli, or even subjective differences the fundamental elements of coordinated move-
between the sensations similarly evoked on the two ments, resided at low levels of the neuraxis. This
sides ..." Thus, in Holmes' view the slowness of was revealed in spinal, pontospinal, and thalamic
movement was not due to an involvement of the animals. Sherrington proposed that in order to
cerebellum in the processing of stimuli so as to study the components of coordinated movement
alter their arrival time at the cortex. The con- in isolation, the higher elements of the cerebrum
clusions that Holmes (1917) reached on the role should be removed. The decerebrated animal,
16. Role of the Cerebellum 307

furthermore, provided the opportunity to obtain These outcomes signified that the basic reflex
a clean measure of cerebellar regulation of the mechanisms necessary for coordinated movement
postural reflexes that comprise coordinated move- not only resided in the spinal cord and lower
ment. Magnus (1916, as cited in Pollock and brain stem but were autonomous of cerebel-
Davis, 1927) demonstrated that walking, jumping, lar function. Nevertheless, cerebellectomized but
and running in the-thalamic animal was indistin- otherwise intact animals demonstrated the pro-
guishable from that in the intact animal. However, minent postural alterations and motor deficits
the thalamic animal without its cerebellum was that comprised the cerebellar syndrome so aptly
no worse behaviorally than the thalamic animal described by Luciani. These outcomes led the
with its cerebellum intact (Magnus, 1924, as cited London conferees to conclude that the cerebellum
in Rawson, 1932). Additionally, Pollock and Davis effected its function only in association with the
(1927) found that cerebellectomy did not affect cerebral cortex. Because Sherrington specified that
postural and spinal reflexes of the decerebrate cat. voluntary movement consisted of sequences of

FLEXIONS
EMG MOVEMENT
ONSET

CONTRDL .~

t
COOL
MEDIAL

COOL
LATERAL t
COOL 1,1'1"
MEDIAL &
LATERAL I
.....---oolllliJlllLl....--
,

CONTROL j ,&,I. .--....-z,o


too
Figure 16.2. The effect of cooling the dentate nucleus aspects of the dentate nucleus were simultaneously
on the onset of conditioned arm movements of monkeys inactivated. During this time both EM G and movement
with flexions shown on the left and extensions on the onsets were increased by approximately 100 ms for
right side of the figure (Meyer-Lohmann et al., 1977). flexions and 130 ms for extensions. Note that normal
Histograms were plotted as number of responses on performance was restored when cooling was terminat-
the ordinate (see calibration bar for to responses in ed at the end of the experiment (compare control data
lower right of figure) as a function of time in ms from of top and bottom rows). Reprinted with permission
CS onset (arrow) on the abscissa. Deficits in reaction from Meyer-Lohmann, Hore, and Brooks, 1977.
time were most prominent when both medial and lateral
308 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

brain stem and spinal reflexes under the guidance voluntary (instrumental) behavior. The following
and control of the motor cortex, the essential effects of cerebellar damage were repeatedly ob-
presence of the cerebrum for cerebellar dysfu~c­ served by all investigators and are of special rele-
tion provided experimental proof of the speCIal- vance for this chapter: a) a decrease in the amplitude
ized role of the cerebellum in the regulation of of instrumental responses, b) a decrease in the fre-
voluntary movement. quency of responses, c) an increase in the onset
latency of responses, d) an increase in the time
Recent Studies required to reach maximal response amplitude,
and e) an increase in the rise time of responses
The primary focus of interest in the latter part of
(i.e., the time between response onset and peak
the 20th century continued to be on the mech-
amplitude). These effects were primarily examined
anisms whereby the cerebellum regulated volun-
in instrumental acts and so the possible effects of
tary motor behavior and the primary etiologies
cerebellar damage on involuntary (reflexive) re-
of the global deficits produced by cerebellar
sponses remained uncertain. However, the obser-
damage. These studies placed an increased em-
vations of both Luciani and Holmes suggested
phasis on precise experimental measurem:nts of
that reflexive acts were also affected, but to a lesser
the instrumental behaviors of nonhuman pnmates
degree. The general agreement was that the cere-
using procedures pioneered by Evarts (1966) in
bellum was important for the performance of
combination with electrophysiologic, electromyo-
instrumental acts, but its possibly wider role in
graphic, and kinematic m~asur:s. By rever.si~ly all responding (instrumental and reflexive) was
disrupting cerebellar functIOn wIth cryogemc In-
not carefully documented by these studies. Addi-
activation, a series of experiments were able to
tional documentation dealing with these issues
'confirm and extend many of Holmes' original
was to come from Pavlovian conditioning studies
descriptions of cerebellar insufficiency (see Brooks,
conducted in Russia.
1986). Most notable of these were deficits in move-
ment onset following cerebellar dysfunction. As
shown in Figure 16.2, inactivating the dentate The Russian Tradition:
nucleus delayed the onset of movement and muscle
activity for both conditioned flexions and exten-
Cerebellar Regulation
sions of the arm (Meyer-Lohmann et aI., 1977). of Reflexive Movement
Cooling the dentate nucleus was shown to increase
reaction times for both conditioned flexions and The Pavlovian Method
extensions of the arm to approximately 130% of
By 1927, the Russian physiologist Pavlov had
normal as well as to decrease the initial velocity
already introduced the concept ofthe conditioned
of the movements. Simultaneous electrophysio-
reflex and the methods for its establishment as a
logical recordings from precentral neurons sug-
means of studying higher forms of nervous ac-
gested that profound inactivation of den~ate fun~­
tivity. A detailed treatise, translated to English,
tion delayed movement onset by remOVIng tomc
summarized more than 25 years of research
facilitation of subcortical nuclei and spinal motor
(1899-1927) on the subject (Pavlov, 1927). T~e
neurons since precentral neurons were not delayed
foundation for this research was firmly rooted In
in their onset. However, when the dentate nucleus
Descartes' concept of the nervous reflex, a funda-
was less severe1y inactivated, the activity of pre-
mental mechanism whereby the central nervous
central neurons was delayed to an extent equiv-
system converted sensory input to motor output.
alent to the increase in reaction time, suggesting
The concept of the reflex had already influenced
that dentate cooling reduced a depolarizing drive
Russian physiological thought, serving as the cor-
on the motor cortex.
nerstone upon which a deterministic theory of
cerebral activity was to be built. This notion was
Summary
advocated by Sechenov, whose "Reflexes of the
The result of the studies cited above led to a clear Brain" (1863) sought to describe the totality of
description of the effects of cerebellar damage on cerebral function through reflex action.
16. Role of the Cerebellum 309

In the course of his physiological studies on the CS would come to elicit a CR of leg flexion,
digestion, Pavlov observed that salivary reflexes the shock UCS would still be delivered at the
were not always elicited by oral stimuli but could specified CS-UCS interval and for the specified
come under apparently "psychic" control. The duration. This method will be referred to as
fundamental observation was that salivary re- Pavlovian conditioning or, simply, conditioning.
flexes were not only elicited unconditionally by The second contained an instrumental (voluntary)
food in the mouth but would come to be elicited component and was methodologically identical
by a neutral stimulus if its occurrence preceded to the conditioned avoidance response. It will
the receipt offood. These neutral stimuli included therefore be referred to as the avoidance method,
those commonly present during an experiment or avoidance conditioning. It should be recalled
such as the food bowl and the experimenter as that Pavlov (1927) considered instrumental be-
well as novel auditory and visual stimuli such as havior to be based on the establishment of condi-
whistles and light bulbs. Reluctant to yield to tioned reflexes.
a subjective terminology that labeled these re- The avoidance method employed the general
sponses as "psychic secretions" or "voluntary re- procedures of Pavlovian conditioning except that
sponding," Pavlov sought to determine objectively the shock UCS could be delivered only when the
the conditions under which salivary reflexes could dog's paw made contact with the floor. Thus,
come to be elicited by previously neutral stimuli. flexion of the leg would immediately terminate
Following the lead of Sechenov, Pavlov consid- the shock. For example, an animal could com-
ered these "psychic secretions" to represent the pletely avoid the shock UCS by flexing its leg to
formation of new reflexes. the CS before UCS onset or it could escape from
The Pavlovian method employed a biological the shock by flexing its leg after UCS onset. It
reflex for which there was a known eliciting stim- should be obvious that the Pavlovian method
ulus, the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), and a was always used in the study of autonomic func-
definable reflexive response, the unconditioned tions such as salivary, pancreatic, gallbladder,
response (UCR). From approximately 1898 to and stomach secretions as well as heart rate
1930, Pavlov directed his research efforts at exam- and blood pressure (Bykov, 1957; Gantt, 1989),
ining the conditions under which stimuli that whereas both methods were employed for leg
were initially neutral with respect to a biological flexion.
reflex could come to elicit that reflex. Such stimuli Pavlov considered the cerebral cortex to be the
were termed conditioned (actually conditional) site of associative learning and the conditioned
stimuli (CSs) and the response they elicited the reflex to be a tool for uncovering cerebral (mental)
conditioned response (CR). Of special interest function. These views determined the experimental
were the temporal relationship between the CS questions in Pavlov's laboratory during his
and UCS, the number and frequency of paired active period of research on the conditioned
presentations of CS and UCS, the modality and reflex (1898-1930). It was only near the end of
intensity of the CS, the intensity of the UCS, the this period that one of his students (Popov,
health ofthe animal, and the context in which the 1929b) examined the role of the cerebellum.
stimuli were delivered. However, while Western scientists were soon in-
Two conditioning procedures were typically troduced to the method of Pavlovian conditioning
employed in the studies to be described below. (e.g., Yerkes and Morgulis, 1909), they remained
The first represented what we now consider to be largely unaware of either the physiological and
the strict Pavlovian method in which the animal pharmacological studies on the conditioned reflex
was restrained and the delivery ofthe stimuli was that were being carried out by Pavlov and his
independent of the animals' behavior. For example, students or of the subsequent studies on the cere-
in this method a tone CS would be paired with a bellum. By contrast, Russian scientists were well
shock UCS applied to the foot of a dog by means aware of Western studies on the cerebellum and
of a cuff electrode. Although the shock UCS employed Pavlovian conditioning to obtain more
would elicit a UCR ofleg flexion, the duration of objective measures of cerebellar pathology. The
the shock was not affected. Similarly, although following is a summary of these studies.
310 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

Popov returned in this animal 10 weeks later. This dog


could not stay afloat after the surgery. Removal
In 1929, one of Pavlov's students, N.F. Popov, of the vermis in a cat induced opisthotonos, hyper-
reported detailed observations of one dog with tonia of the vertebral muscles, agitation and un-
complete cerebellectomy, one dog with unilateral rest, forced rotation, astasia of the head and limbs,
cerebellectomy, and one cat with removal of the clonic movements of the hind leg, dysmetria of
vermis (Popov, 1929a). In large part, Popov's foreleg reaching, and marked extension of the
observations of motor deficits up to 18 months hind limbs while walking.
after surgery were in complete agreement with In interpreting these outcomes, Popov noted
Luciani. Popov reported changes in sensory and that the dominant symptom of cerebellar damage
motor function of dogs and cats with cerebellar was marked hypertonia of the extremities, neck,
damage by employing an array of neurological and back, which was most prominent during ac-
tests. This initial study verified many of the highly tivity. Be described the extremities during walking
documented findings of Luciani and attempted as appearing as if they were unyielding sticks and
to place the observed deficits in sensory and motor noted the greater and more enduring deficits in
function within the context of a Cartesian frame- the hind legs as compared to the forelegs. He
work based on reflex activity. could not clearly demarcate Luciani's three stages
Immediately after cerebellectomy, the clinical of deficits after cerebellar removal. Instead, he
presentation was clonic extension of the forelegs, believed that all of the aforementioned manifesta-
slight flexion of the hind legs, and opisthotonos. tions of cerebellar damage were constantly present
The hind legs were characterized by marked hy- from. the operation until death, simply varying in
pertonia by the second postoperative day but this intensity throughout the animal's life.
was not a constant phenomenon. Hypertonia gave Popov was particularly intrigued by what ap-
way to hypotonia by the eighth day, which, in peared to be a paradoxical effect of cerebellar
turn, persisted until 2 months after the surgery. damage on complex voluntary movements in con-
At this point, hypertonia returned. Chronic trast to the effects on reflexive movements. For
deficits were general agitation and unrest, head example, the movements comprising the complex
oscillations, hen's gait, falling backward, and com- motor acts of walking, reaching, and swimming
pensated ambulation characterized by marked appeared to be intensified, while comparatively
abduction of the limbs. In agreement with previous simple, reflexive movements to pin prick and pain-
observations by Luciani and Holmes, Popov also ful stimuli could be attenuated or sometimes
observed deficits in reflexive behaviors. For abolished.
example, there was an abolition of the response Popov believed that the primary function of
to pin prick, and a general increase in the threshold the cerebellum was to inhibit the reflexive activity
for evoking responses to painful stimuli. These of centers in the midbrain that were responsible
effects were noted up to 1 month after the surgery. for relaying motor commands ofthe cerebrum to
Popov observed that this dog showed a marked the spinal cord. Popov accepted Sherrington's
inability to swim. positions that complex movements depended on
Many ofthe same deficits were observed on the the analytic and synthetic capabilities of the
side of the lesion in a dog with unilateral cere bel- cerebrum and that the principal means whereby
lectomy: ipsilateral extensor rigidity, hypertonia the cerebellum exerted its function was through
of the ipsilateral foreleg, weakness of the ipsilat- the integration of proprioceptive information. The
eral body reflexes, llgitation, and unrest. The hemi- deficits in complex motor acts after cerebellar
cerebellectomized dog also demonstrated stra- damage suggested to Popov that motor centers
bismus, pleurothotonos, and forced rotation. As of the midbrain were in a state of uncoordinated
with a complete cerebellectomy, a unilaterally hyperexcitability and could not relay the cere-
cerebellectomized dog demonstrated marked beral commands for flexion to the limbs. In
hypertonia on the side of the lesion immediately Popov's view, the extensor rigidity after decere-
after the surgery; this was replaced by hypotonia bellation was functionally similar to the profound
on the seventh postsurgical day. Normal tone extensor rigidity that occurred after decerebra-
16. Role of the Cerebellum 311

