Sunteți pe pagina 1din 7

Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Children and Youth Services Review


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/childyouth

Attachment relationships and Internalizing and Externalizing problems among


Italian adolescents
Renata Tambelli a, Fiorenzo Laghi b,⁎, Flaminia Odorisio a, Valentina Notari a
a
Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Via dei Marsi, 78-00185 Rome, Italy
b
Department of Social and Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Via dei Marsi, 78-00185 Rome, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study examined the relationship between parent and peer attachment and Internalizing and Externalizing
Received 29 November 2011 problems in a sample of 816 adolescents (413 males and 403 females) aged 11–19 years old. Their responses to
Received in revised form 3 April 2012 the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment indexed attachment quality, and Internalizing and Externalizing
Accepted 4 April 2012
problems were measured by the Youth Self-Report. Our analyses revealed: a) gender differences in attachment
Available online 12 April 2012
to parents, with females reporting stronger attachment than males, and age differences in attachment to parent
Keywords:
and peer, with middle adolescents reporting stronger attachment than adolescents; b) gender differences in
Internalizing problems Internalizing problems and Total problems, with females reporting higher scores than males, and age differences,
Externalizing problems with adolescents reporting higher scores than middle adolescents. Internalizing problems were predicted by
Parent attachment Alienation by parents and Trust (IPPA-Peer) dimension in the expected directions, and Externalizing problems
Peer attachment were predicted only by parental attachment dimensions. Results showed that adolescents with comorbid
Psychological adjustment Internalizing and Externalizing problems scored significantly lower on the dimensions of parent attachment
Adolescence compared to Internalizing problems (pure), those with Externalizing (pure) problems or the control group.
Adolescents with Internalizing problems (pure) scored significantly lower on the dimensions of peer attach-
ment compared to other groups.
© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Moreover, during adolescence, attachment behavior is often di-


rected toward non-parental (non-caretaking) figures (Goodvin,
The association between attachment quality to parents and psy- Meyer, Thompson, & Hayes, 2008), especially peers, who may be con-
chosocial adjustment in adolescence has been documented in several sidered such on a situational or temporary basis. Particularly peers
empirical studies (Laghi, D'Alessio, Pallini, & Baiocco, 2009; Noom, may become new sources of trust. Although, a particularly important
Dekovi´c, & Meeus, 1999; Rice, 1990), and it is established that posi- aspect of adolescent peer attachment is the peer's ability to support
tive perceptions of self and others in attachment relationships and encourage the adolescent's assumption of growth-promoting
with parents are associated with numerous indicators of psychosocial challenges, researchers have confirmed that adolescents continue
adjustment in adolescence (Laghi, Pallini, D'Alessio, & Baiocco, 2011; to rely on their parents for emotional support and advice (Byers
Rice, 1990; Simons, Paternite, & Shore, 2001), and negatively with prob- et al., 2003; Blyth, Hill, & Thiel, 1982; Gottfried, Gottfried, Bathurst,
lem behaviors (Laible, Carlo, & Raffaelli, 2000), low perception of social Guerin, & Parramore, 2003; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; Nickerson &
support (Larose & Boivin, 1998), feelings of loneliness (Ammaniti, Nagle, 2005) and that attachment security with parents predict an
Ercolani, & Tambelli, 1989; Kems & Stevens, 1996) and psychological individual's well-being across the lifespan (Larson, Richards,
distress (Cooper, Shaver, & Collins, 1998). Moneta, Holmbeck, & Duckett, 1996).
Attachment quality has been positively related to self-esteem Although a person typically has more than one attachment figure, a
(Cassidy, 1988; Clark & Symons, 2000; Verschueren, Marcoen, & hierarchy of attachment exists such that attachment behaviors are usu-
Schoefs, 1996), feelings of competence (Papini & Roggman, 1992), per- ally directed toward a principal attachment figure (Laghi et al., 2009).
ceived social support (Blain, Thompson, & Whiffen, 1993; Larose & Some theorists (Laible et al., 2000) have argued for hierarchical organi-
Boivin, 1998), and a sense of mastery over their worlds (Paterson, Pryor, zation in which the child's representation of the most salient attachment
& Field, 1995). figure is the most influential and therefore the most predictive of devel-
opmental outcomes. Parents directly structure and select their children's
peer contacts, and parents indirectly influence norms and beliefs about
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: + 39 06 49 91 7619; fax: + 39 06 49 91 7672.
E-mail addresses: renata.tambelli@uniroma1.it (R. Tambelli),
appropriate social behavior and the relationship models based on attach-
fiorenzo.laghi@uniroma1.it (F. Laghi), flaminia.odorisio@uniroma1.it (F. Odorisio), ment experiences (Carson & Parke, 1996; Laghi, Liga, Baumgartner, &
valentina.notari@uniroma1.it (V. Notari). Baiocco, 2012; Whitbeck, Conger, & Kao, 1993; Zimmermann, 2004).

0190-7409/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.04.004
1466 R. Tambelli et al. / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471

