Sunteți pe pagina 1din 8

Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Social Science & Medicine


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/socscimed

Autism spectrum disorders: Toward a gendered embodiment model


Keely Cheslack-Postava a, *, Rebecca M. Jordan-Young b
a
Paul F. Lazarsfeld Center for the Social Sciences, Columbia University, 420 W, 118th Street, Mail Code 3355, New York, NY 10027, United States
b
Department of Women’s, Gender, and Sexuality Studies, Barnard College, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: One of the most consistent observations in the epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is the
Available online 12 July 2011 preponderance of male cases. A few hypotheses have been put forth which attempt to explain this
divergence in terms of sex-linked biology, with limited success. Feminist epidemiologists suggest the
Keywords: importance of investigating specific mechanisms for male-female differences in health outcomes, which
Autism spectrum disorders may include sex-linked biology and/or gender relations, as well as complex biosocial interactions.
Gender
Neither domain has been systematically investigated for autism, and the possible role of gender has been
Sex
particularly neglected. In this article, we posit hypotheses about how social processes based on
Biosocial
Gender socialization
perception of persons as male or female, particularly patterns of social and physical interaction in early
Diagnostic bias development, may affect the observed occurrence and diagnosis of ASD. We gesture toward an
Developmental context embodiment model, incorporating hypotheses about initial biological vulnerabilities to autism e which
may or may not be differentially distributed in relation to sex biology e and their interactions with
gender relations, which are demonstrably different for male and female infants. Toward building such
a model, we first review the epidemiology of ASD with an eye toward male-female differences, then
present a theory of gender as a “pervasive developmental environment” with relevance for the excess
burden of autism among males. Finally, we suggest research strategies to further investigate this issue.
Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction may not respond to his/her name, while a toddler may have
delayed speech, fail to engage in “pretend” play, or line up toys
Autistic disorder (which we also refer to as autism) is charac- obsessively. An older child or adult may have difficulty “getting”
terized by impaired development in social interaction and jokes, engaging in conversation, and forming friendships. Cognitive
communication, accompanied by patterns of repetitive behavior or impairment is present in a substantial proportion of cases. The ‘gold
restricted interests, appearing before age 3. The broader category standard’ instruments for diagnosis are a standardized clinical
“autism spectrum disorders” (ASD) encompasses autistic disorder, interview, the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and an
Asperger syndromeewhere language develops typically, despite observation of set activities, the Autism Diagnostic Observation
other impairmentseand Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Schedule (ADOS); a variety of other scales and instruments are used
Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS), diagnosed where some, but not all for clinical, epidemiological, or screening purposes. A key charac-
criteria for an autism diagnosis are present. A complex set of teristic of all such instruments is that they are to some extent
genetic factors is believed to play an important role in the etiology subjective, relying on parental, caregiver, or self-report and/or
of these conditions; however, given that autism is a complex trait clinician observation and assessment.
with great variability in specific manifestations, and because of the One of the most consistent observations in the epidemiology of
diversity of genetic associations that have been suggested thus far, ASDs is the preponderance of male cases. A comprehensive review
a role for environmental factors, broadly construed, is also virtually of 43 studies published between 1966 and 2008 found male:female
certain (Balaban, 2006). (M:F) ratios ranging from 1.33:1 to 16:1, with a mean of 4.2:1;
Autism is diagnosed behaviorally based on a variety of symp- among studies reporting on ASDs combined, the median ratio was
toms and signs that vary by age. For example, an infant with autism 4.0:1 (Fombonne, 2009). This disparity is even greater among
Asperger syndrome and “high functioning” autism cases where it
can approach 10:1 (Fombonne, 1999).
* Corresponding author. Several hypotheses have been advanced to explain the male-
E-mail address: kc2497@columbia.edu (K. Cheslack-Postava). female (MeF) disparity in ASD diagnoses. Among the most

0277-9536/$ e see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.013
1668 K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674

widely cited is Baron-Cohen’s et al. (2005) “extreme male brain” in multiple animal (Majdan & Shatz, 2006; Mello, 2002; Sng &
hypothesis. Baron-Cohen holds that there is an “essential differ- Meaney, 2009) and human (Karmiloff-Smith, 2007) studies.
ence” between male and female cognitive and affective styles, with Moreover, most studies of genetics and health involve behavioral or
males oriented toward understanding and building law-like other phenotypic outcomes that are measured at the level of the
systems (systemizing), and females oriented toward under- whole organism (i.e., the individual). At this level, it is not possible
standing and predicting humans (empathizing). ASDs, in this to separate the individual’s gendered biography from their biology
model, reflect deficits in the cognitive processes of “empathizing”, e in fact, from the moment of birth, gendered processes literally
and strengths in “systemizing” (Baron-Cohen, 2009). Baron-Cohen become biological (see (Fausto-Sterling, 2005; Jordan-Young, 2010).
further suggests that the “male brain” pattern reflects the orga- For other purposes, we might follow Springer et al. (2011) and use
nizing action of testosterone on the developing fetal brain, and has the composite term “sex/gender” to highlight the fundamental
reported some correlations between steroids in amniotic fluid and entanglements of these domains while generally avoiding the
sex-typed behavior in infants and young children (Knickmeyer stand-alone term “sex.” Below, though, we develop a conceptual
et al., 2006; Knickmeyer, Baron-Cohen, Raggatt, & Taylor, 2005; model that is meant to reflect the underlying mechanisms through
Knickmeyer, Baron-Cohen, Raggatt, Taylor, & Hackett, 2006). which sex and gender become sex/gender e that is, the moment(s)
Several serious weaknesses with this line of thinking must be of their entanglement. Thus, we do use the term “sex,” though we
noted, however, beginning with the observation that the concepts caution that its use in the abstract model should not distract from
of “systemizing” and “empathizing” are not well-specified (Grossi & the principle that it will generally not be possible to actually assess
Fine, in press). Further, Baron-Cohen’s studies have not been sex apart from gender. Moreover, when we use the stand-alone
independently replicated, and have been faulted on extensive term “gender,” we refer not to properties of individuals (as in
methodological grounds (Grossi & Fine, in press; Jordan-Young, gender identity or gender roles), but rather to higher-order
2010). Finally, a systematic analysis of studies linking sex-typed processes of interaction and norms, specifically patterns of early
cognitive traits in humans with pre-natal steroid hormone expo- interaction with adults that systematically differ by the perceived
sures suggests that pre-natal steroids probably do not make sex/gender of the child.
