Sunteți pe pagina 1din 2

PFI_12mmX178mm.

pdf + eps format

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 59:5 (2018), pp 487–488 doi:10.1111/jcpp.12925

Editorial: One size does not fit all: addressing the


challenges of intervention for complex
developmental issues
As I come to write this, my last editorial for JCPP, I am effect sizes. Crucially, improvements in sleep quality
once again impressed with the eclectic mix of topics, and quantity fully mediated the effect of CBT for
methods and populations that are contained within these insomnia on symptoms of psychopathology.
pages. Child psychology and psychiatry is certainly a Of course such findings require replication before
broad church and what delights me about JCPP, as changes to clinical practice are mandated. Outcome
someone who primarily works in the field of child measures were self-report and therefore participants
language disorders, is the opportunity to learn from were not blind to treatment status. Participants were
other disciplines. It is always a relief to see that predominantly (75%) female and did not have con-
researchers and clinicians in every field are grappling firmed primary psychiatric disorders; the impact of
with many of the same issues, and to find some such interventions on clinically referred adolescent
commonalities (as well as differences) in our approaches. populations is therefore much needed. The sample
Here, I would like to focus on intervention and the size (~40 per group) is relatively small; there is
wide ranging impacts intervention can have on chil- increasing awareness that small studies tend to have
dren dealing with a variety of developmental con- larger effect sizes than large studies (Button et al.
cerns. Two of the critical trends that have influenced 2013), which may distort findings and decrease the
clinical trials in recent years include the increasing likelihood that they will replicate in larger, or more
focus on implementation (Green, 2016), or turning clinically impaired, samples. Nevertheless, this study
therapeutic research discoveries into practical benefit provides encouraging first evidence that a relatively
for human health and well-being. In practice, this low-cost, low-intensity intervention focused on sleep
means testing interventions that can readily be could yield positive impacts on psychological well-
implemented and ‘scalable’ in real-world settings. being for adolescents with relatively minor psy-
This forces researchers to consider the practical and chopathology symptoms.
financial challenges of intervention delivery and Our research does not occur in a political vacuum
raises important questions regarding dosage and and the unfolding humanitarian crisis in Syria, and
implementation. In addition, we are developing ever other crises around the world, has tested our ability to
more sophisticated techniques to explicitly test devel- support the mental health and well-being of young
opmental cascades. Cascade models presuppose that people in extremis. Panter-Brick and colleagues (in this
changes to one developmental domain, function or issue) are to be congratulated for so successfully
system alter the development of another domain, conducting an intervention trial against all the odds.
function or system over time. Interventions designed They provided 8 weeks (two sessions per week) of group
to target processes for change represent cascade therapy to young people with profound stress experi-
models (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010) and have impor- ences (vs. a waiting list control) that aimed to address
tant implications for preventive interventions. This is symptoms of insecurity, distress, mental health diffi-
because well-timed and targeted interventions could culties, prosocial behaviour and posttraumatic stress.
interrupt negative or promote positive cascades, and Here, the study was powered to detect more modest
thereby attenuate some of the negative downstream effect sizes (d = .30), included a longer term follow-up of
consequences of developmental disorder. Clever trial 7–14 months postgroup treatment, and was delivered
designs that include longitudinal follow-up and mea- by local individuals working for existing organizations
surement of the downstream target are needed to test in the region. A major challenge for the research group
these cascade effects. was the high rate of attrition (43.5%).
Three intervention trials reported in this issue of The authors report small but significant treatment
JCPP illustrate these trends and highlight the chal- effects on three of the five self-report outcome mea-
lenges involved in delivering research with immediate sures, and sustained impact on human insecurity at
application. De Bruin et al. (in this issue) specifically follow-up. There was, not surprisingly, wide variation
test a cascade model in which improvements in ado- in outcome; at an individual level, statistical models
lescent sleep were hypothesized to yield positive suggested age and prior trauma exposure were impor-
impacts on symptoms of psychopathology. The study tant predictors of treatment success. But there were
involved a CBT intervention for insomnia delivered also differences between intervention cycles, with
over six, weekly group sessions; sessions were deliv- more muted effects in the second cycle of the interven-
ered either face-to-face or via the Internet (vs. a wait- tion in which an RCT design was fully implemented
list control), and included a follow-up 2 months later. and the baseline scores of individual participants were
Postintervention symptoms of psychopathology on a less severe. Variation in implementation, and access to
self-report measure decreased significantly for both a wider variety of humanitarian programmes were also
treatment groups, with medium to quite large (d = .97) considered to affect treatment success. To my mind, a

