Sunteți pe pagina 1din 13

Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior

Intrafamilial physical victimization and externalizing behavior problems: Who


remain the ‘forgotten’ children?
Lynette M. Renner ⁎
School of Social Work, The University of Iowa, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Over the past few decades, researchers have documented positive associations between direct child maltreat-
Received 15 September 2011 ment and exposure to interpersonal violence (including intimate partner violence, community violence,
Received in revised form 9 December 2011 school violence, and media violence) and children's externalizing behavior problems. However, current fam-
Accepted 9 December 2011
ily violence literature largely ignores the effects of child abuse on other children in the family. A handful of
Available online 17 December 2011
studies have focused on exposure to child abuse and documented the behavioral effects on siblings, and
Keywords:
these studies lend support for broadening scholarship focused on this type of family violence. This article pre-
Family violence sents empirical research and theories that focus on the relationships between child physical abuse and expo-
Child maltreatment sure to intimate partner violence and children's externalizing behavior problems. Using this literature as a
Physical abuse foundation, an argument is made for the need to focus on children's exposure to child physical abuse. This
Victimization article presents information from the few studies that have focused on children who were exposed to the
Externalizing behaviors physical abuse of a sibling and offers theoretical frameworks, including social learning theory and psycholog-
Sibling ical proximity, as a foundation for future research. The article concludes with a discussion of services that
may be necessary for children who have been exposed to the physical abuse of a sibling, including services
focused on safety and mental health.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 158
2. Review of the literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159
2.1. Child physical abuse and externalizing behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159
2.2. Exposure to intimate partner violence and externalizing behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159
2.3. Exposure to other forms of interpersonal violence and externalizing behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 160
2.4. Exposure to child physical abuse and externalizing behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 160
3. Theoretical frameworks that focus on violence and child functioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164
3.1. The effects of child physical abuse . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164
3.2. The effects of exposure to intimate partner violence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164
3.3. The effects of exposure to child physical abuse . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165
3.3.1. Proximity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 166
4. Implications for research and practice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 166
4.1. Safety and protective services . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
4.2. Mental health services . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
5. Summary and conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 168

1. Introduction

Interpersonal violence is a serious public health concern that affects


⁎ The University of Iowa, School of Social Work, 308 North Hall, Iowa City, IA 52242,
millions of people. It is estimated that 60% of American children have
United States. been exposed to violence, crime, or abuse in their homes, schools, and
E-mail address: lynette-renner@uiowa.edu. communities (Finkelhor, Turner, Ormrod, & Hamby, 2009). Two of the

1359-1789/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.12.003
L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170 159

more prevalent forms of family violence – child physical abuse and ex- Hansen, 2011); however, the developmental effects of children's expo-
posure to intimate partner violence (IPV1) – affect millions of children. sure to child abuse remain grossly understudied.
According to the Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse The objective of this article is to utilize research on the effects of
and Neglect (NIS-4; Sedlak et al., 2010), more than 1.25 million chil- child physical abuse and exposure to physical IPV on children's exter-
dren experienced maltreatment during 2005–2006. Of these children, nalizing behaviors as a rationale for broadening the scope of family vio-
44% were abused (61% were neglected) and physical abuse was the lence. Highlighting tenets of social learning theory and proximity, this
majority type of abuse experienced (58% of the abused children, an article provides information as to why children exposed to a the physi-
estimated total of 323,000). During 2009, an estimated 3.3 million re- cal abuse of a sibling may exhibit behavioral characteristics that are sim-
ferrals, involving the alleged maltreatment of approximately six mil- ilar to children who are physically abused or exposed to physical IPV
lion children, were received by CPS agencies in the United States and may require similar safety and mental health interventions.
(USDHHS, 2010). Of these, an estimated 702,000 children were vic-
tims of maltreatment, with 17.8% having been physically abused. Em-
2. Review of the literature
pirical research estimates 15.5 million children live in families in
which physical IPV occurred at least once in the previous year, with
2.1. Child physical abuse and externalizing behaviors
seven million children living in families in which severe IPV occurred
(McDonald, Jouriles, Ramisetty-Mikler, Caetano, & Green, 2006). A
Although early documentation on the severity of child physical
national study on victimization estimates 16.3% of children ages
abuse was published by Dr. Ambrose Tardieu in France in 1860 (cited
0–17 witness an assault between their parents in their lifetime (Fin-
in Smith, 1975; cited in Wallace, 1996), very little empirical research in-
kelhor et al., 2009).
volving this population was published prior to the 1960s. In 1961, at an
Due to the initial inattention in research, children exposed to IPV
American Academy of Pediatrics symposium, Dr. C. Henry Kempe
have been referred to as ‘forgotten’ (Elbow, 1982), ‘silent’
coined the term ‘battered child syndrome’ (Radbill, 1968; Wallace,
(Augustyn, Parker, Groves, & Zuckerman, 1995; Groves, Zuckerman,
1996). Beginning with the literature on ‘battered child syndrome’
Marans, & Cohen, 1993; Kolar & Davey, 2007), ‘invisible’ (Osofsky,
(Helfer & Kempe, 1968; Kempe, Silverman, Steele, Droegemueller, &
1998) and ‘unintended’ (Rosenbaum & O'Leary, 1981) victims. In
Silver, 1962; Parker, 1965), scholars have increasingly examined the
1982, Pfouts, Schopler and Henley used the word ‘forgotten’ to de-
outcomes of children who have been physically abused. Over the past
scribe children exposed to IPV and children exposed to the abuse of
40 years, a variety of articles and books have been published on child
a sibling. However, in light of the abundance of high-quality empirical
maltreatment, and research on the effects of child physical abuse has
research and practice literature focused on children's exposure to IPV
cited increased medical and neurological, intellectual and cognitive, be-
over past few decades, this term may no longer accurately apply to
havioral, emotional and psychiatric problems (see a brief summary of
this population. The word ‘forgotten’, however, arguably still applies
findings in Miller-Perrin & Perrin, 2007).
to children exposed to the physical abuse of a sibling. 2
Over the past several decades, research specifically on the associ-
Prevalence data on children's exposure to child physical abuse is
ation between child physical abuse and externalizing behaviors has
lacking. However, research conducted with a national sample of 1467
dramatically increased. There is no consensus in the operational def-
children, ages 2–17, found that 7.2% experienced physical abuse, 6.8%
inition of externalizing behavior problems (Keil & Price, 2006); yet
were exposed to IPV, and 2.8% were exposed to the physical abuse of
externalizing problems often refer to a grouping of behaviors that
a sibling (Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2009).
are manifested in children's outward behavior and reflect the child
Empirical research devoted to examining the effects of child mal-
negatively acting on the external environment. Externalizing behav-
treatment and exposure to IPV, community violence, school violence,
iors may include hyperactivity, delinquency, and subtypes of aggres-
and media violence and other forms of trauma on children's develop-
sion (Liu, 2004).
ment have flourished in the past few decades and shown rather con-
Many studies have documented associations between child phys-
sistent findings — especially with regard to children's increased
ical abuse and increased externalizing behavior problems, specifically
externalizing behaviors. Yet, despite increased research on each of
aggressive behaviors, among children (Connor, Doerfler, Volungis,
these types of interpersonal violence, most studies on the effects of
Steingard, & Melloni, 2003; Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1997; Dodge,
family violence remain focused on limited forms. In addition, studies
Pettit, Bates, & Valente, 1995; Feldman et al., 1995; Johnson et al.,
on direct child maltreatment focus primarily on one child in the fam-
2002; Manly, Kim, Rogosch, & Cicchetti, 2001; Prino & Peyrot, 1994;
ily – the child who is maltreated – and do not account for how other
Salzinger, Feldman, Hammer, & Rosario, 1993; Salzinger et al., 2002;
children in the family experience the abuse.
Shields & Cicchetti, 2001; Wolfe & Mosk, 1983). Physical abuse during
Children who are not directly abused but are exposed to the abuse
childhood also increases the likelihood of exhibiting later delinquen-
of a sibling may also be negatively affected. However, this group of
cy and violence perpetration (Fang & Corso, 2007; Fergusson &
children is not widely recognized by mental health and social service
Lynskey, 1997; Hawkins et al., 1998; Herrenkohl, Huang, Tajima, &
professionals or scholars. Some recent studies have noted that siblings
Whitney, 2003; Lansford et al., 2007; Mass, Herrenkohl, & Sousa,
and other non-offending family members are negatively affected by
2008; Smith & Thornberry, 1995; Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber,
incidents of child sexual abuse (Baker, Tanis, & Rice, 2001; Tavkar &
Homish, & Wei, 2001). Other researchers have found support for the
relationship between parental physical discipline and children's in-
creased externalizing behaviors (Gershoff, 2002; Lansford et al.,
1
2011). The literature accumulated over the past few decades provides
Intimate partner violence (IPV) refers to acts of physical, sexual, and/or psycholog-
ical harm by current or former adult partners or spouses. It can also involve financial
significant empirical support for the association between direct child
abuse, threats of harm, and controlling and coercive behaviors. IPV exists along a con- physical abuse and increased externalizing behaviors.
tinuum from a single episode of violence to ongoing battering.
2
Exposure to the physical abuse of a sibling (where the perpetrator is often a parent,
the victim is a child, and the victim's sibling is exposed to the abuse) is a distinct type 2.2. Exposure to intimate partner violence and externalizing behaviors
of family violence that is separate from sibling abuse. Sibling abuse or sibling violence
refers to acts of abuse between siblings (where the perpetrator and victim are sib- Along with research on direct child physical abuse, it is important
lings). This type of family violence has garnered significant attention over the past sev-
eral years (see Eriksen & Jensen, 2006; Felson, 1983; Finkelhor, Turner, & Ormrod,
to understand how exposure to physical IPV is also associated with
2006; Kiselica & Morrill-Richards, 2007; Linares, 2006); however, it is important to dis- children's externalizing behaviors. Similar to empirical research on
tinguish between these two separate types of family violence. the effects of child physical abuse, research on children's exposure
160 L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170