Attachment theory plays an important role in the study of the cogni- der Pahlen, Pelkonen, et al., 2008); only one study (Ferdinand,
tive, social, and emotional adjustment of adolescents. It reflects the core Verhulst, & Wiznitzer, 1995) found that continuity of both depression
aspects of the ways through which they process affects, cognitions and and antisocial behavior did not differ by sex.
behaviors, associated with qualities of past memories, present repre- Some studies have highlighted the moderating effect of gender
sentations and future expectations with regards to attachment and on Internalizing and Externalizing problems in adolescence with
affective bonds (Laghi, Baiocco, Lonigro, Capacchione, & Baumgartner, respect to the quality of relationship (Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Blatt,
2012; Laghi et al., 2009). & Hertzog, 1999). They have demonstrated that Internalizing symp-
Attachment theory posits that, based on the experiences of care toms of female adolescents were partly explained by greater stability
and affective support provided by parents, adolescents develop a feel- in interpersonal vulnerabilities while Externalizing problems of male
ing of security and help-seeking behaviors that function to protect adolescents were partly explained by the greater stability in the
them in situations of distress and to facilitate their exploration of vulnerability to self-criticism.
the social world in general (Bowlby, 1969; Ridenour, Lanza, Donny, The present study aims to investigate the relationship between
& Clark, 2006; Sroufe & Waters, 1977). parent and peer attachment and Internalizing and Externalizing prob-
This configuration of affect and behaviors is thought to be inti- lems. Attachment is conceptualized according to Bowlby's perspec-
mately related to the presence of personal internal working models. tive as a unique emotional bond that enables adolescents to move
These models are constructed on the basis of significant attachment away from the family and explore the world and to establish close
experiences and allow adolescents to judge their self-worth in the relationships with significative figures other than parents. We there-
attachment relationship and to assess the availability of the attach- fore used an attachment measure which captures adolescents' per-
ment figure as a source of comfort and support when they experience ceptions of the quality of the attachment with both parents and
emotional distress (Ammaniti, Van Ijzendoorn, Speranza, & Tambelli, peers evaluating behavioral and affective/cognitive dimensions of
2000; Cook, 2000; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985). both actual adolescents' relationships. First, gender and age differ-
Researchers have inferred the existence of internal working models ences between parent and peer attachment and Internalizing and
based on the consistent links between attachment styles and social Externalizing problems were investigated. More specifically, unique
behavior, self-worth, and relational expectations across the lifespan relationships between parent and peer attachment and Internalizing
(Allen & Land, 1999; Laghi, Baiocco, D'Alessio, Gurrieri, & Mazza, and Externalizing problems were analyzed by performing regression
2008; Shaver & Mikulincer, 2002; Thompson, 1999). analyses. It was expected that parent attachment would be able to ex-
Some studies have indicated that positive role of attachment to plain more of the variance of both Internalizing and Externalizing
parental figures is negatively associated with the presence of exter- problems than peer attachment. This hypothesis is congruent with
nalized problems, such as theft, drug use, vandalism (Noom et al., Laible et al. (2000) who have argued the hierarchical organization
1999), and aggressive behavior (Laible et al., 2000), and internalized of attachment models in which the child's representation of the
problems, such as anxiety and depressive mood (Allen, Moore, most salient attachment figure is the most influential and therefore
Kuperminc, & Bell, 1998; Nada-Raja, McGee, & Stanton, 1992). As a the most predictive of developmental outcomes.
result, attachment quality to parents might provide protection against Secondly, it will be studied to what extent parent and peer attach-
the development of internalized and externalized behavioral prob- ment relationships are specific determinants of Internalizing and Ex-
lems (Duchesne & Larose, 2007). Similarly, secure attachment rela- ternalizing problems distinguishing different category of behavioral
tionships with both peers and parents were also expected to relate and emotional problems. Postulated by attachment perspective, it
to high levels of empathy and low levels of aggressive behavior was hypothesized that adolescents with a combination of Internaliz-
(Laible et al., 2000). Adolescent with insecure attachment exhibit rela- ing and Externalizing problems would report lower levels of parent
tional aggression (Casas et al., 2006) and heightened anger aggression attachment compared to adolescents with Internalizing problems
(DeMulder, Denham, Schmidt, & Mitchell, 2000). only, Externalizing problems only, and adolescents without problems.
Several investigators have sought to better understand the charac- Regarding peer attachment, on the basis of previous research, we
teristics of the adolescent – peer relationships (Furman & Buhrmester, expected that adolescents with Internalizing problems would report
1992; O'Koon, 1997), but only a limited number of them has focused at- lower levels of peer attachment compared to other groups.
tention on the connection between peer-attachment and Internalizing
and Externalizing problems (Ma & Huebner, 2008). Generally, it was 2. Method
argued that adolescents need these relationships since information or
support from a parent may no longer be so relevant (Cotterell, 1992) 2.1. Participants and procedure
and also to help them to establish their identity by comparing opinions
and values with others (Laghi et al., 2011). In addition, research has The sample consisted of 816 students (413 males and 403
shown some gender differences with regard to the relationship be- females) attending middle and high schools in the center of Italy
tween the quality of both parent and peer attachment and psycho- (Lazio). The schools were selected on the basis of their willingness
logical distress: boys have been found to be more likely to develop to participate in the study (N = 6). In terms of the type of high school
Externalizing problems such as acting-out behavior, drug and alcohol 20% of students were enrolled in science-focused schools, 20% were
abuse, whereas girls are more likely to develop Internalizing problems in classical education schools, 20% were in industrial schools, 20%
such as anxiety, depression and suicidal ideation (Garnefski, Kraaij, were in liberal arts-focused schools, and 20% were in teacher-training
& van Etten, 2005; Laghi et al., 2009). Moreover gender differences schools. The average age of the students was 15.89 (SD= 2.23; range
emerge also in attachment pattern because parent attachment is stron- 11–19). For comparative purposes the sample was divided into four
ger in female than male, and females are more able than males to draw age groups: 11–13 years old early-adolescents who attended middle
support from different sources like peers (Ma & Huebner, 2008). schools (N = 151; M = 76; F = 75), 14–15 (N = 132; M = 77; F = 55),
A recent study (Ritakallio, Luukkaala, Marttunen, Pelkonen, et al., 16–17 (N = 312; M = 149; F = 163), and 18–19 years old adolescents
2010), that investigates the effect of perceived social support in the (N = 221; M = 111 F = 110) who attended high schools. Participation
comorbidity between depression and antisocial behavior, found that was obtained through an informed consent procedure asking for
decreased perceived social support was associated with both depres- active consent from both students and parents. Questionnaires were
sion and antisocial behavior in both sexes. There is a general confir- administered in the classroom during lesson time at the end of the
mation that girls present higher levels of Internalizing problems first semester. They took approximately 40 min to complete. Experts in-
than boys (Berkout, Young, & Gross, 2011; Ritakallio, Koivisto, von troduced the questionnaires, giving instructions on their compilation,
R. Tambelli et al. / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471 1467

