Sunteți pe pagina 1din 10

The Effects of Incidental Teaching on Vocabulary Acquisition by Young Children

Author(s): Marta C. Valdez-Menchaca and Grover J. Whitehurst


Reviewed work(s):
Source: Child Development, Vol. 59, No. 6 (Dec., 1988), pp. 1451-1459
Published by: Blackwell Publishing on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130660 .
Accessed: 05/04/2012 18:48

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Blackwell Publishing and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org
The Effects of Incidental Teaching on
Vocabulary Acquisition by Young Children

Marta C. Valdez-Menchaca and Grover J. Whitehurst


State Universityof New Yorkat Stony Brook

C., andWHITEHURST,
MARTA
VALDEZ-MENCHACA, J. TheEffectsof IncidentalTeachingon
GROVER
VocabularyAcquisitionby YoungChildren.CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1988,59, 1451-1459. The effects
on vocabularyacquisition of child-initiatedversus adult-initiatedinstances of adult labeling were
studied. 16 monolingual,English-speakingpreschoolchildren were exposed to a Spanish-speaking
adult. In order to have access to toys placed out of their reach, children had to request the toys in
Spanish.For the experimentalgroup,adult labeling occurredwhen the childrenexpressed interest
in the toy. Control group children were yoked temporallyto experimentalgroup children for the
purpose of adult labeling. Thus, adult labeling for the control group was randomlyrelated to the
children's expressions of interest. Althoughboth groupsof children learned some Spanishwords,
children in the experimentalgroupproducedsignificantlymore Spanishwords duringthe training
sessions and showed superiorperformanceon posttrainingtests of Spanishproduction.Levels of
comprehensionof the Spanishwordswere equivalentforboth groups.Resultsare interpretedin the
contextof the literatureon "incidentalteaching"and are viewed as demonstratingthe criticalrole in
languageacquisitionof the timing of exposureto languagemodels.

Recent research on language interven- elaborate his or her request to fit a target ver-
tion with disadvantaged and developmentally bal response (e.g., "Can I have X so I can Y?")
disabled children has focused on "naturalis- by providing the child with a prompt or cue
tic" approaches that attempt to reproduce the (e.g., "What do you want X for?"). After the
process of language acquisition by normal child produces the correct verbal response, he
children (Warren & Kaiser, 1986). This work or she receives the requested object or activ-
has often been more experimental and defini- ity. Thus, during incidental teaching the child
tive in demonstrating causal effects than the controls the training situation by initiating the
research with normal children that served as interaction and specifying the reinforcer to be
its impetus. As a result, it may be timely for delivered.
the tables to be turned: researchers con-
cerned with language acquisition in normal Extensive applications of incidental
children might usefully investigate variables teaching with disadvantaged and develop-
that have proven effects on developmentally mentally disabled children have repeatedly
disabled children. This could lead to en- demonstrated its effectiveness in training
hanced knowledge of normal development, both receptive language (e.g., McGee, Krantz,
abnormal development, and the relations be- Mason, & McClannahan, 1983) and expres-
tween them. A case in point is incidental sive language (e.g., Fabry, Mayhew, & Han-
teaching, a "naturalistic" procedure that in- son, 1984; Hart & Risley, 1975; McGee,
volves timing of occasions of instruction Krantz, & McClannahan, 1985) and in
based on the child's spontaneous interests facilitating their generalization (e.g., Hart &
(Hart & Risley, 1974, 1975, 1980). Risley, 1980; Neef, Walters, & Egel, 1984).
In an incidental teaching situation, the The incidental teaching approach in-
child is exposed to a natural, unstructured, volves the assumption that learning will be
rich environment containing diverse objects enhanced through the use of the child's spon-
and activities that can be accessed through taneous interest to select instances of instruc-
the use of language. When the child expresses tion. That is, the presentation of a verbal
an interest, either verbally or nonverbally, in model will be more efficient if it follows the
a particular object or activity, a teaching trial child's expression of interest than if it occurs
is initiated. The teacher requires the child to arbitrarily (i.e., following a preselected se-

These data were originally presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in
Child Development, April 1987. This work was supported in part by NICHHD grant 1 R01
HD19245 to the second author. Address inquiries to the second author at: Department of Psychol-
ogy, SUNY, Stony Brook, NY 11794.