to physical IPV has cited increased cognitive, behavioral, emotional, of familial and non-familial violence. The negative effects of direct
physical, and social problems. child physical abuse and exposure to physical IPV on child functioning
One of the first articles addressing domestic violence was published have been well-documented in family violence literature; however,
in 1960 (Schultz, 1960) and a high level of interest in studying this adult the occurrence of family violence affects every member of the family
population emerged in the years following. Studies on children's expo- system. Just as children who are exposed to IPV have been found to
sure to IPV emerged in the 1970s (see Levine, 1975; Moore, 1975) and display adjustment difficulties similar to children who have been
have drawn significant attention from researchers ever since. Early physically abused, children who are exposed to the physical abuse
studies on children's exposure to IPV focused on directly ‘witnessing’ of a sibling are not likely immune to adverse effects.
or ‘observing’ violence. However, more recent studies have used the According to Howells and Rosenbaum (2008), “No attention has
more inclusive term of ‘exposure’ to intimate partner violence and ac- been paid to the potential effects of seeing a sibling being abused by
knowledge there are several ways beyond being an eyewitness that a their parental figures” (p. 203). Indeed, very little recent research
child can be negatively affected by domestic violence. Holden's (2003) has been conducted with regard to the effects of violence on siblings
taxonomy separated exposure into ten categories representing both di- exposed to child physical abuse. However, it seems reasonable to ex-
rect (e.g., child intervenes to stop violence; child observes the assault) pect that children exposed to the physical abuse of a sibling would
and indirect (e.g., child learns of violence through another person; display behavior problems similar to children exposed to other
child sees aftermath of the assault) involvement. forms of violence, such as IPV or community violence. When compar-
As research efforts have increased over the past few decades, the ing children who were abused and their siblings, Kempe and Kempe
association between exposure to IPV and externalizing behaviors, in- (1976) concluded that when a child was abused and his/her sibling
cluding aggression, violence, and delinquency, has become well- was also “adequately evaluated” (p. 123), all children displayed se-
established (Graham-Bermann & Levendosky, 1998; Herrera & vere emotional pathology resulting from the psychological abuse
McCloskey, 2001; Ireland & Smith, 2009; Kernic et al., 2003; and neglect that often accompanies physical abuse. Beezley, Martin,
Litrownik, Newton, Hunter, English, & Everson, 2003; Mahoney, and Alexander (1976) wrote about concerns for children exposed to
Donnelly, Boxer, & Lewis, 2003; McCloskey & Lichter, 2003). Several the physical abuse of a sibling and stated that “the impact of witnes-
reviews and meta-analyses have also documented the relationship sing or being aware of abusive behavior toward a sibling must be a
between exposure to IPV and externalizing behavior problems momentous, psychological trauma” (p. 183).
among children and adolescents (Carlson, 2000; Carpenter & Stacks, A handful of studies conducted in the 1970s and 1980s provided a
2009; Evans, Davies, & DiLillo, 2008; Fowler & Chanmugam, 2007; foundation for this line of inquiry; yet, little research on siblings ex-
Holt, Buckley, & Whelan, 2008; Kitzmann, Gaylord, Holt, & Kenny, posed to child physical abuse has been conducted since. Table 1 pre-
2003; Rudo, Powell, & Dunlap, 1998; Wolfe, Crooks, Lee, McIntyre- sents a summary of research studies that have included siblings
Smith, & Jaffe, 2003). (referred to as nonabused 3 children in some studies) exposed to
child physical abuse. Because so few studies have been conducted,
2.3. Exposure to other forms of interpersonal violence and externalizing Table 1 lists findings across multiple domains of child functioning.
behaviors Those studies that specifically focused on externalizing behaviors
are summarized in the following paragraphs.
Research on children's exposure to community violence, school vi- Following a modeling framework (e.g., social learning theory),
olence, and media violence has also grown over several years. Studies Bolton, Reich, and Gutierres (1977) compared four groups of children
on the behavioral effects of exposure to these non-familial forms of (children who were abused, siblings of children who were abused,
violence have yielded results similar to studies on child physical control children, control siblings) and found that victims of abuse
abuse and children's exposure to IPV. For example, in a sample of had a low frequency (7.8%) of aggressive crimes (e.g., fighting, as-
youth ages 12 to 17 years (N = 423), McCabe, Hough, Yeh, Lucchini, sault) while their siblings had a much higher frequency (17.2%). It
and Hazen (2005) found exposure to community violence contribut- was hypothesized that siblings were more likely to model behaviors
ed to the development of conduct disorder and externalizing symp- that did not appear to have consequences and that children who
toms, after controlling for child maltreatment and exposure to IPV. were abused viewed aggression as hurtful and were less likely to re-
Using a sample of 935 high-school students, O'Keefe (1997) found peat such behaviors. Jean-Gilles and Crittenden (1990) examined the
that exposure to community and school violence was a significant influence of children's shared environment and found children who
predictor of aggression among boys after controlling for the effects were maltreated and their siblings scored significantly above average
of family violence and exposure to school violence significantly pre- on a measure of stressful life events but only 12.5% of children in both
dicted aggression among girls. Other studies have documented in- groups scored above average on behavior problems.
creased externalizing/aggressive behaviors among children and More recent research on siblings exposed to child physical abuse
adolescents who have been exposed to community violence has substantiated the results of earlier studies. For example, in a
(Guerra, Huesmann, & Spindler, 2003; see reviews by Lynch, 2003; study of 2402 children ages 3–18, Renner (2005) found that siblings
Margolin & Gordis, 2000; McDonald & Richmond, 2008; see meta- (ages 6–12) exposed to child physical abuse had higher externalizing
analysis by Fowler, Tompsett, Braciszewski, Jacques-Tiura, & Baltes, behavior scores compared to children in the same age group who had
2009), exposed to school violence (Janosz et al., 2008; Mrug, Loosier, experienced direct child physical abuse or been exposed to IPV. An-
& Windle, 2008; Mrug & Windle, 2010), or exposed to television and other study that focused on 441 children who each experienced a sin-
media violence, including on-line gaming and playing first-person gle type of family violence, Renner (accepted for publication), found
shooters (Anderson et al., 2010; Brown & Witherspoon, 2002; that children ages 3–5 and adolescents ages 13–18 who were exposed
Bushman & Huesmann, 2006; Christakis & Zimmerman, 2007;
Gentile, Lynch, Linder, & Walsh, 2004; Holtz & Appel, 2011; 3
Similar to debates regarding exposure to IPV (see Edleson, Gassman-Pines, & Hill,
Huesmann, Moise-Titus, Podolski, & Eron, 2003; Huesmann & Taylor, 2006; Kantor & Little, 2003; Rankin & Ornstein, 2009; Somer & Braunstein, 1999), it
2006). is important to acknowledge that exposure to child physical abuse could arguably be
considered a unique or existing form (e.g., psychological abuse, neglect) of child mal-
2.4. Exposure to child physical abuse and externalizing behaviors treatment. The term ‘nonabused’ that occurs periodically in this article reflects lan-
guage used in the original sources and refers to siblings that have not been
physically abused. It does not imply the children have never experienced another type
As previously stated, empirical research has shown increased of maltreatment, that they are not at risk of physical abuse, or that exposure to physical
externalizing behaviors among children exposed to multiple types abuse does not constitute child maltreatment.
Table 1
Studies that focused on children who were abused and their siblings.

Author Sample Domains assessed Key measures and/or procedures Study results related to siblings of children who
(Year of publication) were physically abused

Bolton et al. (1977) 774 children referred to the state for 1 of 4 types Criminal behavior Escape crimes included: runaway, truancy, and Children who were abused had a lower
(physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, missing juvenile. Aggressive crimes included: frequency of aggressive crimes and a higher
physical abuse/neglect,) of child abuse and their disturbing the peace, fighting, assault, assault frequency of escape crimes compared to their
sibling comprised the experimental sample. 900 with a deadly weapon, aggravated assault and sibling. Children who were abused had a higher
youth offenders selected through the juvenile battery, armed robbery, and strong arm robbery. frequency of escape crimes than the other three
court and their sibling served as a control groups. Siblings of children who were abused
sample. Each group was divided into “primary had the highest frequency of aggressive crimes
targets” and “siblings” for a total of four groups compared to the other three groups. Siblings of
(abused, sibling of abused, control, sibling of children who were abused had a higher
control). frequency of escape crimes than the two control

L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170


groups.

Halperin (1981, 1983) 20 abusive families and 20 control families with Children’s perceptions of their 10–15 min, semi-structured interviews were No differences were found in the number of
at least one natural parent residing in the home family members used. Each child was asked to describe his/her positive or negative words used by children
and two children between ages 5–12. A total of mother, father, and sibling. The number of neg- who were abused and their siblings when
20 abused children (8 boys, 12 girls ages ative, positive, and neutral words was describing their parents or each other. Children
7–11 years), 20 nonabused siblings (7 boys, 13 documented. who were abused viewed their family members
girls ages 6–10), and 40 control children from less positively and more negatively than
20 families that had never been reported for children in the control families. Siblings from
child abuse. abusive families viewed their family members
less positively than siblings from control
families.

Herrenkohl and 579 children. Of these children, 295 targets of Physical health and development Case records were examined for type of abuse Abused children had shorter gestation periods
Herrenkohl (1979) one or more types of abuse and 284 nonabused Parents' perceptions of the child and child involved. Medical data was gathered than their nonabused siblings. Children who
siblings. Boys = 50.9% of targets and 55.5% of on child's birth weight, weeks of gestation, age had more infancy-related and developmental
nontargets. of mother at delivery, pregnancy and delivery problems than their sibling were more likely to
complications. Interviews were conducted with be targets of life-threatening neglect. Children
151 female heads and 66 male heads of who were abused were described by their
household. Interviews assessed the quality of mothers as having more emotional difficulties
the marital relationship, family stressors, (e.g. excessive eating or refusal to eat, pica,
parent’s experiences of childhood discipline, frequent temper tantrums, moodiness, sleeping
and the quality of the parental relationship with problems). Children who were abused were
each child. said to have negative qualities that reminded
parents of negative aspects of themselves as
children. Mothers perceived themselves as
having less control and influence over their
abused children than they had over their
nonabused children.

Howells and Rosenbaum 675 college students (307 males, 392 females) Behavior Measures of aggression and depressive Participants who witnessed the abuse of their
(2008) who experienced child physical abuse or symptoms were used. Participants were sibling by both parents during childhood had
exposure to IPV before age 18. The analysis classified into one of four groups based on their significantly higher depression and aggression
focused on witnessing the abuse of a sibling exposure to violence. Participants provided scores than participants in the nonwitness
included 385 females. information on the gender and relationship of group. Mean scores for depression were 7.54
the perpetrator when applicable. and 4.28, while means for aggression were
32.38 and 21.29, respectively.

