Sunteți pe pagina 1din 12

Adolescent-Parent Interaction

in Family Decision Making


KAY M. PALAN
ROBERT E. WILKES*

This article presents a classification of both adolescent influence strategies and


parental response strategies, developed from in-depth interviews with adoles-
cents, mothers, and fathers. In addition, the perceived effectiveness of adolescent
influence strategies is examined, revealing that adolescents are most successful
in their influence attempts when they emulate adult strategies. Implications of
these findings for future research are discussed.

A lthough the bulk of research on family purchasing


behavior has focused on spouses, documentation of
the significant role of children in this activity is steadily
ADOLESCENTS AND EAMILY
DECISIONS
accumulating (Mangleburg 1990). The major emphasis in Whereas younger children (i.e., 3-11 years old) are
much of the work that does incorporate children (see, more likely to simply ask for products (Isler, Popper, and
e.g., Jenkins 1979; Szybillo and Sosanie 1977; Ward and Ward 1987), adolescents are more likely to use a variety
Wackman 1972) has been the relative infiuence of chil- of influence strategies (Kim, Lee, and Hall 1991). It is
dren on various aspects of family decisions, although through this interaction with parents that adolescents at-
some studies have examined other aspects, such as the tempt to infiuence decision outcomes. According to
effects of parents' general socialization styles (Carlson Moschis (1985, p. 898), however, little attention has been
and Grossbart 1988) and the relationship between con- given to the content of family communications: "One
sumer socialization and frequency of shopping with chil- finds relatively little theoretical and empirical work re-
dren (Grossbart, Carlson, and Walsh 1991). More often garding the role of interpersonal communication in the
than not, younger children—usually age 12 or less— development of consumer behavior of young people."
have been the subjects in these studies. Recognizing that That adolescents do infiuence family decisions and that
older (and therefore more cognitively complex) children this infiuence may vary across different factors have been
may not be well represented by extant work, researchers established by previous work. For example, adolescents
have begun to target adolescents in the context of family have greater Influence in family purchasing decisions in
purchase decisions (see, e.g., Beatty and Talpade 1994; a concept-oriented communication environment (in which
Foxman, Tansuhaj, and Ekstrom 1989). This article ex- children are encouraged to develop their own ideas) and
pands this knowledge base on adolescents and family when their personal resources are greater (e.g., they earn
purchasing behavior by shifting the focus away from rela- higher grades; Foxman et al. 1989). Other work reports
tive influence to the adolescent-parent interaction itself. that motivational aspects (including product importance
Specifically, the article explores (1) the strategies used and usage) provide "the strongest and most consistent
by adolescents to infiuence decision outcomes, (2) the explanations of teenagers' perceived relative infiuence
responses by parents to these infiuence attempts, and (3) across stages and purchase situations" (Beatty and Tal-
the perceived effectiveness of these infiuence attempts. pade 1994, p. 332). Yet a more detailed consideration
We provide a classification scheme of infiuence strategies of the nature of the interaction between adolescents and
to guide future research in this area. parents as part of decision activity is lacking. The need
for such an investigation was noted earlier by Kim et al.
(1991, p. 43): "adolescents' use of infiuence strategies
*Kay Palan is assistant professor in marketing in the College of has been a largely unexplored area to date." Manchanda
Business, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011-2063. Robert Wilkes and Moore-Shay (1996) echo this need and also urge
is a professor in marketing in the College of Business Administration,
examination of how parents respond to children's infiu-
Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409-42101. The authors thank
the reviewers, the associate editor, and the editor for their helpful com- ence attempts. Moschis (1985, p. 910) has also called
ments. All correspondence regarding this research should be sent to the attention to "the need for better understanding of the
first author. nature of family infiuence."
159
© 1997 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH. Inc. • Vol. 24 • September 1997
All rights reserved. 0093-5301/98/2402-OO03$O3.0O
160 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