tion, both resulting from the inability of the movements of humans. Because of his extensive
cerebrum to express flexion and adduction. The work on the effect of cerebellectomy on move-
reflexive activity of midbrain motor centers, ments of the hind limbs (Popov, 1929a), Popov
unchecked by regulatory inhibition of the cere- studied the defensive reflexes of the hind limbs of
bellum after cerebellar damage, would not permit cerebellectomized dogs. In a first study that
the passage of descending motor commands but employed Pavlovian conditioning, a flashing light
instead would express a dominant predisposition served as the CS and preceded the shock UCS
for excitation of extensor motoneurons. Complex delivered to a leg cuff on each trial. Normal dogs
movements would lose their fluidity and become readily acquired a CR oflimb flexion to the flash-
slow as a result. ing light. Cerebellectomized dogs also acquired
Popov believed that a second function of the pronounced CRs to the CS within eight parings
cerebellum was to intensify the "correcting and of CS and UCS. However, these CRs differed
focusing" impulses of the cerebrum that were from control dogs in the direction of movement.
essential for the proper maintenance of reflex Instead oflimb flexion as a response to the flashing
function. In this way, Popov could explain the light, cerebellectomized dogs responded with limb
apparent paradox of the simultaneous occurrence extension. This was observed on each trial in
of hypometria of the reflexes and hypermetria of which the light was paired with paw shock
more complex motor acts. Popov also believed because in the Pavlovian method shock delivery
that the cerebellum had an important function was independent of whether the animal flexed,
that was independent of the cerebrum. This addi- extended, or did not respond to the light. Cere-
tional role, as he put it, was essential for preparing bellectomy did not affect the unconditioned limb
an "essential ground" for the execution of move- flexion elicited by the shock UCS, which could
ments by ensuring a proper state of tone prepara- be observed on each conditioning trial. The cere-
tory to muscular activity. bellectomized dogs would extend their leg to the
The detailed description of Popov's findings light CS and then flex their leg to the shock UCS.
have been presented in order to emphasize that These dogs were unable to acquire leg flexion as
he obtained the same effects of cerebellectomy the CR even after 100 pairings of the flashing
as had scientists in the West. Moreover, his obser- light CS with the shock UCS.
vations and interpretations were essentially ident- To capture further the pronounced effects of
ical with those of Western scientists, notably cerebellar dysfunction on CRs ofthe limb, Popov
Luciani and Holmes. However, Popov was not subsequently employed the avoidance method of
satisfied with the primarily observational method conditioning. The CS was a metronome beating
on which his observations and those of others at 168 beats per minute and having a duration of
were based. This is clearly stated in his second approximately 40 s. Popov's original recordings
report (Popov, 1929b), which began with the fol- are presented in Figure 16.3. Normal dogs readily
lowing comment: flexed their limb to the metronome, maintained
Destruction of control of voluntary muscles by means flexion for the duration of the stimulus, and
of complete or partial extirpation of the cerebellum thereby avoided the shock (top tracing in left half
presents an extremely complex picture. Because the of Fig. 16.3). However, as in the previous experi-
usual method of observation is completely subjective, ment, cerebellectomized dogs displayed a pro-
a large number of theories arose, giving rise to myriad nounced limb extension as the CR in response to
interpretations and a wholly unscientific terminology the metronome CS (top tracing in right half of
in which to express them. Thus, it is not surprising that Fig. 16.3). This brought the limb in contact with
the study of cerebellar function has remained exactly the floor, resulting in the delivery of the shock
where the classical experiments of Luciani left it, half UCS and, consequently, elicitation of leg flexion
a century agb.
as the UCR. Flexion was not maintained, pre-
For these reasons, Popov applied Pavlovian sumably due to the continued presentation of
conditioning procedures to the study of cerebellar the metronome CS, which would elicit extension
function. I t is clear that Popov considered the CR as the CR. Thus, for as long as the metronome
elicited by a CS to be analogous to voluntary was on, the cerebellectomized dog would vacil-
312 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

Figure 16.3. The effect of cerebellectomy on condi- CS. The dog maintains this flexion CR for the entire
tioned avoidance responding in a dog. The kymo- duration of the CS and thereby avoids onset of the
graphic tracings, from top to bottom, represent motor paw-shock DCS. The right half of the figure presents
reactions of a dog's hind limb (upward and downward a typical conditioning trial after cerebellectomy. The
deflections indicate flexion and extension, respectively), cerebellectomized dog responds with a prominent limb
presentation of the metronome CS (downward deflec- extension (downward deflection of pen) at metronome
tion of tracing 1 indicates CS onset and duration), onset, which brings its paw in contact with shock
delivery of the DCS (each upward deflection of tracing (upward deflection of pen on tracing 2). The shock
2 indicates delivery of shock to the paw), and time (each elicits a flexion response that permits the limb to escape
upward deflection of tracing 3 marks 1 s). The left the shock. For the duration of the metronome stimulus,
half of the figure presents a typical CR in a normal dog. flexion responses elicited by shock are followed by
Note that the dog demonstrates a robust flexion CR extension responses that bring the limb back into con-
(upward deflection of pen) in response to the metronome tact with the shock. (Reprinted from Popov, 1929b.)

late between flexion and extension of the limb extension. due to the removal of the cerebellum's
while the normal dog would maintain a pro- inhibitory regulation of the reflexive mechanisms
nounced flexion response (see legend to Fig. 16.3 that intensified the propensity for extension.
for details). Popov believed that limb flexion to a CS required
In interpreting these outcomes, Popov consi- finer regulation by the cerebral cortex through
dered the fact that a pronounced result of cere- indirect pathways that coursed through the cere-
bellectomy was extensor rigidity. Popov viewed bellum in order to supplement the direct pathways
the cqange in CRs following cerebellectomy as a of the pyramids. In the presence of cerebellar
shift from a propensity for flexion to one for damage " ... only a strong unconditioned peri-
16. Role of the Cerebellum 313

pheral stimulus ... [could] ... elicit a simple de- maintained flexion in the presence of the CS.
fensive reflex in the form of withdrawal." In Gambaryan permitted the symptoms of the im-
conclusion, Popov stated that " ... the cerebellum mediate postoperative period to subside before
plays a large role in the formation of conditioned subjecting the animals to the conditioning para-
motor reflexes." It is clear, however, that Popov digm. This period ranged from 2.5 to 10.5 months.
did not conceive this role to be one of serving as In two puppies with complete cerebellectomy,
the site where learning occurred but rather one conditioned leg flexions were acquired within 90
of regulating the reflexive activity of the brain presentations of a bell CS that signalled shock to
stem so that the output ofthe motor cortex could the hind limb. The absence of the cerebellum in
produce a normally executed CR. one ofthese puppies (Odinokaya) is documented
in the top panel of Figure 16.4, whereas the bottom
panel illustrates conditioned leg flexions elicited
Gambaryan
by the bell CS with no evidence of the leg exten-
The effect of cerebellectomy on conditioned sion that had been noted by Popov. However,
leg flexion was reexamined 30 years later by Gambaryan did report that the leg flexion CRs
Gambaryan (1960) and Fanardjian (1961). were frequently preceded by leg extension, and
Gambaryan employed the avoidance procedure this occurred even after 130 trials when condi-
in a manner similar to Popov's second experi- tioning had been reliably achieved. Gambaryan
ment where the dog could avoid paw shock if it attributed these differences in experimental out-

Figure 16.4. Top panel presents the posterior


view of the brain of a puppy named Odinokay
in which the cerebellum was completely re-
moved. The bottom panel presents condi-
tioned avoidance responses of the hind limb
ofthis puppy after cerebellectomy. CRs ofleg
flexion are elicited by a positive CS (bell + )
but not by a CS of another frequency that
had never been paired with the UCS (bell- ).
The tracings, from top to bottom, indicate
hind limb CRs (upward deflection represents
flexion), CS onset and duration, UCS of paw-
shock delivery, and time in seconds. Note
that the CRs were successfully maintained so
that no shock was delivered. (Reprinted with
permission from Gambaryan, 1960).
314 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

come to the shorter postoperative period em- cerebellum was removed after acquisition training
ployed by Popov. However, conditioned flexion retained the conditioned flexion response. In
responses were acquired at times when the summary, Gambaryan's experiments indicated
animals were severely ataxic and demonstrated that cerebellectomy did not block the acquisition
virtually all of the chronic motor deficiencies or retention ofleg flexion CRs even in the presence
noted by Luciani and Popov. of marked motor deficits.
Puppies with partial removal of the cerebellum
acquired the flexion response three times more
Fanardjian
rapidly than those with total cerebellectomy. A
puppy that had 10.5 months to recover from Fanardjian (1961) examined the effects of cere bel-
cerebellectomy acquired the flexion response even lectomy on conditioned leg flexion in 10 dogs.
more quickly than normal. In addition, a dog The Pavlovian method was employed to measure
with a lesion of the dorsal columns and whose simultaneously the responses of all four limbs

A
,1' -, ~._ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _J
J /.
1

2~~f~--~~~~~--------'-~--------------------~-4~---­
+

J
J~~----------------------------~----
4 ~'-------------_,--J ----
5

6
5,~_==~====~~~;;~~~======~
= --=------t
85
r AI .. '-.~_ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ jrfM '''L
4 .. ......._---.J.f'..,._ __
~~( ________________________--JL_______
J",.Ld. ~'-_________--~

Figure 16.5. Conditioned motor reaction before (A) the lesion the flexion response of the reinforced limb
and after (B) ablation of the paramedian lobe bilaterally (tracing 1) is rapid and flexion is maintained for the
and of the vermis (save lobules I - III). The numbered duration of the CSt B: After cerebellar damage the
tracings, from top to bottom, indicate 1) movements animal initiates CRs but demonstrates prominent
of left rear limb, 2) movements of right rear limb, 3) astasia of the limb before onset of the shock UCS.
movements of left front limb, 4) movements of right Delivery of an inhibitory stimulus (CS-) that was never
front limb, 5) pneumographic recording of respiration, paired with shock (second deflection in tracing 6)
6) mark of CS delivery, 7) mark of UCS delivery, occurred between the two presentations of the positive
and 8) time in seconds. Note that the CS was only CSt The CS- failed to elicit a CR. (Reprinted, with
paired with shock to the left rear limb. Limb flexion is permission, from Fanardjian, 1961.)
indicated by an upward deflection ofthe pen. A: Before
16. Role of the Cerebellum 315

during the pairing of visual or auditory CSs with Fanardjian concluded that the normal CR con-
a shock UCS to one hind limb. The UCS was sisted of two components: a weak tendency for
delivered independently of the animal's behavior extension and a stronger tendency for flexion.
by a cuff attached to the paw. In 4 of the 10 dogs, The effect of a cerebellar lesion, then, was to
CRs were established before surgery. The primary strengthen the extension component of the CR at
result obtained with all 10 dogs was that cerebel- the expense of the flexion component. In agree-
lectomy did not block the retention or prevent ment with Gambaryan and Popov, the tonic
the de novo acquisition of CRs. This can be seen component of the conditioned flexion response
in Figure 16.5, which compares the preoperative was more susceptible to cerebellar dysfunction
performance of CRs in the top portion of the than the phasic component. At early postopera-
figure with the postoperative performance ap- tive periods, the tonic component of limb flexion
proximately 1 month after surgery in the bottom was abolished in some cases while the phasic
portion (see figure legend for details). Conditioned component remained intact. Thus, whereas cere-
flexion responses, although smaller in amplitude bellectomized dogs initiated conditioned flexion
and duration, could be elicited reliably even on responses, they had difficulty maintaining the limb
the second day after surgery, a time when severe in a maximally flexed position for the duration
extensor rigidity and opisthotonos were present of the CS. This can be seen in the oscillatory
(Fig. 16.6; see figure legend for details). However, behavior of the CR in Figure 16.5 and its short
in agreement with Gambaryan and consistent duration in Figure 16.6 (see legends for details).
with the findings of Popov, cerebellectomized This may have in part accounted for the oscil-
dogs exhibited a greater tendency to extend their latory behavior of limb flexion and extension
hind limb in response to the CS, especially in the noted by Popov when employing the avoidance
immediate postoperative period. Normal dogs method (see Fig. 16.3).
sometimes showed a slight conditioned extension By simultaneously measuring all four limbs,
response preceding conditioned flexion. How- Fanardjian (1961) found that normal dogs acquired
ever, the frequency of extension in the intact the flexion response of the reinforced limb while
dog was less than in the cerebellectomized dog. gradually reducing movements in the other limbs.

------~~--~--------------~--------~~--.~----~----

J (

-~~., ... ..... ........... " ...• , .......... , ... , '1'.'