(continued on next page)

161
162
Table 1 (continued)
Author Sample Domains assessed Key measures and/or procedures Study results related to siblings of children who
(Year of publication) were physically abused

Jean-Gilles and Crittenden Subsample of a dataset of 542 maltreating and Behavior Data collection consisted of 2–3 home visits. All Maltreated children and their siblings were
(1990) normative families. Three groups of families Life events members of the family participated in the examined for the effects of shared environment
(families under investigation for maltreatment, Home environment assessment. The assessment included measures influences. Both maltreated children and their

L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170


community sample of volunteers, and families focused on life events, physical and siblings experienced stressful life events that
served by mental health agencies) were interpersonal aspects of the home environment, were significantly above average and one-third
included. The subsample included 82 families child behavior problems, and the severity of of children in these two groups were in the
with two children (a focal child and a sibling) maltreatment. lowest quartile on the home environment scale.
both between ages 2–6 years. Twenty-eight of 12.5% of maltreated children and 12.5% of their
the families were maltreating, 45 were recruited siblings had behavior problem scores above the
from the community, and nine were referred by published norms.
mental health agencies. The focal children had a
mean age of 49.7 months and the siblings had a
mean age of 53.7 months; there were an equal
number of boys and girls.

Johnson and Morse (1968) 101 children identified as abused (and 167 who Physical health and development No specific measures were used. Data came Children who were abused were more aggressive
were not) from 85 families involved with the state's Behavior from police and hospital reports and child toward their siblings than vice versa. “Less than half
Division of Services for Children and Youth welfare workers' descriptions of the children. of the children who were abused showed
Department of Child Welfare. Instances of sexual satisfactory mental and emotional development”
abuse and malnutrition were excluded. Cases and the “uninjured children fared a little better”.
involving children over age 14 were also excluded. Both “injured and uninjured” children were “shy,
Children were considered to be physically injured gloomy and passive” and all children were
(i.e., abused) or uninjured. “deprived of parental care and affection” (p. 150).

Lynch (1975, 1976) 25 children who were abused and had one or more Physical health Information on prenatal and perinatal Mothers reported significantly more abnormal
siblings (n=35) who were not abused. experiences and early life histories was pregnancies, admissions to the hospital during their
gathered from detailed case histories for pregnancy and abnormal labor/deliveries with
children who were abused and their siblings. their child who was later abused compared to the
Factors of interest included abnormal siblings. Children who were abused were also more
pregnancy, whether the mother was admitted likely to have been separated from their mothers
to hospital during pregnancy, abnormal labor/ within the first 48 h and within the first six months
delivery, neonatal separation while in hospital of life compared to their siblings. Seventy-six
or other separations within the first six months, percent (n=19) of the children who were abused
and illness in the child’s first year of life. These had two or more adverse factors in their early lives,
risk factors were used separately and were while 71% (n=25) of their siblings had none.
combined to create a measure of the total
number of adverse factors for each child.

Nakou, Adam, Stathacopoulou, 25 families in which a child b10 years old was Physical health Medical histories, pediatric examinations and Children who were abused had poorer nutrition,
and Agathonos (1982) maltreated to the degree of requiring medical psychosocial assessments were used to gather more feeding problems, more severe handicaps,
intervention. The sample included 32 children (23 information on premature birth, illness in first and were more often born prematurely compared
males, 9females) who had been physically abused months, severe handicap (e.g., severe mental to their siblings.
or neglected by a parent and their 53 siblings (23 retardation, spasticity), milder handicap (e.g.,
males, 30 females). epilepsy, mild mental retardation), poor
nutrition, and feeding problems.
Table 1 (continued)
Author Sample Domains assessed Key measures and/or procedures Study results related to siblings of children who
(Year of publication) were physically abused

Pfouts, Schopler, and Henley 73 families with confirmed incidents of child abuse Academic Information on each child’s academics and In school, 20% of children were withdrawn and 10%
(1982) and IPV. Of the 141 children in the sample, 25 Behavior internalizing behaviors was collected through reportedly acted out with their teachers. Children
witnessed the abuse of their mother and another case records and interviews with the social who witnessed the abuse of a sibling were reported
24 witnessed the abuse of a sibling. worker who was responsible for each family’s to be depressed (37%) and anxious (40%) by their
case. caseworkers. Children who witnessed the abuse of
a sibling had better behavior in school and received
higher grades compared to children who witnessed
the abuse of their mother.

Renner (2005) 2402 children ages 3–18 from 1033 families. The Behavior The child’s caregiver provided information on After controlling for child, parent, and family
sample included three age groups: ages 3–5 years externalizing and internalizing behaviors. characteristics, each type of physical family
(21.69%; 50.29% female), ages 6–12 (52.25%; Externalizing behavior items included how violence was significantly associated with
51.95% female), and, ages 13–18 (26.06%; 50.32% often the child had temper tantrums, fought increased behavior problems for a different age
female). The focus of the study was on three forms with others, threatened or bullied others, got group. Among children ages 3–5, the association
of physical family violence — child physical abuse, angry easily, argued with others, and was was between exposure to IPV and externalizing
exposure to physical IPV, and exposure to child aggressive toward people/objects. Internalizing behavior problem scores, among children ages
physical abuse. behavior items included how often the child 6–12, the association was between exposure to
showed anxiety about being with a group of child physical abuse and externalizing behavior
children, appeared lonely, acted sad or problem scores, and among adolescents ages
depressed, and had low self-esteem. 13–18, the associations were between physical

L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170


abuse and both externalizing and internalizing
behaviors.

Renner (accepted for Subset of 441 children who experienced one of five Behavior The child’s caregiver provided information on Children exposed to a sibling's physical abuse had
publication) types of family violence, including physical abuse, externalizing behaviors. Items included how the highest mean score on the measure of
neglect or sexual abuse, exposure to physical IPV, often the child had temper tantrums, fought externalizing behaviors among 13–18 year olds.
exposure to psychological IPV, and exposure to a with others, threatened or bullied others, got Children exposed to a sibling's physical abuse had
sibling's physical abuse. Children in three age angry easily, argued with others, and was the second highest mean score for externalizing
groups were included: ages 3–5 (n=87), ages 6–12 aggressive toward people/objects. behaviors among the two other age groups (second
(n=233), and ages 13–18 (n=121). to children ages 3–5 who were exposed to
psychological IPV and second to children ages 6–12
who were exposed to physical IPV). All children
exposed to a sibling's physical abuse had higher
mean externalizing behavior scores than children
who were physically abused.

Tanimura, Matsui, and 23 sets of multiple births(22 twins, 1 triplet), Physical health Survey questionnaires were used to assess any A comparison of the abused-nonabused twins
Kobayashi (1990) including 17 pairs of abused-nonabused siblings neonatal and congenital complications for each found that in 16 of the 17 pairs, health problems
and 6 pairs where both children were abused. child. (e.g., serious medical problems, low birth
weight) could only be identified for the abused
child. Most abusers described the abused child
as ‘underdeveloped’, ‘unlovable’, ‘inactive’,
‘unattached to mother’, ‘much trouble’ and
‘autistic and negative attitude’.

Note: Descriptors (e.g., nonabused, targets, witness, uninjured) were taken directly from the original studies.

163
164 L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170

to a sibling's physical abuse had higher mean externalizing scores performance as the main origins of aggression and all three compo-
than children exposed to physical IPV and children who were physi- nents interact in order to learn and repeat violent behavior.
cally abused. Among children ages 6–12, children exposed to physical Social information processing theory has also been used to ac-
IPV had slightly higher externalizing behavior scores than children count for the link between childhood parental abuse and later exter-
exposed to child physical abuse. Children in this age group who nalizing behaviors (Dodge et al., 1995; Dodge et al., 1997). Social
were exposed to physical IPV or physical abuse had higher externaliz- information processing theory posits that children approach situa-
ing behavior scores than children who were physically abused. tions with biologically determined capabilities along with memories
Howells and Rosenbaum (2008) found that female participants who accumulated through past experiences (Crick & Dodge, 1994). The
witnessed the abuse of their sibling had higher aggression and de- way children behaviorally respond to situations is based on how
pression scores than participants in the ‘no exposure’ group. The au- they encode, interpret, and evaluate possible responses (Crick &
thors noted that participants who witnessed the abuse of their Dodge, 1996; Dodge & Crick, 1990). A central hypothesis of social in-
sibling had outcomes that were similar to those found among victims formation processing is that children who interpret information
of child physical abuse. about the behavior and intentions of others in aggressive ways are
more likely to respond with their own aggressive behavior. Children
3. Theoretical frameworks that focus on violence and child who have been abused tend to misread social cues in instances
functioning where threats are unclear or unintended and overestimate the hostile
intentions of others. Through this process, the child or adolescent
3.1. The effects of child physical abuse takes on the role of the aggressor against others who are perceived
as hostile.
Several theories have been used to account for direct effects of Social development model (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996; Hawkins &
victimization on child and adolescent aggressive behavior. One Weis, 1985) integrates social learning theory and social control theo-
hypothesis is that children reenact some version of the behavior ry. This model proposes that antisocial and delinquent behavior in
they experienced from their parent, having learned the uses of vio- youth develops sequentially from socialization in the family, school,
lence. Social learning theory is most often utilized as an explanation and community in which children learn, and are then reinforced for,
for aggressive behavior and is used “almost exclusively to explain poor behavior over time. The biosocial interaction model (Liu, 2004)
how exposure to violent models could lead to elevated externalizing highlights the interaction between biological and social risk factors
behavior problems in children” (Carlson, 2000, p. 323). According to in the development of children's externalizing behaviors.
social learning theory (Bandura, 1973, 1977, 1979, 1986), observa-
tion, imitation, and modeling are mechanisms by which children ex-
hibit aggressive behavior. Physical aggression between family 3.2. The effects of exposure to intimate partner violence
members provides children a model for learning aggressive behaviors
and for viewing violent behaviors within the family as normal and ac- To better understand children's behavioral responses to violence,
ceptable (Bandura, 1973; Feshbach, 1980). Children who are physi- several theories have also been used in research of children exposed
cally abused by a parent or children who are exposed to to IPV. Some studies have found support for the social learning
interparental violence observe angry and aggressive behavior and model in the link between exposure to IPV and adverse developmen-
may interpret it as normative or effective. When aggressive behavior tal outcomes among children (Jouriles, Norwood, McDonald, & Peters,
is modeled and rewarded, children likely learn to model similar be- 2001; McCloskey & Lichter, 2003; O'Keefe, 1996; Spaccarelli,
havior in their own interactions with others, thereby accounting for Coatsworth, & Bowden, 1995). Stress and coping theories have also
increased externalizing behaviors. From the social learning perspec- been used to explain how children respond to IPV, which they inter-
tive, behavior is neither driven by inner forces nor solely attributed pret as placing demands on them that they cannot meet, resulting in a
to environmental influences; rather, it is best understood by recipro- variety of problem-focused and emotion-focused coping responses
cal interactions that continuously occur between individuals and en- (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Exposure to IPV creates stress, fear, and
vironmental determinants (Bandura, 1973). Social learning theory learned helplessness by the child and results in behavioral and emo-
views biological factors, observational learning, and reinforced tional problems (Jaffe, Wolfe, & Wilson, 1990).