We are aware of three prior investigations of influence pate and those who did not. The sample includes 15 step-
strategies used by adolescents. In two studies of power parents who felt they had sufficient interaction with the
strategies in intimate relationships (Cowan and Avants adolescent; a comparison of stepfamily interviews with
1988; Cowan, Drinkard, and MacGavin 1984), adoles- those of famihes without stepparents revealed no substan-
cents were asked in group-administered sessions to pro- tial response differences. Families were predominantly
vide written essays on "How I get my way with my white and middle-class, with both parents employed; all
mother . . . father . . . best friend" when the other families had at least one child in addition to the participat-
person does not accede to the request or desire. Teenagers ing adolescent. The adolescent sample consisted of 46
in a study by Kim et al. (1991) were provided 19 strategy males and 54 females.
statements and asked to indicate how often they acted in
the manner described when their parents did not either
buy or permit the purchase of certain things the students Interview Process
wanted. These studies do provide useful information on
A stmctured depth interview consisting of 10 questions
social influence and even some consumer-specific influ-
ence strategies used by adolescents. Eor example. Cowan provided a framework for the interview process. Given
and Avants (1988) identified social influence strategies the discovery-oriented nature of this research, eight of the
ranging from "doing as I please" to "giving reasons" 10 questions (see the Appendix) were not context-spe-
when an adolescent wanted to go somewhere. Kim et al. cific. Each informant was interviewed privately, by the
(1991) determined five dimensions of infiuence strategies first author, so that conversation would not be overheard
used by adolescents when they wanted something bought, by other family members. Informants were promised
including persuasion, not eating, acting stubbornly, ap- complete confidentiality; all interviews were recorded on
proaching the other parent, and playing on emotion. How- audiotape with informants' permission. Informants ex-
ever, the development of a complete set of either adoles- pressed themselves freely and, in fact, often shared infor-
cent or parental strategies was not the primary intent of mation beyond the scope of the interview.
these studies. Moreover, methods used in the studies raise
questions about their findings. First, since teenagers were Content Analysis
asked, to assume a negative response to a request, the
results obtained likely exclude strategies that do not antic- Developing and refining categories of analysis was ini-
ipate denial. Second, the inability to probe and follow up tially performed by the first author. Subsequently, two
on subjects' responses may have restricted the depth of individuals who were unfamiliar with the study reviewed
understanding of these influence strategies. category definitions and coding guidelines by coding sev-
eral family transcripts; their feedback indicated that the
Our objective here is to document the infiuence strate- categorizations were complete and understandable.
gies that adolescents employ in the context of family After the categorization process was completed, three
purchasing behavior and to detail how parents respond to independent coders (two males and one female) coded
these influence attempts. Through an exploratory study the transcripts. Intercoder reliability was established by
of the triad of adolescent-mother-father (following the having each individual code a subsample consisting of
recommendation of Bell [1968] that parent-child relation- 10 family transcripts. Following Kassarjian's (1977) rec-
ships be examined bilaterally), we provide evidence of ommendations, intercoder reliability was calculated as a
the nature of the adolescent-parent interaction process percentage of agreement between each pair of coders.
that may significantly affect or actually determine the Rehabihties (based on 2,550 judgments) were .93, .89,
outcomes of family purchasing behavior. Understanding and .90 for the three coder pairs, exceeding the threshold
the nature of this interaction is a necessary step in under- of .85 for content analyses (Kassarjian 1977). Individual
standing the basis for the relative infiuence of adolescents category reliabilities ranged from .57 to 1.00, with the
detailed in previous research. most serious disagreements reviewed and resolved during
a coder debriefing session. Then, all 100 family transcripts
were divided equally among the three coders for coding.
METHOD Results of this coding formed the basis for the remainder
of the analysis.
Sample
Potential study families were located with the use of
junior high school directories provided by a school district
Interpretive Analysis
in a large southwestem city. A convenience sample of Categories of adolescent influence strategies and paren-
100 families was selected by calling families at random tal response strategies identified through content analysis
and qualifying the family (i.e., a qualified household in- formed the basis for interpretive analysis. Classification
cluded mother, father, and adolescent between 12 and 15 of both adolescent and parental strategies into groups with
years of age). Approximately 70 percent of those families shared commonalities was accomplished with extant re-
contacted agreed to participate; there was no apparent search on spousal infiuence strategies (Davis 1976; Falbo
difference between those families who chose to partici- and Peplau 1980; Scanzoni and Szinovacz 1980; Spiro
ADOLESCENT-PARENT INTERACTION IN DECISION MAKING 161

1983), children's influence attempts (Atkin 1978; Cowan pay for it. . . . I will offer him some sort of deal because
and Avants 1988; Cowan et al. 1984; Isler et al. 1987; the purchase is unacceptable to me. (Father with son, age
Ward and Wackman 1972), and parental responses 12)
(Cowan and Avants 1988; Cowan et al. 1984). If he wants to wear a brand name type of jeans, we pay
for them up to what it costs to buy.Levi's, and he has to
pay the difference. That's just how we handle it, because
CLASSIFICATION OF ADOLESCENT I can get a good buy on Levi's. (Mother with son, age 15)
AND PARENTAL STRATEGIES
As with money deals, adolescents used other deals
As can be seen in Table 1, the adolescent and parental when a purchase was personally important to them. In
strategies identified through content analysis have been fact, adolescents were sometimes willing to sacrifice sig-
organized into seven different strategy categories through nificant time and effort to obtain something they really
the use of interpretive analysis. Consistent with the ex- wanted:
ploratory intent of this study, all strategies are reported
and categorized; however, discussion is hmited to only I'll work for things I want. I told them I'd mow the lawn
for the whole summer for my drum set. It was a lot of
those strategies reported by a minimum combined average
work, but my drum set is great. (Male, age 13)
of 5 percent of adolescents and parents.
Other adolescents offered other deals, fully realizing that
they would probably never follow through and keep their
Bargaining Strategies end of the deal:
As defined by Davis (1976) and Falbo and Peplau I will say, "if you will buy me this I will do this or that,"
(1980), bargaining strategies are used with the intention and yet I never do it. I kind of forget. Sometimes I will
of creating agreement between family members based on do it. (Female, age 13)
mutual gain and mutually satisfactory outcomes. Spiro
(1983) described bargaining strategies as taking the form If I want something, I would say, "if you get it for me, 1
of "If you do this, I'll do that" (p. 394). Four prominent will keep my room clean for a week," or something like
that, bo I keep it clean for a week? That's another story.
bargaining strategies emerged in this study. Two of these (Female, age 13)
strategies, money deals and other deals, are nearly identi-
cal to Spiro's (1983) description Of bargaining. Money For parents, the offer of other deals was an indication
deals followed the form, "if you do this, I'll do that"; that the adolescent's purchase request was not only ac-
adolescents offered to pay for part of a purchase if their ceptable to them, but also an opportunity for the adoles-
parents would pay for the other part: cent to earn the purchase through some sort of household
chores. Moreover, when parents initiated other deals, they
When I'm shopping for jeans, if I like the more expensive
were more likely to monitor compliance than when ado-
pair, then I say, "here, you pay for half of it and I'll pay
the other half." I do that a lot. (Female, age 14) lescents offered other deals.
He liked a CD player, and we talked about it . . . and 1
Some adolescents, not wanting to be deprived of a told him he could work in the garage with me, we made
purchase for lack of money, offered to pay the entire a deal, that he could earn so much and then we'd go put
purchase price to their parents at some later date in return the CD on layaway so he wouldn't be tempted to spend
for parental assumption of the entire cost at the time of the money. (Father with son, age 12)
purchase.
A third bargaining strategy used by both adolescents
When I got my Super Nintendo, at first it was really kind and parents was reasoning. Reasoning, while definitely
of hopeless. I said, "Dad, can I get a Super Nintendo?" not of the same form as "if you do this for me, I'll do this
even though I already had a Nintendo and a computer. for you," does involve discussion focused on reaching a
He said it would depend on how I paid him back, so we mutually satisfying outcome for parents and adolescents
have a bargain going on paying him back about $20 a
month. . . . Things that are pretty expensive that you can. in purchase decisions. For both adolescents and parents,
pay back over a period of time, those are things I negotiate reasoning involved using logical arguments intended to
deals for. (Male, age 13) reach a purchase agreement with the other party so that
both sides would be satisfied. For example, adolescents
Parents used money deals similar in format to adoles- who desired a certain purchase used logical arguments
cent money deals. Typifying parental money deals was that they knew would be received well by their parents
parental awareness that the purchase was personally im- and thus were likely to be successful:
portant to the adolescent tempered with concern about
the acceptability of the purchase to the parent: When I'm discussing a purchase with them, I'll bring up
the quality of the thing, like if you're going to pay $50
We'd say things like "you're going to be left wearing that more for something very expensive and it's going to last
ugly shirt or weird looking tennis shoes." Sometimes I tell four years or longer, well then it makes good sense. And
him that it's going to come out of his allowance if he wants usually my parents are pretty good about that because my
that particular thing. If he wants it he is going to have to dad likes to buy top-quality things. (Female, age 15)
TABLE 1
CLASSIFICATION OF ADOLESCENT INFLUENCE STRATEGIES AND PARENTAL RESPONSE StRATEGIES