[ChildDevelopment,1988,59, 1451-1459.? 1988by the SocietyforResearchin Child Development,Inc.


All rightsreserved.0009-3920/88/5906-0013$01.00]
1452 Child Development

quence). Although this notion has occasion- is positively correlated with the child's lexical
ally been discussed in the literature (e.g., development (Tomasello & Farrar, 1986).
Hubbell, 1977), it has never been tested di- These correlations suggest that if a parent pro-
rectly. vides a language model when the child is al-
Most studies comparing the effectiveness ready attending to a stimulus, it is more likely
of "structured" or "direct" methods of instruc- that this label will be learned.
tion with "incidental" or "interactive" proce- There has been one experimental test of
dures have evaluated intervention packages the relation between timing of adult labeling
as they are usually applied in clinical or edu- and children's expressions of interest using
cational settings. Thus, the particular effects normal children. Tomasello and Farrar (1986)
of any single variable (i.e., child-initiated presented verbal models of four novel words
trials vs. adult-initiated trials, arbitrary rein- to 1-year-olds under two different conditions.
forcement vs. functional reinforcement, sit- In the "follow-in" condition, a verbal model
ting face-to-face vs. free play) cannot be esti- was presented when the child was attending
mated. In addition, the findings from this to the target object (i.e., holding and looking),
work are controversial. Several reports claim while in the control condition, the verbal
that interactive procedures are superior to di- model was presented when the child was not
rective methods (e.g., McGee et al., 1985; attending to any of the stimuli. Children's
Neef et al., 1984), while other studies fail to comprehension scores were superior for la-
find significant differences (e.g., Cole & Dale, bels that had been presented following the
1986; Friedman & Friedman, 1980). Further- child's focus of attention. There were no ef-
more, a significant interaction has been found fects on production scores.
between type of treatment and linguistic abil-
ities, with higher-functioning children bene- Unfortunately, Tomasello and Farrar's re-
sults are difficult to interpret because of the
fiting more from the interactive procedure
nature of the control condition. Verbal labels
(Friedman & Friedman, 1980). Therefore,
in the control condition were only provided
generalization of findings from develop-
mentally disabled populations to normal chil- contingent upon the children's not attending.
dren requires caution. This contingency for not attending to the ref-
erent could actively suppress learning. As a
In the literature with normal children, result, it is impossible to determine whether
some descriptions of mothers engaging in the differences between the "follow-in" con-
what appear to be incidental teaching epi- dition and the control condition were due to
sodes have been presented. For example, the facilitating effects of "following in," the
Snow (1977) states that mothers are quite inhibiting effects of the control procedure, or
willing to follow their children's topics in or- both. In addition, the failure to find any ef-
der to keep a conversation going. Similarly, fects on production scores by Tomasello and
Collis (cited in Rondal, 1985) observed that Farrar (1986) is troublesome, and may be re-
mothers were particularly likely to label an lated to these investigators' failure to provide
object when they saw their children attending any motivation for their subjects to use the
to that object. words to which they had been exposed.
Correlational research involving natu- This article is designed to extend the ex-
rally occurring verbal interactions between perimental corroboration of the hypothesis
normal children and their mothers has sup- that the relation between the timing of paren-
ported the view that presenting a verbal tal labeling and children's expressions of in-
model based on the child's interests may en- terest is important in vocabulary acquisition.
hance vocabulary acquisition. Della Corte, Specifically, the present study evaluates the
Benedict, and Klein (1983) found that effects of presenting verbal models following
mothers of children with large nominal the children's initiations or expressions of in-
vocabularies produced a larger number of terest on both production and comprehension
descriptions of ongoing or present events abilities measures. The present experimental
compared to mothers of children with small condition was designed to resemble the
nominal vocabularies. Tomasello and col- interactions of children with responsive