explaining that they were voluntary and that responses were anony- 3. Results
mous and confidential. They were at the students' disposal during the
questionnaires' administration to answer questions and give explana- 3.1. Attachment and Internalizing and Externalizing problems: gender
tion. All students responded to the same questionnaire packet. This and age differences
survey was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Commission of the
Department of Clinical Psychology of Sapienza, University of Rome. To investigate gender and age differences, we conducted MANOVA
on parent and peer attachment dimensions, and on Internalizing and
2.2. Measures Externalizing problems. Regarding parent attachment, the analysis
revealed main effect for sex, λ=0.99, F (4,800)=2.91, η2 =.02, pb .001,
2.2.1. Attachment to parents and age, λ=.95, F (12,2396=3.20, η2 =.02, pb .001). There was no
The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA; Armsden & effect of interaction between the variables, λ=.99, F (12,2116)=.57,
Greenberg, 1987) was used to measure the quality of parent attach- p=.86.
ment in adolescence and the affective–cognitive dimension of attach- Results from the univariate tests revealed that groups differed
ment. It consists of twenty-eight items Likert-type scale, on which on the subscale of Communication, F (1,814) = 4.72, p b .001, where
each item has five possible responses (from “completely untrue” to females showed a higher mean score (M = 35.38; SD= 7.83) than
“completely true”). It provides an indication of felt security in the males (M = 33.92; SD= 7.97). Regarding age effect, results from the
relationship with specific attachment figures by measuring the ado- univariate tests (ANOVA) and post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected pairwise
lescent's trust in the availability and sensitivity of an attachment comparisons revealed that the four groups differed on the subscales
figure (sample item, “My parents respect my feelings”; α = 0.77 of Communication, F (3,814)= 7.06, p b .001, Alienation, F (3,812) =
in the present study), the quality of Communication, which fosters 3.34, p b .001, and Total score, F (3,812) = 5.60, p b .001. 11–13 and
comfort in the relationship with an attachment figure (sample item, 14–15 years old adolescents showed a higher mean score on the
“When my parents knows that something is bothering me, they ask Communication (M = 36.98; SD= 7.71; M = 35.02; SD= 7.40 respec-
me about it”; α = 0.83 in the present study) and the extent of tively) and Total score (M = 61.53; SD= 19.05; M = 57.44; SD= 17.82
anger, Alienation and/or hopelessness resulting from an unresponsive respectively) than 16–17 years old adolescents (M = 34.31; SD= 7.71;
or inconsistently responsive attachment figure (sample item, “I don't M = 55.03; SD= 18.51 respectively), and 18–19 years old (M = 33.23;
get much attention from my parents”; α = 0.82 in the present study). SD= 8.34; M = 54.29; SD= 17.32 respectively), that did not differ
The Alienation scale was recoded. significantly from either. Regarding Alienation dimension, 11–13 years
old early adolescents showed a lower mean score (M = 16.28; SD=
2.2.2. Attachment to peer 6.64), than other groups that did not differ significantly from either
Here again twenty-five item versions of the subscales trust, (M = 17.62; SD= 6.34; M = 18.40; SD= 6.87; M = 18.09; SD= 7.12;
Communication and Alienation of the IPPA were filled out by partici- respectively).
pants. The three scales were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from Regarding peer attachment, MANOVA revealed main effect only
“completely untrue” to “completely true” and were used to measure for age, λ = .84, F (12,2116) = 11.26, η 2 = .05, p b .001. There were
the quality of peer attachment in adolescence. The Trust scale mea- no effect of sex, λ = .99, F (4,800 = 1.83, p = .37, and interaction be-
sures the extent to which an adolescent trusts peer to respect and tween the variables, λ = .99, F (12,2116) = 0.82, p = .54. Results from
accept his or her feelings and wishes (sample item, “My friends the univariate tests (ANOVA) and post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected pair-
respect my feelings”; α = 0.79 in the present study). The Communica- wise comparisons revealed that the four groups differed on the sub-
tion scale measures the extent an adolescent experiences having high scales of Communication, F (3,812)=38.24; pb .001, and Total score,
quality of Communication with peer (sample item, “When my friends F (3,812)=21.89, pb .001. 16–17 and 18–19 years old adolescents
know that something is bothering me, they ask me about it”; α = 0.83 showed a higher mean score on the Communication (M=41.66; SD=
in the present study). The Alienation scale measures the degree to 18.32; M=44.59; SD=20.76 respectively) and Total score (M=67.86;
which an adolescent experiences negative feelings toward peer (sam- SD=22.99; M=70.44; SD=27.12 respectively) than 11–13 years old
ple item, “I don't get much attention from my friends”; α = 0.84 in early adolescents (M=27.34; SD=8.95; M=52.73; SD=18.70 respec-
the present study). Here again the Alienation scale was recoded. The tively), and 14–15 years old adolescents (M=32.83; SD=13.74;
IPPA has been used in a number of studies, and its reliability and M=58.39; SD=21.87 respectively), that did not differ significantly
validity have been shown to be satisfactory (Laghi et al., 2009; Pace, from either.
San Martini, & Zavattini, 2011). Regarding Internalizing and Externalizing problems, the analysis
revealed main effect for sex, λ =.95, F (3,794)=13.66, η2 =.05,
2.2.3. Internalized and Externalized problems pb .001, and age, λ =.92, F (9,1932)=6.68, η2 =.02, pb .001). There
To assess the adolescents' view of their behavior and socioemotional was no effect of interaction between the variables, λ =.98, F (9,1932) =
functioning, the version of the Youth Self-Report (YSR) by Achenbach 1.05, p=.39. Results from the univariate tests revealed that groups
(1991) was used. This questionnaire has to be completed by the 11-to differed on the subscale of Internalizing problems, F (1,814)=30.34,
18-year-old adolescent and contains 112 problem items covering behav- pb .001, and Total problems, F (1,814)=5.20, pb .001, where females
ioral, emotional, and social problems that occurred during the past showed a higher mean score (M=13.96; SD=8.39; M=65.12;
6 months. Respondent are asked to rate the occurrence of problem on SD=22.87 respectively) than males (M=10.42; SD =7.45; M=60.74;
a 3-point scale. The YSR can be scored on syndrome scales: Anxious/ SD=21.80 respectively). Regarding age effect, results from the uni-
Depressed, Withdrawn/Depressed, Somatic Complaints, Social Prob- variate tests (ANOVA) and post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected pairwise
lems, Thought Problems, Attention Problems, Aggressive Behavior, comparisons revealed that the four groups differed on the subscales
and Rule-Breaking Behavior. The Internalizing scale can be derived of Internalizing problems, F (3,812)=3.78, pb .001, and Externalizing
from the first three syndrome scales (in the present study Cronbach's problems, F (3,812)=18.15, pb .001. 18–19 years old showed a higher
alpha= .83), and the Externalizing scale from the last two (in the pre- mean score on both scales (M=13.08; SD=8.28; M=15.61;
sent study Cronbach's alpha = .84). The Total problems scale is derived SD=8.02 respectively), than 16–17 years old adolescents (M=11.62;
by summing the individual item scores (in the present study Cronbach's SD=7.99; M=13.58; SD=8.28 respectively), 14–15 (M=11.02;
alpha= .84). This measure has demonstrated very good day test–retest SD=8.79; M=12.66; SD=8.20 respectively), and 11–13 years old
reliability, cross-informant agreement, and success in discriminating adolescents (M=10.57; SD =7.18; M=11.37; SD=6.79 respectively)
between referred and no referred adolescents (Achenbach, 1991). who did not differ significantly from either.
1468 R. Tambelli et al. / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471