© 2018 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
488 Editorial J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2018; 59(5): 487–8

more nuanced understanding of what individual and Third, we need to accept that many developmental
contextual factors contribute to treatment success or concerns (such a language disorder) are incredibly
failure is more clinically relevant than a simple ‘does stable. In these cases, change requires sustained input,
this work?’ question. Blanket approaches in such especially as child
volatile settings are unlikely to fully address individual behaviours and the envi- There is increasing
needs. ronments children expe- focus on implementa-
Finally, Burgoyne et al. (in this issue) investigate rience are often changing.
tion, or turning
an adaptation of a tried and tested approach to What if our research ques-
developing children’s oral language skills that uses tion shifted from ‘does this
therapeutic research
parents, as opposed to clinicians or educators, as the work’ to ‘what would it discoveries into practi-
agents of change. The treatment was much more take to achieve the desired cal benefit for human
intensive, designed to be delivered several times a outcome?’ With that in health and well-being.
week over a 30-week period, and included a follow-up mind, intervention trials In practice, this means
assessment at 6 months that tested both core out- could usefully compare testing interventions
comes, and cascading impacts on early literacy. different implementation that can readily be
Parents received a modest amount of training methods, varying dosage implemented and
(1.5 hr) to deliver the programme and thus, this has and agents of delivery. ‘scalable’ in real-world
potential to be extremely cost-effective given the Technology presents an settings. This forces
intended intensity. important tool for meeting
researchers to consider
The statistical approach to analysis employed demand when resources
latent variables, which have the advantage of explor- are stretched, but there
the practical and
ing change in the underlying construct, unfettered by may be limited contexts in financial challenges of
measurement error. This large study provided evi- which this is applicable. intervention delivery
dence of a moderate (d = ~.35) treatment effect on Finally, in many fields, and raises important
language that was maintained at follow-up and we see a move to provi- questions about
accompanied by a similar degree of difference on sion of ‘universal’ ser- dosage and implemen-
early literacy measures. These differences were vices, designed to be tation.
harder to detect on individual tests, where measure- delivered to all children
ment error may obscure real change. with a view to reducing the numbers of children
A challenge for this type of programme is that referred to tertiary services. While these efforts are
parents, on average, completed only 50% of the laudable, there will always be a need for specialist
intended sessions. Issues of parent language and interventions targeted at individuals who will not
literacy (as the programme involved shared book benefit from a universal approach. We therefore need
reading) may limit the extent to which some families to maintain flexibility in the system to deal with
may access this intervention. Once again, participants varying and ongoing clinical need. One size does not
were not clinically referred, with only 20% falling below fit all, and while this presents challenges, it is also
commonly accepted cut-offs for language disorder. It is what drives innovation and our understanding of
encouraging that these children make as much pro- developmental psychopathology.
gress during the intervention period as more verbally
able peers, but replication in clinical cohorts is needed.
Each of these studies represents an important Courtenay Frazier Norbury
advance in their respective fields and should influence C.N. is professor of developmental language and
choices about effective approaches to supporting chil- communication disorders at the University College
dren and young people faced with developmental chal- London. She is a joint editor for JCPP. She has
lenges. Collectively, they also inform us about some declared that she has no competing or potential
common issues that require wider dialogue and con- conflicts of interest in relation to this editorial.
sideration by those of us undertaking intervention
research. First, testing developmental cascade models
Courtenay Frazier Norbury
requires longitudinal data, and very few intervention
studies include follow-up periods of sufficient duration
References
to really test cascading effects. Second, statistical power
Bornstein, M.H., Hahn, C.-S., & Putnick, D.L. (2016). Stability
is a critical issue that should concern us all. We of core language skill across the first decade of life in
know that underpowered studies can reveal positive children at biological and social risk. Journal of Child
findings that are likely to be statistical artefacts, rather Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 1434–1443.
than true effects (Button et al. 2013). There is a growing Button, K.S., Ioannidis, J.P.A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B.A., Flint,
consensus that trials of psychological processes should J., Robinson, E.S.J., & Munafò, M.R. (2013). Power failure:
why small sample size undermines the reliability of neu-
be powered to detect medium sized effects, are practical
roscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 365–376.
in terms of the sample sizes required, and can provide Green, J. (2016). Editorial: Ingenious designs and causal
meaningful results. In addition, even seemingly inference in child psychology and psychiatry. Journal of
small changes can have large cascading impacts Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 549–551.
(Bornstein, Hahn, & Putnick, 2016), but we need to Masten, A.S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades.
explicitly model these using longitudinal trial designs. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491–495.

© 2018 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

S-ar putea să vă placă și