Report frequency

Adolescent

Category and strategy/definition Male Female Mother Father


Bargaining:
Adolescent:
Money deals (offer to pay for all or part of purchase) 38 33
27 27
Other deals (offer to do certain behaviors in exchange for purchase) 22 23 43 36
Reasoning (use logical, practical arguments) 22 23 37 25
Negotiation (compromise on purchase options with parents) 1 1 4 7
Parental:
Money deals (offer to pay for part of purchase if adolescent helps pay) 5

CJ)
12 8
Other deals (offer purchase in return for certain behaviors) 3 6 14 11
Reasoning (use logical practical arguments) 13 15 35 39
Alternative (suggest other product choices or shopping locations) 10 11 32 21
Negotiation (compromise on purchase options with adolescent) 1 2 4
Discussion (discuss purchase with adolescent)
2
1 2 1 1
Persuasion:
Adolescent:
Opinionates (express opinions, likes, and dislikes about purchase) 27 28 49 52
Persistence (ask repetitively without irritation) 10 13 34 33
Begging (pleading) 20 25 24 16
Whining (use plaintive tone of voice) 9 12 14 13
Everyone else (base purchase need on fact that other friends have "it") 12 12 30 15
Manipulation (use context or people to indirectly influence purchase) 1 3 8 10
Nagging (ask repetitively in manner that irritates parents) 3 1 1 6
Reasonable request (request purchase that parent considers reasonable) 1 2 5 5
Parental:
Opinionates (express opinions, likes, and dislikes about purchase) 20 29 37 29
Manipulation (use context or people to indirectly influence purchase) 0 0 0 1
Emotional:
Adolescent: i
Anger (show temper, yell) 6 5 7 6
Pouting (cry, withdraw, give "silent treatment") 6 8 18 14
Sweet talk (be unnaturally nice or affectionate to parents) 1
Guilt trip (make parents feel guilty)
2 9 7
3 3 6 3
Humor (use of humor, joking around)
1 0 2 0
Parental:
Ignore (refuse to pay attention to adolescent) 0 0 0 1
Shame (shame or embarrass adolescent purposely) 0 0 0 1
Request:
Adolescent:
Direct ask (request purchase simply without emotion) 20 14 28 22
Express need (state a purchase need without reasons) 5 18 20 14
Express want (state a purchase want without reasons) 12 21 29 29
Demand (insist on having something) 1 0 1 0
Expert
Parental:
Teach skills (teach consumer-related skills purposefully) 12 25 29
Legitimate:
Parental:
Can't afford (deny purchase based on inability to afford item) 3 6 19 17
Delay (postpone purchase decision for some period of time) 2 5 15 3
Simple answer (give yes/no answer to purchase request without reasons) 9 7 16 19
Need only (limit purchases to only things that adolescent needs) 1 3 6 3
Reward (make purchases to reward good behavior) 0 1 5 2
Directive:
Parental:
Ask opinions (invite adolescent likes/dislikes about purchases) 8 8 9 17
Need vs. want (decide if adolescent really needs or merely wants item) 4 5 15 14
Parameters (establish price/product boundaries to guide purchases) 4 7 27 6
Go ask (tell adolescent to go ask other parent for purchase) 0 2 1 2
NOTE.—Reports are frequency of response with n = 46 (males), n = 54 (females), n = 100 (mothers and fathers).
ADOLESCENT-PARENT INTERACTION IN DECISION MAKING 163