Figure 16.6. All conventions are the same as in onset of the UCS is followed by a large flexion UCR.
Figure 16.5. Conditioned reflexes of a dog's hind limb On the second trial, the CS elicits a CR characterized
on the second day after cerebellar ablation. Virtually by a long onset latency and small amplitude. This
all of the cerebellum was removed in this dog except a initial CR is followed by a second, large flexion CR
small portion of the inferior vermis. Only two condi- that is not maintained. As on the first trial, onset ofthe
tioning trials are presented. On the first trial, the CS UCS elicits a robust UCR. (Reprinted, with permission,
elicits a small flexion CR of short duration. However, from Fanardjian, 1961.)
316 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

Some cerebellectomized dogs, however, were un- a cerebellar lesion. Additionally, Orbeli noted
able to inhibit the three nonreinforced limbs; the changes in the ratio of chronaxy in antagonistic
CS elicited flexion responses in all of the limbs. muscles and the breakdown of reciprocal inner-
Fanardjian concluded that an inability to inhibit vation (Sherrington, 1906) in movements elicited
responding in the three nonreinforced limbs was by stimulation of afferent nerves. In addition to
a manifestation of the removal of a specific func- the well recognized role ofthe cerebellum in motor
tion of the cerebellum which normally acted to function, Orbeli viewed the cerebellum as an
suppress inappropriate and superfluous move- important regulator of sensory function. He
ments. Such a role for the cerebellum was seen by referred to studies of his own, employing external
Fanardjian as enabling an animal's behavior to stimuli, that demonstrated increases in the stimu-
become "specialized" and thus more prepared to lus thresholds for eliciting various unconditioned
meet the demands of the environment. Fanardjian reflexes. He thus corroborated the previous
concluded that: observations of Luciani, Holmes, and Popov
concerning the susceptibility of unconditioned
... insufficiencies of the cerebellum are hard to detect
by observing contraction of one muscle or even a limb,
reflexes to cerebellar damage. Noting that the
since the disorders affect all levels of the central nervous thresholds for eliciting reflexes of cerebellecto-
system, eventually causing changes in space and in mized animals varied greatly from day to day,
time of the delicate and interrelated activity of the Orbeli likened these alterations in sensory func-
motoneurons of the spinal cord. tion to a "sensory ataxia."
Orbeli also noted changes in digestive function
Accordingly, greater deficits following cerebel- following cerebellar extirpation. These changes
lar lesions were more easily observed by taking included retardation of intestinal motility, attenua-
simultaneous measures of multiple behaviors and tion in secretory activity of organs of digestion
determining changes in their relation with one to pharmacological agents, and changes in pan-
another. Fanardjian recognized that deficiencies creatic function to glycemicchallenge. Alterations
of cerebellar function and the centripetal effects in basal metabolism, delays in thermoregulation,
of cerebellar lesions on the rest of the central and delays in the initiation of baroreceptor re-
nervous system were difficult to distinguish from
flexes to changes in blood pressure were other
one another on the behavioral level. Moreover,
changes in autonomic function noted after cere-
abnormal function of spinal motor neurons could bellar extirpation. Orbeli concluded that the
not be discounted as an important factor causing cerebellum had an adaptive and trophic influence
irregular conditioned limb movement following
over all bodily tissues. The outstanding question
cerebellar damage.
for Orbeli was the mechanism whereby the cere-
bellum exerted its trophic function on the sensory
Orbeli
and motor systems. Orbeli proposed that the
New notions of cerebellar function were intro- change in autonomic function following cerebel-
duced by the Russian physiologist Orbeli (1940). lar extirpation was a consequence of widespread
Summarizing 12 years of work (1928-1940), alterations in the "vegetative" function of the
Orbeli confirmed the classic findings of Luciani entire body. Generalized alterations in vegetative
and extended the role of the cerebellum into the function were considered to be the primary etio-
realms of sensory and autonomic function. Re- logy of the decrements in excitability of the central
garding motor function, Orbeli noted changes in nervous system following cerebellar extirpation.
the excitability of motor nerves and muscles fol-
lowing cerebellar damage. Orbeli noted that the
Livshits
time required for excit'ation of motor nerves and
muscles was often extended after cerebellar Orbeli's ideas served as the basis for a series of
damage. Changes in chronaxy of muscles and investigations by Livshits (1947) and Krasusky
motor nerves were not isomorphic so that the (1957) on cerebellar involvement in salivary CRs.
ratio of the excitability of the muscle and the It was believed that conditioned salivation pro-
nerve deviated from normal in the animal with vided a better measure of the role of the cerebel-
16. Role of the Cerebellum 317

lum in the formation of associations because basal Gambaryan and Fanardjian, clearly indicated
salivary activity was far less disrupted than the that the length of the postoperative period is an
skeletal motor system following cerebellar damage. important variable in assessing the effects of cere-
Livshits (1947) provided a detailed account of bellar extirpation on conditioned reflexes.
two dogs, Barbos and Kulak, whose conditioned In the second dog (Kulak), profuse conditioned
salivary activity was measured before and after salivary responding was observed before surgery
complete cerebellar removal. CSs that preceded to each of the CSs paired with presentation of
the presentation offood were a metronome beat- food (Fig. 16.8A). The entire cerebellum was
ing 100 times per minute, a bell, a tone, and a removed except for a portion of the posterior
light. The CSs preceded the presentation of the vermis and a small portion of the left hemisphere.
food by 30 s. Before the cerebellar lesion, Barbos As in the case of Barbos, Kulak demonstrated a
normally salivated from two to five drops to the virtual absence of conditioned salivary respond-
metronome, tone, and bell CSs (Fig. 16.7). Very ing to each of the CSs when tested on the tenth
little responding occurred to the light. Later, the day after cerebellar extirpation (Fig. 16.8A). How-
entire posterior vermis, most of the hemispheres, ever, some recovery of conditioned salivation
and all of the deep nuclei were removed. The only was observed by the 25th day after the lesion
remaining cerebellar tissue after the surgery was (Fig. 16.8A). Despite the absence of the cerebel-
the flocculus and portions of the anterior lobe. lum, food deprivation, which would be expected
Ten days after surgery, there was no salivary to increase the response-evoking properties of
responding to the metronome, bell, or light, and the CS, restored conditioned salivation to pre-
a mean level of responding to the tone of only 0.5 operative levels for all stimuli except the tone
drop. However, 5 months after the lesion, Barbos' (Fig. 16.8B; see legend for details). The restoration
salivary responding approached its preoperative of conditioned salivation produced by food
norm. These data, along with the observations of deprivation was not due to an increase in the
baseline level of salivation because responding to
conditioned inhibitory stimuli (CS-) remained
BARBaS absent. This was in contrast to Kulak's behavior
EFFECT OF CEREBELLAR EXTIRPATION before the lesion when food rationing enhanced
6 6 responding to both conditioned excitatory and
inhibitory stimuli.
5 5
v
VI
4 4 VI Krasusky
a. v a.
o v ~ The effect of cerebellar lesions on conditioned
0::
o
3
v 3
o
v salivation was further examined by Krasusky
2 v 2
v (1957). This study employed nine dogs that had
v
v acquired salivary CRs before cerebellectomy. In

o
v
v
M T L 8
n
MT L B
tl
MT L B
o
the immediate postoperative period, conditioned
salivary activity of every dog was significantly
PREOP 10 DAYS 5 MONTHS
depressed. Some dogs remained depressed at this
6/10-13/33 4/5/34 9/1-23/34
postoperative level for many months. Krasusky
measured conditioned salivation in these dogs
Figure 16.7. Effect of cerebellar damage on conditioned for up to 35 months after the surgery. The CRs
salivation of the dog Barbos. Ordinate presents the
of every dog showed some recovery from their
mean number of drops of saliva to four different CSs:
M, metronome beating 100 times per sec; T, tone; L,
immediate postoperative levels. However, the rate
light; B, bell. Preoperative data were taken from experi- of recovery was highly variable among the nine
ments 124 and 126, June 1933. Postoperative data were dogs. At the time of each dog's sacrifice, four re-
taken from experiment 193, April 1934 (10 days), and sponded at levels below their preoperative norm,
experiments 274 and 278, September 1934 (5 months). two regained their preoperative level, and two
See text for details. (Data from Livshits, 1947.) exceeded their preoperative level of performance.
318 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

A B
16 16

14 14

12 12
10 III
a.. 10
III
a..
0 0
a:: 8 8 a::
0 0
6 6

4 4

2 2
0 0
t.4TLB TLB
t.4 t.4TLB TLB
t.4 TLB
t.4 t.4TLB
PREOP 10 DAYS 25 DAYS PRE FAST 3/4 FAST FAST
6/19/3" 7/3/3" 7/11-18/3" 2/10/35 2/11/35 2/15/35

Figure 16.8. Data presented and abbreviations as in fourth of normal (~ FAST) enhanced conditioned
Figure 16.7. A: Effect of cerebellar extirpation on con- salivation (data presented as means of Experiment
ditioned salivation of the dog Kulak. Preoperative 474). Total food deprivation for 40 hours (FAST)
data were taken from Experiment 380, June 1934. Post- restored conditioned salivation to preoperative levels
operative data were taken from Experiment 381 (10 for each of the stimuli except the tone (data from
days) and Experiments 388-394 (25 days), July 1934. Experiment 477). Fasting did not enhance responding
See text for details. B: Effect of food deprivation on to conditioned inhibitory stimuli (data not shown),
conditioned salivation in the cerebellectomized dog, suggesting that an increase in baseline responding did
Kulak. Before fasting (PRE FAST), responding to each not contribute to the increases in salivation elicited by
of the CSs was quite low (data presented as means from the positive CSs. (Data from Livshits, 1947.)
Experiment 473). Restriction of Kulak's food to one

Averages offive dogs whose conditioned salivary ing the first minute, increased during the second
activity was measured up to 15 months after the minute, and reached a maximum in the third
surgery are presented in Figure 16.9. This figure minute.
shows that conditioned salivary activity was re- The characteristics of the responses following
duced to 30% of its preoperative level in the cerebellar damage, an absolute reduction in sali-
month following cerebellar lesions. However, over vation immediately after the surgery and a reduc-
the course of 15 months, conditioned salivation tion ofthe magnitude of salivation at early relative
returned to a level within the range of preoperative to late latencies during the CS, signified to
values. Dogs with cerebellar lesions demonstrated Krasusky that the animals were demonstrating a
changes in the pattern of salivation to CSs of ex- generalized attenuation in the excitability of the
tended duration. Before surgery, each dog sali- nervous system.
vated in a characteristic way to a CS of 3 minutes'
duration. For instance, the magnitude of saliva-
Karamyan
tion could be the same for each minute of the CS,
the magnitude of responding could be maximal Because of his interest in the theory of brain
in the first minute and decrease to a minimum in evolution proposed by Sechenov (1852, cited in
the third, or relatively low during the first minute Karamyan, 1959), Karamyan carried out an ex-
and increase to a maximum in the third. For all tensive series of studies to examine the evolution
dogs tested in this manner at 20 to 29 months of cerebellar function. He did so by examining
after the surgery, the pattern of salivation was the effects of cerebellectomy on motor functions,
changed so that salivation was relatively low dur- including CRs, in fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds,
16. Role of the Cerebellum 319

140 another but were readily acquired each day after


7 to 10 pairings. Removal of the cerebellum in
120 carp and goldfish prevented acquisition of condi-

1
LJ
tioned defensive responses even after 20 days of
100 conditioning. Partial lesions of the fish cerebellum
en
a.. • produced a wide range of effects. All fish

I ]1--./1 i 1
0 80 demonstrated profound reductions in CRs.
a::: Whereas most retained permanent deficits in CRs,
0 60
some reacquired their preoperative level of per-
.c.o formance 1 month after the lesion. Removal of

20
.iT 1
.I.
the forebrain and optic tectum did not affect CRs
acquired to visual or auditory CSs. Karamyan
et aI. (1969) concluded that:

0123 6 12 15
the fish are the first representatives in the phylogenetic
MONTHS POSTOPERATIVE scale of vertebrates, where a new system of integration,
the cerebello-mesencephalic, is developed, which
Figure 16.9. Effect of cerebellar extirpation on con- provides for a high motor activity composed of both
ditioned salivary responses as a function of post opera- conditioned and nonconditioned components of reflex
tive interval. Data are presented as mean ± standard activity. (p. 640)
error of the mean of the cumulative records of salivary
drops to CSs. Each point represents the mean of the
five dogs named Melin, Smirny, Sery, Tourist, and Of the class amphibia, frogs and toads exhibited
Naida. See text for details. (Data from Krasusky, 1957.) changes in tone and posture at rest and during
swimming, abolition of rhythmic motion during
swimming, and extensor rigidity after removal of
the cerebellum. Defensive CRs of the hind leg
were established by pairing a bell or light CS with
rodents, and cats (Karamyan, 1959, 1962, 1968; a shock UCS to the hind leg. Normal frpgs did
Karamyan et aI., 1969). not attain stable levels of conditioning. Toads
Severe motor disorders were noted in dogfish were found to be better than frogs and could
after extirpation of the cerebellum: disturbance acquire stable CRs over 20 days oft raining. Cere-
of gill movement, attenuated reaction to pin prick, bellectomy transiently abolished the CRs of toads
erratic swimming movements, circling toward the but they were reacquired on the third day after
side of the lesion. The disorders were less severe the operation. Karamyan concluded that the
in skates and consisted of increases in motor acquisition of CRs in amphibians was not asso-
activity and changes in posture. Karamyan com- ciated with the cerebellum.
pared these effects in the chondrichthyes (dogfish Of the class aves, Karamyan studied pigeons
and skate) with removal of the cerebellum in and poultry. Confirming the findings of Flourens,
in osteichthyes fish (goldfish, pike, carp, and cerebellectomized pigeons exhibited profound
perch). Lesions in the latter class resulted in many alterations in motor function characterized by
sensory-motor disorders that varied with the swaying of the head, nystagmus, agitated behav-
species studied. Reduction in pain sensitivity and ior, retraction of the head, increase in extensor
disorganized swimming behavior were noted in tonus during movements, and reduction of muscle
all species. Perch and pike were most susceptible tone at rest. Abduction of the limbs during walk-
to cerebellar damage, lying on the surface with ing, astasia of the body, and undirected flying
arched back, and swimming upside-down in an additionally characterized pigeons with partial
uncoordinated manner. Intact carp developed cerebellar extirpation. Cerebellar damage did not
defensive CRs to visual and auditory CSs paired profoundly alter the motor behavior of ducks
with electric shock to the tail as the UCS. Defen- and roosters as compared to pigeons. Cerebellar
sive CRs were not retained from one day to extirpation caused alterations in tone of the neck
320 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

A A

f ",II" II ",tl ,in" II "",,"'_'M~"hI"" hi.......,........