laborators (Tomasello & Farrar, 1986; To- mothers. These mothers are usually tuned to
masello & Todd, 1983) have found a their children's interests and initiations, but
significant correlation between children's vo- they are occasionally directive in that they
cabulary size and the way parents direct their label objects to which their children are not
children's attention. More specifically, the attending. The control condition corresponds
number of object references presented fol- to the interactions of children with directive
lowing the child's ongoing focus of attention mothers. These mothers usually attempt to
Valdez-Menchaca and Whitehurst 1453
draw their children's attention and redirect private preschool every other day. Based on
their interest, but they are occasionally re- parental report, the children had no previous
sponsive in that they label objects following knowledge of the Spanish language.
their children's initiations and expressions of
interest. Pairs of children were matched for
Pairs of monolingual, English-speaking chronological age. Members from each pair
were randomly assigned to either the experi-
2-year-olds were exposed to models of Span- mental or control group, the only restriction
ish labels for toys in a relatively natural nurs-
being that there be the same number of boys
ery school environment. This setting has pre- and girls in each group.
viously been shown to result in high levels of
verbal production and to be very sensitive The experimenter was an adult bilingual
to variations in the learning environment female who had been present in the chil-
(Whitehurst & Valdez-Menchaca, 1988). In dren's preschool class for several sessions
the experimental condition, the Spanish ver- prior to the study. The experimenter used
bal models were presented contingent upon only Spanish to communicate with the chil-
each child's expression of interest in the toys. dren before and during the study.
Given that children interacted in pairs, the
second child could be exposed to the Spanish Setting and materials.-The study took
verbal models without expressing interest in a place in a room at the children's preschool.
The room contained a table and chairs for the
toy. In the control condition, the Spanish ver- children and several children's books. A mi-
bal models were presented based on temporal
crophone connected to a tape recorder lo-
yoking. In this procedure, presentation of ver-
bal models for individual subjects in the con- cated in an adjacent room was used to record
the training sessions. Ten unfamiliar toys,
trol condition occurred at the times when
instances of labeling occurred for matched previously demonstrated to be highly attrac-
subjects in the experimental group. This ran- tive to children in this age group, were the
stimulus materials. The Spanish names of the
dom relation between labeling and children's
behavior resulted in control children's expres- toys were the target words.
sions of interest being followed by Spanish Procedure.-Prior to training, an individ-
models randomly. In addition, similar to the ual assessment of each child's preference for
experimental children, control children were the toys was conducted by an assistant. The
exposed to verbal models without having ex- experimenter and the assistant escorted the
pressed interest in a toy, based on the behav- child to the experimental playroom where
ior of the other child in the pair. Thus, the toys were located. The assistant con-
although both groups of children were pre- ducted the preference test by explaining to
sented with Spanish models independent of the child in English how each toy functioned
their interests, it was only for the experimen- without giving a label for the toys (e.g., you
tal children that expressions of interest in a blow it; it clicks). The child was encouraged
toy were always followed by modeling of the to play with each toy. After the child played
corresponding Spanish word. with all the toys, the assistant asked which toy
the child liked the most. The preferred object
Two hypotheses were tested in this
was then removed and the process was re-
study. First, children in the experimental
peated until all toys were selected. During
group were expected to produce more Span- this session, the experimenter occasionally
ish words than children in the control condi-
commented about the child's activities using
tion. Second, children in the experimental
only Spanish, but did not provide Spanish
group were expected to perform better on a labels for the toys. The order in which the
formal assessment of their production and
toys were selected was used as the measure of
comprehension of the Spanish words than the child's preference for the toys. Based on