Parents, for their part, used reasoning as a bargaining following excerpt eloquently depicts the use of opinionat-
tool to convince their adolescents about the wisdom of ing by an adolescent for her parents' gain:
certain purchases. The reasoning was usually related to I'll tell them that whatever it is that they are trying on
the practicality or economic consequences of a purchase, looks good on them. (Female, age 14)
as seen in the following example:
While the adolescent did not gain materially from this
When we're purchasing things for him, shirts, pants, shoes, form of opinionating, it may be that the adolescent gained
whatever it is, "Son, look at this. This is just going to some other intangible reward through reinforcement of
wash nicer, it will come through the laundry nicer, and
her parents' product choices. Of course, more typical was
you do a lot of laundry yourself, and I just would rather
that it's something that would wash easy, that doesn't have the use of opinionating for adolescent gain:
to he ironed, that isn't 100 percent cotton." (Mother with If I'm shopping for clothes, I tell them that I like it and
son, age 13) that I'd look really cool in it and "the girls would like it."
(Male, age 13)
A bargaining strategy used only by parents was the
suggestion of alternative purchase choices or shopping When we got carpet for the basement, I sort of went around
locations. Containing elements of both reasoning and and tried to get everyone to get the color I liked. . . . I
deals, suggesting alternative items was used when parents always try to persuade them to get what I want, usually by
telling them what I like. (Female, age 15)
believed that the adolescent purchase request was unrea-
sonably priced. Rather than expecting the adolescent to Parents, in contrast, reported using opinionating for the
share the costs of the purchase (i.e., money deals and purpose of persuading the adolescent to select the pur-
other deals), parents reason with their adolescents to shop chase choice most acceptable to the parent, even though
for the same item at a lower cost or a similar but lower- adolescents rarely heeded their parents' advice.
priced item.
I try to say what I think is good. I usually just try to say
All the shirts that kids wear look just alike. You can go to what I think is nice, but that doesn't work because her
Dillard's and buy a shirt that costs $50-$55 and you can tastes and my tastes are different. (Mother with daughter,
go to Sam's and buy the very same shirt for $16. That's . age 12)
what I try to show him. (Mother with son, age 12)
When she was smaller, we could dress her and buy what-
Well, I usually do try to get her to shop a little bit more ever we wanted to dress her in. A while back we were
and see if she can't find the same item for a little less, so shopping and I told her she needed to get this Western
I always suggest, "Look around, just because you found shirt. . . . She said "Dad, when I was little you could
this one, maybe you can find another one that's a better dress me. Now that I'm big, I can make my own decision."
buy." Or I get her to try to think about sales, maybe it's I respect that. She's right. But I still tell her. (Father with
not exactly what she wanted, but I try to get her to go that daughter, age 14)
route. (Mother with daughter j age 14)
Three persuasion strategies used by adolescents—per-
sistence, begging, and whining—are similar in meaning,
yet distinct enough to be identified separately with content
Persuasion Strategies analysis. Persistence was present when adolescents repeti-
Persuasion in family decision making is described as tively made purchase influence attempts, usually over a
convincing an opposing family member to resolve deci- long time period.
sion conflict in the persuader's favor (Davis 1976; Falbo With my parents, if I just keep at it, I usually get it. Like
and Peplau 1980). Thus, persuasion, like bargaining, is with this computer . . . I dreamed up the idea and got
concerned with mutual agreement between family mem- my parents to agree to get the computer for a family Christ-
bers. However, persuasion strategies differ from bar- mas gift. I've been at it for four months now, and it's
gaining strategies in that the focus is on unilateral gain come to the point where my dad is about to pick one out.
for the persuader rather than mutual gain for both the Persistence. You have to keep at it. (Male, age 15)
opposer and the persuader. Thus, persuasion strategies Sometimes for clothes and stuff that my mom doesn't really
may involve some level of manipulation or maneuvering like . . . I just keep telling her, "I want it, 1 want it" and
intended to exploit a situation to the persuader's advan- how much I like it until I finally get it, until she breaks
tage. In addition, persuasion strategies are characterized down. Sometimes I have to do that over a long period of
by an absence of emotion. time. (Female, age 13)
Only one persuasion strategy—"opinionates"—was
frequently used by both parents and adolescents. Opin- Begging was present when adolescents pleaded for a
ionates is simply the expression of likes or dislikes related purchase, typically by saying "please" over and over
to a purchase consideration. A subtle form of persuasion, again. Consider the following excerpt from one mother:
opinionates was rarely manipulative; in addition, even Well, if it's something that she really, really, really wants
though the outcome of unilateral gain was obtained, it for her, like some kind of nail polish or some kind of a
was not always the persuader who gained materially. The thing, she will beg, ' 'Please, please, please, please, please.
164 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Please, please, please, please, please." It just drives me Emotional Strategies


crazy. But she's a beggar and she wants everything she
sees. And she's that way in the grocery store, too. I mean, Emotional strategies entailed the intentional use of
if she does happen to come down the grocery aisle with
me, she'll pick out the wildest, craziest cereal or something emotion, either directly or indirectly, when trying to in-
that we never buy, Ramen noodles or something, ' 'Please, fluence other family members in the purchase decision-
please, please, please, let's try these." (Mother with daugh- making process. Several researchers have identifled the
ter, age 13) use of emotion-laden tactics in decision making, including
crying, pouting, withdrawing, or giving the silent treat-
One adolescent even went so far as to get down on his ment (Falbo and Peplau 1980; Scanzoni and Szinovacz
knees when he begged: 1980; Spiro 1983); anger (Spiro 1983); and having a posi-
tive affect, such as "I smile a lot. . . . I am especially
I beg for things that I want more than things that I need. affectionate" (Falbo and Peplau 1980, p. 621).
I get on my hands and knees and say "please." (Male, Anger was typically used by adolescents after a pur-
age 13)
chase request had been refused and with the full knowl-
Whining refers to the use of a distinct tone of voice edge that it would probably not change the outcome of a
different from the usual tone of voice used by adolescents purchase decision.
and frequently was reported concurrently with both nag- I usually get mad when I don't get what I want and some-
ging and begging. When parents reported the use of whin- times I do that to try to get them to change their minds.
ing to the interviewer, they often imitated whining by (Female, age 14)
using a plaintive, high-pitched voice:
I get mad sometimes . . . but that hardly ever works.
If she wants it bad enough, her tone of voice is pretty (Male, age 12)
noticeable, she'll get real whiny and pushy and whining
for it. (Father with daughter, age 13) Frequently accompanying the use of anger was pouting,
which consisted of behaviors such as crying or withdraw-
A popular persuasion strategy used by adolescents was ing. Like anger, pouting was used with a sense of fatality:
"everyone else." A persuasion argument, everyone else
tries to convince parents to buy an object desired by the I go to my room and shut the door and ignore my parents,
adolescent solely because the adolescent's peers, that is, but they ignore me back. (Male, age 12)
everyone else, already has it. Thus, everyone else was
related to peer pressure. Unlike anger and pouting, a third emotional strategy
used by adolescents involved the use of positive affect and
Usually I say I need it and if I don't get it my friends will was labeled sweet talk. Sweet talk involves adolescent use
make fun of me or laugh at me. (Male, age 14) of verbal and nonverbal behaviors that convey more than
usual affection for parents. For instance, in the following
Recently, I've been wanting a new pair of basketball shoes, passage, an adolescent used nonverbal sweet talk:
and I've been trying to persuade them by telling them that
the pair I have now is not as good as the shoes that have
come out now; these are out because I have the old Jordans, To persuade them . . . I. put on a puppy dog face, and
not the new Jordans, and I'm trying to persuade them that sometimes that works with my mom. (Male, age 13)
the new Jordans are in, and the old ones are out. But they
just won't listen to me. I've been telling them over and Verbal sweet talk, on the other hand, is aptly represented
over. I try to tell them that people don't really make fun in the following excerpt:
of me, but I try to tell them that kids have mentioned that
these shoes are out and the others are in, so I try to tell them He takes on a more childlike disposition and calls me
that all my friends are talking about my shoes. (Female, age "mommy" and puts his arms around my shoulders.
12) (Mother with son, age 13)