a meaningful contribution to MeF cognitive differences (Jordan- Before proceeding further, we find it important to clarify the
Young, 2010). relationship between the argument we advance here, and prior
Early hormone exposures could alternatively alter expression of models of autism that invoked the social environment, specifically
specific disease related genes, either increasing or decreasing risk parentechild relations. As Siller and Sigman (2002, p. 78) have
(Kaminsky, Wang, & Petronis, 2006). Specific mechanisms are as yet noted, " The possible contributions of parents to their [autistic]
undetermined, but could include sensory perception, neuroendo- children’s development have been neglected, probably because of
crine reward processes involved in social-emotional interaction a sensitivity of investigators, clinicians, and parents to previous,
and learning, or factors related to ease of building or pruning neural fallacious, psychogenic theories of autism." In contrast to prior
connections, to name a few (see the risk model developed by "psychogenic" theories that have blamed parenting styles, espe-
Dawson (2008) for elaboration). Genetic sex could also affect risk cially of mothers, for autism, we wish to be clear that our model
for ASD through X-linked traits. Having two X chromosomes does not concern deficits in parenting in any sense. Instead, it
protects females from some X-linked disordersdknown or engages the idea that “normal" and normative parenting, like all
hypothesized mechanisms include genes outside the pseudoauto- other family and community relations, is gendered, meaning that
somal region that escape X-inactivation; genes expressed only from boy and girl children, on average, encounter different experiences,
the paternally inherited allele through imprinting, a parent-specific interactions, and environments. Moreover, we assert that those
process of gene silencing; and production of some level of func- aspects of the social environment that differ, on average, for boys
tional gene product. For example, Fragile X syndrome, involving and girls are not enough in and of themselves to generate ASDs:
mutations in the FMR1 gene on the long arm of the X chromosome there must be some sort of underlying vulnerability.
resulting in decreased levels of its protein product, affects females
more rarely and less severely than males, and as many as 30% of Epidemiology of ASD and maleefemale comparisons
those with Fragile X are also diagnosed with ASDs (Marco & Skuse,
2006). However, only a small percentage of ASD cases overall have ASDs are characterized by marked heterogeneity in symptoms,
been associated with X-linked disorders. More perfunctory theories developmental course, and associated co-morbidities. By way of
include disruptions in the vasopressin system or a protective role of example, subsets of cases experience developmental regression;
oxytocin (Carter, 2007) and sex differences in neurodevelopmental differences in head circumference, serotonin levels, and immuno-
susceptibility to environmental insults (Dean & McCarthy, 2008). logical parameters; epilepsy, gastrointestinal complaints, and sleep
Varying degrees of evidence support each of these theories, but disturbances (Levy, Mandell, & Schultz, 2009; Newschaffer et al.,
to date, none has provided a strong account for MeF differences in 2007). Some heterogeneity likely stems from the exponential
autism. While it is beyond the scope of this article to evaluate this increase in recognized cases from 1943 when Kanner published the
evidence, what these theories hold in common is that they place first case series of eleven patients with “autistic disturbances”
the mechanisms responsible for these observed discrepancies (Kanner, 1943), to a 1966 survey of autism in the UK reporting
firmly in the realm of sex-linked biology (“sex”), which is concep- a prevalence of 0.04% (Fombonne, 2003), to a 2009 CDC publication
tualized as distinct from the social realm of MeF difference estimating ASD prevalence at 1% of U.S. 8-year olds (Autism and
(“gender”). However, considering how social as well as biological Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 2009). The
forces might be acting to cause observed disparity may provide extent to which changes in diagnostic criteria, awareness, de-
new insights into its etiology. stigmatization, and diagnostic substitution can account for the
Though sex and gender may be conceptually distinct, they are in trend is not certain.
practice often inseparable (see Springer, Stellman, and Jordan- Etiology also appears heterogeneous. Approximately 10% of
Young, 2011). Even if an investigator posits a mechanism cases are linked to recognized genetic syndromes, and associations
involving sex chromosomes, the identities of which are laid down with genes or loci on nearly every chromosome have been reported
prior to the possibility of any socialization, gene expression is (Abrahams & Geschwind, 2008). With technological advances, rare
influenced by experience and learning, as has been demonstrated copy number variations (CNVs) e insertions or deletions of
K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674 1669

relatively large segments of DNA e have emerged as possibly somewhat at odds with the pattern of higher prevalence among
important. Yet, because CNVs have been found in a wide variety of socially advantaged groups. The intersection of sex/gender with
locations in the genome, and in unaffected subjects, they may have race, SES, or immigrant status, though on occasion presented
limited explanatory power (Abrahams & Geschwind, 2008). Several descriptively (see for instance (Bhasin & Schendel, 2007)), has not
pre-natal exposures have also been implicated, including thalido- been systematically explored as a potential window on the joint
mide, anti-convulsant drugs, and immune factors (Newschaffer roles of multiple social processes in ASD risk and diagnosis.
et al., 2007); however these encompass a very small subset of
total cases. The most consistently identified risk factors for ASD are Gender as a “Pervasive Developmental Environment”:
having an affected sibling, older parental ages, and male sex/ potential roles of gendered social processes in producing M:F
gender, with the former two hypothesized to arise from genetic patterns in autism
factors. Meanwhile, in trying to determine the mechanisms behind
the MeF disparity, it makes sense to ask whether there is In the following sections, we introduce a simplified model of
systematic variation in M:F ratios for ASD which may provide developmental trajectories for an infant with underlying suscepti-
insight about the role of sex/gender in etiology and diagnosis, and bility for ASD that may or may not eventually lead to an ASD
would suggest that mechanisms associating sex/gender with diagnosis (Fig. 1). Given the etiologic heterogeneity of ASDs, and the
autism risk are subject to modification. large and uncertain array of candidates for the specific disruptions
There are in fact several instances of such variation in M:F ratios. underlying ASDs, we cannot be certain about specific elements in
A number of examples suggest that in the presence of additional the diagram. For example, we expect that an accurate model would
developmental insults or delays, M:F ratios for autism are lower require multiple different forms of initial vulnerability, any of
(though there are exceptions; see for example Schendel, Autry, which might be differentially distributed by (genetic) sex, or not.