3.2. Parent and peer attachment as predictors of Internalizing and Table 2


Externalizing problems Differences between NP, IP, EP, and IEP groups on the IPPA Parent subscales.

NP IP EP IEP
Next, using hierarchical multiple regression (Table 1), we further
(N = 368) (N = 73) (N = 87) (N = 55)
examined the associations between parent, peer attachment, and
Alienation 15.17c (5.84) 21.26b (6.94) 19.45b (6.13) 23.36a (7.03)
Internalizing and Externalizing problems. In each regression, we
Communication 35.97a (7.80) 34.29b (7.25) 32.60b (8.30) 30.65c (8.32)
entered age and gender in the first step. The IPPA Parent dimen- Trust 41.56a (7.38) 38.86b (7.01) 37.00b (7.56) 34.85c (9.19)
sions were entered in the second step, and peer dimensions in the Total score 62.29a (16.62) 52.10b (18.17) 50.26b (18.69) 42.09c (21.20)
third step. Regressions were repeated separately for Internalizing Note: Standard deviations are in parentheses. Post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected pairwise
and Externalizing problems. The inclusion of the Alienation dimen- comparisons (p b .001): different letters indicate mean differences between groups.
sion (IPPA-Parents) and the dimension of Trust (IPPA-Peers) in the
model increased the variance accounted for by 21%. Alienation and
Trust were significantly associated with Internalizing problems in On the basis of their responses to the Youth Self-Report measure,
the expected directions. The regression predicting Externalized prob- adolescents were assigned to one of four groups: Internalizing
lems revealed that the addition of Alienation dimension and Trust problems only (IP; N = 73; 8.9%), Externalizing problems only (EP;
(IPPA-Parents) in the model increased the variance accounted for by N = 87; 10.7%), both Internalizing and Externalizing problems (IEP;
21%. Peer attachment dimensions were not related with externalized N = 55; 6.7%), neither Internalizing and Externalizing problems (NP;
problems. N = 368; 45.1%). 233 adolescents (28.6%) were not classified because
one or both of their problem scores fell between the 60th percentile
and 80th percentile.
3.3. Internalized and Externalized membership and parent and peer To examine differences in parent and peer attachment dimensions
attachment between the four groups, a MANCOVA model with Bonferroni's correc-
tion was conducted using Youth self report group as the fixed factor
As reported by Garnefski et al. (2005), a distinction was made (IP vs. EP vs. IEP vs. NP); effects of gender and age were adjusted as
between four groups of adolescents: (1) Internalizing-problem group covariates. The α level was divided by the number of statistical tests per-
(IP), consisting of adolescents scoring above the 80th percentile of the formed (4 for IPPA dimensions; the adjusted P values were α = 0.012).
Internalizing scale and below the 60th percentile in the Externalizing For this multivariate analysis, Wilks' λ criterion was used.
scale; (2) Externalizing-problem group (EP) with adolescents scoring Regarding parent attachment, MANCOVA revealed overall group
above the 80th percentile of the Externalizing scale and below the 60th effect on the dependent variables, λ = .79, F (12,1508) = 11.58,
percentile of the Internalizing scale; (3) Internalizing-and-Externalizing- η 2 = .07, p b .001. Overall, age and sex showed significant covariations
problem group (IEP), with adolescents scoring above the 80th percentile with groups for the IPPA dimensions, λ = .98, F (4,570) = 2.94,
of both the Internalizing and the Externalizing scale; and (4) No-problem η2 =.02, pb .001; λ=.97, F (4,570)=4.09, η2 =.07, pb .001, respectively.
group (NP), consisting of adolescents who scored below the 60th Results from the univariate tests (ANCOVA) and post-hoc Bonferroni-
percentile in both scales. The cut-off score at the 80th percentile was corrected pairwise comparisons revealed that the four groups
based on research by Garnefski et al. (2005), who had shown that – differed on the subscales of Alienation, F (3, 579) = 41.83; p b .001),
in terms of minimizing false negatives and false positives – the most ac- Trust , F (3,579)= 8.09, p b .001, Communication, F (3,579) = 17.71;
curate cut points for discriminating between referred and non-referred p b .001, and Total score, F (3,579) = 27.15, p b .001. Estimated means
adolescents for both Internalizing and Externalizing scales were at are reported in Table 2. IEP group had significantly higher mean levels
about the 80th percentile of the normative sample. The 60th percentile of Alienation than did the EP group and IP group that did not differ
cut point score was added in the present study to further minimize the significantly from either. NP group obtained the lowest mean levels
number of false classifications. than other groups. Regarding Trust, Communication, and Total score,
According to these criteria, adolescents who had scored between NP group had significantly higher mean levels than IP and EP groups,
the 60th and the 80th percentile on either the Internalizing or Exter- which did not differ significantly from either. IEP showed the lowest
nalizing scale were excluded, to ensure that groups designated as free mean levels than other groups.
of problems would not include adolescents who had just marginally Regarding peer attachment, MANCOVA revealed overall group ef-
failed to fulfil the criteria for inclusion in the problem groups fect on the dependent variables, λ = .94, F (12,1508) = 2.77, η 2 = .02,
(Garnefski et al., 2005). p b .001. Overall, only age showed significant covariations with groups