Manipulation involves indirect and underhanded meth- Adolescents used guilt trips in an attempt to create guilt
ods, even subterfuge, to influence purchase decisions. in their parents based on family relationships. Typically
Consider the following excerpts describing manipulation this guilt focused on the argument that since the adoles-
tactics. cent's parents had been spending money on other siblings,
the parents should feel guilty about not spending an equal
When she wants something, she won't ask me. Instead, amount of money on the adolescent.
she'll call my mother and my aunt and get them to tell me
that she should get to go to a concert or shopping. (Mother You've bought- • [brother] the last two pairs of tennis
with daughter, age 14) shoes, and I'm due a pair of shoes. (Female, age 12)
If we are grocery shopping, I just go and get what I want If my sister gets something that they bought for her, then
and put it in the basket. . . . I just sneak it in and hope I'll usually tell them that if they bought something for her
they won't say anything. (Female, age 13) they really should get something for me. (Male, age 13)
ADOLESCENT-PARENT INTERACTION IN DECISION MAKING 165

Request Strategies ing, and give her a sense of being a critical consumer.
(Father with daughter, age 13) •
Adolescents used strategies that revolve around asking
for a particular item to be purchased. In past studies, these
tactics have ranged from a fairly direct request, such as Legitimate Strategies
"Can I have this?" (Atkin 1978; Cowan et al. 1984; Isler
et al. 1987), to a more demanding request, such as "Get According to French and Raven (1959), legitimate
this for me when you go to the store" (Atkin 1978; Cowan power is based on the legitimate right of one individual to
et al. 1984). "Direct ask" was present when adolescents influence another individual, who in turn has internalized
values that recognize and acquiesce to this legitimate
requested purchases simply and unemotionally, as in the
right. Inherent in the parent-child relationship is the
following passages:
shared belief that parents have legitimate authority over
I would Just say, "Mom, can I have this please?" (Female, their children; of course, parents realize that as children
age 13) get older, legitimate authority decreases. However, in this
study, parents used five different strategies uniquely based
If I want something, I will just ask and my dad will say
on their roles as legitimate authorities, and the adolescents
"yes" or "no." (Male, age 13)
generally recognized and respected this authority. Legiti-
Two other types of request strategies involved express- mate strategies did not involve making deals or mutual
ing either a need or a want for a particular item. Purchase discussion, as did bargaining strategies; rather, they con-
requests were formulated by using the word "need" or sisted of unilateral, authoritative declarations by the par-
"want" and were unaccompanied by reasons. More so- ent to the adolescent. With few exceptions, legitimate
phisticated than direct ask, "express need" and "express authority was exerted primarily in circumstances when
want'' assume that parents will infer a purchase request parents refused an influence attempt.
from simple statements. One way in which parents rebuffed influence attempts
I guess I use the old "I need this very badly." (Female, was to use the can't afford tactic. With this strategy,
age 15) parents refused influence attempts on the basis that the
item of interest was too expensive to be considered for
I tell them I want it, and then they'll look at it. (Male, age purchase either at the present time or in the future.
12)
It's usually, if we say, "No, we can't afford it," that the
matter is dropped. (Father with son, age 12)
Expert Strategies
Another effective rebuffing strategy was delay. Delay
Expert power, according to French and Raven (1959), is the postponement of a purchase decision until some
rests on the extent of the knowledge or perception that
later point in time and often was a substitute for saying
one individual attributes to another individual within a
" n o " ; that is, the parent had no intention of buying the
given area. This definition was expanded to the use of
item even at a later date.
expert influence between spouses by Spiro (1983). In this
study, one expert strategy emerged, "teach skills," which If he wants something, I usually will say, "We'll see" or
involves the deliberate sharing of parental consumer something like that. When I say, "We'll see," it means
knowledge with adolescents for the express purpose of no, but it's usually my answer. (Mother with son, age 15)
teaching children how to be effective consumers. Teach
skills included instruction related to the price-value rela- A third type of legitimate strategy was simple answer,
tionship, branded products versus generic products, prod- which took one of two forms; in response to purchase
uct quality, budgeting, money-saving skills, and purchas- influence attempts, parents either answered " n o " or
ing skills. The following passages poignantly illustrate "yes." In either case, parents relied on the authority of
the purpose with which parents used teach skills: their position to make purchase decisions without further
input from the adolescent or explanations for their deci-
He gets money every day, so I tell him that he needs to sions.
save his money so that he can purchase the video games
he wants. I'll tell him that he needs to learn to save his Now, if I say, "No, you can't have it," and I have no
money. (Mother with son, age 12) intention of bargaining with her at all, it's usually some-
One thing that we always talk about when we're looking thing in my tone that she knows "that's it, I know not to
at something is the price of it. "For what you're buying, ask anymore." I don't know if it's in the way I say it to
is the price worth the quality of what you're buying?" her or whatever, she can tell the difference. I think she
(Mother with son, age 13) knows when she can and when she can't. (Mother with
daughter, age 13)
I'm always trying to get her to learn the relative value of
things and particularly the impact of advertising and its Well, if he asks for something that I think has great utility
effects on driving purchases and desires. So we try to talk and the price is right, then I won't mind paying for it, and
about that. I point out manipulative or deceptive advertis- I'll just get it. (Father with son, age 13)
166 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Directive Strategies influence strategies affects parents' purchase decisions;