• This level includes children who do not witness the violence; however, • This level includes children who witness the violence.
they do hear, know about and/or see the effects of the violence firsthand. • These children are less vulnerable to the effects of violence than
• These children are less vulnerable to the effects of violence than children children in the inner circle, but are still quite vulnerable.
in either of the inner two circles, but they are still vulnerable.

Children exposed to the


physical abuse of a sibling

Victims

• Children in this level experience direct violence.


• This level includes children who have the most distance from the • These children are the most vulnerable to the
violence. They may indirectly witness or learn about the violence effects of violence.
through the media or other sources.
• These children are the least vulnerable to the effects of violence.

Fig. 1. Levels of physical proximity adapted from Rosenfeld (2001).


L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170 165

• This level includes children who know the victims as acquaintances • This level includes children who are socially close to people who
or as friends but are not considered ‘close.’ experience violence, such as members of their family, friends, and
• These children are less vulnerable to the effects of violence than others with whom they have a close personal relationship .
children in the second circle, but they are still vulnerable. • These children are extremely vulnerable to the effects of violence.

Children exposed to the


physical abuse of a sibling

Victims

• This level includes children who identify with the victims. They may
see themselves as similar to the victims in age, family life, etc. • Children in this level experience direct violence.
• Although less vulnerable than children in the other circles, these • These children are the most vulnerable to the
children are still vulnerable to the effects of violence. effects of violence.

Fig. 2. Levels of psychological proximity adapted from Rosenfeld (2001).

Whereas social learning theory can be used to understand how protection (Davies & Woitach, 2008). Within emotional security the-
children exposed to IPV learn to model violence, other models may ory, however, maintaining security is a primary goal in other family
be needed to help explain additional challenges in adjustment after relationships beyond parent–child (Davies & Woitach, 2008). Thus,
violence. These include relationship theories (see Graham-Bermann, emotional insecurity within the family may be interparental and par-
1998), developmental-ecological and developmental psychopatholo- ent–child. Emotional security theory posits that exposure to IPV leads
gy models (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993; Cicchetti & Rizley, 1981; to distress, which leads to behaviors a child uses to maintain emo-
Cicchetti & Toth, 1995; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1998) and a trauma per- tional security. The theory also posits that the degree of emotional se-
spective (Carlson & Dalenberg, 2000). Trauma theory has specifically curity is reflected in a child's ability to regulate emotions and
been put forth to explain the internalizing emotional and cognitive behaviors. Insecurity in the interparental relationship may result in
problems of children who are abused (Graham-Bermann, 1998; a wide range of behaviors including fear, distress and vigilance, ag-
Graham-Bermann & Hughes, 1998; Pynoos, Sorenson, & Steinberg, gressive behaviors, flight behaviors, and avoidance (Davies &
1993). Trauma theory focuses on children's negative adjustment fol- Woitach, 2008). Davies et al. (2002) found that children's insecurity
lowing a traumatic childhood event or experience and posits that “re- in the interparental relationship and attachment insecurity were
peated trauma in childhood forms and deforms the personality” each uniquely associated with concurrent levels of adolescent inter-
(Herman, 1997, p. 96). Using aspects of trauma theory and nalizing symptoms (e.g., depression, anxiety) and externalizing
cognitive-behavioral theory, Briere (1992, 2002) formulated the symptoms (e.g., aggression, delinquency). Emotional security was
self-trauma model which may be used to understand the distorted also identified as an explanatory mechanism for children's internaliz-
cognitive patterns, emotional disturbance, and interpersonal prob- ing and externalizing problems (Cummings, Schermerhorn, Davies,
lems associated with child physical abuse and exposure to IPV. Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2006).
Two other conceptual models have been proposed to explain ad-
justment problems in children exposed to parental conflict, with 3.3. The effects of exposure to child physical abuse
agreement that emotional, cognitive, and behavioral processes are
potential mediators. The cognitive-conceptual framework (Grych & The similar and differential effects of shared experiences of chil-
Fincham, 1990) has been used to understand how children evaluate dren's exposure to IPV (Jaffee, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Arseneault,
conflict, perceive threats and view their own ability to cope as impor- 2002; Richmond & Stocker, 2003; Skopp, McDonald, Manke, &
tant processes in shaping emotional and behavioral responses. Grych, Jouriles, 2005) and child neglect (Hines, Kantor, & Holt, 2006) have
Fincham, Jouriles, and McDonald (2000) expanded the original received some investigation, but “the effects on siblings who have
cognitive-contextual framework and proposed children's appraisals not shared the same experiences, but continue to live with the affect-
of threat and self-blame would mediate their exposure to interparen- ed child, have been relatively ignored” (Newman, Black, & Harris-
tal conflict and adjustment problems. The original framework was ex- Hendriks, 1997, p. 140). Several frameworks may be helpful for un-
panded again to account for acts of violence between adults (Fosco, derstanding why children exposed to the physical abuse of a sibling
DeBoard, & Grych, 2007) and is now more applicable to research on may exhibit a variety of negative outcomes, including increased ex-
the effects of exposure to IPV among children. ternalizing behaviors. These include social learning theory, a stress
Emotional security theory (Davies & Cummings, 1994) shares process model, empathy deficits, and proximity.
some similarities with social learning theory (Davies, Harold, Social learning theory appears to be a promising framework to use
Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002) in that it acknowledges exposure in research focused on the behavioral effects of exposure to child phys-
to IPV has a direct effect on child functioning that cannot be ical abuse, especially when highlighting the transmission of aggressive
accounted for by parenting. However, the theory does highlight a behaviors. Similar to how social learning theory (previously described
more indirect model to explain child functioning. Emotional security in this article) has been used to explain how children who are physical-
theory also shares some similarities with attachment theory in that ly abused and/or exposed to physical IPV develop aggressive behaviors
it acknowledges the harmful effects of parental difficulties (e.g., low (via modeling), children who are exposed to the physical abuse of a
warmth, unresponsiveness) on child adjustment problems due to sibling may also learn that aggressive actions are appropriate to use
the child's difficulties in using the parents as sources of support and in family and peer situations. Thus, children exposed to child physical
166 L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170

abuse may exhibit increased aggressive behaviors due to modeling The authors noted that greater physical proximity to community vio-
other family relationships. Siblings may identify with the aggressor lence may lead to positive reinforcement for the use of violence as a
(e.g., parent) through silent collusion and use physical aggression to- method of problem-solving as well as to emotional numbing that can
ward the child who has been abused (Tower, 1993) or use aggression increase aggressive behaviors. However, physical proximity cannot
as a means of self-protection or as a defense to cover feelings of fear fully explain the vicarious stress reactions found in people who were
that they may be the next child in the family to be abused. However, emotionally close to the victims of trauma (Shahinfar & Fox, 1997)
a child exposed to the physical abuse of their sibling may also realize but were not present for the actual traumatic event. Physical proximity
the pain and suffering such behaviors bring, and siblings with an espe- has been briefly mentioned in relation to children's exposure to IPV
cially close and connected relationship to the abused child may develop (see Groves, 1999); while, psychological proximity has not been read-
internalized behaviors as part of a ‘learned’ process. ily integrated into family violence literature.
Foster and Brooks-Gunn (2009) designed a stress process model Rosenfeld (2001) presented circles of physical vulnerability and
to bring together research in several dominant areas of literature the circles of psychological/social vulnerability to help explain how
focused on violence that are often disconnected. This model specif- children were negatively affected by the events of September 11,
ically focused on understanding children's responses to three forms 2001. Psychological proximity differs from physical proximity be-
of physical violence, including child abuse, exposure to IPV and ex- cause it encompasses an emotional connection between the victim
posure to community violence. The model focused on how multiple of violence and another person. Elements of psychological proximity,
stressors may occur in multiple ecological contexts (e.g., individual, security and attachment to others have been utilized to understand
family, and neighborhood) and how these may directly influence how children are affected by exposure to terrorism and war
child functioning (e.g., externalizing and internalizing behaviors, (Pfefferbaum et al., 2004; Pine, Costello, & Masten, 2005), exposure
academic outcomes). The model also included mediating and mod- to community violence (Bell & Jenkins, 1991; Lynch & Cicchetti,
erating resources at various ecological levels (e.g., child, family, 1998; Pynoos & Nader, 1988), and exposure to a sniper attack on
school, and neighborhood factors) that indirectly influence this re- school grounds (Pynoos et al., 1987). Although proximity is more
lationship. Although presented as a model focused on child physical often used in studies focusing on children's exposure to war or com-
abuse, exposure to IPV, and exposure to community violence, this munity violence, it can be extended to research on family violence.
stress process model could likely be extended to include other Figs. 1 and 2 adapt Rosenfeld's (2001) ideas to children who are ex-
forms of interpersonal violence that negatively influence child and posed to the physical abuse of a sibling.
adolescent functioning — specifically, another form of physical fam- Children who are exposed to the physical abuse of a sibling may be
ily violence, such as exposure to child physical abuse. influenced by both physical and psychological proximity. When an in-
According to Hollingsworth, Glass, and Heisler (2007), empathy is cident of child physical abuse occurs, siblings may be physically pre-
an essential concept for understanding the intergenerational trans- sent or nearby. Thus, siblings would be represented in the two
mission of maltreating behaviors. The authors have presented a middle circles in Fig. 1, which represents physical proximity, and
model for the development of empathy deficits that they believe they would be quite vulnerable to the negative effects of child physi-
exist in the siblings of children who are abused. Through this cal abuse. Siblings would be represented near the inner circle in Fig. 2,
model, which developed out of clinical practice, siblings may internal- which represents psychological proximity, and they would be espe-
ize the parents' view that the child who is abused is ‘bad’ and conse- cially vulnerable to the negative effects of child physical abuse. Al-
quently develop empathy deficits. Empathy deficits may also develop though not focused on externalizing behaviors, Pynoos and Nader's
in siblings who unsuccessfully attempt to intervene on behalf of the (1988) triage model posits the number and type of post-traumatic
child who is abused and are subsequently punished for their actions symptoms are largely dependent on the child's proximity to violence.
by a parent; furthermore, these deficits may develop when the parent Similar to Rosenfeld's (2001) circles of vulnerability, Pynoos and
develops an alliance with the siblings against the child who is abused, Nader (1988) argue that children most directly exposed to violence
or when siblings witness consequences for the child who is abused will be the most negatively affected. In families where a child is phys-
and become fearful of disobeying the parent. This model focuses on ically abused, proximity may be especially useful for understanding
explaining some aspects of a siblings' functioning, such as denial, de- how other children (e.g., siblings) in the family may also be negative-
tachment, numbing, and avoidance. Components of the empathy def- ly affected. Although it is not yet known whether physical or psycho-
icit model may contribute to understanding the development of logical proximity has a greater effect on children's outcomes, the
externalizing behavior problems; however, because it focuses on in- notion of proximity to violence provides a rationale for why all chil-
ternal states, it may be more valuable to understanding why children dren in a maltreating family may be in need of various interventions.
exposed to the abuse of a sibling may exhibit increased internalizing
behaviors, such as depression, withdrawal, and hopelessness. 4. Implications for research and practice