children in the control condition.
this preference measure, yoked pairs of chil-
dren (one control and one experimental) were
Method formed by matching those children who had
selected the same sets of three toys as the
Subjects.-Sixteen monolingual, En-
most preferable and least preferable.
glish-speaking, middle-class children, 10 boys
and six girls, served as subjects. Children Training was conducted during 12 10-
ranged in age from 28 to 34 months (mean age 12-min sessions, spread over approximately 4
= 31.06 months, SD = 3.15). The experimen- weeks. Sessions were carried out with a pair
tal children's mean age was 31.75 months (SD of children in the same experimental condi-
= 2.78) and the control children's was 30.75 tion. The experimenter communicated with
months (SD = 3.45). All subjects attended a the children using only Spanish. No nouns
1454 Child Development
were used except for the children's names Thus, while the experimenter's labeling for
and the target words. Verbal exchanges were the experimental group was responsive to the
socially relevant, such as greetings, answers children's initiatives, labeling occurred arbi-
to questions, comments, etc. All the exper- trarily for the control group. If a control or
imenter's verbalizations were accompanied experimental child requested a toy in English
with nonverbal communication such as ges- or pointed at it, the experimenter would nod
tures, facial expressions, and conventional her head, shrug her shoulders, raise her arms,
sounds (e.g., "se cayo, boom"/"it fell, boom"). and say: "No entiendo" (I do not understand).
Children's books were placed on a low When a child in the control group produced a
table and were available to all children Spanish word to request a toy, whether or not
throughout training. The stimulus toys were the experimental child had also requested it,
located on two shelves within sight, but out of the toy was handed to the control child.
reach of the children. In each instance of Hence, the contingency for access to the toys
labeling, the experimenter would take a par- was the same for both groups; they differed
ticular toy, show it to the child, and model its only in the way in which the verbal models
Spanish name three times. Labeling occurred were provided.
twice within declarative sentences (e.g., Tests of production and comprehension
"Aqui esta el payaso") and once using the of the Spanish names of the toys were ad-
single word (e.g., "payaso"). The target word ministered to all children individually on
was always presented by accentuating each the day following the last training session.
syllable and in a rising tone of voice. In order The production assessment always preceded
for any child to have access to a particular toy, the comprehension test. The production test
he or she had to produce the toy's name in was conducted by showing one toy and ask-
Spanish. Children were allowed to keep the ing the child to name it (e.g., "What is this
toy for one minute each time they produced called?"). Any approximate pronunciation of
the Spanish word. the Spanish name was considered a correct
For the experimental group, labeling oc- response (e.g., "yaso" for "payaso"). If an En-
curred every time a child expressed interest glish response was given, additional instruc-
in a toy, either by requesting it in English or tions were provided (e.g., "What do we call it
through gestures. For the control group, label- when we play here?"). Three trials were pro-
ing was yoked temporally to the labels pro- vided for each item if the child did not pro-
vided for the matched experimental child vide a response. If after three trials it was not
with similar toy preferences. Each training clear that the child understood the instruc-
session for the yoked control pair was carried tions, the experimenter moved on to the next
out after the session for the corresponding ex- items and later came back to the initial ones.
perimental pair. A written record indicating Comprehension was assessed by presenting
the time at which any particular toy was three toys at a time and asking the child to
labeled in Spanish for the experimental child point to one of them. Instructions were given
was obtained from the audiotape of the ex- in English and the toy's name was given in
perimental session. The matched control Spanish (e.g., "Point to the payaso"). Again,
child was then presented with the Spanish three trials were provided in an absence of a
labels at the same time during the session as response. No reinforcement was provided
the experimental child with whom he or she contingent upon the children's responses on
had been paired according to toy preferences. either test.