Table 1
Hierarchical regression analyses for parent and peer attachment predicting Internalizing and Externalizing problems.

Internalizing problems Externalizing problems

B SE B β R2 B SE B β R2

Step 1: sex and age .05 .07


Sex (0 = girls; 1 = boys) − 3.22 .52 −.19⁎ 1.14 .52 .07
Age .29 .12 .08 .88 .13 .24⁎
Step 2: IPPA Parent subscales .19 .21
Alienation .43 .05 .36⁎⁎ .32 .04 .27⁎⁎
Communication −.01 .04 −.01 .04 .04 .04
Trust −.01 .05 −.01 −.20 .05 −.19⁎
Step 3: IPPA Peer subscales .21 .21
Alienation .05 .06 .03 .05 .06 .03
Communication .00 .01 .001 −.01 .01 −.04
Trust −.09 .03 −.09⁎ .006 .03 .006
Total R .46 .46

Note: The tabled values for beta reflect Bs after step 3.


⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .001.
R. Tambelli et al. / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471 1469

Table 3 variance accounted for by 21%. This result is consistent with studies
Differences between NP, IP, EP, and IEP groups on the IPPA Peer subscales. that explain how different aspects of familiar context (such as paren-
NP IP EP IEP tal pattern based on cruel and neglecting attitude and based on hard
discipline) have a major effect on expression of Externalizing prob-
(N = 368) (N = 73) (N = 87) (N = 55)
b a a
lems (Farrington, 2005). Indeed, a positive relationship with parents,
Alienation 14.06 (4.68) 16.27 (5.35) 16.01 (4.91) 16.18a (5.17)
based on appropriate monitoring and on coherent discipline (such as
Communication 37.71a (17.26) 35.45b (17.34) 40.41a (20.48) 37.49a (16.42)
Trust 41.53a (7.96) 39.10b (10.09) 39.73b (8.29) 40.16b (7.13) reinforcement of positive behavior, high level of support and a good
Total score 65.17a (22.37) 58.27b (25.46) 64.27a (27.70) 61.56a (21.43) relationship with peers) limits the appearance of behavioral problems
Note: Standard deviations are in parentheses. Post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected pairwise
(Brody et al., 2001; Gaik, Abdullaha, Habibah, & Jegak, 2010; Hill &
comparisons (p b .001): different letters indicate mean differences between groups. Herman-Stahl, 2002; Hilton, Anngela-Cole, & Wakita, 2010; Mounts
& Steinberg, 1995; Sternberg, Lamb, Guterman, & Abbott, 2006).
In this study the role of peers seems to be less important in respect
for the IPPA dimensions, λ = .88, F (4,570) = 19.45, η 2 = .12, p b .001, to the influence on behavioral problems. According to Laible et al.
and not sex, λ = .99, F (4,570) = 1.50, p = .19. Results from the uni- (2004) it is not clear, however, how both peer and parent attachment
variate tests (ANCOVA) and post-hoc Bonferroni-corrected pairwise relationships become integrated into an adolescent's internal work-
comparisons revealed that the four groups differed on the subscales ing model of the self, especially when experiences with parents
of Alienation, F (3, 579) = 7.49, p b .001), Trust, F (3,579) = 3.64, and peers are highly divergent. There is general confirmation of the
p b .001), Communication, F (3,579) = 1.95, p b .001, and Total score, hypothesis that parent–peer influence is situational. A mechanism is
F (3,579) = 5.03, p b .001. Estimated means are reported in Table 3. that parents influence the kind of peers with whom their children
NP group had significantly lower mean levels of Alienation and affiliate, not only by offering this relational model but also through
higher levels of Trust than the other groups that did not differ signif- more distant social characteristics such as social class and educational
icantly from either. Regarding Communication and Total score, IP level.
showed the lowest mean levels than other groups that did not differ Finally, our study indicates that comorbid group (IEP) expressed
significantly from either. anger toward parents and feel a strong sense of rejection from
them, compared to adolescents without problems (NP) who per-
4. Discussion and conclusions ceived parent as supportive and sensitive. Therefore the increase of
risks implies also an increased perception in adolescents of anger
The findings of the present study support the conclusion that and sense of rejection from parents. A stable and reliable relationship
secure attachment with both parents and peers is a protective factor has a protective function on deviant and harmful behaviors for ado-
for adolescent's adjustment. Our results show that females were lescents' well-being. Several researches (Dogan, Conger, R.D., Kim, &
more helpful and open to the communications with parents than Masyn, 2007; Sousa et al., 2011; Vermeersch, T'Sjoen, Kaufman, &
males. Consistent with several studies in Western cultures (Blyth Vincke, 2008) have highlighted that quality of parental care could
et al., 1982; Laghi et al., 2009; Laible, Carlo, & Roesch, 2004), Italian contribute to develop Externalizing problems in adolescents; specifi-
females reported stronger parent attachment than did males. cally both hard discipline and a hostile and coercive parental style are
Several researches have highlighted that adolescent's ability present in families with deviant adolescents. Particularly parental
to confide in parents poses limits to the possibility to be involved in rejection in toddlerhood can help predicting Externalizing problems
deviant behavior (Vieno, Nation, Pastore, & Santinello, 2009). A posi- in adolescence (Trentacosta & Shaw, 2008).
tive attachment relationship provides a secure base enabling the reg- Regarding peer attachment relationships, the results show that
ulation of distress with comfort and support (Scott Brown & Wright, adolescents without problems (NP) did not perceive rejection by
2001; Shirk, Gudmundson, & Burwell, 2005). peers and they were confident to receive help and support by them.
Additionally, this study shows that there are some consistencies in Insecure attachment, through lack of psychological availability of
the development of adolescent attachment and their associations the attachment figure, is considered a risk factor for dysfunctional
with Internalizing and Externalizing problems. According to litera- adjustment (Cicchetti, Cummings, Greenberg, & Marvin, 1990) and
ture, females showed higher levels of Internalizing and Total prob- depression (Kobak, Sudler, & Gamble, 1991).
lems. These results are congruent with the researches (Bleiberg, This is consistent with the basic assumptions of the attachment
2001;Burt & Neiderhiser, 2009; Garnefski et al., 2005; Hicks et al., theory, according to which autonomous exploration is a direct func-
2007; Hill & Herman-Stahl, 2002) that reported females develop In- tion of secure attachment. Based on our data, confidence in attach-
ternalizing problems such as anxiety, depression and suicidal ment figures and in peer relationships is indeed crucial to the
ideation. perception of oneself as an active and self-determined individual:
Our data suggest that there is a progressive decline in the per- those who are secure in their attachment and in peer relationships
ceived quality of parent–child attachment relationship and a contem- consider themselves to be more capable and willing to explore,
porary development of high levels of peer attachment relationships: which is widely intended as an autonomous and proactive ability to
adolescents express a better communication and they have more make future plans.
trust to receive help by peers. Peer attachment might favor a unique Certainly this study has some limits for example the use of single
context in which adolescents develop certain types of socioemotional informant self-report measures raises issues around response bias;
competence, such as empathy, and pro-social behavior. But this per- we are only able to report adolescents' perceptions of relationships.
spective does not rule out the importance of parent–adolescent No diagnostic data on depression are included. Our approach to
attachment relationship. A secure attachment with parents in late analysis, although allowing statistical prediction, cannot yield infor-
adolescence seems linked with multiple aspects of well-being as mation on causality. Longitudinal analysis of relationships between
well as high self-esteem and low psychological distress (Bradford & attachment, cognitive variables, and depression would be an optimal
Lyddon, 1993; Laible et al., 2004). approach.
Additionally, regression results showed that Alienation (IPPA-
Parents) and Trust (IPPA-Peer) were signficantly associated with References
Internalizing problems in the expected directions. The regression pre-
dicting Externalized problems revealed that the addition of Alienation Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the youth self-report form and profile. Burlington,
dimension and Trust (IPPA-Parents) in the model increased the VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
1470 R. Tambelli et al. / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471