3.3.1. Proximity It seems that no child living with family violence can escape the
The element of proximity has also been used in trauma literature to negative effects of violence, and yet, intervention strategies, clinical
explore a child's reactions to trauma or violence. Proximity can be services, and resources are most heavily focused on the child who is
viewed as geographic as well as emotional (Webb, 2004). Proximity re- abused and the abusing parents while little recognition is given to
fers to both physical and psychological closeness, the combination of the siblings who may also be left with severe consequences. In
which has been found to affect the prediction of traumatic stress reac- 1982, Pfouts et al. stated, “Long-term outcomes for these silent wit-
tions (Shahinfar & Fox, 1997). Physical proximity has been used to as- nesses to sibling abuse may have been largely overlooked by both re-
sess individual's reactions to traumatic events based on how close they searchers and clinicians” (p. 368). As demonstrated in this article,
were to the actual event (see Giannopoulou et al., 2006; Groome & little research has been conducted with this group of children. Family
Soureti, 2004; and Shannon, Lonigan, Finch, & Taylor, 1994 for studies violence affects all members in a family and contributes to an espe-
focused on the effects of natural disasters). In a meta-analysis of the ef- cially unsettling foundation for children as it shapes their own behav-
fects of community violence, Fowler et al. (2009) reported a positive iors and interpersonal relationships. Therefore, it is important to
relationship between physical proximity to violence and children's ex- increase research efforts to understand how child physical abuse af-
ternalizing problems, with direct victimization predicting more exter- fects all children in the family.
nalizing problems than witnessing violence and witnessing violence Future research on how family violence affects children who are
predicting more externalizing problems than hearing about violence. exposed to child abuse and the sibling relationship may provide a
L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170 167

more in-depth understanding of the impact of family violence upon The accumulating research on children's exposure to IPV has
multiple family members. Previous research studies have neglected caused changes to several states' child welfare systems. Recognizing
to include siblings who were indirectly affected by violence and the potentially devastating effects that exposure to IPV can have on
have consequently provided an incomplete understanding of the ef- child development, nearly half of U.S. states have statutes that cur-
fects of family violence. Studying all children living in a maltreating rently address the issue of children's exposure to IPV (Children's
family, along with children's relationships with a sibling who is Bureau, 2009). However, there is still no agreement on which service
physically abused and relationships with the abusing and non- system (e.g., child welfare or domestic violence) is most responsible
abusing parent, would add considerable breadth and depth to the for children exposed to IPV, and children exposed to child maltreat-
family violence literature. Increased research efforts would allow for ment are not yet included in this debate. Child protection agencies
a more comprehensive understanding of the effects of family violence are mandated to assure children's safety and training on assessment
on children's development, and utilizing the notion of proximity and interventions related to IPV and becoming increasingly common-
would allow for a more developed theoretical understanding of the place among child welfare assessment procedures. Because the goal
radiating impact of family violence. Additional research focused on of child welfare is to protect children, the safety of all children in
children exposed to child physical abuse will also have implications the family should be a priority when any abuse is encountered.
for practice, especially when it is focused on safety and mental health
services. 4.2. Mental health services

4.1. Safety and protective services Previous research suggests that parents underestimate their chil-
dren's exposure to incidents of IPV (O'Brien, John, Margolin, & Erel,
Early literature cited ‘scapegoated’ children – one child who 1994) and this is likely the case for exposure to parent–child mal-
served as the target of abuse – as commonplace in abusing families treatment. According to Beezley et al. (1976), “All members of the
(Cirillo & DiBlasio, 1992; Green, 1985; Justice & Justice, 1990) and family of an abused child have need of psychological help….. And sib-
there is some evidence to suggest that when one child in a family is lings of the abused child have psychological needs that are relevant
physically abused, other children are not. Using information from and important in their own right” (p. 169). Assessments focused on
case records, early research found that in more than 87% of child wel- physical safety and risks of harm are needed for all siblings in a family
fare cases, only one child in the family was abused (Thompson, Paget, where one child is abused. However, in light of psychological proxim-
Bates, Mesch, & Putnam, 1971). A more recent study of 310 families ity and the vulnerability of siblings to be negatively affected by child
referred to a child protection unit for the abuse of one child (i.e., the physical abuse in non-physical domains of functioning, the assess-
index child), found 44% of index children had no sibling that had ment process should not end with physical safety alone.
been maltreated, while 56% had at least one sibling who was also Recognizing that abuse affects all members of a family, Kempe and
abused (Hamilton-Giachritsis & Browne, 2005). Kempe (1976) called for physical and neurological examinations, de-
Although the prevalence of children exposed to their sibling's velopmental assessments or psychological testing for “the abused
physical abuse is not well-established, some authors have argued child and his siblings” (p. 123). Similarly, Halperin (1981) suggested
that all siblings in a family should be assessed for possible abuse a three-phase process whereby all members of a family – the child
when one child is physically abused, especially due to aspects of who is abused, his/her parents, and his/her siblings – are assessed
shared environment and risks due to physical proximity. Hamilton- and treated. Practice literature has focused on the involvement of sib-
Giachritsis and Browne (2005) found that parental (e.g., alcohol ling groups in treatment because the sibling relationship can be an es-
abuse, mental health difficulties, intellectual disability, self-harm) sential element to healing from family trauma. However, this
and family (e.g., unstable lifestyle, criminality) characteristics were treatment focus typically involves siblings who experience the same
positively associated with risk to some or all siblings in families type of family violence (see Leavitt, Gardner, Gallagher, & Schamess,
where one child experienced maltreatment. 1998; Tyndall-Lind, Landreth, & Giordano, 2001). In the case of child
Campbell, Bogen, and Berger (2006) surveyed 93 U.S. physicians maltreatment, siblings should not merely be contributors to treat-
recognized as experts in child abuse medicine to determine consen- ment offered to children who are abused. It is important to recognize
sus regarding the evaluation of contact children (i.e., other children that siblings exposed to child abuse also experience negative conse-
in the home, likely siblings, of a child that was physically abused). quences and are also in need of individualized services.
Seventy-eight percent of survey respondents believed that contact Psychological proximity and characteristics of the sibling bond
children occasionally had findings of physical abuse. In addition, all may explain some of the early findings of abused children and their
respondents indicated that physicians determining physical abuse of sibling. One can hypothesize that even in violent families, feelings of
an index child should recommend a medical evaluation of other chil- connectedness occur between members — especially among siblings.
dren in the home, although there was a wide range both of how often Cicirelli (1996) points out that the theory of attachment is not re-
this was done in practice and what diagnostic evaluations were stricted to the mother–child interaction and children may develop
recommended. Using three case scenarios, Vitale, Squires, strong attachments to other individuals (e.g., siblings) who are re-
Zuckerbraun, and Berger (2010) compared recommendations provid- sponsive and supportive. Studies have found that a close sibling rela-
ed by Child Protective Services (CPS) workers and child abuse physi- tionship may serve as a buffer to the impact of parental conflict
cians (CAPs) on whether contact children (i.e., children living in a (Jenkins & Smith, 1990; Waddell, Pepler, & Moore, 2001) and that
home where an index child was physically abused) were also in children who are distressed due to exposure to parental conflict
need of medical evaluations. The results revealed significant disagree- often seek out a sibling for comfort and support (Bank & Kahn,
ment between the two groups of professionals. Across the three sce- 1982; Jenkins, Smith, & Graham, 1989). Sibling groups may receive
narios, 97–100% of CAPs recommended a medical evaluation for the differential treatment from their parents, and differential treatment
contact children in the home, while 38–73% of CPS workers made is associated with emotional and behavioral difficulties in children
this recommendation. The authors stipulated that this difference (Brody, 2004). Even if treated differently, however, children may de-
may be due to CPS workers' initial desire to conduct a home visit velop similar problems in functioning.
and gathering information on medical and nonmedical risk before Children who are exposed to the physical abuse of a sibling may
recommending a medical evaluation or that CPS workers' may be have similar responses to children exposed to IPV. Siblings who are
working under policies and procedures that limit their ability to not physically abused may develop feelings of guilt because they
bring in contact children for medical evaluations. were not abused, helplessness because they could not stop the
168 L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170