Wines, and Moore (2009) regarding major birth defects). Intellec- However, we can identify points where gendered social processes,
tual disability (ID, also referred to as mental retardation or cogni- functioning through adult perception of, response to and interac-
tive impairment, typically IQ below 70 or equivalent developmental tion with an infant may interact with biology producing probabi-
test performance) is one of the most prevalent co-morbidities to listic ‘switches’ whereby males end up both more likely than
ASD. M:F ratios decline with increasingly severe ID (Fombonne, females to express ASD symptoms, and more likely to be diagnosed,
1999; Nicholas et al., 2008; Volkmar, Szatmari, & Sparrow, 1993; given expression of symptoms.
Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003). For example, M:F ratios in ASD Our aim, then, is not to create a singular model of development
among 3e10 year olds in Atlanta were 6.7:1 among children in autism but instead to show the structure of developmental
without ID, 4.4:1 in those with IQ 50e70, and 1.3:1 among those cascades that always involve both biological and environmental
with IQ < 20 (Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003). The correlation between factors. Social interactions have material effects on the developing
epilepsy and ID likely explains a reported M:F ratio of 2:1 for autism brain, which in turn affect the biological substrate at the next level
with epilepsy versus 3.5:1 without (Amiet et al., 2008). Lower M:F of development. Indeed, brain development requires interactions.
ratios have also been reported among cases with dysmorphic As Karmiloff-Smith has noted, "the neonate cortex is neither
features or microcephaly (Miles et al., 2005), and various studies localized nor very specialized at birth. This allows interaction with
have found associations between autism and obstetric and peri- the environment to play a crucial role in gene expression and in the
natal complications (reviewed in: (Bilder, Pinborough-Zimmerman,
Miller, & McMahon, 2009; Kolevzon, Gross, & Reichenberg, 2007)).
In a cohort of over 400,000 births in metropolitan Atlanta, Schendel
and Bhasin (2008) found that M:F ratios for autism decreased with
decreasing birth weight or gestational age, among all children with
autism and by presence of ID or other developmental disability.
These observations collectively suggest that while there is some-
thing protective about being female with regard to ASD, that
protective element is less functional when accompanied by other
impairments or vulnerabilities. If there are subtle protective
elements of female gender socialization, then infants affected by ID,
or prematurity, for example, may be less able to benefit from such
experiences. Neonatal interventions common to both male and
female infants with early pre-term birth or other complications
could also plausibly reduce MeF differences in early social
environment.
Differences in M:F ratios for ASD across other social categories
such as race, socioeconomic status (SES), or immigrant status could
reflect variation in the social structuring of gender, and a corre- Fig. 1. Hypothesized model for effects of gender environment on developmental and
sponding impact on developmental or diagnostic processes. A diagnositic trajectories for an infant with initial underlying susceptibility for ASD,
range of studies have explored variation in ASD diagnoses across which may or may not be differentially distributed by genetic sex. Adult perception of,
single dimensions of social difference. Several have reported asso- response to, and interaction with the infant is shaped by perceived gender, so that
certain types of interaction are more common with male than female infants, and vice
ciations of autism with measures of higher SES, though not all
versa. Some element(s) of this differential social environment that is more frequently/
studies find such an association (see Durkin et al., 2010 for intensely experienced by female infants acts as a protective factor, muting the emer-
summary and references). Researchers tend to conclude that such gence or expression of ASD-related symptoms. Among children who express symp-
associations reflect ascertainment bias, however Durkin et al. toms of ASD compatible with diagnosis, recognition and identification is more likely
reported a positive gradient in ASD prevalence by SES even among among males. The model is simplified to focus on gendered environment in that it
excludes other environmental or contextual inputs which may affect probability of
children who had not previously received a diagnosis (Durkin et al., symptom expression or diagnosis. Additionally, it is expected, given etiologic hetero-
2010). Higher prevalence has also been reported in some immi- geneity of ASDs, that hypothesized gender-linked mechanisms may be operant under
grant minority groups (Keen, Reid, & Arnone, 2010), which is some forms of initial underlying vulnerability but not others.
1670 K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674

ultimate cognitive phenotype.. epigenesis is probabilistic, and communication (Lytton & Romney, 1991). Notably, that review
gene expression is activity-dependent" (Karmiloff-Smith, 2007, p. found larger sex-typing effects for younger children, and for studies
84). As we explore below, empirical evidence on gender and social where parents are actually observed, rather than queried (Lytton &
interactions demonstrates that experiences potentially impinging Romney, 1991).
on the ultimate cognitive phenotype are profoundly shaped by Suggestive evidence that gender socialization may be relevant
gender. for traits related to autism is provided by studies of subtle differ-
ences in how adults communicate with infants of different genders.
Hypothesizing developmental mechanisms: sex and gender in A recent study of 6377 infant-mother dyads from the (U.S.)
interaction nationally-representative Early Childhood Longitudinal Study Birth
Cohort found that verbal stimulation has a strong positive effect on
Many developmental scenarios could explain a "true" difference infants’ social-emotional and cognitive development, and also
in rates of autism between males and females. Here, we suggest found a small but significant bias toward mothers offering more
three plausible scenarios involving the presence of initial verbal stimulation to girls than boys (Page, Wilhelm, Gamble, &
biologically-based vulnerabilities, related for example to sensory Card, 2010). Likewise, a recent observational study of 36 infant-
and/or information processing abilities. Such vulnerabilities may or mother dyads found that "Mothers of daughters made more
may not be sex-linked. That is, initial "vulnerable" phenotypes may interpretations and engaged in more conversation with their
be randomly distributed among male and female infants, but MeF daughters, whereas mothers of sons made more comments and
differences might emerge via interaction of vulnerabilities with attentionals, which were typified by instructions rather than
gendered aspects of the developmental context. The first scenario conversation" (Clearfield & Nelson, 2006, p. 127). Mothers also
thus begins with vulnerabilities that are randomly distributed spent more time engaged in activity with daughters versus
among males and females, then expressed differentially as ASD watching sons (Clearfield & Nelson, 2006). In this study, investi-
symptoms because of gendered socialization, resulting in increased gators did not find maleefemale differences in infant contact/
M:F ratios. A second scenario would involve vulnerabilities that are proximity to mothers or looking behavior (Clearfield & Nelson,
more prevalent in males, but are amplified through gendered 2006), however individual children were not observed over time.