TABLE 1
DEFINITIONS AND EXAMPLESOF CHILDREN'S VERBAL BEHAVIORCATEGORIES

Definitions Examples
Spanish words: production of any single word or its approximation in Span-
ish. (A Spanish word was categorized as "spontaneous" if it was pro-
duced in the absence of a verbal model during the preceding 10 sec.) ... Payaso; 'yaso
Requests: requests for any of the stimulus toys using English. I want it
............
Statements related to the target objects: comments in English concerning
any of the stimulus toys ....................................... I'm feeding the clown
Statements not related to the target objects: comments in English about
events not related to the stimulus toys ............................... Come here!
Valdez-Menchaca and Whitehurst 1455
Results subsequent two-tailed t test of the regression
coefficients of production of Spanish words
Interobserver agreement.-Independent
records of 12 sessions, randomly selected on training sessions by each experimental
across the training phase, were checked for group was conducted. This analysis revealed
interobserver agreement. These records were that the slopes of the regression lines for the
coded by two experimentally blind, bilingual experimental and control groups differed sig-
observers using the behavior codes presented nificantly, t(20) = 2.45, p < .05; b = .641 for
in Table 1. Interobserver agreement for train- the experimental group, and b = .363 for the
control group. This finding indicates that
ing sessions was computed using the statistic
while both groups of children increased their
kappa. The comparison of the records showed
an overall mean kappa value of .96, with the production of Spanish words across sessions,
individual categories ranging from .95 to .98. experimental children were producing Span-
ish words at a higher rate than control chil-
Training sessions.-Analyses were con- dren.
ducted utilizing the multivariate approach for The frequency of Spanish words pro-
repeated measures (Bock, 1975). A two-group duced in the absence of a verbal model (i.e.,
(experimental vs. control) x 12 training ses- spontaneous words) was also analyzed. Spon-
sions MANOVA was conducted for each of
taneous Spanish words were a subcategory of
the four dependent measures (i.e., Spanish the general category of Spanish words and
words, English requests, English verbaliza- were included in the analyses described
tions related to the toys, and English verbali- above. Given that there was a very low fre-
zations not related to the toys). These anal-
quency of spontaneous words per session, it
yses revealed no significant differences in any was necessary to collapse across training ses-
measure of English verbal production. Table sions to perform the statistical analysis. Thus,
2 displays the mean frequencies of each ver-
a one-way analysis of variance (experimental
bal behavior category per child per session
group vs. control group) for total number of
per group. spontaneous words produced per child was
Analyses of Spanish words showed that conducted. This analysis indicated that chil-
children in the experimental group produced dren in the experimental group produced
significantly more Spanish words than the significantly more spontaneous Spanish
control children, F(1,14) = 9.62, p < .008 (see words than the controls, F(1,14) = 8.324, p <
fig. 1). A significant effect for training sessions .013. The mean frequency of spontaneous
was also found for Spanish words, F(11,4) = Spanish words produced by experimental
6.74, p < .04, but the interaction between group was 5.375 (SD = 4.719, range: 0-14),
training session and experimental condition while for the control group it was 0.5 (SD =
was not significant, F(11,4) = 1.817, p > .29. 0.726, range: 0-2).
These analyses indicate that both groups of A one-way analysis of variance (experi-
children increased the number of Spanish mental group vs. control group) for the num-
words produced over sessions. However, an ber of different Spanish words produced
examination of Figure 1 shows a greater dif- across training per child was conducted. This
ference between the production of Spanish analysis revealed a marginally significant ef-
words by the two groups toward the end of fect for experimental condition, F(1,14) =
training than at the initial sessions. Thus, a 3.858, p < .08. On the average, children in the

TABLE 2
MEAN FREQUENCIES PER CHILD PER SESSION OF VERBAL PRODUCTIONS
BY CHILDREN IN THE EXPERIMENTALAND CONTROL GROUPS

GROUP

Experimental Control
VERBALCATEGORY Mean SD Mean SD
Spanish words .............................. 6.15 3.76 3.28 1.86
Requests 6.27 2.51 6.58 3.27
Statements.....................................
related to the target objects ......... 5.19 3.14 4.75 2.65
Statements not related to the target objects ...... 7.20 2.79 8.31 2.97
1456 Child Development
- e- Control
----- Experimental
10-

9-

7-
0
a) 6
L 5-

3-

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

TrainingSessions
FIc. 1.-Mean frequency per session per child of use of Spanish words by children in the experimen-
tal and control groups.