abuse, fear they will become the next child to be abused, and shame Briere, J. (2002). Treating adult survivors of severe childhood abuse and neglect: Fur-
ther development of an integrative model. In J. E. B. Myers, L. Berliner, J. Briere,
over efforts to protect themselves from becoming abused (Gil, C. T. Hendrix, C. Jenny, & T. A. Reid (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreat-
1983). They may also feel responsible for the abuse or for not protect- ment (pp. 175–203). (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
ing the victim (Office for Victims of Crime, 1999). These feelings and Brody, G. H. (2004). Siblings' direct and indirect contributions to child development.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 124–126.
actions may manifest into externalizing behaviors and aggression to- Brown, J. D., & Witherspoon, E. M. (2002). The mass media and American adolescents'
ward others or internalizing behaviors, especially when the sibling health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31, 153–170.
bond is strong. After finding that siblings exposed to child physical Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2006). Short-term and long-term effects of violent media
on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160,
abuse had similar outcome measures as children who were directly 348–352.
abused, Howells and Rosenbaum (2008) concluded that this likely oc- Campbell, K. A., Bogen, D. L., & Berger, R. P. (2006). The other children: A survey of child
curred because “participants may identify more with their sibling” (p. abuse physicians on the medical evaluation of children living with a physically
abused child. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160, 1241–1246.
207).
Carlson, B. E. (2000). Children exposed to domestic violence: Research findings and im-
Findings from the existing research that focused on siblings who plications for intervention. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 1(4), 321–342.
were exposed to child physical abuse indicated the need for these Carlson, E. B., & Dalenberg, C. (2000). A conceptual framework for the impact of trau-
children to be assessed for possible consequences to their physical, matic experiences. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 1(1), 4–28.
Carpenter, G. L., & Stacks, A. M. (2009). Developmental effects of exposure to intimate
emotional, cognitive and behavioral functioning. Siblings exposed to partner violence in early childhood: A review of the literature. Children and Youth
child physical abuse would likely benefit from social skills groups fo- Services Review, 31(8), 831–839.
cused on reducing aggressive behaviors and mental health services Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (1996). The social development model: A theory of an-
tisocial behavior. In J. D. Hawkins (Ed.), Delinquency and crime: Current theories
focused on internalizing behavior problems. Siblings might also ben- (pp. 149–197). New York: Cambridge University Press.
efit from improving academic skills and classroom behavior through Children's Bureau (2009). Child witnesses to domestic violence: Summary of state laws.
study skills groups and mentoring programs. Improving the safety Available through Child Welfare Information Gatewayhttp://www.childwelfare.gov/
systemwide/laws_policies/statutes/witnessdv.cfm
of the home environment and improving the quality of intra- Christakis, D. A., & Zimmerman, F. J. (2007). Violent television viewing during preschool is
familial interpersonal relationships have enormous potential to associated with antisocial behavior during school age. Pediatrics, 120(5), 993–999.
make positive differences in the lives of children exposed to child Cicchetti, D., & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of commu-
nity violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children's development.
physical abuse. Psychiatry, 56(1), 96–118.
Cicchetti, D., & Rizley, R. (1981). Developmental perspectives on the etiology, interge-
nerational transmission, and sequelae of child maltreatment. New Directions for
5. Summary and conclusions Child Development, 11, 31–55.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental psychopathology perspective on
All forms of family violence can have devastating consequences on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 34(5), 541–565.
child functioning. Substantial increases in research on child physical Cicirelli, V. G. (1996). Sibling relationships in middle and old age. In G. H. Brody (Ed.),
abuse and children's exposure to IPV over the past few decades Sibling relationships: Their causes and consequences (pp. 47–73). Norwood, NJ:
have resulted in a more sophisticated understanding of the effects Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Cirillo, S., & DiBlasio, P. (1992). Families that abuse. Diagnosis and therapy. New York:
of these forms of family violence — specifically on children's external-
W.W. Norton & Company.
izing behavior problems. This expanded understanding has translated Connor, D. F., Doerfler, L. A., Volungis, A. M., Steingard, R. J., & Melloni, R. H., Jr. (2003). Aggres-
into improvements in practice to increase children's safety and im- sive behavior in abused children. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008,
79–90.
prove their well-being. Despite these research and practice gains, lit-
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information proces-
tle is still known about how children are affected by exposure to child sing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74–101.
physical abuse in the family environment. Only a handful of studies Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reac-
have focused on this group of children, but the results indicate that tive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993–1002.
Cummings, E. M., Schermerhorn, A. C., Davies, P. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, J. S.
their functioning is also negatively affected. In order to better under- (2006). Interparental discord and child adjustment: Prospective investigations of
stand the consequences of family violence, children exposed to par- emotional security as an explanatory mechanism. Child Development, 77(1), 132–152.
ent–child maltreatment must be included in future research. The Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emo-
tional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 387–411.
safety and well-being of these children must also be given adequate Davies, P. T., Harold, G. T., Goeke-Morey, M., & Cummings, E. M. (2002). Children's
attention by social service, mental health, and educational systems. emotional security and interparental conflict. Monographs of the Society for Re-
search in Child Development, 67, 1–129.
Davies, P. T., & Woitach, M. J. (2008). Children's emotional security in the interparental
References relationship. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 269–274.
Dodge, K. A., & Crick, N. R. (1990). Social information-processing bases of aggressive
Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., et al. behavior in children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16(1), 8–22.
(2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1997). How the experience of early physical
in Eastern and Western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, abuse leads children to become chronically aggressive. In D. Cicchetti, & S. L. Toth
136(2), 151–173. (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on trauma: Theory, research, and intervention
Augustyn, M., Parker, S., Groves, B. M., & Zuckerman, B. (1995). Silent victims: Children (pp. 263–288). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
who witness violence. Contemporary Pediatrics, 12(8), 35–57. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E. (1995). Social information-processing
Baker, J. N., Tanis, H. J., & Rice, J. B. (2001). Including siblings in the treatment of child patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct prob-
sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 10, 1–16. lems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(4), 632–643.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. New York: Prentice Hall. Edleson, J. L., Gassman-Pines, J., & Hill, M. B. (2006). Defining child exposure to domes-
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. tic violence as neglect: Minnesota's difficult experience. Social Work, 51(2),
Bandura, A. (1979). The social learning perspective: Mechanisms of aggression. In H. 167–174.
Toch (Ed.), Psychology of crime and criminal justice (pp. 198–236). New York: Elbow, M. (1982). Children of violent marriages: The forgotten victims. Social Case-
Holt, Rinehart and Winston. work: The Journal of Contemporary Social Work, 63(8), 465–471.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Eriksen, S., & Jensen, V. (2006). All in the family? Family environment factors in sibling
Englewood, CA: Prentice-Hall, Inc. violence. Journal of Family Violence, 21(8), 497–507.
Bank, S. P., & Kahn, M. D. (1982). The sibling bond. New York: Basic Book Publishers. Evans, S. E., Davies, C., & DiLillo, D. (2008). Exposure to domestic violence: A meta-analysis
Beezley, P., Martin, H., & Alexander, H. (1976). Comprehensive family oriented therapy. of child and adolescent outcomes. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 131–140.
In R. E. Helfer, & C. H. Kempe (Eds.), Child abuse and neglect. The family and the com- Fang, X., & Corso, P. S. (2007). Child maltreatment, youth violence, and intimate part-
munity. (pp. 169–194) Cambridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Company. ner violence: Developmental relationships. American Journal of Preventive Medi-
Bell, C. C., & Jenkins, E. J. (1991). Traumatic stress and children. Journal of Health Care for cine, 33(4), 281–290.
the Poor and Underserved, 2(1), 175–185. Feldman, R. S., Salzinger, S., Rosario, M., Alvarado, L., Caraballo, L., & Hammer, M.
Bolton, F. G., Reich, J. W., & Gutierres, S. E. (1977). Delinquency patterns in maltreated (1995). Parent, teacher, and peer ratings of physically abused and nonmaltreated
children and siblings. Victimology: An International Journal, 2(2), 349–357. children's behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 317–334.
Briere, J. (1992). Child abuse trauma: Theory and treatment of the lasting effects. New- Felson, R. B. (1983). Aggression and violence between siblings. Social Psychology Quar-
bury Park, CA: Sage Publications. terly, 46(4), 271–285.
L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170 169

Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1997). Physical punishment/maltreatment during Hines, D. A., Kantor, G. K., & Holt, M. K. (2006). Similarities in siblings' experiences of
childhood and adjustment in young adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21(7), 617–630. neglectful parenting behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30(6), 619–637.
Feshbach, S. (1980). Child abuse and the dynamics of human aggression and violence. Holden, G. W. (2003). Children exposed to domestic violence and child abuse: Termi-
In G. Gerbner, C. J. Ross, & E. Zigler (Eds.), Child abuse: An agenda for action nology and taxonomy. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6(3), 151–160.
(pp. 48–60). New York: Oxford University Press. Hollingsworth, J., Glass, J., & Heisler, K. W. (2007). Empathy deficits in siblings of se-
Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R. K., & Turner, H. A. (2009). Lifetime assessment of poly-victimization verely scapegoated children: A conceptual model. Journal of Emotional Abuse,
in a national sample of children and youth. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33(7), 403–411. 7(4), 69–88.
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., & Ormrod, R. (2006). Kid's stuff: The nature and impact of peer and Holt, S., Buckley, H., & Whelan, S. (2008). The impact of exposure to domestic violence
sibling violence on younger and older children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30(12), on children and young people: A review of the literature. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32,
1401–1421. 797–810.
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., Ormrod, R., & Hamby, S. L. (2009). Violence, abuse, and crime ex- Holtz, P., & Appel, M. (2011). Internet use and video gaming predict problem behavior
posure in a national sample of children and youth. Pediatrics, 124(5), 1411–1423. in early adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 49–58.
Fosco, G. M., DeBoard, R. L., & Grych, J. H. (2007). Making sense of family violence: Im- Howells, N. L., & Rosenbaum, A. (2008). Effects of perpetrator and victim gender on
plications of children's appraisals of interparental aggression for their short- and negative outcomes of family violence. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 203–209.
long-term functioning. European Psychologist, 12(1), 6–16. Huesmann, L. R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C., & Eron, L. D. (2003). Longitudinal relations
Foster, H., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). Toward a stress process model of children's expo- between children's exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and violent be-
sure to physical family and community violence. Clinical Child and Family Psychol- havior in young adulthood: 1977–1992. Developmental Psychology, 39(2),
ogy Review, 12(2), 71–94. 201–221.
Fowler, D. N., & Chanmugam, A. (2007). A critical review of quantitative analyses of Huesmann, L. R., & Taylor, L. D. (2006). The role of media violence in violent behavior.
children exposed to domestic violence: Lessons for practice and research. Brief Annual Review of Public Health, 27, 393–415.
Treatment and Crisis Intervention, 7(4), 322–344. Ireland, T. O., & Smith, C. A. (2009). Living in partner-violent families: Developmental
Fowler, P. J., Tompsett, C. J., Braciszewski, J. M., Jacques-Tiura, A. J., & Baltes, B. B. (2009). links to antisocial behavior and relationship violence. Journal of Youth and Adoles-
Community violence: A meta-analysis on the effect of exposure and mental health out- cence, 38(3), 323–339.
comes of children and adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 227–259. Jaffe, P., Wolfe, D., & Wilson, S. K. (1990). Children of battered women. Newbury Park:
Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of violent video Sage Publications, Inc.
game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school performance. Jaffee, S. R., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & Arseneault, L. (2002). Influence of adult
Journal of Adolescence, 27(1), 5–22. domestic violence on children's internalizing and externalizing problems: An envi-
Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors ronmentally informative twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(9), 1095–1103.
128(4), 539–579. Janosz, M., Archambault, I., Pagani, L. S., Pascal, S., Morin, A. J. S., & Bowen, F. (2008). Are
Giannopoulou, I., Strouthos, M., Smith, P., Dikaiakou, A., Galanopoulou, V., & Yule, W. there detrimental effects of witnessing school violence in early adolescence? Jour-
(2006). Post-traumatic stress reactions of children and adolescents exposed to nal of Adolescent Health, 43(6), 600–608.
the Athens 1999 earthquake. European Psychiatry, 21(3), 160–166. Jean-Gilles, M., & Crittenden, P. M. (1990). Maltreating families: A look at siblings. Fam-
Gil, E. (1983). Outgrowing the pain: A book for and about adults abused as children. New ily Relations, 39(3), 323–329.
York: Dell Publishing. Jenkins, J. M., & Smith, M. A. (1990). Factors protecting children living in disharmoni-
Graham-Bermann, S. A. (1998). The impact of woman abuse on children's social devel- ous homes: Maternal reports. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
opment: Research and theoretical perspectives. In G. W. Holden, R. Geffner, & E. N. cent Psychiatry, 29(1), 60–69.
Jouriles (Eds.), Children exposed to marital violence: Theory, research, and applied is- Jenkins, J. M., Smith, M. A., & Graham, P. J. (1989). Coping with parental quarrels. Jour-
sues (pp. 21–54). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. nal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(2), 182–189.
Graham-Bermann, S. A., & Hughes, H. M. (1998). The impact of domestic violence and Johnson, R. M., Kotch, J. B., Catellier, D. J., Winsor, J. R., Dufort, V., Hunter, W., et al.
emotional abuse on children: The intersection of research, theory, and clinical in- (2002). Adverse behavioral and emotional outcomes from child abuse and wit-
tervention. Journal of Emotional Abuse, 1(2), 1–21. nessed violence. Child Maltreatment, 7(3), 179–186.
Graham-Bermann, S. A., & Levendosky, A. A. (1998). The social functioning of Johnson, B., & Morse, H. A. (1968). Injured children and their parents. Children, 15(4),
preschool-age children whose mothers are emotionally and physically abused. 147–152.
Journal of Emotional Abuse, 1(1), 59–84. Jouriles, E. N., Norwood, W. D., McDonald, R., & Peters, B. (2001). Domestic violence and
Green, A. H. (1985). Children traumatized by physical abuse. In S. Eth, & R. S. Pynoos child adjustment. In J. H. Grych, & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Interparental conflict and
(Eds.), Post-traumatic stress disorder in children (pp. 135–154). Washington, DC: child development: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 315–336). Cambridge, En-
American Psychiatric Press, Inc. gland: Cambridge University Press.
Groome, D., & Soureti, A. (2004). Post-traumatic stress disorder and anxiety symptoms in Justice, B., & Justice, R. (1990). The abusing family (Revised ed.). New York: Plenum Press.
children exposed to the 1999 Greek earthquake. British Journal of Psychology, 95(3), Kantor, G. K., & Little, L. (2003). Defining the boundaries of child neglect: When does
387–397. domestic violence equate with parental failure to protect? Journal of Interpersonal
Groves, B. M. (1999). Mental health services for children who witness domestic vio- Violence, 18(4), 338–355.
lence. The Future of Children, 9(3), 122–132. Keil, V., & Price, J. M. (2006). Externalizing behavior disorders in child welfare settings:
Groves, B. M., Zuckerman, B., Marans, S., & Cohen, D. J. (1993). Silent victims. Children who Definition, prevalence, and implications for assessment and treatment. Children
witness violence. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 269(2), 262–264. and Youth Services Review, 28, 761–779.
Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and children's adjustment: A Kempe, R., & Kempe, C. H. (1976). Assessing family pathology. In R. E. Helfer, & C. H. Kempe
cognitive-contextual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 108(2), 267–290. (Eds.), Child abuse and neglect: The family and the community (pp. 115–126). Cam-
Grych, J. H., Fincham, F. D., Jouriles, E. N., & McDonald, R. (2000). Interparental conflict bridge, MA: Ballinger Publishing Company.
and child adjustment: Testing the meditational role of appraisals in the cognitive- Kempe, C. H., Silverman, F. N., Steele, B. F., Droegemueller, W., & Silver, H. K. (1962). The
contextual framework. Child Development, 71(6), 1648–1661. battered child syndrome. Journal of the American Medical Association, 181(1), 17–24.
Guerra, N. G., Huesmann, L. R., & Spindler, A. (2003). Community violence exposure, so- Kernic, M. A., Wolf, M. E., Holt, V. L., McKnight, B., Huebner, C. E., & Rivara, F. P. (2003).
cial cognition, and aggression among urban elementary school children. Child De- Behavioral problems among children whose mothers are abused by an intimate
velopment, 74(5), 1561–1576. partner. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(11), 1231–1246.
Halperin, S. L. (1981). Abused and non-abused children's perceptions of their mothers, Kiselica, M. S., & Morrill-Richards, M. (2007). Sibling maltreatment: The forgotten
fathers and siblings: Implications for a comprehensive family treatment plan. Fam- abuse. Journal of Counseling and Development, 85(2), 148–161.
ily Relations, 30(1), 89–96. Kitzmann, K. M., Gaylord, N. K., Holt, A. R., & Kenny, E. D. (2003). Child witness to do-
Halperin, S. M. (1983). Family perceptions of abused children and their siblings. Child mestic violence: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
Abuse & Neglect, 7, 107–115. ogy, 71(2), 339–352.
Hamilton-Giachritsis, C. E., & Browne, K. D. (2005). A retrospective study of risk to sib- Kolar, K. R., & Davey, D. (2007). Silent victims: Children exposed to family violence. The
lings in abusing families. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(4), 619–624. Journal of School Nursing, 23(2), 86–91.
Hawkins, J. D., Herrenkohl, T., Farrington, D. P., Brewer, D., Catalano, R. F., & Harachi, T. Lansford, J. E., Criss, M. M., Laird, R. D., Shaw, D. S., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., et al. (2011).
W. (1998). A review of predictors of youth violence. In R. Loeber, & D. P. Farrington Reciprocal relations between parents' physical discipline and children's externaliz-
(Eds.), Serious and violent juvenile offenders: Risk factors and successful interventions ing behavior during middle childhood and adolescence. Development and Psycho-
(pp. 106–146). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. pathology, 23, 225–238.
Hawkins, J. D., & Weis, J. G. (1985). The social development model: An integrated approach Lansford, J. E., Miller-Johnson, S., Berlin, L. J., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S.
to delinquency prevention. Journal of Primary Prevention, 6(2), 73–97. (2007). Early physical abuse and later violent delinquency: A prospective longitu-
Helfer, R. E., & Kempe, C. H. (1968). The battered child. Chicago: The University of Chicago dinal study. Child Maltreatment, 12(3), 233–245.
Press. Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.
Herman, J. L. (1997). Trauma and recovery. New York, NY: Basic Books. Leavitt, K. S., Gardner, S. A., Gallagher, M. M., & Schamess, G. (1998). Severely trauma-
Herrenkohl, E. C., & Herrenkohl, R. C. (1979). A comparison of abused children and their non- tized siblings: A treatment strategy. Clinical Social Work Journal, 26(1), 55–71.
abused siblings. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 18(2), 260–269. Levine, M. B. (1975). Interparental violence and its effect on the children: A study of 50
Herrenkohl, T. I., Huang, B., Tajima, E. A., & Whitney, S. D. (2003). Examining the link families in general practice. Medicine, Science, and the Law, 15(3), 172–176.
between child abuse and youth violence: An analysis of mediating mechanisms. Linares, L. O. (2006). An understudied form of intra-family violence: Sibling-to-sibling
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(10), 1189–1208. aggression among foster children. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11(1), 95–109.
Herrera, V. M., & McCloskey, L. A. (2001). Gender differences in the risk of delinquency Litrownik, A. J., Newton, R., Hunter, W. M., English, D., & Everson, M. D. (2003). Expo-
among youth exposed to family violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25(8), 1037–1051. sure to family violence in young at-risk children: A longitudinal look at the effects
170 L.M. Renner / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 158–170