socialization. A third scenario would involve vulnerabilities that are In a longitudinal study in which infants were observed within days
more prevalent in males but unaffected by socialization. of birth by an adult interactor unaware of child’s sex, male and
In the first two scenarios, biological vulnerabilities play a role in female infants did not exhibit any significant differences in mutual
disparities without constituting an "innate" sex difference in eye gaze with the adult; however, by four months of age differences
autism. This is more obvious in the first scenario, where initial in eye contact behavior appeared, these being accounted for by an
vulnerabilities are randomly distributed, but it is also the case when increase in eye contact made by girls (Leeb & Rejskind, 2004). While
the initial vulnerabilities are not randomly distributed, so long as this study did not directly measure adult behavior in relation to
the final MeF difference in autism emerges only in the context of child gender, it is compelling in demonstrating infant differences by
gender-differentiated socialization. That is, the model treats the gender emerging only after inception of the earliest social inter-
developmental context as a crucial component in the development actions, in a behavior with direct relevance to ASD, namely eye
of autism, not a lesser "modifier" of a biological substrate conceived contact.
as prior to and separate from developmental context. Both Another body of research has documented that attributions of
hypotheses explain disparities in autism by taking seriously gender profoundly shape the way observers perceive emotions and
extensive data demonstrating pervasive gender differences in behavior in children. Often referred to as “baby X studies,” such
children’s early socialization. Currently, however, only the third research involves creatively manipulating the perceived sex of
scenario is represented in the literature. By omitting gendered identical infants or very small children, and analyzing how
socialization from the array of factors considered in producing MeF observers describe or react to the children when labeled as female
differences, researchers make the implicit assumption that it has no versus male. One well-known study involved a short film of a baby
effect. in an infant seat presented with four different stimuli ea teddy
Gender socialization occurs through interactions with other bear, a jack-in-the-box, a doll, and a buzzer e each presented
people, as well as through exposure to different physical environ- several times in succession. More than 200 observers were
ments, even from very young ages. As an example of the latter, randomly assigned the information that the child was a boy versus
studies have found pronounced differences in the toys and physical a girl, without making an obvious point of this. There were
environments of girl versus boy infants (Nash & Krawczyk, 1994; remarkable differences in the way that observers saw the same
Pomerleau, Bolduc, Malcuit, & Cossette, 1990). In this discussion infant when they thought they were watching “David” versus
we focus on social interactions, encompassing both verbal and “Dana,” especially when presented with the child’s somewhat
physical exchanges. These are shaped by a wide range of factors ambiguous response to the jack-in-the-box. Those observers who
that include characteristics of the infant, as well as the intentions, thought they were watching “David” were much more likely to
implicit beliefs, and sensitivity (i.e., skill in perceiving and decoding describe the child as “angry,” while those who thought they were
infant cues) of the infant’s social partner. There is evidence that the watching “Dana” described the child as “afraid” (Condry & Condry,
latter three factors are all influenced by the perceived gender of the 1976). Subsequent studies have extended these observations,
infant. Scores of studies have documented parents’ differential including by controlling for actual variations in infant characteris-
socialization of boys and girls (Fine, 2010; Harris, 2009), though the tics (e.g., (Culp, Cook, & Housley, 1983; Delk, Madden, Livingston, &
degree and precise domains of differential treatment are somewhat Ryan, 1986; Donovan, Taylor, & Leavitt, 2007; Lyons & Serbin, 1986;
controversial. One meta-analysis suggests that while there is Seavey, Katz, & Zalk, 1975; Sidorowicz & Lunney, 1980)). On the
evidence of parents’ sex-typed perception and encouragement of other hand, one review of infant gender labeling studies concluded
sex-typed activity, they may treat boys and girls more or less that overall effects, though present, were not strong or consistent
similarly in most other broad domains, including overall amount (Stern & Karraker, 1989) belying the limitations of this experimental
of interaction; achievement encouragement; restrictiveness method for understanding real-world phenomena. Nonetheless,
versus encouragement of independence; warmth, nurturance, and a clue about how differences in perception of infant expressions by
responsiveness; disciplinary strictness; and clarity of perceived gender may translate to differences in response is given
K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674 1671

by a study of Donovan et al. (2007) who found that mothers of 6- with males, would be more readily recognized in male versus
month old infants were more sensitive to changes in facial female children, given equal presentation of symptoms. Well-
expressions of a similarly-aged infant when the child was pre- documented examples of disease being more readily diagnosed in
sented as female. males due to such labeling effects include heart disease (Goldberg,
It is not clear precisely what effect described differences in adult 2002; Institute of Medicine, 2003) and HIV/AIDS (Goldstein &
perception, response or interaction might have on infants with ASD Manlowe, 1997). The extremely prolific citation, both within and
vulnerabilities. For one thing, none of these studies focused on outside psychology, of Baron-Cohen’s theory of autism as repre-
children at risk for ASD, and may not be generalizable. Nonetheless, senting an “extreme male brain”, lend credence to this scenario.
sex-typing may condition responses to both gender-typical and Conversely, we should not discount the possibility that parents
gender atypical behaviors in vulnerable girls, in ways that consti- would be likely to notice and be concerned about traits if they are
tute early, naturalistic interventions by parents and caregivers. "gender atypical." In this case, ASD symptoms (such as disliking
Siller and Sigman (2002), in a longitudinal study, found that among touch or difficulty displaying mutuality in interaction) might raise
children with autism, levels of synchronization between caregiver concern at a lower threshold in girls as opposed to boys. This would
interactions and child’s focus of attention during play were asso- run counter to the observed trend of a higher M:F ratio. Yet again,
ciated with gains in joint attention and language up to 16 years given the association of autism with males, parents and clinicians
later. If findings of increased sensitivity to female infants extend to may more readily label an uncommunicative girl as “timid” or “shy”
focus of attention, then female infants may experience on average rather than entertaining a diagnosis of ASD.