that is, which adolescent strategies are effective and
Similar to legitimate strategies, directive strategies are which are not? Two of the interview questions collected
based on parental authority (Cowan and Avants 1988). informants' perceptions on adolescent strategy effec-
However, whereas legitimate strategies are based on the tiveness. Effectiveness is defined in terms of successful
use of parental authority to make purchase decisions with- accomplishment of the adolescent's goal, that is, get-
out any additional adolescent involvement, directive strat- ting what s/he wants, via the use of an influence strat-
egies involve the use of parental authority to go beyond egy. The more effective strategies, therefore, are those
simple decisions by guiding the purchase decision-mak- reported as being the most successful in getting the
ing process along a particular path. adolescent what s/he wanted, while the least effective
"Ask opinions" involved parents inviting their adoles- strategies are those reported as not working at all in
cents to express what they either liked or did not like getting the adolescent what s/he wanted. Table 2 lists
about a particular purchase. In either situation, parents the three most effective and three least effective adoles-
were directive by drawing their adolescents further into cent influence strategies as perceived by adolescents,
the purchase decision process than had been previously mothers, and fathers, on the basis of a tabulation of"
sought by the adolescent. Consider the following passage responses; it is not surprising that informants had dif-
from an interview with a father: ferent opinions regardirig effectiveness. However, since
this type of analysis fails to capture an understanding
Occasionally I will ask her to go along with me when I of what makes these strategies effective or ineffective,
need to select a new cologne or a new tie or a new shirt
and get some feedback from her as to her opinion of how the interview transcripts were reexamined, comparing
it looks and how it would fit into today's goings-on. I parental strategies to perceptions of effective adoles-
specifically ask her to come along, or if we are at the mall cent influence strategies within families. Interpretation
and we are going through the mall we discuss what I like of the texts in this manner revealed that effective ado-
and what she likes and what she likes me to look like, just lescent strategies closely corresponded to the types of
some interaction along those lines. Whether we agree on parental response strategies reported as being used in a
it or not is two different things; you know, I value her family in two ways. First, effective adolescent influence
opinion. (Father with daughter, age 12) strategies duplicated parental response strategies; for
example, in families where parents reported using rea-
Need versus want is used to discern whether or not an soning strategies, the most effective adolescent influ-
adolescent's declaration of need or desire for an object ence strategy was reported as reasoning. Second, effec-
(i.e., express need, express want) truly is a need or rather tiveness was related to a logical relationship between
is simply a want. Need versus want is directive because the parental response strategy and the adolescent influ-
it forces adolescents to go beyond simply stating, "I need ence strategy. For example, in families where parents
this" or "I want this," to give reasons and explanations used can't afford, adolescents knew that it was effective
that convince their parents that there is a need or a reason- to use strategies that decrease cost, such as money
able want. Consider the following examples. deals, other deals, and reasonable requests.
We really just want to know why she wants something,
and basically she has to give us some reasons. It just can't We found both intrarole and intrafamily correspon-
be "because I want them." It has to be pretty well thought dence among our informants. Intrarole correspondence,
out and if she doesn't do it, then she won't get it. (Father on the one hand, refers to the agreement between a
with daughter, age 13) single informant's (adolescent, mother, or father) re-
But he wants a weight set now. We've been discussing sponses on the most effective adolescent influence
that. . . . "Do you really need it or is it just a want? Do strategy with the types of parental strategies used. For
you really need it?" (Father with son, age 13) example, intrarole correspondence was present when an
adolescent reported that the most effective adolescent
When parents used parameters, it was for the purpose influence strategy to use was reasoning and also re-
of directing adolescents on either price or product limita- ported that his or her parents used reasoning as a paren-
tions, corresponding to a strategy identified by Cowan tal response strategy. Overall, the percentagis agree-
and Avants (1988). Consider the following excerpt.
ment for intrarole correspondence was 40 percent, 35
Normally, when we go in to buy, I give them a price range, percent, and 27 percent, for adolescents, mothers, and
if we're iooking for shoes or shirts or whatever. "Whatever fathers, respectively.
you buy, it must cost between $10 to $30." And it is his Intrafamily correspondence, on the other hand, refers
choice within those limits. (Mother with son, age 15) to the agreement between the adolescent and a parent
on the most effective adolescent influence strategy and
EFFECTIVENESS OF ADOLESCENT parental strategies used. For example, when an adoles-
INFLUENCE STRATEGIES cent reported that the most effective adolescent influ-
ence strategy to use was reasoning and his/her mother
Perhaps most important, at least from a marketing (or father) reported that s/he used reasoning as a parent,
standpoint, is understanding how the use of adolescent then intrafamily correspondence was present. The per-
ADOLESCENT-PARENT INTERACTION IN DECISION MAKING 167

TABLE 2

ADOLESCENT INFLUENCE STRATEGY EFFECTIVENESS

Informant

Adolescent Mother Father

Most effective
strategies Money deals (20%), reasoning Reasoning (18%), reasonable Reasoning (13%), other deals (12%),
(17%), direct ask (15%) request (14%), other deals (11%) direct ask (10%)
Least effective
strategies Begging (19%), everyone else (13%), Whining (19%), everyone else (15%), Begging (18%), anger (17%),
anger (12%) anger (14%) whining (13%), demands (13%)