Allen, J., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy, & P. Shaver (Eds.), Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., Iacono, W. G.,
Handbook of attachment (pp. 319–335). New York: Guilford. et al. (2007). Gender differences and developmental change in externalizing disor-
Allen, J. P., Moore, C. M., Kuperminc, G. P., & Bell, K. L. (1998). Attachment and adoles- ders from late adolescence to early adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Journal of
cent psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 69, 1406–1419. Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 433–447.
Ammaniti, M., Ercolani, A. P., & Tambelli, R. (1989). Loneliness in female adolescents. Hill, N. E., & Herman-Stahl, M. (2002). Neighborhood safety and social involvement:
Journal of Youth and Adolescent, 18, 321–329. Associations with parenting behaviors and depressive symptoms among African
Ammaniti, M., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Speranza, A. M., & Tambelli, R. (2000). Internal American and Euro-American mothers. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 209–219.
working models of attachment during late childhood and early adolescence: An Hilton, J. M., Anngela-Cole, L., & Wakita, J. (2010). A cross-cultural comparison of
exploration of stability and change. Attachment & Human Development, 2, 328–346. factors associated with school bullying in Japan and the United States. The Family
Armsden, G., & Greenberg, M. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment: Journal, 18(4), 413–422.
Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adoles- Kems, K. A., & Stevens, A. C. (1996). Parent–child attachment in late adolescence: Links
cence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427–454. to social relations and personality. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25(3),
Berkout, O. V., Young, J. N., & Gross, A. M. (2011). Mean girls and bad boys: Recent 323–342.
research on gender differences in conduct disorders. Aggression and Violent Behavior, Kobak, R. R., Sudler, N., & Gamble, W. (1991). Attachment and depressive symptoms during
16, 503–511. adolescence: A developmental pathways analysis. Development and Psychopathology,
Blain, M. D., Thompson, J. M., & Whiffen, V. E. (1993). Attachment and perceived socials 3, 461–474.
support in late adolescence: The interaction between working models of self and Laghi, F., Baiocco, R., D'Alessio, M., Gurrieri, G., & Mazza, M. (2008). Identity status and
others. Journal of Adolescent Research, 21, 226–241. time perspective in adolescence. International Journal of Psychology, 43(3–4), 148.
Bleiberg, E. (2001). Treating personality disorders in children and adolescents. A relational Laghi, F., Baiocco, R., Lonigro, A., Capacchione, G., & Baumgartner, E. (2012). Family
approach. New York: Guilford. functioning and binge drinking among Italian adolescents. Journal of Health
Blyth, D. A., Hill, J., & Thiel, K. (1982). Early adolescents' significant others: Grade and Psychology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105311430005.
gender differences in perceived relationships with familial and non familial adults Laghi, F., D'Alessio, M., Pallini, S., & Baiocco, R. (2009). Attachment representations and
and young people. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 11, 425–450. time perspective in adolescence. Social Indicators Research, 90, 181–194.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Attachment, Vol. 1, . New York: Basic Books. Laghi, F., Liga, F., Baumgartner, E., & Baiocco, R. (2012). Identity and conformism among
Bradford, E., & Lyddon, W. (1993). Current parental attachment: Its relations to Italian adolescents who binge eat and drink. Health, Risk and Society, http:
perceived psychological distress and relationship satisfaction in college students. //dx.doi.org/10.1080/13698575.2012.680952.
Journal of College Students Development, 34, 256–260. Laghi, F., Pallini, S., D'Alessio, M., & Baiocco, R. (2011). Development and validation of the
Brody, G. H., Ge, X., Conger, R., Gibbons, F. X., Murray, V. M., Gerrard, M., et al. (2001). efficacious self-presentation scale. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172(2), 209–219.
The influence of neighborhood disadvantage, collective socialization, and parenting Laible, D., Carlo, G., & Raffaelli, M. (2000). The differential relations of parent and peer
on African American children's affiliation with deviant peers. Child Development, 72, attachment to adolescent adjustment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 45–59.
1231–1246. Laible, D., Carlo, G., & Roesch, S. (2004). Pathways to self-esteem in late adolescence:
Burt, S. A., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2009). Aggressive versus non-aggressive antisocial The role of parent and peer attachment, empathy, and social behavior. Journal of
behavior: Distinctive etiological moderation by age. Developmental Psychology, Adolescence, 27, 703–717.
45(4), 1164–1176. Larose, S., & Boivin, M. (1998). Attachment to parents, social support expectations, and
Byers, E. S., Sears, H. A., Voyer, S. D., Thurlow, J. L., Cohen, J. N., & Weaver, A. D. (2003). socioemotional adjustment during the high school–college transition. Journal of
An adolescent perspective on sexual health education at school and at home: II. Research on Adolescence, 8(1), 1–27.
Middle school students. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 12, 19–33. Larson, R., Richards, M., Moneta, G., Holmbeck, G., & Duckett, E. (1996). Changes in
Carson, J., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocity of parent–child negative affect and adolescents' daily interactions with their families from ages 10 to 18: Disengage-
children's social competence. Child Development, 67, 2217–2226. ment and transformation. Developmental Psychology, 32, 744–754.
Casas, J. F., Weigel, S. M., Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., Woods, K. E., Yeh, E. A., et al. (2006). Leadbeater, B. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Blatt, S. J., & Hertzog, C. (1999). A multivariate model
Early parenting and children's relational and physical aggression in the preschool of gender differences in adolescents' internalizing and externalizing problems.
and home contexts. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 209–227. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1268–1282.