higher levels of such synchronization. Additionally, a focus of Another possibility is that given the same underlying deficits,
parental participation in early intervention for ASD is to adapt “female autism” tends to look different than “male autism” in terms
interactive styles to promote early social engagement and of symptom expression, resulting in female cases that share these
communication (Dawson, 2008). Studies above suggest that the deficits but go undiagnosed or receive alternative diagnoses. Nearly
social environment of a female infant may provide on average more two decades ago, Kopp and Gillberg described six girls who met
bids for social engagement, whether through format of speech, diagnostic criteria for autism, but took several years of clinical
play, or eye contact. Another possibility is that certain behav- encounters to receive autism diagnoses (Kopp & Gillberg, 1992).
iorsdlike eye contact, or vocal response to questionse may be Likely contributing to the delay were qualitative differences in
more heavily ‘coached’ in girls, so that behaviors appear more these girls’ behavior versus the expected behavioral presentation
‘typical’ and may mask other ASD symptoms. While the studies that these authors described. Their social impairment was
discussed here have important limitations and the potential rele- described as tending “more toward ‘clinging’ to other people,
vance of any specific scenario to ASD is unknown, the main point is imitating their speech and movements without a deeper under-
that development is a "looping process" in which gender is a crit- standing., and only brief periods of aloofness” (Kopp & Gillberg,
ical, ongoing factor: it shapes initial social and tactile input that the 1992, p. 96). They also showed high frequency of language use,
child receives from human interaction, as well as other people’s though often echolalic and uncommunicative, as opposed to
responses to the child’s behaviors, in an iterative manner. muteness more common in males. Despite such suggestive
evidence, there is still a dearth of research on MeF differences in
Gender bias in diagnosis presentation (Thompson, Caruso, & Ellerbeck, 2003), a clear
impediment being that recognized female cases are likely to be
Some of the observed MeF difference in autism may stem from those most similar to male cases. Lack of knowledge about the
parents’, clinicians’, and others’ different perceptions and expec- nature and extent of such differences may contribute to the
tations for boys versus girls, beginning in early infancy. Lest readers observed M:F disparity.
are tempted to speculate that gender labeling is a thing of the past, Some evidence of gender bias in identification of ASD exists. The
it is worth noting that recent studies generally indicate labeling United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Autism
effects as strong as those found in the earliest studies, more than 30 and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network consists of
years ago. Reliance on cognitive schemas about gender to label sites in several U.S. states that conduct surveillance to identify
behavior not only affects how we perceive specific people and their children with ASDs. Beginning in 2000, school and medical records
actions, it loops back to reinforce belief in fundamental, generalized of 8-year-old children have been periodically examined to deter-
gender differences. Importantly, this process leads people to mine the prevalence of children meeting a study case definition for
discount actual evidence of intra-sex variability and inter-sex ASD (Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network,
similarity, and operates even in the face of commitments to simi- 2009). Because identification is not dependent on previous diag-
larity or a ‘wish’ for similarity. That is, being egalitarian in orien- nosis, this method may allow detection of diagnostic biases,
tation doesn’t seem to affect gender labeling very much (Fine, although case identification still depends on relevant behaviors
2010). having been recorded, and a record existing in the first place. For
Infants randomly labeled as male are perceived to be stronger children born in 1994, the median age of diagnosis was a half-year
and more masculine than infants labeled as female (Burnham & later in girls than in boys (Shattuck et al., 2009), and boys with ASD
Harris, 1992), and newborn girls are described as littler, softer, were significantly more likely to have been so identified than were
and more finer featured, even in when they do not differ on any girls (Mandell et al., 2009). Although in other studies, reports of
objective measures (Rubin, Provenzano, & Luria, 1974). Mothers in later age or longer time to diagnosis in girls versus boys failed to
one study were asked to estimate how capable their infants would reach statistical significance (Mandell, Listerud, Levy, & Pinto-
be in crawling up a ramp; mothers exaggerated their infant sons’ Martin, 2002; Mandell, Novak, & Zubritsky, 2005; Ouellette-Kuntz
physical abilities, while underestimating their girls’ abilities, rela- et al., 2009), significance may be affected by low numbers of
tive to objective tests of infants’ abilities made immediately female cases and study designs dependent on prior diagnosis with
following these estimates (Mondschein, Adolph, & Tamis-LeMonda, ASD. Two recent studies compared rates of ASD diagnosis among
2000). siblings of children with ASD (Constantino, Zhang, Frazier,
In terms of how gendered expectations might affect ASD diag- Abbacchi, & Law, 2010) and subjects from the 14,000 member
nosis, the simplest and most likely scenario is that ASD, as Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) (Russell,
a syndrome that is commonly and epidemiologically associated Steer, & Golding, in press) in males and females who showed
1672 K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674

symptoms of autism derived from other study measures. 27% of underlying susceptibility, irrespective of whether or not they
female siblings (versus 47% of males) who scored above cut-offs on express symptoms.
quantitative scales of social communication or responsiveness, or The ratio Me:Fe can then be determined if we know Mo:Fo and
showed a history of language delay and autistic speech character- the probabilities of diagnosis for males (dm) and females (df),
istics had received an ASD diagnosis (Constantino et al., 2010). In respectively, who show symptoms of ASD:
ALSPAC, 18% of girls, versus 37% of boys who showed develop-
mental impairments most predictive of ASD diagnosis had actually Mo Fo df
Me : Fe ¼ : ¼ Mo : Fo 
received such a diagnosis in the community (Russell et al., in press). dm df dm
These observations suggest that ASD-related characteristics can be
more readily observed and/or more rapidly translated into clinical We assume Mo:Fo to be 4:1, and plot Me:Fe over a range of values of
diagnoses for boys than for girls; this could occur through differ- df/dm in Fig. 2A. Limited data suggest that df/dm may fall between
ential interpretation of behaviors or levels of awareness and 0.5 (Constantino et al., 2010; Russell et al., in press) and 0.75
concern on the part of caregivers, educators, or clinicians. (OR ¼ 1.35 for previous diagnosis in males versus females (Mandell
et al., 2009)), giving Me:Fe values ranging from about 2:1 to 3:1.