NOTE.—Percentages are the ratio of number of reports to sample size: n = 100.

centage agreement, intrafamily, was 30 percent for ado- perceived influence—at least self-ascribed influence by
lescent-mother pairs and 40 percent for adolescent- adolescents—in purchasing decisions should manifest
father pairs. Admittedly, this is an initial attempt at this effect. Although their study was based on perceptions
understanding how interaction between adolescents and from teenagers only, Kim et al. (1991) found some evi-
parents impacts purchase decisions. Still, the results dence of this, at least for female adolescents and certain
suggest that adolescents have keen insight into how influence strategies. Consequently, future studies examin-
their parents make purchase decisions and how their ing influence strategies may benefit by also examining
parents will respond to adolescent influence attempts. perceived influence.
Moreover, adolescents know that effective influence Another avenue for future research is to examine the'
attempts are likely when they match their influence underlying motivations of strategy usage. For example,
strategies to their parents' decision-making styles. adolescents' strategy choices might be influenced by
their expectations of parental resistance to influence,
as suggested by Cowan and Avants (1988) and by Man-
DISCUSSION chanda and Moore-Shay (1996). Level of involvement
with the purchase decision for adolescents and parents
This study provides the first comprehensive identifica- may also trigger the use of certain influence and re-
tion and categorization of influence strategies used by
adolescents to influence the outcome of family purchasing sponse strategies.
decisions. In addition, we have documented parental re- Future research might also investigate more closely
sponses to the influence strategies employed by their ado- how family structure and parental rearing practices af-
lescent children. Our results contrast sharply with the fect adolescent participation in family decision making.
dominant "simply ask" strategy of children under age For example, perceived parental warmth has been re-
11 reported by Isler et al. (1987). Although adolescents lated to greater use of social support and problem-fo-
in our study did frequently employ direct requests, our cused coping and reduced emotion-focused coping by
results suggest a more complex pattern of interaction as children and adolescents (Mclntyre and Dusek 1995).
teenagers make use of a variety of influence strategies Carlson, Grossbart, and Stuenkel (1992) found that
and anticipate parental responses to their influence at- mothers who generally respect and solicit children's
tempts. This is consistent with previous research describ- opinions tend to utilize messages that encourage the
ing age-related changes in adolescent thinking (Keating development of consumption decision-making abilities
1990) and transitions in decision-making competence dur- in children. Although limited by a small sample, Man-
ing adolescence (Mann, Harmoni, and Power 1989). We chanda and Moore-Shay (1996) did not find a relation-
show that adolescents are especially likely to use bar- ship between the use of high power strategies by ado-
gaining (money deals, other deals, reasoning) and persua- lescents and either authoritative or permissive parents.
sion (opinionates, begging) as strategies to influence deci- A somewhat different perspective comes from Brown
sion outcomes. Kim et al. (1991) also reported high and Mann (1990), who argue that adolescent participa-
popularity of a reasoning-based strategy by adolescents. tion in family decisions is determined more by exigen-
Cowan et al. (1984) did not report frequency of use but cies (e.g., single parents) or deliberate policy (as found
did report several similar strategies, such as asking, rea- in adaptable families) than by parental style or example.
soning, bargaining. Overall, research suggests that too much or too little
As adolescents learn which strategies "work" for them positive parental involvement promotes negative out-
in the context of family purchasing decisions, they are comes for adolescents (Murry and Bell-Scott 1994).
likely to feel empowered in that process. Measures of Carlson and Grossbart's (1988) work on consumer so-
168 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