Figure 16.10. Effect of cerebellectomy on


cardiac and respiratory conditioned reflexes
J in the pigeon. A: Top three tracings present:
14.7 1) EKG with the mark of the electric shock
UCS superimposed on it, 2) pneumographic
Iltll' ifni IIl""'" " . i 11th' I f1 h ,If" i iti' rli iii i Iii i i rr i r-rt\, i nhli C" recording of respiration, and 3) time marker
in seconds with CS onset indicated by inter-
ruption oftime marks. Note that onset of the
CS elicits a marked tachycardia and increase
B 5 in respiration. B: Bottom three tracings de-
monstrate that a stimulus (CS-) not paired
~
with the UCS fails to elicit any changes in
heart rate or respiration. (Reprinted, with
Ju permission, from Karamyan, 1959.)

A A

2 .!.l.LlJj , ] I I II I , I 1l.J I , I ( J I ( I I I I Ie l , I LJ ; i.' I !J ill 111 '''':' I

B 6

i l l I ,! ! ( I !J , II II II I I L I I II I ] I I ! I: I ! I , ! I,I : I , l'lJl] III

Figu.re 16.11. The effect of cerebellectomy on con- ditioned reduction in respiration is elicited by the posi-
ditioned respiratory reflexes of the cat. A: The top three tive CS. B: A stimulus (CS-) that was never paired with
tracings represent: I) plethysmographic recording of shock also elicits a respiratory CR indicating an inability
respiration, 2) EKG with mark of the electric shock to inhibit responding after cerebellectomy. (Reprinted,
UCS superimposed on it, 3) time base in seconds with with permission, from Karamyan, 1959.)
CS presentation superimposed on it. Note that a con-
[6. Role of the Cerebellum 321

and limb muscles. In contrast to the motor agita- cats manifested a distinct increase in excitability
tion observed in pigeons, roosters and ducks as demonstrated by enhanced responding to both
became unnaturally quiet after cerebellar extirpa- excitatory and inhibitory CSs. By the third month
tion. Cerebellectomy weakened but did not after cerebellar ablation, positive CRs regained
abolish respiratory and cardiac CRs in pigeons their preoperative level of performance whereas
(Fig. 16.10). inhibition of CRs to a CS - did not (Fig. 16.11).
In mammals, Karamyan removed the entire Similar results were obtained by pairing a metro-
:::erebellum in rats and cats. Deficits in rats after nome CS with the presentation of ammonia vapor
removal of the cerebellum were extensor rigidity to the nose as the DCS (Fig. 16.12). In this para-
during walking and atonia of the neck and limb digm, an apnea CR was acquired to the CS after
muscles during rest. The deficits were greater and 30 pairings whereas stable differentiation could
more enduring in the hind legs as compared not readily be acquired in cerebellectomized cats.
to the forelegs. Behavioral compensation was
characterized by prominent astasia and ataxia.
Summary
Increased sensitivity to pain and tactile stimuli
characterized the cerebellectomized rat. Removal This section has presented the results obtained
of the cerebellum in cats weakened withdrawal by seven investigators from 1929 to 1962 using
reflexes and enhanced scratching and licking the Pavlovian method. The effects of cerebel-
reflexes. Cerebellectomized cats extended their lectomy that they noted through observation
limb and tail more forcefully to a hammer tap of organisms ranging from fish to mammals were
than normals. Removal of the cerebellum induced in essential agreement with those reported by
more severe and long-lasting deficits in CRs of Western investigators. More importantly, all
cats as compared to the birds. For 1 month after seven investigators were agreed that cerebellar
extirpation of the cerebellum, cats demonstrated damage did not abolish an animal's retention of
highly variable levels of CRs to both excitatory conditioned reflexes nor did it prevent an animal
CSs paired with an electric shock and inhibitory from acquiring conditioned reflexes. These results
CSs. During the second month after the surgery, were based on studies employing a wide range of

A

======================r===~~~==J
________________~________________________~~~.~t______
.~t~__ •

dill.
------------------~~~--------------~---------
« • ,t"" 4 t' t t t t , • , , • t t , , t
, . f n tIt' I , "f I • t t, '" r t ' t I • t

Figure 16.12. Conditioned respiratory reflexes of an 2) mark of the CS, 3) mark of the VCS, 4) time in
acerebellarcat acquired to a metronome stimulus paired seconds. B: Apnea during CS - indicates failure of
with ammonia vapor to the nose. A: Conditioned re- differentiation after cerebellectomy. (Reprinted with
duction in breathing to a positive CS. From top to permission from Karamyan, 1962.)
bottom: 1) pneumographic recording of respiration,
322 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

species (toads, pigeons, cats, and dogs) and a wide been well documented by Holmes in his studies
variety of conditioned reflexes (leg flexion, body on voluntary movements.
withdrawal, salivation, cardiac, and respiratory).
Cerebellar lesions did impair the ability of animals
to perform CRs appropriately or with the same Learning, Memory,
frequency and vigor as they had preoperatively. and the Cerebellum
However, many of these performance deficits Types of Learning
gradually abated with time after surgery. The
and Memory
effects of cerebellectomy were the same whether
the conditioned reflex was established by the strict J. Hughlings Jackson proposed that higher mental
Pavlovian method or by the avoidance method, functions are not indivisible abilities but are
which allows for voluntary responses. Finally, as composed of more basic skills. The ability to
would be anticipated from Western studies, both learn, for example, depends not on a "learning
instrumental responses and conditioned reflexes center" but rather a combination of interrelated
were more vulnerable to the effects of cerebellar functions. Depending on the learning task, these
damage than were unconditioned reflexes. How- functions could include some combination of
ever, these Russian studies suggested that this visual, auditory, and/or somesthetic perceptions
increased vulnerability might be more apparent and their interactions, fine motor coordination,
than real. spatial localization, and kinesthetic-proprio-
First, there was some evidence of deficits in ceptive feedback. Thus, learning or memory and
unconditioned skeletal defensive reflexes and in their expression in behavior require the co-
autonomic reflexes. This suggested that the per- ordinated activity of sensory systems, associa-
formance deficits in the CRs may have been due, tion cortex, motor cortex as well as the cere-
in part, to a deficit in the final pathway through bellar and extrapyramidal systems that allow for
which both the CRs and UCRs were expressed. the proper integration and modulation of motor
Unfortunately, the Russian studies never closely responses. Impairment of any of these functions
examined this possibility. However, the similar could either interfere directly with the occurrence
effects of cerebellar lesions on unconditioned and of learning and memory or impair the optimal
conditioned leg flexion noted by Popov and performance of what had been learned. This point
Fanardjian strongly supported this view. of view is consistent with current evidence ob-
Another possibility raised by the Russian tained from both clinical and experimental studies
studies was that the CS employed to elicit the indicating that learning and memory are not
CR was not strong enough to overcome the unitary phenomena. There are many different
decrease in excitability resulting from cere- forms of learning and many different types of
bellectomy. Food deprivation is known to in- memory (Squire, 1987). Presumably different
crease the strength of the unconditioned as well forms of learning and memory occur because
as the conditioned salivary reflex. The finding by differences in the nature of the tasks required
Livshits (1947) that food deprivation in a cere- of an organism result in a different degree of
bellectomized dog returned the salivary CR to involvement of various neural systems. Conse-
normal strongly suggested that deprivation had quently, damage to any particular brain region
increased the response-evoking properties of the can leave some forms of learning and memory
CS sufficiently to overcome the effects ofthe cere- intact while apparently eliminating others.
bellar lesion. This interpretation is strengthened Recent studies in humans and other primates
by the observation that initiation of salivary CRs has led to the suggestion that there may be two
was delayed in cerebellectomized dogs (Krasusky, broad categories oflearning and memory: decla-
1957). In short, the Russian studies suggested that rative and procedural (Squire, 1987). Declarative
cerebellectomy had increased the time taken for or explicit memory allows for knowledge about
the onset of a response, as well as the frequency the world around us, and is available to conscious
and amplitude of the response-effects that had recollection. Remembering a person's name or
16. Role of the Cerebellum 323

face would be an example of declarative memory. Nevertheless, beginning with the publication
Procedural or implicit memory allows for knowl- of a theoretical paper by Marr (1969), there have
edge of "how," and current thinking (Squire, 1987) been increasing speculations that some forms of
is that it would include learned motor skills motor learning such as the vestibular ocular reflex
acquired through instrumental or Pavlovian might be acquired and stored in the cerebellar
conditioning, cognitive skills (e.g., remembering cortex (see Ito, 1984). However, there is no direct
the rules for addition), and the phenomenon of evidence that the theoretical speculations (e.g.,
priming (the improvement of memory through Albus, 1971; Houk et aI., 1990; Moore and Blazis,
the prior presentation of certain stimuli). 1989) or the results of in vitro studies (e.g.,
Human amnesia has been linked with damage Sakurai, 1989) are relevant to the in vivo situa-
to the medial temporal region and to the dien- tion. On the contrary, recent findings suggest
cephalic midline. However, the amnesia that is that the locus of learning and memory for the
associated with such damage is only for declara- vestibular ocular reflex (Lisberger, 1988; Weiser
tion memory and not for procedural memory. et aI., 1989), for other forms of vestibular com-
Since the loci of procedural memory are not pensation (Llinas and Walton, 1979), and for
known, there has been a great deal of speculation long-term habituation (Lopiano et aI., 1989) is
concerning the possible neural systems involved within the brain stem.
(Squire, 1987). A recent study of patients with A renewed interest in the involvement of the
Huntington's or Parkinson's disease has led one cerebellum in learning and memory was gener-
group of investigators to suggest that motor skill ated when McCormick etai. (1981) reported that
learning may be mediated by a corticostriatal Pavlovian conditioning of the nictitating mem-
system (Heindel et aI., 1989). brane reflex in the rabbit was impaired by
An older line of research has focused on the hemicerebellectomy. Given the rich history of
cerebellum as the site of motor learning. Lashley Pavlovian conditioning studies on cerebellar
and McCarthy (1926) provided one ofthe earliest involvement in the performance of conditioned
experimental studies on the role of the cerebellum reflexes reviewed above, the susceptibility of yet
in learning and memory. Using maze learning, another conditioned reflex to cerebellar damage
they found that cerebellar damage did not affect was not unexpected. What was unexpected was
the ability to remember the correct sequence of the subsequent claim that learning and memory
responses in a maze. Their report on one rat with underlying the conditioned nictitating membrane
a large cerebellar lesion is worth quoting: response (NMR) was located in the cerebellum
(e.g., Thompson, 1986).
Number 4. Adult female. The performance in initial
learning was: Time, 2,388 seconds; Errors, 33; Trials, Cerebellar Regulation
35. Preliminary retention tests gave errors in only first of the Conditioned N M R
two trials. Time, 229 seconds; Errors, 7; Trials 12. After
operation there was extreme contraction of the left The unique aspect of the deficits in nictitating
hind leg and extension of the right. In walking there membrane CRs following hemicerebellectomy
was rotation to the left and frequent backward was that the CRs appeared to be abolished and
somersaulting. When picked up the animal clutched could not be reacquired (McCormick et aI., 1981).
wildly at anything within reach and gave indications Furthermore, this effect could be reproduced by
of vertigo. For the first days she was force-fed, then relatively small lesions in the area of the anterior
began to eat spontaneously but showed little improve-
aspects of the interpositus nucleus with no
ment in symptoms. Thirty days after operation the
obvious sensory or motor dysfunction in the reflex
motor syml'toms were still pronounced. She walked as
if drawing a heavy weight, with fore and hind leg pathway of the UCR or in the animal in general
extended forward and dragging her along in a series of (McCormick and Thompson, 1984; Yeo et aI.,
lunges. At this time she was tested in the maze and 1985). Unilateral lesions in the interpositus
made a perfect retention record: time, 801 seconds; errors, nucleus abolished CRs of the nictitating mem-
0; trials 10. (emphasis added) brane on the side ipsilateral to the lesion without
324 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