Ms:Fs is then a function of Me:Fe and risks in males (rm) and
Estimating Cumulative impact on observed M:F ratios females (rf) of expressing autism symptoms, given underlying
susceptibility:
The lines of thought entertained here raise the question of what
proportion of the observed discrepancy in diagnoses may be Me Fe
attributed to social processes related to gender. While such esti- Ms : Fs ¼ : ¼ Me : Fe  RRf vs;m
rm rf
mates involve considerable speculation, it is helpful to examine the
range of possibilities. To do so, we define three M:F ratios. Mo:Fo is In Fig. 2B, we begin with several values for Me:Fe ranging from 2
the observed ratio of males to females among cases receiving to 3.2. For each of these values, we plot Ms:Fs for RRf vs.m values
diagnoses in the community. Me:Fe is the ratio of males to females ranging from 1 to 0.5, assuming that being female is a protective
among persons in the population expressing symptoms of autism factor, associated with a relative risk below 1. These equations show
sufficient for diagnosis, irrespective of actually receiving a diag- that the relationship between observed M:F ratios and M:F ratios
nosis; and Ms:Fs is the ratio of males to females among persons with for underlying susceptibility to ASD depends on levels of diagnostic

Fig. 2. Calculated range of M:F ratios for A) expression of ASD symptoms compatible with diagnosis; and B) underlying susceptibility to ASD; assuming a 4:1 observed M:F ratio.
K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674 1673

bias and the relative effect of gendered social environment on approach may not be widely generalizable. A retrospective case-
symptom expression. Assuming reasonable values for these vari- control design could capitalize on archives of video footage of
ables results in Ms:Fs ranging from 3:1 to much closer to 1:1, sug- young children prior to diagnosis, but would require sufficient
gesting that underlying biologic susceptibility may well be footage of typical interaction with caregivers and would not allow
approximately equally distributed with respect to biologic sex. observation of a standard set of activities. 3) Assess association
between exposures and outcome. Because exposures of interest are
Questions and strategies for research likely to be experienced by all infants to varying degrees, this will
involve determining whether there is a threshold above which
Clearly, the role of gender in ASD etiology and diagnosis is ripe certain exposures become protective. The ultimate test of any such
for further research. With regard to diagnosis, questions remain as identified protective ‘exposures’ would be to incorporate them into
to whether, and to what extent, girls are under-diagnosed, which the design of randomized intervention studies. If this approach was
may vary by population, time period, case definition, and methods able to help identify elements of an effective very early intervention
used for diagnosis or case ascertainment. It is difficult to imagine that could improve developmental trajectories for some children at
a scenario through which population-level screening or surveil- risk of ASDs, it would be the ultimate proof of utility for this
lance could be conducted in a manner truly blinded as to sex/ biosocial approach from a public health perspective.
gender. Therefore, the best approach for population-based esti-
mates probably derives from comparisons of community diagnosis Conclusion
with estimates for “true” ASD status obtained using more general
criteria. It is also important to gain a better understanding of The case of autism illustrates how gender theory can be used not
whether ASDs manifest differently in a subset of girls, and if so, to just to more precisely illuminate the nature and etiology of male-
develop strategies for identifying this subset. An alternative female health differentials, but also contribute to fundamental
approach would involve examining whether professional percep- knowledge of specific developmental disorders, and the complex
tion of ASD symptoms varies by sex/gender of the subject, e.g., by interaction of biology and the social environment. Our argument is
presenting vignettes that vary only in whether a girl or boy is fully biosocial, and our main points in advancing it are to articulate
described, an approach previously used to investigate race and SES a model for autism, specifically for explaining the male-female
in autism and ADHD diagnosis (Cuccaro et al., 1996). The role of disparities in prevalence, that does not exclude social-
perceived sex/gender in clinical assessment could be even more environmental variables, and is therefore more biologically satis-
directly explored by presenting video footage of clinical interac- fying; and to demonstrate concrete mechanisms whereby autism
tions which are digitally manipulated to vary the apparent gender may become more prevalent in males as a result of social structures
of the subject. Because parental efforts may be key in obtaining an and processes related to gender. What we have presented suggests
ASD diagnosis, MeF disparity in ASD-specific concern elicited by a domain of research questions and strategies specific to ASDs that
potentially related symptoms or behaviors among parents is also have as of yet been little explored; and a theory of gender as
relevant. a pervasive developmental environment with potential broader
The epidemiology of MeF differences in ASD has been reason- applicability to understanding differential health states for males
ably well explored with respect to intellectual disability and related and females, particularly those arising in early life.
factors. Differences in M:F ratios for ASD across other social cate-
gories such as race, SES, or immigrant status have been less Acknowledgments
explored and could reflect variation in the social performance of
gender, and a corresponding impact on developmental or diag- The authors thank the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health
nostic processes. It would be useful to position such analyses & Society Scholars program and the Foundation for Worker,
against a backdrop of information on variation in gendered Veteran, and Environmental Health for financial support. We thank
socialization across these categories. It may also be informative to participants in the Gender and Health Working Group at Columbia
explore change over time, and across any of these categories University for their many helpful comments during the develop-
attention should be paid to formally testing significance for any ment of this paper.
evidence of variation. Finally, variation by sex/gender in associated
features, biomarkers, co-morbidities and developmental trajecto-
References
ries could be cataloged. Gaining a better understanding overall of
heterogeneity in this ratio should provide clues about underlying Abrahams, B. S., & Geschwind, D. H. (2008). Advances in autism genetics: on the
mechanisms. threshold of a new neurobiology. Nature Reviews Genetics, 9(5), 341e355.