Cassidy, J. (1988). Child–mother attachment and the self in six-year-olds. Child Development, Ma, C. Q., & Huebner, S. (2008). Attachment relationships and adolescents' life satisfaction:
59, 121–134. Some relationships matter more to girls than boys. Psychology in the Schools, 45(2),
Cicchetti, D., Cummings, M., Greenberg, M. T., & Marvin, R. S. (1990). An organizational 177–190.
cognitions and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Maccoby, E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child
Adolescent Psychology, 34, 172–181. interaction. In E. Heatherington (Ed.), Mussen manual of child psychology (pp. 1–102).
Clark, S., & Symons, D. (2000). A longitudinal study of Q-sort attachment security and (4 rd Ed.). New York: Wiley.
self-processes at age five. Infant and Child Development, 9, 91–104. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood:
Cook, W. L. (2000). Understanding attachment security in family context. Journal of A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton, & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing
Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 285–294. points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Cooper, M. L., Shaver, P. R., & Collins, N. L. (1998). Attachment styles, emotion regula- Child Development (pp. 66–104).
tion, and adjustment in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Mounts, N. S., & Steinberg, L. (1995). An ecological analysis of peer influence on adoles-
74(5), 1380–1397. cent grade point average and drug use. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 915–922.
Cotterell, J. (1992). The relation of attachments and supports to adolescent well-being Nada-Raja, Shyamala, McGee, Rob, & Stanton, Warren R. (1992). Perceived attach-
and school adjustment. Journal of Adolescent Research, 17, 28–42. ments to parents and peers and psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal
DeMulder, E. K., Denham, S., Schmidt, M., & Mitchell, J. (2000). Q-sort assessment of Youth and Adolescence, 21(4), 471–485.
of attachment security during the preschool years: Links from home to school. Nickerson, A., & Nagle, R. (2005). Parent and peer attachment in late childhood and
Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 274–282. early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 25, 223–249.
Dogan, S. J., Conger, Rand D., R.D., Kim, K. J., & Masyn, K. E. (2007). Cognitive and Noom, M. J., Dekovi´c, M., & Meeus, W. H. J. (1999). Autonomy, attachment and psycho-
parenting pathways in the transmission of antisocial behavior from parents to social adjustment during adolescence: A double-edged sword? Journal of Adolescence,
adolescents. Child Development, 78(1), 335–349. 22, 771–783.
Duchesne, S., & Larose, S. (2007). Adolescent parental attachment and academic moti- O'Koon, J. (1997). Attachment to parents and peers in late adolescence and their rela-
vation and performance in early adolescence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, tionship with self-image. Adolescence, 32, 471–482.
37(7), 1501–1521. Pace, C. S., San Martini, P., & Zavattini, G. C. (2011). The factor structure of the Inventory
Farrington, D. (2005). Childhood origins of antisocial behavior. Clinical Psychology & of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA): A survey of Italian adolescents. Personality
Psychotherapy, 12, 177–190. and Individual Differences, 51, 83–88.
Ferdinand, R. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Wiznitzer, M. (1995). Continuity and change of self- Papini, D. R., & Roggman, L. A. (1992). Adolescent perceived attachment to parents in
reported problem behaviors from adolescence into young adulthood. Journal of the relation to competence, depression and anxiety: A longitudinal study. Journal of
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(5), 680–690. Early Adolescence, 12, 420–440.
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions of Paterson, J., Pryor, J., & Field, J. (1995). Adolescent attachment to parents and friends in
networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63, 103–115. relation to aspects of self-esteem. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24, 365–376.
Gaik, L. P., Abdullaha, M. C., Habibah, E., & Jegak, U. (2010). International Conference on Rice, K. (1990). Attachment in adolescence: A narrative and meta-analytic review.
Learner Diversity 2010 Development of Antisocial Behaviour. Procedia Social and Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 511–538.
Behavioral Sciences, 7, 383–388. Ridenour, S. T., Lanza, E. C., Donny, E. C., & Clark, D. B. (2006). Different lengths of times for
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & van Etten, M. (2005). Specificity of relations between adoles- progressions in adolescent substance involvement. Addictive Behaviours, 31, 962–983.
cents' cognitive emotion regulation strategies and Internalizing and Externalizing Ritakallio, M., Koivisto, A. M., von der Pahlen, B., Pelkonen, M., Marttunen, M., &
psychopathology. Journal of Adolescence, 28, 619–631. Kaltiala-Heino, R. (2008). Continuity, comorbidity and longitudinal associations
Goodvin, R., Meyer, S., Thompson, R. A., & Hayes, R. (2008). Self-understanding in early between depression and antisocial behaviour in middle adolescence: A 2-year pro-
childhood: Associations with attachment security, maternal perceptions of the spective follow-up study. Journal of Adolescence, 31(3), 355–370.
child, and maternal emotional risk. Attachment & Human Development, 10(4), Ritakallio, M., Luukkaala, T., Marttunen, M., Pelkonen, M., & Kaltiala-Heino, R. (2010).
433–450. Comorbidity between depression and antisocial behaviour in middle adolescence:
Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Bathurst, K., Guerin, D. W., & Parramore, P. (2003). The role of perceived social support. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 64(3), 164–171.
Socioeconomic status in children's development and family environment: Infancy Scott Brown, L., & Wright, J. (2001). Attachment theory in adolescence and its rele-
through adolescence. In M. H. Bornstein, & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic vance to developmental psychopathology. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy,
status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. 8(1), 15–32.
R. Tambelli et al. / Children and Youth Services Review 34 (2012) 1465–1471 1471