cialization and parenting style would be a useful guide together as a family. Whose idea was it to go out to
for research in this area. eat, where did you go, and how was the decision
Certain limitations to our study also serve as sugges- made about where to go?
tions for future work. For example, different age-groups
will likely manifest different types of interaction with
their parents. Although we found little difference in ado- [Received November 1995. Revised December 1996.
lescent strategy usage by sex, future studies should exam- Brian Sternthal served as editor and Deborah Roedder
ine the impact of adolescent sex on interactions with par- John served as associate editor for this article.]
ents. Different family structures should also be examined.
Finally, since the self-selected nature of the sample may REFERENCES
have biased the results, efforts to obtain a random sample
should be made. Atkin, Charles K. (1978), "Observation.of Parent-Child Interac-
By going well beyond a descriptive approach, this ex- tion in Supermarket Decision-Making," Journal of Mar-
keting, 42 (October), 41-45.
ploratory study of adolescent influence attempts and re-
Beatty, Sharon E. and Salil Talpade (1994), "Adolescent Influ-
sponses to such attempts by parents has provided useful ence' in Family Decision Making: A Replication with Ex-
information about adolescent-parent interactions and has tension," Joumal of Consumer Research, 21 (September),
provided a rich foundation for future research. 332-341.
Bell, R. Q. (1968), "A Reinterpretation of the Direction of Ef-
APPENDIX fects in Studies of Socialization," Psychological Review,
75, 81-95.
Interview Format' Brown, J. E. And L. Mann (1990), "The Relationship between
Family Structure and Process Variables and Adolescent
1. Tell me how you get involved in purchases that your Decision-Making," Journal of Adolescence, 13 (March),
mom and dad make—what kinds of things do you 25-37.
like to go with them to shop for? What wouldn't you Carlson, Les and Sanford Grossbart (1988), "Parental Style and
go shopping for? Consumer Socialization of Children," Joumal of Con-
2. When you think of all the purchases your mom and sumer Research, 15 (June), 77-94.
dad make, how often do you think you get involved , Sanford Grossbart, and J. Kathleen Stuenkel (1992),
in their purchase decisions? All of the time, most of "The Role of Parental Socialization Types on Differential
Family Communication Patterns regarding Consumption,"
the time, occasionally, rarely, or never? Joumal of Consumer Psychology, 1 (1), 31-52. .
3. Do you ever try to influence purchase decisions? Do Cowan, Gloria and S. Kelly Avants (1988), "Children's Influ-
you try to get what you want? ence Strategies: Structure, Sex Differences, and Bilateral
4. What kinds of things do you say or do in a purchase Mother-Child Influence," Child Development, 59 (Octo-
situation to try to get what you want? ber), 1303-1313.
5. Of all the things you say or do, what works the best? , Joan Drinkard, and Laurie MacGavin (1984), "The
When are you successful in getting what you want? Effects of Target, Age, and Gender on Use of Power Strate-
6. Of all the things you say or do, what doesn't work gies," Joumal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47
at all? When are your parents most likely to say (December), 1391-1398.
"no"? Davis, Harry L. (1976), "Decision Making within the House-
hold," yowrna/ of Consumer Research, 2 (March), 2 4 1 -
7. What kinds of things do your parents say or do in a 260.
purchase situation in response to you? How do they Falbo, Toni and Letitia Anne Peplau (1980), "Power Strategies
try to influence you? in Intimate Relationships," Journal of Personality and So-
8. Do you approacii your parents differently in a pur- cial Psychology, 38 (4), 618-628.
chase situation? If so, how do you approach your Foxman, Ellen R., Patriya S. Tansuhaj, and Kadn M. Eksu-om
mom and how is that different from how you would (1989), "Adolescents' Influence in Family Purchase Deci-
approach your dad? sions: A Socialization Perspective," Journal of Business
9. Tell me about the last time you went shopping for Research, 18 (March), 159-172.
clothes for you with your mom and dad. Whose idea French, John R. P. and Bertram Raven (1959), "The Bases of
was it to go shopping, what did you go shopping for, Social Power," in Studies in Social Power, ed. Dorwin
and how did everyone get involved in the shopping Cartwdght, Ann Arbor: Research Center for Group Dy-
namics, Institute for Social Research, University of Michi-
process?
gan, 150-167.
10. Tell me about the last time you all went out to eat Grossbart, Sanford, Les Carlson, and Ann Walsh (1991), "Con-
sumer Socialization and Frequency of Shopping with Chil-
dren," Joumal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 19
'This is the interview format for adolescents. The questions for moth- (Summer), 155-163.
ers and fathers were exactly the same, except the referents changed for Isler, Leslie, Edward T. Popper, and Scott Ward (1987), "Chil-
each question. For example, the first question becomes, "Tell me how dren's Purchase Requests and Parental Responses," Jour-
your son (daughter) gets involved in purchases that you make—what nal of Advertising Research, 27 (October-November), 2 8 -
kinds of things does s/he like to go shopping for?"
39.
ADOLESCENT-PARENT INTERACTION IN DECISION MAKING 169

Jenkins, Roger L. (1979), "The Influence of Children in Family Mann, Leon, Ros Harmoni, and Colin Power (1989), "Adoles-
Decision Making: Parents' Perceptions," in Advances in cent Decision-Making: The Development of Compe-
Consumer Research, Vol. 6, ed. W. L. Wilkie, Ann Arbor, tence," Journal of Adolescence, 12 (September), 265-278.
MI: Association for Consumer Research, 413-418. Mclntyre, Julie Guay and Jerome B. Dusek (1995), "Perceived
Kassarjian, Harold H. (1977), "Content Analysis in Consumer Parental Rearing Practices and Styles of Coping," Journal
Research," Journal of Consumer Research, 4 (June), 8 - of Youth and Adolescence, .24 (August), 499-509.
18. Moschis, George P. (1985), "The Role of Family Communica-
Keating, D. P. (1990), "Adolescent Thinking," in At the tion in Consumer Socialization of Children and Adoles-
Threshold: The Developing Adolescent, ed. S. Feldman and cents," Journal of Consumer Research, II (March), 8 9 8 -
G. Elliott, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 913.
Kim, Chankon, Hanjoon Lee, and Katherine Hall (1991), "A Murry, Velma McBride and Patricia Bell-Scott (1994), "Deal-
Study of Adolescents' Power, Influence Strategy, and In- ing with Adolescent Children," in Families and Change:
fluence on Family Purchase Decisions,'' in 199J AMA Win- Coping with Stressful Events, ed. Patrick C. McKenry and
ter Educators' Proceedings, ed. Terry L. Childers et al., Sharon J. Price, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 88-110.
Chicago: American Marketing Association, 37-45. Scanzoni, John and Maximiliane Szinovacz (1980), Family De-
cision-Making: A Developmental Sex Role Model, Beverly
Manchanda, Rajesh V. and Elizabeth S. Moore-Shay (1996), Hills, CA: Sage.
"Mom, I Want That! The Effects of Parental Style, Gender Spiro, Rosann L. (1983), "Persuasion in Family Decision-Mak-
and Materialism on Children's Choice of Influence Strat- ing," Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (March), 3 9 3 -
egy," in 1996 Winter Marketing Educators' Conference 402. '
Proceedings, Vol. 7, ed. Edward A. Blair and Wagner Szybillo, George J. and Arlene K. Sosanie (1977), "Family
A. Kamakura, Chicago: American Marketing Association, Decision Making: Husband, Wife, and Children," in Ad-
81-90. vances in Consumer Research, Vol. 4, ed. W. D. Perrault,
Mangleburg, Tamara F. (1990), "Children's Influence in Pur- Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, 4 6 -
chase Decisions: A Review and Critique," in Advances in 49.
Consumer Research, Vol. 17, ed. Marvin E. Goldberg, Ger- Ward, Scott and Daniel B. Wackman (1972), "Children's Pur-
ald Gom, and Richard W. Pollay, Provo, UT: Association chase Influence Attempts and Parental Yielding," Journal
for Consumer Research, 813-825. of Marketing Research, 9 (August), 316-319.

S-ar putea să vă placă și