Testing the hypothesis that female gender socialization acts as Amiet, C., Gourfinkel-An, I., Bouzamondo, A., Tordjman, S., Baulac, M., Lechat, P.,
et al. (2008). Epilepsy in autism is associated with intellectual disability and
a protective factor for ASD will be theoretically and logistically gender: evidence from a meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 64(7), 577e582.
challenging, and will need to include the following steps: 1) Iden- Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, CDC. (2009). Preva-
tification of specific socially-based exposures associated with lence of autism spectrum disorders - autism and developmental disabilities
monitoring network, United States, 2006. MMWR Surveillance Summaries,
gender, particularly in infancy, that could hypothetically affect 58(10), 1e20.
probability of ASD manifestation in a vulnerable infant; for Balaban, E. (2006). Cognitive developmental biology: history, process and fortune’s
example, interpretive versus instructive statements. 2) Qualitative wheel. Cognition, 101(2), 298e332.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Autism: The empathizing-systemizing (e-s) theory. Annals
and quantitative measurement of exposure to these factors in of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 68e80.
infants at risk for ASD, which will include observing interaction Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte, M. K. (2005). Sex differences in the
with caregivers in as naturalistic a setting as possible. In order to brain: implications for explaining autism. Science, 310(5749), 819e823.
Bhasin, T. K., & Schendel, D. (2007). Sociodemographic risk factors for autism in a US
gain sufficient power, given low overall prevalence of ASDs,
metropolitan area. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4),
a prospective approach could focus on a high-risk population, such 667e677.
as “baby sibs” of children with ASD diagnoses. However, because Bilder, D., Pinborough-Zimmerman, J., Miller, J., & McMahon, W. (2009). Prenatal,
the additional cases from such families will by definition arise from perinatal, and neonatal factors associated with autism spectrum disorders.
Pediatrics, 123(5), 1293e1300.
“multiplex” (more than one child affected) families, which may Burnham, D. K., & Harris, M. B. (1992). Effects of real gender and labeled gender on
have different etiology than simplex families, results from this adults’ perceptions of infants. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153(2), 165e183.
1674 K. Cheslack-Postava, R.M. Jordan-Young / Social Science & Medicine 74 (2012) 1667e1674

Carter, C. S. (2007). Sex differences in oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents differential socialization of boys and
autism spectrum disorders? Behavioural Brain Research, 176(1), 170e186. girls - a metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267e296.
Clearfield, M. W., & Nelson, N. M. (2006). Sex differences in mothers’ speech and Majdan, M., & Shatz, C. J. (2006). Effects of visual experience on activity-dependent
play behavior with 6-, 9-, and 14-month-old infants. Sex Roles, 54, 127e137. gene regulation in cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 9(5), 650e659.
Condry, J., & Condry, S. (1976). Sex-differences - study of eye of beholder. Child Mandell, D. S., Listerud, J., Levy, S. E., & Pinto-Martin, J. A. (2002). Race differences in
Development, 47, 812e819. the age at diagnosis among medicaid-eligible children with autism. Journal of
Constantino, J. N., Zhang, Y., Frazier, T., Abbacchi, A. M., & Law, P. (2010). Sibling the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(12), 1447e1453.
recurrence and the genetic epidemiology of autism. American Journal of Mandell, D. S., Novak, M. M., & Zubritsky, C. D. (2005). Factors associated with age of
Psychiatry, 167(11), 1349e1356. diagnosis among children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 116(6),
Cuccaro, M. L., Wright, H. H., Rownd, C. V., Abramson, R. K., Waller, J., & Fender, D. 1480e1486.
(1996). Professional perceptions of children with developmental difficulties: Mandell, D. S., Wiggins, L. D., Carpenter, L. A., Daniels, J., DiGuiseppi, C.,
the influence of race and socioeconomic status. Journal of Autism and Devel- Durkin, M. S., et al. (2009). Racial/ethnic disparities in the identification of
opmental Disorders, 26(4), 461e469. children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Public Health,
Culp, R. E., Cook, A. S., & Housley, P. C. (1983). A comparison of observed and 99(3), 493e498.
reported adult-infant interactions: effects of perceived sex. Sex Roles, 9(4), Marco, E. J., & Skuse, D. H. (2006). Autism-lessons from the X chromosome. Social
475e479. Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 183e193.
Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the preven- Institute of Medicine. (2003). Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic
tion of autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), disparities in healthcare. Washington DC: National Academies Press.
775e803. Mello, C. V. (2002). Mapping vocal communication pathways in birds with inducible
Dean, S. L., & McCarthy, M. M. (2008). Steroids, sex and the cerebellar cortex: gene expression. Journal of Comparative Physiology A Neuroethology, Sensory,
implications for human disease. Cerebellum, 7(1), 38e47. Neural and Behavioural Physiology, 188(11e12), 943e959.
Delk, J. L., Madden, R. B., Livingston, M., & Ryan, T. T. (1986). Adult perceptions of the Miles, J. H., Takahashi, T. N., Bagby, S., Sahota, P. K., Vaslow, D. F., Wang, C. H., et al.
infant as a function of gender labeling and observer gender. Sex Roles, 15, 527e534. (2005). Essential versus complex autism: definition of fundamental prognostic
Donovan, W., Taylor, N., & Leavitt, L. (2007). Maternal sensory sensitivity and response subtypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 135(2), 171e180.
bias in detecting change in infant facial expressions: maternal self-efficacy and Mondschein, E. R., Adolph, K. E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2000). Gender bias in
infant gender labeling. Infant Behavior & Development, 30, 436e452. mothers’ expectations about infant crawling. Journal of Experimental Child
Durkin, M. S., Maenner, M. J., Meaney, F. J., Levy, S. E., DiGuiseppi, C., Nicholas, J. S., Psychology, 77(4), 304e316.
et al. (2010). Socioeconomic inequality in the prevalence of autism spectrum Nash, A., & Krawczyk, R. (1994). Boys’ and girls’ rooms revisited: The contents of
disorder: evidence from a U.S. cross-sectional study. PLoS One, 5(7), e11551. boys’ and girls’ rooms in the 1990s. Conference on Human Development.
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2005). Bare bones of sex: part I, sex & gender. Signs, 30(2), Pittsburgh, PA.