experimental group produced 7.375 (SD = Discussion


2.446) different Spanish words, and children
in the control group produced 5.125 (SD = The facilitative function of the use of
children's spontaneous interests to time lan-
2.167) different Spanish words.
guage instruction was supported in the pres-
A Spearman rank correlation between ent research with normal children. When
the frequency in which a particular toy was Spanish models were presented contingent
labeled in Spanish and the order in which upon children's requests for a toy, children
this toy was selected during the preference produced more Spanish words than if Spanish
test was conducted for each child. The mean models were presented independent of their
correlation coefficient for each experimental interests. These results are consistent with
group was computed (rho = .437, SD = .15, work with disadvantaged and developmen-
for the experimental group; rho = .414, SD = tally disabled children in which incidental
.17, for the control group). The mean correla- teaching has proved to be effective in teach-
tion coefficient for each group was then tested ing new forms of language (e.g., McGee et al.,
for significance by combining the degrees of 1985; Neef et al., 1984). In addition, the pres-
freedom from each individual correlation as ent results suggest that, independent of the
recommended by Guilford (1950). Both mean effects of the use of functional reinforcers and
coefficients reached significance at the .01 the type of teaching situation, the timing of
level in a two-tailed test. the presentation of verbal models is a relevant
Test scores.-Two factor that contributes significantly to the suc-
one-way analyses of
variance (experimental group x control cess of incidental teaching.
group) were performed for the scores on the A common finding in the incidental
production and comprehension tests. The teaching research with developmentally dis-
analysis of the production test showed that abled children is an increase in spontaneous
children in the experimental group (X = 4.38, language use (e.g., Carr & Kologinsky, 1983;
SD = .92) performed significantly better, Hart & Risley, 1980). Similar effects were ob-
F(1,14) = 6.66, p < .02, than children in the served in this study. Toward the end of train-
control group (X = 2.63, SD = 1.69). On the ing, experimental children were more likely
comprehension test, children in the experi- than control children to request toys spon-
mental group (X = 8.38, SD = 1.3) per- taneously using their Spanish names. Seven
formed better than the control children (X = experimental children produced at least one
7.13, SD = 1.36); however, this difference spontaneous word, whereas only three control
was not statistically significant. children produced Spanish words spontane-
Valdez-Menchaca and Whitehurst 1457
ously. Accordingly, the total number of spon- First, as suggested by Tomasello and Far-
taneous words produced across sessions was rar, their lack of significant results on the pro-
significantly higher for the experimental duction test may be due to the low levels of
group than for the control group. These verbal production characteristic of all their
findings are consistent with Hubbell's (1977) subjects. The present work enhanced the con-
claim that a reduction of the adult constraints ditions for verbal production by utilizing sub-
and the presentation of verbal input based on jects with higher linguistic abilities, present-
the child's present interests are two basic ele- ing attractive stimuli (i.e., manipulative toys),
ments that facilitate spontaneous language and requiring the children to label them in
use. order to play with them. Tomasello and Far-
The similarity between the results of this rar, on the other hand, worked with younger
children, used less interesting objects, and
study and those reported with disadvantaged
and developmentally disabled children has did not apply any contingency for the use of
implications for the controversy regarding the language.
superiority of incidental teaching over struc- Second, the effects of the "follow-in"
tured approaches. The finding that normally condition on comprehension in Tomasello
developing children benefited from the use of and Farrar's research could have been due to
the interactive procedure is consistent with the use of a single presentation of the verbal
previous results showing that developmen- model. If comprehension depends on detect-
tally disabled children who have higher lev- ing the relation between referent and label
els of verbal ability are more likely to ben- and if that requires attending to the referent,
efit from the use of interactive techniques then a single presentation of a label might not
(Friedman & Friedman, 1980). Hence, this re- allow the child time to find the referent while
search suggests that comparable learning pro- still remembering the label. Since in this
cesses may be taking place in normal children study three verbal models were provided in
and in those developmentally disabled chil- each instance of labeling, the successive pre-
dren for whom the incidental teaching proce- sentation of labels might have given the child
dure has proved to be successful. However, a who was not attending to the referent time to
different learning model may be required for do so.