affecting CRs of the contralateral side. These test trials. Animals were first trained to post-
effects could be obtained with either visual or asymptotic levels of responding (greater than 90%
auditory CSs. CRs) and then tested for CR retention 3 weeks
In the final analysis, the conclusion that the after surgery.
interpositus nucleus was the locus of memory In agreement with Yeo et aI., (1985), four groups
essential for learning and retention of the condi- of animals could be distinguished during postop-
tioned NMR was based on two arguable pro- erative testing on the basis of the nonoverlapping
positions: the assumption that deficits specific to behavioral effects as measured by the frequency
learned responses indicated a role of the damaged ofCRs occurring during the CS-UCS interval of
area in learning or memory, and the belief that conditioning trials. Also, in agreement with Mc-
learning and memory were localized in discrete Cormick and Thompson (1984) and Yeo et al.
brain areas (see Thompson, 1986). More recently, (1985), one group of 15 animals with unilateral
Welsh and Harvey (1989a) reexamined the effects cerebellar lesions that included the dorsolateral
of electrolytic lesions in the anterior aspects of aspects of the anterior interpositus nucleus dem-
the interpositus nucleus on Pavlovian condition- onstrated abolition of ipsilateral CRs during
ing of the rabbit's NMR. The results of this and conditioning trials. A second group of 47 animals
subsequent studies employing intracerebellar whose lesions never encroached on the anterior
lidocaine to produce reversible lesions (Welsh interpositus nucleus showed no retention deficits
and Harvey, 1989b, 1991) clearly demonstrated: during conditioning trials. There were two inter-
that the previous findings summarized by mediate groups that demonstrated substantial
Thompson (1986) were due to performance de- decrease in CRs on the first retention day. One
ficits and not to effects on learning and memory. group of 14 animals recovered to normal levels,
These studies will now be described in greater while another group of 6 animals did not.
detail. The use oftest trials allowed Welsh and Harvey
In the first study (Welsh and Harvey, 1989a), (1989a) to obtain a precise measurement of CR
conditioning of the rabbit's NMR was accom- topography and this in turn revealed that cere-
plished using standard procedures. Each condi- bellar lesions did not simply abolish the occur-
tioned trial consisted of the paired presentation rence of CRs, but rather induced a continuum of
of a tone and an air-puff UCS. The UCS was de- deficits in response frequency, latency, rise time,
livered to the right cornea 285 ms after CS onset. and amplitude. These deficits all acted to reduce
In accordance with common practice, a response the probability of observing CRs within the limit-
was defined as at least 0.5 mm of nictitating ed windows of observation employed during con-
membrane extension and was scored as a UCR ditioning trials. Thus, 11 of the 15 animals that
if it occurred after UCS onset and as a CR if it had demonstrated abolition of CRs during paired
occurred within the 285 ms between CS and UCS CS-UCS trials revealed a substantial number of
onsets. It should be noted that conditioning trials CRs when given the opportunity to respond on
do not allow one to observe any CRs that might the tone-alone test trials. No differences could be
have occurred with latencies longer than the CS- detected between the placements oflesions in the
UCS interval of 285 ms, since the UCR elicited 11 animals that did demonstrate CRs during test
by the UCS would mask their presence. Con- trials and the 4 that did not.
cerned with the possibility that animals with Figure 16.13 presents topographical represen-
cerebellar damage might have problems in the tations of CRs of the four groups of lesioned
initiation ofCRs, Welsh and Harvey (1989a) used animals and a group of normal controls during
test trials that were interspersed among the test trials when no UCS was presented. This figure
conditioning trials. During a test trial, the tone shows that the CR of normal animals is uniphasic,
CS was presented to the animal as before, but the reaching a peak amplitude of approximately 5 mm
UCS was omitted. This allowed for the observa- of membrane extension at 350ms after CS onset.
tion of responses having latencies in excess of The 47 animals that showed a good retention of
285 ms. Thus, the performance of each animal CRs postoperatively also demonstrated normal
could be compared between conditioning and CR topopraphies during test trials, maintaining
16. Role of the Cerebellum 325

----- Normal (n = 82)


5

r
----- No - Deficit (n = 47)
Recovered (n = 14)
4
Impaired (n = 6)

3 Dr·metric (n =15)
E
E
2

o 250 500 1000 1500

msec
Figure 16.13. The effects of unilateral electrolytic lesions positus nucleus while the anterior aspect of the inter-
in the region of the deep cerebellar nuclei on the topo- positus nucleus of virtually all rabbits of the no-deficit
graphy of ipsilateral conditioned NMR of rabbits. Each group remained intact. The topography of the CR of
curve represents the amplitude of nictitating membrane normal rabbits represents the average of all rabbits in
extension elicited by a 250-ms tone CS for 1800-ms the lesion groups on the day before cerebellar lesions.
after tone onset on trials in which the tone was presented The vertical line at 285 ms after tone onset represents
alone. Rabbits of the impaired and dysmetric groups the time that the DCS would have been delivered on
had equivalent amounts of damage to the interpositus conditioning trials. Compare the topography of nicti-
nucleus with damage consistently sustained to the tating membrane responses ofthe normal and dysmetric
dorsolateral aspects ofthe anterior interpositus nucleus rabbits to the topography of the hand grasps in the
and adjacent white matter. Rabbits of the recovered human with cerebellar damage in Figure 16.1.
group on average sustained less damage to the inter-

normal amplitudes and rise times (no-deficit cerebellar lesions continued to display deficits in
group of Fig. 16.13). However, 11 of the 15 ani- CR amplitude (recovered group of Fig. 16.13). It
mals that showed a total abolition of CRs during should be noted that the changes in response
paired trials demonstrated dysmetric CRs during topography demonstrated by the dysmetric group
test trials that were characterized by low ampli- are quite similar to those obtained by Holmes in
tudes, longer onset latencies, and increased rise a human patient with unilateral damage to the
times (dysmetric group of Fig. 16.13). Two groups cerebellum (compare Figs. 16.1 and 16.13).
demonstrated intermediate effects of cerebellar In each of the groups affected by cerebellar
lesions on CR topography. The six animals that lesions, tone-alone trials revealed greater levels
demonstrated impaired CR frequency and did of responding than was observed on conditioning
not recover (impaired group of Fig. 16.13) also trials. The reason for this can be more easily seen
demonstrated CRs that were as deficient in ampli- in Figure 16.14, which presents in greater detail
tude as the dysmetric group but retained normal CR topography during the 285 ms after CS onset.
onset.latency and rise time. The group of 14 This time period is identical to the interval be-
animals that recovered normal CR frequency after tween CS and UCS onsets employed on condi-
326 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

4 ,-
L
4
3.5 ,-
,- 3.5
3 /
3
/
2.5 / 2.5
E 3
E 2 2 3
1.5 1.5
1
0.5 0.5
0 0

0 50 100 150 200 250


msec
Figure 16.14. A detailed view of conditioned nictitating and UCS onsets employed on conditioning trials. Verti-
membrane extension on tone-alone trials for 285 ms cal marks extending downward from each curve
after tone onset. Each curve represents the average represent the time at which 0.06 mm of membrane
topography of the CRs of normal rabbits and the four extension of the average topography was attained. This
lesion groups depicted in Figure 16.12. The 285-ms measure represented the best estimate of the time of
time period corresponds to the interval between CS response onset.

tioning trials. Accordingly, the amplitude of each in the no-deficit and recovered groups because
response at 285 ms after tone onset represents the more than 98% of these responses exceeded the
extent to which one could observe CR in each of 0.5 mm criterion by the time the UCS was pres-
the lesion groups during paired CS-UCS condi- ented. However, greater responding was detected
tioning trials. The remainder of the CR could not on tone-alone trials in the impaired and dysmetric
be observed on conditioning trials because the groups because a significant percentage of re-
UCR elicited by the corneal air-puff obscured its sponses did not reach the amplitude criterion by
measurement. the time the corneal air puff was presented on
Membrane extension of normal rabbits and conditioning trials. This was most dramatically
lesioned rabbits in the no-deficit group reached demonstrated by the dysmetric group, in which
approximately 4 mm of extension by 285 ms after less than 4% of the responses reached the criterion
tone onset. The response of the recovered group in the limited 285-ms window of observation used
reached, on average, 2.5 mm of membrane exten- during CS-UCS pairings on conditioning trials,
sion by 285 ms. Membrane extension for the as compared with an average of 21 % CRs during
impaired and dysmetric groups at 285 ms only test trials. Thus, the combined effects of a reduc-
reached 0.75 and 0.25 mm, respectively (Fig. 16.14). tion in amplitude and an increase in rise time
It is important to note that the definition of a made it possible to overestimate the effect of cere-
response uniformly employed among researchers bellar lesions on CR frequency when only limited
studying nictitating membrane conditioning is a windows of observation were employed. This is
0.5-mm or greater extension of the nictitating so because cerebellar lesions brought conditioned
membrane. Accordingly, only a small difference membrane extensions closer to the threshold of
in CR frequency (less than 2%) was observed exclusion from the response category. For a more
between conditioning and tone-alone test trials detailed analysis of response topography and
16. Role of the Cerebellum 327

VIII
CS" CN

CR
UCR

Figure 16.15. Schematic illustration of current views interpositus nucleus; PC, Purkinje cell; PN, pontine
concerning the pathways of the conditioned and un- nuclei; PO, pars oralis ofthe trigeminal sensory complex;
conditioned NMR reflex (filled arrows) and the path- RFJI>H, the preblink area of the reticular formation;
ways through which the cerebellum may modulate RN, red nucleus. Note that there is a direct projection
these reflexes. V, VI, and VIII, 5th, 6th, and 8th cranial from pars oralis to both IP and GC as well as the
nerves, respectively; ACC, accessory abducens nucleus; projection from 10 via climbing fibers. There is no
CN, cochlear nucleus; GC, granule cell of cerebellar evidence for projections from PN to IP.
cortex; IC, inferior colliculus; 10, inferior olive; IP,

kinematics in these animals before and after cere- ducing reflex facilitation as measured by a tone-
bellar lesions see Welsh (1992). induced increase in the amplitude of the UCR
(Harvey et ai., 1985). The filled arrows in Figure
16.15 represent a simplified diagram of the CS
Cerebellar Regulation
and UCS projections onto a preblink area located
of the Unconditioned N M R
in the reticular formation (RF/PB). The RF/PB
One of the advantages of the NMR is that the might be the point at which the CS can activate
CR and UCR are expressed through a common the motor system ofthe UCR (Harvey et ai., 1984;
final pathway. Thus, a tone CS and air-puff Holstege et ai., 1986).
UCS are capable of activating retractor bulbi Acquisition of CRs to a tone CS appears to
motor neurons located in the accessory abducens involve synaptic facilitation that converts a pre-
nucleus (Gray et ai., 1981; Harvey et ai., 1984; viously weak, subliminal stimulus into one that
Holstege et ai., 1986). Activation of the accessory can recruit a sufficient number of retractor bulbi
abducens nucleus leads, sequentially, to contrac- motor neurons to elicit an overt response. As
tion of the retractor bulbi muscle via the Vlth learning proceeds, the CS acquires the ability to
cranial nerve, retraction of the eyeball into the elicit CRs having higher amplitudes and shorter
orbit, and extension of the nictitating membrane onset latencies. These CRs can thus appear to be
(Prince, 1964). Retraction of the eyeball is the as robust as the UCR. However, even after ex-
necessary and sufficient condition for occurrence tended training, the response-evoking properties
of the NMR when tactual stimulation of the cornea of the CS always remain less than that of the
serves as the UCS (Marek et ai., 1984). The audi- UCS, whose intensity in conditioning studies
tory system is prewired into the unconditioned (approximately 2.2 kg/cm 2 source pressure) is su-
nictitating membrane reflex. Although auditory pramaximal, approximately 25 times greater than
stimuli cannot evoke an overt response before threshold. In this regard, even after 16 days of
learning, they do activate motor elements of this conditioning at optimal stimulus parameters, the
reflex (Berthier and Moore, 1983), thereby pro- force of the NMRs elicited by a tone CS is only
328 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

equivalent to the force of a UCR elicited by a dysmetric rabbits including those that failed to
corneal air puff of 0.55 kgjcm 2 , an intensity that demonstrate CRs during either conditioning or
is one quarter that employed in conditioning test trials also demonstrated impaired UCRs as
studies (Welsh, 1992). These data suggest that, at compared with controls. These deficits were most
the level of the motor substrate for NMRs, a prominent at low UCS intensities (Fig. 16.16).
well conditioned tone has only one quarter the The UCRs of normal rabbits, indicated by the
response-engendering power ofthe UCS employed dotted lines, progressively increased in amplitude
in conditioning studies. as UCS intensity was systematically increased
Previous claims that cerebellar lesions impair from 0.09 to 2.93 kgjcm 2 . The dysmetric animals
the ability of the CS to evoke CRs with no impair- (solid lines) also demonstrated an increase in am-
ment of the ability of the UCS to elicit UCRs plitude and rise time with increasing UCS inten-
have assumed that the response-evoking proper- sity, but their UCRs were consistently lower in
ties of CS and UCS are equivalent. In order to amplitude and had longer rise times as compared
test the hypothesis that CRs were being differen- with the UCRs of normal rabbits at equal UCS
tially affected because the CS was a weaker sti- intensities. At the lowest UCS intensity, dysmetric
mulus than the UCS, Welsh and Harvey (1989a) rabbits demonstrated deficits in the UCR that
measured the UCRs elicited across a 30-fold range approximated those of the CR.
of air-puffUCS intensities. This procedure clearly Welsh and Harvey (1989b) reexamined the
indicated that the deficits observed in CRs follow- equivalent effect of anterior interpositus lesions
ing lesions of the area of the interpositus nucleus on the regulation of CRs elicited by a tone CS
were not specific to the learned response. Thus, and UCRs elicited by a low intensity UCS. This