1491e1528. Newschaffer, C. J., Croen, L. A., Daniels, J., Giarelli, E., Grether, J. K., Levy, S. E., et al.
Fine, C. (2010). Delusions of gender: How our minds, society, and neurosexism create (2007). The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annual Review of Public
difference. New York, NY: Norton. Health, 28, 235e258.
Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychological Medicine, Nicholas, J. S., Charles, J. M., Carpenter, L. A., King, L. B., Jenner, W., & Spratt, E. G.
29(4), 769e786. (2008). Prevalence and characteristics of children with autism-spectrum
Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive disorders. Annals of Epidemiology, 18(2), 130e136.
developmental disorders: an update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Ouellette-Kuntz, H. M., Coo, H., Lam, M., Yu, C. T., Breitenbach, M. M.,
Disorders, 33(4), 365e382. Hennessey, P. E., et al. (2009). Age at diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders in
Fombonne, E. (2009). Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatric four regions of Canada. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 100(4), 268e273.
Research, 65(6), 591e598. Page, M., Wilhelm, M. S., Gamble, W. C., & Card, N. A. (2010). A comparison of
Goldberg, N. (2002). Women are not small men: Life-saving strategies for preventing maternal sensitivity and verbal stimulation as unique predictors of infant
and healing heart disease in women. New York: Ballantine Books. social-emotional and cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development,
Goldstein, N., & Manlowe, J. L. (1997). The gender politics of HIV/AIDS in women: 33(1), 101e110.
Perspectives on the pandemic in the United States. New York: New York Univer- Pomerleau, A., Bolduc, D., Malcuit, G., & Cossette, L. (1990). Pink or blue - envi-
sity Press. ronmental gender stereotypes in the 1st 2 years of life. Sex Roles, 22, 359e367.
Grossi, G., & Fine, C. (in press). The role of fetal testosterone in the development of Rubin, J. Z., Provenzano, F. J., & Luria, Z. (1974). The eye of the beholder: parents‘
"The essential difference" between the sexes: Some essential issues. In R. views on sex of newborns. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44(4), 512e519.
Bluhm, A. J. Jacobson, & H. Maibom (Eds.), Neurofeminism: Issues at the Russell, G., Steer, C., & Golding, J. (in press). Social and demographic factors that
intersection of feminist theory and cognitive neuroscience. New York, NY: influence the diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Social Psychiatry and
Palgrave Macmillan. Psychiatric Epidemiology.
Harris, A. (2009). Gender as soft assembly. New York: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group. Schendel, D., & Bhasin, T. K. (2008). Birth weight and gestational age characteristics
Jordan-Young, R. M. (2010) Brain storm: The flaws in the science of sex differences. of children with autism, including a comparison with other developmental
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. disabilities. Pediatrics, 121(6), 1155e1164.
Kaminsky, Z., Wang, S. C., & Petronis, A. (2006). Complex disease, gender and Schendel, D. E., Autry, A., Wines, R., & Moore, C. (2009). The co-occurrence of autism
epigenetics. Annals of Medicine, 38(8), 530e544. and birth defects: Prevalence and risk in a population-based cohort. Develop-
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51(10), 779e786.
217e250. Seavey, C. A., Katz, P. A., & Zalk, S. R. (1975). Baby X. Sex Roles, 1(2), 103e109.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Atypical epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10(1), 84e88. Shattuck, P. T., Durkin, M., Maenner, M., Newschaffer, C., Mandell, D. S., Wiggins, L.,
Keen, D. V., Reid, F. D., & Arnone, D. (2010). Autism, ethnicity and maternal immi- et al. (2009). Timing of identification among children with an autism spectrum
gration. British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 274e281. disorder: findings from a population-based surveillance study. Journal of the
Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Fane, B. A., Wheelwright, S., Mathews, G. A., American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 474e483.
Conway, G. S., et al. (2006). Androgens and autistic traits: a study of individuals Sidorowicz, L. S., & Lunney, G. S. (1980). Baby X revisited. Sex Roles, 6, 67e73.
with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 50(1), 148e153. Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism
Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., & Taylor, K. (2005). Foetal testosterone, predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal
social relationships, and restricted interests in children. Journal of Child of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77e89.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(2), 198e210. Sng, J., & Meaney, M. J. (2009). Environmental regulation of the neural epigenome.
Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2006). Fetal Epigenomics, 1(1), 131e151.
testosterone and empathy. Hormones and Behavior, 49(3), 282e292. Springer, K. W., Stellman, J. M., & Jordan-Young, R. M. (2011). Beyond a catalogue of
Kolevzon, A., Gross, R., & Reichenberg, A. (2007). Prenatal and perinatal risk factors differences: a theoretical frame and good practice guidelines for researching sex/
for autism: a review and integration of findings. Archives of Pediatrics and gender in human health. Social Science & Medicine. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.
Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 326e333. 2011.05.033, in this special issue.
Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Girls with social deficits and learning problems: Stern, M., & Karraker, K. H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: a review of gender
autism, atypical asperger syndrome or a variant of these condidtions. European labeling studies. Sex Roles, 20, 501e522.
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 1(2), 89e99. Thompson, T., Caruso, M., & Ellerbeck, K. (2003). Sex matters in autism and other
Leeb, R. T., & Rejskind, F. G. (2004). Here’s looking at you, kid! A longitudinal study developmental disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 7(4), 345e362.
of perceived gender differences in mutual gaze behavior in young infants. Sex Volkmar, F. R., Szatmari, P., & Sparrow, S. S. (1993). Sex differences in pervasive
Roles, 50, 1e14. developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(4),
Levy, S. E., Mandell, D. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2009). Autism. Lancet, 374(9701), 579e591.
1627e1638. Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Rice, C., Karapurkar, T., Doernberg, N., Boyle, C., & Murphy, C.
Lyons, J. A., & Serbin, L. A. (1986). Observer bias in scoring boys and girls agression. (2003). Prevalence of autism in a US metropolitan area. Journal of the American
Sex Roles, 14, 301e313. Medical Association, 289(1), 49e55.

S-ar putea să vă placă și