those developmentally disabled children for
whom artificial and highly structured training Hence, the present findings complement
Tomasello and Farrar's results rather than
techniques are necessary.
contradict them. Both studies are consistent
The analyses of the test scores indicated with the notion that the timing of presentation
that, in addition to producing a higher num- of verbal models based on the child's behav-
ber of Spanish words, children in the experi- ior is a relevant factor in the process of learn-
mental condition were also able to label more ing new vocabulary. The different results
of the toys in Spanish than the control chil- derived from these studies suggest that inci-
dren. However, understanding of the Spanish dental teaching (or "joint attention learning")
words was comparable in the two groups; may be a complex process that is influenced
children in both the experimental and control by other factors such as general level of verbal
conditions performed above chance on the production, contingencies operating in the
comprehension test. This differential effect environment for verbal responses, and fre-
on the production abilities is concordant with quency of verbal models provided. A dif-
the view that verbal production is a skill ference in the variables that influence the
(Moerk, 1986), and suggests that practice ef- processes of language production and com-
fects may be operative in this finding. Com- prehension is also implied. Further research
prehension, on the other hand, does not re- is necessary to identify the specific effects of
quire overt practice, and thus may reach high these and other factors on different aspects of
levels based only on associative learning language development.
(Whitehurst & DeBaryshe, in press).
The differential effect found in this study
The present results of the assessment for the incidental teaching condition on the
of Spanish words are not in keeping with production measures is relevant to interven-
Tomasello and Farrar's (1986) previous tion procedures for children who experience
findings. These authors showed a significant language delay. Whitehurst, Fischel, Caul-
effect for the "follow-in" procedure on the field, DeBaryshe, and Valdez-Menchaca (in
comprehension measures but not on the pro- press) have systematically varied the learn-
duction measures. Several differences in the ing conditions and contingencies for language
methods used in the two studies may explain use operating in the home environment of a
these conflicting results. group of children with specific expressive lan-
1458 Child Development
guage delay. A procedure including inciden- vided per se cannot explain differences in
tal teaching produced significant progress in rate of lexical acquisition.
expressive language with treated children
when compared with nontreated controls. In conclusion, the present work suggests
The present results validate the inclusion of that a view of language acquisition based ex-
incidental teaching in an intervention pack- clusively on the study of verbal input, disre-
age by demonstrating its effects in a more garding the timing and function of the input,
is insufficient. Research directed at a fine-
tightly controlled situation than would ever
be possible with a clinical population. grained analysis of the relations between pa-
rental and child behavior appears to provide a
The present results are also relevant to promising area for a more complete under-
correlational analyses that have shown a posi- standing of the acquisition of language by
tive relation between maternal labeling in re- normal and developmentally disabled chil-
sponse to children's attention or interests and dren.
children's nominal vocabulary size (e.g.,
Della Corte et al., 1983; Masur, 1982; To-
References
masello & Todd, 1983; Tomasello & Farrar, Bock, R. D. (1975). Multivariate statistical methods
1986). Our results are consistent with these in behavioral research. New York: McGraw-
studies and provide experimental cor- Hill.
roboration. Carr, E. G., & Kologinsky, E. (1983). Acquisition of
sign language by autistic children: II. Spon-
It is important to note that incidental
taneity and generalization effects. Journal of
teaching is not the only way that mothers Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 297-314.
might use their children's attention and inter- Cole, K. N., & Dale, P. S. (1986). Direct language
ests to teach them new labels. For example, a instruction and interactive language instruction
mother might label an object that the child is with language delayed preschool children: A
not attending to but that is very likely to be of
comparison study. Journal of Speech and Hear-
high interest given the mother's knowledge of ing Research, 29, 206-217.
the child (e.g., "Look, there's a helicopter"). Della Corte, M., Benedict, H., & Klein, D. (1983).
The role of the value of the referent in The relationship of pragmatic dimensions of
mothers' speech to the referential-expressive
vocabulary acquisition is relevant to this point
to the extent that mothers understand and distinction. Journal of Child Language, 10, 35-
utilize their children's preference when label- 43.
ing objects for them. Whitehurst, Kedesdy, Fabray, B. D., Mayhew, G. L., & Hanson, A. (1984).
and White (1982) found that children's learn- Incidental teaching of mentally retarded stu-