UNCONDITIONED RESPONSE

5 Normol

4
/
Cerebellar
.3
E
E 2

a ~~-----------------------------------

a 250 500 1000 1500


msec
Figure 16.16. Interpositus lesions alter the topography sities. The subtle differences in topography between
of unconditioned NMRs especially at low intensities normal and interpositus-lesioned rabbits become ex-
of corneal air puff. Each dotted line represents the aggerated as the intensity of the air puff is lowered so
average topography of the UCR of normal rabbits for that significant changes in topography are easily noticed
nine intensities of corneal air puff. Increasing the air- at the lowest intensities. Compare the topography of
puff intensity increases the unconditioned NMR but UCRs elicited by the lowest intensities of air puff in
does not affect the rise time. Each solid line represents this figure with the topography of the CRs of the
the average topography of UCRs of interpositus dysmetric group in Figure 16.13.
lesioned rabbits across the same range of air-puffinten-
16. Role of the Cerebellum 329

was accomplished through the use of reversible the previous studies of Welsh and Harvey (1989b)
lesions produced by microinjections of the local demonstrated that lidocaine infused in the region
anesthetic lidocaine into the region of the anterior of the interpositus nucleus could reversibly
interpositus nucleus. Lidocaine injections revers- abolish CRs. Second, it is well known that pre-
ibly abolished CRs acquired to a 100-dB tone venting response occurrence in a reversible fashion,
CS and at the same time significantly impaired by pharmacological or physiological means, does
the UCRs elicited by a low intensity UCS (0.09 kg/ not prevent learning during Pavlovian condition-
cm 2 ). UCRs elicited by this low intensity UCS ing. For example, atropine was used to block
were reduced in frequency and amplitude, and salivation during conditioning ofthe salivary res-
increased in rise time. These effects of lidocaine ponse. When tested later, without atropine, the
on CRs and UCRs were completely reversible. animals produced salivary CRs to the CS, indi-
Because lesions or reversible inactivation of cating that learning had occurred (Crisler, 1930;
the interpositus nucleus produced similar disrup- Finch, 1938). Similarly, crushing the motor nerves
tions of the CR and UCR, one can conclude that to produce paralysis of the leg did not prevent
the cerebellum is important in the regulation of the acquisition ofleg-flexion CRs, since such CRs
both the learned and unlearned reflexes and that could be evoked by the CS after the nerve had
this regulation is most likely on the retractor bulbi regenerated and movement was possible (Beck
motor neurons, which form a final pathway for and Doty, 1957; Kellogg et aI., 1940; Light and
both responses. This hypothesis is supported by Gantt, 1936).
the additional fact that all NMRs, whether they We therefore examined whether rabbits could
are elicited by visual or auditory CSs (Yeo et aI., acquire CRs during the time that their ability to
1985), whether they are augmented UCRs result- perform CRs was impaired by infusions of lido-
ing from the pairing of two UCSs (Skelton et aI., caine into the interpositus nucleus. If the inter-
1988), or whether they are conditioned under positus nucleus was important for learning, then
Pavlovian (McCormick et aI., 1981) or instru- animals should not acquire CRs during inter-
mental (Polen char et aI., 1985) contingencies, are positus inactivation by lidocaine infusion and
disrupted by interpositus lesions. All of the experi- they should not demonstrate any retention of
ments reviewed above suggest to us that the inter- CRs when tested without lidocaine. On the other
positus lesion has little to do with the disruption hand, if the interpositus was only involved in the
of a specific sensory system or the supposed performance of CRs through an action on the
memorial processes specific to a particular form motor side of the reflex, then animals would be
of conditioning. Reduced excitability of the acces- able to learn even though they would not be able
sory abducens nucleus and/or retractor bulbi to perform the learned response. Consequently,
muscle, however, is consistent with all ofthe data these animals should demonstrate full retention
and could be the etiology of the performance ofCRs when subsequently tested with a normally
deficits of conditioned NMRs following inter- functioning interpositus.
positus lesions. The results from the first animal used in such
a study were reported by Welsh and Harvey at a
conference on the olivocerebellar system in motor
Learning During Inactivation of Interpositus
control held in Turin, Italy, October 1987 and are
While we have concluded that the CR deficits presented in Panels A, B, and C of Figure 16.17.
seen after interpositus lesions were a manifestation This animal was first trained to make nictitating
of a general performance deficit, one could still membrane CRs to a light CS that was paired with
argue that the CR impairments were due to the an air-puffUCS. On a subsequent day the animal
combined effects of a performance and learning demonstrated 88% CRs to the light CS (Panel A,
deficit. For this reason, a final set of experiments open circle above the letter C). The animal was
was undertaken to resolve this issue by examin- then injected with 1 III of lidocaine into the inter-
ing the retention oflearning in the absence of per- positus nucleus at time zero (arrow in Panel A),
formancedeficits (Welsh and Harvey, 1991). Such which resulted in a total abolition of CRs to the
a study was possible because of two factors. First, light CS for a period of 15 min followed by a rapid
330 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

recovery to 100% CRs by 25 min after injection. session and exposed to light-conditioning trials
The animal was then given a second injection of (Panel C, open circle). In the absence oflidocaine,
lidocaine (first arrow of Panel B), which produced the animal gave 100% CRs to the light CS, indi-
an abolition of CRs to the light CS over the next cating that the effects of previous lidocaine injec-
2.5 min (first open circle in Panel B), indicating tions had completely dissipated. More impor-
the reliability of the effects of lidocaine. Begin- tantly, even though lidocaine injected into the
ning 2.5 min after this second injection, the animal interpositus nucleus had blocked the performance
was given a 70-min conditioning session during of CRs during acquisition (Panel B, solid circles),
which a tone CS was paired with the air-puff this animal demonstrated an average of98% CRs
UCS. During this session, 1 pI of lidocaine was to the tone CS during retention (Panel C, solid
injected into the interpositus nucleus every 15 min circles). In short, the interpositus nucleus was not
as indicated by the arrows in Panel B. The rabbit essential for learning. In addition, these results
showed little evidence of CR acquisition to the indicated that inactivating the rostral interpositus
tone CS as CRs never exceeded 11 % within any had not blocked the sensory effects of either the
10-min block of trials during this session (Panel CS or UCS since any such effect would also have
B, solid circles). In order to monitor the degree retarded learning. Therefore, one can conclude
of inactivation of the interpositus nucleus by lido- that the interpositus is neither part of a pathway
caine, light-conditioning trials were interspersed involved in learning nor a site of learning.
with tone-conditioning trials during this 70-min Using a modification of this procedure, Welsh
session. The infusion of lidocaine was effective in and Harvey (1991) have replicated and extended
maintaining a consistent impairment of CRs to this basic finding with the inclusion of controls
the light CS as indicated by the low level of re- receiving infusions of saline and controls for non-
sponding on light-conditioning trials, with overall associative responding. The results of this most
responding to the light CS being 14% (Panel B, recent study confirmed that the interpositus is
open circle between brackets). Two days later this essential for performance but not for acquisition
animal was again returned to the conditioning or retention of learned responses.

A 8 C
100 00 o e-e'e_e

80
0
I
0
0 - 0 CR TO LIGHT CS:
e-e CR TO TONE CS i
1/1
0:: :1-
:11)
u
I-
z
w
60
I
0
:w
:0::
II)
U
0:: 40 I I I I I I
~
a
w I I I I I I
0- N
I I I I I I.
....
I .... .... .... .... ....
I I I I I

co w ~ 0 W ~ ~ ~ 0 W ~

TIME (MIN)
Figure 16.17. Effects oflidocaine inactivation of inter- the light CS and closed circles to the tone CS. Arrows
positus on learning. This experiment was carried out indicate the injection of 1 flilidocaine into the anterior
in March/April, 1987. Open circles represent CRs to interpositus. See text for further details.
16. Role of the Cerebellum 331

VIII~
CS"~ SOC
~ AUD
~ CT

VI \
CR ~.-/RF\ ~ ~
UCR~~~~

ucs"
v
PO

Figure 16.18. Schematic illustration of pathways that


L~~~M
of CS and UCS could lead to synaptic facilitation
may be involved in the conditioned NMR reflex. The required for learning in order to stress the role of the
solid lines indicate established connections for the un- motor cortex and thus emphasize another site at which
conditioned NMR and open lines some ofthe possible cerebellar influences might be brought upon the per-
pathways for the CS and UCS. This figure illustrates formance of the CR. AUD CTX, auditory cortex; IC
only two of the possible sites at which associative MG, inferior colliculus and medial geniculate nucleus;
strength might accrue for acquisition of CRs. In this MOT CTX, motor cortex; SOC, superior olivary com-
case we have simply indicated how a brain stem circuit plex; SOM CTX, somatosensory cortex; VN, ventral
and some neocortical circuit might represent sites (e.g., thalamic complex;? CTX, unknown site in association
RFIPB and association cortex) at which the interaction cortex. Other abbreviations are as in Figure 16.15.

Use of the Nictitating Membrane cerebellum may receive information concerning


Reflex to Study Cerebellar Function CS and UCS onsets and modulate motor systems
involved in the NMR (filled arrows; see legend
There are several advantages in the use of the for details). Figure 16.18 emphasizes that there
conditioned NMR for the study of learning and are multiple systems involved in learning. Each
memory, which have been described in detail of these systems contains elements through which
(e.g., Gormezano et aI., 1983). The conditioned the cerebellum is known to influence movement.
NMR has also been shown to be a powerful tool Thus, the cerebellum could directly affect the pre-
for studying the effects of drugs on learning and blink area through its projections to the red
memory under conditions that require one to nucleus (Fig. 16.15), as well as its influence over
distinguish between sensory, associative, and the thalamus and motor cortex (Fig. 16.18).
motor effects (Harvey, 1987). The results of Welsh There are some unique aspects to the NMR
and Harvey (1989a-1991) indicate that the condi- that are relevant for the study of motor functions.
tioned NMR can also be used to distinguish be- First, it provides a measure of cerebellar regula-
tween the possible effects of cerebellar damage on tion of a cranial reflex that involves a small popu-
sensory processes, associative learning, and motor lation of motor neurons (approximately 100 re-
functions with a high degree of resolution and tractor bulbi motor neurons in the rabbit, Gray
reliability. Thus, the NMR can provide an addi- et aI., 1981), a final pathway via the Vlth nerve
tional tOQl for examining cerebellar function. that is well defined, and a single muscle (the re-
Figures 16.15 and 16.18 emphasize the multiple tractor bulbi) that is overwhelmingly responsible
systems through which cerebellar influences might for producing the NMR. In addition, the NMR
be able to modify the performance of the CR and is a behavior that is produced only through an
UCR. Figure 16.15 presents some possible brain agonist system, because there are no antagonist
stem pathways (open arrows) through which the- muscles for eyeball retraction. Despite the simpli-
332 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

city of this reflex and response system, cerebellar voluntary command when the cerebellum is
lesions produced many of the same effects on damaged, and this is directly analogous to the
measures of the NMR as compared with more decreased ability of CSs to elicit CRs in experi-
complex voluntary movements (compare Figs. mental animals.
16.1 with 16.13 and 16.16). Another advantage of One can conclude that the purpose ofthe cere-
the NMR preparation is that it provides a high bellum is to ensure that motor nuclei respond
level of control over stimulus presentations so with a strength proportional to the response-
that psychophysical measures of sensory processes evoking properties of the eliciting stimuli as
can be readily accomplished. A third advantage determined by their associative strength, intensity,
ofthe NMR is that conditions can be arranged so and biological significance (motivational/drive
that both a learned and an unlearned response state of the organism). Cerebellar damage does
may be measured from the same effector system, not affect the ability to learn or remember motor
increasing the probability that the same muscles skills but rather shifts the excitability thresholds
and motor neurons will be recruited for both the of motor nuclei so that they no longer depolarize
CR and UCR. Any changes in the performance to an extent proportional to the afferent volleys
of the UCR can provide an index of the viability carrying the commands for movement.
of the final pathway for the CR.
Acknowledgments. We thank Gertrud G. Champe,
Conclusions Director of the University of Iowa Translation
Current thinking on cerebellar function agrees Laboratory, and her colleagues, for their excellent
with, and has not challenged, Rolando's earliest translation of the Russian works. We especially
viewpoint that the cerebellum has a profound role thank Drs. Ramon A. Grigorian and Victor V.
in ensuring optimal performance of the voluntary Fanardjian for their generous donation of many
motor act. However, this review of the literature, Russian works on the issue of the cerebellum and
especially the recent findings on the NMR, has Pavlovian conditioning.
indicated the need for a reappraisal of the role of This work was supported by United States
the cerebellum in behavior. The common belief Public Health Service Grant MH16841. 1.P.W.
that the cerebellum has an exclusive role in volun- was supported by United States Public Health
tary behavior should be replaced with the re- Service Training Grant MH 15773.
cognition that the cerebellum regulates both
voluntary and involuntary movements. Once re- References
cognized, the reason why the voluntary move-
ment is so highly susceptible to cerebellar damage Albus, 1.S. (1971): A theory of cerebellar function. Math.
Biosci., 10, 25-61.
can be reevaluated.
Beck, E.c., and Doty, R. W. (1957): Conditioned flexion
A primary difference between reflexive and reflexes acquired during combined catalepsy and de-
learned movements is the nature of the eliciting efferentiation. J. Camp. Physiol. Psychol., 50, 211-
stimuli, the strength of which may determine the 216.
relative importance ofthe cerebellum in ensuring Berthier, N.E., and Moore, J.W. (1983): The nictitating
optimal performance of movements that they membrane response: An electrophysiological study
elicit. The strength of the involuntary or uncondi- of the abducens nerve and nucleus and the accessory
tioned movement is directly proportional to the abducens nucleus in rabbit. Brain Res., 258, 201-210.
intensity ofthe eliciting stimulus and the motiva- Brooks, y.B. (1986): The Neural Basis of Motor Control.
tional/arousal state of the organism. However, New York: Oxford University Press.
the ability of a CS or a voluntary command to ini- Bykov, K.M. (1957): The Cerebral Cortex and the Inter-
nal Organs (W.H. Gantt, ed.). New York: Chemical
tiate movement depends on three factors: stimu-
Rubber Co.
ulus intensity, motivational/arousal level, and Crisler, G. (1930): Salivation is unnecessary for the
the amount of associative strength that has devel- establishment of the salivary conditioned reflex
oped through learning. The statement of cere- induced by morphine. Am. J. Physiol., 94, 553-556.
bellar patients that they cannot promptly initiate Dickenson, W.H. (1865): On the functions of the cere-
movements despite any degree of effort epitomizes bellum. Br. For. Medico-Chirurgical Rev., 26, 455-
the inability of the motor system to utilize the 480.
16. Role of the Cerebellum 333