of victimization and witnessed physical and psychological aggression. Journal of Radbill, S. X. (1968). A history of child abuse and infanticide. In R. E. Helfer, & C. H. Kempe
Family Violence, 18(1), 59–73. (Eds.), The battered child (pp. 3–17). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Liu, J. (2004). Childhood externalizing behavior: Theory and implications. Journal of Rankin, J. M., & Ornstein, A. E. (2009). A commentary on mandatory reporting legisla-
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 17(3), 93–103. tion in the United States, Canada, and Australia: A cross-jurisdictional review of
Lynch, M. A. (1975). Ill-health and child abuse. The Lancet, 306, 317–319. key features, differences, and issues. Child Maltreatment, 14(1), 121–123.
Lynch, M. (1976). Risk factors in the child: A study of abused children and their siblings. In Renner, L.M. (2005). An integrated understanding of the effects of physical family vio-
H. P. Martin (Ed.), The abused child (pp. 43–56). Cambridge, Mass: Ballinger. lence on children's behavior problems. (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Lynch, M. (2003). Consequences of children's exposure to community violence. Clinical Wisconsin-Madison). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Thesis database.
Child and Family Psychology Review, 6(4), 265–274. (UMI No. 3186061).
Lynch, M. E., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). An ecological-transactional analysis of children and Renner, L.M. (accepted for publication). Single types of family violence victimization and
contexts: The longitudinal interplay among child maltreatment, community violence, externalizing behaviors among children and adolescents. Journal of Family Violence.
and children's symptomatology. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 235–257. Richmond, M. K., & Stocker, C. M. (2003). Siblings' differential experiences of marital conflict
Mahoney, A., Donnelly, W. O., Boxer, P., & Lewis, T. (2003). Marital and severe parent- and differences in psychological adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(3),
to-adolescent physical aggression in clinic-referred families: Mother and adoles- 339–350.
cent reports on co-occurrence and links to child behavior problems. Journal of Fam- Rosenbaum, A., & O'Leary, K. D. (1981). Children: The unintended victims of marital vi-
ily Psychology, 17(1), 3–19. olence. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 51(4), 692–699.
Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions of child mal- Rosenfeld, L. B. (2001). Children's response to terrorism. (Retrieved December 1, 2011,
treatment and children's adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and from,). http://www.socialworkers.org/pressroom/events/911/rosenfeld.asp?print=1
subtype. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 759–782. Rudo, Z. H., Powell, D. S., & Dunlap, G. (1998). The effects of violence in the home on
Margolin, G., & Gordis, E. B. (2000). The effects of family and community violence on children's emotional, behavioral, and social functioning: A review of literature.
children. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 445–479. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 6(2), 94–113.
Mass, C., Herrenkohl, T. I., & Sousa, C. (2008). Review of research on child maltreatment Salzinger, S., Feldman, R. S., Hammer, M., & Rosario, M. (1993). The effects of physical
and violence in youth. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 9(1), 56–67. abuse on children's social relationships. Child Development, 64, 169–187.
McCabe, K. M., Hough, R. L., Yeh, M., Lucchini, S. E., & Hazen, A. (2005). The relation be- Salzinger, S., Feldman, R. S., Ng-Mak, D. S., Mojica, E., Stockhammer, T., & Rosario, M.
tween violence exposure and conduct problems among adolescents: A prospective (2002). Effects of partner violence and physical child abuse on child behavior: A
study. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 75(4), 575–584. study of abused and comparison children. Journal of Family Violence, 17(1), 23–52.
McCloskey, L. A., & Lichter, E. L. (2003). The contribution of marital violence to adolescent ag- Schultz, L. (1960). The wife assaulter. Journal of Social Therapy, 6(2), 103–112.
gression across different relationships. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(4), 390–412. Sedlak, A. J., Mettenburg, J., Basena, M., Petta, I., McPherson, K., Greene, A., et al. (2010).
McDonald, R., Jouriles, E. N., Ramisetty-Mikler, S., Caetano, R., & Green, C. E. (2006). Es- Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS-4): Report to Con-
timating the number of American children living in partner-violent families. Jour- gress, executive summary. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human
nal of Family Psychology, 20(1), 137–142. Services, Administration for Children and Families.
McDonald, C. C., & Richmond, T. R. (2008). The relationship between community vio- Shahinfar, A., & Fox, N. A. (1997). The effects of trauma on children: Conceptual and
lence exposure and mental health symptoms in urban adolescents. Journal of Psy- methodological issues. In D. Cicchetti, & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Developmental perspec-
chiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 15, 833–849. tives on trauma: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 115–139). Rochester, NY:
Miller-Perrin, C. L., & Perrin, R. D. (2007). Child maltreatment: An introduction (2nd ed.). University of Rochester Press.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Shannon, M. P., Lonigan, C. J., Finch, A. J., & Taylor, C. M. (1994). Children exposed to di-
Moore, J. G. (1975). Yo-yo children — Victims of matrimonial violence. Child Welfare, saster: I. Epidemiology of post-traumatic symptoms and symptom profiles. Journal
54(8), 557–566. of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(1), 80–93.
Mrug, S., Loosier, P. S., & Windle, M. (2008). Violence exposure across multiple con- Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Parental maltreatment and emotion dysregulation as
texts: Individual and joint effects on adjustment. The American Journal of Orthopsy- risk factors for bullying and victimization in middle childhood. Journal of Clinical
chiatry, 78(1), 70–84. Child Psychology, 30(3), 349–363.
Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2010). Prospective effects of violence exposure across multiple Skopp, N. A., McDonald, R., Manke, B., & Jouriles, E. N. (2005). Siblings in domestically
contexts on early adolescents' internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of violent families: Experiences of interparent conflict and adjustment problems.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(8), 953–961. Journal of Family Psychology, 19(2), 324–333.
Nakou, S., Adam, H., Stathacopoulou, N., & Agathonos, H. (1982). Health status of Smith, S. M. (1975). The battered child syndrome. London: Butterworth & Co.
abused and neglected children and their siblings. Child Abuse & Neglect, 6(3), Smith, C., & Thornberry, T. P. (1995). The relationship between childhood maltreat-
279–284. ment and adolescent involvement in delinquency. Criminology, 33(4), 451–481.
Newman, M., Black, D., & Harris-Hendriks, J. (1997). Victims of disaster, war, violence, Somer, E., & Braunstein, A. (1999). Are children exposed to interparental violence being
or homicide: Psychological effects on siblings. Child Psychology & Psychiatry Review, psychologically maltreated? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 4(4), 449–456.
2(4), 140–149. Spaccarelli, S., Coatsworth, J. D., & Bowden, B. S. (1995). Exposure to serious family vi-
O'Brien, M., John, R. S., Margolin, G., & Erel, O. (1994). Reliability and diagnostic efficacy olence among incarcerated boys: Its association with violent offending and poten-
of parents' reports regarding children's exposure to marital aggression. Violence tial mediating variables. Violence and Victims, 10(3), 163–182.
and Victims, 9(1), 45–62. Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Loeber, R., Homish, D. L., & Wei, E. (2001). Maltreatment of
O'Keefe, M. (1996). The differential effects of family violence on adolescent adjust- boys and the development of disruptive and delinquent behavior. Development
ment. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 13(1), 51–68. and Psychopathology, 13(4), 941–955.
O'Keefe, M. (1997). Adolescents' exposure to community and school violence: Preva- Tanimura, M., Matsui, I., & Kobayashi, N. (1990). Child abuse of one of a pair of twins in
lence and behavioral correlates. Journal of Adolescent Health, 20, 368–376. Japan. The Lancet, 336, 1298–1299.
Office for Victims of Crime (1999). Breaking the cycle of violence: Recommendations to Tavkar, P., & Hansen, D. J. (2011). Interventions for families victimized by child sexual
improve the criminal justice response to child victims and witnesses. Washington, abuse: Clinical issues and approaches for child advocacy center-based services. Ag-
DC: U.S. Department of Justice (Retrieved December 1, 2011 from,). http://www. gression and Violent Behavior, 16(3), 188–199.
ojp.usdoj.gov/ovc/publications/factshts/pdftxt/monograph.pdf Thompson, E. M., Paget, N. W., Bates, D. W., Mesch, M., & Putnam, T. I. (1971). Child
Osofsky, J. D. (1998). Children as invisible victims of domestic and community vio- abuse: A community challenge. Buffalo, NY: Henry Stewart, Incorporated.
lence. In G. W. Holden, R. Geffner, & E. N. Jouriles (Eds.), Children exposed to marital Tower, C. C. (1993). Understanding child abuse and neglect (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &
violence: Theory, research, and applied issues (pp. 95–117). Washington, DC: Amer- Bacon.
ican Psychological Association. Tyndall-Lind, A., Landreth, G. L., & Giordano, M. A. (2001). Intensive group play
Parker, G. E. (1965). The battered child syndrome. Medicine, Science, and the Law, 5(3), therapy with child witnesses of domestic violence. International Journal of Play
160–163. Therapy, 10(1), 53–83.
Pfefferbaum, B. J., De Voe, E. R., Stuber, J., Schiff, M., Klein, T. P., & Fairbrother, G. (2004). U.S. Department of Health and Human Services [USDHHS], Administration for Children
Psychological impact of terrorism on children and families in the United States. and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Children's Bureau.
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 9(3–4), 305–317. (2010). Child Maltreatment 2009. Available from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/
Pfouts, J. H., Schopler, J. H., & Henley, H. C., Jr. (1982). Forgotten victims of family vio- programs/cb/stats_research/index.htm#can.
lence. Social Work, 27, 367–368. Vitale, M. A., Squires, J., Zuckerbraun, N. S., & Berger, R. P. (2010). Evaluation of the sib-
Pine, D. S., Costello, J., & Masten, A. (2005). Trauma, proximity, and developmental psy- lings of physically abused children: A comparison of child protective services case-
chopathology: The effects of war and terrorism on children. Neuropsychopharma- workers and child abuse physicians. Child Maltreatment, 15(2), 144–151.
cology, 30, 1781–1792. Waddell, J., Pepler, D., & Moore, T. (2001). Observations of sibling interactions in vio-
Prino, C. T., & Peyrot, M. (1994). The effect of child physical abuse and neglect on aggres- lent families. Journal of Community Psychology, 29(3), 241–258.
sive, withdrawn, and prosocial behavior. Child Abuse & Neglect, 18(10), 871–884. Wallace, H. (1996). Family violence. Legal, medical, and social perspectives. Needham
Pynoos, R. S., Frederick, C., Nader, K., Arroyo, W., Steinberg, A., Eth, S., et al. (1987). Life Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
threat and posttraumatic stress in school-age children. Archives of General Psychia- Webb, N. B. (2004). The impact of traumatic stress and loss on children and families. In
try, 44, 1057–1063. N. B. Webb (Ed.), Mass trauma and violence: Helping families and children cope
Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1988). Psychological first aid and treatment approach to chil- (pp. 3–22). New York: The Guilford Press.
dren exposed to community violence: Research implications. Journal of Traumatic Wolfe, D. A., Crooks, C. V., Lee, V., McIntyre-Smith, A., & Jaffe, P. G. (2003). The effects of
Stress, 1(4), 445–473. children's exposure to domestic violence: A meta-analysis and critique. Clinical
Pynoos, R. S., Sorenson, S., & Steinberg, A. (1993). Interpersonal violence and traumatic Child and Family Psychology Review, 6(3), 171–187.
stress reactions. In L. Goldberger, & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Theoretical and clinical as- Wolfe, D. A., & Mosk, M. D. (1983). Behavioral comparisons of children from abusive
pects (pp. 550–573). (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Free Press. and distressed homes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51(5), 702–708.

S-ar putea să vă placă și