Shaver, P., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment-related psychodynamics. Attachment Trentacosta, C. J., & Shaw, D. S. (2008). Maternal predictors of rejecting parenting
& Human Development, 4, 133–161. and early adolescent antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,
Shirk, S. R., Gudmundson, G. R., & Burwell, R. A. (2005). Links among attachment- 247–259.
related cognitions and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child Vermeersch, H., T'Sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., & Vincke, J. (2008). The role of testosterone
and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 172–181. in aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking in adolescent boys. Hormones and
Simons, K. J. M., Paternite, C., & Shore, C. (2001). Quality of parent/adolescent attach- Behavior, 53, 463–471.
ment and aggression in young adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working model of
182–203. the self, attachment and competence in five-year-olds. Child Development, 67,
Sousa, C., Herrenkohl, T. I., Moylan, C. A., Tajima, E. A., Klika, J. B., Herrenkohl, R. C., et al. 2493–2511.
(2011). Longitudinal study on the effects of child abuse and children's exposure to Vieno, A., Nation, M., Pastore, M., & Santinello, M. (2009). Parenting and antisocial
domestic violence, parent–child attachments and antisocial behaviour in adoles- behaviour: A model of the relations between adolescent self-disclosure, parental
cence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26(1), 111–136. closeness, parental control, and adolescent antisocial behavior. Developmental
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Psychology, 45, 1509–1519.
Development, 48, 1184–1199. Whitbeck, L., Conger, R., & Kao, M. Y. (1993). The influence of parental support,
Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., Guterman, E., & Abbott, C. B. (2006). Effects of early and later depressed affect, and peers on the sexual behaviors of adolescent girls. Journal of
family violence on children's behavior problems and depression: A longitudinal, Family Issues, 14, 261–278.
multi-informant perspective. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30(3), 283–306. Zimmermann, P. (2004). Attachment representation and characteristics of friendship
Thompson, R. (1999). Early attachment and later behavior. In J. Cassidy, & P. Shaver (Eds.), relations during adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 83–101.
Handbook of attachment: Research, theory, & clinical applications (pp. 265–286). New
York: The Guilford Press.

S-ar putea să vă placă și