Evarts, E.V. (1966): Pyramidal tract activity associated Sutton, and PJ. Werbos, eds). Cambridge: MIT Press,
with a conditioned hand movement in the monkey. pp.301-348.
J. Neurophysiol., 29, 1011-1027. Ito, M. (1984):- The Cerebellum and Neural Control.
Fanardjian, V.V. (1961): The influence of cerebellar New York: Raven Press.
ablation on conditioned motor reflexes in dogs (G.G. Karamyan, A.I. (1959): The evolution of corticocere-
Champe, Trans!., J.P. Welsh, and J.A. Harvey, eds). bellar functional relationships (G.G. Champe, Trans!.,
Zhurnal Vyshei Nervoi D'eyatel'nosti, 11,920-926. J.P. Welsh, and J.A. Harvey, eds). Zhurnal Vyshei
Finch, G. (1938): Salivary conditioning in atropinized Nervoi D'eyatel'nosti, 3,346-444.
dogs. Am. J. Physiol., 124, 136-141. Karamyan, A.I. (1962): Evolution qfthe Function oJthe
Gambaryan, L.S. (1960): Conditioned avoidance re- Cerebellum and Cerebral Hemispheres (M. Roublev,
flexes induced in dogs with cerebellar lesions. Trans!.). Washington D.e.: National Science Foun-
Physiologia Bohemoslovenica, 9, 261-266. dation.
Gantt, W.H. (1989): Notes on Pavlov written while I Karamyan, A.1. (1968): On the evolution of the integra-
was in Russia (1922-1927). Pavlovian J. Bioi. Sci., 1, tive activity of the central nervous system in the
1-5. phylogeny of vertebrates. In: Progress in Brain Re-
Gormezano, I., Kehoe, EJ., and Marshall, B.S. (1983): search, Vol. 22, Brain Reflexes (E.A. Asratyan, ed.).
Twenty years of classical conditioning with the rabbit. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 427- 447.
In: Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Karamyan, A.I., Fanardjian, V.V., and Losareva, A.A.
Psychology, Vo!. 10 (1. M. Sprague, and A.N. Epstein, (1969): The functional and morphological evolution
eds). New York: Academic Press, pp. 197-275. of the cerebellum and its role in behavior. In: N euro-
Gray, T.S., McMaster, S.E., Harvey, 1.A., and Gormezano, biology qfCerebellar Evolution and Development (R.
I. (1981): Localization of retractor bulbi motoneurons L1imis, ed.). Chicago: American Medical Association
in the rabbit. Brain Res., 226, 93-106. Education and Research Foundation, pp. 639-673.
Harvey, J.A. (1987): Effects of drugs on associative Kellogg, W.N., Scott, V.B., Davis, R.e., and Wolf, I.S.
learning. In: Psychopharmacology, the Third Genera- (1940): Is movement necessary for learning? J. C omp.
tion oj Progress (H. Meltzer, ed.). New York: Raven Psychol., 29, 43-74.
Press, pp. 1485-1491. Krasusky, V.K. (1957): General nature of changes of
Harvey, J.A., Gormezano, I., and Cool-Hauser, V.A. food conditioned reflexes iII dogs following a surgical
(1985): Relationship between heterosynaptic reflex lesion of the cerebellum (G.G. Champe, Trans!., J.P.
facilitation and acquisition of the nictitating mem- Welsh and J.A. Harvey, eds). Zhumal Vyshei Nervoi
brane response in control and scopolamine injected D'eyatel'nosti, 5, 733-740.
rabbits. J. Neurosci., 5, 596-602. Lashley, K.S., and McCarthy, D.A. (1926): The survival
Harvey, J.A., Land, T., and McMaster, S.E. (1984): of the maze habit after cerebellar injuries. J. Compo
Anatomical study of the rabbit's corneal-Vlth nerve Physiol. Psycho!., 6, 423-433.
reflex: Connections between cornea, trigeminal sen- Light, J.S., and Gantt, W.H. (1936): Essential part of
sory complex, and the .abducens and accessory ab- reflex arc for establishment of conditioned reflex.
ducens nuclei. Brain Res., 301, 307-321. Formation of conditioned reflex after exclusion of
Heindel, W.e., Salmon, D.P., Shults, e.W., Walicke, motor peripheral end. J. Compo Psychol.,2I, 19-36.
P.A., and Butters, N. (1989): Neuropsychological Lisberger, S.G. (1988): The neural basis for learning of
evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: A simple motor skills. Science, 242, 728-735.
comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Livshits, N.N. (1947): Effect of cerebellar extirpation
Parkinson's disease patients. J. Neurosci., 9, 582- on the conditioned reflexes of dogs. In: Trudy Fisi-
587. ologicheskogo Instituta im. Pavlova,.Vo!. II (G.G.
Holmes, G. (1917): The symptoms of acute cerebellar Champe, Trans!., J.P. Welsh, and J.A. Harvey, eds).
injuries due to gunshot injuries. Brain, 40, 461-535. Moscow: USSR Academy of Sciences, pp. 11-50.
Holmes, G. (1939): The cerebellum of man. Brain, 62, L1imis, R., and Walton, K. (1979): Place of the cerebellum
1-30. in motor learning. In: Brain Mechanisms in Memory
Holstege, G., Tan, 1., van Ham, J.J., and Graveland, and Learning: From the Single Neuron to Mall (M.A.B.
G.A. (198.6): Anatomical observations on the afferent Brazier, ed.). New York: Raven Press, pp. 17-36.
projections to the retractor bubli motoneuronal cell Lopiano, L., de'Sperati, e., Bergui, M., and Montarolo,
group and other pathways possibly related to the P.G. (1989): Role of the cerebellar vermis in the long-
blink reflex in cat. Brain Res., 374,321-334. term habituation of the acoustic startle response in
Houk, J.e., Singh, S.P., Fisher, e., and Barto, A.G. the rat. In: The Olivocerebellar System in Motor
(1990): An adaptive sensorimotor network inspired Control, Exper. Brain Res. Series 17 (P. Strata, ed.).
by the anatomy and physiology of the cerebellum. Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 380-384.
In: Neural NetworksJor Control (W.T. Miller, R.S. Luciani, L. (1915): The hind-brain. In: Human Physiology
334 John P. Welsh and John A. Harvey

W. Welby, Transl., G.M. Holmes, ed.). London: Rawson, N.R. (1932): The story of the cerebellum. Can.
MacMillan and Co., pp. 419-486. Med. Assoc. J., 26, 220-225.
Marek, GJ., McMaster, S.E., Gormezano, I., and Sakurai, M. (1989): Depression and potentiation of
Harvey, J.A. (1984): The role of the accessory ab- parallel fiber-purkinje cell transmission in in vitro
ducens nucleus in the rabbit nictitating membrane cerebellar slices. In: The Olivocerebellar System in
response. Brain Res., 299, 215-229. Motor Control, Exper. Brain Res. Series 17 (P. Strata,
Marr, D. (1969): A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. ed.). Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 221-
(Lond.), 202, 437-470. 230.
McCormick, D.A., Lavond, D.G., Clark, G.A., Kettner, Sechenov, I. (1863): Reflexes of the Brain. In: Selected
R.E., Rising, C.E., and Thompson, R.F. (1981): The Works, (A.A. Subhov, Transl. and ed.), Moscow-
engram found? Role of the cerebellum in classical Leningrad: State House for Biological and Medical
conditioning of the nictitating membrane and eyelid Literature (1935), pp.263-336.
responses. Bull. Psychonomic Soc., 18, 103-105. Sherrington, C. (1906): The Integrative Action of the
McCormick, D.A., and Thompson, R.F. (1984): Cere- Nervous System. New Haven: Yale University Press.
bellum: Essential involvement in the classically con- Skelton, R.W., Mauk, M.D., and Thompson, R.F. (1988):
ditioned eyelid response. Science, 233, 296-299. Cerebellar nucleus lesions dissociate alpha condi-
Meyer-Lohmann, J., Hore, J., Brooks, V.B. (1977): tioningfrom alpha responses in rabbits. Psychobiology,
Cerebellar participation in generation of prompt 16, 126-134.
arm movements. J. Neurophysiol., 40,1038-1050. Squire, L.R. (1987): Memory: Neural organization and
Moore, lW., and B1azis, D.EJ. (1989): Cerebellar im- behavior. In: Handbook of Physiology- The Nervous
plementation of a computational model of classical System V(F. Plum,ed.). Bethesda, MD: Am. Physiol.
conditioning. In: The Olivocerebellar System in Motor Soc., pp. 295-371.
Control, Exper. Brain Res. Series 17 (P. Strata, ed.). Thompson, R.F. (1986): The neurobiology oflearning
Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 387-399. and memory. Science, 233, 941-947.
Orbeli, L.A. (1940): New concepts concerning cerebellar Walshe, F.M.R. (1927): The significance ofthe voluntary
function (G.G. Champe, Transl., J.P. Welsh and J.A. element in the genesis of cerebellar ataxy. Brain, 50,
Harvey, eds). Usp'ekhi Sovrem'ennoy Biolog;;, 13, 377-390.
207-220. Weiser, M., Pastor, A.M., and Baker, R. (1989): Mono-
Pavlov, I.P. (1927): Conditioned Reflexes (G.V. Anrep, cular adaptive gain control of the vestibulo-ocular
Transl.). New York: Dover. reflex in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Soc. N eurosci.
Polenchar, B.E., Patterson, M.M., Lavond, D.G., and Abstr., 15, 807.
Thompson, R.F. (1985): Cerebellar lesions abolish Welsh, J,P. (1992): Changes in the motor pattern of
an avoidance response in the rabbit. Behav. Neural learned and unlearned responses following cerebellar
Bioi., 44, 221-227. lesions: A kinematic analysis of the nictitating mem-
Pollock, LJ., and Davis, L. (1927): Influence of the brane reflex. Neurosci. 47,1-19.
cerebellum upon the reflex activities of the decereb- Welsh,lP., and Harvey, lA. (1989a): Cerebellarlesions
rate animal. Brain, 50,277-312. and the nictitating membrane reflex: Performance
Popov, N.F. (1929a): Notes for the study of cerebellar deficits of the conditioned and unconditioned re-
function. In: Vishaya N'ervnaya D'eyatel'nost': Sbornik sponse. J. Neurosci., 9,299-311.
Trudov Instituta, Vol. 1 (D.S. Fursikov, M.D. Welsh, J.P., and Harvey, J.A. (1989b): Modulation of
Gurevich, A.N. Zalmanzon, eds). (G.G. Champe, conditioned and unconditioned reflexes. In: The O/i-
Trans., J.P. Welsh and J.A. Harvey, eds). Moscow: vocerebellar System in Motor Control, Exper. Brain
Komunisticheskaya Akademiya Institutvishey Res. Series 17 (P. Strata, ed.). Berlin-Heidelberg:
Nervnoy D'eyatel'nosti, pp. 93-139. Springer-Verlag, pp. 374-379.
Popov, N.F. (1929b): The role of the cerebellum in the Welsh, lP., and Harvey, J.A. (1991): Pavlovian condi-
formation of conditioned motor reflexes. In: Vishaya tioning in the rabbit during inactivation of the inter-
N'ervnaya D'eyatel'nost': Sbornik Trudov Instituta, positus nucleus. J. Physiol. (Lond.), 444, 459-480.
Vol. 1, (D.S. Fursikov, M.D. Gurevich, A.N. Yeo, c.H., Hardiman, M.J., and Glickstein, M. (1985):
Z~lmanzon, eds) (G.G. Champe, Transl., J.P. Welsh, Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane
and J.A. Harvey, eds). Moscow: Komunisticheskaya responses of the rabbit. I. Lesions of the cerebellar
Akademiya Institutvishey Nervnoy D'eyatel'nosti, nuclei. Exp. Brain Res., 60,87-98.
pp. 140-148. Yerkes, R.M., and Morgulis, S. (1909): The method of
Prince, lH. (1964): The Rabbit in Eye Research. Pavlov in animal psychology. Psychol. Bull., 6,257-
Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. 273.
Index

A-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole-propionate Cerebellar cortex connections,


(AMPA),64 topographical organization of, 5-19
Acetylcholinesterase (AChE), 23-24,31-33,45,213, Cerebellar development, 5, 84
256,258,261,264 Cerebellar grafting,
Afferent convergence, 149,161-162 long-term survivals after, 85-99
D-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (APV), 62-64 short-term survivals after, 99-111
Amino acid-activated channels, 196-198 Cerebellar granule cells,
ARG, see Autoradiography in culture, 56-57
Aspartate, 116, 120, 125-129 survival requirements of, 57-62
Autoradiography (ARG), 15-17 chronic depolarization in, 57-58
Axis cylinder, 72-73 effect of NMDA in, 58-62
Cerebellar module, 40-41
Cerebellar morphogenesis,
micro tubule-associated protein in, 72-81
Babinski, 283
principal events in, 99
Basilar pontine nuclei (BNP), 1, 135-138, 140-141,
Cerebellar neurogenesis,
144, 149-162
description by Ramon y Cajal, 72
functional operations in rat, 149-158
putative role of cytoskeleton in, 72-73
local-circuit neurons of, 159-161
Cerebellar organotypic cultures,
noncortical afferent systems of, 136-141, 158-159
cellular and tissue differentiation in, 185
GABA-ergic components in, 136,138-141
preparation of, 184-185
synaptic features in, 141-149
Cerebellar output,
Brachium pontis (BP), 140
defining control signals from, 283-292,296
dentate, 283, 285-288, 291
fastigial, 283, 287-288, 290-291
Ca channels, 189 interposites, 283, 286-288, 291, 293-294
Calcium-binding protein (CaBP), 7-11, 18 Cerebellar sagittal zones,
CaM kinase, functional basis of, 267-279
in mediating tropic effect of EAA, 67 functional importance of, 275-277
Central nervous system (CNS), neurons of, 116, 121, historic descriptions of, 267-268
124-125 Cerebellum,
biophysical techniques in study of, 182-183 and cartography, 48-51
loss of during CSN development, 56-68 compartmentation in,
patch-clamp approach to, 198-199 higher levels of organization than, 38-40
Cerebellar compartmentation, molecular markers in, 22-51
conserved across phylogeny, 41 learning and memory in, 322-330
Cerebellar connectivity, during inactivation of interpositus, 329-330
events leading to, 41,43-48 types of, 322-327