ing of labels of novel objects was significantly dents in a token system. American Journal of
correlated with the level of preference of the Mental Deficiency, 89, 29-36.
children for those objects. These results were Friedman, P., & Friedman, K. (1980). Accounting
replicated in this study by the significant posi- for individual differences when comparing the
tive correlation between the order in which effectiveness of remedial language teaching
toys were selected in the preference test and methods. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 151-
the frequency with which these toys were 170.
labeled during training. These findings lend Guilford, J. P. (1950). Fundamental statistics in
support to the "preference hypothesis" psychology and education (2d ed.). New York:
(Whitehurst, 1979; Whitehurst et al., 1982) for McGraw-Hill.
vocabulary learning, which holds that labels Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1974). Using preschool
for those referents most valued or preferred material to modify the language of disadvan-
by a child are most likely to be learned. taged children. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 7, 243-256.
The current findings also have implica- Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1975). Incidental teaching
tions for Nelson's (1973) unexpected failure to of language in the preschool. Journal of Ap-
find a significant relation between frequency plied Behavior Analysis, 8, 411-420.
of maternal labeling and children's vocab- Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1980). In vivo language
ulary development. This work shows that intervention: Unanticipated general effects.
learning of vocabulary is a function of when Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13,
the verbal model is presented and that very 407-432.
low rates of acquisition can occur despite fre- Hubbell, R. D. (1977). On facilitating spontaneous
quent exposure. This is consistent with other talking in young children. Journal of Speech
work (Schwartz & Terrel, 1983) showing that and Hearing Disorders, 42, 216-231.
the number of times a verbal model is pro- Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers' responses to infants'
Valdez-Menchaca and Whitehurst 1459
object-related gestures: Influences on lexical Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint atten-
development. Journal of Child Language, 9, tion and early language. Child Development,
23-30. 57, 1454-1463.
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mason, D., & McClan- Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention
nahan, L. E. (1983). A modified incidental- and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4,
teaching procedure for autistic youth: Acqui- 197-211.
sition and generalization of receptive object Warren, S. F., & Kaiser, A. P. (1986). Incidental
labels. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, language teaching: A critical review. Journal of
16, 329-338. Speech and HearingDisorders,51, 291-299.
McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. Whitehurst, G. J. (1979). Meaning and semantics. In
(1985). The facilitative effects of incidental G. J. Whitehurst & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.),
teaching on preposition use by autistic chil- The functions of language and cognition (pp.
dren. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 115-139). New York: Academic Press.
18, 17-31. Whitehurst, G. J., & DeBaryshe, B. D. (in press).
Moerk, E. L. (1986). Environmental factors in early Observational learning and language acquisi-
language acquisition. In G. J. Whitehurst (Ed.), tion: Principles of learning, systems, and tasks.
Annals of child development(Vol. 3, pp. 191- In G. E. Speidel & K. E. Nelson (Eds.), The
235). Greenwich, CT: JAI. many faces of imitation in language learning.
Neef, N. A., Walters, J., & Egel, A. L. (1984). Estab- New York: Springer-Verlag.
lishing generative yes/no responses in devel- Whitehurst, G. J., Fischel, J. E., Caulfield, M. B.,
opmentally disabled children. Journal of Ap- DeBaryshe, B. D., & Valdez-Menchaca, M. C.
plied BehaviorAnalysis, 17, 453-460. (in press). Assessment and treatment of early
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning expressive language delay. In P. R. Zelazo & R.
to talk. Monographs of the Society for Re- Barr(Eds.),Challengesto developmentalpara-
search in Child Development, 38(1-2, Serial digms: Implicationsfor assessment and treat-
No. 149). ment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rondal, J. A. (1985). Adult-child interaction and Whitehurst, G. J., Kedesdy, J., & White, T. G.
the process of languageacquisition.New York: (1982). A functional analysis of meaning. In S.
Praeger. KuczajII (Ed.), Language development: Syn-
Schwartz, R. G., & Terrel, B. Y. (1983). The role of tax and semantics (Vol. 1, pp. 397-428). Hills-
input frequency in lexical acquisition. Journal dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
of Child Language, 10, 57-64. Whitehurst, G. J., & Valdez-Menchaca, M. C.
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversa- (1988). What is the role of reinforcement in lan-
tions between mothers and babies. Journal of guage acquisition? Child Development, 59,
Child Language,4, 1-22. 330-340.

S-ar putea să vă placă și