335
336 Index

Cerebellum (cont.) EAA, see Excitatory amino acids


with ordered organizational pattern Eccentricity (ECC), 220-221
role in voluntary and reflexive movements, 301- EGL, see External granular layer
332 Electromyography (EMG), 285-287
Climbing fibers (CF), 22,46-50,96, 106, 108-109, EMG, see Electromyography
113,125,127-129,178-179,213-214,268, EPSP, see Excitatory postsynaptic potential
270,274,278 Ethylene glycol tetraacetic acid (EGTA), 173
activation of, 179 Excitatory amino acids (EAA), 56, 62-68,
and bands of Zebrin 1 PCs, 16-17 maturational stage
in motor function, 268-271 dependence of effects of, 64-66
in motor learning, 267, 271-273 neurobiology of, 56-68
operations in cerebellar cortex, 268-273 Excitatory post synaptic potential (EPSP), 96
and patterns of Zebrin 1 immunoreactivity, 18 External granular layer (EGL), 5
precerebellar, Eye movements,
capability to signal peripheral events, 226-227 evoked, 262-263
connection with mossy fiber system, 226-252
as conveyor of phasic information, 226
as conveyor of tonic information, 226, 229 Fast Blue (FB), 243, 248
projection terminals of, 34 Flourens, 283, 292, 302-304, 319
relation between anatomical departments FTX, see Funnel-web spider toxin
and aiTerents of, 261 Function,
visual, 255-256,263-264 theories of, 267
system, 226-232,237 Funnel-web spider toxin (FTX), 174-175
CO, see Cytochrome oxidase
Complex spike (CS), 214,226-227,229-244
Conditioned response (CR), 309,311-332 GABA, see Gamma aminobutyric acid
Conditioned stimuli (CSs), 309,311-322,324-332 GAD, see Glutamic acid decarboxylase
CS, see Complex spike Gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA), 24, 67
Cyclic guanosine monophosphate (GMJ)-dependent 110-111, 116-122, 124-125, 129, 165,
protein kinase (cGK), 7-9 177-179, 193-195, 198
Cyclic guanosine 3': 5' phosphate-dependent -ergic channels, 165, 198
protein kinase (cPK), 41,44,46 -ergic neurons, 135-137,140-141,144,147,
Cysteine sulphinic acid decarboxylase (CSAD), 24- 149-152,159-162,211,250
25, 33, 121-122, 124 Gamma-D-glutamylaminomethyl sulphonic acids
Cytochrome oxidase (CO), 31-33,45-46 (GAMS),62
Gaze shift (GS),
saccadic, 215-216, 218-224
DAB, see Diamino benzidine Gaze-sustained deviation (GSD), 218-219, 222
DAO, see Dorsal accessory olive Glutamate, 116-117,120,124-125,127
Davis, L., 301,307 Glutamate (Glu) receptors, 58, 62-66
Deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN), 5, 156,297 Glutamatergic channels, 196-198
Descartes, 308 Glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD), 24, 33,
DHP, see Dihydropuridine 116-117, 121, 138-139, 141, 160
Diaminobenzidine (DAB) reaction, 16, 18, 141 Glutaminase, 129
Diamidine Yellow Dihydrochloride (DY, 2HCI), 243- Glutamine, 116, 120, 128-129
244, 248, 251 Glutamine synthetase, 129
Dickenson, W.H., 303 Glycine, 116-121
Dihydropuridine (DHP) calcium effectors, 58, 64, 68 Golgi cells, 5,23,30,40,76,117-19,121,126,135, 159
Dorsal accessory olive (DAO), 6 Grafted Purkinje cell (GPC),
Dorsal column nuclei (DorCN) cells, 135, 155-156, dendrites of, 92,94-95,97,105-106,108,110-112
161 dendritic differentiation of, 105-107
Durcopan, 116 growth and navigation ofaxons
Dynamic selection hypothesis, 267-279 originated from, 96-99
preliminary experimental evaluation of, 277-278 and host cortical cerebellar circuits, 95-96
Index 337

location and dendritic arrangement of, 92-95 Lashley, K.S., 323


migratory routes of, 101-105, 112 Lewandowsky, M., 303
in nervous mutant mice, 92 Low thoracic-high lumbar (LTHL),
in normal cerebellum, 89 spinocerebellar projections, 34
in pcd mice, 89-92,95-96,98-102, 104, 109-110 terminal fields, 35-36, 48
procedure of 85-86 Low threshold Ca spikes (LTS), 203, 206-208, 210
proliferation period of, 101 LTHL, see Low thoracic-high lumbar
selective invasion of, 85-96 Luciani, L., 301,311-313,314,316
immunological distinction of categories in, 87-88 Lussana, 303
mechanisms involved in, 87-88
nonrandomness of, 87
starting point of, 113
synapse formation of, Magnus, R., 306-307
procedure of, 55 Main cuneate nucleus (MCN) neurons, 242-252
resulting in integration of, 108-111 MAPs, see Microtubule-associated proteins
synaptic integration of, 92-96 Marr, D., 323
Grafted Purkinje cell dendrite (GPD), 110-111 MCN neurons, see Main cuneate nucleus
GS, see Gaze shift MF, see Mossy fiber
GSD, see Gaze-sustained deviation Medial accessory olive (MAO), 6
Microtubule-associated proteins (MAPs), 72-81
chemical structure of, 81
Haggqvist method, 22 evolutionary conservation in properties of, 75
Holmes, G., 283,292,294,301,304-306, expression of in cerebellum, 75-77
308, 310-311, 316, 322, 324 expression of in cerebellum-derived cultures, 77-79
Horseradish peroxidase (HRP), 6, 261 -mRNA in the developing cerebellum, 80-81
conjugated to wheat germ agglutinin putative role of in axon and dendrite growth, 73-74
(WGA-HRP), 6-7,13,34-35, 136-141, Mossy fiber (MF), 22, 34, 36, 40, 47-51, 58,
155-156, 158-159, 162,243-247,248-252 96, 119, 125~126, 128-129, 175, 177, 179,213,
Huntington's disease, 323 226, 242
-granule cell activation, 40
somototopic organization of, 268
Inferior olivary nucleus, rhythmogenesis in, 201-211 terminal fields,
Inferior olive (10) neurons, 165, 213, 242-245, 247, mediolateral compartmentation due to, 36-38
251 Motilin, 24, 33
oscillatory activity of, 165, 203-211 Movement coordination, multiple maps in, 283-298
analog simulation of, 203-205 Myotomes, in the coronal dimension, 296-297
hybrid system to determine extent of, 205-210
preferred frequencies of, 207-209
Inhibitory post-synaptic potentials (IPSPs), 96, 159,
177, 179, 273 Na channels, 190-191
Internal granular layer (IGL), 58, 65 Nerve growth factor (NGF), 56
Intracerebellar compartments, Nervous mutations (NR), 10
development of, 41,43 Neural grafting, see Grafted Purkinje cell
matching connections to, 46-51 Neuroactive amino acids, see Purkinje cell, cerebellar
10, see Inferior olive Neurogenesis, see Cerebellar neurogenesis
IPSPs, see Inhibitory post-synaptic potentials Neuronal differentiation, 84
Neuronal replacement,
degree of, 85
when effective, 84
Jackson, J. Hughlings, 322
Neurotropic effect, positive, 92
Neurotransmitter amino acids,
microscopic immunocytochemistry of, 116-129
K channels, 191-193 Nictitating membrane response (NMR), 323, 328
Kainate (KA), 62-63, 67, 195 conditioned, 323-325,327,329,331-332
Kynurenic acid (KYN), 62 cerebellar regulation of, 323-327
338 Index

Nictitating membrane response (cont.) cell-attached recordings of, 188-189


unconditioned, in outside-out patches, 190-193, 198
cerebellar regulation of, 327-329 transmitter-activated, 193-194
use in study of cerebellar function, 331-332 voltage-gated, 185-188
N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), 58-68, 165, P channels in, 174-175
194, 196-198 plateau potentials in, 170-174
NMR, see Nictitating membrane response sensitivity to neuroactive transmitters, 194-
Nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis (NRT), 1-2 195
synaptic input in, 175-177
GABA-ergic responses and, 177-178
OCP, see Olivocerebellar projection heterogeneity of, 5-19
Olivary neurons, in adult cerebellum, 9-12
electroresponsive properties of, 201-202 as disclosed by cerebellar mutations, 9-10
as limit cycle oscillators, 203 level of, 11-12
Olivocerebellar projection (OCP), 36 in prenatal development, 7-9
development of, 6 relation between cerebellar connections
to Purkinje cells, 268 and, 12-18
subdivisions of, 13-16 host, 85-94
Orbeli, L.A., 316 immuno reactivity, 12-13
relationship between CS and SS of, 233-239
motor cortex effect on, 239-252
P channel, 165 response properties of saggital strips of, 273-275
Parallel-fiber, activation of, 179, 296-297 tridimensional arrangement of dendrites in, 84
Parkinson's disease, 323 voltage- and transmitter-gated channels in 182-
Pavlov, I.P., 301, 308-310 199
Pavlovian conditioning, 301-302, 308-309, 311, Purkinje cell degeneration (PCD), 10
324, 329, 3;23 Purkinje cell-specific glycoprotein (PSG), 7-8, 41
PC, see Purkinje cell
PEP 19,7-9
Peristimulus time histograms (PSTH), 227,229-
232,235-236,240,242-243 Quisqualate (QA), 62-64, 66, 195
Peroxidase anti-peroxidase (PAP), 141
Perturbed locomotion, 267,273-278
Perturbed volitional movement, 276 Rabbit flocculus,
Pinseau Terminal, 177 eye movements of, 255-264
Post saccadic drift (PSD), 220-221 topographical organization of, 264
Post synaptic potential (PSP), 273 zonal structure of, 255-257
P-PATH, as marker for Zebrin-negative PC, 33 Ramon y Cajal, 1, 72-73, 81, 84, 89, 97, 99, 105,
Presynaptic parallel fibers, stabilization of, 84 108, 112-113, 116, 135, 159, 177, 279, 283
Protocolumnar organization, 7 Real time postsynaptic response (RTPR), 273-275,
PSD, see Post saccadic drift 277-278
PSTH, see Peristimulus time histogram Reflexive movements,
Purkinje cell (PC), see also Grafted Purkinje role of cerebellum in, 301, 308-332
cell (GPC) Russian tradition of, 308-322
cerebellar, Rolando, 302-304, 306, 332
compartments of, 36 RTPR, see Real time postsynaptic response
corticonuclear projection and, 38
competition with grafted, 85-92
dendritic inhibition in, 177-178 Saccadic eye movements
dendritogenesis in, 112 cerebellar control of, 215-224
electrophysiology of, 165, 167-179 effect ofthe flocculus-paraflocculus on, 222-224
electroresponsiveness of, 167 effect of inferior olive lesion on, 216-222
dendritic action potentials in, 174 effect of the lesion of the posterior vermis
dendritic conductances in, 170 on, 222-224
ionic conductances in, 167-170,185-198 Saccadic system, 213,215-216
Index 339

Sechenpv, 301, 308-309, 318 Unconditioned stimulus (UCS), 309,311-315,


Sherrington, c., 303,306-307,310,316 319-321, 324, 326-331
Simple spike (SS), 214,226,233-236,238-239,
241-244, 270, 274-278 Vestibular ocular reflex (VOR), pathways, 213,
Sperry's hypothesis, 11 256, 262, 264
Spinocerebellar projection, Voltage-sensitive Ca2+ channels (VSCC), 58, 64,
development of, 6-7 68
pattern of in agranular cerebella, 7 Voluntary movement,
SS, see Simple spike role of cerebellum in, 301-332
Synaptic adhesion warning, of Larramendi, 111 western tradition of cerebellar regulation in, 302-
Synaptophysin, 30-31 308
VOR, see Vestibular ocular reflex
Tambaleante (TBL), 10
Taurine, 116-117, 120-125
WGA-HRP, see Horseradish peroxidase, conjugated
TEA, see Tetrathylammonium
Tetramethyl benzidine (TMB) method, 13 with
Tetrodotoxin (TTX), 169-172,174-175, Wheat germ agglutinin, see Horseradish peroxidase,
186, 190, 194-195,203,208 conjugated with
Tetrathylammonium (TEA), 185-186, 188-189, 192
TTX, see Tetrodotoxin Zebrin I , 12-19,25-41,43-46
Tubulin isotypes, in cerebellum, 79-81 immunoreactivity pattern of, 18
not regulated by afferent input, 44-45
Unconditioned response (UCR), 309,311,322- Zebrin II, 25-32,43-45
324, 326-329, 332 Zebrin III-V, 30,32,43

S-ar putea să vă placă și